Sight Singing in The Beginning High School Classroom
Sight Singing in The Beginning High School Classroom
Sight-Singing in the Beginning High School Choral Classroom: A Survey of Virginia High
Joseph Eveler
Abstract
The purpose of this survey is to establish current practices, behaviors, and attitudes of high
school choral directors in the state of Virginia. As of the submission of this paper the survey has
not been completed; the author plans to distribute the survey in the fall of 2012 to the state choral
directors.
SIGHT-‐SINGING
IN
BEGINNING
HIGH
SCHOOL
CHOIR
3
The skill of reading music in a choral setting would seem to be necessary to develop
independent, life-long performers. From the earliest incarnations of public school music
education with Lowell Mason, connecting the written pitch to vocalized sound is considered by
most music educators to be a necessary skill (Demorest, 2001). It is even included in our
National Standards for Music Education, content standard number five: “reading and notating
music” (Consortium of National Arts Education Associations, 1994). It is also becoming more
and more common for district and state level music assessments to include a sight-singing
component, with 25 states as of 2004 having some form of sight-singing assessment at the
that there are one or two systems and methods that have been universally adopted by music
educators. That, however, is not the case at all. According to Alan McClung there are no fewer
than nine different pitch systems in use today: (1) interval names; (2) inflected letter-names; (3)
non-inflected letter-names; (4) fixed-do; (5) scale-degree numbers with 1 always being tonic; (6)
scale degree numbers with 1 as tonic in major, and 6 as tonic in minor; (7) moveable-do, with
do-based minor; (8) moveable-do with la-based minor; and (9) neutral syllables (McClung,
2001). In his survey of high school all-state choristers from Alabama, Arkansas, Georgia,
Louisiana, Mississippi, and Tennessee McClung found that the most common systems were
scale-degree numbers, moveable-do and fixed-do. In Jane Kuehne’s survey of Florida middle-
school choral educators it was shown that moveable-do is much more common than a fixed-do
system, and that solfege syllables and Curwen hand-signs are highly important (Kuehne, 2007).
SIGHT-‐SINGING
IN
BEGINNING
HIGH
SCHOOL
CHOIR
4
As McClung (2001) concludes, “… the debate over which system is best suited for teaching
preference.”
Many factors play a role in the musical development of students, but there are several
factors that serve as strong predictors of students success at reading music. Demorest and May
(1995) found that, even while factoring in different methods, systems, and assessment
procedures, the most important factor was prior school choral experience, followed closely by
piano experience, and instrumental experience, and vocal lessons. Several studies (Daniels,
1986; Henry, 2011; Killian & Henry, 2005; McClung, 2008) found a high correlation between
Rose Daniels (1986) surveyed a selection of 20 high schools to determine what factors, if
any, could accurately determine sight-reading ability in the school’s mixed chorus programs. She
selected four major variables: the school, the music curriculum, the chorus teacher, and the
chorus students. Through a detailed look at each variable, she found that the curriculum had least
influence on sight-singing ability. The single best predictor was ethnic make-up of the school.
She lists the following factors, in combination, as being the strongest predictors of sight-reading
ability: the ethnic make-up of the school, a large percentage of choir students with a piano at
home, a school in a rural setting, an occasional use of rote teaching procedures, a large
percentage of all-state chorus participants, large number of students with instrumental experience,
and a chorus teacher who believes that sight-singing is an important objective for high school
music (Daniels, 1986). Of these factors, the one most able to be influenced is the attitude of the
While there may not be empirical evidence determining the most effective sight-singing
system, there is research to support specific sight-singing strategies and techniques. Janice
Killian and Michele Henry found that there are desirable strategies during a practice period that
are much more common in high-ability level sight-singers (2005). These strategies include
tonicizing the key out loud, use of hand signs, practicing the example out loud, finishing the
example in the allotted study time, and isolating problem areas. Those students who performed
the examples most accurately also used these strategies most frequently. Strategies considered to
be undesirable, such as abandoning the steady beat, stopping during the melody, and taking eyes
off the music, were found in highest numbers in the low scoring participants. In the lowest
scoring participants the researchers found the 30-second preparation time to be nearly useless. A
lack of instruction in strategies for approaching sight-singing is a key factor in the low
performance (Killian & Henry, 2005). To expand on this finding, Henry conducted a study in
2008 that found even a short thirty-minute cram session of these strategies can improve the sight-
singing performance of low scoring sight-singers, with a 19% increase in their scores from
pretest to posttest. It did not, however, increase the scores of the high scoring participants (Henry,
2008). Henry and Killian’s research showed that the presence of multiple desirable strategies is
However, McClung found in 2008 that students who were extensively trained in Curwen
hand signs performed no better with or without the use of the hand signs during assessment. In
fact, students with no instrumental experience actually performed better without the use of hand
signs (McClung, 2008). In addition, his research found data to support a commonly held belief-
students with instrumental experience score significantly higher than students without
SIGHT-‐SINGING
IN
BEGINNING
HIGH
SCHOOL
CHOIR
6
instrumental experience. Instrumentalists scored 37% higher in the study than non-
instrumentalists (2008).
There have been several studies dedicated to determining the usefulness of specific sight-
singing teaching strategies. Henry (2004) found that by targeting specific pitch skills students’
mean scores rose significantly. She developed fifteen core pitch skills that were introduced and
practiced over a 12-week period. One group was instructed on the skills using unfamiliar tonal
patterns, while the other group was instructed with familiar melodies exhibiting the pitch skills.
Both groups showed significant gains from pretest to posttest ( (Henry, 2004). In a study with
college undergraduate music majors J. David Boyles and Keitha Lucas (1990) found that a tonal
significantly improved the mean scores of the participants. It also showed that the greatest
Henry (2011) found that pitch and rhythm skills retain their relative difficulties,
independent of other factors present in the music. She also found that while pitch accuracy was
not affected by rhythmic difficulty, rhythmic accuracy was affected by pitch difficulty. It was
much more common in this study for the participant to complete the pitch skills correctly but the
rhythm skills incorrectly than to complete rhythmic skills correctly but pitch skills incorrectly.
Therefore, Henry writes, “Rhythmic success was found to be a significant predictor of pitch
teacher in a classroom setting and the use of a self-instructional method using written and
prerecorded audio material. The study aimed to find if either method was more effective at
increasing the sight-reading scores of high school choral students. While it was a limited study at
SIGHT-‐SINGING
IN
BEGINNING
HIGH
SCHOOL
CHOIR
7
a summer music camp without any equating of the control and experimental groups, it was
shown that there was no significant difference between either method. Kanable felt that this
student’s needs and allow for more targeted instruction, especially for students who seek to
There has been some research to determine whether the type of assessments being used
affect the performance of students. In most choral classrooms sight-singing is taught to the
ensemble as a whole, but the way in which assessment occurs can very widely. Demorest (2001)
reports from an informal survey that 75% of directors have some kind of singing test during the
year, with 33% of directors testing at least three times per year. Common modes of assessment
include sight-singing alone in rehearsal, alone on tape, alone in front of the teacher, and in
quartets in rehearsal. Henry (2001) tested an assessment method where, rather than score every
single pitch and rhythm uniquely, there is an inventory of pitch skills that would be used to
develop musical examples, and the performer would be graded on the completion of each skill.
The reasoning behind this method is that this would create a quicker and more quantitative
assessment, and therefore useful in a setting where numerous students need to be tested in a short
amount of time. Her study found the test to be highly reliable, and possessed the qualities needed
Inventory, 2001). This test, known as the Vocal Sight-Reading Inventory (VSRI) is unique in
that it provides specific feedback to the performer and instructor as to the skills that are mastered
by the performer, and those that are not. In 2003 Henry fine-tuned this test, created a
comprehensive test and a leveled test. The comprehensive test, which consists of six melodies
covering all 28 skills, would be useful in a pretest or benchmark situation. The leveled test,
SIGHT-‐SINGING
IN
BEGINNING
HIGH
SCHOOL
CHOIR
8
which consists of melodies of increasing difficulty, could be used to assess progress in certain
pitch skills after the comprehensive test has set a baseline (Henry, 2003).
Henry (2012) did a recent study to find if music technology was a viable choice for
assessing sight-singing, as well as exploring student attitudes towards the use of computerized
their comfort with sight-singing, self-evaluation of their sight-singing ability, and their views on
using technology in music. All participants were high school students at a summer music camp.
The students were given the questionnaire, then were given a sight-singing assessment through
the SmartMusic 2012 software, and then given a follow-up questionnaire to assess their feelings
of the test, their perceptions of the software, and their likelihood of using this type of technology
in the future. Henry found, regarding instruction and assessment, (1) most instruction in choral
classrooms is happening in a large group setting, with only 12 of 138 participants not reporting
that as a means of instruction; (2) small-group or peer instruction was reported by only 50 of 138
participants; (3) 70% of participants had been assessed at sight-singing as an individual, and
assessment was far higher than the beginning-level sight-singers (2012). She also found that
students who had never used computer-based assessment found it an unpleasant experience the
first time, those students who took a class on the software at the music camp had a dramatic shift
of opinion, finding that it “enforced a strict tempo,” “make them better sight-readers,” “easy to
experiment with six high school choirs in Washington state to test the hypothesis: “Choir
students given a regular program of individual testing will show a significantly greater
SIGHT-‐SINGING
IN
BEGINNING
HIGH
SCHOOL
CHOIR
9
alone” (Demorest S. M., Improving Sightsinging Performance in the Choral Ensemble: The
Effect of Individual Testing, 1998). 306 participants from six schools were involved in the study.
Two choirs were used at each school, and each choir was randomly placed in either the control or
experimental group. The control groups received only group instruction, while the experimental
groups also had individual assessment three times over as many months. The study showed a
significant improvement, even over one semester of study, in the group that had used individual
assessment as part of the instructional process. Even when accounting for differences in methods
and school demographics, this study shows a clear path for improvement in individual sight-
While the research has found numerous factors that correlate to successful sight-singing,
the most important factor that is directly affected by the teacher is individual student assessment
(Demorest S. M., 2001). And while much has been written about the instruction of sight-singing
as a skill, little has been written about the connection of sight-singing skills to the performance
repertoire that is the majority of a choral rehearsal. This survey of Virginia high school choral
(2) If so, how often do they teach sight-singing, and for what amount of time?
(5) Are teachers applying the skills taught during sight-singing instruction to the learning
This survey is neither scientific nor experimental, but rather a descriptive look at the
SIGHT-‐SINGING
IN
BEGINNING
HIGH
SCHOOL
CHOIR
10
Method
An online survey will be emailed to the high school choral teachers in Virginia, via an
Survey
If yes:
period
7. How many times a week do you see your beginning choir classes?
a. 1 b. 2
SIGHT-‐SINGING
IN
BEGINNING
HIGH
SCHOOL
CHOIR
11
c. 3 e. 5
d. 4 f. more than 5
8. In the classes you teaching sight-singing, how much time, on average, is spent
d. 15 to 20 minutes
10. What system of rhythm do you use most frequently in choral class?
b. rhythmic syllables (i.e.. four quarter notes would be counted “ta ta ta ta” or similar)
c. counting of individual note values( i.e., four quarter notes would be counted “1 1 1 1”)
11. How many times a marking period are students assessed on sight-singing individually?
a. 1 c. 3
b. 2 d. 4
SIGHT-‐SINGING
IN
BEGINNING
HIGH
SCHOOL
CHOIR
12
e. 5 f. more than 5
12. What methods do you use for assessing students individually? Check all that apply.
ensemble singing
13. Which best describes the instruction of sight-singing in your beginning choral classes?
c. evenly split between large group instruction and small group/peer instruction
The following statements will be evaluated in a 1-10 scale, 1 being “strongly disagree” and 10
17. I would not spend as much time on sight-singing if it was not required at certain events.
18. I tend to spend more time on sight-singing instruction right before major events such as
19. I often reduce time spent on sight-singing instruction before school concerts and
performances.
The following statements will be evaluated on a 1-10 scale, 1 being “never,” 5 being
22. I give a pretest at the beginning of the year to assess my students’ sight-singing abilities.
23. When teaching new repertoire to my beginning choir students I teach them the music by
rote teaching.
24. When teaching new repertoire to my beginning choir students I allow them to write in
pitches based on the system we use for sight-singing and have students read the repertoire
25. When teaching new repertoire to my beginning choir students I have them read the
26. I teach new repertoire by playing the piano along with the students.
27. I teach new repertoire by having the students sing as a large group a cappella, only giving
28. I teach new repertoire by having students sing in small sections using peer instruction.
If no:
5. What are the reasons for not teaching sight-singing to your students? Check all that apply.
d. I have not had enough training in f. Students bore easily during sight-
Bibliography
Boyle, J., & Lucas, K. V. (1990). The Effect of Context on Sightsinging. Bulletin of the Council
Consortium of National Arts Education Associations. (1994). National Standards for Arts
Education: What every young American should know and be able to do in the arts.
Daniels, R. D. (1986). Relationships Among Selected Factors and the Sight-Reading Ability
of High School Mixed Choirs. Journal of Research in Music Education , 34 (4), 279-289.
Demorest, S. M., & May, W. V. (1995). Sight-Singing Instruction in the Choral Ensemble:
43 (2), 156-167.
Henry, M. L. (2003). A Comparison of Testing Formats for Vocal Sight Reading. Texas Music
Henry, M. L. (2004). The Use of Targeted Pitch Skills for Sight-Singing Instruction in the
Henry, M. L. (2008). The Use of Specific Practice and Performance Strategies in Sight-
16.
Henry, M. L. (2011). The Effect of Pitch and Rhythm Difficulty on Vocal Sight-Reading
217-226.
Killian, J. N., & Henry, M. L. (2005). A Comparison of Successful and Unsuccessful Strategies
Training in Moveable Solfege Syllables and Curwen Hand Signs. Journal of Research