Diff Field Notes & MEMOs
Diff Field Notes & MEMOs
Nursing Research
Volume 29 Number 1
February 2007 65-79
In this article the authors expose some of the mystique surrounding field
notes and theoretical memos in a Glaserian grounded theory study.
Definitions, types, and content of field notes and theoretical memos are pre-
sented. Exemplars from a study of mothers living with serious mental illness
are provided to illustrate how these forms of documentation evolved during
the course of the study. The authors argue that, although the processes of field
noting and memoing may blur as a study progresses, they nevertheless retain
their independent functions. The authors contend that without understanding
the complementary function of these two types of documentation, data can-
not evolve to a higher interpretive level. This article contributes specific ideas
for improving the methods used by qualitative nurse researchers.
Authors’ Note: Thank you to Dr. Nancy Edwards’s 5th annual Multiple Intervention Program
Summer Research Interns for their constructive feedback regarding an initial draft of this article.
65
Field Notes
Field notes are commonly defined as written records of observational
data produced by fieldwork (Hammersley & Atkinson, 2002; Jackson,
1990). As a data-gathering strategy often used by ethnographers, field notes
consist of descriptions of social interactions and the context in which they
occurred (Roper & Shapira, 2000). They represent “the process of transfor-
mation of observed interaction to written public communication” (Jackson,
1990, pp. 6-7). In a survey of ethnographers regarding field notes, Jackson
found no standard definition of precisely what constitutes a field note.
Rather, ethnographers expressed strong feelings associated with field notes.
January, Brooke
In response to my clarification regarding her efforts to “protect” her children,
Brooke interrupts with an assertive response, “Definitely.” Then, she
becomes tearful and looks away. In a lower tone, she adds that her children
are “normal” with a qualifier “to the extent that normal can be right now.”
Illness “creates chaos.” There is a pause. I sense discomfort for both of us.
Brooke then shifts the flow of the conversation using terms such as “fun
loving,” “very nurturing,” and “well-balanced” to describe her children.
Theoretical Memos
Memos, on the other hand, are records of the researcher’s developing
ideas about codes and their interconnections (Glaser, 1998). Memos are a
documentation of the researcher’s thinking processes rather than a descrip-
tion of a social context. By theorizing from the data, memos transform
field-note descriptions into theoretical accounts. In 1978, Glaser identified
four goals and 12 rules outlining the pragmatics of memoing. Subsequently,
he critiqued the formality of this structure stating that its rigidity impeded
conceptualization (Glaser, 1998).
According to Schreiber (2001), there are three levels of coding in
grounded theory. They involve moving from: first-level coding, the use of
participants’ words resulting from line-by-line analysis; to second-level
coding, categorizing of first-level codes; to third-level coding, the selection
Field Notes
A number of field-note typologies have been presented in the literature
(Clifford, 1990; LeCompte & Preissle, 1993; Lofland & Lofland, 1999;
Table 1
Guidelines for Field Note Taking
Guideline In Practical Terms
Source: Modified from LeCompte and Preissle (1993) and Lofland and Lofland (1999).
the mothers’ perspectives and the process of grounded theory research. The
resultant study field notes portrayed a mixture of individualistic, distinct per-
ceptions derived from being present with a mother in a moment of time.
LeCompte and Preissle (1993) suggested that instead of being overly
concerned about the type of field note, pioneering a mixture of note taking
that best preserves the researcher’s observations and impressions may be
more useful. Similarly, Hammersley and Atkinson (2002) stated:
Like most aspects of intellectual craft, some care and attention to detail are
prerequisites: satisfactory note-taking needs to be worked at. It is a skill
demanding repeated assessment of purposes and priorities, and of the costs
and benefits of different strategies. (pp. 175-176)
Theoretical Memos
Glaser (1992, 1998) did not support types of memos. He suggested that the
adoption of a typology of memos such as developed by Strauss and Corbin
(1990; code notes, theoretical notes, operational notes, diagrams, logical dia-
grams, and integrative diagrams) limits the abstraction of data beyond the
descriptive level (Glaser, 1992). For Glaser (1998), a theoretical memo simply
captures the “meaning and ideas for one’s growing theory at the moment they
occur” (p. 178). Although the style of Glaserian theoretical documentation
may vary from a few words to a “ten page conceptual paper” (Glaser, 1998),
memos essentially reflect the researcher’s conceptual speculations.
The following three theoretical memos are included to illustrate the
researcher’s thoughts about the first-level code hide. Although this was a
recurring term used by some participants, it was initially overlooked by the
researcher. Attending to Glaser’s (1992, 1998) emphasis on the process,
rather than the form of memos, increased the researcher’s sensitivity to the
meaning “behind the words” as concurrent data collection and analysis
occurred. The following three memos show progressively more depth to
understanding the code hide. For example, in the February memo, Brook
hides illness away from others:
mother, Brooke lets “no one see” behind her mask. Brooke wants her
children to witness ideals that she hopes “they will follow.” She fears that if
she lives in illness, life for her children will be framed by illness.
able to sustain closeness, but only to a point as illness often disabled their
mothering abilities.
To further develop a conceptual understanding of what was happening
for this group of mothers, the ideation of the June memo about invisibility
was considered in view of other first-level codes, hitting bottom and falling.
Integrating conceptual ideas yielded the following memo tentatively
labelled as visibility as a result of reconstructing the theoretical story:
This latter memo demonstrates that additional memos are constructed on other
memos. These theoretically “more mature” memos are not directly sparked by
data appearing in a field note. Instead, memos of “momentary ideation” are
assembled into conceptual relationships toward developing a framework. As
Miles and Huberman (1994) suggested, the standardization of memo types,
and even formats, risks impeding a researcher’s ability to creatively sort indi-
vidual ideas for the purpose of developing a higher theoretical explanation.
Field Notes
The information in Table 1 guided the field noting in the above study.
This information may appear unsophisticated. Nevertheless, at the time
of the current study, it was useful as a broad guide for note taking.
Furthermore, the guidelines are not meant to be dogmatic, as there is no one
right way to do field noting (Patton, 1990). Researchers must have a clear
field-note-taking approach that best suits their area of inquiry. For example,
in the current grounded theory study a decision was made not to jot notes
in the presence of the mother recognizing that this might disrupt the telling
of their experiences. Instead, immediately after data gathering, data record-
ing was initiated by jotting down a list of observations such as the process,
the context, and the features of the field. These observations then became a
source for further descriptive elaboration.
The following field-note excerpt illustrates data from an initial entry.
Some contextual, behavioral, and process features of the researcher’s inter-
view with Sally are described. In particular, a methodological issue relating
to the data-gathering process is identified in the third paragraph. This field
note contains more low- rather than high-inference descriptors (LeCompte
& Preissle, 1993). It includes concrete descriptions of her behavior (“her
speech is thick”) rather than an interpretation of the observations:
January, Sally
Sally was identified as a potential participant by a nurse manager in an inpa-
tient unit. The nurse informed me that I “probably know of her” since she is
often readmitted to hospital. Sally has been an inpatient for more than three
weeks and is now being “prepared for discharge.” After introducing myself
and before I can explain the study to her, Sally begins sharing her experiences
as a mother. I made some mental notes while attempting to focus on the eth-
ical process. Once she signs the consent, she invites me to return so that “she
can help me” understand. When I returned, she informs me that she has just
rested as her medications make her “so tired.” This becomes evident toward
the end of this 50-minute interview.
I invite her to begin where she is most comfortable. She responds, “Ask
me.” I pose an opening question and she partially repeats it and then pauses.
Her next response of five minutes is about “a disciplined self.” Her speech is
thick, her words are slurred, and she often takes small drinks during the inter-
view. She has the movements of a person receiving antipsychotics. Sally
requires time to verbalize her thoughts. She responds in concrete terms to my
statements. She can focus and at times becomes tearful in reference to her
oldest daughter. Her tone softens in reference to her children.
Methodologically speaking, early in this initial interview I sensed that the
guiding questions did not allow for comfortable dialogue. The question and
answer format is too rigid. Our interaction seemed to change when I move
away from a structural to a conversational style of interviewing. Using
Sally’s language, inviting her to share examples, lots of pausing, and seeking
clarification are skills that encouraged her to talk more spontaneously.
As illustrated in this field note, the scope of initial field notes addressed as
many aspects of the encounter as the researcher could recall. In retrospect,
January, Sally
By way of initial impressions, mothering involves a “disciplined self” in order
to create a “bond” with her children that is “so strong” it is unbreakable by
others or circumstances such as illness and limited resources. The “bond” is a
symbol of motherhood. In the presence of being “ill, ill, ill” this bond is “tested”
especially since her oldest has not seen her for several years. “No one knows
[why the daughter doesn’t visit] and I even asked.” Much of her time outside of
hospital is directed at “creating a space” for her and her children. Despite the
increasing severity of illness, Sally tries to “stay close as much as I can.”
July, Nancy
Nancy was identified by a primary health care worker as being “stable and
receptive” to participating in the study. The interview was conducted in a small,
windowless interview room within an inpatient setting. Prior to beginning the
interview, Nancy reports she was resting. Within the first five minutes of the
interview, she becomes tearful and anxious. I suggest we rearrange for another
time, but she refuses. Within the initial twenty minutes, she spontaneously
shares: the “pain” of illness and treatment, her desire to be with her children,
her inability to be with her children because of illness, a fear and uncertainty
of the “unknown,” and the “blue haze.” The interview ends shortly thereafter.
The “haze” suggests the “unreal”, “unpredictable” and “unbearable” suf-
fering of mothering in illness. To hide her symptoms, Nancy, like other moth-
ers in the study, withdraws her presence by using substances, considering
suicide, making excuses to spend less time in the home, pretending, and keep-
ing secrets. Nancy’s use of the term “battle” seems fitting in that for her defen-
sive manoeuvres are necessary to endure—“afraid about my life.” For Nancy,
she moved [consciously or unconsciously?] deeper into the “haze” associated
with overwhelming illness. The further Nancy travels into the “haze,” the less
transparent [hence its shade of “blue”] it becomes. With time in illness, unfor-
tunately, her suffering and turmoil increases. Like other mothers, Nancy per-
ceives the need to continually balance attending to her responsibilities as a
mother and removing self to control illness and suffering. By removal of self-
as-mother she is not able to “keep close” to her children. Again, chaos inter-
feres with a mother’s ability to “see” and to be “watchful” of their children
[from field notes about mothers who “watch”] as well as threatening others.
Theoretical Memos
Glaser (1992, 1998) advocated for the freedom to document “anything”
in memos. He contended (1998) that the content of memos is an “ideational
and conceptual production that come[s] to mind during coding, collection,
analysing, and theoretically sampling” (p. 180). Ultimately, memos on
memos sort into a conceptual framework. Unlike field notes that form the
basis for the construction of memos, memos play a key role in the devel-
opment of the theory.
As mentioned earlier, the aim of the current study was to explain the main
concern as perceived by a group of mothers with SMI. To this end, the con-
tent of the memos represented the researcher’s dialogue with the data. Such
dialogue was prompted by asking Glaserian-type (Glaser, 1978, 1992) ques-
tions such as: “What is happening within these lines of data?” “What under-
pins the mothers’ action/event?” “How does this code label relate to an
earlier one?” “How does this memo compare with that memo?” In actuality,
memos reflect a spectrum of theoretical clarity about mothering and illness.
At the outset of the current study, field noting and theoretical memoing
were understood as two parallel processes. As the study progressed, the
inclusion of interpretative aspects of observations was at times difficult to
distinguish from theoretical development within the memos. Indeed, with
the unanticipated expansion of the field notes to include interpretative and
descriptive data, the two documentation processes seemed to blur. This, in
part, may be attributed to the earlier discussion about the variety of perspec-
tives concerning typology of field notes. For example, LeCompte and
Preissle (1993) equate high-inference content of field notes to Glaser’s
memos, and what Sanjek (1990) referred to as field reports. Nevertheless, as
Glaser (1998) contended, although field notes evolve into illustrations of
particular categories conceptualized in memos, only memos are annotates of
conceptual ideas; that is, even though portions of the content of field notes
may include an interpretative abstraction of a phenomenon, they remain as
an element of data from which memos are created. Furthermore, memos
evolve beyond the level of interpretation reflected in individual field notes.
Memos, in contrast, focus on ideas that by fit, relevance and workability,
find their way into the emerging theoretical explanation (Glaser, 1998).
Conclusion
References
Bachrach, L. L. (1985). Chronic mentally ill women: Emergence and legitimisation of
program issues. Hospital and Community Psychiatry, 36(10), 1963-1969.
Clifford, J. (1990). Notes on (field) notes. In R. Sanjek (Ed.), Fieldnotes: The makings of
anthropology (pp. 47-70). Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.
Eastlick Kushner, K., & Morrow, R. (2003). Grounded theory, feminist theory, critical theory:
Toward theoretical triangulation. Advances in Nursing Science, 26(1), 30-43.
Emerson, R. M., Fretz, R. I., & Shaw, L. L. (1995). Writing ethnographic fieldnotes. Chicago:
University of Chicago Press.
Glaser, B. G. (1978). Theoretical sensitivity: Advances in the methodology of grounded
theory. Mill Valley, CA: Sociology Press.
Glaser, B. G. (1992). Basics of grounded theory analysis: Emergence vs. forcing. Mill Valley,
CA: Sociology Press.
Glaser, B. G. (1998). Doing grounded theory: Issues and discussion. Mill Valley, CA:
Sociology Press.
Hammersley, M., & Atkinson, P. (2002). Ethnography: Principles in practice (2nd ed.).
London: Routledge.
Hutchinson, S., & Wilson, H. S. (2001). Grounded theory: The method. In P. L. Munhall (Ed.),
Nursing research: A qualitative perspective (3rd ed., pp. 209-244). Boston: Jones and Bartlett.
Jackson, J. E. (1990). “I am a fieldnote”: Fieldnotes as a symbol of professional identity. In
R. Sanjek (Ed.), Fieldnotes: The makings of anthropology (pp. 3-33). Ithaca, NY: Cornell
University Press.
LeCompte, M. D., & Preissle, J. (1993). Ethnography and qualitative design in educational
research (2nd ed.). London: Academic Press Limited.
Lofland, J., & Lofland, L. H. (1999). Data logging in observation: Fieldnotes. In A. Bryman
& R. G. Burgess (Eds.), Qualitative research: Vol. 3 (pp. 3-12). London: Sage.
Miles, M. B., & Huberman, A. M. (1994). Qualitative data analysis: An expanded sourcebook
(2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Montgomery, P., Tompkins, C., Forchuk, C., & French, S. (2006). Keeping close: Mothering
with serious mental illness. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 54(1), 20-28.
Morse, J. M., & Richards, L. (2002). Read me first for a user’s guide to qualitative methods.
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Ottenberg, S. (1990). Thirty years of fieldnotes: Changing relationships to the text. In
R. Sanjek (Ed.), Fieldnotes: The makings of anthropology (pp. 139-160). Ithaca, NY:
Cornell University Press.
Patton, M. Q. (1990). Qualitative evaluation and research methods. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Robinson, Z. (2003). Exploring the audit trail for qualitative investigators. Nurse Educator,
28(4), 175-178.
Rogers, B. L., & Cowles, K. V. (1993). The qualitative research audit trail: A complex collec-
tion of documentation. Research in Nursing and Health, 16, 219-226.
Roper, J. M., & Shapira, J. (2000). Ethnography in nursing research: Methods in nursing
research. London: Sage.
Sandelowski, M. (1998). Writing a good read: Strategies for re-presenting qualitative data.
Research in Nursing and Health, 21, 375-382.
Sanjek, R. (1990). A vocabulary for fieldnotes. In R. Sanjek (Ed.), Fieldnotes: The makings of
anthropology (pp. 92-121). Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.
Schreiber, R. S. (2001). The “how to” of grounded theory: Avoiding the pitfalls. In
R. S. Schreiber & P. Noerager Stern (Eds.), Using grounded theory in nursing (pp. 55-83).
New York: Springer.
Schreiber, R. S., & Noerager Stern, P. (Eds.). (2001). Using grounded theory in nursing. New
York: Springer.
Strauss, A., & Corbin, J. (1990). Basics of qualitative research: Grounded theory procedures
and techniques. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.