Abstract
The emotional Stroop task is an experimental paradigm developed to study the relationship between emotion and cognition. Human participants required to identify the color of words typically respond more slowly to negative than to neutral words (emotional Stroop effect). Here we investigated whether chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) would show a comparable effect. Using a touch screen, eight chimpanzees were trained to choose between two simultaneously presented stimuli based on color (two identical images with differently colored frames). In Experiment 1, the images within the color frames were shapes that were either of the same color as the surrounding frame or of the alternative color. Subjects made fewer errors and responded faster when shapes were of the same color as the frame surrounding them than when they were not, evidencing that embedded images affected target selection. Experiment 2, a modified version of the emotional Stroop task, presented subjects with four different categories of novel images: three categories of pictures of humans (veterinarian, caretaker, and stranger), and control stimuli showing a white square. Because visits by the veterinarian that include anaesthetization can be stressful for subjects, we expected impaired performance in trials presenting images of the veterinarian. For the first session, we found correct responses to be indeed slower in trials of this category. This effect was more pronounced for subjects whose last anaesthetization experience was more recent, indicating that emotional valence caused the slowdown. We propose our modified emotional Stroop task as a simple method to explore which emotional stimuli affect cognitive performance in nonhuman primates.
Similar content being viewed by others
Notes
It should be noted that for the color discrimination training A, the onsets of the trial initiation display, the 500-ms waiting display, the stimuli, and the feedback interval were each accompanied by additional program-execution-related average delays of approximately 1–16 ms. For the color discrimination training B, the onsets of the trial initiation display and of the 500-ms waiting display, the first onset of the stimuli, and the feedback interval onset were each accompanied by additional program-execution-related average delays of approximately 7–16 ms.
Following the suggestion of an anonymous reviewer, we also explored latencies in incorrect trials. Across sessions and categories, while response latencies in our sample of seven subjects tended to be slower in incorrect trials (mean of latency medians: M = 862.00 ms) than in correct trials (M = 768.07 ms), this effect was not statistically significant, t(6) = 1.55, p = .173. We further investigated specifically for trials presenting the veterinarian, whether response latencies from subjects with anaesthetization experience were slower in incorrect than in correct trials: Considering data from all three sessions, we did not find a significant difference between latencies in correct vs. incorrect vet trials, t(5) = 1.43, p = .211. Considering Session 1 alone, in spite of a sizable mean difference in response latency between incorrect vet trials (M = 1320.08 ms) and correct vet trials (M = 908.75 ms), this effect was not statistically significant, t(5) = 1.98, p = .105. We would like to add a note of caution with regard to these results, however. For several subjects, the rate of incorrect responses was very low. In particular, when considering vet trials alone, this means that some of the latency scores that had to be used in these analyses were based on as little as two to four data points (three subjects in Session 1). Such small numbers imply low measurement reliability, even when medians are used as measures of central tendency. For the same reason, latency comparisons between different categories that were restricted to incorrect trials were not carried out because several subjects made no errors in one or more of the non-veterinarian categories in one or more of the three sessions.
References
Algom D, Chajut E, Lev S (2004) A rational look at the emotional Stroop phenomenon: a generic slowdown, not a stroop effect. J Exp Psychol Gen 133:323–338. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.133.3.323
Bar-Haim Y, Lamy D, Pergamin L, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Van Ijzendoorn MH (2007) Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and nonanxious individuals: a meta-analytic study. Psychol Bull 133:1–24. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.133.1.1
Bateson M, Nettle D (2015) Development of a cognitive bias methodology for measuring low mood in chimpanzees. PeerJ 3:e998. doi:10.7717/peerj.998
Ben-Haim MS, Mama Y, Icht M, Algom D (2014) Is the emotional Stroop task a special case of mood induction? Evidence from sustained effects of attention under emotion. Atten Percept Psychophys 76:81–97. doi:10.3758/s13414-013-0545-7
Bethell EJ (2015) A “how-to” guide for designing judgment bias studies to assess captive animal welfare. J Appl Anim Welf Sci 18(sup1):18–42. doi:10.1080/10888705.2015.1075833
Bethell EJ, Holmes A, Maclarnon A, Semple S (2012) Cognitive bias in a non-human primate: husbandry procedures influence cognitive indicators of psychological well-being in captive rhesus macaques. Anim Welf 21:185–195. doi:10.7120/09627286.21.2.185
Bovet D, Vauclair J (2000) Picture recognition in animals and humans. Behav Brain Res 109:143–165. doi:10.1016/S0166-4328(00)00146-7
Boysen ST, Berntson GG (1989) Conspecific recognition in the chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes): cardiac responses to significant others. J Comp Psychol 103:215–220. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.103.3.215
Buckley TC, Blanchard EB, Neill WT (2000) Information processing and PTSD: a review of the empirical literature. Clin Psychol Rev 20:1041–1065. doi:10.1016/S0272-7358(99)00030-6
Cohen J, Cohen P (1983) Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences. Erlbaum, Hillsdale
Constantine R, McNally RJ, Hornig CD (2001) Snake fear and the pictorial emotional Stroop paradigm. Cognit Ther Res 25:757–764. doi:10.1023/A:1012923507617
De Ruiter C, Brosschot JF (1994) The emotional Stroop interference effect in anxiety: attentional bias or cognitive avoidance? Behav Res Ther 32:315–319. doi:10.1016/0005-7967(94)90128-7
Fagot J, Martin-Malivel J, Dépy D (2000) What is the evidence for an equivalence between objects and pictures in birds and nonhuman primates. In: Fagot J (ed) Picture perception in animals. Psychology Press, New York, pp 295–320
Field M, Cox WM (2008) Attentional bias in addictive behaviors: a review of its development, causes, and consequences. Drug Alcohol Depen 97:1–20. doi:10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2008.03.030
Fox E, Griggs L, Mouchlianitis E (2007) The detection of fear-relevant stimuli: Are guns noticed as quickly as snakes? Emotion 7:691–696. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.7.4.691
Frings C, Englert J, Wentura D, Bermeitinger C (2010) Decomposing the emotional Stroop effect. Q J Exp Psychol 63:42–49. doi:10.1080/17470210903156594
Hirata S et al (2013) Brain response to affective pictures in the chimpanzee. Sci Rep. doi:10.1038/srep01342
Kano F, Tanaka M, Tomonaga M (2008) Enhanced recognition of emotional stimuli in the chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes). Anim Cogn 11:517–524. doi:10.1007/s10071-008-0142-7
Kindt M, Brosschot JF (1997) Phobia-related cognitive bias for pictorial and linguistic stimuli. J Abnorm Psychol 106:644–648. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.106.4.644
King JE, Figueredo AJ (1997) The five-factor model plus dominance in chimpanzee personality. J Res Pers 31:257–271. doi:10.1006/jrpe.1997.2179
King HM, Kurdziel LB, Meyer JS, Lacreuse A (2012) Effects of testosterone on attention and memory for emotional stimuli in male rhesus monkeys. Psychoneuroendocrino 37:396–409. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2011.07.010
Koda H, Sato A, Kato A (2013) Is attentional prioritisation of infant faces unique in humans?: comparative demonstrations by modified dot-probe task in monkeys. Behav Process 98:31–36. doi:10.1016/j.beproc.2013.04.013
Koster EH, Crombez G, Van Damme S, Verschuere B, De Houwer J (2004) Does imminent threat capture and hold attention? Emotion 4:312–317. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.4.3.312
Lacreuse A, Schatz K, Strazzullo S, King HM, Ready R (2013) Attentional biases and memory for emotional stimuli in men and male rhesus monkeys. Anim Cogn 16:861–871. doi:10.1007/s10071-013-0618-y
Lang PJ, Davis M, Öhman A (2000) Fear and anxiety: animal models and human cognitive psychophysiology. J Affect Disord 61:137–159. doi:10.1016/S0165-0327(00)00343-8
Lavy E, Van den Hout M (1993) Selective attention evidenced by pictorial and linguistic Stroop tasks. Behav Ther 24:645–657. doi:10.1016/S0005-7894(05)80323-5
Mackay DG, Shafto M, Taylor JK, Marian DE, Abrams L, Dyer JR (2004) Relations between emotion, memory, and attention: evidence from taboo Stroop, lexical decision, and immediate memory tasks. Mem Cogn 32:474–488. doi:10.3758/BF03195840
MacLeod C, Mathews A, Tata P (1986) Attentional bias in emotional disorders. J Abnorm Psychol 95:15–20. doi:10.1037//0021-843X.95.1.15
Marzouki Y, Gullstrand J, Goujon A, Fagot J (2014) Baboons’ response speed is biased by their mood. PLoS ONE 9(7):e102562. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0102562
Mathews A, MacLeod C (1985) Selective processing of threat cues in anxiety states. Behav Res Ther 23:563–569. doi:10.1016/0005-7967(85)90104-4
Mauer N, Borkenau P (2007) Temperament and early information processing: temperament-related attentional bias in emotional Stroop tasks. Pers Indiv Differ 43:1063–1073. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2007.02.025
McKenna FP, Sharma D (2004) Reversing the emotional Stroop effect reveals that it is not what it seems: the role of fast and slow components. J Exp Psychol Learn 30:382–392. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.30.2.382
Mogg K, Bradley BP (2003) Selective processing of nonverbal information in anxiety: attentional biases for threat. In: Philippot P, Feldman RS, Coats EJ (eds) Nonverbal behavior in clinical settings. Oxford University Press, New York, pp 127–143
Öhman A, Mineka S (2001) Fears, phobias, and preparedness: toward an evolved module of fear and fear learning. Psychol Rev 108:483–522. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.108.3.483
Parr LA (2001) Cognitive and physiological markers of emotional awareness in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Anim Cogn 4:223–229. doi:10.1007/s100710100085
Paul ES, Harding EJ, Mendl M (2005) Measuring emotional processes in animals: the utility of a cognitive approach. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 29:469–491. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.01.002
Phaf RH, Kan K-J (2007) The automaticity of emotional Stroop: a meta-analysis. J Behav Ther Exp Psy 38:184–199. doi:10.1016/j.jbtep.2006.10.008
Pomerantz O, Terkel J, Suomi SJ, Paukner A (2012) Stereotypic head twirls, but not pacing, are related to a ‘pessimistic’-like judgment bias among captive tufted capuchins (Cebus apella). Anim Cogn 15:689–698. doi:10.1007/s10071-012-0497-7
Pratto F, John OP (1991) Automatic vigilance: the attention-grabbing power of negative social information. J Pers Soc Psychol 61:380–391. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.61.3.380
Robbins SJ, Ehrman RN (2004) The role of attentional bias in substance abuse. Behav Cogn Neurosci Rev 3:243–260. doi:10.1177/1534582305275423
Schmidt LJ, Belopolsky AV, Theeuwes J (2014) Attentional capture by signals of threat. Cogn Emot. doi:10.1080/02699931.2014.924484
Shibasaki M, Kawai N (2009) Rapid detection of snakes by Japanese monkeys (Macaca fuscata): an evolutionarily predisposed visual system. J Comp Psychol 123:131–135. doi:10.1037/a0015095
Shibasaki M, Isomura T, Masataka N (2014) Viewing images of snakes accelerates making judgements of their colour in humans: red snake effect as an instance of ‘emotional Stroop facilitation’. R Soc Open Sci. doi:10.1098/rsos.140066
Siegrist M (1995) Effects of taboo words on color-naming performance on a Stroop test. Percept Motor Skill 81:1119–1122. doi:10.2466/pms.1995.81.3f.1119
Underwood AJ (1997) Experiments in ecology: their logical design and interpretation using analysis of variance. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge
Waters AJ, Sayette MA, Wertz JM (2003) Carry-over effects can modulate emotional Stroop effects. Cogn Emot 17:501–509. doi:10.1080/02699930143000716
Weiss A et al (2009) Assessing chimpanzee personality and subjective well-being in Japan. Am J Primatol 71:283–292. doi:10.1002/ajp.20649
Williams JMG, Mathews A, MacLeod C (1996) The emotional Stroop task and psychopathology. Psychol Bull 120:3–24. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.120.1.3
Witthöft M, Rist F, Bailer J (2008) Enhanced early emotional intrusion effects and proportional habituation of threat response for symptom and illness words in college students with elevated health anxiety. Cogn Ther Res 32:818–842. doi:10.1007/s10608-007-9159-5
Yiend J (2010) The effects of emotion on attention: a review of attentional processing of emotional information. Cogn Emot 24:3–47. doi:10.1080/02699930903205698
Yiend J, Barnicot K, Koster EH (2013) Attention and emotion. In: Robinson MD, Watkins ER, Harmon-Jones E (eds) Handbook of cognition and emotion. Guilford Press, New York, pp 97–116
Acknowledgments
The authors wish to thank the staff at Leipzig Zoo, particularly the zoo veterinarian and the chimpanzee caretakers, for their various contributions to stimulus preparation and data collection. We thank Thurston Cleveland Hicks and Fabrizio Maffessoni for their contributions to stimulus preparation. We thank Alexander Weiss for providing a German version of the Hominoid Personality Questionnaire. We thank Daniel Geissler, Stefan Leideritz, Johannes Grossmann, and Sarah Peoples for providing personality ratings of the chimpanzees.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Animal husbandry and research comply with the “EAZA Minimum Standards for the Accommodation and Care of Animals in Zoos and Aquaria,” the “WAZA Ethical Guidelines for the Conduct of Research on Animals by Zoos and Aquariums,” and the “Guidelines for the Treatment of Animals in Behavioral Research and Teaching” of the Association for the Study of Animal Behavior (ASAB).
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Allritz, M., Call, J. & Borkenau, P. How chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) perform in a modified emotional Stroop task. Anim Cogn 19, 435–449 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-015-0944-3
Received:
Revised:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-015-0944-3