Chapter 2
Chapter 2
Review of Literature
“The prefix “dis” denotes separation, negation or reversal; the core reference is
“apart,” the simplest equivalent is not. The prefix signifies a contrast with
whatever the suffix may be. Words containing “dis” as a prefix, thus are very
“disfluencies”
The prefix “Dys” refers to something that is abnormal. This prefix has been used
13
stuttering” (Wingate, 1984).
instances of “disfluency.”
exist between the disfluency of those who stutter and those who are considered
normal speakers. This would depend on the definition of abnormal fluency and
pivotal role in his diagnosogenic theory of stuttering. His theory stated that
certain parents or other adults are unusually anxious or perfectionistic about the
14
child‟s speech fluency. Such adults tend to react to the child‟s normal
avoidance reactions through anxiety about the speech hesitancy. Before its
cases according to this theory, it does not differ markedly in amount or quality
from that of other children who do not come to be regarded by their parents as
evaluations placed upon a child‟s fluency. The avoidance theory says that if one
child‟s speech repetitions are normal, there is little justification for calling
another child‟s speech repetitions anything else just because there are more of
them. An important implication of this theory was that a relatively large amount
stutterers tended to describe the same disfluencies in their children‟s speech, this
children are not categorically different. This differed from Johnson‟s advocation
According to this view, Bloodstein (1960a, 1970, 1975) discussed that mild
failures that are severe and chronic, they tend to be identified as episodes of
stuttering. This view emphasizes that there is an overlap between stuttering and
15
normal nonfluency and that “there are no “stutter-like” disfluencies that are not
that stuttering may be normal disfluency that has increased in frequency due to
continuity hypothesis says that if one child‟s speech repetitions are stuttering,
there is little reason to call another child‟s repetitions anything else merely
The two hypotheses have a vast distinction between them in their ultimate
implications for theory and therapy. First of all, the source of the child‟s problem
the child comes to regard disfluencies as a matter for concern and to struggle to
avoid them. Second, the causes of the problem lies largely in the nature of the
stuttering that are latent in almost all children‟s speech and in the variety of
One of the pioneering studies on disfluencies seen in children with stuttering and
their normally fluent counterparts comes from the research carried out by
Johnson and Associates (1959). In the third phase of their study, the authors
group of 2.5 to 8 years and an equal number of genders and age matched normal
speaking children. The data were analyzed separately for the two genders and for
16
suggested that their data may be used as normative reference in evaluating
sex, age and socioeconomic groups. They emphasize that “nonfluencies occur in
the speech of children generally, and that their distribution is dimensional rather
subjects, were sound and syllable repetitions, word repetitions and interjections.
In, females of the experimental group, although findings were similar, greater
found. Slightly greater than three fourths of nonfluencies of both male and
female subjects in the control group were interjections, revisions and word
repetitions, with sound and syllable repetitions occurring with much lesser
frequency.
Group differences revealed significant difference (p< 0.01) for the frequency
index measures of male experimental and control groups for types such as
sounds and for all the categories combined and broken words (p< 0.05). In all
these, means were higher in the experimental group compared to the control
for sound and syllable repetitions, word repetitions and all categories combined
17
(p< 0.01) and prolongations (p< 0.05). Again in all these experimental group
means were higher than the control group. No significant differences were
broken words.
Although Johnson‟s study has provided very useful and relevant information, it is
criticized on the fact that (1) there are large differences between two and eight
year old children in almost all areas of development which makes analysis of
group data from children in this age range invalid and (2) the prolonged interval
from onset reported for the stuttering in their study (approx. 18 months) makes it
hard to consider the features of their disfluent speech to be that of early stuttering
(Zebrowski, 1995). Hence in later studies (e.g., Yairi and Lewis, 1984, Yairi and
age range and within a small interval from the reported stuttering onset.
Yairi (1981) studied the spontaneous speech outputs of 33 children, 18 girls and
15 boys ranging between 24 and 33 months. Speech samples (of 500 words
and classified as given by Johnson (1961) and Williams, Silverman and Kools
words and poly syllable words. The frequencies of occurrence of each disfluency
type and of the total number of disfluencies were tallied to provide individual
and group data. The author found that the total number of disfluencies varied
widely across subjects, from 0 to 25.6 disfluencies per 100 words spoken. Four
18
interjection and revision were observed. The standard deviations were large,
exceeding group mean values in several instances, which indicate that 2 year
olds are extremely heterogeneous in respect to disfluency. Also, boys were found
to be more disfluent than the girls. The largest differences occurred in part word
repetitions and interjections. However, using two-tailed t tests (p= .05; df=31) no
types or in the total number of disfluencies. The author also studied the
frequency ranges. The data showed that although all types of disfluency were
represented in the speech of 2 year olds, not all the subjects uttered all the types.
Over 80% of the subjects did not exhibit even one instance of polysyllabic word
total words which they repeated exceeded the percentage of single-syllable words
which they uttered in their speech samples. The authors made three important
observations. First, more than half of the subjects exhibited two types of
percentage of subjects did not use revisions in their speech. Third, higher
addition to the frequency, the author also examined the extent of each instance of
disfluency, i.e., how many times did subjects repeat a particular disfluency?
Repetition units were defined as the number of utterances in excess of the first
the number also included the first production. It was found that although there
19
part-word, word or phrase repetition were short, consisting of the original
occurred more often on single syllable words, interjections were also almost
Yairi (1982) analyzed spontaneous verbal output of 500 words, of 33, 2 year old
children (18 girls and 15 boys). The subjects were divided into two groups.
Subgroup II comprised of the older children (10 boys and 10 girls), in the age
between 29 to 33 months. Four spontaneous speech samples were taken for each
month interval. The classification of disfluencies used by Yairi (1981) was used
in this study. Data was calculated to compute frequency of disfluency per 100
words. Since no significant gender difference was reported in the previous study,
data for boys and girls were combined. Results showed that for the entire group,
there was a reduction in disfluency, throughout the 8- month span during which
three speech samples were recorded. The average total number of disfluencies,
declined from 6.5 disfluencies per 100 words at the beginning of the study, (29
months) to 5.10 (33 months) to 4.1 (37 months). Subgroup I revealed a general
trend of increase in the number of disfluencies from the beginning to the end of
the third year of their life. However, the subjects in Subgroup II showed a sharp
that the number of disfluencies at the beginning of third year, is low, but
gradually increases with time. The normal disfluency peaks in the latter part of
20
the year. Data also showed that increase in disfluency for Subgroup I was mainly
due to two disfluencies – Revision and Phrase Repetition. In subgroup II, there
was consistent decline across all disfluency types but noticed more for Part word
repetitions and Interjections. Hence “taken together, the overall picture obtained
from these observations, is that the year between the age 2 and 3 is a volatile
weeks may occur in some children, which may be regarded as parents or other
observers as stuttering (Yairi, 1982). The author concluded that two year old
The above studies included English speaking children in the age range of 24 to
non stuttering males aged 2, 4 and 6 yrs (12 in each age group) (types of
disfluency and relationships among the different types). Speech samples of 100
than one disfluency on the same word or consecutive words or both, and
measured as percent for 100 words. Within each age group the frequency of
21
individual disfluency types were compared. The authors reported that the two
most commonly occurring disfluency types at each age levels were revision-
repetitions and word repetitions. The least occurring type was part-word
phrase repetitions, and word repetitions occurred least frequently for 4 and 6 year
olds. Further, a comparison of the disfluency types was made across the different
age groups. The results indicated that 2 year olds had significantly higher
differences between the age groups were not statistically significant (supporting
studies by Wexler, 1978, 1982). Also, high variability between subjects was
noticed in 2 year old children compared to the older age groups on most
measures. The authors concluded that (1) certain disfluency types occur more
frequently than others within each age group, (2) Patterns of disfluency types had
almost similar distribution across these ages, except for greater frequencies for
certain types in the speech of 2 year olds and (3) Significant differences were
changes in disfluency.
22
incomplete phrase and word repetitions and between phrase repetitions and
and phrase repetitions, between tense pause and word repetitions, and between
phonations, between part word repetitions and phrase repetitions, although part
word repetitions correlated with word repetitions and word repetitions with
phrase repetitions were also noted. The authors concluded that “there is a range
disfluency types exist inviting speculation about different sources for the
with certain disfluency types, as opposed to a motor factor in other types. The
interjections and between RIPs and word repetitions tend to support the linguistic
hypothesis. The relatively lower disfluency frequencies observed for the types
that may appear, to have a motor component, i.e., part-word repetitions and
disrhythmic phonations, especially in the speech of the youngest age group, tend
DeJoy and Gregory (1985) identified and studied nine types of disfluency, in 60
non stuttering males aged 3.5 and 5 years of age. The speech samples were
transcribed verbatim and the following disfluency types were identified (1) part
word repetitions, (2) word repetitions, (3) phrase repetitions, (4) revision, (5)
23
Silverman and Kools, 1968). Two types of pauses grammatical and
number of occurrences per 100 words. The results indicated that certain
disfluency types that discriminated significantly between the older and younger
children were part word repetitions and disrhythmic phonations, which the
Mysak, 1982). These two disfluency types correlated significantly in the older
group, suggesting that they vary independently in younger children but together
may reflect a less mature speech motor system in the later preschool years. They
also found that interjections and ungrammatical pauses did not decline
significantly across the ages, suggesting that these are disfluencies that may
Disfluencies in 10 male and 10 female black first grade children (5.6 to 7.3
years) was examined by Brutten and Miller (1988). They found that frequencies
of disfluencies per 100 words ranged from 2.74 to 0.365. Interjections and
broken words were the most and the least frequently seen disfluencies. Whole
word repetitions were more than both part-word and phrase repetitions. The
main effect for sex was not found to be statistically significant. Girls evidenced
overall amount of disfluency was quite similar. Whole word repetitions and
24
revisions were noticeably seen more in girls than boys. The interaction between
sex and type of disfluency was not statistically significant. The authors imply
that their findings may put to rest the belief that greater disfluency among boys is
The above major studies were carried out to provide information about
norms, that can be used in refining and validating differential diagnostic schemes
Yairi and Lewis (1984) collected speech samples from 10 2-year old and 10 3-
year old children, (5 boys and 5 girls) with stuttering and 10 matched normally
approximately 500 syllables long. They found that overall stutterers were three
and a half times more disfluent than the control subjects. (M=21.54 for
stutterers, versus, M= 6.16 for the non stuttering group). According to them, in
spite of large variability within each group, stutterers and non stutterers could be
that normally speaking 2 and 3 year olds, exhibited almost all types of
disfluencies. Mostly they exhibited interjections, revisions and single unit part
25
the stuttering group. This increase was most significant for part word repetition
and disrhythmic phonation. Also, stutterers repeated part words twice or more
per instance, while non stutterers rarely repeated a segment more than once.
subjects were stutterers and 12 were non stutterers. The three sessions included
each child interacting with his own mother, an unfamiliar mother of a stutterer,
were transcribed from the videotaped play periods. Nonfluent words were
and tense pauses. Normal disfluency types included whole-word and phrase
given type) by the total number of words. Results of the study showed that,
children. The most common stuttering type of disfluency for the stutterers was
3%), tense pauses (M = 1.6%, SD = l.0 %), and broken words (M = 0.l%, SD =
0.3%) occurred less frequently. Nonstuttering children had few part word-
26
repetitions (M = 0.2%, SD = 0.4%) and emitted no prolongations, tense pauses,
or broken words. In terms of the normal disfluency types, the nonstutterers had
(1.1%) did not differ in the emission of other normal types of disfluency (i.e.,
phrase repetitions, incomplete phrases, and intejections). The author also found
that there were no differences between the children interacting with the different
mothers on the speech and fluency measures. Meyers concluded that in terms of
stuttering behaviours, her results support the hypothesis that stuttering children
consistent for both groups of children in all the three sessions except for two
significant variability across sessions. This was the first report on stuttering type
age-4 yrs). The time since onset of stuttering, was 1 year or less as reported by
the mothers. They found stuttering children to be more disfluent than their non
stuttering peers. The average frequency of all speech disfluency (M=13%), per
100 words, was significantly higher than that of non stuttering children. (M=5%).
27
was 556 ms and 520 ms respectively. The mean duration of sound
prolongations of stutterers and non stutterers was 435 ms and 404 ms. Also,
stuttering children averaged 2.4 and 2.1 repeated units per instance of
repetition. In word repetitions, it was 2.3 and 2.2 for stuttering and non
the two groups in either the average duration of sound/ syllable repetitions and
The above studies have several limitations. The number of children included was
small, children were within wide age ranges and the size of the speech samples
was small. To explain, Johnson and associates used a wide age range from 2 to 8
years, Yairi (1981, 1982) used 33 children, Wexler and Mysak (1982) used only
12 children in the age group of 2, 4 and 6 years and De Joy and Gregory (1985)
studied children in the range 3.5 to 5 years. Wexler and Mysak used only 100
utterances while Yairi and Lewis used only 500 syllables for disfluency analysis.
Meyers (1986) used samples of only 10 min length. These diversities among
studies prompted Ambrose and Yairi (1999) to provide a normative reference for
early stuttering which would provide a basis for the differential diagnosis of
stuttering from normal disfluency. They also attempted to discuss the possible
children. Speech samples were recorded from 90 children with stuttering in the
age range of two to five years within six months of stuttering onset and from 54
28
age matched normally fluent children. They used a six category classification
system for the disfluencies. These included (1) part word repetitions, (2) single
abandoned utterances and (6) multisyllable or phrase repetitions. The first three
types were combined to form Stuttering like Disfluencies (SLD) and the second
three categories formed Other Disfluencies (OD). For part word and single
syllable word repetitions, if a segment was repeated more number of times, the
number of extra times it was repeated was computed as Repetition Units (RU).
The authors found that the main difference between the groups was determined
by the proportion of SLD. These are thus the types of fluency disruptions that are
most typical in persons with stuttering. For the group with stuttering, SLD
constituted 66% of the total disfluency whereas in the normal group it was 34%.
Other disfluencies constituted 34% of the total disfluency for the stuttering group
and 76% for the control group. Part word repetitions occurred with maximum
frequency in the stuttering group while in the control group, interjections and
control groups were found. Also, no statistically significant age difference was
found for both the groups. However, part word repetitions and repetition units
Ambrose and Yairi (1999) also developed a weighted measure for SLD which
was the weighted sum of part and one syllable word repetitions and disrhythmic
29
phonation per 100 syllables (repetitive disfluencies * mean number of iterations
+ disrhythmic phonations * 2). Using this measure, with a cut off of 4%, all
children were correctly assigned to their respective fluency groups i.e., weighted
SLD scores were found to be continuous across age groups and lie below 4.00
for all the control group and above 4.00 for the experimental group.
stuttering and the remaining 36 were age and gender matched normal speakers.
They measured SLD, OD, total disfluencies (TD) and the number of repetition
units (RU). They also calculated the weighted SLD measure as described by
between the groups for stuttering like disfluencies and total disfluencies. 81%
and 42% of the total disfluencies comprised of SLDs in the experimental and
the controls and 19 % in the group with stuttering. For the weighted SLD
measure, results showed significant overall difference between the two groups.
The mean weighted SLD measure was 20.1 and 1.2 in children with stuttering
and normals, respectively. Hence in this study, a weighted SLD value of 4% and
stuttering or normal.
All the above studies are in English. However, stuttering is a fluency disorder
observed across languages and cultures (Bloodstein, 1995, Cooper & Cooper,
1998; Shapiro, 1999, Van Borsel, Maes & Foulon, 2001; Van Riper, 1971). One
30
linguistic and cultural groups. In this context, Carlo and Watson (2003) studied
children from Puerto Rico, The total frequencies and types of speech disfluencies
were examined in 15 children (8 girls and 7 boys) aged 3.5 to 4 years and 17
children (8 girls and 9 boys) aged 5.0 to 5.5 years. The study examined the
influence of gender and age on the speech disfluencies, the amount and type of
and comparing the results of their study with those using English speaking
children. Spontaneous speech samples were elicited during a free play interactive
activity which yielded a sample size between 500 and 800 syllables. The first 50
classification systems described by De Joy and Gregory (1985), Yairi (1981) and
Campbell and Hill (1987). For the inter observer reliability, another qualified
deviations, and ranges of total disfluencies per 100 syllables were computed for
boys and girls in the younger and older groups. Findings suggested that means
of total disfluencies ranged from 5.21 for the younger boys to 8.26 for the older
girls. With age and gender groups combined, the frequency range of total speech
girls or by the younger and older age groups. Interaction effect between age
and gender was also not seen. This differed from the earlier findings of English
chronological age (DeJoy & Gregory, 1985; Wexler, 1982; Wexler and Mysak,
1982; Yairi, 1997). The absence of gender effect was in congruence with earlier
31
reports of English speaking children (Ambrose and Yairi, 1999; Haynes and
Hood, 1977; Kools and Berryman, 1971; Yairi, 1981, 1982; Yairi and Lewis,
1984). High amounts of variability were also observed in the types of speech
proportions of disfluency types exhibited by the two age groups. Rank orders of
the disfluency types revealed that for both groups, single-syllable word
repetitions, revisions, and interjections were the most frequent disfluency types.
Broken words, blocks and repetitions of more than one syllable were less
frequent types. They also did not find any significant difference in frequency of
total speech disfluencies between boys and girls. The authors thus suggest a
possibility that the relationship between age and disfluency may be different for
gender and age matched comparison children who do not stutter. They wanted to
describe how German-speaking preschool children who stutter and do not stutter
explain how powerful classification measures for the diagnosis of stuttering are
native English and German speaking children close to the onset of stuttering
differ. Speech samples were both audio and video recorded. Speech samples
32
was added. To avoid warming up effects, at the beginning of the session, the first
100 syllables of each speech sample were excluded from disfuency analysis.
sounds, syllables and one syllable words were defined and grouped as Stuttering-
combined as other disfluencies (OD). For repetitive disfluency types, the number
of iterations were coded and counted separately. For analysis, a syllable based
metric was used. Percentages for each disfluency type and the number of
iterations were calculated for each participant and were used to derive group
means. Wilcoxon signed rank tests revealed that differences between mean
Disfluency data showed that CWS and CWNS showed all types of SLD. All
types of SLD, as well as grouped SLD, were shown more frequently by CWS
than by CWNS and this was statistically significant (p<0.001). For OD, however,
there were no differences between the groups, with the exception of multi-
syllable word repetitions, which were shown more frequently by CWS than by
number than CWNS in SLD repetitive disfluencies (sound, syllable and one
syllable repetitions) with p-values < .009. OD repetitive disfluencies (phrase and
multi syllable word repetitions) did not differ significantly in the number of
iterations (p> .28) between CWS and CWNS. A weighted SLD measure was
additionally calculated according to Ambrose and Yairi (1999). With a cut off
33
of 4% of the weighted SLD measure, three CWS are below the cut off and no
CWNS above.
Their results were consistent with reports for English speaking children, of this
age group (Ambrose & Yairi, 1999; Pellowski and Conture, 2002) in that specific
types of disfluencies for CWS and CWNS differ significantly at a very early age
and all disfluency types classified as SLD are produced significantly more often
by children who stutter than by children who do not stutter. However, the
difference between part word repetitions and other disfluency types was larger
for English samples than in German samples. The overall frequency of SLDs
from the English samples was found to be higher (11%, range 3-40%) than those
German speaking children. This was the same cut off used by Pellowski and
Conture (2002) for English speaking children. The overall results were consistent
with the findings of Ambrose and Yairi (1999) that “very early stuttering is
distinct from normal disfluency” and “therefore it cannot be said that all children
Boey, Wuyts, Heyning, Bodt and Heylen (2007) conducted a study to compare
children who stutter (n= 693) with a group of normally speaking children (n=79).
Their findings suggested that the frequency of SLD in the stuttering group (M=
15. 71) was significantly higher than in the control group (M= 0.42); 3% SLD
criterion, for distinguishing children who do from children who do not stutter
34
stutter) and high specificity (0.98) (i.e., the proportion of individuals who do not
stutter); approximately 77% of the children who stutter exhibited some form of
prolongation or blocks; duration of SLD was longer for stuttering children than
non stuttering children and physical tension during SLD was estimated higher for
age children (mean age 10 years) was studied by Valente and Jesus (2011). They
criterion of 3 or more SLD per 100 words was used for the group of children
who stutter. Speech samples were obtained through the reading task. The authors
have developed an assessment tool (Jesus and Valente, 2010), incorporating the
evaluation of factors that characterize SLD i.e. frequency and types of SLD,
2009). The frequency and types of SLD during reading in both groups were
analyzed. The authors identified a total of 75 SLD in the group of children with
stuttering and 8 in the normally fluent group. They found that SLD were more
frequent in children who stutter than in the normally fluent children. The groups
total SLD was statistically significant. However, the results for monosyllabic
whole word repetitions and sound/syllable repetitions (SLDs observed in both the
groups) were not. The authors also found that the mean number of repetition
units for the group with stuttering was higher than for the normal group, a
finding in congruence with studies by (Ambrose and Yairi, 1999) and Boey et
al. (2007). The mean duration of different types of SLD was higher in first
35
group (stuttering) than the controls. However, statistically significant
differences could not be obtained. In their study, the authors also make a
reference to the physical tension associated with SLD. They found some amount
of physical tension associated with SLD in the stuttering group and no tension
was seen in the normally fluent group. The authors inferred from their results that
type, frequency and physical tension of SLD are parameters which differentiate
children who stutter from matched normally fluent children. Duration and
In the Indian context, Indu (1990), Nagapoornima (1990), Yamini (1990) and
Kannada, a south Indian Dravidian Language. This was based on disfluency data
of 12 children in the age groups of 3-4 (Nagapoornima), 4-5 (Indu), 5-6 (Yamini)
and 6-7 (Rajendraswamy) years. Simple pictures, cartoons and pictures depicting
Pancatantra stories were used in these tests. A total disfluency of greater than 25
Geetha, Karanth, Rao & Ravindra (2000) aimed at classifying and sub grouping
computer program, was used for this purpose on two groups (of 25 and 21
children each) of disfluent children below the age of 6 years. The authors
36
developed Disfluency Assessment Procedure for Children (DAPC) in Kannada.
groups but their frequency, number of unit repetitions, presence of schwa, and
audible and inaudible, were present in both NNF and stuttering groups, specially
the audible ones. There was slight over-lap in the frequency of disfluencies
among the NNF and stuttering children when sound syllable repetitions and
ranged from 3–30% in stuttering children, with majority showing around 10%
disfluencies; all NNF children had fleeting disfluencies while the stuttering
(in about 60–70%), the most common ones being eye blinking, nose flaring,
grimaces, or frowning; the rate of speech was faster in stuttering children (60–
70%), compared with NNF children; with respect to severity of the problem
based on Stuttering Severity Instrument, there was slight overlap among the NNF
and stuttering children. On the historical and attitudinal factors there was overlap
in scores in about 10% of NNF and very mild stuttering children. In general,
NNF children had scores below 5 whereas most stuttering children‟s range was
between 5–15. “Attitudinal factors (of self and significant others) are important
37
in determining stuttering disfluencies as negative attitudes have important
stuttering and NNF groups- NNF children had zero scores whereas it ranged
from 3–20 for stuttering children; Also, articulation and language problems were
the most common associated problems found in stuttering children. ANN could
predict the sub classifications of normal nonfluency and stuttering with 92%
accuracy although the size of the training sample (25) was comparatively small.
children in each age group. A total of seven types of disfluencies were identified.
They included filled pauses (pauses filled with sounds like “mm”…”um” etc);
unfilled pauses (silent pauses having duration greater than 300 ms); repetition
indicated pauses to have highest percentage of disfluencies in all the age groups;
increasing trend till 7 years with age and reduced by 8 years; prolongation was
the only disfluency that was not seen in any of the children between 4-8 years
(only one child exhibited between 5-6 years) and the total percentage of
38
disfluencies ranged from 17.1 (7-8 years) to 30 (5-6 years). However, the
findings of the study were not in congruence with the Western studies.
disfluencies have been studied both in the speech of children with no stuttering
(CWNS) and children with stuttering (CWS). Analysis of these features may also
word repetitions.
(e.g., the number of disfluent words in 100 words; the number of disfluent
When the frequencies of all of the child‟s disfluencies are measured, we need to
know how many of them are normal. One of the earliest databases in this regard
was compiled by Johnson and associates (1959). They examined the evidence for
the child is first “diagnosed” a stutterer by his parents, the child‟s disfluencies are
no different from those of non stuttering children. These authors compared the
39
2.5 to 8 years of age and a like number of age and gender matched non stuttering
children (68 boys and 21 girls). Results indicated that although there was some
overlap, the children who stuttered were more than twice as disfluent overall
100 words compared with 7 disfluencies per 100 words) and produced more of
Haynes and Hood (1977) included 30 non stuttering elementary school children
groups- 4, 6 and 8 years. They found that the groups did not differ significantly
output of 33 2-year old normally speaking children. They found that less than 6
disfluencies per 100 words in about half of the subjects while the other half
subjects close to the lower end i.e., less than two disfluencies per 100 words.
In Wexler and Mysak‟s (1982) study, frequency of disfluency was calculated per
100 words spoken for non stuttering male children in the age group of 2, 4 and 6
years. They found that the mean frequency of total disfluency was greater for
two year olds (M=14.6) when compared to 4 and 6 year olds (M=9.1) for both
groups. Also 2 year olds had significantly higher frequencies for disfluencies
such as word repetitions and phrase repetitions than the 4 and 6 year olds. There
was more variability between the 2 year olds (represented by high standard
deviation in scores) than for the four and six year olds.
40
Yairi and Lewis (1984) analyzed the disfluencies in the speech of a group of 10
two and 10 three year old children within two months of stuttering onset, and
those produced by their normally fluent peers. Their results revealed that on an
average, the stuttering children were three and a half times more disfluent
overall than their control counterparts (M=21.54 vs. M=6.16 disfluencies per
DeJoy and Gregory (1985) found that 3.5 year olds were more disfluent
(M=11.40) than the 5 year olds (M=9.30) in the frequency count of total
disfluencies.
Carlo and Watson (2003) in their study of 15 children (8 girls and 7 boys) aged
3.5 to 4 years and 17 children (8 girls and 9 boys) aged 5.0 to 5.5 years, found
that that means of total disfluencies ranged from 5.21 for the younger boys to
8.26 for the older girls. With age and gender groups combined, the frequency
range of total speech disfluencies was between 3.0 and 5.99. Results were not
spoken, there exists a good argument for measuring disfluencies per number of
syllables spoken. Andrews and Ingham (1971) first recommended the practice of
41
assessing frequency of stuttering in relation to the syllables spoken. Following
their logic, Yairi (1997) noted that as children get older, they are more likely to
use multisyllable words, and it is possible to have disfluencies on more than one
al.have assessed disfluencies in children as number per 100 syllables (Hubbard &
Yairi, 1988; Yairi & Lewis, 1984; Yairi and Ambrose, 1996). The results of all
these studies indicate that the average, normally disfluent preschool child has
about seven disfluencies for every 100 words spoken. If measured in syllables,
this would be closer to six disfluencies per 100 syllables. This figure might be a
little high throughout the preschool period (Yairi, 1997a), but many children go
through a period of increased disfluency at age 2 or 3 years that will reach this
associates (1959) and Yairi (1981) found that although many non stuttering
children have only one or two disfluencies per 100 words, at least one child in
their samples had slightly more than 25 disfluencies per 100 words. Hence,
that in conversation with their mothers, young stuttering children were between
two and half and three and half times more disfluent than age and gender
children to be almost 2 and half times more disfluent (M=13%) when compared
to normals (M=5%). Pellowski and Conture (2002) found that among 3 and 4
42
year olds, CWNS showed total disfluencies more than 4 times greater than CWS.
The results of all these studies provide interesting findings. First, reports of the
for different age groups. With younger children (around 2-3 years) showing
higher frequencies compared to the older group (4 – 6 year olds). However, some
disfluencies of stuttering and non stuttering children, reported that children who
stuttered were at least twice as disfluent as non stuttering children, with average
syllables spoken. Third, with each in each groups studied, there is lots of
Deviation obtained in the scores. SDs are noted to be approximating the mean.
Frequency of speech disfluency is highly variable both amongst and between the
necessary to take into account the types of disfluencies that children produce, as
stuttering or normally disfluent. There have been several studies (Zebrowski and
Conture, 1989; Conture, 1990 among others) of interest reporting several types
their distribution. Most of these studies report that irrespective of age, or the time
43
between children who stutter and their non stuttering counterparts, in the types of
disfluencies they produce. It has been consistently found that children who
stutter, even very young, ones relatively close to the onset of the problem,
produce more within word speech disfluencies (Zebrowski and Conture, 1989;
As per the results of Johnson and associates (1959), 89 stuttering and 89 non
disfluency (18 disfluencies per 100 words, versus 7 disfluencies per 100 words).
Also, they found that both groups of children essentially produced the same types
of disfluencies. As mentioned earlier it was this finding, along with his analysis
produced by the two groups, and there was no specifically distinctive type of
parent‟s response and reaction to their child‟s speech disfluencies that give rise to
concluded that, “most, and possibly all, very young children speak with sufficient
44
Johnson opined that there is similarity in types of disfluencies produced by the
stuttering and non stuttering children. However, a re- analysis of the data
types produced. The children who stuttered produced significantly more sound
and syllable, whole word and phrase repetitions, broken words and prolonged
sounds than the non stuttering children. Also, among the nonstuttering children,
interjections, revisions and word repetitions were the most common disfluency
types.
Kools and Berryman (1971), in their study of 92 first grade children, found that
non stutterers showed more interjections than any other type of disfluency.
Word repetitions, revisions and incomplete phrases were also the prevalent types.
The authors opine that “the tendency for non stutterers to exhibit marked
Yairi (1981) reported, in his 33 2- year old normal subjects, two clusters of
(one syllable words or parts of words were repeated) and interjections and
revisions. The most common disfluency type appears to change as the child
grows older. In a follow-up of his earlier study, Yairi (1982) found that children
between 2 and 3 and 1/2 years showed an increase in revisions and phrase
45
Yairi‟s study indicated that as non stuttering child matures, part-word repetitions
decline, even if other disfluency types increase. Further, Yairi suggests that
concern.
ten two and three year old children and their matched controls. They found that
both the groups produced “all recognized types of disfluency”, but in varying
word disfluencies differentiated the two groups; with the children who stuttered
producing significantly more part word- repetitions and sound prolongations than
Similarly, Meyers (1986) reported that four – and five- year old boys who
did normally disfluent boys of the same age. Meyers observed that the non
broken words”, an observation which may be related to the age of these children.
46
disfluency that was exhibited by either the stuttering or non stuttering children”
months of stuttering onset and a group of age and gender matched normally
disfluent children. However, the findings were concordant with earlier studies, in
that the stuttering children produced more within word disfluecies, overall than
Yairi and Ambrose (1999) found that the main difference between the stuttering
and control groups was determined by the proportion of SLD (types of fluency
disruptions that are most typical in persons with stuttering). For the group with
stuttering, SLD constituted 66% of the total disfluency whereas in the normal
group it was 34%. Other disfluencies constituted 34% of the total disfluency for
the stuttering group and 76% for the control group. Part word repetitions
occurred with maximum frequency in the stuttering group while in the control
count. Weighted SLD scores were found to be continuous across age groups and
score below 4.00 for used to assign subjects in the control group and above
between the groups for stuttering like disfluencies and total disfluencies. 81%
and 42% of the total disfluencies comprised of SLDs in the experimental and
47
control groups respectively while, OD occupied 58% of the total disfluencies in
the controls and 19 % in the group with stuttering. According to them, weighted
young children has been reported by a number of investigators (e.g., De Joy and
Gregory, 1985; Haynes and Hood, 1977; Wexler and Mysak, 1982; Yairi, 1981,
1997). Also, the scheme of stuttering like and other disfluencies, helps to better
stuttering than children with no stuttering. OD seems to overlap between the two
groups.
the acoustic energy associated with the disfluent initial sound in the word to the
cessation of acoustic energy for the audibly prolonged or final iteration of the
Zebrowski (1991) reported that the mean duration of these children‟s sound/
these measures and those obtained from a group of matched non stuttering
48
children. Zebrowski (1994) suggested that “the clinical utility of duration lies not
children, but its relationship to other aspects of stuttered and fluent speech of
articulatory speech rate and therefore these children may be more likely to
require direct intervention for stuttering compared to their normally fluent peers.
Gorden and Luper (1992a, b), suggested that in several widely used protocols for
distinguish stuttering from normal disfluency (e.g., Cooper and Cooper, 1985;
Curlee, 1980; Pindzola, 1987; Van Riper, 1982). The important findings from
these studies include (1) children who stutter produce instances of stuttering
which are typically shorter than one second in duration; (2) sound/syllable
Conture and Kelly (1991), Kelly and Conture (1992), Louko, Edwards and
Conture (1990), Niermann, Yairi and Peterson (1993) and Yaruss and Conture
(1993) have all found that for a large part young children who stutter produce
Number of repeated units within sound, syllable and word repetitions: The
duration of sound/ syllable part word and whole word repetitions is expressed as
49
the number of repeated units (RU). The number of extra times a segment is
repeated for part and single syllable word repetition is tabulated as repetition
Branscom, Hughes and Oxtoby (1955) were one of the first to use the concept of
year olds, 79% of word and syllable repetitions contained just one extra unit.
contained 5 units. In Johnson et al.‟s study, combining part and whole word
repetitions for males and females, the average number of syllable and word
repetition units was 1.46 for children who stutter, and 1.09 for the normally
Adams (1980), Curlee (1980), and Pindzola (1987) all support that
In Yairi (1981)‟s study on 33 2-year olds, for boys, the mean number of units
ranged from 1.12 (part word repetition), to 1.28 (monosyllabic word repetition).
Girls had a score of 1.08 (part word repetitions) and 1.07 (single-syllable word
repetitions). Yairi and Lewis (1984) reported that the young , beginning
50
stuttering children in their study, produced significantly more repeated units per
instance of part word repetition than did their non stuttering peers (M=1.72
repeated units for the stuttering children and M=1.12 repeated units for the non
stuttering children). The authors also found almost no overlap between the two
groups in terms of the number of repetition units (for part word repetitions). The
respectively. Also, instances of repeating part words more than once were
infrequent and instances of repeating more than twice was rare in normally
same two talker groups (M= 2.4 repeated units for stuttering children versus M=
2.1 for the non stuttering children. in her study, the final production of the target
sound was also counted in the repetition units. However, Johnson et al. (1959),
and Yairi and Lewis, (1984) have counted only those segments in excess of the
intended segment.
Ambrose and Yairi (1995) found that the maximum number of repetition units
per instance of disfluency was 17 for children who stutter and 4 among the
normally disfluent children. The mean number of repetition units per instance of
repetition was calculated for each subject. The individual scores were used to
derive group means for males and females in the two groups. The range of
individual means in the stuttering group was nearly 5 units per instance with a
total group mean of 1.70. For the normally disfluent group, the range of
individual means was 1.44 units per instance with a total group mean of 1.16.
The authors reported that the group of children who stuttered had significantly
51
more repetition units per instance than the control group. But, no significant
difference was obtained between genders on repetition units. The ratio of single-
unit to multiple unit repetition was 2.06 in the stuttering group and 6.58 in the
control group.
Pellowski and Conture (2002) found a mean number of repetition units (based on
(SD=1) with individual subject mean ranging from 1.0 to 5.5 units of repetition
in CWS, while for CWNS, it was 1.1 (SD= 0.48), with a range of 1.0 to 1.6.
Natke et al., (2006) in their study of 24 German children (5 years or below) who
disfluencies (phrase and multi syllable word repetitions) were not significantly
Counting Total disfluencies: This simply means counting all the disfluencies
normal. Yairi (1997) stated that “disfluency counts have been the classic metric
clinical and experimental studies”. This metric is archetypal as it has not changed
substantially since it was first introduced by Johnson in the late 1950s (Johnson,
1961; Johnson et al., 1959). Here, the clinician / researcher transcribes a speech
sample and then classifies each transcribed disfluency to a particular type. Thus,
52
stuttering. Adams (1977) suggested that a child should be designated as
stuttering, when greater than 10 % of the words spoken are disfluent. Ambrose
(Ambrose and Yairi, 1999; Pellowski and Conture, 2002). Johnson‟s system was
used its many variants. For example, Gregory and Hill (1993) and Zebrowski
disfluency types. Yairi and Ambrose (1992) used 8, Yairi, Ambrose and
Niermann (1993) used 7, Yairi and Ambrose (1999) used only 6 disfluency
types.
Stuttering Like Disfluencies: The SLDs measure was introduced by Yairi and
as “stuttering”, and recognizes that not all disfluencies of persons who stutter are
necessarily “stuttering””.
Ambrose and Yairi (1999) reported a normative reference for early stuttering.
6 months of stuttering onset and from 54 age matched normally fluent children.
Disfluencies in the speech samples were identified and classified and modified as
per the system given by Johnson et al. (1959). Prolongations, blocks and broken
words were combined into a category called disrhythmic phonation. Yairi and
53
Lewis (1984) had modified the system by dividing the category of word
repetition into single syllable word and multisyllable word repetition. Yairi and
Ambrose further eliminated the between word category of tense pause because of
the unusual difficulty in identification and the frequent confusion with external
multi-syllable word repetition was rare in English speaking children‟s usage, and
was similar to phrase repetition (i.e., both contained more than one syllable),
The six remaining categories included (a) part-word repetition, (b) single-syllable
words, (d) interjection, (e) revision /abandoned utterances and (f) multi-
syllable/phrase repetition. The first three categories were combined to get the
also combined and labeled as other disfluencies (OD). “Other disfluencies” was
more than one disfluency type occurred in a word, each was counted to provide
accurate representation of the child‟s disfluent speech. The number of extra times
a segment was repeated for part and single-syllable word repetition was also
The disfluency types that constitute an SLD have varied to a great extent. Yairi
(1997) reported SLD as constituting: part word and mono syllabic word
repetition, disrhythmic phonation and tense pauses. Justification for using these
terms to define SLDs comes from earlier study by Yairi and Lewis (1984), who
54
used parent judgments to classify their children as having a stuttering problem or
as being normally fluent. The children‟s speech samples were analyzed, and the
reported that only two disfluency types distinguished between the stuttering and
frequency of single- syllable word repetitions did not differentiate between the
two groups, however, the number of iterations per instance of repetition did.
normally fluent speech. However, Ambrose and Yairi (1999) and Yairi, Watkins,
Ambrose and Paden (2001) opined that monosyllabic repetitions are distinctive
Williams, Silverman and Kools (1968) labeled disrhythmic phonation and tense
behaviours that occurred between words (or between part words or non words)
prolongations, silent blocks, and also broken words and other within word
interruptions (but not repetitions) that disturb the continuity of words.” Finally,
tense pause was omitted from the SLD metric because of the “difficulty in
55
reliable identification” (Ambrose and Yairi, 1999). Pellowski and Conture
(2002), and Carlo and Watson (2005) use different sets of disfluencies within
their “SLD” and “other disfluency” measures. In summary, the SLD unit was
described as linked to a word; it did not refer to events that occurred between
differentially diagnose from normal disfluency. Yairi and colleagues opine that a
child must display at least three SLDs per 100 syllables in a speech sample in
Yairi and Ambrose (1999), Pellowski and Conture (2002), and Zakheim and
Conture (2003) included three or more SLDs per 100 words (not syllables) in a
speech sample. However, not all SLDs are stuttering events. For e.g. single part
occur within the speech of non stuttering children and listeners have no difficulty
emphasized the fact that SLD should not be considered as a synonym for
stuttering. Yairi and Ambrose (1999) opined that the three SLD threshold need
stuttering. However, they also admit that “few young normally fluent children,
diagnostic strength of the SLD measure, Ambrose and Yairi (1999) proposed a
Weighted Measure: The weighted SLD score is a metric that considers the
56
presence and duration of disrhythmic phonation. It is calculated by using the
following formula:
infrequent in early stuttering and rare in normally fluent children (Johnson et al.,
1959; Yairi & Lewis, 1984). They have also traditionally been considered an
perception of severity (Costello & Ingham, 1984; Zebrowski & Conture, 1989).
Ambrose and Yairi (1999) claim that a weighted SLD score will “provide more
powerful information about the presence and severity of stuttering”. They also
regardless of the number of SLD”. However, these claims are not well supported.
According to Ambrose and Yairi (2001), the weighted score “is a mathematical
that the resulting score “is not to be taken as an absolute line of demarcation”.
normal speaking children tends to decrease with age. Colburn and Mysak (1982),
57
longitudinal studies. Evidently, some children become more disfluent as
language emerges. Several investigators (Haynes and Hood, 1977; Wexler, 1982;
De Joy and Gregory (1985) report decrease in the frequency of disfluency with
however exhibited a noticeable downward shift. Shifting was also observed for
Hood, 1977). De Joy and Gregory (1985) observed a decrease in word, part-word
from 3.5 to 5.5 years. Ambrose and Yairi (1999) studied did not find any
significant difference for the age factor, or for the interaction between age and
group in children from 2 to 5 years of age. However, they found that among the
SLDs, part- word repetitions, single syllable word repetitions and repetition units
were highest in the 3 year old range which decreased later. Thus the literature is
Gender: Glasner and Rosenthal (1957) found sex ratio was only 1.4 to 1 among
153 children who were reported by their parents to have stuttered at some time
before entering the first grade. Andrews and Harris (1964) found that the sex
ratios based on incidence figures tend to be lower than ratios based on the
58
stuttering in girls than in boys. They reported that the sex ratio averaged from 2.6
to 1. Johnson and Associates (1959), Kools and Berryman (1971), Haynes and
Hood (1977), or Ratusnik, Kiriluk and Ratusnik (1979), Ambrose and Yairi
Kools and Berryman (1971) analyzed speech samples from 46 male and 46
genders. Only incomplete phrases were significantly different, with more males
Subjects included lower status black, lower status white, middle social status
black and middle social status white children. They identified 6 disfluency types
revisions and incomplete phrases - in a 300 word sample. They found disfluency
on 7% of the words. They reported that “sex was not associated with rate of
disfluency, and there were no interactions between sex and social status
functionally correct responses. Berryman and Kools (1975) found that among 92
59
first graders, disfluency was not related to reading ability, intelligence or judges‟
ratings of language level. Haynes and Hood (1977) found that among children
aged 4, 6 and 8 years, there was no relationship between disfluency and syntactic
and Gregory (1985) selected 10 3-5 year old children with the highest number
and 10 with the lowest number of disfluencies of various types and compared the
two groups of samples with respect to the Developmental Sentence Score. The
disfluent children scored lower in syntactic maturity than did the fluent ones in
case of word repetitions, pauses, incomplete phrases, and for the 5- year olds in
of 12 normally fluent children (6 boys and 6 girls, ages, 44-64 months). Only 50
– utterance spontaneous speech samples were analyzed for the presence of more
phrases, revisions and interjections) and less typical disfluencies ( seen more in
calculated. The authors found that disfluent utterances were longer and more
conversational speech.
60
disfluent normals scored significantly lower than the typical normals on most of
the measures. There was no difference between the highly disfluent normal
speakers and the stutterers. Twenty four children (3.1 – 5.11 years - 12 were
CWS and 12 CWNS) were studied for the influence of utterance length and
(nonSLDs) for CWNS. Four measures were obtained - utterance length (MLU),
phrase repetitions, interjections, revisions) per 100 words; also number of SLDs
or nonSLDs per utterance. Results showed that utterances above children‟s MLU
complex. The authors concluded that that “the relative “match” or “mismatch”
2003).
Additional factors that are known to affect disfluency include situational factors
and familiality. These are however, not discussed here as they were not
The review of literature indicated that (1) the frequency of speech disfluencies
reportedly decreases after age 3. Also, researchers have observed decrease in certain
61
types of disfluencies (whole and part word repetitions, phrase repetitions, incomplete
phrases, revisions and disrhythmic phonations) with age. Some disfluency types like
interjections have been reported to increase with age; (2) Some studies have shown
boys to be more disfluent than girls. However, most of the studies show no
statistically significant gender differences; (3) Both repetition units and absolute
studies reveal that CWNS produce a maximum of 1-2 iterations per instance of
although CWS may produce disfluencies that are slightly longer. However, on an
average CWS and CWNS produce disfluencies that are within 1 sec; (4) Disfluency is
found to be related to syntactic complexity i.e. disfluencies occur on longer and more
complex syntactic utterances; and (5) Few studies described the fluency behavior of
preschool children in languages other than English, like Spanish, German, Dutch and
Kannada. With respect to Indian languages, normative data in the preschool period are
not available.
In this context, the present study investigated speech disfluencies in 2.1 to 6 year old
typically developing Kannada speaking children. The specific objectives of the study
were to investigate (a) number, type, and duration of speech disfluencies exhibited by
(b) the effect of gender (Boys and Girls only) and age on speech disfluencies in these
children, and (c) to examine the clinical relevance of the measures derived from the
study.
62