1939 Spitzer PDF
1939 Spitzer PDF
EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY
Volume XXX December, 1939 Number 9
STUDIES IN RETENTION
HERBERT F. SPITZER
State University of Iowa*
Gates2 found that some recall in the form of recitation was an aid in
memorizing biographical prose. This was true when tests were given
immediately and also after four hours. Gates also reports finding a
high positive correlation between immediate and delayed recall.
Yoakam7 concluded that an immediate recall in the form of a test was
of more value to retention than was a single reading. Jones3 found
that recall tests aided the retention of information acquired from
lectures by college students Other studies which are related to some
phases of this study are reported by Dietze,1 Keys,4 Raffel,6 and
Young.8
SUBJECTS AND MATERIAL
Time in days 0 1 7 14 21 28 63
Groups
I B, B, B,
II B, B, B,
III B, B\
IV Bi B2
V B. B,
VI B, B,
VII B,
VIII • • •• B,
which had little relation to the content of Article B. This test was
given in order to keep them from expecting a later test. The real
Test B was given to these six groups (III-VIII) at varying time
intervals after the start of the experiment. A diagram of the testing
procedure followed is given in Table I. This table shows that the
pupils of Group I took Test Bi immediately after reading Article B,
repeated the test (Test 2?2) after one day, and again repeated the same
test (Test B3) after twenty-one days. The pupils of Group IV took
Test JBI seven days after reading Article B and repeated the test
(Test B^ twenty-one days after reading the article. The procedure
followed by the other six groups is shown in the table.
The reading materials were not referred to after the initial study
period, nor did the pupils know that there were to be delayed tests.
The teachers were instructed not to discuss the articles or the tests
with the pupils. Pupils were given eight minutes to read the articles
and ten minutes for each of the tests.
The pupils of Group IX read both articles and took both tests on
the first day of the experiment. They took Test B a second time
immediately after completing the first attempt. For this second test
they were instructed to try to improve their first score. This pro-
cedure was followed for the purpose of obtaining information on the
effect of repetition of the tests. The pupils of Group X took Test B
without having read Article B, for the purpose of obtaining data on
previous knowledge.
RESULTS
I 286 15.03 B, 13 23 4 69
I 284 B, 13 07 4 57
I 266 B, 12.18 4 59
II 338 15 05 B, 13 20 4.50
II 335 B, 11 84 4 64
II 312 B, 10 74 4.22
III 367 15.00 B, 9 56 4.24
III 349 Bt 8.93 4 06
IV 337 15.00 B, 7 87 3 56
IV 323 B, 8.15 3.83
V 371 15.04 B, 6 97 3 53
V 353 B, 7 10 3.21
VI 379 15 04 B, 6.49 2 91
VI 352 B, 7.07 3 08
VII 365 15 00 B, 6.80 3 03
VIII 350 15.03 B, 6 38 2.71
* For identification of various Test B's and the tune after learning that each
was taken, see Table I.
The mean scores of all groups of pupils who took Test B are shown
in Table II. In interpreting the data of this table the assumption is
made that the eight groups of pupils profited equally from the reading
and that the groups possessed equal ability to retain the effects of the
reading. This assumption is based on the fact that the groups were
equated on the sectioning test (Test A). According to the assumption
stated above, Group I would have made a mean score of approximately
9.56 (mean score of Group III on Test Bi) one day after reading the
article if the group had not been given the immediat erecall test. This
last statement is based on the fact that Groups I and III were originally
646 The Journal of Educational Psychology
equal. Therefore, had Group I delayed taking Test B until one day
after reading, the mean score made by the group would have been
the same as that made by Group III, or 9.56. On the same basis, had
Groups III, IV, V, VI, VII, and VIII been tested immediately after read-
ing Article B, each group would have made a mean score of approxi-
mately 13.22. The data of this table show that more is forgotten in one
day without recall than is forgotten in sixty-three days with the aid of
recall, as is shown by a comparison of the scores of II B3 and VIII Bt.
The differences between originally comparable groups and now differing
by only one recall and the critical ratio of these differences are shown
in Table III. These critical ratios would have been larger had the
standard error of difference formula for matched groups been used.*
This formula was not used because a statistically significant difference
was obtained without its use. The data summarized in Tables II
and III show clearly that retention benefited significantly by recall.
The effects of recall on retention are shown graphically in the
figures (see Figs. 1, 2,3, and 4). The points on the graph represent the
mean performance of the different groups. For example, the "Point
III Bi" represents the mean score of the pupils of Group III on their
I B, 13 07 .27
III B, 9.56 .22 3.51 346 10.10
I B, 12.18 28
IV Bs 8.15 .21 4.03 .350 11.51
II Bt 11.84 .25
IV B, 7.87 .19 3 97 317 12 50
II B, 10.74 .24
VI B, 7.07 .16 3 67 288 12.74
III B, 8.93 .22
V B, 6.97 .18 1 96 286 6.85
IV B, 8 15 .21
VI B, 6.49 .16 1 66 .264 6.29
V B, 7.10 18
VII Bx 6.80 .16 .30 241 1.25
first attempt on Test B. This trial was taken one day after reading
Article B. Since the eight groups were equalized according to their
performance on Test A, which was quite similar to Test B, it is
assumed that the facts which Groups I and II acquired through reading
would have been forgotten at the rate shown by the solid line if they
had not been given the intervening recall tests. Thus, the space
separating a point on the solid line and a vertical point on any of the
broken lines represents the effect of the recall test on retention for that
particular situation. Figures 1 and 2, which are based on the entire
population, show that immediate forgetting unaided by recall was very
rapid and that in every case recall was beneficial to retention.
M
\w -
18
12 SB,
10 1 M7B,
SB,
a g JVS,
»-
r
- . 1
P « Unaided tv
* .
to
Aided ty
to
0
14 21
TIME IN DATS (
When the papers were corrected for guessing through use of the
W
formula, S = R — (N _ ,y the critical ratios of differences remained
practically the same as those shown in Table III.
The one hundred sixty-nine children of Group IX who were given
a second Test B immediately after taking the first test improved their
mean score only .03. Thus, an immediate repetition of the test did not
result in a large increase in the number of facts acquired.
The mean score of the three hundred one children of Group X who
were tested for previous knowledge was 5.27. The assumption that
this score represents the previous knowledge of Groups I-VIII is
rather questionable since the foils or wrong responses of items in the
test were not as plausible for those who had not read the article
(group tested for previous knowledge) as these responses were for those
648 The Journal of Educational Psychology
pupils who had read the article. The previous knowledge of Groups
I-VIII, then, was probably less than 5.27. Since no other measure of
previous knowledge was available, this amount was subtracted from
the mean score of each group in obtaining the data from which Fig. 2
was constructed. This figure, subject to the limitation given above,
shows the curves of retention when the amount retained is given as a
per cent of the amount learned. The figure shows the same trend as
that shown in Fig. 1. In this case, however, forgetting is more rapid
• h—
100
' • *
••5, 1
JUS*
A'c/rtY ty
»-O
•
re, va,
VA — _
s52.
7 M 21
TIME IN DAYS
Differ- SE
SE,
Group Division Test AM AM ence of differ- CR
AM's ence
\l!B,
.aw1
zae,
Una/dec/ £y
7 14 21
TIME IN DAYS
1
14
ire,
W 21
TIME IN DAYS
of their original score in one day while the pupils in the lower one third
forgot twenty-eight per cent. For one week, the figures were thirty-
nine per cent and forty-four per cent. When scores were corrected
for guessing the upper one third forgot within one day thirty-three
per cent while the lower one third forgot forty-nine per cent. It
should be remembered that the upper and lower thirds referred to are
Studies in Retention 651
based on the pupils' scores on Test A. The data show that the pupils
in the lower third tend to have a more rapid initial rate of forgetting.
Additional data on the relationship between learning ability and
retention were secured by correlating the immediate score of pupils on
Test A with their delayed score on Test B. The score on Test A was
considered a measure of the pupils' ability to learn, while the delayed
score on Test B was considered as a measure of the pupils' ability to
retain. The " r ' s " obtained in the above manner ranged from .60 for
one day to .44 for sixty-three days. Since the correlation between the
two tests on immediate recall was only .76, the " r ' s " given for the rela-
tionship between immediate and delayed recall, or between ability to
~- "-—1
— _ - ^ —
_
14 21 63 PREVIOUS
KNOWLEBGC
- TIME IN DAYS -
From an item analysis of the papers of all pupils who took Test B,
the data on forgetting of individual items shown in Table VI were
652 The Journal of Educational Psychology
obtained. The first seven columns represent the per cent of pupils
tested at various times who answered each item correctly. The last
column shows the per cent of the pupils tested for previous knowledge
who answered the items correctly. An examination of the data in
Table VI will show that there is little relationship between the rate of
forgetting of items and the initial difficulty of the items. This fact is
shown graphically in Fig. 5. The four items (9, 11, 15, and 23) for
which curves of retention are shown were approximately of equal
difficulty on the initial test. Two of the items (11 and 23) were also
about equal on the previous knowledge score. In spite of these simi-
larities, widely differing curves of retention were found. The one
general characteristic of the data on each item is the fact that the rate
TABLE VI.—THB P E R CENT OP CORRECT RESPONSES FOR EACH OF THE ITEMS ON
TEST B
Item I and IV
number II III V VI VII VIII X
1 40 28 21 19 17 20 14 10
2 61 30 20 15 16 16 15 14
3 81 62 52 45 43 43 50 60
4 80 56 38 36 31 33 28 18
5 55 42 40 37 39 41 46 56
6 76 57 57 55 59 61 61 66
7 43 40 33 29 25 24 25 17
8 32 24 18 19 22 18 19 9
9 75 60 53 46 45 44 40 26
10 55 28 21 20 14 18 13 11
11 72 50 29 23 23 23 31 38
12 29 22 19 16 15 14 10 6
13 67 30 24 19 20 25 26 19
14 77 64 57 44 46 41 33 20
15 73 46 34 24 26 22 14 9
16 41 28 27 17 13 17 14 7
17 40 28 22 18 19 19 13 8
18 37 20 19 17 14 14 9 4
19 68 39 32 22 17 17 9 4
20 53 39 32 34 33 27 32 21
21 63 27 16 14 16 13 13 14
22 24 17 14 19 16 13 17 9
23 73 56 54 52 50 51 50 34
24 35 31 27 27 26 29 28 22
25 31 21 21 19 17 21 20 10
of forgetting is more rapid during the first day than during any sub-
sequent period.
The data for items 3, 5, 6, and 11 (Table VI) show that reading or
study of material can be detrimental to success on a test over the
content of that material. Children who had read the article made
lower scores on some tests over these four items than those who had not
read the articles. In the case of these four items, the difference
ascribed to this interference was statistically significant.
The data on items 13, 24, and 25 seem to be evidence for reminis-
cence. However, none of the differences or gains ascribed to reminis-
cence are statistically significant.
When the amount of previous knowledge (last column in Table VI)
is subtracted from the per cents given in the other columns of Table VI,
a very different picture is presented. (See Table VII.) The difficulty
TABLE VII.—ITEMS IN RANK ORDER OF PER CENT ANSWERED CORRECTLY AFTER
SUBTRACTION OP THE AMOTJNT CREDITED TO PREVIOUS KNOWLEDGE
15 64 35 25 15 17 13 5
19 64 35 28 18 13 13 5
4 62 38 20 18 13 15 10
14 57 44 37 24 26 21 13
9 49 34 27 20 19 18 14
13 48 11 5 0 1 6 7
21 47 13 2 0 2 - 1 - 1
2 47 16 6 1 2 2 1
10 44 17 10 9 3 7 2
23 39 22 20 18 16 17 16
11 34 12 - 9 -15 -15 -15 - 7
16 34 21 20 10 6 10 7
18 33 16 15 13 10 10 5
17 32 20 14 10 11 11 5
20 32 18 11 13 12 6 11
1 30 18 11 9 7 10 4
7 26 23 16 12 8 7 8
8 23 15 9 10 13 9 10
12 23 16 13 10 9 8 4
3 21 2 - 8 -15 -17 -17 -10
25 21 11 11 9 7 11 10
22 13 8 5 10 7 4 8
24 13 9 5 5 4 7 6
6 10 - 9 - 9 -11 - 7 - 5 - 5
5 - 1 -14 -16 -19 -17 -15 -10
654 The Journal of Educational Psychology
Group Group
Group
II per II per
VTTT
Differ- Per cent Differ- Per cent
V XXX
Item number cent cent ence for- ence for-
correct correct per cent Bt-B, gotten B.-VIII gotten
on Bi on B,
1 39 25 14 14 36 25 64
2 61 40 15 21 34 46 75
3 81 63 50 18 22 31 38
4 80 67 28 13 16 52 65
5 55 61 46 - 6 -11 9 16
6 76 81 61 - 5 - 7 15 20
7 42 33 25 9 21 17 40
8 31 17 19 14 45 12 39
9 74 65 40 9 12 34 46
10 51 37 13 14 27 38 75
11 72 41 31 31 43 41 57
12 28 21 10 7 25 18 64
13 63 43 26 20 32 37 59
14 76 65 33 11 14 43 57
15 72 55 14 17 24 58 81
16 41 33 14 8 20 27 66
17 40 35 13 5 13 27 68
18 37 37 9 0 0 28 76
19 68 52 9 16 24 58 85
20 54 36 32 18 33 22 41
21 59 37 13 22 37 46 78
22 26 22 17 2 8 9 35
23 69 68 50 1 1 19 28
24 35 37 28 - 2 - 6 7 20
25 31 26 20 5 16 11 35
BIBLIOGRAPHY