Mpodozis
Mpodozis
4757, 2000
Abstract
The optic tectum of the pigeon is a highly organized, multilayered structure that receives a massive polystratied afference of at least
ve different populations of retinal ganglion cells and gives rise to various anatomically segregated efferent systems. The synaptic
organization of retino-tectal circuitry is, at present, mostly unknown. To investigate the spatiotemporal prole of synaptic activation
produced by differential (electrical and visual) stimulation of the retinal inputs, we performed a high-spatial-resolution current source
density analysis in the optic tectum of the anaesthetized pigeon. Electrical stimuli consisted of brief pulses of different durations
applied to the optic nerve head, while visual stimuli consisted of light ashes of different intensities. Electrical stimulation generated
sinks conned to retinorecipient layers. The temporal structure, spatial location and thresholds of these sinks indicated that they are
all due to primary tectal synapses of retinal bers with different conduction velocities. Sinks evoked by the fastest retinal axons were
more supercially located than sinks produced by slower retinal bers. Visual stimulation, on the other hand, resulted in a more
complex pattern of current sinks, with various sinks located in the retinorecipient layers and also well below. Visual stimulation
induced action potentials at supercial as well as deep tectal levels. We conclude that electrical stimulation activates most of the
populations of ganglion cells as well as their primary tectal synapses, but is unable to elicit a signicant activation of secondary tectal
synapses. Visual stimulation, on the contrary, activates just some of the incoming retinal populations, but in a way that produces
noticeable secondary activation of intratectal circuits. Laminar segregation of retinally evoked tectal activity, as reported here, has
also been found in other vertebrates. Similarities and differences with previous studies are discussed.
Introduction
The rst central stage of the retino-tecto-fugal pathway in all
vertebrates is the optic tectum (called in mammals `superior
colliculus'). The avian optic tectum is a large, topographically
organized, mesencephalic structure consisting of 15 clearly dened
plexiform and cellular layers (Fig. 1B) (Ramon y Cajal, 1911; Cowan
et al., 1961). In pigeons it receives at least 90% of all the ganglion
cell axons (total 2.4 million) that originate in the contralateral retina
(Binggeli & Paule, 1969; Mpodozis et al., 1995). These axons
terminate in unequal numbers in layers 2, 3, 4, 5 and 7. Layer 5
receives the major portion of the retinal input, as demonstrated by the
total volume and density of retinal arborizations it contains (Fig. 1A)
(Hunt & Webster, 1975; Karten et al., 1997). The retinal input to the
tectum is not homogeneous; it is made of at least ve populations of
bers with different conduction velocities, ranging from 20 m/s to 1
2 m/s (Mpodozis et al., 1995; Dye & Karten, 1996). These
populations may have different visual properties, as a variety of
responses have been described to exist among the optic nerve bers
of birds (movement-sensitive, direction-sensitive, pure `off', pure
`on' and `on/off') (Miles, 1972; Hughes & Pearlman, 1974; Holden,
1977). Furthermore, retinal endings present characteristically distinct
morphologies at each tectal layer (Ramon y Cajal, 1911; Hunt &
Webster, 1975; Karten et al., 1997). This evidence suggests that the
retinal input is spatially segregated and that each retinorecipient layer
may contain terminals of a single specic kind of ganglion cell
(Ramon y Cajal, 1911; Reperant & Angaut, 1977; Karten et al.,
1997).
In birds, the optic tectum gives rise to various anatomically
independent visually driven efferent pathways, which terminate in
different structures such as the dorso-medial isthmus, the lateral pons,
the dorsal and ventral pretectal complex, the nucleus rotundus and
other thalamic nuclei (Benowitz & Karten, 1976; Reiner et al., 1979;
Reiner & Karten, 1982; Hunt & Brecha, 1984; Uchiyama et al.,
1996). Each of these pathways arises from specic cell groups located
mainly in layers 914. In between the supercial retinorecipient
layers and these output layers, a complex network of radial and
horizontal local interactions exists (Ramon y Cajal, 1911; Stone &
Freeman, 1971; Hunt & Webster, 1975; Hardy et al., 1985).
Despite both the description of the tectal eld potentials (TFP)
evoked by electrical stimulation of the retinal input and the
characterization of tectal receptive elds begun 30 years ago
(Holden, 1968a; Jassik-Gerschendfeld & Guichard, 1972; Mori,
1973; Jassik-Gerschenfeld et al., 1975; Frost & DiFranco, 1976; Frost
48 J. C. Letelier et al.
FIG. 1. Tectal organization and TFP proles. (A) Labelling of the uppermost seven layers of the tectum after the injection of cholera toxin in the contralateral eye. The label is conned to the terminal arborizations
of retinal axons and the bands mark the retinorecipient layers; layer 5 receives a particularly dense retinal input. (B) Correlation between the tectal layers, revealed by Nissl stain (left panel) and the electrically
evoked TFPs obtained at representative depths (right panel). Compare the TFP prole at supercial layers (23) with the TFP recorded at layers 1213, the inversion point is located approximately at layer 6. Scale
bar in A, 200 mm.
Methods
Animal preparation
Adult feral pigeons (Columba livia) of either sex, with a body weight
of 300350 g, purchased from a local provider and kept in an
institutional animal facility, were used. For surgical procedures, each
pigeon was deeply anaesthetized with equithesin injected intramuscularly (0.3 mL/100 g body weight). A canula inserted in the
pectoral muscle allowed supplementary doses. The pigeon was then
placed in a head-holding device at the orientation dened by the
stereotaxic atlas (Karten & Hodos, 1967). After opening the skin
posterior to the orbit, the temporal muscle was retracted and a
craniotomy was performed, exposing the centro-dorsal tectal region
for electrical recordings. During the experiment, the ECG was
continuously monitored and body temperature was kept at 4041 C
by means of a DC powered electric blanket. Anaesthesia was
maintained by injecting 0.3 mL of equithesin every 2 h. All
procedures were approved by the Ethical Committee of the
Facultad de Ciencias of the Universidad de Chile.
Stimulation and Recording
Tectal eld potentials were recorded using tungsten microelectrodes
(14 MW @1 kHz, Frederick Haer, ME, USA) along radially
orientated tracks that penetrated the optic tectum. The TFPs were
elicited by direct electrical stimulation of the optic nerve or by light
ashes. For electrical stimulation, the cornea was cut and the lens
carefully removed. Bipolar electrodes, consisting of two parallel
insulated tungsten wires separated by 1 mm, were placed near the
optic nerve head. Electrical stimuli consisted of rectangular current
pulses of 0.11 mA in amplitude, lasting 30, 100 or 300 ms. The
pulses were produced by a constant current stimulator (CCUI-8,
Frederick Haer), controlled by a pulse generator and slaved to the
main data acquisition program written in LabView 3.1 (National
Instruments, TX, USA). Our standard procedure was to place the
recording electrode just below the tectal surface and select the lowest
current of a 100-ms stimulation pulse that produced stable TFPs. We
then used 30, 100 and 300 ms pulses to record TFPs at every depth. In
experiments where light ashes served as stimuli, the eyelid was
maintained open by a suture, and two hemispherical diffusing screens
were inserted in the light path to ensure homogeneous illumination.
The light ashes were delivered by a conventional photographic ash
located at 2 m from the eye. The intensity of the ash was graduated
using calibrated neutral density lters in steps of 0.12 log units. Flash
intensities covering a total of 4 log units were used.
49
Results
Description of the temporal course of electrically and visually
evoked TFPs
To differentially stimulate the various groups of retinal axons, single
electrical pulses of different durations were used. At short stimulus
duration (30 ms) the supercial TFP exhibited a single negative peak
that developed with a 4.3-ms onset latency (6 1.8, n = 18), had a peak
latency of 5.1 ms (6 1.2, n = 18), and lasted 8.7 ms (6 2.1, n = 18)
50 J. C. Letelier et al.
(Fig. 3A, left panel). At increasing depths, 300 mm, the TFP prole
changed. Its initial phase became positive and its maximum was
reached at 45 ms. After correlating the readings of the microelectrode advancer with the histological analysis of electrolytic
lesions, the inversion zone was located at the bottom of the
retinorecipient layers (Fig. 1B). In the inversion zone, TFPs showed
complex, low-amplitude (2050 mV) features superimposed on the
main positive component, and only visible for 60100 mm. Below the
retinorecipient area the inversion was complete and the small features
disappeared. Tectal eld potentials remained positive until the bottom
of layer 14 and their amplitude decreased gradually with depth.
At intermediate stimulus duration (100 ms) (Fig. 3A, centre panel),
the amplitude of the initial negative phase increased between threeand ve-fold. Furthermore, depending on the experiment, one or
more secondary negative peaks appeared or were hinted at by an
inexion point in the trailing edge of the rst negative peak, with
peak latencies ranging between 7 and 15 ms. As was the case with
short stimuli, at intermediate stimulus duration the rst negative peak
reversed its polarity at 300 mm. The secondary negative peaks also
changed their polarity in the same region and small features appeared
around the inversion zone. Below the retinorecipient layers, only a
single positive peak was visible.
With maximal stimulus duration (300 ms), the peaks hinted at with
intermediate stimulus durations became clearly visible (Fig. 3A, right
panel) as three to ve negative peaks could be elicited at the tectal
surface. The mean onset latency of the rst peak was 4.3 ms (6 1.5 ,
n = 18), while the secondary peaks had onset latencies of 8 ms (6 2.1,
n = 18), 11 ms (6 3.5, n = 18), 15 ms (6 4.0, n = 14) and 17 ms (6 4.0,
n = 11), respectively. The rst peak had the largest amplitude, 1000
1500 mV, similar to the amplitude reached with intermediate
stimulation, whilst the amplitude of the longer-latency peaks was
much smaller, in the range 100200 mV. This complex prole
changed polarity with depth. Albeit not every peak reversed at
exactly the same depth, at 600 mm the TFP polarity was completely
reversed. Below 1000 mm a large positive peak was visible and,
sometimes, a smaller positive peak could be detected at 20 ms
latency.
The invariant features of electrically evoked TFPs could be
summarized as: (i) an initial negative phase with a 45 ms latency that
51
FIG. 3. Examples of tectal eld potentials evoked by electrical and visual stimulation. (A) Proles of electrically evoked TFPs at different depths and stimulus
durations. With a 30-ms pulse (left panel) the TFP shows, near the surface, an initial negative phase with a peak latency of 5 ms. Small, low-amplitude features are
visible near the inversion zone (). At 390 mm the initial phase becomes and remains positive until layer 13. With 100-ms pulses (centre panel) the TFP shows the
same qualitative prole but with a larger amplitude. The existence of secondary negative peaks is hinted at by the inexion points (*). With 300-ms pulses (right
panel) the supercial TFP prole shows four negative peaks with different amplitudes (r). The rst peak becomes positive at 390 mm. At the bottom of layer 13
(1400 mm) the waveform shows only two positive peaks, with different amplitudes. (B) The left and right panels show visually evoked TFPs triggered by light
ashes with a 1 : 2 intensity ratio. Visually evoked TFPs are more complex than electrically evoked ones. At low ash intensity (B, left panel) two clear negative
phases, separated by 30 ms, are visible near the surface (*). At 390 mm a positive peak can be detected but, as its latency does not correspond to the latency of the
negative peak recorded at the tectal surface, this positive peak is not the simple inversion of the negative peak found at the surface. Below the retinorecipient layers
the TFP prole is not completely positive. Negative components with 5070 ms latencies remain and do not change polarity as the electrode goes deeper into the
tectum. At higher ash intensity, a more complex TFP prole appears (B, right panel). Near the surface the number of negative components increases and the
latency of the rst negative component decreases. The complete visually evoked TFP lasts for 120 ms and contains many peaks. At 390 mm (corresponding to layer
5) the initial negative peak remains, but the rst positive peak is much larger. Below the retinorecipient layer (at 600 mm) the rst peak is totally inverted but the
secondary negative peaks are not.
area, extended for 100 mm, had an onset latency of 3.0 ms and
lasted for 6 ms. The histological analysis showed that this depth
corresponded to tectal layer 5 with the lower source located
between layer 7 and layer 9. No additional sinks were detected
below the retinorecipient layers. In two cases a very small sink
could be detected at the very surface with an onset latency of
3.0 ms (Fig. 4A, sink p).
At intermediate stimulus duration (100 ms), the CSD pattern
usually exhibited more sinks with longer latencies. In some
preparations two sinks were visible, while in others up to four sinks
appeared. The rst sink, S, resolved itself in two components (S1 &
S2 in Fig. 4B) with peak latencies of 4.2 and 6 ms. Two additional
sinks, with peak latencies of 7 and 12 ms, could be distinguished (S3
& S4). These subsequent sinks tended to appear 50100 mm below the
rst sink (S1) (Fig. 4B).
With longer pulses (300 ms) a total of ve sinks was normally
obtained. A new sink (S5) (Fig. 4C) with an onset latency of 17.5 ms
and a peak latency of 19 ms appeared between 200 and 300 mm. The
four sinks (S1, S2, S3 and S4) detected with 100 ms pulses continued
to be present at the same locations and latencies but with higher
amplitudes. In the case displayed in Fig. 4C, sink S4 appeared spread
out indicating the possible presence of other sinks with even longer
latencies and high thresholds. As was the case with 30- and 100-ms
52 J. C. Letelier et al.
FIG. 4. Colour maps of CSDs evoked by electrical stimulation. Colour-coded representation of CSD proles obtained by recording TFPs at 30-mm separation.
Sinks appear as red-yellow, sources appear as deep blue. The regular blue-green bands are a side-effect of the CSD calculation. (A) At low stimulus intensity, a
prominent sink with a peak latency of 5.5 ms lasting 7 ms appeared at 300 mm (S). At the very surface a small amplitude and short latency sink can be distinguished
(P) reecting the massive and synchronous activation of the fastest retinal axons. (B) With 100 ms pulses new sinks appear and a clearer spatiotemporal resolution
is obtained. The S sink triggered by 30 ms pulses is resolved in two components. The rst component (S1) has a peak latency of 4.2 ms and the second (S2) 6.2 ms.
The second component reaches the 400 mm level. Two additional sinks (S3 and S4), with onset latencies of 7 and 12 ms, can be distinguished in the 300400 mm
region. The sink P is now clearly visible at the surface. (C) With 300-ms pulses a massive recruitment of sink/source pairs mostly located in layers 57 is evident.
Five postsynaptic sinks can be distinguished between 200 and 400 mm (S1, S2, S3, S4 and S5) with peak latencies of 4, 5.3, 7, 12 and 19 ms, respectively.
less intense than the inferior source. The mean extent of the sources
was 188 6 96 mm (n = 18), and correlated with the mean extent of
sinks (r = 0.80). The neutral zone separating each sink from its
53
FIG. 5. Colour maps of CSDs evoked by two ash intensities. The sink/source prole obtained by visual stimulation was highly variable among different animals.
At low ash intensity (A), the presence of many sinks in the supercial as well as the deep tectal layers can be observed. The earliest sinks have a latency of
24 ms. At higher ash intensity (B), more sink/source pairs appear and the earliest sinks decrease their latency to 20 ms. Compare the presence of sinks below
the retinorecipient layers with Fig. 4. (Colour map as in Fig. 4)
54 J. C. Letelier et al.
and 40), selectivity for motion stimulus and strong habituation to
repetitive stimulation; all suggest that they originate from tectal cells.
In particular, the frequency of these recordings coincided with the
crossing of the electrode through the tectal cellular layers (especially
layers 68, 10 and 13). Light ashes were able to trigger these tectal
spikes, at least in a fraction of the units recorded.
In the case of electrical stimulation, multiunitary activity in phase
with the negative component of TFPs was recorded at the level of
retinorecipient layers. This indicates that the recordings were taken
from the tectal area activated by the stimulus. Below those layers no
spikes, time-locked to stimulus onset, were found, even at the level of
layers 10 or 13, where well-resolved recordings of isolated spikes are
common because of the large size of the neurons. These ndings
correlate with differences in the CSD proles obtained for the two
types of stimulation, as explained in the Discussion.
Discussion
Analysis of TFPs and CSD
Tectal eld potentials evoked by short electrical pulses (30 ms) exhibit
a single negative peak at the most supercial tectal layers. This peak
reverses its polarity around 300400 mm, and becomes a positive
wave toward the deeper layers. Increasing the duration of the pulses
increases the number of negative components, until four or ve peaks
are obtained with 300 ms pulses. This correlation probably resulted
from the recruitment of additional populations of retinal bers with
progressively slower conduction velocities. The classic results of
Bishop (Bishop et al., 1953) showed that in the optic nerve of cats,
brief pulses of 30 ms stimulate thick myelinated bers with fast
conduction velocities, whereas longer pulses recruit unmyelinated
bers with slower conduction velocities. In pigeons, recordings made
in the optic nerve of the compound action potential while
antidromically stimulating the optic tectum have directly shown that
increasing the duration of the stimulating pulse produces the
recruitment of slower conduction velocity groups, up to ve for the
tectum (Mpodozis et al., 1995). The wave of activity of each
population arriving sequentially in the tectum would then produce a
sequence of postsynaptic potentials recorded as negative components
in the TFPs. Another mechanism that may contribute to the
generation of delayed negative components is the synchronized
activation of postsynaptic intratectal circuits. However, as we argue
next, our CSD analysis and spike recordings do not favour this
interpretation.
The spatio-temporal structure of electrically evoked synaptic
currents shows that a single stimulation pulse delivered to the optic
nerve can trigger many sinks located in the retinorecipient layers and
none below layer 7. The recruitment of longer latency sinks as
stimulus duration increased (Fig. 4) reects both the range of
conducting velocities found among retino-tectal axons (202 m/s)
and the increasingly higher threshold of slow-conducting retinal
axons. Also, the temporal coherence of longer latency sinks is lower
than those of earlier sinks. This time-spreading is correlated with the
wider distribution of conduction velocity among slow-conducting
retinal bers (Clare et al., 1969). Taken together all this evidence
(dependency on stimulus duration, relation between temporal
coherence and onset latency, location inside the retinorecipient
layers) strongly suggests that, with electrical stimulation of retinal
bers, every sink detected in the retinorecipient layers is monosynaptically driven by a specic group of retinal axons. Furthermore,
the absence of sinks between layer 7 and layer 13 correlates well with
the absence of neuronal responses locked to the electrical pulse in that
region. Nevertheless, because one-dimensonal CSD analysis only
55
FIG. 7. Comparison of sinks triggered by electrical stimulation of the optic nerve in the frog pigeon and cat. Summary of tectal CSDs evoked after electrical
stimulation of the optic nerve in frogs, cats and this study. In frogs, sink A is directly produced by retinal input. Sinks C and E result from intratectal circuits and
sinks B and D are triggered by a mixture of direct and indirect inputs. In cats, sinks A and D are produced by a direct retinal input, sink B by an indirect
retinocortical input and sinks C and E by a mixture of both kinds of inputs. In all three systems retinal inputs are segregated according to conduction velocity. In
pigeons the fastest bers terminate more supercially than the rest of the retinal axons and the electrical activation of the optic nerve does not trigger secondary
sinks as in the frog. All sinks appear to be generated by a direct retinal input and not by an indirect telencephalic input as in the cat.
illumination (Duff & Cohen, 1975) indicate that retinal ganglion cells
respond with a train of spikes to a single light ash. This is also
corroborated by direct recordings made in the optic tract, as the
compound action potential evoked by a ash showed a very regular
waveform with four or ve evenly spaced peaks (8 ms) of similar
amplitude (Fig. 6A). Thus, some of the supercial sinks are probably
due to a complex interaction among the bursty structure of spike trains
of retinal ganglion cells, their different conduction velocities and the
nonhomogeneous visual properties of these cells. This complexity of
the retinal input makes it difcult to investigate whether some of the
supercial sinks, especially the later ones, could correspond to
polysynaptic activation (or postsynaptic ring). Although the overall
pattern of supercial sinks was similar in all the preparations
stimulated with light ashes, the precise spatio-temporal pattern of
sinks and sources is more variable than the pattern obtained with
electrical stimulation. Below the retinorecipient layers, light ashes
triggered clear and intense sinks. The most intense sinks appear at
700 mm. In association with these clear sinks, neuronal responses,
time-locked to the ash, appear at the same level, thus providing
evidence that secondary circuits are triggered by light ashes.
Spatial segregation of retinal inputs
Besides the temporal segregation of electrically evoked sinks, our
results also demonstrate spatial segregation. The earliest sink,
produced by fast-conducting axons, is more supercially located
than sinks with longer latencies produced by slower-conducting
axons. This difference indicates that, on average, fast retinal bers
arborize more supercially than slow ones. Recent anatomical
ndings (Karten et al., 1997) indicate that layer 5, the main
retinorecipient layer, contains terminal arborizations of the w-5b
population of ganglion cells that have the nest unmyelinated axons
and would thus have the slowest conduction velocity. In agreement
with these observations, we found that the long latency sinks elicited
by electrical stimulation are commonly located at the level of layer 5.
On average then, most of the activity elicited in the tectum by
electrical stimulation of retinal afferences takes place at that tectal
layer.
56 J. C. Letelier et al.
The presence of strong sinks near layer 5 also correlates with the
anatomical observations showing that the more abundant kind of
tecto-rotundal projecting neurons, Type I cells, have dendritic
expansions in layer 5, and that they are probably monosynaptically
connected with the retinal endings at that layer (Karten et al., 1997;
Luksch et al., 1998). Type I neurons project to the anterior and
centralis subdivisions of the nucelus rotundus (Karten et al., 1997).
Extracellular recordings have shown that each of these regions of the
rotundus responds to different visual dimensions (Wang et al., 1993).
Taken together, the aforementioned evidence, the results presented
here and the well-known correlation between conduction velocity
classes and functional types [exemplied by the Y, X, W subsystems
(Bishop et al., 1964; Stone, 1983)] suggest that the retinal ganglion
cells projecting into layer 5b, instead of forming a single denite
functional class, are a heterogeneous collection of ganglion cells that
might contribute to synthesize some of the various visual properties
detected upstream in the ascending tectofugal pathway.
Comparison with previous studies in the pigeon
The TFPs obtained in this study differ from those described in
previous studies of the pigeon's optic tectum where only a single `N'
wave has been described (Holden, 1968a; Stone & Freeman, 1971;
Mori, 1973; Bagnoli et al., 1977; Holden, 1980; Dye & Karten, 1996).
Here we report the existence of more complex waveforms with many
negative components. We found a direct relation between the number
of peaks detected and the duration of the stimulus. At short stimulus
duration, our TFPs showed a single `N-like' negativity whose
features (latency, duration and vertical location of the inversion
point) resemble that of the previous studies. Mori (Mori, 1973) also
reported that increasing the strength of the stimulation produces
multipeaked TFPs. However, in his studies he only used data from
experiments where a single peak was elicited.
Another result of the current study, the absence of evoked action
potentials by electrical stimulation, differs from intracellular recording of tectal cells performed in anaesthetized pigeons (Hardy et al.,
1984; Leresche et al., 1986) and from the work of Holden (Holden,
1968b). In the intracellular studies it was found that 25% of tectal
cells responded to an electrical pulse delivered to the optic nerve with
an EPSP followed by at least one action potential. In this study we
explored intensively below the retinorecipient layers and specically
at and near the output layer 13, and no cellular responses were found.
Besides differences in experimental procedures, two reasons could
explain the difference between this study and previous studies.
Firstly, because the depth of the intracellular responses is not
specied in the original study, it could be that some of the responses
were obtained from the retinorecipient layers. Secondly, a well
known side-effect of cellular impalement by a microelectrode is to
render the cell more excitable because the puncture slightly
depolarizes the cells. Thus, EPSPs that would, under normal
conditions, be subthreshold might generate an action potential. In
any case we feel that this question requires further research.
Comparison with other vertebrates
In the frog, electrical stimulation of the optic nerve elicits ve sinks
located at different tectal levels (Fig. 7). The rst sink to appear (sink
A) is located at the bottom of the retinorecipient layers (layer 8) and
is produced by fast, myelinated retinal bers (Witpaard & Keurs,
1975). The second and fourth sinks to appear (B and D) are located
more supercially (at retiniorecipient layer 9) and appear to be a
mixture produced by the activation of slower retinal axons and of
polysynaptic intratectal circuits. The third and fth sinks (C and E)
are both located outside the retinorecipient layers, and can be
Acknowledgements
We thank Elisa Sentis for technical assistance. This research was supported by
grant #1950687 from the Chilean Scientic Council (CONYCIT) to J.C.L.
Abbreviations
CSD, current source density; SGS, stratum griseum supercialis; TFP, tectal
eld potential.
References
Bagnoli, P., Francesconi, W. & Magni, F. (1977) Visual Wulst inuences on
the optic tectum of the pigeon. Brain Behav. Evol., 14, 217237.
Benowitz, L.I. & Karten, H.J. (1976) Organization of the tectofugal visual
pathway in the pigeon: a retrograde transport study. J. Comp. Neurol., 167,
503520.
Binggeli, R.L. & Paule, W.J. (1969) The pigeon retina: Quantitative aspects of
the optic nerve and ganglion cell layer. J. Comp. Neurol., 137, 118.
57