Abstract
When we make saccadic eye movements or goal-directed arm movements, there is an infinite number of possible trajectories that the eye or arm could take to reach the target1,2. However, humans show highly stereotyped trajectories in which velocity profiles of both the eye and hand are smooth and symmetric for brief movements3,4. Here we present a unifying theory of eye and arm movements based on the single physiological assumption that the neural control signals are corrupted by noise whose variance increases with the size of the control signal. We propose that in the presence of such signal-dependent noise, the shape of a trajectory is selected to minimize the variance of the final eye or arm position. This minimum-variance theory accurately predicts the trajectories of both saccades and arm movements and the speed–accuracy trade-off described by Fitt's law5. These profiles are robust to changes in the dynamics of the eye or arm, as found empirically6,7. Moreover, the relation between path curvature and hand velocity during drawing movements reproduces the empirical ‘two-thirds power law’8,9. This theory provides a simple and powerful unifying perspective for both eye and arm movement control.
This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution
Access options
Subscribe to this journal
Receive 51 print issues and online access
$199.00 per year
only $3.90 per issue
Buy this article
- Purchase on SpringerLink
- Instant access to full article PDF
Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout





Similar content being viewed by others
References
Bernstein, N. The Coordination and Regulation of Movements(Pergamon, London, (1967)).
Wolpert, D. M. Computational approaches to motor control. Trends Cogn. Sci. 1, 209–216 (1997).
Morasso, P. Spatial control of arm movements. Exp. Brain Res. 42, 223–227 (1981).
Collewijn, H., Erkelens, C. J. & Steinman, R. M. Binocular coordination of human horizontal saccadic eye-movements. J. Physiol. 404, 157–182 (1988).
Fitts, P. M. The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movements. J. Exp. Psychol. 47, 381–391 (1954).
Shadmehr, R. & Mussa-Ivaldi, F. Adaptive representation of dynamics during learning of a motor task. J. Neurosci. 145:, 3208–3224 (1994).
Brashers-Krug, T., Shadmehr, R. & Bizzi, E. Consolidation in human motor memory. Nature 382, 252–255 (1996).
Lacquaniti, F., Terzuolo, C. A. & Viviani, P. The law relating kinematic and figural aspects of drawing movements. Acta Psychologica 54, 115–130 (1983).
Viviani, P. & Schneider, R. Adevelopmental study of the relationship between geometry and kinematics in drawing movements. J. Exp. Psychol. HPP 17, 198–218 (1991).
Enderle, J. D. & Wolfe, J. W. Time-optimal control of saccadic eye-movements. IEEE Trans. Biomed. Eng. 34, 43–55 (1987).
Harris, C. M., Wallman, J. & Scudder, C. A. Fourier analysis of saccades in monkeys and humans. J. Neurophysiol. 634:, 877–886 (1990).
Harris, C. M. On the optimal control of behaviour: A stochastic perspective. J. Neurosci. Meth. 83, 73–88 (1998).
Hogan, N. An organizing principle for a class of voluntary movements. J. Neurosci. 4, 2745–2754 (1984).
Flash, T. & Hogan, N. The co-ordination of arm movements: An experimentally confirmed mathematical model. J. Neurosci. 5, 1688–1703 (1985).
Uno, Y., Kawato, M. & Suzuki, R. Formation and control of optimal trajectories in human multijoint arm movements: Minimum torque-change model. Biol. Cybern. 61, 89–101 (1989).
Wolpert, D. M., Ghahramani, Z. & Jordan, M. I. An internal model for sensorimotor integration. Science 269, 1880–1882 (1995).
Clammam, P. H. Statistical analysis of motor unit firing patterns in human skeletal muscle. Biophysics J. 9, 1233–1251 (1969).
Matthews, P. B. C. Relationship of firing intervals of human motor units to the trajectory of post-spike after-hyperpolarization and synaptic noise. J. Physiol. 492, 597–628 (1996).
Meyer, D. E., Abrams, R. A., Kornblum, S., Wright, C. E. & Smith, J. E. K. Optimality in human motor performance: Ideal control of rapid aimed movements. Psychol. Rev. 98, 340–370 (1988).
Harris, C. M. Does saccadic under-shoot minimize saccadic flight-time? A Monte Carlo study. Vision Res. 35, 691–701 (1995).
Robinson, D. A., Gordon, J. L. & Gordon, S. E. Amodel of the smooth pursuit eye movement system. Biol. Cybern. 55, 43–57 (1986).
van Gisbergen, J. A. M., Robinson, D. A. & Gielen, C. C. A. M. Aquantitative analysis of generation of saccadic eye movements by burst neurons. J. Neurophysiol. 45, 417–442 (1981).
van der Helm, F. C. T. & Rozendaal, L. A. in Biomechanics and Neural Control of Movement(eds Winters, J. M. & Crago, P. E.) (Springer, New York, in the press).
Kelso, J. A. S., Southard, D. L. & Goodman, D. On the nature of human interlimb coordination. Science 203, 1029–1031 (1979).
Lackner, J. R. & DiZio, P. Rapid adaptation to Coriolis force perturbations of arm trajectory. J. Neurophysiol. 72, 299–313 (1994).
Sainburg, R. L. & Ghez, C. Limitations in the learning and generalization of multi-joint dynamics. Soc. Neurosci. Abstr. 21, 686 (1995).
Flash, T. & Gurevich, I. in Self-Organization, Computational Maps and Motor Control(eds Morasso, P. G. & Sanguinetti, V.) 423–481 (Elsevier, Amsterdam, (1997)).
Goodbody, S. J. & Wolpert, D. M. Temporal and amplitude generalization in motor learning. J. Neurophysiol. 79, 1825–1838 (1998).
Kawato, M. in Attention and Performance, XVI(eds Inui, T. & McClelland, J.) 335–367 (MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, (1996)).
Jeannerod, M. The Neural and Behavioural Organization of Goal-directed Movements(OUP Psychology Ser. No. 5, Oxford, (1988)).
Acknowledgements
We thank S. Goodbody, Z. Ghahramani and M. Pembrey for comments on the manuscript. This project was supported by the MRC, the Wellcome Trust, Help a Child to See charity, the IRIS fund, the Child Health Research Appeal Trust and the Royal Society.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Harris, C., Wolpert, D. Signal-dependent noise determines motor planning. Nature 394, 780–784 (1998). https://doi.org/10.1038/29528
Received:
Accepted:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/29528