Dual Pillars Grit
Dual Pillars Grit
Jon M. Jachimowicz
Andreas Wihler
Adam D. Galinsky
Author Note
J.M. Jachimowicz designed and conducted the study. J.M. Jachimowicz and A. Wihler analyzed
the data. J.M. Jachimowicz wrote the manuscript, and A. Wihler and A.D. Galinsky provided
critical revisions. All authors approved the final version of the manuscript for submission. The
authors thank Erica Bailey, Angela Duckworth, Kristen Duke, Lauren Eskreis-Winkler, Andrea
Freund, Sandra Matz, Evan Nesterak, Christopher To, Aurora Turek, and Kristina Wald for
Abstract
Previous research has found mixed evidence regarding the relationship between grit—defined as
perseverance and passion for long-term goals—and performance. We propose that this
inconsistency has occurred because prior research has emphasized perseverance, both
assesses whether individuals attain desired levels of passion will predict performance. A survey
of employees matched to supervisor-rated job performance (N = 422) found that the combination
of perseverance, assessed through the original measure of grit, and passion attainment, measured
through a separate scale, predicted employees’ performance. The current results help resolve
prior mixed evidence of grit’s relationship with performance: while grit has been defined as the
combination of perseverance and passion, grit’s original measure failed to capture passion. By
adequately measuring both components, the current research uncovers grit’s predictive power.
“I think the misunderstanding—or, at least, one of them—is that it’s only the
perseverance part that matters… But I think that the passion piece is at least as
important. I mean, if you are really, really tenacious and dogged about a goal that’s not
meaningful to you, and not interesting to you—then that’s just drudgery. It’s not just
determination—it’s having a direction that you care about.”
[Angela Duckworth, May 9, 2016 (New York Magazine)]
While an individuals’ skills and talent promote future performance (Collings & Mellahi,
2009; Lewis & Heckman, 2006), a recent program of research suggests that grit—defined as
“perseverance and passion for long-term goals” (Duckworth et al., 2007, p. 1087)—predicts
additional variance in future success (Duckworth, Peterson, Matthews, & Kelly, 2007;
Duckworth & Quinn, 2009). Indeed, grit relates to several performance intermediaries, including
increased deliberate practice (Duckworth, Kirby, Tsukayama, Berstein, & Ericsson, 2011),
sustained retention in difficult jobs (Duckworth et al., 2007; Duckworth & Quinn, 2009), and task
persistence (Lucas, Gratch, Cheng, & Marsella, 2015). These insights have even influenced the
design of school curriculums to make students “grittier” to boost their future success (Smith,
2014).
Given this evidence, one would expect to find copious studies showing that grit predicts
performance. Surprisingly, evidence linking grit and performance is actually pretty sparse. To the
best of our knowledge, only one recent study directly investigated the grit-performance
relationship, which found an insignificant effect (Ion, Mindu, Gorbăbescu, 2017). In addition,
several studies, as well as a recent meta-analysis, have not found a significant relationship
between grit and various indicators of success (Chang, 2014; Credé, Tynan, & Harms, 2017;
Ivcevic & Brackett, 2014; Rimfield, Kovas, Dale & Plomin, 2016). Overall, prior research has
thus found inconclusive evidence regarding the relationship between grit and performance.
We propose that this inconsistency has occurred because, although the definition of grit
contains the two conceptual components of perseverance and passion, the grit literature and
Running Head: THE DUAL PILLARS OF GRIT 4
consequently its measurement has focused on perseverance and does not adequately capture
passion. Indeed, the current measure of grit likely reflects perseverance alone; related to this
point, note that studies which find effects of the original grit measure on performance use
outcomes that are highly connected to perseverance (e.g., increased deliberate practice, sustained
The neglect of passion in the measurement of grit is particularly problematic because prior
research stresses how passion produces beneficial effects on performance through a focus on
important and meaningful goals (Cardon, Wincent, Singh, & Drnovsek, 2009; Chen, Yao, &
Kotha, 2009; Perrewé et al., 2014; Vallerand et al., 2003). In the current paper, we thus propose
that assesses whether individuals attain desired levels of passion will predict performance. We
test this proposition in a field study with employees at a technology company. In doing so, we
also address calls to examine the effects of grit outside of scholastic performance (Credé et al.,
2017).
From its inception, grit has stood on the dual pillars of perseverance and passion. The
definition of grit includes the word passion: “perseverance and passion for long-term goals”
(Duckworth et al., 2007, p. 1087), and the scale used to assess grit features two components
—“persistence of effort” and “consistency of interests”—the latter of which has been suggested
Despite the conceptual importance of passion in grit, there are theoretical and empirical
concerns about whether the original measure of grit truly captures passion. Theoretically, the
claim that “consistency of interests” equates to passion is not supported by the passion literature.
Passion has been conceptualized as the engagement in “tasks and activities that hold identity
Running Head: THE DUAL PILLARS OF GRIT 5
meaning” (Cardon et al., 2009, p. 521), which individuals both “like” and find “personally
important” (Vallerand et al., 2003, p. 757). While “consistency of interests” may be statistically
correlated with personally important and meaningful interests, they are conceptually distinct.
Consider this example: an academic may maintain a high consistency in an interest (e.g., the
revision of manuscripts as part of the publication process), but the interest itself may have little
interests” and passion. Consider that a recent study finds no significant relationship between self-
rated “consistency of interests” and other-rated passion (Grohman, Ivcevic, Silvia, & Kaufman,
2017). This concern is also borne out in a recent meta-analysis (Credé et al., 2017), which reports
This correlation size supports the presence of a higher-order construct (Judge, Erez, Bono, &
Thorensen, 2002), leading Credé and colleagues (2017) to conclude that the original grit scale
measures perseverance, rather than two separate components (i.e., perseverance and passion).
This discrepancy may have occurred because the grit literature has not sufficiently
connected with prior passion research. In Duckworth et al.’s (2007) seminal paper introducing
grit, the word “passion” only appears in the title of the manuscript, the definition of grit, and as a
discussion or reference to passion other than the definition of grit which includes the word
passion (e.g., Duckworth & Quinn, 2009; Duckworth, Kirby, Tsukayama, Berstein, & Anders
Ericsson, 2011; Eskreis-Winkler, Shulman, Beal, & Duckworth, 2014; Robertson-Kraft &
Taken together, there is considerable theoretical and empirical evidence that the scale
perseverance. We therefore subsequently refer to what the original grit scale measures as
perseverance. However, the importance of passion has been a key component of grit’s purported
beneficial effects, as exemplified in the opening quote (see also Duckworth et al., 2007;
Duckworth, 2016). Uncovering grit’s predictive power thus requires adequate measurement of
both perseverance and passion, which prior studies have not done.
The original specification of grit proposed that the combination of perseverance and
passion predicts performance (Duckworth et al., 2007). Indeed, passion is essential for
perseverance to unfold its’ beneficial effects because it steers individuals toward goals they deem
valuable and important (Chen et al., 2009; Cardon et al., 2009; Vallerand et al., 2003). While
perseverance helps individuals in goal pursuit by remaining engaged, meaningful goals are
essential for individuals to attain higher performance (Vinchur, Schippmann, Switzer, & Roth,
1998). When employees pursue the wrong type of goals—those that are not valuable or important
—perseverance may not produce increased performance. In contrast, when employees pursue
meaningful and important goals, as prompted by their passion, higher perseverance may improve
Indeed, many highly persevering individuals achieve success only when pursuing goals
they are passionate about. Consider the Italian singer Andrea Bocelli, who frequently points to
his perseverance as a key to his success. However, Bocelli originally started his career as a
lawyer—after college studies that required substantial persistence, particularly given his
blindness—and only attained success after switching to singing, “a passion he couldn’t shake”
(PBS, 2015). This example illustrates that assessing the purported benefits of the combined
effects of perseverance and passion originally proposed by grit researchers requires the adequate
The current research further highlights the importance of passion attainment, i.e.,
assessing whether people experience desired levels of passion (Jachimowicz, To, Menges, &
Akinola, 2017; Jachimowicz et al., 2018). In prior research, passion has commonly been assessed
using an adaptation of Vallerand et al.’s (2003) harmonious passion scale, which was originally
developed to assess how individuals feel about activities that are “very dear to their heart”
(Vallerand et al., 2003, p. 758). However, individuals commonly assess whether they are
experiencing desired levels of passion (Buckley, Fedor, Veres, Wiese, & Carraher, 1998; Major,
Kozlowski, Chao, & Gardner, 1995) and are guided by whether they have met this expectation
(Podsakoff, LePine, & LePine, 2007; Wanous, Poland, Premack, & Davis, 1992; Wilson, Lisle,
Kraft, & Wetzel, 1989). Indeed, a recent stream of research highlights that the experience of
passion is guided by whether an individual attains or falls short of desired levels of passion and
not by their absolute levels of harmonious passion (Jachimowicz et al., 2017, 2018). Thus, we
propose whether an individual has attained desired levels of passion will be the critical construct
Study
Context
predicted that perseverance, as measured by the original grit scale, would only relate to increased
supervisor-rated performance when employees attained desired levels of passion, capturing the
combined benefits of both grit components. In contrast, we predicted that for employees who did
not attain desired levels of passion, there would be no significant relationship between
The study was conducted with employees of a technology company located in a Spanish-
speaking country. Employees work in teams guided by a supervisor who also provides annual
Employees were contacted by an email sent out by the first author, which contained a link to the
survey hosted on Qualtrics.com. Prior to this email, the company’s Human Resources department
asked employees to take part in a study conducted by the first author. Employees were
guaranteed that their responses would be kept entirely confidential, and that their employer would
not have access to any of their responses. The email was sent out to all the firm’s 2,293
employees, and 178 emails bounced back. In addition to the employee survey data, the
company’s Human Resources team provided performance ratings for each employee. These
performance ratings were conducted in the month before the survey was sent to employees.
Prior to our data collection, we conducted an a priori power analysis using conventional
values for a small to medium effect size (f² = .075, α = .05, β = .95; 3 variables; Cohen, 1988), as
would be expected for typical interaction effects in social science (ranging from 3% to 10%;
Chaplin, 1991; Champoux & Peters, 1987). Calculations reveal that we would need a sample of
We received partial survey data from 1,265 employees and could match 560 employee-
supervisor dyads. After dropping dyads with missing data on our focal variables, implausible
data, or where we were unable to identify the corresponding supervisors, our final sample
consisted of 422 employees (response rate: 33.5%). This subset of employees did not differ in
age, gender, or tenure from partial respondents (all ps > .22). Our sample included 169 female
employees, ages ranged from 18 and 63 (M = 30.96 years, SD = 6.99), and employees had been
working in the organization for an average of 3.69 years (SD = 2.88). Overall, 85 supervisors
Running Head: THE DUAL PILLARS OF GRIT 9
provided performance ratings with an average of 5 subordinate ratings per supervisor (SD =
5.25).
Measures
Given the company’s location, we used the translation procedure outlined by Schaffer and
Riordan (2003) to adapt our measures to Spanish. Where not stated otherwise, the measures used
Perseverance. To measure perseverance, we used the Short Grit Scale (Grit-S; Duckworth
& Quinn, 2009). This scale consists of 8 items of the Grit Scale developed and validated by
Duckworth, Peterson, Matthews, and Kelly (2007). The Short Grit Scale has improved
psychometric properties over the full Grit Scale (Duckworth & Quinn, 2009). This measure
consists of 4 items each for two different factors, “perseverance of effort” and “consistency of
interests.” Sample items are “I have overcome setbacks to conquer an important challenge
(perseverance of effort)” and “I often set a goal but later choose to pursue a different one
(reversed; consistency of interest).” Following Duckworth et al. (2007) and Duckworth and
Passion Attainment. We measured the extent to which employees feel they attain their
desired level of passion for their work with three items developed and previously used by
Jachimowicz et al. (2017). These items read “I am less passionate for my work than I should be,”
“I often feel as if I have to be more passionate for my work,” and “I frequently feel obliged to be
more passionate for my work than I currently am.” We reverse-scored the items, such that higher
Harmonious Passion. To highlight that the results are driven by passion alignment, we
also assessed harmonious passion with Vallerand et al.’s (2003) harmonious passion scale (α
Running Head: THE DUAL PILLARS OF GRIT 10
= .90). Sample items include “My work is in harmony with the other activities in my life” and
supervisors use for annual performance evaluation. This measure was conducted and provided by
the HR department of the company, and covers different aspects of work that are important for
the organization. The performance measure varies on a scale of 1 (very poor performance) to 5
(very good performance). Given the average job performance score of 4.39 (SD = .52), we further
evaluated the skewness and kurtosis of the performance measure. Both skewness (-.80) and
Control Variables. In our analyses, we also controlled for age, gender, tenure, and
motivation. Age was included because older people tend to receive worse performance
evaluations (Waldman & Avolio, 1986) and perseverance increases with age (Duckworth &
Quinn, 2009). We controlled for gender because research cannot rule out whether female
employees receive worse performance ratings (Bowen, Swim, & Jacobs, 2000); and for
organizational tenure since it has been shown to be related to job performance (Ng & Feldman,
2010). Finally, to distinguish our effects of the interaction between perseverance and passion
attainment from motivation (Von Culin et al., 2014), and to provide a more rigorous test of our
hypothesis, we also controlled for prosocial (α = .85), intrinsic (α = .88), and extrinsic (α = .83)
motivation using measures from Grant (2008). We included all control variables in subsequent
steps after first testing our predicted interaction effect separately following recommendations by
Becker (2005) and Becker et al. (2016) and to avoid spurious results in our hypothesis tests.
Results
Prior to testing our hypothesis, we assessed the distinctiveness of the constructs (i.e.,
confirmatory factor analyses. In the first model, items of each construct loaded onto their
respective factor (grit scale items loaded on both sub-components, which in turn loaded on a
higher-order factor of perseverance). The fit-indices were good: Χ²(334) = 845.64, RMSEA = .06,
CFI = .92, SRMR = .08. Next, we compared this model to a model where we loaded all items
from every measure on one common factor. This model fit the data worse: Χ²(350) = 3110.19,
RMSEA = .14, CFI = .55, SRMR = .12. Additionally, the fit of the first model was significantly
motivation, we conducted additional confirmatory factor analyses. In the first model, items of
perseverance, passion attainment, and intrinsic motivation loaded on their respective factor. The
model fit was good: Χ²(85) = 249.07, RMSEA = .07, CFI = .94, SRMR = .06. In the second
model, the items of perseverance and intrinsic motivation loaded together on one factor. The
model fit (Χ²(89) = 884.32, RMSEA = .15, CFI = .70, SRMR = .13) was significantly worse: ΔΧ²
= 635.25, Δdf = 4, p < .001. Finally, in the third model, the items of passion attainment and
intrinsic motivation loaded together on one factor. Again, the model fit (Χ²(87) = 478.68,
RMSEA = .10, CFI = .85, SRMR = .08) was significantly worse: ΔΧ² = 229.61, Δdf = 2, p < .001.
In addition, we also evaluated whether passion attainment and harmonious passion are
distinct constructs. In the first model, items of passion attainment and harmonious passion loaded
on their respective factor. The model fit (Χ²(26) = 91.99, RMSEA = .08, CFI = .97, SRMR = .03)
was good. Next, we combined both factors and the model fit (Χ²(27) = 324.49, RMSEA = .16,
CFI = .86, SRMR = .08) was significantly worse compared to the first model: ΔΧ² = 232.50, Δdf
= 1, p < .001. In sum, these results let us conclude that both perseverance as well as passion
Running Head: THE DUAL PILLARS OF GRIT 12
attainment are distinct constructs which can be differentiated from intrinsic motivation as well as
harmonious passion.
Correlation Table
Table 1 presents the means, standard deviations, correlations, and internal consistency
reliability estimates for all variables. Neither perseverance, passion attainment, harmonious
passion, nor the motivations were correlated with job performance (-.07 < r < .02, all ns.). The
non-significant direct predictors of performance are not surprising given several previous studies
(Birkeland & Buch, 2015; Grant, 2008; Grant, Nurmohamed, Ashford, & Dekas, 2011). More
relevant for the current investigation, these correlations suggest that perseverance might have a
Multilevel Analyses
therefore evaluated the ICC1 for job performance to examine whether multilevel analyses were
warranted. The ICC exhibited a value of .58, highlighting the need to apply multilevel analyses
(Hox, 2010). For our multilevel analyses, we centered all variables before computing the
Table 2 shows the results of our multilevel analyses. Our hypothesis was that passion
attainment moderates the relationship between perseverance and job performance. As shown in
Model 2, the corresponding interaction effect was significant (estimate = .03, S.E. = .01, γ = .14,
following established guidelines (Dawson, 2014). When passion attainment was high (1 SD
above the mean), perseverance was positively related to job performance (B = .08, S.E. = .03, p
Running Head: THE DUAL PILLARS OF GRIT 13
< .001). However, when passion attainment was low (1 SD below the mean), the relationship was
not significant (B = -.02, S.E. = .03, p = .55). In addition, we computed regions of significance
(Preacher, Curran, & Bauer, 2006) that indicated a significant relationship between perseverance
and job performance for values of passion attainment above 5.27 (.09 SD above the mean).
Therefore, when employees attained desired levels of passion, higher levels of perseverance as
measured through the original grit scale were positively associated with supervisor-rated
performance. In contrast, when employees lacked the passion they desired, higher levels of
We subsequently tested whether our results remain significant when adding the control
variables. First, we added gender, age, and organizational tenure (Model 3). Next, we included
harmonious passion, prosocial, intrinsic, and extrinsic motivation (Model 4). Finally, we also
controlled for the interaction between perseverance and harmonious passion (Model 5). As Table
2 shows, the interaction effect remains significant in all models (Model 3: estimate = .03, S.E.
= .01, γ = .16, p = .01; Model 4: estimate = .03, S.E. = .01, γ = .16, p = .01; Model 5: estimate
= .04, S.E. = .01, γ = .19, p < .001). The values of the simple slopes also remained the same. In
Discussion
Prior research has found mixed evidence regarding the relationship between grit and
performance. In the current research, we proposed that this inconsistency has occurred because
even though grit’s definition includes perseverance and passion the grit measure solely reflects
perseverance and does not adequately capture passion. Instead, we suggested that a combination
In support of our hypothesis, a field study with employees at a technology company found
that the combination of perseverance and passion attainment, the former assessed through the
original measure of grit, predicted performance: perseverance was significantly related to higher
performance only when employees attained desired levels of passion. The current results thus
help resolve prior mixed evidence of grit’s relationship with performance. In adequately
measuring both perseverance and passion attainment, the current research uncovers grit’s
predictive power.
The current research extends the grit literature by more explicitly highlighting and
measuring the role of passion. Although the definition of grit includes the two components of
perseverance and passion, the measurement of grit has insufficiently captured passion. Instead,
extant research suggests that the traditional grit scale solely captures perseverance. Making an
explicit connection to the passion literature is important because grit researchers have
conceptually proposed that passion is essential for perseverance to have a positive effect on
performance (Duckworth et al., 2007; Duckworth, 2016). The interactive effect of perseverance
and passion attainment we find in the current research provides evidence that perseverance only
propels employees forward when they experience desired levels of passion, in part because
passion direct individuals toward the pursuit of important and meaningful goals (Cardon et al.,
2009; Chen et al., 2009; Perrewé et al., 2014; Vallerand et al., 2003). As a result, the current
study also addresses prior calls to examine the effects of grit outside of scholastic performance
The current research has limitations that provide opportunities for future research. The
study design was cross-sectional, which omits the possibility of investigating potential long-term
Running Head: THE DUAL PILLARS OF GRIT 15
effects of grit. In addition, the cross-sectional nature of this study does not allow us to address
concerns about reverse causality: it is possible that individuals who received lower performance
ratings also reported lower passion attainment, changing their attitudes as a response to negative
feedback regarding their competence (Wicklund & Brehm, 1968). This concern was addressed in
immediately prior to the survey, were only disclosed to employees after the survey data collection
was completed. Nonetheless, future research should further investigate the causal nature of the
cultural influences may have affected the results (Gelfand, Erez, & Aycan, 2007). We note,
however, that the grit scale has been applied all over the world, with remarkably consistent
results (Credé et al., 2017; Rimfeld et al., 2016). Future research should investigate whether the
Conclusion
Prior evidence regarding the relationship between grit and performance has been mixed.
The current results provide a conceptual window into these inconsistent results: although grit has
been defined in terms of both perseverance and passion, its measurement has focused on solely
on perseverance and neglected passion. In contrast, the current research demonstrates that
because perseverance and the experience of passion are crucial components of grit, only adequate
measurements of both combine to predict performance. The present research thus emphasizes the
need to more aptly incorporate research on passion in the theory and measurement of grit, which
subsequently enables the recognition of the predictive power of grit. Persevering without passion
attainment is mere drudgery, but perseverance with passion attainment propels individuals in
References
Abuhassàn, A., & Bates, T. C. (2015). Grit: Distinguishing effortful persistence from
relationship: The mediating role of organizational identification and moderating role of fit
Bagozzi, R. P., Yi, Y., & Phillips, L. W. (1991). Organizational Research. Administrative Science
Barrick, M. R., & Mount, M. K. (1991). The big five personality dimensions and job
Methods, 8, 274–289.
Becker, T. E., Atinc, G., Breaugh, J. A., Carlson, K. D., Edwards, J. R., & Spector, P. E. (2016).
Berger, L. A. & Berger, D. R. (Eds.) (2011). The talent management handbook: Creating a
sustainable competitive advantage by selecting, developing, and promoting the best people
Bowen, C.-C., Swim, J. K., & Jacobs, R. R. (2000). Evaluating Gender Biases on Actual Job
2194–2215.
Running Head: THE DUAL PILLARS OF GRIT 17
Birkeland, I. K., & Buch, R. (2015). The dualistic model of passion for work: Discriminate and
predictive validity with work engagement and workaholism. Motivation and Emotion,
39(3), 392–408.
Cardon, M. S., Wincent, J., Singh, J., & Drnovsek, M. (2009). The nature and experience of
Champoux, J., & Peters, W. (1987). Form, effect size, and power in moderated regression
Chang, W. (2014). Grit and academic performance: Is being grittier better? (unpublished doctoral
Chen, X. P., Yao, X., & Kotha, S. (2009). Entrepreneur passion and preparedness in business
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed). Hillsdale, N.J.:
L. Erlbaum Associates.
Collings, D. G., & Mellahi, K. (2009). Strategic talent management: A review and research
Credé, M., Tynan, M. C., & Harms, P. D. (2017). Much ado about grit: A meta-analytic synthesis
of the grit literature. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 113(3), 492–511.
Curran, P., West, S. G., & Finch, J. (1996). The robustness of test statistics to nonnormality and
Dawson, J. F. (2014). Moderation in Management Research: What, Why, When, and How.
DeNisi, A. S., & Sonesh, S. (2011). The Appraisal and Management of Performance at Work. In
S. Zedeck (Ed.), APA Handbook of Industrial and Organizational Psychology (pp. 255–
Duckworth, A., & Gross, J. J. (2014). Self-control and grit: Related but separable determinants of
Duckworth, A. L., Kirby, T. A., Tsukayama, E., Berstein, H., & Ericsson, K. A. (2011).
Deliberate practice spells success: Why grittier competitors triumph at the National
Duckworth, A. L., Peterson, C., Matthews, M. D., & Kelly, D. R. (2007). Grit: Perseverance and
passion for long-term goals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 1087–1101.
Duckworth, A. L., & Quinn, P. D. (2009). Development and validation of the short grit scale
Eskreis-Winkler, L., Duckworth, A. L., Shulman, E. P., & Beal, S. (2014). The grit effect:
Predicting retention in the military, the workplace, school and marriage. Frontiers in
Gelfand, M. J., Erez, M., & Aycan, Z. (2007). Cross-cultural organizational behavior. Annual
Grant, A. M. (2008). Does intrinsic motivation fuel the prosocial fire? Motivational synergy in
48–58.
Grant, A. M., Nurmohamed, S., Ashford, S. J., & Dekas, K. (2011). The performance
Grohman, M. G., Ivcevic, Z., Silvia, P., & Kaufman, S. B. (2017). The Role of Passion and
online publication.
Heidemeier, H., & Moser, K. (2009). Self–other agreement in job performance ratings: A meta-
Ho, V. T., Wong, S. S., & Lee, C. H. (2011). A tale of passion: Linking job passion and cognitive
Hox, J. J. (2010). Multilevel analysis: Techniques and applications (2nd ed). Quantitative
Ion, A., Mindu, A., & Gorbănescu, A. (2017). Grit in the workplace: Hype or ripe? Personality
Ivcevic, Z., & Brackett, M. (2014). Predicting school success: Comparing conscientiousness, grit,
Jachimowicz, J.M., To., C., Menges, J.I., & Akinola, M. (2017). Passion Gaps: Why People Quit
Jachimowicz, J.M., Wihler, A., & Galinsky, A.D. (2018). When the Passionate Succeed: Passion
For Work Increases Job Performance Only When Their Supervisors Attain Desired Levels
Judge, T. A., Erez, A., Bono, J. E., & Thoresen, C. J. (2002). Are measures of self-esteem,
Lewis, R. E., & Heckman, R. J. (2006). Talent management: A critical review. Human Resource
Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (2002). Building a Practically Useful Theory of Goal Setting and
Lucas, G. M., Gratch, J., Cheng, L., & Marsella, S. (2015). When the going gets tough: Grit
Morgeson, F. P., Campion, M. A., Dipboye, R. L., Hollenbeck, J. R., Murphy, K., & Schmitt, N.
Ng, T. W., Eby, L. T., Sorensen, K. L., & Feldman, D. C. (2005). Predictors of objective and
Ng, T. W., & Feldman, D. C. (2010). Organizational Tenure and Job Performance. Journal of
Ones, D. S., Dilchert, S., Viswesvaran, C., & Judge, T. A. (2007). In support of personality
Podsakoff, N. P., LePine, J. A., & LePine, M. A. (2007). Differential challenge stressor-
hindrance stressor relationships with job attitudes, turnover intentions, turnover, and
Perrewé, P. L., Hochwarter, W. A., Ferris, G. R., McAllister, C. P., & Harris, J. N. (2014).
Preacher, K. J., Curran, P. J., & Bauer, D. J. (2006). Computational tools for probing interaction
effects in multiple linear regression, multilevel modeling, and latent curve analysis. Journal
Rimfeld, K., Kovas, Y., Dale, P. S., & Plomin, R. (2016). True grit and genetics: Predicting
111, 780-789.
Robertson-Kraft, C., & Duckworth, A. L. (2014). True grit: Trait-level perseverance and passion
for long-term goals predicts effectiveness and retention among novice teachers. Teachers
169–215.
Smith, T. (2014, March 17). Does Teaching Kids To Get 'Gritty' Help Them Get Ahead? NPR.
Vallerand, R. J., Blanchard, C., Mageau, G. A., Koestner, R., Ratelle, C., Léonard, M., ... &
Marsolais, J. (2003). Les passions de l’aime: on obsessive and harmonious passion. Journal
Vinchur, A. J., Schippmann, J. S., Switzer, F. S., & Roth, P. L. (1998). A meta-analytic review of
predictors of job performance for salespeople. Journal of Applied Psychology, 83, 586-597.
Von Culin, K. R., Tsukayama, E., & Duckworth, A. L. (2014). Unpacking grit: Motivational
correlates of perseverance and passion for long-term goals. The Journal of Positive
Wicklund, R. A., & Brehm, J. W. (1968). Attitude change as a function of felt competence and
Wilson, T. D., Lisle, D. J., Kraft, D., & Wetzel, C. G. (1989). Preferences as expectation-driven
Zigarmi, D., Nimon, K., Houson, D., Witt, D., & Diehl, J. (2009). Beyond engagement: Toward a
framework and operational definition for employee work passion. Human Resource
M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10
1 Gender 1.40 0.49
2 Age 30.96 6.99 -.12*
3 Organizational Tenure (in years) 3.69 2.88 -.15** .53**
4 Prosocial Motivation 6.55 0.76 -.04 .10* .05 (.85)
5 Intrinsic Motivation 5.73 1.28 -.07 .15** -.02 .37** (.88)
6 Extrinsic Motivation 3.88 1.69 .01 -.02 .03 -.04 -.19** (.83)
7 Harmonious Passion 5.49 1.28 -.09 .13** .03 .28** .69** -.26** (.90)
8 Perseverance 5.62 0.89 .06 .22** .11* .24** .28** -.27** .32** (.73)
9 Passion Attainment 5.12 1.69 -.00 .13** .00 .16** .54** -.38** .56** .39** (.82)
10 Job Performance (supervisor-rated) 4.39 0.52 .01 -.13** -.16** -.01 -.00 -.07 .02 .02 -.03 (.98)
Note. Nemployee = 422, Nsupervisor = 85, Gender (1 = male, 2 = female); *p < .05; **p < .01.
Table 2