PDF-Assessing Motivation To Communicate 2nded
PDF-Assessing Motivation To Communicate 2nded
Assessing Motivation to Communicate: Willingness to Communicate and Personal Report of Communication Apprehension 2nd Edition
Edited By Sherwyn P. Morreale University of Colorado at Colorado Springs
All NCA Publication Program materials are reviewed within the spirit of academic freedom, promoting the free exchange of ideas. The contents of this publication are the responsibility of its authors and do not necessarily reect the ofcial policies or positions of the National Communication Association, its members, its ofcers, or its staff.
2007 National Communication Association. All rights reserved. Brief portions of this publication may be copied and quoted without further permission with the understanding that appropriate citations of the source will accompany any excerpts. A limited number of copies of brief excerpts may be made for scholarly or classroom use if: the materials are distributed without charge or no fees above the actual duplication costs are charged; the materials are reproductions, photocopies, or copies made by similar processes, and not reprints or republications; the copies are used within a reasonable time after reproduction; the materials include the full bibliographic citation; and the following is also clearly displayed on all copies: "Copyright by the National Communication Association Reproduced by permission of the publisher." This permission does not extend to situations in which extensive amounts of material are reproduced or stored in an electronic or similar data retrieval system, a fee above actual duplicating costs is charged or if there exists a reasonable expectation of prot, or the material is reproduced or reprinted for other than scholarly or educational purposes. In such cases, permission must be obtained prior to reproduction and generally a reasonable fee will be assessed. Requests for permission to reproduce should be addressed to the Publications Manager. National Communication Association 1765 N Street, NW Washington, D.C., 20036 Library of Congress Catalog Card Number: ISBN: 0-944811-13-2
Assessing Motivation to Communicate: Willingness to Communicate and Personal Report of Communication Apprehension
This program includes two standardized and tested instruments to be used in assessing motivation to communicate at the higher education level. The instruments were developed and tested by James McCroskey and are included in this program along with reference materials to inform their use. This program and the instruments it contains may be used: (a) to assess two dimensions of the motivational domain of students; (b) for testing-in or testing-out (placement) purposes; (c) as a tool for instructing and advising students; and (d) to generate assessment data for departmental or institutional accountability. The author/editor acknowledges the contributions to this program of James McCroskey, who developed the instruments contained here, as well as the efforts of many communication scholars who tested and used these instruments over many years. The efforts on the annotated bibliography of research assistant, Robert Hrdlickha, are also gratefully acknowledged. The technological support of Shawn Morgan of the University of Colorado at Colorado Springs provided the electronic versions of the two instruments that are included with this manual. Philip Backlund of Central Washington University, in his role as chair of the NCA Assessment Division, both encouraged and supported the development of this second edition.
NCA Non-Serial Publication Series Gust Yep, Editor San Francisco State University
The NCA Non-serial Publications (NSP) Program publishes book-length projects focusing on theoretical and/or pedagogical issues related to the study and practice of human communication in a variety of contexts. Projects grounded in social scientic, interpretive, critical, performance, and rhetorical approaches and methodologies are included. Diverse views of communication ranging from microscopic (e.g., social cognition, affect and emotion in communication) to macroscopic (e.g., public discourse, systems of representation) are also included. Topics that have been central to the history of the discipline as well as those that have been marginalized and excluded in the discipline are included as are projects with an inclusive, interdisciplinary, and social justice agenda.
Table of Contents
I. BACKGROUND . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . What is motivation as an important part of communication competence? . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . How can motivation be measured? . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . What is communication apprehension? . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . What is willingness to communicate? . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . II. FACT SHEET ABOUT THIS PROGRAM . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Contents of the program and the two instruments . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . History and description of the program . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Recommendations for using the program . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 7 7 7 7 7 8 8 8 8
III. THE TWO INSTRUMENTS. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 10 Personal Report of Communication Apprehension . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 10 Willingness to Communicate . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 12 IV. LOGISTICS AND ADMINISTRATION OF THE INSTRUMENTS . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Using Web-based instruments . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Providing background to students about the instruments . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Creating a safe environment for administration of the instruments . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 14 14 14 14
V. ANNOTATED BIBLIOGRAPHY OF RESEARCH ARTICLES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 14 VI. APPENDICES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 24 NCA Criteria for the Assessment of Oral Communication . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 24 About the author of the instruments and the editors of this program . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 28
I. BACKGROUND
This program contains two self-report instruments, both of which assess the motivational domain of communication, also referred to as the attitudinal or affective domain. In using either of these instruments, it is helpful to understand the important role of motivation in the overall process of communicating competently. with the dimension of oral communication being assessed. Knowledge and attitudes/motivation may be assessed through paper and pencil instruments, but speaking and listening skills must be assessed through actual performance in social settings. The two instruments in this program are paper and pencil assessment instruments, also referred to as self-report tools. The Personal Report of Communication Apprehension focuses on assessing the negative side of motivation and the Willingness to Communicate focuses on assessing the positive side of motivation.
Sources:
Beatty, M.J., McCroskey, J.C., & Heisel, A.D. (1998) Communication apprehension as temperamental expression: A communibiological paradigm. Communication Monographs, 65, 197-219.
Morreale, S., Spitzberg, B., & Barge, K. (2006). Human communication: Motivation, knowledge, and skills. Belmont, CA: Thomson Wadsworth.
low levels of willingness to communicate may need remedial training or courses. Conversely, students with low levels of apprehension and high levels of willingness to communicate might be placed in more advanced training situations or courses. (c) Either or both instruments may be used as a tool for instructing and advising students regarding the importance of the motivational domain of communication. The students would be adminis-
tered the instruments, followed by a discussion of their scores by comparison to the national norms for each instrument. (d) Either or both instruments could be used to generate assessment data for departmental or institutional accountability. The instruments could be administered to all students on a campus, for example, as part of a general education assessment program.
Group Discussion Score: _______ Interpersonal Score: _______ Meetings Score: _______ Public Speaking Score: _______ To obtain your total score for the PRCA, simply add your sub-scores together. _______ Scores can range from 24-120. Scores below 51 represent people who have very low CA. Scores between 51-80 represent people with average CA. Scores above 80 represent people who have high levels of trait CA. NORMS FOR THE PRCA-24: (based on over 40,000 college students; data from over 3,000 non-student adults in a national sample provided virtually identical norms, within 0.20 for all scores.) Mean Total Score: 65.6 Group: 15.4 Meeting: 16.4 Dyad (Interpersonal): 14.2 Public: 19.3 Standard Deviation 15.3 4.8 4.2 3.9 5.1 High > 80 > 20 > 20 > 18 > 24 Low < 51 < 11 < 13 < 11 < 14
Source:
McCroskey, J. C. (2005). An introduction to rhetorical communication (9th ed). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Friend: Add scores for items 6, 9, 14, 19; then divide by 4. To compute the total WTC score, add the sub scores for stranger, acquaintance, and friend. Then divide by 3. All scores, total and sub-scores, will fall in the range of 0 to 100 Norms for WTC Scores: Group discussion >89 High WTC, <57 Low WTC Meetings >80 High WTC, <39 Low WTC Interpersonal conversations >94 High WTC, <64 Low WTC Public Speaking >78 High WTC, <33 Low WTC Stranger >63 High WTC, <18 Low WTC Acquaintance >92 High WTC, <57 Low WTC Friend >99 High WTC, <71 Low WTC Total WTC >82 High Overall WTC, <52 Low Overall WTC
Sources:
McCroskey, J. C. (1992). Reliability and validity of the willingness to communicate scale. Communication Quarterly, 40, 1625. McCroskey, J. C., & Richmond, V. P. (1987). Willingness to communicate. In J. C. McCroskey & J. A. Daly (Eds.), Personality and interpersonal communication (pp. 119-131). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Research reported by Porter (1981) and Parks (1980) has raised signicant reservations concerning the content validity of the items on the early versions of the PRCA. The present study investigated the content validity of the most recent version of the instrument, PRCA-24. The results of this research indicate that the scores generated by the new instrument are relatively independent of the context-based content of the items employed and are capable of substantially predicting apprehension in a context not represented directly in the items on the new form. Loffredo, D. & Opt, S. (2000). Rethinking communication apprehension: A Myers-Briggs perspective. Journal of Psychology, 134(5), 556. This study is an examination of relationships between Myers-Briggs personality type preferences, based on Jungian theory, and communication apprehension. Results showed that participants who preferred introversion or sensing reported signicantly higher levels of communication apprehension in general and across the group, dyadic, meeting, and public contexts than did participants who preferred extraversion or intuition. In addition, participants who preferred feeling reported higher levels of communication anxiety in the public context than those who preferred thinking. Findings support the assumption that communication apprehension is biologically based; suggest that the Myers-Briggs type preference framework offers an alternative way of understanding communication apprehension; and, point out the need for new approaches to understanding the phenomenon of communication apprehension. Wright, K. (2000). Social support satisfaction online communication apprehension and perceived life stress within computer-mediated support groups. Communication Research Reports, 17(2), 139-147. This study used an on-line questionnaire to examine the relationship between social support satisfaction, online communication time, online communication apprehension, and perceived life stress among members of various online support groups (N = 140). The results indicated that online support satisfaction was predictive of online communication time while online communication apprehension was not related to online communication time. Online communication apprehension was found to be predictive of online support satisfaction. Online support satisfaction was found to be predictive of perceived life stress. The implications of ndings for
communication and social support research as well as the limitations of the study are discussed. Burk, J. (2001). Communication apprehension among masters of business administration students: Investigating a gap in communication education. Communication Education, 50(1), 51-58. Masters of Business Administration students at a large Midwestern university were administered the Personal Report of Communication Apprehension24. The students also responded to a questionnaire that generated data for six independent variables and were analyzed in relation to the PRCA-24 scores via a multiple regression analysis. The ndings indicate that communication apprehension (CA) exists among the MBA students with the average overall score slightly below national average. The students had low dyadic, but high meeting and public speaking apprehension. Undergraduate major and culture signicantly predicted the PRCA-24 scores. Students with math-related majors had signicantly lower CA than students with business-related or other undergraduate majors. The ndings suggest that MBA programs are not addressing CA in their curricula. Campbell, S. & Neer, M. (2001). The relationship of communication apprehension and interaction involvement to perceptions of computer-mediated communication. Communication Research Reports, 18(4), 391-398. This study investigated how the interpersonal traits of communication apprehension (CA) and interaction involvement (II) are related to ones attitudes toward and use of computer-mediated communication (CMC). Data were collected by administering a self-report survey to a sample of 133 participants, and results were analyzed with multiple regression. Findings revealed that CA and II failed to predict CMC attitudes. However, each trait predicted communication style during online interaction. Findings are interpreted within the context of current theories of CMC. Jones-Corley, J. & Messman, S. (2001). Effects of communication environment, immediacy, and communication
large-lecture/break-out sections) versus self-contained sections. The results indicated that students cognitive learning outcomes were slightly greater in the mixed-size sections versus self-contained sections. In addition, affective learning decreased for all students from the rst day of class, though it decreased slightly more for students in the large-lecture/break-out sections. When the teacher was perceived as highly immediate, however, there was no difference in affective learning due to format. Toale, M. (2001). Ethnocentrism and trait communication apprehension as predictors of interethnic communication apprehension and use of relational maintenance strategies in interethnic communication. Communication Quarterly, 49(1), 70-83. The rst of two studies investigated the differences between reported relational maintenance strategy usage by high and low interethnic communication apprehensives (IECAs). An instrument based on Canary and Staffords (1992) relational maintenance strategies taxonomy and Neuliep and McCroskeys (1997a) Personal Report of Interethnic Communication Apprehension (PRECA) was employed. The results indicated that individuals who were low IECAs reported utilizing signicantly more of the task, network, and positivity strategies. Differences in openness and assurance strategies followed the same pattern but were not signicant. The participants reported usage and IECA score were inversely related. The second study replicated the rst and explored two theoretical explanations for the results. This study revealed signicant differences on all of the dimensions and signicant negative correlations. This study also examined whether trait communication apprehension {disregarding ethnicity) and/or ethnocentrism the presumed foundational components of IECA) could account for the differences in reports of relational communication behavior. The results of the second study indicated that both trait CA and ethnocentrism contributed to the prediction of IECA and to overall reported strategy usage, and that ethnocentrism was the better predictor. Behnke, R & Sawyer, C. (2002). Reduction in public speaking state anxiety during performance as a function of sensitization processes. Communication Quarterly, 50(1), 112-121. Recently, scholars have suggested that biological factors, such as temperament, inuence human social behavior, particularly in the formation of traits, such as communication apprehension. Despite progress in this 16 Assessing Motivation to Communicate
area, the relationship between temperament and states, such as speech anxiety, remains unclear. Theories of temperament predict that the rate at which subjects habituate to stress varies inversely with the degree to which they are sensitized during initial confrontation with stress-producing stimuli. The inverse relationship between habituation and sensitization, in the context of public speaking state anxiety, is examined in this report. In two separate studies, using both physiological and psychological measures of state anxiety, the inverse relationship between sensitization and habituation was conrmed. Specically, sensitization accounted for 69.1% and 50.3% of the variance in physiological and psychological habituation, respectively. Bline, D., Lowe, D., Meixner, W., & Nouri, H. (2003). Measurement data on commonly used scales to measure oral communication and writing apprehensions. Journal of Business communication, 40(4), 266-288. Curriculum changes and training advances in business communication have provided students and practitioners with an opportunity to develop and improve communication skills. Despite such changes, research continues to demonstrate that communication apprehension can injuriously impede skills attainment. Yet, the measurement properties of instruments used to measure oral and writing apprehension have received limited attention. In particular, research has not fully explored the impact of question order on the measurement properties of these instruments. This article presents the results of an investigation about the effect of question order randomization on the psychometric properties of two frequently used oral and written apprehension instruments. Results showed that the measurement properties of these instruments were signicantly altered when the question order was randomized. Bodie, G. & Villaume, W. (2003). Aspects of receiving information: The relationship between listening preferences, communication apprehension, receiver apprehension, and communicator style. International Journal of Listening, 17, 47-57. This study investigated connections between listening preferences and patterns of communicator style and apprehension. An initial discriminant analysis was conducted to test whether six categories of listening styles are systematically discriminated by communicator style, communication apprehension, and receiver apprehension. There was one signicant discriminant function, whose interpretation was somewhat questionable. Sub-
sequently, a canonical correlation was conducted to test if four interval level listening preferences are systematically related to communicator style, communication apprehension, and receiver apprehension. The results were highly signicant and identied three patterns of association between the set of listening preferences and the set of communicator style and apprehension variables. One: High people-orientation in listening is systematically associated with lower receiver apprehension and dyadic communication apprehension and with a more relationally oriented communication style that attends to and afrms the other person. Two: The combination of high content- and action-orientations is associated with a precise and attentive style of arguing the issues that leaves a strong impression on other people. Three: The conguration of high time- and actionorientations along with a lack of content-orientation is associated with higher receiver apprehension but lower dyadic communication apprehension, and also with a dramatic, animated and forceful style that asserts ones goals/concerns and tends to dominate the other person. Cole, J. & McCroskey, J. (2003). The association of perceived communication apprehension, shyness, and verbal aggression with perceptions of source credibility and affect in organizational and interpersonal contexts. Communication Quarterly, 51(1), 101-110. This is a report of two studies that examined the association of receivers perceptions of sources levels of several communication traits (shyness, communication apprehension, verbal aggressiveness) with the receivers reported levels of affect for the source (general affect and/or liking) and the receivers perceptions of the sources credibility (competence, trustworthiness, goodwill). Study 1 examined employees perceptions of their supervisors communication trait behaviors in the organizational context, while Study 2 examined the students perceptions of their roommates communication trait behaviors in an interpersonal context. As predicted on the basis of previous theory and research, both perceived communication apprehension and perceived verbal aggressiveness of the source were found to be substantially negatively correlated with credibility and affect and/ or liking reported by the receiver. Contrary to our hypothesis, perceived behavioral shyness was not meaningfully associated with either credibility or affect. It is concluded that these results provide important information for distinguishing between the theoretical constructs of shyness and communication apprehension and theory in this area in general.
Rancer, A., & Yang, L. (2003). Ethnocentrism, intercultural communication apprehension, intercultural willingness-to-communicate, and intentions to participate in an intercultural dialogue program: Testing a proposed model. Communication Research Reports, 20(2), 189-190. The article focuses on a model on ethnocentrism and intercultural communication apprehension, intercultural willingness-to-communicate (IWTC) and intentions to participate in an intercultural dialogue program. Results of the study suggest that the proposed model renement. The data suggest a modied model renement which argues that ethnocentrism and intercultural communication apprehension are related. It also argues that ethnocentrism and intercultural communication apprehension are both related to IWTC. Ethnocentrism and IWTC are related to intentions to participate in cultural exchange program. Thus IWTC is inuenced by both ethnocentrism and intercultural communication apprehension. Both ethnocentrism and IWTC have a direct inuence on individuals intentions to participate in intercultural dialogue program. The implication of this nding is that if one wants to get an estimate of an individuals intentions to participate in such program or other intercultural encounter, it would be prudent to measure ethnocentrism and IWTC. Bartoo, H. & Sias, P. (2004). When enough is too much: Communication apprehension and employee information experiences. Communication Quarterly, 52(1), 15-26. This study examines the relationships between supervisor communication apprehension, employee communication apprehension, and employees reports of information received from their supervisor. Results indicated that supervisor communication apprehension was negatively related to employees reports of information received. Employee communication apprehension and employees reports of information received from supervisors were not signicantly related. Supervisor communication apprehension was negatively related to the information load reported by employees. Finally, a positive relationship was identied between employee communication apprehension and the information load reported by employees. Hsu, C. (2004). Sources of differences in communication apprehension between Chinese in Taiwan and Americans. Communication Quarterly, 52(4), 370389. Assessing Motivation to Communicate 17
This study investigated whether cross-cultural differences in communication apprehension (CA) can be explained with regard to cultural orientations, personality traits and component theory. To this end, a total of 618 undergraduates, studying in Taiwan (n = 298) and the United States (n = 320), participated in this study. Participants lled out the Personal Report of Communication Apprehension along with Self-Construal Scale, the Revised NEO Personality Inventory, Fear of Negative Evaluation, and Self-Perceived Communication Competence scale. The results indicated that Chinese in Taiwan scored signicantly higher in communication apprehension than Americans. The inuence of culture on CA was mediated by independence self-construal, neuroticism, extroversion, fear of negative evaluation, and communication competence. The strongest mediating effect was found for self-perception of communication competence. The implications of these ndings were further discussed. Hye, Y. J. & McCroskey, J. (2004). Communication apprehension in a rst language and self-perceived competence as predictors of communication apprehension in a second language: A study of speakers of English as a second language. Communication Quarterly, 52(2), 170-181. This study addresses the communication apprehension of the non-native English speaker in the U.S. Previous studies which have examined the implications of communication apprehension (CA) for bilingual, nonnative communicators have generated results which indicate that trait-like CA is consistent across rst and second language-speaking situations. However, none of these studies have probed the cause of the cross-linguistic consistency of CA. This research is designed to provide a scientic explanation for the etiology of CA by applying the communibiological paradigm to CA theory and research. By selecting the situational constraints of international students, this study tests a theory based on proposition 4 of the communibiological paradigm (Beatty & McCroskey w/Valencia, 2001, p. 128): Environment or situation has only a negligible effect on interpersonal behavior. The results of this study replicate the strong relationship previously observed between CA in a rst language and CA in a second language. It also found that the genetic markers employed (Eysencks Big 3 temperament variables) predicted rst and second language CA approximately equally. The results indicate that, although both rst and second languages are learned, the CA associated with them most likely is not. 18 Assessing Motivation to Communicate
McCroskey, J., Richmond, V., Johnson, A., & Smith, H. (2004). Organizational orientations theory and measurement: Development of measures and preliminary investigations. Communication Quarterly, 52(1), 1-14. Four studies are reported which focus on organizational orientations theory and relevant measuring instruments. An initial study designed to develop measures of the three components believed to constitute organizational orientation (upward mobile, indifferent, and ambivalent) is reported. Since it was believed that valid measures of organizational orientations should be associated with the way workers communicate, a second study designed to determine the association of organizational orientations with communication apprehension, immediacy, assertiveness, responsiveness, and job satisfaction was conducted as a preliminary validity test. Results of the rst two studies pointed to both the reliability and the validity of the new measures. A third study was conducted which included new items designed to increase the reliability of the scales. The results generated revised measures with higher reliability. The fourth study was designed to expand the validation of the instruments by testing their associations with temperament, job satisfaction, and subordinates perceptions of the credibility of their supervisors. Results suggest that the organizational orientations are associated with the BIG THREE temperament variables (extraversion, neuroticism, psychoticism) and are predictive of both job satisfaction and perceptions of supervisor credibility. Suggestions for future research and the limitations of the research program at this point are discussed. Lippert, L., Titsworth, B., & Hunt, S. (2005). The ecology of academic risk: Relationships between communication apprehension, verbal aggression, supportive communication, and students academic risk status. Communication Studies, 56 (1), 1-21. Contemporary research exploring at-risk student populations has generally used the epidemiological model, the constructivist model, or the ecological model to explain processes involved in academic risk. This study applies communication constructs to the ecological model of academic risk, which proposes that academic risk is a function of individual, social, and cultural communication phenomena. A survey of 232 students found that (a) at-risk students communicated more with friends about school than did regular-admission students, (b) levels of communication apprehen-
sion differed depending on at-risk status and sex, and (c) levels of verbal aggression differed depending on at-risk status. These results are discussed in terms of theoretical and applied implications. Scott, C. & Timmerman, E. (2005). Relating computer, communication, and computer-mediated communication apprehension to new communication technology used in the workplace. Communication Research, 32(6), 683-725. This study explores three issues regarding the use of multiple workplace communication technologies: the relationships between distinct forms of apprehension (computer, communication, and writing) and use, the relative contribution of computer-mediated communication (CMC) apprehension for predicting use, and changes in these relationships over time. A trend study, which consisted of the collection of data from two samples (N = 205) separated by a 5-year interval, suggests full or partial support for the hypotheses involving computer and communication apprehension. Although apprehension levels remain stable, usage frequency changed for several of the technologies examinedresulting in stronger relationships between apprehensions and those technologies for which use has changed the most in the past 5 years. Most notably, a new measure of CMC apprehension generally predicts communication technology useespecially text-based and conferencing toolsmore strongly than do more traditional apprehension types. Zhang, Q. (2005). Teacher immediacy and classroom communication apprehension: A cross-cultural investigation. Communication Research, 34(1/2), 50-64. The present study investigates classroom communication apprehension in relation to perceived teacher verbal and nonverbal immediacy in Chinese and U.S. college classrooms. The objectives of this study are three-fold: to compare classroom communication apprehension and perceived teacher verbal and nonverbal immediacy, and to examine the impact of teacher immediacy on classroom communication apprehension in Chinese and U.S. college classrooms. This study reports three major ndings, including (a) Chinese students have a signicantly higher classroom communication apprehension than their U.S. counterparts, (b) Chinese student perceptions of teacher verbal immediacy are signicantly higher than U.S. student perceptions, but their perceptions of teacher nonverbal immediacy are not signicantly different, and (c) classroom communi-
cation apprehension is correlated negatively with US student perceptions of teacher nonverbal immediacy, but not with verbal immediacy; classroom communication apprehension is not correlated negatively with Chinese student perceptions of teacher verbal and nonverbal immediacy. Zhang, Q. & Zhang J. (2005). Teacher clarity: Effects on classroom communication apprehension, student motivation, and learning Chinese college classrooms. Communication Research, 34(3/4), 255-266. Teacher clarity is central to overall teaching effectiveness and student learning. The purpose of this study is to extend the line of research on teacher clarity from U.S. classrooms to Chinese classrooms. Specically, it investigates the effects of teacher clarity on classroom communication apprehension, student motivation, and affective and cognitive learning in Chinese college classrooms. Pearson correlation suggests that teacher clarity is associated negatively with classroom communication apprehension, but positively with student motivation to learn and affective and cognitive learning in Chinese classrooms.
The nature and assumptions underlying the Willingness to Communicate (WTC) scale are outlined and discussed. Data are discussed which relate to the reliability and validity of the instrument. It is concluded that the scale is of sufcient quality to be recommended for research and screening purposes. Kearne, P. & Waldeck, J., (2001). Teacher e-mail messages strategies and students willingness to communicate online. Journal of Applied Communication Research, 29(1), 54. In light of the prevalent use of e-mail between teachers and students, researchers have highlighted the need to assess its uses and effects. Relying on the research and thinking of teacher immediacy and extra-class communication, we developed a scale that measures those e-mail message strategies that inuence students willingness to communicate online with their teachers. Next, we isolated those reasons that students use e-mail to interact with their teachers: (1) to clarify course material and procedures, (2) as a means of efcient communication, and (3) for personal/social reasons. Finally, we examined student characteristics such as gender and ethnicity, and history of e-mail use that may (or may not) inuence their attitudes toward e-mail exchanges with teachers. Olaniran, B.& Roach, D. (2001). Intercultural willingness to communicate and communication anxiety in international teaching assistants. Communication Research Reports, 18(1), 26-35. This study addressed patterns and correlates of communication apprehension, intercultural communication apprehension, and intercultural willingness to communicate in international teaching assistants. Results indicated an inverse relationship between communication apprehension of international TAs and their satisfaction with students, relationship with students, and perceptions of student ratings of instruction. Similar results were found for ITA state anxiety. Inverse relationships were also found between ITA intercultural communication apprehension and relationship with students and perceptions of student ratings of instruction. International teaching assistant CA and ICA were positively related with ITA state anxiety. Miller, J., & Morgan, S. (2002). Beyond the organ donor card: The effect of knowledge, attitudes, and values on willingness to communicate about organ donation to family members. Health Communication, 14(1), 121-134. 20 Assessing Motivation to Communicate
Although numerous studies have examined many of the predictors of signing an organ donor card, including knowledge, attitudes, values, and demographic variables, very few have examined the factors associated with individuals willingness to communicate about organ donation with family members. Because organ donation does not take place without the permission of a persons next-of-kin, government agencies and organ procurement organizations have targeted communication with family members as a primary objective of organ donation campaigns. This study reports the results of a survey of a stratied random sample of adults at 2 local sites of a national employer. Results indicate that knowledge, attitude, and altruism are signicantly related to 2 measures of willingness to communicate: past behavior (whether respondents had already discussed organ donation with family members) and a scale measuring willingness to communicate about organ donation in the future. Because the quality of discussions between the potential donor and his or her family will depend on how well the donor is able to address vital issues regarding donation, it is concluded that campaigns seeking to promote communication between family members about organ donation must simultaneously seek to increase knowledge, debunk myths, and bolster positive attitudes about donation. Yahima, T. (2002). Willingness to communicate in a second language: The Japanese EFL context. Modern Language Journal, 86(1), 54. Willingness to communicate (WTC) is emerging as a concept to account for individuals rst language (L1) and second language (L2) communication. This study examined relations among L2 learning and L2 communication variables in Japanese English as a foreign language context using the WTC model and the socioeducational model as a framework. A L2 communication model was constructed and tested using AMOS version 4.0, with a sample of 297 Japanese university students. In the model, a latent variable, international posture, was hypothesized to capture the general attitude toward the international community and foreign language learning in Japan. From structural equation modeling, it appeared that international posture inuences motivation, which, in turn, inuences prociency in English. Motivation affected self-condence in L2 communication which led to willingness to communicate in a L2. In addition to this indirect path, a direct path from international posture to WTC in a L2 was signicant. The models tness to data was good, which indicates the
potential for using the WTC and other constructs to account for L2 communication. Baker, S., Clement, R., Donavan, L., & MacIntyre, P. (2003). Sex and age effects on willingness to communicate, anxiety perceived competence, and L2 motivation among junior high school French immersion students. Language Learning, 53, 137-166. The students who participate in immersion education are an impressive group. In the present study we looked at students in a junior high school in Nova Scotia. In the local area, English is far and away the dominant language, though there are French-speaking communities within a two-hour drive and Canada is an ofcially bilingual country. Therefore, the students are not in a foreign language-learning environment, but in all probability, they are not likely to encounter spoken French in their daily lives. The students have all the challenges of adolescence to contend with: moving from an elementary to a junior high school in grade 7, the wonders of puberty, growing academic expectations from teachers, demands from school administration to speak only French while at school, and the burgeoning social life of a newly minted teenager. On top of all this, participants in this research are required to give up their well-developed native language, English, and undertake to be educated in a second language, French. Impressive. The present study reports a cross-sectional investigation of second language communication among students in a junior high French late immersion program. The effects of language, sex, and grade on willingness to communicate (WTC), anxiety, perceived communication competence, and frequency of communication in French and on attitude/motivation variables are examined globally and at each grade level. Most of these variables have been widely studied among adult learners, most often at the university level. The present study attempted to look at a much younger group to examine the patterns earlier in the language learning process. We found that students second language WTC, perceived competence, and frequency of communication in French increased from grades 7 to 8 and that these increases were maintained between grades 8 and 9, despite a drop in motivation between grades 7 and 8 and a steady level of anxiety across the three grades. Gender differences in language anxiety in grade 9. However, the differences between WTC across the rst and second language narrowed as students progressed through the program. Martin, M., Mottet T., & Myers, S. (2003). Relationships among perceived instructor verbal approach
and avoidance relational strategies and students motives for communicating with their instructors. Communication Education, 53(1), 116-122. In light of a new student engagement benchmark for teaching and institutional effectiveness in higher education, this study focused on the relationships between perceived instructor use of verbal approach and avoidance relational strategies and students motives for communicating with their instructors. The data suggest that perceived instructor use of verbal approach relational strategies positively inuences students motivation to engage with their instructors for relational, participatory, excuse-making, and sycophantic reasons. Perceived instructor use of verbal avoidance relational strategies, however, was uncorrelated with students motives to communicate. The results also failed to conrm previous ndings that the functional motive to communicate is more related to task purposes than to relational purposes. Findings of this study do imply that student engagement can be enhanced by instructors more emphatically expressing messages of inclusion, appreciation, willingness to communicate, and the like. Rancer, A., Sunhee L., & Yang, L. (2003). Ethnocentrism and intercultural willingness to communicate: A cross-cultural comparison between Korean and American college students. Journal of Intercultural Communication Research, 32(2), 117-129. This study is a cross-cultural comparison of ethnocentrism and intercultural willingness to communicate between Korean and American college students. Two hundred and eighty-two Korean students and 319 American students completed a measure of ethnocentrism and intercultural willingness to communicate. The results revealed that Korean students had signicantly lower scores on both ethnocentrism and intercultural willingness to communicate than American students. In addition, male students reported being more ethnocentric than female students across both cultures, and male students reported being less willing to communicate interculturally than female students in American culture. Implications of these nding are discussed. Kazuaki Shimuzu, L., Tomoko Yashima, S., & ZenukNishide, L. (2004). The inuence of attitudes and affect on willingness to communicate and second language communication. Language Learning, 54(1), 119-152. This article investigates results and antecedents of willingness to communicate (WTC) in a second lanAssessing Motivation to Communicate 21
guage (L2) through 2 separate investigations conducted with Japanese adolescent learners of English. In the rst investigation, involving 160 students, a model was created based on the hypothesis that WTC results in more frequent communication in the L2 and that the attitudinal construct international posture leads to WTC and communication behavior. This model was tested with structural equation modeling and was found to t the data well. The second investigation with 60 students who participated in a study-abroad program in the United States conrmed the results of the rst. Finally, frequency of communication was shown to correlate with satisfaction in interpersonal relationships during the sojourn. Kopfman, J., Lindsey, L., Smith, S., & Yoo, J. (2004). Encouraging family discussion on the decision to donate organs: The role of the willingness to communicate scale. Health Communication, 16(3), 333-346. Family discussion of organ donation has been found to double rates of family consent regarding organ donation. Therefore, family discussion is an important communication process to study in the effort to get more people to become organ donors. This investigation concerns the willingness to communicate about organ donation and its relationship to other variables and processes related to family discussion of organ donation. Previous research on willingness to communicate examined the antecedent variables of knowledge, attitude toward organ donation, and altruism. This research found that being willing to communicate about organ donation with ones family is related to prior thought and intent to sign an organ donor card, to perceiving organ donation messages as credible, and to feeling relatively low anxiety after reading organ donation messages. One week after being presented with the messages, willingness to communicate was found to be positively associated with worrying about the lack of donors, engaging in family discussion about organ donation, and having an organ donor card witnessed. It was negatively related to feeling personally uneasy about organ donation during the past week. Miczo, N. (2004). Humor ability, unwillingness to communicate, loneliness, and perceived stress: Testing a security theory. Communication Studies, 55(2), 209226. The purpose of this study was to develop and test a theory of the laughter-humor link in interpersonal communication. The basic premise of the theory is that a 22 Assessing Motivation to Communicate
sense of security underlies the ability to encode humor in everyday conversation. It was hypothesized that communication-related security (i.e., willingness to communicate) predicts humor ability, which in turn negatively predicts loneliness and perceived stress. Undergraduates completed a survey including the following scales: Unwillingness-to-Communicate, Humor Orientation, Coping Humor, revised UCLA Loneliness, and Perceived Stress. Regression analyses conrm that willingness to communicate predicted humor orientation, while humor orientation mediated the relationship between willingness to communicate and coping humor. Humor orientation negatively predicted loneliness and perceived stress, although in both cases, willingness to communicate mediated the relationships. The discussion highlights methodological limitations (e.g., use of self-report) and reiterates the need for interaction- and context-based studies of the laughter-humor relationship. Rancer, A., Trimbitas, O., & Yang, L.(2005). Ethnocentrism and intercultural-willingness-to-communicate: A cross-cultural comparison between Romanian and US American collage students. Journal of Intercultural Communication Research, 34(1/2), 138-151. Differences on communication traits and predispositions can inuence an individuals behavior especially when engaged in intercultural interaction. This study is a cross cultural comparison of ethnocentrism and intercultural-willingness-to-communicate between Romanian and American college students. One hundred and ten Romanian students and 151 American students completed a measure of ethnocentrism and intercultural-willingness-to-communicate. The results revealed that Romanian students had signicantly higher scores on ethnocentrism and had signicantly lower scores on intercultural-willingness-to-communicate than US American students. Implications of these ndings are discussed Discussions of the differences on these two communication predispositions provide information for individuals of the respective cultures to develop more effective strategies to communicate with each other.
ingness to communicate: A comparison with rst and second languages in Micronesia. Communication Research Reports, 20(3), 230-239. Research involving communication apprehension (CA) and related constructs such as self-perceived communication competence (SPCC) and willingness to communicate (WTC) has been conducted in a wide variety of cultures. In general, relationships among these variables have been found to be quite similar across cultures, even when substantial mean differences have been observed. An exception to this pattern was an extremely high (r =.80) correlation between SPCC and WTC observed in a study conducted in Micronesia (Burroughs & Marie, 1990). Other relationships observed in the study were generally consistent with those found in other cultures. Since this study involved individuals in a context where they were forced to communicate in a second language much of the time, and the data were collected in that second language, it was suspected this anomaly was what produced the aberrant nding. The present study obtained data from the same population but referenced the participants rst languages and was administered in their rst language. Results indicated a relationship between SPCC and WTC consistent with that found in other cultures. It was also observed that, while there was substantially lower perceived communication competence for the second language than for the rst language, there was no such differential for communication apprehension. Rancer, A., & Yang, L. (2003). Ethnocentrism, intercultural communication apprehension, intercultural willingness-to-communicate, and intentions to participate in an intercultural dialogue program: Testing a proposed model. Communication Research Reports, 20(2), 189-190. The article focuses on a model on ethnocentrism and intercultural communication apprehension, intercultural willingness-to-communicate (IWTC) and intentions to participate in an intercultural dialogue program.
Results of the study suggest that the proposed model renement. The data suggest a modied model renement that argues that ethnocentrism and intercultural communication apprehension are related. It also argues that ethnocentrism and intercultural communication apprehension are both related to IWTC. Ethnocentrism and IWTC are related to intentions to participate in cultural exchange program. Thus IWTC is inuenced by both ethnocentrism and intercultural communication apprehension. Both ethnocentrism and IWTC have a direct inuence on individuals intentions to participate in intercultural dialogue program. The implication of this nding is that if one wants to get an estimate of an individuals intentions to participate in such program or other intercultural encounter, it would be prudent to measure ethnocentrism and IWTC. Donavan, L. & MacIntyre, P. (2004). Age and sex differences in willingness to communicate, communication apprehension, and self-perceived competence. Communication Research Reports. 21(4), 420-427. Age and sex differences in willingness to communicate (WTC), communication apprehension, and selfperceived communication competence were examined using three age cohorts of participants drawn from junior high, high school, and university student populations. Results indicate that junior high females are higher in WTC than their male counterparts and females at the university level are higher in communication apprehension and lower in self-perceived competence than are male university students. Communication apprehension and self-perceived competence show a consistent negative relationship that does not vary with age or sex in the present sample. The degree to which communication apprehension and self-perceived competence predict WTC varies with age and sex. In all three age cohorts, communication apprehension is a signicant predictor of WTC among women. Among men, self-perceived competence emerges as a signicant predictor of WTC in all three age groups.
THE NATIONAL COMMUNICATION ASSOCIATIONS CRITERIA FOR THE ASSESSMENT OF ORAL COMMUNICATION
A National Context
Assessment has received increasing attention throughout the 1970s and into the 1990s. Initially appearing in the standards developed by state depart-mints of education, by 1980 over half of the states had adopted statewide student-testing programs. In Educational Standards in the 50 States: 1990, the Educational Testing Service reported that by 1985, over 40 states had adopted such programs, and between 1985 and 1990, an additional ve states initiated statewide student- testing programs, bringing the number of such program to 47. During the 1970s and 1980s, the number of different subjects and skills tested has also consistently increased, with additional attention devoted to how assessments are executed. Moreover, during this period, organizations, such as the National Assessment of Educational Progress, intensied and expanded the scope of their assessment procedures as well as extensively publicized the results of their ndings nationally and annually. By the end of 1989, the public recognized the signicance of national educational assessments. In the Phi Delta Kappan-Gallup poll reported in the September 1989 issue of Phi Delta Kappan, 77 percent of the respondents favored requiring the public schools in this community to use standardized national testing programs to measure academic achievement of students, and 70 percent favored requiring the public schools in this community to conform to national achievement standards and goals. Likewise, towards the end of the 1980s, colleges and universities began to realize that formal assessment issues were to affect them. For example, in its 1989-1990 Criteria for Accreditation, the Southern Association of Colleges and Schools--which provides institutional certication for over 800 colleges and universities in the South-held that complete requirements for an associate or baccalaureate degree must include competence in reading, writing, oral communications and fundamental mathematical skills. They also held that the general education core of colleges and universities must provide components designed to ensure competence in reading, writing, oral communication and fundamental mathematical skills. In 1990, a series of reports appeared which suggested that systematic and comprehensive assessment should 24 Assessing Motivation to Communicate become a national educational objective. In February 1990, for example, the National Governors Association, in the context of President George H.W. Bushs set of six educational goals, argued that, National education goals will be meaningless unless progress toward meeting them is measured accurately and adequately, and reported to the American people. The nations governors argued that doing a good job of assessment requires that what students need to know must be dened, it must be determined whether they know it, and measurements must be accurate, comparable, appropriate, and constructive. In July 1990, President Bush reinforced this line of reasoning in The National Education Goals: A Report to the Nations Governors. And, in September 1990, the National Governors Association extended and elaborated its commitment to assessment in Educating America: State Strategies for Achieving the National Education Goals: Report of the Task Force on Education. Additionally, in 1990, in their report From Gatekeeper to Gateway: Transforming Testing in America, the National Commission on Testing and Public Policy recommended eight standards for assessment, arguing for more humane and multicultural assessment systems. Among other considerations, they particularly maintained that testing policies and practices must be reoriented to promote the development of all human talent, that test scores should be used only when they differentiate on the basis of characteristics relevant to the opportunities being allocated, and that the more test scores disproportionately deny opportunities to minorities, the greater the need to show that the tests measure characteristics relevant to the opportunities being allocated.
skills. CAT has been one of the most active, consistent, and productive of NCAs various committees and task forces. Under the guidance of CAT, NCA has published several volumes exploring formal methods for assessing oral communication. These publications began to appear in the 1970s and have continued into the 1990s. In 1978, for example, the National Communication Association published Assessing Functional Communication, which was followed in 1984 by two other major publications, Large Scale Assessment of Oral Communication Skills: Kindergarten through Grade 12 and Oral Communication Assessment Procedures and Instrument Development in Higher Education. In 1979, in Standards for Effective Oral Communication Programs, NCA adopted its rst set of standards for assessment and evaluation. The rst standards called for school-wide assessment of speaking and listening needs of students, qualied personnel to utilize appropriate evaluation tools, a variety of data and instruments which encourage students desire to communicate. In 1986, in Criteria for Evaluating Instruments and Procedures for Assessing Speaking and Listening, NCA adopted an additional 15 content and technical considerations dealing primarily with the substance of speaking and listening instruments and matters such as reliability, validity and information on administration. These criteria included the importance of focusing on demonstrated speaking skills rather than reading and writing ability, adopting assessment instruments and procedures which are free of sexual, cultural, racial, and ethnic content and/or stereotyping, employing familiar situations which are important for various communication settings in test questions, using instruments which permit a range of acceptable responses and generate reliable outcomes, employing assessments which are consistent with other results and have content validity, and employing standardized procedures which approximate the recognized stress level of oral communication which are also practical in terms of cost and time and suitable for the developmental level of the individual being tested. In 1987, at the NCA Wingspread Conference, conference participants recommended that the chosen instrument conform to NCA guidelines for assessment instrument, and they specically suggested that strategies for assessing speaking skills should be directly linked to the content of oral communication performances and student speaking competencies. Prescribed
communication practices were to determine the choice of assessment strategies, with the following content standards guiding formal evaluations: determine the purpose of oral discourse; choose a topic and restrict it according to the purpose and the audience; fulll the purpose by formulating a thesis statement, providing adequate support material, selecting a suitable organization, demonstrating careful choice of words, providing effective transitions, demonstrating suitable inter-personal skills; employing vocal-variety in rate, pitch, and intensity; articulate clearly; employ the level of American English appropriate to the designated audience; and demonstrate nonverbal behavior that supports the verbal message. Additionally, the Wingspread Conference participants considered strategies for assessing listening and for training assessors [see: Communication Is Life: Essential College Sophomore Speaking and Listening Competencies (Washington, D.C.: National Communication Association, 1990, pp. 51-74). In 1988, the NCA Flagstaff Conference generated a series of resolutions calling for a national conference and task force on assessment because previous experience in developing standardized assessment has met with problems of validity, reliability, feasibility, ethics, and cultural bias [in The Future of Speech Communication Education: Proceedings of the 1988 National Communication Association Flagstaff Conference, ed. by Pamela J. Cooper and Kathleen M. Galvin (Annandale, VA: National Communication Association, 1989, p. 80)]. In July 1990, a National Conference on Assessment was sponsored by NCA, the NCA Committee on Assessment and Testing or CAT, and the NCA Educational Policies Board (EPB). The conference generated several resolutions regarding assessment.* Some of these resolutions reafrm existing NCA oral communication assessment policies. Others provide criteria for resolving new issues in assessment. Still others seek to integrate and establish a more coherent relationship among
*The criteria contained in this document were originally adopted as resolutions at the NCA Conference on Assessment in Denver, Colorado, in July 1990. Several of the criteria were authored by the Committee on Assessment and Testing Subcommittee on Criteria for Content, Procedures, and Guidelines for Oral Communication Competencies composed of James W. Crocker-Lakness (Subcommittee Chair), Sandra Manheimer, and Tom E. Scott. The introduction sections, entitled A National Context and NCAs Assessment Activities, were authored by James W. Chesebro, NCA Director of Education Services.
the criteria governing oral communication assessment. The recommended assessment criteria are detailed on the next page.
General Criteria
1. Assessment of oral communication should view competence in oral communication as a gestalt of several interacting dimensions. At a minimum, all assessments of oral communication should include an assessment of knowledge (understanding communication process, comprehension of the elements, rules, and dynamics of a communication event, awareness of what is appropriate in a communication situation), an assessment of skills (the possession of a repertoire of skills and the actual performance of skills), and an evaluation of the individuals attitude toward communication (e.g., value placed on oral communication, apprehension, reticence, willingness to communicate, readiness to communicate). 2. Because oral communication is an interactive and social process, assessment should consider the judgment of a trained assessor as well as the impressions of others involved in the communication act (audience, interviewer, other group members, conversant), and may include the self report of the individual being assessed. 3. Assessment of oral communication should clearly distinguish speaking and listening from reading and writing. While some parts of the assessment process may include reading and writing, a major portion of the assessment of oral communication should require speaking and listening. Directions from the assessor and responses by the individual being assessed should be in the oral/aural mode. 4. Assessment of oral communication should be sensitive to the effects of relevant physical and psychological disabilities on the assessment of competence. (e.g., with appropriate aids in signal reception, a hearing impaired person can be a competent empathic listener.) 5. Assessment of oral communication should be based in art on atomistic/analytic data collected and on a holistic impression.
and non-verbal aspects of communication and should consider competence in more than one communication setting. As a minimum assessment should occur in the one-to-many setting (e.g. public s peaking, practical small group discussion) and in the one-to-one setting (e.g., interviews, interpersonal relations). 7. Assessment of speech majors and other oral communication specialists could include in addition assessment in specialized elds appropriate to the course of study followed or the specialty of the person being assessed.
NATIONAL COMMUNICATION ASSOCIATION 1765 N Street, Washington, D.C., 20036 202-464-4622 www.natcom.org Assessing Motivation to Communicate 27
One of his daughters, Lynda L. McCroskey, who completed her doctorate at the University of Oklahoma, currently is a tenured associate professor teaching communication at California State University, Long Beach.