Ijmperd - Multitasking
Ijmperd - Multitasking
MULTITASKING
VYANKATESH S. KULKARNI1 & BRAJESH TRIPATHI2
1
Research Scholar, Department of Mechanical Engineering, JSPM-BIT, Barshi, Solapur, Maharashtra, India
2
ABSTRACT
Multitasking (often referred to as timesharing) has been extensively studied from a mental workload and
humanperformance perspective. However, a relatively small amount of research has been conducted in the
manufacturingdomain (Wickens, 1992). As the level of system automation increases, the role of the human has shifted
from thatof a manual controller to system supervisor (Sheridan and Johannsen, 1976). According to Sheridan
(1994),human operators in AMS make their way among machines, inspecting parts, observingdisplays, and
modifyingcontrol settings or keying in commands, most of it through computer-mediated control panels adjacent to
variousmachines. This role of human operators in AMS has been identified as supervisory control in this paper
KEYWORDS: Human Supervisory Control, General paradigm of supervisory control (Sheridan, 1976), Capabilities of
human and computer in planning/scheduling tasks of AMS (Nakamura, and Salvendy, 1994), Different type of
Received: Mar 01, 2016; Accepted: Mar 10, 2016; Published: Apr 07, 2016; Paper Id.: IJMPERDAPR20164
INTRODUCTION
Original Article
disturbances in AMS (Kuivanen, 1996) , Determinants of Multitasking Performance, Performance-resource function for
Multitasking (often referred to as timesharing) has been extensively studied from a mental workload and
human performance perspective. However, a relatively small amount of research has been conducted in the
manufacturing domain (Wickens, 1992). As the level of system automation increases, the role of the human has
shifted from that of a manual controller to system supervisor (Sheridan and Johannsen, 1976). According to
Sheridan (1994), human operators in AMS make their way among machines, inspecting parts, observing displays,
and modifying control settings or keying in commands, most of it through computer-mediated control panels
adjacent to various machines. This role of human operators in AMS has been identified as supervisory control. 26
Human Supervisory Control
Supervisory control refers to one or more human operators programming and receiving information from a
computer that interconnects through artificial effectors and sensors to the controlled process or task environment
(Sheridan, 1987). Ammons, Govindaraj, and Mitchell (1988) described the supervisory controller as an operator
responsible for a group of complex machinery where the operations require intermittent attention and depend
on higher-levelperceptual and cognitive functions. Sheridan (1976) defined a general paradigm of supervisory
control consisting of five functions: 1) Plan, 2) Teach, 3) Monitor, 4) Intervene, and 5) Learn. For each of the main
supervisory functions the computer provides decision-aiding and implementation capabilities, as shown in Figure 1.
A description of these functions is presented in Figure 2.
Job scheduling, inventory planning, and problem solving (disturbance control) have been among
www.tjprc.org
editor@tjprc.org
38
the supervisory control responsibilities commonly assigned to human operators in AMS (Suri and Whitney, 1984;
Ammons et al., 1988). The capabilities of humans and computers in AMS planning/scheduling tasks are presented in Table
1 (Nakamura and Salvendy, 1994). Table 2 shows examples of different types of unexpected contingencies (disturbances)
in AMS (Kuivanen, 1996). Ammons et al. (1988) stated that two ways in which the unique skills of the human decision
maker are used in supervisory control are to fine-tune or refine standard operating procedures for particular system states
and to compensate for unplanned events and unexpected contingencies.
Decide on overall goal or goals, the objective function, tradeoffs among goals, and criteria for handling
uncertainties
Teach
Multitasking
39
Monitor
Allocate attention appropriately among the various subsystems to measure salient state variables
Intervene
Make minor adjustments of system parameters when necessary, as the automatic control continues take over
Learn
Figure 2: Temporal Nesting of the General Paradigm of Supervisory Control Functions (Sheridan, 1976)
www.tjprc.org
editor@tjprc.org
40
Multitasking
41
editor@tjprc.org
42
multitasking performance (McCloy, Derrick, and Wickens, 1983; Bi and Salvendy, 1994). Figure 3 shows the relationship
among theperformance-resource function for multitasking (Wickens, 1992). Sheridan (1994) stated that mental workload is
very important for supervisory control in AMS where the human operator is constantly called upon to do multiple complex
sensory and judgmental tasks. The central issue for vigilance research is to determine the effect of the additional tasks to
the vigilance performance (Craig, 1991).
CONCLUSIONS
The experiment tested the following hypotheses:
Hypothesis 1: The operators decision making component of the quality inspection task in AMS will be
significantly affected by the appearance of different types of defects in the units being produced.
Hypothesis 2: The operators decision making component of the quality inspection task in AMS will be
significantly affected by multitasking.
Hypothesis 3: The operators decision making component of the quality inspection task in AMS will be
significantly affected by the interaction of multitasking with the appearance of different types of defects in the units being
produced.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
To my Guide and Parents for their expertise, encouragement, invaluable assistance, guidance, advice and their
patience with me throughout my study. All the learners who selflessly volunteered to be part of this study and most of all
their parents for giving them permission to participateI wish to express my sincere gratitude
Multitasking
43
REFERENCES
1.
2.
3.
Drury, 1992b
4.
5.
6.
7.
www.tjprc.org
editor@tjprc.org