A Picture Book of Invisible Worlds
A Picture Book of Invisible Worlds
the banquet
stephen loo
undine sellbach
A PICTURE BOOK OF
INVISIBLE WORLDS
semblances of insects and
humans in jakob von
uexklls laboratory
together. In particular, one might begin to
wonder about those members of the animal
kingdom not included in the picture. There
are no insects, for example, or other invertebrates recorded at the banquet. Yet once one
reects on this absence it begins to feel as
though these small creatures might already be
there under the table, in the food perhaps
and certainly in the guts of the guests. It is
also imaginable that in anticipation of salvation,
the stomachs of the righteous ll with butteries, their thoughts buzz and their skin crawls.
This suggests that new sets of humananimal
relations are not merely cognitive ones they
are felt and imagined by different parts of the
body.1
45
invisible worlds
Agamben does not ask whether there might
also be insects attending the banquet. Nevertheless, the main scenes of eating he goes on to
describe in The Open all involve insects and
other invertebrates in a laboratory setting.2
This shift is striking. If, as Agamben imagines,
the table is the place where the relations
between humanity and animality could one
day be recongured, then the laboratory seems
to be the place where the distinction between
human subject and animal behaviour is most
powerfully drawn. In one laboratory experiment
in The Open, a bee is placed in front of a cup of
honey. As the bee begins to drink, its abdomen
is cut away, but yet it is observed to keep
sucking as the honey ows out of its open
stomach (52). As Agamben points out, this
experiment is used by Heidegger to distinguish
the instinctive captivation he believes is typical
of animal behaviour, from the openness particular to human existence.
Yet by placing these two scenes of eating side
by side, Agamben may be doing more than
drawing a contrast between the division of
human from animal as enacted in the laboratory,
and the new conjunctions he imagines at the
messianic table. The experiments that
Agamben describes in The Open belong to the
unconventional biologist Jakob von Uexkull,
who broke with the scientic paradigm of the
time by refusing to study animals as a series of
isolated behavioural traits. Drawing on careful
observations of animals interacting with their
environments, Uexkull set out to intuit the
lived worlds of the small creatures he worked
with. For Uexku ll, genuine biological investigation entails a certain willingness, on the part
of the scientist, to evoke in the minds eye
what is forever inaccessible to our physical
senses the radically different spatial, perceptual, temporal and affective worlds of other
animals. Uexku ll, as we will argue, can be understood as a pioneer of ecological thinking,
because the observational and imaginative techniques he develops in the laboratory for attending to insects and other invertebrates have the
potential to recongure traditional hierarchical
divisions towards new conjunctions between
animals, humans and environments.
46
47
uexklls laboratory
In this context, Uexkulls unconventional laboratory is of particular interest. He does not
extend subjective concepts and feelings from a
human-centred world to these small creatures,
invisible worlds
but nor does he interpret them in terms of xed
drives and external traits. Instead, he posits that
every small animal has an Umwelt a unique
foreign subjectivity of its own. By provoking
his readers to consider the lived worlds of
simple instinctual organisms the grasshopper,
the tick, the hermit crab, the jellysh, the
housey and the snail Uexkull confounds the
divide between subjective relational life and
blind organic existence that is drawn by the
anthropological machine. As Sagan writes in
his Introduction to A Foray into the Worlds
of Animals and Humans,
the phenomenon might be described as the
return of the scientically repressed: what is
excluded for the sake of experimental simplicity eventually shows itself to be relevant
after all [] With Uexkull the inner real
comes back in the realization that not only do
we sense and feel, but so do other sentient
organisms; and that our interactions and signalling perceptions have consequences beyond the
deterministic oversimplications of a modern
science that has bracketed off all causes that
are not immediate and mechanical. (8)
48
At once dedicated naturalist and biologistsharman, Uexkull identies the task of the
ecologist to reach the limits of animal worlds.
But what techniques does he use to attend to
these limits? In the preface to A Stroll
through the Worlds of Animals and Men,
Uexkull describes his method: This little monograph does not claim to point the way to a new
science. Perhaps it should be called a stroll into
unfamiliar worlds; worlds strange to us but
known to other creatures, manifold and varied
as the animals themselves (5). His investigations
of the limits of the dwelling-worlds of other
animals will take the form of a collection of imaginative forays, a reading that is afrmed by the
original subtitle of his book: A Picture Book of
Invisible Worlds.
49
invisible worlds
50
51
invisible worlds
technics in the laboratory told
through a picture book
The Grasshopper Cabaret is a story we made
up as part of a series of childrens writing experiments based on Uexkulls laboratory.12 The
story is a response to a picture of an experiment
that Uexkull conducted on grasshoppers (illustrated by G. Kriszat) in order to demonstrate
how the actions of these small animals are not
goal oriented but follow the plan of nature
inherent in their Umwelt (Foray 88).
Read as a conventional experiment one might
say that here Uexku ll is demonstrating, in a pictorial manner, the closed loop of relations
between animal and environment. In order to
investigate the limits of the grasshoppers
world, he uses the tools of his laboratory to
break their functional cycles. By retelling this
as a story in a Picture Book, we notice the presence of tools and technologies in the scene,
which were there all along as techniques of
seeing, but which need to remain in the background in order for the effect of the closed
loop to be demonstrated.
In this context, Bernard Stiegler has provided us with a way to look upon the actual
strategies and technologies of the laboratory
that is not merely reductive. To Stiegler it is difcult, if not impossible, to fathom the evolution
of what is the human from the evolution of
technics, which he denes as the exterior
organized realm of inorganic matter (Technics
and Time, 1 17). In Stieglerian terms, both
the anthropological machine as a conceptual
mechanism and the scientic laboratory as an
actual one, upon which human self-denition
rely, would be two technics amongst many in
a larger technical consciousness at the origin
of the human being.13
Technics allows us to see how external
realms of scientic experimentation tools,
diagrams, equipment, data, languages, codes,
epistemologies are folded into the internal
denition of the human. Stiegler posits that
these externalities or articial technical apparatus make possible, or in fact are, the retention
of human experience and memory. It follows,
for Stiegler, that variations in the evolution of
52
53
invisible worlds
the stories that biology is capable of telling us
can turn out to be even stranger than fairytales.
By entering the world of the scientic experiment through the Picture Book frame we are
invited to imagine the unknowable worlds of
the grasshoppers through over-determined
anthropomorphic frameworks that collapse
logical sense to perform an alternative logic of
sensation and affect. This technique decentres the human being in the laboratory,
allowing other kinds of relations to become
visible. Here, we no longer see a complete
picture of the animal Umwelt; instead, the
Picture Book frame assembles, in partially realized ways, different grasshopper life-worlds,
each effected in its own way by the presence
of human tools and out of phase with one
another. These imaginings take the form of
expressions, which are speculative and incomplete, because one folds into the next.
Performed through the Picture Book, we can
see that the limits of the Umwelt are not xed
but drawn and redrawn in these partial expressive ways, through the uncanny picturing of
different congurations of insect, technology,
human relations. Here, the tools of the laboratory are also volatized, opening up the speculation that small laboratory animals may also in
some way be de-centred by their comportment
to human technologies.
54
55
invisible worlds
A Conversation Arising from the Story of
The Spider & the Fly
The nal picture still needs to be made. What to draw?
Uexku ll says that spiders are good at making pictures of ies. The
threads of a spider web are perfectly measured to t the ying y.
But the spider in the story is young and has never met a y before.
So how does the picture of the y appear?
The spider is so affected by the missing y, that its web becomes
y-like. Imagine! The young spider is making a picture of
something it feels but has not yet lived.
What happens next?
Imagine the y ying down the village street.
Does it see the web?
The web is so ne the y cannot make it out.
It is indeed a rened picture of the y which the spider produces
in its web, says Uexkull. (158)
But what does the spiders invisible picture show?
The y of course!
It is so not (like) the y even the y cannot see it.
It shows the limits of the ys world.
But it is also a picture of the y at the limits of its world.
The y ies into the web.
And gets caught in its own picture?
56
57
invisible worlds
organic life] is some denite type of prehension
transmitted from occasion to occasion of its existence (18). As Uexkulls animal, human and
environmental entities differentially prehend
each other, we can imagine the stretching and
deforming of the limits of their life-worlds,
towards overlapping and involuting Umwelts.
So, the Picture Book is a Whiteheadean
occasion whereby individual entities, whether
human, animal or technical, partially concretize
as their life-worlds enmesh. The becoming of
individuals is never stable but metastable,23
as resultant forms of the individual ontologically
contain the tendency to recongure themselves,
based on a constantly shifting conjunction of
actual and affectual internal forces struck by
external forces as organized technics. In the
case of our argument, the technicity of the
Picture Book, in performing the foreign subjectivities of other animals as abstractions, enables
a form of thought and feeling where animality
and humanity take on partially concrete shapes
that are not xed. Each time the Picture Book
is performed, the individuation of entities also
shifts.24 Herein lies the great difference in comportment between the distanced scientic observer of animals, and the ecologist imaginatively
engaging with Picture Books! As Uexkull
writes, from the conventional viewpoint of
science, there is no mammal in itself as intuitable object, only as a notional abstraction, as a
concept which we use as a means of analysis
but never encounter in life. With the tick, this
is completely different (Theory of Meaning
179). The tick encounters the mammal as a
living abstraction, and we the readers of the
Picture Book, Uexkull goes on to imply, may
also use our concepts in ways that think-feel
(Massumi, Semblance and Event 39) the
strange partial concretization of our human
being that the ticks intuition entails.
The shifting lines drawn between animal and
human mediated by the technicity of the Picture
Book may seem to re-engage the anthropological
machine as a mobile mechanism (as Agamben
has pointed out) in ways that are not so different
from the scientic laboratory. But as the process
of individuation is only always partial, Umwelts
brillate between being open and closed: open
58
59
epilogue
One way of imagining the coming community
that Agamben anticipates in The Open would
be that in the end human beings learn to
accept their (base) animality, and animals
come to be accepted for their human-like
qualities (their intelligence, emotions, selfawareness). But if through the Picture Book
we attend to the invertebrates that are with
us in the laboratory and at the table, then it
no longer seems to be a matter of transposing
existing qualities between human and animal
but of creating an aesthetic medium in
which new and unforeseen sensibilities might
emerge.
invisible worlds
THE STARLING & THE FLY
The following story is told by the scientist Uexku ll.
A researcher who is a friend [] raised a young starling in a room,
and the bird had no opportunity ever to see a y, much less to catch
one. Then he observed that the bird suddenly started after an
unseen object, snapped it up in midair, brought it back to its perch
and began to hack away at it with its beak, as all the starlings do
with the ies they catch, and then swallowed the unseen thing.
(Foray 12021)
notes
The figures reproduced in this paper are from Jakob
von Uexkll, A Foray into the Worlds of Animals and
60
61
invisible worlds
Approaches symposium, Critical Studies in Architecture, KTH Stockholm, Stockholm, 2425 May 2012.
13 Stiegler, in Technics and Time, 1, in an effort to
postulate the origins of the human, explains the
originary default in the constitution of the
human through the myth of Epimetheus who
forgot to confer upon humans any special gifts,
and that the current power of the human being
and its ability to know, think and exteriorize is
the fire stolen from the gods by Prometheus.
14 Although, as Nathan van Camp has pointed out,
by recognizing the tertiary memories of human
life deposited in externalities, the human is decentred in ways that may disrupt the anthropological machine, we have also argued that tertiary memories deposited in the tools of the laboratory tend
back towards a picture of the centrality of the
human which covers over this destabilizing effect.
15 Of all the animals, insects appear to have the
highest technicity, so one way to adapt the conventional reading of Stiegler to human animal relations is
to say that these small creatures also operate, like
the tools of the laboratory, as tertiary memories
of the human being. Along these lines, Jussi Parikkas
recent book Insect Media investigates how insect
modes of organization swarms, webs and distributed agencies provide new ways of understanding
media technology and its relationship to biology,
which do not rest on the notion of individual
agents or a deterministic account of technology.
By reading insects via media formations, his book
opens up a new and rich account of the ways in
which digital culture helps to form and de-centre
human beings. This is a productive approach to
pursue. However, we would like to avoid a
reading that reduces insects to externalizations of
the human, because this potentially misses the
otherness of insect life that Uexkll emphasizes,
and the difficult question of their place in our
ethical thought and consideration.
16 For Stiegler, the capturing and formation of
attention in the young by technical and media industries threatens processes of what he calls human
maturity, and global social and cultural development (see Stiegler, Taking Care of Youth 78). By
contrast, we are suggesting that the Picture Book
is part of the formation of nascent attention as
imagination. For Stiegler, the partial or transitional
objects opened through childhood play are the first
forms of tertiary retention that can only appear in
62
bibliography
Agamben, Giorgio. Idea of Prose. Trans. Michael
Sullivan and Sam Whitsitt. New York: State U of
New York P, 1995. Print.
63
invisible worlds
Worlds. Instinctive Behavior: The Development of a
Modern Concept. Trans. Claire H. Schiller.
New York: International Universities P, 1957. 5
80. Print.
Uexkll, Jakob von, and G. Kriszat. Streifzge durch
die Umwelten von Tieren und Menschen: Ein
Bilderbuch
unsichtbarer
Welten.
Sammlung:
Verstndliche Wissenschaft, Berlin, 1934.
Van Camp, Nathan. Animality, Humanity and
Technicity. Transformations: Journal of Media and
Culture
17
(2009).
Web.
<http://www.
transformationsjournal.org/journal/issue_17/
article_06.shtml>.
Whitehead, Alfred North. Process and Reality: An
Essay in Cosmology. 1929. New York: Free, 1978.
Print.
Wolfe, Cary, ed. Zoontologies: The Question of the
Animal. Minneapolis: U of Minnesota P, 2003. Print.
Stephen Loo
School of Architecture & Design
University of Tasmania
Locked Bag 1323
Launceston 7250
Tasmania
Australia
E-mail: stephen.loo@utas.edu.au
Undine Sellbach
School of Philosophy and School of
Architecture & Design
University of Tasmania
Locked Bag 1323
Launceston 7250
Tasmania
Australia
E-mail: undine.sellbach@utas.edu.au