100% found this document useful (5 votes)
2K views52 pages

Learning and Teaching Early Math PDF

Uploaded by

Nenad Milinkovic
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
100% found this document useful (5 votes)
2K views52 pages

Learning and Teaching Early Math PDF

Uploaded by

Nenad Milinkovic
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
You are on page 1/ 52

Learning and Teaching Early Math

In this important book for pre- and in-service teachers, early math experts Douglas Clements and
Julie Sarama show how “learning trajectories” help diagnose a child’s level of mathematical under-
standing and provide guidance for teaching. By focusing on the inherent delight and curiosity behind
young children’s mathematical reasoning, learning trajectories ultimately make teaching more
joyous. They help teachers understand the varying levels of knowledge exhibited by individual
students, which, in turn, allows them to better meet the learning needs of all children. Using straight-
forward, no-nonsense language, this book summarizes the current research about how children
learn mathematics, and how teachers can build on what children already know to realize more effec-
tive teaching.
This second edition of Learning and Teaching Early Math remains the definitive, research-based
resource to help teachers understand the learning trajectories of early mathematics and thus to
become quintessential professionals. Updates to the new edition include:

• Explicit connections between “learning trajectories” and the Common Core State Standards;
• New coverage of patterns and patterning;
• The incorporation of hundreds of recent research studies.

Douglas H. Clements is the Kennedy Institute’s Endowed Chair in Early Childhood Learning,
Executive Director of the Marsico Institute of Early Learning and Literacy, part of the Morgridge
College of Education, and a Professor in the Department of Educational Research, Policy, and
Practice at the University of Denver, USA.

Julie Sarama is the Kennedy Institute’s Endowed Chair in Innovative Learning Technologies and a
Professor in the Department of Educational Research, Policy, and Practice at the University of
Denver, USA.
Studies in Mathematical Thinking and Learning
Alan H. Schoenfeld, Series Editor

Artzt, A. F., Armour-Thomas, E., & Curcio, F. R. (2008). Becoming a Reflective Mathematics Teacher:
A Guide for Observations and Self-Assessment (2nd ed.).
Baroody, A. J. & Dowker, A. (2002). The Development of Arithmetic Concepts and Skills: Constructive
Adaptive Expertise.
Boaler, J. (2002). Experiencing School Mathematics: Traditional and Reform Approaches to Teaching
and Their Impact on Student Learning (rev. ed.).
Carpenter, T. P., Fennema, E., & Romberg, T. A. (Eds.) (1992). Rational Numbers: An Integration of
Research.
Chazan, D., Callis, S., & Lehman, M. (2009). Embracing Reason: Egalitarian Ideals and the Teaching of
High School Mathematics.
Clements, D. H. & Sarama, J. (2014). Learning and Teaching Early Math: The Learning Trajectories
Approach (2nd ed.).
Clements, D. H., Sarama, J., & DiBiase, A.-M. (2003). Engaging Young Children in Mathematics:
Standards for Early Childhood Mathematics Education.
Cobb, P. & Bauersfeld, H. (Eds.) (1995). The Emergence of Mathematical Meaning: Interaction in
Classroom Cultures.
Cohen, S. (2004). Teachers’ Professional Development and the Elementary Mathematics Classroom:
Bringing Understandings to Light.
English, L. D. (1997). Mathematical Reasoning: Analogies, Metaphors, and Images.
English, L. D. (2004). Mathematical and Analogical Reasoning of Young Learners.
Fennema, E. & Romberg, T. A. (1999). Mathematics Classrooms that Promote Understanding.
Fennema, E. & Nelson, B. S. (1997). Mathematics Teachers in Transition.
Fernandez, C. & Yoshida, M. (2004). Lesson Study: A Japanese Approach to Improving Mathematics
Teaching and Learning.
Greer, B., Mukhopadhyay, S., Powell, A. B., & Nelson-Barber, S. (Eds.) (2009). Culturally Responsive
Mathematics Education.
Kaput, J. J., Carraher, D. W., & Blanton, M. L. (Eds.) (2007). Algebra in the Early Grades.
Kitchen, R. S. & Civil, M. (Eds.) (2010). Transnational and Borderland Studies in Mathematics
Education.
Lajoie, S. P. (Ed.) (1998). Reflections on Statistics: Learning, Teaching, and Assessment in Grades K–12.
Lehrer, R. & Chazan, D. (1998). Designing Learning Environments for Developing Understanding of
Geometry and Space.
Li, Y. & Huang, R. (Eds.) (2012). How Chinese Teach Mathematics and Improve Teaching.
Ma, L. (2010). Knowing and Teaching Elementary Mathematics: Teachers’ Understanding of
Fundamental Mathematics in China and the United States (2nd ed.).
Martin, D. B. (2006). Mathematics Success and Failure Among African American Youth: The Roles of
Sociohistorical Context, Community Forces, School Influence, and Individual Agency (2nd ed.).
Martin, D. B. (Ed.) (2009). Mathematics Teaching, Learning, and Liberation in the Lives of Black
Children.
Petit, M. M., Laird, R. E., & Marsden, E. L. (2010). A Focus on Fractions: Bringing Research to the
Classroom.
Reed, S. K. (1999). Word Problems: Research and Curriculum Reform.
Remillard, J. T., Herbel-Eisenmann, B. A., & Lloyd, G. M. (Eds.) (2011). Mathematics Teachers at
Work: Connecting Curriculum Materials and Classroom Instruction.
Romberg, T. A. & Shafer, M. C. (2011). The Impact of Reform Instruction on Student Mathematics
Achievement: An Example of a Summative Evaluation of a Standards-Based Curriculum.
Romberg, T. A., Carpenter, T. P., & Dremock, F. (Eds.) (2005). Understanding Mathematics and
Science Matters.
Romberg, T. A., Fennema, E., & Carpenter, T. P. (Eds.) (1993). Integrating Research on the Graphical
Representation of Functions.
Sarama, J. & Clements, D. H. (2009). Early Childhood Mathematics Education Research: Learning
Trajectories for Young Children.
Schliemann, A. D., Carraher, D. W., & Brizuela, B. M. (2006). Bringing Out the Algebraic Character of
Arithmetic: From Children’s Ideas to Classroom Practice.
Schoenfeld, A. H. & Sloane, A. H. (Eds.) (1994). Mathematical Thinking and Problem Solving.
Schoenfeld, A. H. (2010). How We Think: A Theory of Goal-Oriented Decision Making and its
Educational Applications.
Senk, S. L. & Thompson, D. R. (2003). Standards-Based School Mathematics Curricula: What Are
They? What Do Students Learn?
Sherin, M., Jacobs, V., & Philipp, R. (Eds.) (2010). Mathematics Teacher Noticing: Seeing Through
Teachers’ Eyes.
Solomon, Y. (2008). Mathematical Literacy: Developing Identities of Inclusion.
Sophian, C. (2007). The Origins of Mathematical Knowledge in Childhood.
Sternberg, R. J. & Ben-Zeev, T. (Eds.) (1996). The Nature of Mathematical Thinking.
Stylianou, D. A., Blanton, M. L., & Knuth, E. J. (Eds.) (2010). Teaching and Learning Proof Across the
Grades: A K–16 Perspective.
Sultan, A. & Artzt, A. F. (2010). The Mathematics that Every Secondary School Math Teacher Needs to
Know.
Watson, A. & Mason, J. (2005). Mathematics as a Constructive Activity: Learners Generating Examples.
Watson, J. M. (2006). Statistical Literacy at School: Growth and Goals.
Wilcox, S. K. & Lanier, P. E. (2000). Using Assessment to Reshape Mathematics Teaching: A Casebook
for Teachers and Teacher Educators, Curriculum and Staff Development Specialists.
Wood, T., Nelson, B. S., & Warfield, J. E. (2001). Beyond Classical Pedagogy: Teaching Elementary
School Mathematics.
Zazkis, R. & Campbell, S. R. (Eds.) (2006). Number Theory in Mathematics Education: Perspectives
and Prospects.
This page intentionally left bank
Learning and Teaching Early Math
The Learning Trajectories Approach

Second edition

Douglas H. Clements and Julie Sarama


Second edition published 2014
by Routledge
8th Floor, 711 Third Avenue, New York, NY 10017, USA
and by Routledge
2 Park Square, Milton Park, Abingdon, Oxford, OX14 4RN, UK
Routledge is an imprint of the Taylor & Francis Group, an informa business
© 2014 Taylor & Francis
The right of Douglas H. Clements and Julie Sarama to be identified as
authors of this work has been asserted by them in accordance with
sections 77 and 78 of the Copyright, Designs and Patents Act 1988.
All rights reserved. No part of this book may be reprinted or reproduced or
utilized in any form or by any electronic, mechanical, or other means,
now known or hereafter invented, including photocopying and recording,
or in any information storage or retrieval system, without permission in
writing from the publishers.
Trademark notice: Product or corporate names may be trademarks or
registered trademarks, and are used only for identification and
explanation without intent to infringe.
First edition published in 2009 by Taylor & Francis
Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data
Clements, Douglas H.
Learning and teaching early math : the learning trajectories approach /
Douglas H. Clements, Julie A. Sarama. – Second edition.
pages cm. – (Studies in mathematical thinking and learning series)
Includes bibliographical references and index. 1. Mathematics–Study and
teaching (Early childhood) 2. Educational psychology. 3. Child
development. 4. Curriculum planning. I. Sarama, Julie. II. Title.
QA135.6.C55 2014
372.7’049–dc23
2013037230

ISBN: 978–0–415–82851–2 (hbk)


ISBN: 978–0–415–82850–5 (pbk)
ISBN: 978–0–203–52057–4 (ebk)

Typeset in Minion
by RefineCatch Limited, Bungay, Suffolk, UK
Contents

Preface ix
Acknowledgments xii

1. Young Children and Mathematics Learning 1


2. Quantity, Number, and Subitizing 9
3. Verbal and Object Counting 22
4. Comparing, Ordering, and Estimating 50
5. Arithmetic: Early Addition and Subtraction and Counting Strategies 68
6. Arithmetic: Composition of Number, Place Value, and Multidigit Addition
and Subtraction 93
7. Spatial Thinking 124
8. Shape 141
9. Composition and Decomposition of Shapes 170
10. Geometric Measurement: Length 186
11. Geometric Measurement: Area, Volume, and Angle 198
12. Other Content Domains 214
13. Mathematical Processes and Practices 230
14. Cognition, Affect, and Equity 237
15. Early Childhood Mathematics Education: Contexts and Curricula 266
16. Instructional Practices and Pedagogical Issues 291

References 334
Index 363

vii
This page intentionally left bank
Preface

Who dares to teach must never cease to learn.


(John Cotton Dana, 1856–1929)

Think of the biggest number you can. Now add five. Then, imagine if
you had that many Twinkies. Wow, that’s five more than the biggest
number you could come up with!
(Child, age 6)

Everyone knows that effective teaching involves “meeting the students where they are” and helping
them build on what they know. But that’s easier said than done. Which aspects of mathematics
are important, which less so? How do we diagnose what a child knows? How do we build on that
knowledge—in which directions, and in what ways?
We believe that “learning trajectories” answer these questions and help teachers become more
effective professionals. Just as importantly, they open up windows to seeing young children and math
in new ways, making teaching more joyous because the mathematical reasoning of children is
impressive and delightful.
Learning trajectories have three parts: (a) a specific mathematical goal, (b) a path along which
children develop to reach that goal, and (c) a set of instructional activities that help children move
along that path. So, teachers who understand learning trajectories understand the math, the way
children think and learn about math, and how to help children learn it better.
Learning trajectories connect research and practice. They connect children to math. They connect
teachers to children. They help teachers understand the level of knowledge and thinking of their
classes and the individuals in their classes as key in serving the needs of all children. (Equity issues
are important to us and to the nation. This entire book is designed to help you teach all children, but
equity concerns are discussed specifically and at length in Chapters 14, 15, and 16.) Learning and
Teaching Early Math will help you understand the learning trajectories of early mathematics and so
to become a quintessential professional.
Learning and teaching, of course, take place in a context. For the last decade, we have had
the honor and advantage of working with several hundred early childhood teachers, who have
worked with us creating new ideas for teaching and invited us into their classrooms to test these
ideas with the children in their charge. We wish to share with you a bit about this collaborative
work.

ix
x • Preface

Background
In 1998, we began a four-year project funded by the National Science Foundation (NSF). The
purpose of “Building Blocks—Foundations for Mathematical Thinking, Pre-Kindergarten to Grade
2: Research-Based Materials Development” was to create and evaluate mathematics curricula for
young children based on a theoretically sound research and development framework. Based
on theory and research on early childhood learning and teaching, we determined that Building
Blocks’ basic approach would be finding the mathematics in, and developing mathematics from, chil-
dren’s activity. To achieve this, all aspects of the Building Blocks project have been based on learning
trajectories. Teachers have found this combination of the Building Blocks’ approach and
learning trajectories to be a powerful teaching tool.
More than a decade later, we are still finding new opportunities for exciting research and develop-
ment in early mathematics. Funding from the U.S. Department of Education’s Institute of Education
Sciences (IES) and the NSF has allowed us to work closely with hundreds of teachers and thousands
of children over the past ten years. All of these agencies and individuals have contributed ideas to
this book and its companion. In addition, these projects have increased our confidence that our
approach, based on learning trajectories and rigorous empirical testing at every step, can, in turn,
make a contribution to all educators in the field of early mathematics. The model for working
with educators in all positions—from teachers to administrators, trainers to researchers—has been
developed with IES funding to our TRIAD (technology-enhanced, research-based instruction,
assessment, and professional development)1 project.

The “Companion” Books


We believe that our successes are due to the people who have contributed to our projects, as well
as to our commitment to grounding everything that we have done in research. Because the work
has been so drenched in research, we decided to publish two books. The companion to the
present book—Early Childhood Mathematics Education Research: Learning Trajectories for Young
Children (Sarama & Clements, 2009)—reviews the research underlying our learning trajectories,
emphasizing the research that describes the paths of learning; that is, children’s natural progressions
in developing the concepts and skills within a certain domain of mathematics (most research
citations for these are in the companion book, although we have added recent ones to this edition).
The present book describes and illustrates how these learning trajectories can be implemented in the
classroom.

Reading this Book


In straightforward, no-nonsense language, we summarize what is known about how children learn,
and how to build on what they know. In Chapter 1, we introduce the topic of mathematics education
for very young children. We discuss why people are particularly interested in engaging young
children with mathematics and what President Bush’s National Mathematics Advisory Panel (often
called the “National Math Panel” or NMP) (NMP, 2008) recommended. Next, we describe the idea
of learning trajectories in detail. We end with an introduction to the Building Blocks project and
explain how learning trajectories are at its core.
Most of the following chapters address one mathematics topic, and we describe how children
understand and learn about that topic. These descriptions are brief summaries of the more elaborate
reviews of the research that can be found in the aforementioned companion book, Early Childhood
Mathematics Education Research: Learning Trajectories for Young Children (Sarama & Clements,
2009). Next, we describe how experiences—from the beginning of life—and classroom-based
Preface • xi

education affect children’s learning of the topic. Chapters 2 to 11 then culminate in a detailed
description of learning trajectories for the chapter’s topic.
Read more than the topic chapters, even if you just want to teach a topic! In the last three chapters,
we discuss issues that are important for putting these ideas into practice. In Chapter 14, we describe
how children think about mathematics and how their feelings are involved. Equity concerns complete
that chapter. In Chapter 15, we discuss the contexts in which early childhood education occurs, and
the curricula that are used. In Chapter 16, we review what we know about specific instructional prac-
tices. The topics of these three chapters are unique to this book. Because there are no corresponding
chapters in the companion book for these three important chapters, we review more research in this
book. We have made the implications for practitioners clear.
To teach children with different needs, and to teach effectively, make sure you read Chapters 14, 15,
and especially 16. Some readers may wish to read those chapters immediately after having read
Chapter 1! Whichever way you choose, please know that the learning trajectories that describe
children’s learning and effective teaching for each topic are only part of the story—the other, critical
part is found in those final three chapters.
This is not a typical book of “cute teaching ideas.” We believe, however, that it may be the most
practical book that you, as a teacher of early mathematics, could read. The many teachers with whom
we have worked claim that, once they understood the learning trajectories and ways to implement
them in their classrooms, they—and the children they teach—were changed for the better forever.
Moreover, they also changed their beliefs, shedding the unfortunate misconceptions that many
teachers hold about early mathematics education, such as:

• Young children are not ready for mathematics education.


• Mathematics is for some bright kids with mathematics genes.
• Simple numbers and shapes are enough.
• Language and literacy are more important than mathematics.
• Teachers should provide an enriched physical environment, step back, and let the children
play.
• Mathematics should not be taught as a stand-alone subject matter.
• Assessment in mathematics is irrelevant when it comes to young children.
• Children learn mathematics only by interacting with concrete objects.
• Computers are inappropriate for the teaching and learning of mathematics.
(From Sun Lee & Ginsburg, 2009)

Note
1 Like many acronyms, TRIAD almost works . . . we jokingly ask people to accept the “silent p” in “professional development.”
Acknowledgments

Appreciation to the Funding Agencies


We wish to express our appreciation for the funding agencies that have not only provided financial
support but also intellectual support, in the form of guidance from program officers (most notably
and recently, Caroline Ebanks and Christina S. Chhin from the IES and Edith S. Gummer from the
NSF) as well as opportunities to collaborate with other projects and attend conferences to exchange
ideas with colleagues.
The ideas and research reported here have been supported by all of the following grants. Any
opinions, findings, and conclusions or recommendations expressed in this material are those of the
authors and do not necessarily reflect the views of the funding agencies.

Grants
Barrett, J., Clements, D. H., & Sarama, J. A longitudinal account of children’s knowledge of measure-
ment. Awarded by the NSF (Directorate for Education & Human Resources (EHR), Division of
Research on Learning in Formal and Informal Settings (DRL)), award no. DRL-0732217. Arlington,
VA: NSF.

xii
Acknowledgments • xiii

Barrett, J., Clements, D. H., Sarama, J., & Cullen, C. Learning trajectories to support the growth of
measurement knowledge: Prekindergarten through middle school. Awarded by the NSF (EHR, DRL),
award no. DRL-1222944. Arlington, VA: NSF.
Clements, D. H. & Sarama, J. Building blocks—Foundations for mathematical thinking, prekinder-
garten to grade 2: Research-based materials development. Awarded by the NSF (EHR, Division of
Elementary, Secondary & Informal Education (ESIE), Instructional Materials Development (IMD)
program), award no. ESI-9730804. Arlington, VA: NSF.
Clements, D. H. & Sarama, J. Scaling up TRIAD: Teaching early mathematics for understanding with
trajectories and technologies—Supplement. Awarded by the IES as part of the Interagency Education
Research Initiative (IERI) program, a combination of the IES, the NSF (EHR, Division of Research,
Evaluation and Communication (REC)), and the National Institutes of Health (NIH) (National
Institute of Child Health and Human Development (NICHD)). Washington, D.C.: IES.
Clements, D. H. Conference on standards for preschool and kindergarten mathematics education.
Supported in part by the NSF (EHR, ESIE) and the ExxonMobil Foundation, award no. ESI-9817540.
Arlington, VA: NSF. In Clements, D. H., Sarama, J., & DiBiase, A.-M. (Eds.). (2004). Engaging young
children in mathematics: Standards for early childhood mathematics education. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates.
Clements, D. H., Sarama, J., & Layzer, C. Longitudinal study of a successful scaling-up project: Extending
TRIAD. Awarded by the IES (Mathematics and Science Education program), award no. R305A110188.
Washington, D.C.: National Center for Education Research (NCER), IES.
Clements, D. H., Sarama, J., & Lee, J. Scaling up TRIAD: Teaching early mathematics for understanding
with trajectories and technologies. Awarded by the IES as part of the IERI program, a combination of
the IES, the NSF (EHR, REC), and the NIH (NICHD). Washington, D.C.: IES.
Clements, D. H., Sarama, J., & Tatsuoka, C. Using rule space and poset-based adaptive testing method-
ologies to identify ability patterns in early mathematics and create a comprehensive mathematics ability
test. Awarded by the NSF, award no. 1313695 (previously funded under award no. DRL-1019925).
Arlington, VA: NSF.
Clements, D. H., Sarama, J., Bodrova, E., & Layzer, C. Increasing the efficacy of an early mathematics
curriculum with scaffolding designed to promote self-regulation. Awarded by the IES, Early Learning
Programs and Policies program, award no. R305A080200. Washington, D.C.: NCER, IES.
Clements, D. H., Sarama, J., Klein, A., & Starkey, P. Scaling up the implementation of a pre-kindergarten
mathematics curricula: Teaching for understanding with trajectories and technologies. Awarded by the
NSF as part of the IERI program, a combination of the NSF (EHR, REC), the IES, and the NIH
(NICHD). Arlington, VA: NSF.
Clements, D. H., Watt, D., Bjork, E., & Lehrer, R. Technology-enhanced learning of geometry in elemen-
tary schools. Awarded by the NSF (EHR, ESIE), Research on Education, Policy and Practice (REPP)
program. Arlington, VA: NSF.
Sarama, J. & Clements, D. H. Planning for professional development in pre-school mathematics: Meeting
the challenge of Standards 2000. Awarded by the NSF (EHR, ESIE), Teacher Enhancement (TE)
program, award no. ESI-9814218. Arlington, VA: NSF.
Sarama, J., Clements, D. H., Duke, N., & Brenneman, K. Early childhood education in the context of
mathematics, science, and literacy. Awarded by the NSF, award no. 1313718 (previously funded under
award no. DRL-1020118). Arlington, VA: NSF.
Starkey, Prentice, Sarama, J., Clements, D. H., & Klein, A. A longitudinal study of the effects of a prekin-
dergarten mathematics curriculum on low-income children’s mathematical knowledge. Awarded by the
Office of Educational Research and Improvement (OERI), U.S. Department of Education, as
Preschool Curriculum Evaluation Research (PCER) project. Washington, D.C.: OERI.
xiv • Acknowledgments

Appreciation to SRA/McGraw-Hill
The author and publisher wish to express appreciation to SRA/McGraw-Hill for kindly giving
permission for the many screen shots provided by them for use throughout this title.
1
Young Children and Mathematics Learning

Snow was falling in Boston and preschool teacher Sarah Gardner’s chil-
dren were coming in slowly, one bus at a time. She had been doing high-
quality mathematics all year, but was still amazed at her children’s ability
to keep track of the situation: The children kept saying, “Now, 11 are
here and 7 absent. Now, 13 are here and 5 absent. Now . . . .”

Why are so many people interested in mathe-


matics for very young children?1 According to the During most of the 20th century, the
findings of the National Mathematics Advisory United States possessed peerless
Panel created by former U.S. President George mathematical prowess—not just as
W. Bush, mathematics is increasingly important measured by the depth and number of the
in a modern global economy, but mathematics mathematical specialists who practiced
achievement in the USA has been declining. Also, here but also by the scale and quality of its
U.S. math achievement is far lower than that of engineering, science, and financial
most other countries, as early as first grade, leadership, and even by the extent of
kindergarten . . . and even preschool (e.g., Mullis, mathematical education in its broad
Martin, Foy, & Arora, 2012; see Sarama & population. But, without substantial and
Clements, 2009, for full references)! American sustained changes to its educational
children do not even get the chance to learn the system, the United States will relinquish
more advanced mathematics taught in many its leadership in the 21st century.
other countries (again, remember that most The National Math Panel2 (NMP, 2008,
research references are in the companion book: p. xi)
Sarama & Clements, 2009).
An even larger and more damaging gap lies
between children growing up in higher- and lower-resource communities. Both the income gap and
the achievement gap have been increasing for decades (Reardon, 2011). For these children especially,
the long-term success of their learning and development requires high-quality experience during
their early “years of promise” (Carnegie Corporation, 1998). These early years have been found to be
especially important for mathematics development. From the first years of life, children have an ability
to learn math and develop their interest in math. What they know when they enter kindergarten and

1
2 • Mathematics Learning

first grade predicts their mathematics achieve-


ment for years to come—even throughout their Most children acquire considerable
school career. Moreover, what they know in math knowledge of numbers and other aspects
predicts their reading achievement later. Their of mathematics before they enter
early knowledge of literacy also predicts their later kindergarten. This is important, because
reading ability . . . but that’s all. Because math the mathematical knowledge that
predicts later math and later reading, mathematics kindergartners bring to school is related to
appears to be a core component of cognition their mathematics learning for years
(Duncan et al., 2007; Duncan & Magnuson, 2011). thereafter—in elementary school,
If our country’s children have limited knowledge middle school, and even high school.
initially and achieve less later in school, compared Unfortunately, most children from
to children in other countries, can there possibly be low-income backgrounds enter school
a bright spot? Yes. In high-quality early childhood with far less knowledge than peers
education programs, young children can engage in from middle-income backgrounds,
surprisingly deep investigations of mathematics and the achievement gap in mathematical
ideas. They can learn skills, problem solving, and knowledge progressively widens
concepts in ways that are natural and motivating to throughout their pre-K–12 years.
them. This brings us to the main reason to engage The National Math Panel (NMP, 2008,
young children in mathematics: Young children love p. xvii)
to think mathematically. They become exhilarated
by their own ideas (like the 6-year-old quoted at
the beginning of the preface) and the ideas of
others. To develop the whole child, we must develop Fortunately, encouraging results have
the mathematical child. Further, teachers enjoy been obtained for a variety of
the reasoning and learning that high-quality instructional programs developed to
mathematics education brings forth from their improve the mathematical knowledge of
children. High-quality mathematics throughout preschoolers and kindergartners,
early childhood does not involve pushing elemen- especially those from low-income
tary arithmetic onto younger children. Instead, backgrounds. There are effective
good education allows children to experience techniques—derived from scientific
mathematics as they play in and explore their world. research on learning—that could be
A higher proportion of children are in early care put to work in the classroom today to
and education programs every year. We teachers improve children’s mathematical
are responsible for bringing the knowledge and knowledge.
intellectual delight of mathematics to all children, The National Math Panel (NMP, 2008,
especially those who have not yet had many high- p. xvii)
quality educational experiences. Good teachers can
meet this challenge with research-based “tools.”
These tools include specific guidance on how to help children learn in ways that are both appro-
priate and effective. In this book, we pull that knowledge together to provide a core tool: “learning
trajectories” for each major topic in early mathematics.

What are Learning Trajectories?


Children follow natural developmental progressions in learning and development. As a simple
example, they learn to crawl, then walk, then run, skip, and jump with increasing speed and dexterity.
These are levels in the developmental progression of movement. Children follow natural develop-
mental progressions in learning math, too, learning mathematical ideas and skills in their own way.
Mathematics Learning • 3

Figure 1.1 Carmen Brown encourages a preschooler to “mathematize.”

When teachers understand these developmental progressions for each major domain or topic of
mathematics, and sequence activities based on them, they build mathematics learning environments
that are particularly developmentally appropriate and effective. These developmental paths are the
basis for this book’s “learning trajectories.” Learning trajectories help us answer several questions:
What objectives should we establish? Where do we start? How do we know where to go next? How
do we get there?
Learning trajectories have three parts: (a) a mathematical goal, (b) a developmental path along
which children develop to reach that goal, and (c) a set of instructional activities, or tasks, matched
to each of the levels of thinking in that path that help children develop ever-higher levels of thinking.
Let’s examine each of these three parts.

Goals: The Big Ideas of Mathematics


The first part of a learning trajectory is a mathematical goal. Our goals include the “big ideas of
mathematics”—clusters of concepts and skills that are mathematically central and coherent, consis-
tent with children’s thinking, and generative of future learning. These big ideas come from several
large projects, including those from the National Council of Teachers of Mathematics (NCTM) and
the National Mathematics Advisory Panel (NMP) (Clements & Conference Working Group, 2004;
NCTM, 2006; NMP, 2008) and especially the Common Core State Standards (CCSSO/NGA, 2010).
For example, one big idea is counting can be used to find out how many in a collection.

Development Progressions: The Paths of Learning


The second part of a learning trajectory consists Humans are born with a fundamental
of levels of thinking, each more sophisticated than sense of quantity.
the last, through which children develop on their (Geary, 1994, p. 1)
way to achieving the mathematical goal. That is,
4 • Mathematics Learning

the developmental progression describes a typical path children follow in developing an under-
standing and skill about that mathematical topic.
This development of mathematics abilities begins when life begins. As we will see, young
children have certain mathematical-like competencies in number, spatial sense, and patterns from
birth. However, young children’s ideas and their interpretations of situations are uniquely different
from those of adults. For this reason, good early childhood teachers are careful not to assume that
children “see” situations, problems, or solutions the way adults do. Instead, good teachers interpret
what the child is doing and thinking, and attempt to see the situation from the child’s point of view.
Similarly, when they interact with the child, these teachers also consider the instructional tasks and
their own actions from the child’s point of view so they can help the child develop the next level of
thinking. This makes early childhood teaching both demanding and rewarding.
Our learning trajectories provide simple labels and examples for each level of each developmental
progression. The “Developmental Progression” column in Table 1.1 describes three main levels of
thinking in the counting learning trajectory (this is just a sample of levels that actually have other
levels in between them—the full learning trajectory is described in Chapter 3). Under each descrip-
tion is an example of children’s thinking and behavior for each level.

Instructional Tasks: The Paths of Teaching


The third part of a learning trajectory consists of a set of instructional tasks matched to each of the
levels of thinking in the developmental progression. These tasks are designed to help children learn

Table 1.1 Samples from the Learning Trajectory for Counting.

Age Developmental Progression Instructional Tasks

1–2 Chanter Verbal Chants “sing-song” or sometimes Repeated experience with the counting sequence in
indistinguishable number words. varied context. This can include songs; finger plays,
Count for me. such as “This Old Man”; counting going up and
“One, two-twee, four, sev-, en, ten.” down stairs; and just verbal counting for the fun of it
(how high can you go?)!
3 Corresponder Keeps one-to-one correspondence Kitchen Counter: Students click on objects one at a
between counting words and objects (one word for time while the numbers from one to ten are counted
each object), at least for small groups of objects laid aloud. For example, they click on pieces of food and
in a line. a bite is taken out of each as it is counted.

Counts:
■ ■ ■ ■
“1, 2, 3, 4”
But may answer the question, “How many?” by
re-counting the objects or naming any number
word.

Counter (10) Counts arrangements of objects to 10. Counting Towers (Up to 10): A day before, read
May be able to write numerals to represent 1 to 10. Shape Space. Ask what shapes work well in which
May be able to tell the number just after or just part of a tower. Set up stations with different
before another number, but only by counting up objects to stack. Encourage children to stack as
from 1. many as they can, and count them to see how
Accurately counts a line of 9 blocks and says there are 9. many they stacked.
What comes after 4? “1, 2, 3, 4, 5. 5!”
Mathematics Learning • 5

the ideas and skills needed to achieve that level of thinking. That is, as teachers, we can use these tasks
to promote children’s growth from the previous level to the target level. The last column of Table 1.1
provides example instructional tasks. (Again, the complete learning trajectory in Chapter 3 includes
not only all the developmental levels but also several instructional tasks for each level.)
In summary, learning trajectories describe the goals of learning, the thinking and learning
processes of children at various levels, and the learning activities in which they might engage. People
often have several questions about learning trajectories. You may wish to read our responses to those
questions that interest you now and return to this section after you read more about specific learning
trajectories in the chapters that follow.

Frequently Asked Questions (FAQ) about Learning Trajectories


Why Use Learning Trajectories? Learning trajectories allow teachers to build the mathematics of
children—the thinking of children as it develops naturally. Because they are formed on research of
children’s natural thinking, we know that all the goals and activities are within the developmental
capacities of children. We also know that each level provides a natural developmental building block
to the next level. Finally, we know that the activities provide the mathematical building blocks for
school success, because the research on which they are based typically involves more children who
have had the educational advantages that allow them to do well at school.
When are Children “At” a Level? Children are identified to be “at” a certain level when most of their
behaviors reflect the thinking—ideas and skills—of that level. Often, they show a few behaviors from
the next and previous levels as they learn.
Can Children Work at More Than One Level at the Same Time? Yes, although most children work
mainly at one level or in transition between two levels (of course, if they are tired or distracted, they
may operate at a lower level). Levels are not “absolute stages.” They are “benchmarks” of complex
growth that represent distinct ways of thinking. So, another way to think of them is as a sequence of
different patterns of thinking and reasoning. Children are continually learning, within levels and
then moving from one level to the next.
Can Children Jump Ahead? Yes, especially if there are separate “subtopics.” For example, we have
combined many counting competencies into one “counting” sequence with subtopics, such as verbal
counting skills. Many children learn to count to 100 at age 6 after learning to count objects to 10 or
more; some may learn that verbal skill earlier. The subtopic of verbal counting skills would still be
followed. There is another possibility: Children may learn deeply and thus appear to jump ahead
several “levels” after a rich learning experience.
Are all Levels Similar in Nature? Most levels are levels of thinking—a distinct period of time of
qualitatively distinct ways, or patterns, of thinking. However, a few are merely “levels of attain-
ment,” similar to a mark on a wall to show a child’s height; that is, a couple signify simply that a child
has gained more knowledge. For example, consider reading numerals such as “2” or “9.” Children
do following a learning trajectory of first matching, then recognizing, then naming numerals (Wang,
Resnick, & Boozer, 1971). However, once they have reached that level, children must learn simply to
name (and write) more numerals, but at this level, that does not require deeper or more complex
thinking. Thus, some trajectories are more tightly constrained by natural cognitive development
than others. Often a critical component of such constraints is the mathematical development in a
domain; that is, mathematics is a highly sequential, hierarchical domain in which certain ideas and
skills must be learned before others.
How are Learning Trajectories Different from just a Scope and Sequence? They are related, of course.
But they are not lists of everything children need to learn, because they don’t cover every single “fact”
and they emphasize the “big ideas.” Further, they are about children’s levels of thinking, not just
6 • Mathematics Learning

about the answer to a mathematics question. So, for example, a single mathematical problem may be
solved differently by students at different (separable) levels of thinking.
Does Every Trajectory Represent Just “One Path”? In broad terms, there is one main developmental
path; however, for some topics, there are “subtrajectories”—strands within the topic. For example, as
previously stated, the counting learning trajectory in Chapter 3 includes both verbal and object
counting—they are related, but can develop somewhat independently. In some cases, the names
make this clear. For example, in Comparing and Ordering, some levels are about the “Comparer”
levels, and others about building a “mental number line.” Similarly, the related subtrajectories of
“Composition” and “Decomposition” are easy to distinguish. Sometimes, for clarification, subtrajec-
tories are indicated with a note in italics after the title. For example, in Shapes, “Parts” and
“Representing” are subtrajectories within the Shapes trajectory.

Frequently Asked Questions (FAQ) about Using the Learning Trajectories


How Do These Developmental Levels Support Teaching and Learning? The levels help teachers, as well
as curriculum developers, assess, teach, and sequence activities. Teachers who understand learning
trajectories (especially the developmental levels that are at their foundation) are more effective and effi-
cient. Through planned teaching and also by encouraging informal, incidental mathematics, teachers
help children learn at an appropriate and deep level.
There are Ages in the Charts. Should I Plan to Help Children Develop Just the Levels that Correspond
to my Children’s Ages? No! The ages in the table are typical ages at which children develop these ideas.
But these are rough guides only—children differ widely. Furthermore, the ages are often lower
bounds on what children achieve without high-quality instruction. So, these are “starting levels,”
not goals. We have found that children who are provided high-quality mathematics experiences are
capable of developing to levels one or more years beyond their peers.
Are the Instructional Tasks the Only Way to Teach Children to Achieve Higher Levels of Thinking? No,
there are many ways. In some cases, however, there is some research evidence that these are especially
effective ways. In other cases, they are simply illustrations of the kind of activity that would be appro-
priate to reach that level of thinking. Further, teachers need to use a variety of pedagogical strategies
in teaching the content, presenting the tasks, guiding children in completing them, and so forth.
Are Learning Trajectories Consistent with Teaching the Common Core? Unfortunately, some people
have interpreted that “teaching the Common Core” means teaching each standard only once and
then moving on. Our view and that of many studies is that learning is not an all-or-nothing acquisi-
tion of knowledge or skills (Sarama & Clements, 2009; Sophian, 2013). The Common Core and CFP
goals are benchmarks, but good curricula and teaching always weave the learning trajectories
throughout children’s lives. They learn the ideas at higher levels of sophistication and generality.
Finally, when we wrote the Common Core we started by writing learning trajectories—at least the goals
and developmental progressions. Thus, learning trajectories are at the core of the Common Core. And
learning trajectories are not based on the idea to “teach once and drop it.”

Other Critical Goals: Strategies, Reasoning, Creativity, and a Productive Disposition


Learning trajectories are organized around topics, but they include far more than facts and ideas.
Processes, or mathematical practices, and attitudes are important in every one. Chapter 13 focuses
on general processes, such as problem solving and reasoning. But these general processes are also
an integral part of every learning trajectory. Also, specific processes are involved in every learning
trajectory. For example, the process of composition—putting together and taking apart—is
fundamental to both number and arithmetic (e.g., adding and subtracting) and geometry (shape
composition).
Mathematics Learning • 7

Finally, other general educational goals must


never be neglected. The “habits of mind” mentioned As important as mathematical content
in the box include curiosity, imagination, inven- are general mathematical processes such
tiveness, risk-taking, creativity, and persistence. as problem solving, reasoning and proof,
These are some of the components of the essential communication, connections, and
goal of productive disposition. Children need to representation; specific mathematical
view mathematics as sensible, useful, and worth- processes such as organizing information,
while and view themselves as capable of thinking patterning, and composing, and habits
mathematically. Children should also come to of mind such as curiosity, imagination,
appreciate the beauty and creativity that is at the inventiveness, persistence, willingness
heart of mathematics. to experiment, and sensitivity to
All these should be involved in a high-quality patterns. All should be involved in a
early childhood mathematics program. These high-quality early childhood
goals are included in the suggestions for teaching mathematics program.
throughout this book. Further, Chapters 14, 15, (Clements & Conference Working
and 16 discuss how to achieve these goals. These Group, 2004, p. 57)
chapters discuss different learning and teaching
contexts, including early childhood school settings
and education, equity issues, affect, and instruc-
tional strategies.

The overriding premise of our work is that


Learning Trajectories and the “Building Blocks”
throughout the grades from pre-K through
Project
8 all students can and should be
The “Building Blocks” project was funded by the mathematically proficient. [p. 10]
National Science Foundation (NSF)3 to develop
Mathematical proficiency . . . has five
prekindergarten (pre-K) to grade 2, software-
strands:
enhanced, mathematics curricula. Building Blocks
was designed to enable all young children to build 1. conceptual understanding—
mathematics concepts, skills, and processes. The comprehension of mathematical
name “Building Blocks” has three meanings (see concepts, operations, and relations
Figure 1.2). First, our goals are to help children 2. procedural fluency—skill in carrying
develop the main mathematical building blocks— out procedures flexibly, accurately,
that is, the big ideas described previously. Second efficiently, and appropriately
is the related goal to develop cognitive building 3. strategic competence—ability to
blocks: general cognitive and metacognitive formulate, represent, and solve
(higher-order) processes such as moving or mathematical problems
combining shapes to higher-order thinking 4. adaptive reasoning—capacity for
processes such as self regulation. The third is the logical thought, reflection, explanation,
most straightforward—children should be using and justification
building blocks for many purposes, but one of 5. productive disposition—habitual
them is for learning mathematics. inclination to see mathematics as
Based on theory and research on early child- sensible, useful, and worthwhile,
hood learning and teaching (Bowman, Donovan, coupled with a belief in diligence and
& Burns, 2001; Clements, 2001), we determined one’s own efficacy.
that Building Blocks’ basic approach would be (Kilpatrick, Swafford, & Findell, 2001,
finding the mathematics in, and developing mathe- p. 5)
matics from, children’s activity. To do so, all aspects
8 • Mathematics Learning

Figure 1.2 The Building Blocks project was named because we wanted to use manipulatives like children’s building blocks (on and
off the computer) to help children develop mathematical and cognitive building blocks—the foundations for later learning (see http://
buildingblocksmath.org).

of the Building Blocks project are based on learning trajectories. Therefore, most of the examples of
learning trajectories stemmed from our work developing, field-testing, and evaluating curricula
from that project.

Final Words
Against this background, let us explore the learning trajectories in Chapters 2 through 12. Chapter 2
begins with the critical topic of number. When do children first understand number? How do they
do it? How can we help children’s initial ideas develop? Throughout, we emphasize mathematical
processes, or practices, and attitudes. Further, the last few chapters provide guidance regarding
understanding children, communities, and cultures and tools such as effective teaching strategies.
You may want to at least skim Chapter 13 before reading the following chapters on learning
trajectories.

Notes
1 As stated in the Preface, an elaborate review of the research supporting this and all other statements in this book can be found in the
companion book, Early Childhood Mathematics Education Research: Learning Trajectories for Young Children (Sarama & Clements, 2009).
2 One of the authors, Douglas Clements, was a member of the National Math Panel and co-author of the report, which can be found at
http://www.ed.gov/about/bdscomm/list/mathpanel/.
3 The “Building Blocks—Foundations for Mathematical Thinking, Prekindergarten to Grade 2: Research-based Materials Development”
project was funded by the NSF (award no. ESI-9730804; granted to D. H. Clements and J. Sarama) to create and evaluate mathematics
curricula for young children based on a theoretically sound research and development framework. We describe the framework and re-
search in detail in Chapter 15. For the purposes of full disclosure, note that we have subsequently made this curriculum available through
a publisher and thus receive royalties. All research was conducted with independent assessors and evaluators.
References

Adey, P., Robertson, A., & Venville, G. (2002). Effects of a cognitive acceleration programme on Year 1 pupils. British Journal of
Educational Psychology, 72(Pt. 1), 1–5.
Agodini, R., & Harris, B. (2010). An experimental evaluation of four elementary school math curricula. Journal of Research on
Educational Effectiveness, 3(3), 199–253. doi: 10.1080/19345741003770693
Aleven, V. A. W. M. M., & Koedinger, K. R. (2002). An effective metacognitive strategy: Learning by doing and explaining with a
computer-based Cognitive Tutor. Cognitive Science, 26(2), 147–179.
Alexander, K. L., & Entwisle, D. R. (1988). Achievement in the first 2 years of school: Patterns and processes. Monographs of the
Society for Research in Child Development, 53(2), 1–157.
Alfieri, L., Brooks, P. J., Aldrich, N. J., & Tenenbaum, H. R. (2010). Does discovery-based instruction enhance learning? Journal of
Educational Psychology, 103(1), 1–18. doi: 10.1037/a0021017
Allen, J., Watson, J. A., & Howard, J. R. (1993). The impact of cognitive styles on the problem solving strategies used by preschool
minority children in Logo MicroWorlds. Journal of Computing in Childhood Education, 4(3–4), 205–217.
Anderson, A., Anderson, J., & Shapiro, J. (2004). Mathematical discourse in shared storybook reading. Journal for Research in
Mathematics Education, 35(1), 5–33.
Anderson, G. R. (1957). Visual-tactual devices and their efficacy: An experiment in grade eight. Arithmetic Teacher, 4(5),
196–203.
Anderson, J. R. (Ed.). (1993). Rules of the mind. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Anghileri, J. (2001). What are we trying to achieve in teaching standard calculating procedures? In M. v. d. Heuvel-Panhuizen
(Ed.), Proceedings of the 25th Conference of the International Group for the Psychology in Mathematics Education (Vol. 2,
pp. 41–48). Utrecht, the Netherlands: Freudenthal Institute.
Anghileri, J. (2004). Disciplined calculators or flexible problem solvers? In M. J. Høines & A. B. Fuglestad (Eds.), Proceedings of the
28th Conference of the International Group for the Psychology in Mathematics Education (Vol. 2, pp. 41–46). Bergen, Norway:
Bergen University College.
Ansari, D., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2002). Atypical trajectories of number development: A neuroconstructivist perspective. Trends
in Cognitive Sciences, 6(12), 511–516.
Anthony, J., Hecht, S. A., Williams, J., Clements, D. H., & Sarama, J. (2011). Efficacy of computerized Earobics and Building Blocks
instruction for kindergarteners from low SES, minority, and ELL backgrounds: Year 2 results. Paper presented at the Institute of
Educational Sciences Research Conference, Washington, D.C.
Arditi, A., Holtzman, J. D., & Kosslyn, S. M. (1988). Mental imagery and sensory experience in congenital blindness. Neuropsy-
chologia, 26(1), 1–12.
Arnold, D. H., & Doctoroff, G. L. (2003). The early education of socioeconomically disadvantaged children. Annual Review of
Psychology, 54, 517–545.
Arnold, D. H., Fisher, P. H., Doctoroff, G. L., & Dobbs, J. (2002). Accelerating math development in Head Start classrooms:
Outcomes and gender differences. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94(4), 762–770.
Ashcraft, M. H. (2006, November). Math performance, working memory, and math anxiety: Some possible directions for neural
functioning work. Paper presented at the Neural Basis of Mathematical Development, Nashville, TN.
Ashkenazi, S., Mark-Zigdon, N., & Henik, A. (2013). Do subitizing deficits in developmental dyscalculia involve pattern recogni-
tion weakness? Developmental Science, 16(1), 35–46. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2012.01190.x
Askew, M., Brown, M., Rhodes, V., Wiliam, D., & Johnson, D. (1997). Effective teachers of numeracy in UK primary schools:
Teachers’ beliefs, practices, and children’s learning. In M. v. d. Heuvel-Panhuizen (Ed.), Proceedings of the 21st Conference
of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education (Vol. 2, pp. 25–32). Utrecht, the Netherlands: Freu-
denthal Institute.
Aubrey, C. (1997). Children’s early learning of number in school and out. In I. Thompson (Ed.), Teaching and learning early num-
ber (pp. 20–29). Philadelphia, PA: Open University Press.
Aunio, P., Ee, J., Lim, S. E. A., Hautamäki, J., & Van Luit, J. E. H. (2004). Young children’s number sense in Finland, Hong Kong and
Singapore. International Journal of Early Years Education, 12(3), 195–216.
Aunio, P., Hautamäki, J., & Van Luit, J. E. H. (2005). Mathematical thinking intervention programmes for preschool children with
normal and low number sense. European Journal of Special Needs Education, 20(2), 131–146.
Aunio, P., Hautamäki, J., Sajaniemi, N., & Van Luit, J. E. H. (2008). Early numeracy in low-performing young children. British
Educational Research Journal, 35(1), 25–46.

334
References • 335

Aunio, P., Niemivirta, M., Hautamäki, J., Van Luit, J. E. H., Shi, J., & Zhang, M. (2006). Young children’s number sense in China
and Finland. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 50(5), 483–502.
Aunola, K., Leskinen, E., Lerkkanen, M.-K., & Nurmi, J.-E. (2004). Developmental dynamics of math performance from preschool
to grade 2. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96(4), 699–713.
Aunola, K., Nurmi, J.-E., Lerkkanen, M.-K., & Rasku-Puttonen, H. (2003). The roles of achievement-related behaviours and
parental beliefs in children’s mathematical performance. Educational Psychology, 23(4), 403–421.
Austin, A. M. B., Blevins-Knabe, B., & Lindauer, S. L. K. (2008). Informal and formal mathematics concepts of children in center and
family child care. Unpublished manuscript.
Austin, A. M. B., Blevins-Knabe, B., Ota, C., Rowe, T., & Lindauer, S. L. K. (2010). Mediators of preschoolers’ early mathematics
concepts. Early Child Development and Care, 181(9), 1181–1198. doi: 10.1080/03004430.2010.520711
Aydogan, C., Plummer, C., Kang, S. J., Bilbrey, C., Farran, D. C., & Lipsey, M. W. (2005). An investigation of prekindergarten cur-
ricula: Influences on classroom characteristics and child engagement. Paper presented at the National Association for the
Education of Young Children (NAEYC).
Bacon, W. F., & Ichikawa, V. (1988). Maternal expectations, classroom experiences, and achievement among kindergartners in the
United States and Japan. Human Development, 31(6), 378–383.
Balfanz, R. (1999). Why do we teach young children so little mathematics? Some historical considerations. In J. V. Copley (Ed.),
Mathematics in the early years (pp. 3–10). Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
Ball, D. L. (1992). Magical hopes: Manipulatives and the reform of math education. American Educator, 16(2), 14; 16–18; 46–47.
Baratta-Lorton, M. (1976). Mathematics their way: an activity-centered mathematics program for early childhood education. Menlo
Park, CA: Addison-Wesley.
Barbarin, O. A., Downer, J. T., Odom, E., & Head, D. (2010). Home–school differences in beliefs, support, and control during pub-
lic pre-kindergarten and their link to children’s kindergarten readiness. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 25(3), 358–372.
doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2010.02.003
Barnett, W. S., Frede, E. C., Mobasher, H., & Mohr, P. (1987). The efficacy of public preschool programs and the relationship of
program quality to efficacy. Educational Evaluation and Policy Analysis, 10(1), 37–49.
Barnett, W. S., Hustedt, J. T., Hawkinson, L. E., & Robin, K. B. (2006). The state of preschool 2006: State preschool yearbook. New
Brunswick, NJ: National Institute for Early Education Research (NIEER).
Barnett, W. S., Yarosz, D. J., Thomas, J., & Hornbeck, A. (2006). Educational effectiveness of a Vygotskian approach to preschool
education: A randomized trial. New Brunswick, NJ: National Institute of Early Education Research (NIEER).
Baroody, A. J. (1986). Counting ability of moderately and mildly handicapped children. Education and Training of the Mentally
Retarded, 21(4), 289–300.
Baroody, A. J. (1987). Children’s mathematical thinking. New York, NY: Teachers College.
Baroody, A. J. (1989). Manipulatives don’t come with guarantees. Arithmetic Teacher, 37(2), 4–5.
Baroody, A. J. (1990). How and when should place value concepts and skills be taught? Journal for Research in Mathematics Educa-
tion, 21, 281–286.
Baroody, A. J. (1996). An investigative approach to the mathematics instruction of children classified as learning disabled. In
D. K. Reid, W. P. Hresko, & H. L. Swanson (Eds.), Cognitive approaches to learning disabilities (3rd ed., pp. 547–615). Austin,
TX: Pro-Ed.
Baroody, A. J. (1999). The development of basic counting, number, and arithmetic knowledge among children classified as men-
tally handicapped. In L. M. Glidden (Ed.), International review of research in mental retardation (Vol. 22, pp. 51–103). New
York, NY: Academic Press.
Baroody, A. J. (2003). The development of adaptive expertise and flexibility: The integration of conceptual and procedural
knowledge. In A. J. Baroody & A. Dowker (Eds.), The development of arithmetic concepts and skills: Constructing adaptive
expertise (pp. 1–33). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Baroody, A. J. (2004a). The developmental bases for early childhood number and operations standards. In D. H. Clements, J.
Sarama, & A.-M. DiBiase (Eds.), Engaging young children in mathematics: Standards for early childhood mathematics educa-
tion (pp. 173–219). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Baroody, A. J. (2004b). The role of psychological research in the development of early childhood mathematics standards. In
D. H. Clements, J. Sarama, & A.-M. DiBiase (Eds.), Engaging young children in mathematics: Standards for early childhood
mathematics education (pp. 149–172). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Baroody, A. J., & Benson, A. P. (2001). Early number instruction. Teaching Children Mathematics, 8(3), 154–158.
Baroody, A. J., & Dowker, A. (2003). The development of arithmetic concepts and skills: Constructing adaptive expertise. Mahwah,
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Baroody, A. J., & Tiilikainen, S. H. (2003). Two perspectives on addition development. In A. J. Baroody & A. Dowker (Eds.), The
development of arithmetic concepts and skills: Constructing adaptive expertise (pp. 75–125). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates.
Baroody, A. J., Bajwa, N. P., & Eiland, M. (2009). Why can’t Johnny remember the basic facts? Developmental Disabilities, 15(1),
69–79.
Baroody, A. J., Eiland, M. D., Purpura, D. J., & Reid, E. E. (2012). Fostering at-risk kindergarten children’s number sense. Cognition
and Instruction, 30(4), 435–470. doi: 10.1080/07370008.2012.720152
Baroody, A. J., Eiland, M. D., Purpura, D. J., & Reid, E. E. (2013). Can computer-assisted discovery learning foster first
graders’ fluency with the most basic addition combinations? American Educational Research Journal, 50(3), 533–573. doi:
10.3102/0002831212473349
Baroody, A. J., Eiland, M., Su, Y., & Thompson, B. (2007). Fostering at-risk preschoolers’ number sense. Paper presented at the
American Educational Research Association.
Baroody, A. J., Lai, M.-L., & Mix, K. S. (2005, December). Changing views of young children’s numerical and arithmetic competencies.
Paper presented at the National Association for the Education of Young Children, Washington, D.C.
Baroody, A. J., Lai, M.-L., & Mix, K. S. (2006). The development of young children’s number and operation sense and its implica-
tions for early childhood education. In B. Spodek & O. N. Saracho (Eds.), Handbook of research on the education of young
children (pp. 187–221). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
336 • References

Baroody, A. J., Li, X., & Lai, M.-L. (2008). Toddlers’ spontaneous attention to number. Mathematical Thinking and Learning, 10(3),
240–270.
Bartsch, K., & Wellman, H. M. (1988). Young children’s conception of distance. Developmental Psychology, 24(4), 532–541.
Battista, M. T. (1990). Spatial visualization and gender differences in high school geometry. Journal for Research in Mathematics
Education, 21(1), 47–60.
Beilin, H. (1984). Cognitive theory and mathematical cognition: Geometry and space. In B. Gholson & T. L. Rosenthal (Eds.),
Applications of cognitive-developmental theory (pp. 49–93). New York, NY: Academic Press.
Beilin, H., Klein, A., & Whitehurst, B. (1982). Strategies and structures in understanding geometry. New York, NY: City University
of New York.
Beilock, S. L., Gunderson, E. A., Ramirez, G., & Levine, S. C. (2010). Female teachers’ math anxiety affects girls’ math achievement.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107(5), 1860–1863.
Benigno, J. P., & Ellis, S. (2004). Two is greater than three: Effects of older siblings on parental support of preschoolers’ counting
in middle-income families. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 19(1), 4–20.
Bennett, N., Desforges, C., Cockburn, A., & Wilkinson, B. (1984). The quality of pupil learning experiences. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates.
Berch, D. B., & Mazzocco, M. M. M. (Eds.). (2007). Why is math so hard for some children? The nature and origins of mathematical
learning difficulties and disabilities. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brooks.
Bereiter, C. (1986). Does direct instruction cause delinquency? Response to Schweinhart and Weikart. Educational Leadership,
44(3), 20–21.
Bergin, D. A., Ford, M. E., & Mayer-Gaub, G. (1986). Social and motivational consequences of microcomputer use in kindergarten.
San Francisco, CA: American Educational Research Association.
Berliner, D. C. (2006). Our impoverished view of educational research. Teachers College Record, 108(6), 949–995.
Best, J. R., Miller, P. H., & Naglieri, J. A. (2011). Relations between executive function and academic achievement from ages 5
to 17 in a large, representative national sample. Learning and Individual Differences, 21(4), 327–336. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.
2011.01.007
Bilbrey, C., Farran, D. C., Lipsey, M. W., & Hurley, S. (2007, April). Active involvement by rural children from low income families in
prekindergarten classrooms: Predictors and consequences. Paper presented at the Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research
in Child Development, Boston, MA.
Binder, S. L., & Ledger, B. (1985). Preschool computer project report. Oakville, Ontario, Canada: Sheridan College.
Bishop, A. J. (1980). Spatial abilities and mathematics education—A review. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 11(3),
257–269.
Bishop, A. J. (1983). Space and geometry. In R. A. Lesh & M. S. Landau (Eds.), Acquisition of mathematics concepts and processes
(pp. 7–44). New York, NY: Academic Press.
Bishop, A. J., & Forgasz, H. J. (2007). Issues in access and equity in mathematics education. In F. K. Lester, Jr. (Ed.), Second hand-
book of research on mathematics teaching and learning (pp. 1145–1167). New York, NY: Information Age Publishing.
Björklund, C. (2012). What counts when working with mathematics in a toddler-group? Early Years, 32(2), 215–228. doi:
10.1080/09575146.2011.652940
Black, P., & Wiliam, D. (1998). Assessment and classroom learning. Assessment in Education: Principles, Policy & Practice, 5(1),
7–76.
Blair, C. (2002). School readiness: Integrating cognition and emotion in a neurobiological conceptualization of children’s
functioning at school entry. American Psychologist, 57(2), 111–127.
Blair, C., & Razza, R. P. (2007). Relating effortful control, executive function, and false belief understanding to emerging math and
literacy ability in kindergarten. Child Development, 78(2), 647–663.
Blanton, M. L., & Kaput, J. J. (2011). Functional thinking as a route into algebra in the elementary grades. In J. Cai & E. J. Knuth
(Eds.), Early algebraization: A global dialogue from multiple perspectives (pp. 5–23). New York, NY: Springer.
Blanton, M. L., Stephens, A. C., Knuth, E. J., Gardiner, A. M., Isler, I., Marum, T. et al. (2012). The development of children’s
algebraic thinking using a learning progressions approach. Paper presented at the Research Presession of the 2012 Annual
Meeting of the National Council of Teachers of Mathematics, Philadelphia, PA.
Blevins-Knabe, B., & Musun-Miller, L. (1996). Number use at home by children and their parents and its relationship to early
mathematical performance. Early Development and Parenting, 5(1), 35–45.
Blevins-Knabe, B., Berghout Austin, A., Musun-Miller, L., Eddy, A., & Jones, R. M. (2000). Family home care providers’ and
parents’ beliefs and practices concerning mathematics with young children. Early Child Development and Care, 165(1),
41–58. doi: 10.1080/0300443001650104
Blevins-Knabe, B., Whiteside-Mansell, L., & Selig, J. (2007). Parenting and mathematical development. Academic Exchange
Quarterly, 11, 76–80.
Bley, N. S., & Thornton, C. A. (1981). Teaching mathematics to the learning disabled. Rockville, MD: Aspen Systems Corporation.
Blöte, A. W., Van der Burg, E., & Klein, A. S. (2001). Students’ flexibility in solving two-digit addition and subtraction problems:
Instruction effects. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93(3), 627–638.
Bodovski, K., & Farkas, G. (2007). Mathematics growth in early elementary school: The roles of beginning knowledge, student
engagement, and instruction. The Elementary School Journal, 108(2), 115–130.
Bodovski, K., & Youn, M.-J. (2011). The long term effects of early acquired skills and behaviors on young children’s achievement
in literacy and mathematics. Journal of Early Childhood Research, 9(1), 4–19.
Bodovski, K., & Youn, M.-J. (2012). Students’ mathematics learning from kindergarten through 8th grade: The long-term influ-
ence of school readiness. International Journal of Sociology of Education, 1(2), 97–122. doi: 10.4471/rise.2012.07
Bodovski, K., Nahum-Shani, I., & Walsh, R. (2013). School climate and students’ early mathematics learning: Another search for
contextual effects. American Journal of Education, 119(2), 209–234. doi: 10.1086/667227
Bodrova, E., & Leong, D. J. (2001). The tools of the mind: A case study of implementing the Vygotskian approach in American early
childhood and primary classrooms. Geneva, Switzerland: International Bureau of Education.
Bodrova, E., & Leong, D. J. (2006). Self-regulation as a key to school readiness: How can early childhood teachers promote this
critical competency? In M. Zaslow & I. Martinez-Beck (Eds.), Critical issues in early childhood professional development
(pp. 203–224). Baltimore, MD: Brookes Publishing.
References • 337

Bodrova, E., Leong, D. J., Norford, J. S., & Paynter, D. E. (2003). It only looks like child’s play. Journal of Staff Development, 24(2),
47–51.
Bofferding, L., & Alexander, A. (2011). Nothing is something: First graders’ use of zero in relation to negative numbers. Paper
presented at the American Educational Research Association, New Orleans, LA.
Bonny, J. W., & Lourenco, S. F. (2013). The approximate number system and its relation to early math achievement: Evidence
from the preschool years. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 114(3), 375–388. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.
jecp.2012.09.015
Bowman, B. T., Donovan, M. S., & Burns, M. S. (Eds.). (2001). Eager to learn: Educating our preschoolers. Washington, D.C.:
National Academy Press.
Bransford, J. D., Brown, A. L., & Cocking, R. R. (Eds.). (1999). How people learn. Washington, D.C.: National Academy Press.
Brinkley, V. M., & Watson, J. A. (1987–88a). Effects of MicroWorld training experience on sorting tasks by young children. Journal
of Educational Technology Systems, 16(4), 349–364.
Brinkley, V. M., & Watson, J. A. (1987–88b). Logo and young children: Are quadrant effects part of initial Logo mastery? Journal
of Educational Technology Systems, 19(1), 75–86.
Broberg, A. G., Wessels, H., Lamb, M. E., & Hwang, C. P. (1997). Effects of day care on the development of cognitive abilities in
8-year-olds: A longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 33(1), 62–69.
Brooks-Gunn, J. (2003). Do you believe in magic? What we can expect from early childhood intervention programs. Social Policy
Report, 17(1), 1, 3–14.
Brooks-Gunn, J., Duncan, G. J., & Britto, P. R. (1999). Are socioeconomic gradients for children similar to those for adults?
In D. P. Keating & C. Hertzman (Eds.), Developmental health and the wealth of nations (pp. 94–124). New York, NY:
Guilford Press.
Brosnan, M. J. (1998). Spatial ability in children’s play with Lego blocks. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 87(1), 19–28. doi: 10.2466/
pms.1998.87.1.19
Brosterman, N. (1997). Inventing kindergarten. New York, NY: Harry N. Abrams.
Brown, S. I., & Walter, M. I. (1990). The art of problem posing. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Brownell, W. A., & Moser, H. E. (1949). Meaningful vs. mechanical learning: A study in grade III subtraction. Durham, NC: Duke
University Press.
Browning, C. A. (1991). Reflections on using Lego© Logo in an elementary classroom. In E. Calabrese (Ed.), Proceedings of the
Third European Logo Conference (pp. 173–185). Parma, Italy: Associazione Scuola e Informatica.
Brulles, D., Peters, S. J., & Saunders, R. (2012). Schoolwide mathematics achievement within the gifted cluster grouping model.
Journal of Advanced Academics, 23(3), 200–216. doi: 10.1177/1932202x12451439
Bryant, D. M., Burchinal, M. R., Lau, L. B., & Sparling, J. J. (1994). Family and classroom correlates of Head Start children’s
developmental outcomes. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 9(3–4), 289–309.
Bull, R., & Scerif, G. (2001). Executive functioning as a predictor of children’s mathematics ability: Inhibition, switching, and
working memory. Developmental Neuropsychology, 19(3), 273–293.
Bull, R., Espy, K. A., & Wiebe, S. A. (2008). Short-term memory, working memory, and executive functioning in preschoolers:
Longitudinal predictors of mathematical achievement at age 7 years. Developmental Neuropsychology, 33(3), 205–228.
Bulotsky-Shearer, R. J., Fernandez, V., Dominguez, X., & Rouse, H. L. (2011). Behavior problems in learning activities and social
interactions in Head Start classrooms and early reading, mathematics, and approaches to learning. School Psychology Review,
40(1), 39–56.
Burchinal, M. R., Field, S., López, M. L., Howes, C., & Pianta, R. (2012). Instruction in Spanish in pre-kindergarten classrooms
and child outcomes for English language learners. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 27(2), 188–197. doi: http://dx.doi.
org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2011.11.003
Burchinal, M. R., Peisner-Feinberg, E., Pianta, R., & Howes, C. (2002). Development of academic skills from preschool
through second grade: Family and classroom predictors of developmental trajectories. Developmental Psychology, 40(5),
415–436.
Burden, M. J., Jacobson, S. W., Dodge, N. C., Dehaene, S., & Jacobson, J. L. (2007). Effects of prenatal alcohol and cocaine exposure
on arithmetic and “number sense.” Paper presented at the Society for Research in Child Development.
Burger, W. F., & Shaughnessy, J. M. (1986). Characterizing the van Hiele levels of development in geometry. Journal for Research
in Mathematics Education, 17(1), 31–48.
Burkhardt, H. (2006). From design research to large-scale impact: Engineering research in education. In J. J. H. van den Akker,
K. P. E. Gravemeijer, S. McKenney, & N. Nieveen (Eds.), Educational design research (pp. 133–162). London, England:
Routledge.
Burns, M. K., Kanive, R., & DeGrande, M. (2012). Effect of a computer-delivered math fact intervention as a supplemental interven-
tion for math in third and fourth grades. Remedial and Special Education, 33(3), 184–191. doi: 10.1177/0741932510381652
Burny, E. (2012). Towards an understanding of children’s difficulties with conventional time systems. In Time-related competences
in primary education (Chapter 2), doctoral dissertation, Ghent University, Belgium.
Burny, E., Valcke, M., & Desoete, A. (2009). Towards an agenda for studying learning and instruction focusing on time-related
competences in children. Educational Studies, 35(5), 481–492. doi: 10.1080/03055690902879093
Burny, E., Valcke, M., & Desoete, A. (2012). Clock reading: An underestimated topic in children with mathematics difficulties.
Journal of Learning Disabilities, 45(4), 351–360. doi: 10.1177/0022219411407773
Burny, E., Valcke, M., Desoete, A., & Van Luit, J. E. H. (2013). Curriculum sequencing and the acquisition of clock-reading skills
among Chinese and Flemish children. International Journal of Science and Mathematics Education, 11, 761–785.
Butterworth, B., Varma, S., & Laurillard, D. (2011). Dyscalculia: From brain to education. Science, 332(6033), 1049–1053.
Callahan, L. G., & Clements, D. H. (1984). Sex differences in rote counting ability on entry to first grade: Some observations.
Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 15, 378–382.
Cameron, C. E., Brock, L. L., Murrah, W. M., Bell, L. H., Worzalla, S. L., Grissmer, D. et al. (2012). Fine motor skills and
executive function both contribute to kindergarten achievement. Child Development, 83(4), 1229–1244. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-
8624.2012.01768.x
Campbell, F. A., Pungello, E. P., Miller-Johnson, S., Burchinal, M., & Ramey, C. T. (2001). The development of cognitive and
academic abilities: Growth curves from an early childhood educational experiment. Developmental Psychology, 37, 231–242.
338 • References

Campbell, P. F. (1987). Measuring distance: Children’s use of number and unit. Final report submitted to the National Institute
of Mental Health Under the ADAMHA Small Grant Award Program. Grant No. MSMA 1 R03 MH423435–01, University of
Maryland, College Park.
Campbell, P. F., & Silver, E. A. (1999). Teaching and learning mathematics in poor communities. Reston, VA: National Council of
Teachers of Mathematics.
Cannon, J., Fernandez, C., & Ginsburg, H. P. (2005, April). Parents’ preference for supporting preschoolers’ language over
mathematics learning: A difference that runs deep. Paper presented at the Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in
Child Development, Atlanta, GA.
Cannon, J., Levine, S. C., & Huttenlocher, J. (2007, March). Sex differences in the relation between early puzzle play and mental
transformation skill. Paper presented at the Biennial Meeting of the Society of Research in Child Development, Boston, MA.
Canobi, K. H., Reeve, R. A., & Pattison, P. E. (1998). The role of conceptual understanding in children’s addition problem solving.
Developmental Psychology, 34, 882–891.
Carey, B. (2009, December 21). Studying young minds, and how to teach them, New York Times, p. A1; A23.
Carey, S. (2004). Bootstrapping and the origin of concepts. Daedulus, 133(1), 59–68.
Carmichael, H. W., Burnett, J. D., Higginson, W. C., Moore, B. G., & Pollard, P. J. (1985). Computers, children and classrooms: A
multisite evaluation of the creative use of microcomputers by elementary school children. Toronto, Ontario, Canada: Ministry
of Education.
Carnegie Corporation. (1998). Years of promise: A comprehensive learning strategy for America’s children [Electronic version].
Retrieved June 13, 1998, from http://www.carnegie.org/sub/pubs/execsum.html
Carpenter, T. P., & Levi, L. (1999). Developing conceptions of algebraic reasoning in the primary grades. Montreal, Canada: American
Educational Research Association.
Carpenter, T. P., & Lewis, R. (1976). The development of the concept of a standard unit of measure in young children. Journal for
Research in Mathematics Education, 7, 53–58.
Carpenter, T. P., & Moser, J. M. (1982). The development of addition and subtraction problem solving. In T. P. Carpenter,
J. M. Moser, & T. A. Romberg (Eds.), Rational numbers: An integration of research. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates.
Carpenter, T. P., & Moser, J. M. (1984). The acquisition of addition and subtraction concepts in grades one through three. Journal
for Research in Mathematics Education, 15, 179–202.
Carpenter, T. P., Ansell, E., Franke, M. L., Fennema, E. H., & Weisbeck, L. (1993). Models of problem solving: A study of kindergar-
ten children’s problem-solving processes. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 24, 428–441.
Carpenter, T. P., Coburn, T., Reys, R. E., & Wilson, J. (1975). Notes from National Assessment: Basic concepts of area and volume.
Arithmetic Teacher, 22, 501–507.
Carpenter, T. P., Fennema, E. H., Franke, M. L., Levi, L., & Empson, S. B. (1999). Children’s mathematics: Cognitively guided instruc-
tion. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann.
Carpenter, T. P., Franke, M. L., Jacobs, V. R., Fennema, E. H., & Empson, S. B. (1998). A longitudinal study of invention and
understanding in children’s multidigit addition and subtraction. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 29, 3–20.
Carpenter, T. P., Franke, M. L., & Levi, L. (2003). Thinking mathematically: Integrating arithmetic and algebra in elementary school.
Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann.
Carper, D. V. (1942). Seeing numbers as groups in primary-grade arithmetic. The Elementary School Journal, 43, 166–170.
Carr, M., & Alexeev, N. (2011). Fluency, accuracy, and gender predict developmental trajectories of arithmetic strategies. Journal
of Educational Psychology, 103(3), 617–631.
Carr, M., & Davis, H. (2001). Gender differences in arithmetic strategy use: A function of skill and preference. Contemporary
Educational Psychology, 26, 330–347.
Carr, M., Shing, Y. L., Janes, P., & Steiner, H. H. (2007). Early gender differences in strategy use and fluency: Implications for the
emergence of gender differences in mathematics. Paper presented at the Society for Research in Child Development.
Carr, M., Steiner, H. H., Kyser, B., & Biddlecomb, B. (2008). A comparison of predictors of early emerging gender differences in
mathematics competence. Learning and Individual Differences, 18, 61–75.
Carr, M., Taasoobshirazi, G., Stroud, R., & Royer, M. (2011). Combined fluency and cognitive strategies instruction improves
mathematics achievement in early elementary school. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 36, 323–333.
Case, R., Griffin, S., & Kelly, W. M. (1999). Socioeconomic gradients in mathematical ability and their responsiveness to inter-
vention during early childhood. In D. P. Keating & C. Hertzman (Eds.), Developmental health and the wealth of nations
(pp. 125–149). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Casey, B., Andrews, N., Schindler, H., Kersh, J. E., Samper, A., & Copley, J. (2008). The development of spatial skills through inter-
ventions involving block building activities. Cognition and Instruction, 26(3), 1–41.
Casey, B., Erkut, S., Ceder, I., & Young, J. M. (2008). Use of a storytelling context to improve girls’ and boys’ geometry skills in
kindergarten. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 29(1), 29–48.
Casey, B., Kersh, J. E., & Young, J. M. (2004). Storytelling sagas: An effective medium for teaching early childhood mathematics.
Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 19(1), 167–172.
Casey, B., Nuttall, R. L., & Pezaris, E. (1997). Mediators of gender differences in mathematics college entrance test scores: A com-
parison of spatial skills with internalized beliefs and anxieties. Developmental Psychology, 33(4), 669–680.
Casey, B., Nuttall, R. L., & Pezaris, E. (2001). Spatial–mechanical reasoning skills versus mathematics self-confidence as mediators
of gender differences on mathematics subtests using cross-national gender-based items. Journal for Research in Mathematics
Education, 32(1), 28–57.
Catsambis, S., & Buttaro, A., Jr. (2012). Revisiting “Kindergarten as academic boot camp”: A nationwide study of ability grouping
and psycho-social development. Social Psychology of Education, 15(4), 483–515. doi: 10.1007/s11218-012-9196-0
CCSSO/NGA. (2010). Common core state standards for mathematics. Washington, D.C.: Council of Chief State School Officers
and the National Governors Association Center for Best Practices.
Celedón-Pattichis, S., Musanti, S. I., & Marshall, M. E. (2010). Bilingual elementary teachers’ reflections on using students’
native language and culture to teach mathematics. In M. Q. Foote (Ed.), Mathematics teaching & learning in K–12: Equity
and professional development (pp. 7–24). New York, NY: Palgrave Macmillan.
References • 339

Cepeda, N. J., Pashler, H., Vul, E., Wixted, J. T., & Rohrer, D. (2006). Distributed practice in verbal recall tasks: A review and
quantitative synthesis. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 354–380.
Chang, A., Sandhofer, C. M., & Brown, C. S. (2011). Gender biases in early number exposure to preschool-aged children. Journal
of Language and Social Psychology, 30(4), 440–450.
Chang, A., Zmich, K. M., Athanasopoulou, A., Hou, L., Golinkoff, R. M., & Hirsh-Pasek, K. (2011). Manipulating geometric forms
in two-dimensional space: Effects of socio-economic status on preschoolers’ geometric-spatial ability. Paper presented at the
Society for Research in Child Development, Montreal, Canada.
Char, C. A. (1989). Computer graphic feltboards: New software approaches for young children’s mathematical exploration. San
Francisco, CA: American Educational Research Association.
Chard, D. J., Clarke, B., Baker, S., Otterstedt, J., Braun, D., & Katz, R. (2005). Using measures of number sense to screen for difficul-
ties in mathematics: Preliminary findings. Assessment for Effective Intervention, 30(2), 3–14.
Chen, C., & Uttal, D. H. (1988). Cultural values, parents’ beliefs, and children’s achievement in the United States and China.
Human Development, 31, 351–358.
Chernoff, J. J., Flanagan, K. D., McPhee, C., & Park, J. (2007). Preschool: First findings from the third follow-up of the early childhood
longitudinal study, birth cohort (ECLS-B) (NCES 2008–025). Washington, D.C.: National Center for Education Statistics,
Institute of Education Sciences, U.S. Department of Education.
Cheung, C., Leung, A., & McBride-Chang, C. (2007). Gender differences in mathematics self concept in Hong Kong children: A
function of perceived maternal academic support. Paper presented at the Society for Research in Child Development.
Cheung, Cecilia, & McBride-Chang, Catherine. (2008). Relations of perceived maternal parenting style, practices, and learning
motivation to academic competence in Chinese children. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 54(1), 1–22.
Chien, N. C., Howes, C., Burchinal, M. R., Pianta, R. C., Ritchie, S., Bryant, D. M. et al. (2010). Children’s classroom engagement
and school readiness gains in prekindergarten. Child Development, 81(5), 1534–1549. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01490.x
Christiansen, K., Austin, A., & Roggman, L. (2005, April). Math interactions in the context of play: Relations to child math ability.
Paper presented at the Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Atlanta, GA.
Cirino, P. T. (2010). The interrelationships of mathematical precursors in kindergarten. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,
108(4). doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2010.11.004
Clark, C. A., Pritchard, V. E., & Woodward, L. J. (2010). Preschool executive functioning abilities predict early mathematics
achievement. Developmental Psychology, 46(5), 1176–1191. doi: 10.1037/a0019672
Clark, C. A. C., Sheffield, T. D., Wiebe, S. A., & Espy, K. A. (2013). Longitudinal associations between executive control and
developing mathematical competence in preschool boys and girls. Child Development, 84(2), 662–677. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-
8624.2012.01854.x
Clarke, B., & Shinn, M. R. (2004). A preliminary investigation into the identification and development of early mathematics
curriculum-based measurement. School Psychology Review, 33(2), 234–248.
Clarke, B., Smolkowski, K., Baker, S., Fien, H., Doabler, C. T., & Chard, D. (2011). The impact of a comprehensive Tier
I core kindergarten program on the achievement of students at risk in mathematics. Elementary School Journal, 111(4),
561–584.
Clarke, B. A., Clarke, D. M., & Horne, M. (2006). A longitudinal study of children’s mental computation strategies. In J. Novotná,
H. Moraová, M. Krátká, & N. Stehlíková (Eds.), Proceedings of the 30th Conference of the International Group for the Psychol-
ogy in Mathematics Education (Vol. 2, pp. 329–336). Prague, Czech Republic: Charles University.
Clarke, D. M., Cheeseman, J., Gervasoni, A., Gronn, D., Horne, M., McDonough, A., et al. (2002). Early numeracy research project:
Final report. Department of Education, Employment and Training, the Catholic Education Office (Melbourne), and the
Association of Independent Schools, Victoria, Australia.
Clements, D. H. (1983–1984). Supporting young children’s Logo programming. The Computing Teacher, 11(5), 24–30.
Clements, D. H. (1984). Training effects on the development and generalization of Piagetian logical operations and knowledge of
number. Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 766–776.
Clements, D. H. (1986). Effects of Logo and CAI environments on cognition and creativity. Journal of Educational Psychology, 78,
309–318.
Clements, D. H. (1987). Longitudinal study of the effects of Logo programming on cognitive abilities and achievement. Journal of
Educational Computing Research, 3, 73–94.
Clements, D. H. (1989). Computers in elementary mathematics education. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Clements, D. H. (1991). Current technology and the early childhood curriculum. In B. Spodek & O. N. Saracho (Eds.), Yearbook
in early childhood education, Volume 2: Issues in early childhood curriculum (pp. 106–131). New York, NY: Teachers College
Press.
Clements, D. H. (1994). The uniqueness of the computer as a learning tool: Insights from research and practice. In J. L. Wright &
D. D. Shade (Eds.), Young children: Active learners in a technological age (pp. 31–50). Washington, D.C.: National Association
for the Education of Young Children.
Clements, D. H. (1995). Teaching creativity with computers. Educational Psychology Review, 7(2), 141–161.
Clements, D. H. (1999a). “Concrete” manipulatives, concrete ideas. Contemporary Issues in Early Childhood, 1(1), 45–60.
Clements, D. H. (1999b). Subitizing: What is it? Why teach it? Teaching Children Mathematics, 5, 400–405.
Clements, D. H. (1999c). Teaching length measurement: Research challenges. School Science and Mathematics, 99(1), 5–11.
Clements, D. H. (2000). Translating lessons from research into mathematics classrooms: Mathematics and special needs students.
Perspectives, 26(3), 31–33.
Clements, D. H. (2001). Mathematics in the preschool. Teaching Children Mathematics, 7, 270–275.
Clements, D. H. (2007). Curriculum research: Toward a framework for “research-based curricula”. Journal for Research in Math-
ematics Education, 38, 35–70.
Clements, D. H., & Battista, M. T. (1989). Learning of geometric concepts in a Logo environment. Journal for Research in Math-
ematics Education, 20, 450–467.
Clements, D. H., & Battista, M. T. (1990). Constructivist learning and teaching. Arithmetic Teacher, 38(1), 34–35.
Clements, D. H., & Battista, M. T. (1992). Geometry and spatial reasoning. In D. A. Grouws (Ed.), Handbook of research on math-
ematics teaching and learning (pp. 420–464). New York, NY: Macmillan.
340 • References

Clements, D. H., & Battista, M. T. (2000). Designing effective software. In A. E. Kelly & R. A. Lesh (Eds.), Handbook of research
design in mathematics and science education (pp. 761–776). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Clements, D. H., & Battista, M. T. (Artist). (1991). Logo geometry. Morristown, NJ: Silver Burdett & Ginn.
Clements, D. H., & Burns, B. A. (2000). Students’ development of strategies for turn and angle measure. Educational Studies in
Mathematics, 41, 31–45.
Clements, D. H., & Callahan, L. G. (1983). Number or prenumber experiences for young children: Must we choose? The Arithmetic
Teacher, 31(3), 34–37.
Clements, D. H., & Callahan, L. G. (1986). Cards: A good deal to offer. The Arithmetic Teacher, 34(1), 14–17.
Clements, D. H., & Conference Working Group. (2004). Part one: Major themes and recommendations. In D. H. Clements,
J. Sarama, & A.-M. DiBiase (Eds.), Engaging young children in mathematics: Standards for early childhood mathematics
education (pp. 1–72). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Clements, D. H., & Meredith, J. S. (1993). Research on Logo: Effects and efficacy. Journal of Computing in Childhood Education,
4, 263–290.
Clements, D. H., & Meredith, J. S. (1994). Turtle math [Computer software]. Montreal, Quebec: Logo Computer Systems,
Inc. (LCSI).
Clements, D. H., & Nastasi, B. K. (1985). Effects of computer environments on social-emotional development: Logo and computer-
assisted instruction. Computers in the Schools, 2(2–3), 11–31.
Clements, D. H., & Nastasi, B. K. (1988). Social and cognitive interactions in educational computer environments. American
Educational Research Journal, 25, 87–106.
Clements, D. H., & Nastasi, B. K. (1992). Computers and early childhood education. In M. Gettinger, S. N. Elliott, & T. R. Krato-
chwill (Eds.), Advances in school psychology: Preschool and early childhood treatment directions (pp. 187–246). Mahwah, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Clements, D. H., & Nastasi, B. K. (1993). Electronic media and early childhood education. In B. Spodek (Ed.), Handbook of
research on the education of young children (pp. 251–275). New York, NY: Macmillan.
Clements, D. H., & Sarama, J. (1996). Turtle Math: Redesigning Logo for elementary mathematics. Learning and Leading with
Technology, 23(7), 10–15.
Clements, D. H., & Sarama, J. (1997). Research on Logo: A decade of progress. Computers in the Schools, 14(1–2), 9–46.
Clements, D. H., & Sarama, J. (2002). Teaching with computers in early childhood education: Strategies and professional develop-
ment. Journal of Early Childhood Teacher Education, 23, 215–226.
Clements, D. H., & Sarama, J. (2003a). DLM Early Childhood Express Math Resource Guide. Columbus, OH: SRA/McGraw-Hill.
Clements, D. H., & Sarama, J. (2003b). Strip mining for gold: Research and policy in educational technology—A response to
“Fool’s Gold”. Educational Technology Review, 11(1), 7–69.
Clements, D. H., & Sarama, J. (2003c). Young children and technology: What does the research say? Young Children, 58(6), 34–40.
Clements, D. H., & Sarama, J. (2004a). Building Blocks for early childhood mathematics. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 19,
181–189.
Clements, D. H., & Sarama, J. (2004b). Learning trajectories in mathematics education. Mathematical Thinking and Learning, 6,
81–89.
Clements, D. H., & Sarama, J. (2007a). Building Blocks—SRA Real Math, Teacher’s Edition, Grade PreK. Columbus, OH: SRA/
McGraw-Hill.
Clements, D. H., & Sarama, J. (2007b). Building Blocks—SRA Real Math, Grade PreK. Columbus, OH: SRA/McGraw-Hill.
Clements, D. H., & Sarama, J. (2007c). Effects of a preschool mathematics curriculum: Summative research on the Building Blocks
project. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 38, 136–163.
Clements, D. H., & Sarama, J. (2008). Experimental evaluation of the effects of a research-based preschool mathematics
curriculum. American Educational Research Journal, 45, 443–494.
Clements, D. H., & Sarama, J. (2010). Technology. In V. Washington & J. Andrews (Eds.), Children of 2020: Creating a better to-
morrow (pp. 119–123). Washington, D.C.: Council for Professional Recognition/National Association for the Education of
Young Children.
Clements, D. H., & Sarama, J. (2011). Early childhood mathematics intervention. Science, 333(6045), 968–970. doi: 10.1126/
science.1204537
Clements, D. H., & Sarama, J. (2013). Building Blocks (Volumes 1 and 2). Columbus, OH: McGraw-Hill Education.
Clements, D. H., & Stephan, M. (2004). Measurement in pre-K–2 mathematics. In D. H. Clements, J. Sarama, & A.-M. DiBi-
ase (Eds.), Engaging young children in mathematics: Standards for early childhood mathematics education (pp. 299–317).
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Clements, D. H., & Swaminathan, S. (1995). Technology and school change: New lamps for old? Childhood Education, 71,
275–281.
Clements, D. H., Battista, M. T., & Sarama, J. (1998). Students’ development of geometric and measurement ideas. In R. Lehrer
& D. Chazan (Eds.), Designing learning environments for developing understanding of geometry and space (pp. 201–225).
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Clements, D. H., Battista, M. T., & Sarama, J. (2001). Logo and geometry. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education Mono-
graph Series, 10.
Clements, D. H., Battista, M. T., Sarama, J., & Swaminathan, S. (1996). Development of turn and turn measurement concepts in a
computer-based instructional unit. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 30, 313–337.
Clements, D. H., Battista, M. T., Sarama, J., & Swaminathan, S. (1997). Development of students’ spatial thinking in a unit on
geometric motions and area. The Elementary School Journal, 98, 171–186.
Clements, D. H., Battista, M. T., Sarama, J., Swaminathan, S., & McMillen, S. (1997). Students’ development of length measure-
ment concepts in a Logo-based unit on geometric paths. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 28(1), 70–95.
Clements, D. H., Nastasi, B. K., & Swaminathan, S. (1993). Young children and computers: Crossroads and directions from
research. Young Children, 48(2), 56–64.
Clements, D. H., Russell, S. J., Tierney, C., Battista, M. T., & Meredith, J. S. (1995). Flips, turns, and area. Cambridge, MA: Dale
Seymour Publications.
References • 341

Clements, D. H., Sarama, J., & DiBiase, A.-M. (2004). Engaging young children in mathematics: Standards for early childhood math-
ematics education. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Clements, D. H., Sarama, J., Spitler, M. E., Lange, A. A., & Wolfe, C. B. (2011). Mathematics learned by young children in an inter-
vention based on learning trajectories: A large-scale cluster randomized trial. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education,
42(2), 127–166.
Clements, D. H., Sarama, J., Wolfe, C. B., & Spitler, M. E. (2013). Longitudinal evaluation of a scale-up model for teaching
mathematics with trajectories and technologies: Persistence of effects in the third year. American Educational Research
Journal, 50(4), 812–850. doi: 10.3102/0002831212469270
Clements, D. H., Swaminathan, S., Hannibal, M. A. Z., & Sarama, J. (1999). Young children’s concepts of shape. Journal for Research
in Mathematics Education, 30, 192–212.
Clifford, R., Barbarin, O., Chang, F., Early, D., Bryant, D., Howes, C., et al. (2005). What is pre-kindergarten? Characteristics of
public pre-kindergarten programs. Applied Developmental Science, 9, 126–143.
Cobb, P. (1990). A constructivist perspective on information-processing theories of mathematical activity. International Journal
of Educational Research, 14, 67–92.
Cobb, P. (1995). Cultural tools and mathematical learning: A case study. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 26, 362–385.
Cobb, P., Wood, T., Yackel, E., Nicholls, J., Wheatley, G., Trigatti, B., et al. (1991). Assessment of a problem-centered second-grade
mathematics project. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 22(1), 3–29.
Cobb, P., Yackel, E., & Wood, T. (1989). Young children’s emotional acts during mathematical problem solving. In D. B. McLeod
& V. M. Adams (Eds.), Affect and mathematical problem solving: A new perspective (pp. 117–148). New York, NY: Springer-
Verlag.
Codding, R. S., Hilt-Panahon, A., Panahon, C. J., & Benson, J. L. (2009). Addressing mathematics computation problems: A review
of simple and moderate intensity interventions. Education and Treatment of Children, 32(2), 279–312.
Cohen, J. (1977). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (Rev. ed.). New York, NY: Academic Press.
Cohen, R., & Geva, E. (1989). Designing Logo-like environments for young children: The interaction between theory and practice.
Journal of Educational Computing Research, 5, 349–377.
Coley, R. J. (2002). An unequal start: Indicators of inequality in school readiness. Princeton, NJ: Educational Testing Service.
Confrey, J., & Kazak, S. (2006). A thirty-year reflection on constructivism in mathematics education in PME. In A. Gutiérrez &
P. Boero (Eds.), Handbook of research on the psychology of mathematics education: Past, present, and future (pp. 305–345).
Rotterdam, the Netherlands: Sense Publishers.
Cook, G. A., Roggman, L. A., & Boyce, L. K. (2012). Fathers’ and mothers’ cognitive stimulation in early play with toddlers:
Predictors of 5th grade reading and math. Family Science, 2, 131–145.
Cook, T. D. (2002). Randomized experiments in educational policy research: A critical examination of the reasons the educational
evaluation community has offered for not doing them. Educational Evaluation and Policy Analysis, 24, 175–199.
Cooper, R. G., Jr. (1984). Early number development: Discovering number space with addition and subtraction. In C. Sophian
(Ed.), Origins of cognitive skills (pp. 157–192). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Cordes, C., & Miller, E. (2000). Fool’s gold: A critical look at computers in childhood. Retrieved November 7, 2000, from http://
www.allianceforchildhood.net/projects/computers/computers_reports.htm
Correa, J., Nunes, T., & Bryant, P. (1998). Young children’s understanding of division: The relationship between division terms in
a noncomputational task. Journal of Educational Psychology, 90, 321–329.
Cowan, N., Saults, J. S., & Elliott, E. M. (2002). The search for what is fundamental in the development of working memory. Ad-
vances in Child Development and Behavior, 29, 1–49.
Crosnoe, R., & Cooper, C. E. (2010). Economically disadvantaged children’s transitions into elementary school: Linking family
processes, school contexts, and educational policy. American Educational Research Journal, 47, 258–291.
Curby, T. W., Brock, L. L., & Hamre, B. K. (2013). Teachers’ emotional support consistency predicts children’s achievement gains
and social skills. Early Education & Development, 24(3), 292–309. doi: 10.1080/10409289.2012.665760
Curby, T. W., Rimm-Kaufman, S. E., & Ponitz, C. C. (2009). Teacher–child interactions and children’s achievement trajectories
across kindergarten and first grade. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101(4), 912–925. doi: 10.1037/a0016647
Currie, J., & Thomas, D. (1995). Does Head Start make a difference? American Economic Review, 85, 341–364.
Curtis, R. P. (2005). Preschoolers’ counting in peer interaction. Paper presented at the American Educational Research Association,
New Orleans, LA.
Cvencek, D., Meltzoff, A. N., & Greenwald, A. G. (2011). Math–gender stereotypes in elementary school children. Child Develop-
ment, 82(3), 766–779. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01529.x
Davidson, J., & Wright, J. L. (1994). The potential of the microcomputer in the early childhood classroom. In J. L. Wright & D. D.
Shade (Eds.), Young children: Active learners in a technological age (pp. 77–91). Washington, D.C.: National Association for
the Education of Young Children.
Davis, R. B. (1984). Learning mathematics: The cognitive science approach to mathematics education. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
Davydov, V. V. (1975). On the formation of an elementary concept of number by the child. In J. Kilpatrick & I. Wirszup (Eds.),
Soviet studies in the psychology of learning and teaching mathematics (Vol. 13). Stanford, CA: School Mathematics Study
Group, Stanford University.
Dawson, D. T. (1953). Number grouping as a function of complexity. The Elementary School Journal, 54, 35–42.
Day, J. D., Engelhardt, J. L., Maxwell, S. E., & Bolig, E. E. (1997). Comparison of static and dynamic assessment procedures and
their relation to independent performance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89(2), 358–368.
Dearing, E., Casey, B. M., Ganley, C. M., Tillinger, M., Laski, E., & Montecillo, C. (2012). Young girls’ arithmetic and spatial skills:
The distal and proximal roles of family socioeconomics and home learning experiences. Early Childhood Research Quarterly,
27, 458–470.
DeCaro, M. S., & Rittle-Johnson, B. (2012). Exploring mathematics problems prepares children to learn from instruction. Journal
of Experimental Child Psychology, 113(4), 552–568. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2012.06.009
Degelman, D., Free, J. U., Scarlato, M., Blackburn, J. M., & Golden, T. (1986). Concept learning in preschool children: Effects of a
short-term Logo experience. Journal of Educational Computing Research, 2(2), 199–205.
Dehaene, S. (1997). The number sense: How the mind creates mathematics. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
342 • References

DeLoache, J. S. (1987). Rapid change in the symbolic functioning of young children. Science, 238, 1556–1557.
DeLoache, J. S., Miller, K. F., & Pierroutsakos, S. L. (1998). Reasoning and problem solving. In D. Kuhn & R. S. Siegler (Eds.),
Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 2. Cognition, perception, & language (5th ed.) (pp. 801–850). New York, NY: Wiley.
DeLoache, J. S., Miller, K. F., Rosengren, K., & Bryant, N. (1997). The credible shrinking room: Very young children’s performance
with symbolic and nonsymbolic relations. Psychological Science, 8, 308–313.
Denton, K., & West, J. (2002). Children’s reading and mathematics achievement in kindergarten and first grade. 2002, from http://
nces.ed.gov/pubsearch/pubsinfo.asp?pubid=2002125.
Desoete, A., Ceulemans, A., De Weerdt, F., & Pieters, S. (2012). Can we predict mathematical learning disabilities from symbolic
and non-symbolic comparison tasks in kindergarten? Findings from a longitudinal study. British Journal of Educational
Psychology, 82(1), 64–81. doi: 10.1348/2044-8279.002002
Dewey, J. (1938/1997). Experience and education. New York, NY: Simon & Schuster.
DHHS. (2005). Head Start impact study: First year findings. Washington, D.C.: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services;
Administration for Children and Families.
Diamond, A., Barnett, W. S., Thomas, J., & Munro, S. (2007). Preschool program improves cognitive control. Science, 318,
1387–1388.
Diaz, R. M. (2008). The role of language in early childhood mathematics: A parallel mixed method study. Doctoral dissertation,
Florida International University. Retrieved from http//search.proquest.com/docview/304815869.
Dinehart, L., & Manfra, L. (2013). Associations between low-income children’s fine motor skills in preschool and academic
performance in second grade. Early Education & Development, 24(2), 138–161. doi: 10.1080/10409289.2011.636729
Dixon, J. K. (1995). Limited English proficiency and spatial visualization in middle school students’ construction of the concepts
of reflection and rotation. The Bilingual Research Journal, 19(2), 221–247.
Doabler, C. T., Cary, M. S., Jungjohann, K., Clarke, B., Fien, H., Baker, S. et al. (2012). Enhancing core mathematics instruction for
students at risk for mathematics disabilities. Teaching Exceptional Children, 44(4), 48–57.
Dobbs, J., Doctoroff, G. L., Fisher, P. H., & Arnold, D. H. (2006). The association between preschool children’s socio-emotional
functioning and their mathematical skill. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 27, 97–108.
Doig, B., McCrae, B., & Rowe, K. (2003). A good start to numeracy: Effective numeracy strategies from research and practice in early
childhood. Canberra ACT, Australia: Australian Council for Educational Research.
Donlan, C. (1998). Number without language? Studies of children with specific language impairments. In C. Donlan (Ed.), The
development of mathematical skills (pp. 255–274). East Sussex, UK: Psychology Press.
Dowker, A. (2004). What works for children with mathematical difficulties? (Research report no. 554). Nottingham, England:
University of Oxford/DfES Publications.
Dowker, A. (2005). Early identification and intervention for students with mathematics difficulties. Journal of Learning Disabili-
ties, 38, 324–332.
Dowker, A. (2009). What works for children with mathematical difficulties? The effectiveness of intervention schemes. Nottingham,
England: DCSF Publications.
Dowker, A., & Sigley, G. (2010). Targeted interventions for children with arithmetical difficulties. British Journal of Educational
Psychology Monographs, II(7), 65–81.
Downs, R. M., & Liben, L. S. (1988). Through the map darkly: Understanding maps as representations. The Genetic Epistemologist,
16, 11–18.
Downs, R. M., Liben, L. S., & Daggs, D. G. (1988). On education and geographers: The role of cognitive developmental theory in
geographic education. Annals of the Association of American Geographers, 78, 680–700.
Draisma, J. (2000). Gesture and oral computation as resources in the early learning of mathematics. In T. Nakahara & M. Koyama
(Eds.), Proceedings of the 24th Conference of the International Group for the Psychology in Mathematics Education (Vol. 2,
pp. 257–264).
Driscoll, M. J. (1983). Research within reach: Elementary school mathematics and reading. St. Louis: CEMREL, Inc.
du Boulay, B. (1986). Part II: Logo confessions. In R. Lawler, B. du Boulay, M. Hughes, & H. Macleod (Eds.), Cognition and com-
puters: Studies in learning (pp. 81–178). Chichester, England: Ellis Horwood Limited.
Duncan, G. J., & Magnuson, K. (2011). The nature and impact of early achievement skills, attention skills, and behavior problems.
In G. J. Duncan & R. Murnane (Eds.), Whither opportunity? Rising inequality and the uncertain life chances of low-income
children (pp. 47–70). New York, NY: Russell Sage Press.
Duncan, G. J., Brooks-Gunn, J., & Klebanov, P. K. (1994). Economic deprivation and early childhood development. Child Develop-
ment, 65, 296–318.
Duncan, G. J., Claessens, A., & Engel, M. (2004). The contributions of hard skills and socio-emotional behavior to school readiness.
Evanston, IL: Northwestern University.
Duncan, G. J., Dowsett, C. J., Claessens, A., Magnuson, K., Huston, A. C., Klebanov, P. et al. (2007). School readiness and later
achievement. Developmental Psychology, 43(6), 1428–1446.
Early, D., Barbarin, O., Burchinal, M. R., Chang, F., Clifford, R., Crawford, G., et al. (2005). Pre-kindergarten in eleven states:
NCEDL’s multi-state study of pre-kindergarten & study of State-Wide Early Education Programs (SWEEP). Chapel Hill, NC:
University of North Carolina.
Early, D. M., Iruka, I. U., Ritchie, S., Barbarin, O. A., Winn, D.-M. C., Crawford, G. M. et al. (2010). How do pre-kindergarteners
spend their time? Gender, ethnicity, and income as predictors of experiences in pre-kindergarten classrooms. Early Child-
hood Research Quarterly, 25(2), 177–193. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2009.10.003
Ebbeck, M. (1984). Equity for boys and girls: Some important issues. Early Child Development and Care, 18, 119–131.
Edens, K. M., & Potter, E. F. (2013). An exploratory look at the relationships among math skills, motivational factors and activity
choice. Early Childhood Education Journal, 41(3), 235–243. doi: 10.1007/s10643-012-0540-y
Edwards, C., Gandini, L., & Forman, G. E. (1993). The hundred languages of children: The Reggio Emilia approach to early childhood
education. Norwood, N.J.: Ablex Publishing Corp.
Ehrlich, S. B., & Levine, S. C. (2007a, March). What low-SES children DO know about number: A comparison of Head Start and
tuition-based preschool children’s number knowledge. Paper presented at the Biennial Meeting of the Society of Research in
Child Development, Boston, MA.
References • 343

Ehrlich, S. B., & Levine, S. C. (2007b, April). The impact of teacher “number talk” in low- and middle-SES preschool classrooms. Paper
presented at the American Educational Research Association, Chicago, IL.
Ehrlich, S. B., Levine, S. C., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2006). The importance of gesture in children’s spatial reasoning. Developmental
Psychology, 42(6), 1259–1268. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.42.6.1259
Eimeren, L. v., MacMillan, K. D., & Ansari, D. (2007, April). The role of subitizing in children’s development of verbal counting. Paper
presented at the Society for Research in Child Development, Boston, MA.
Elia, I., Gagatsis, A., & Demetriou, A. (2007). The effects of different modes of representation on the solution of one-step additive
problems. Learning and Instruction, 17, 658–672.
Elliott, A., & Hall, N. (1997). The impact of self-regulatory teaching strategies on “at-risk” preschoolers’ mathematical learning in
a computer-mediated environment. Journal of Computing in Childhood Education, 8(2/3), 187–198.
Emihovich, C., & Miller, G. E. (1988). Talking to the turtle: A discourse analysis of Logo instruction. Discourse Processes, 11,
183–201.
Entwisle, D. R., & Alexander, K. L. (1990). Beginning school math competence: Minority and majority comparisons. Child De-
velopment, 61, 454–471.
Entwisle, D. R., & Alexander, K. L. (1997). Family type and children’s growth in reading and math over the primary grades. Journal
of Marriage and the Family, 58, 341–355.
Ericsson, K. A., Krampe, R. T., & Tesch-Römer, C. (1993). The role of deliberate practice in the acquisition of expert performance.
Psychological Review, 100, 363–406.
Ernest, P. (1985). The number line as a teaching aid. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 16, 411–424.
Espada, J. P. (2012). The native language in teaching kindergarten mathematics. Journal of International Education Research, 8(4),
359–366.
Espinosa, L. M. (2005). Curriculum and assessment considerations for young children from culturally, linguistically, and
economically diverse backgrounds. Psychology in the Schools, 42(8), 837–853. doi: 10.1002/pits.20115
Evans, D. W. (1983). Understanding infinity and zero in the early school years. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of
Pennsylvania.
Falk, R., Yudilevich-Assouline, P., & Elstein, A. (2012). Children’s concept of probability as inferred from their binary choices—
revisited. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 81(2), 207–233. doi: 10.1007/s10649-012-9402-1
Farran, D. C., Kang, S. J., Aydogan, C., & Lipsey, M. (2005). Preschool classroom environments and the quantity and quality of
children’s literacy and language behaviors. In D. Dickinson & S. Neuman (Eds.), Handbook of early literacy research, Vol. 2.
New York, NY: Guilford Publications.
Farran, D. C., Lipsey, M. W., Watson, B., & Hurley, S. (2007). Balance of content emphasis and child content engagement in an early
reading first program. Paper presented at the American Educational Research Association.
Farran, D. C., Silveri, B., & Culp, A. (1991). Public preschools and the disadvantaged. In L. Rescorla, M. C. Hyson, & K. Hirsh-Pase
(Eds.), Academic instruction in early childhood: Challenge or pressure? New directions for child development (pp. 65–73). San
Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.
Fennema, E. H. (1972). The relative effectiveness of a symbolic and a concrete model in learning a selected mathematics principle.
Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 3, 233–238.
Fennema, E. H., & Tartre, L. A. (1985). The use of spatial visualization in mathematics by girls and boys. Journal for Research in
Mathematics Education, 16, 184–206.
Fennema, E. H., Carpenter, T. P., Frank, M. L., Levi, L., Jacobs, V. R., & Empson, S. B. (1996). A longitudinal study of learning to use
children’s thinking in mathematics instruction. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 27, 403–434.
Fennema, E. H., Carpenter, T. P., Franke, M. L., & Levi, L. (1998). A longitudinal study of gender differences in young children’s
mathematical thinking. Educational Researcher, 27, 6–11.
Feuerstein, R., Rand, Y. a., & Hoffman, M. B. (1979). The dynamic assessment of retarded performers: The Learning Potential
Assessment Device, theory, instruments, and techniques. Baltimore, MD: University Park Press.
Finn, J. D. (2002). Small classes in American schools: Research, practice, and politics. Phi Delta Kappan, 83, 551–560.
Finn, J. D., & Achilles, C. M. (1990). Answers and questions about class size. American Educational Research Journal, 27(3),
557–577.
Finn, J. D., Gerber, S. B., Achilles, C. M., & Boyd-Zaharias, J. (2001). The enduring effects of small classes. Teachers College Record,
103(2), 145–183.
Finn, J. D., Pannozzo, G. M., & Achilles, C. M. (2003). The “why’s” of class size: Student behavior in small classes. Review of Edu-
cational Research, 73, 321–368.
Fisher, K., Hirsh-Pasek, K., & Golinkoff, R. M. (2012). Fostering mathematical thinking through playful learning. In S. Suggate &
E. Reese (Eds.), Contemporary Debates in Childhood Education and Development. New York, NY: Routledge.
Fisher, P. H., Dobbs-Oates, J., Doctoroff, G. L., & Arnold, D. H. (2012). Early math interest and the development of math skills.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 104(3), 673–681. doi: 10.1037/a0027756
Fitzpatrick, C., & Pagani, L. S. (2013). Task-oriented kindergarten behavior pays off in later childhood. Journal of Developmental
& Behavioral Pediatrics, 34(2), 94–101 doi: 10.1097/DBP.0b013e31827a3779
Fitzpatrick, M. D., Grissmer, D., & Hastedt, S. (2011). What a difference a day makes: Estimating daily learning gains during
kindergarten and first grade using a natural experiment. Economics of Education Review, 30(2), 269–279. doi: http://dx.doi.
org/10.1016/j.econedurev.2010.09.004
Fletcher, J. D., Hawley, D. E., & Piele, P. K. (1990). Costs, effects, and utility of microcomputer assisted instruction in the classroom.
American Educational Research Journal, 27, 783–806.
Fletcher-Flinn, C. M., & Gravatt, B. (1995). The efficacy of computer assisted instruction (CAI): A meta-analysis. Journal of
Educational Computing Research, 12, 219–242.
Flexer, R. J. (1989). Conceptualizing addition. Teaching Exceptional Children, 21(4), 21–25.
Fluck, M. (1995). Counting on the right number: Maternal support for the development of cardinality. Irish Journal of Psychology,
16, 133–149.
Fluck, M., & Henderson, L. (1996). Counting and cardinality in English nursery pupils. British Journal of Educational Psychology,
66, 501–517.
344 • References

Ford, M. J., Poe, V., & Cox, J. (1993). Attending behaviors of ADHD children in math and reading using various types of software.
Journal of Computing in Childhood Education, 4, 183–196.
Forman, G. E., & Hill, F. (1984). Constructive play: Applying Piaget in the preschool (rev. ed.). Menlo Park, CA: Addison Wesley.
Fox, J. (2005). Child-initiated mathematical patterning in the pre-compulsory years. In H. L. Chick & J. L. Vincent (Eds.),
Proceedings of the 29th Conference of the International Group for the Psychology in Mathematics Education (Vol. 2,
pp. 313–320). Melbourne, AU: PME.
Fox, J. (2006). A justification for mathematical modelling experiences in the preparatory classroom. In P. Grootenboer,
R. Zevenbergen, & M. Chinnappan (Eds.), Proceedings of the 29th annual conference of the Mathematics Education Research
Group of Australia (pp. 221–228). Canberra, Australia.: MERGA.
Franke, M. L., Carpenter, T. P., & Battey, D. (2008). Algebra in the early grades. In J. J. Kaput, D. W. Carraher, & M. L. Blanton
(Eds.), (pp. 333–359). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Frazier, M. K. (1987). The effects of Logo on angle estimation skills of 7th graders. Unpublished master’s thesis, Wichita State
University.
French, L., & Song, M.-J. (1998). Developmentally appropriate teacher-directed approaches: Images from Korean kindergartens.
Journal of Curriculum Studies, 30, 409–430.
Friedman, L. (1995). The space factor in mathematics: Gender differences. Review of Educational Research, 65(1), 22–50.
Friel, S. N., Curcio, F. R., & Bright, G. W. (2001). Making sense of graphs: Critical factors influencing comprehension and
instructional implications. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 32, 124–158.
Frontera, M. (1994). On the initial learning of mathematics: Does schooling really help? In J. E. H. Van Luit (Ed.), Research on learn-
ing and instruction of mathematics in kindergarten and primary school (pp. 42–59). Doetinchem, the Netherlands: Graviant.
Fryer, J., & Levitt, S. D. (2004). Understanding the Black–White test score gap in the first two years of school. The Review of
Economics and Statistics, 86(2), 447–464.
Fuchs, L. S., Compton, D. L., Fuchs, D., Paulson, K., Bryant, J. D., & Hamlett, C. L. (2005). The prevention, identification, and
cognitive determinants of math difficulty. Journal of Educational Psychology, 97, 493–513.
Fuchs, L. S., Fuchs, D., & Karns, K. (2001). Enhancing kindergartners’ mathematical development: Effects of peer-assisted learning
strategies. Elementary School Journal, 101, 495–510.
Fuchs, L. S., Fuchs, D., Hamlett, C. L., Powell, S. R., Capizzi, A. M., & Seethaler, P. M. (2006). The effects of computer-assisted
instruction on number combination skill in at-risk first graders. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 39, 467–475.
Fuchs, L. S., Geary, D. C., Compton, D. L., Fuchs, D., Schatschneider, C., Hamlett, C. L. et al. (2013). Effects of first-grade
number knowledge tutoring with contrasting forms of practice. Journal of Educational Psychology, 105(1), 58–77.
doi: 10.1037/a0030127.supp
Fuchs, L. S., Powell, S. R., Cirino, P. T., Fletcher, J. M., Fuchs, D., & Zumeta, R. O. (2008). Enhancing number combinations fluency
and math problem-solving skills in third-grade students with math difficulties: A field-based randomized control trial. Paper
presented at the Institute of Education Science 2007 Research Conference.
Fuchs, L. S., Powell, S. R., Hamlett, C. L., Fuchs, D., Cirino, P. T., & Fletcher, J. M. (2008). Remediating computational deficits at
third grade: A randomized field trial. Journal of Research on Educational Effectiveness, 1, 2–32.
Fuchs, L. S., Powell, S. R., Seethaler, P. M., Cirino, P. T., Fletcher, J. M., Fuchs, D. et al. (2010). The effects of strategic counting
instruction, with and without deliberate practice, on number combination skill among students with mathematics
difficulties. Learning and Individual Differences, 20(2), 89–100. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2009.09.003
Fuson, K. C. (1988). Children’s counting and concepts of number. New York, NY: Springer-Verlag.
Fuson, K. C. (1992a). Research on learning and teaching addition and subtraction of whole numbers. In G. Leinhardt, R. Putman,
& R. A. Hattrup (Eds.), Handbook of research on mathematics teaching and learning (pp. 53–187). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates.
Fuson, K. C. (1992b). Research on whole number addition and subtraction. In D. A. Grouws (Ed.), Handbook of research on
mathematics teaching and learning (pp. 243–275). New York, NY: Macmillan.
Fuson, K. C. (1997). Research-based mathematics curricula: New educational goals require programs of four interacting levels of
research. Issues in Education, 3(1), 67–79.
Fuson, K. C. (2004). Pre-K to grade 2 goals and standards: Achieving 21st century mastery for all. In D. H. Clements,
J. Sarama, & A.-M. DiBiase (Eds.), Engaging young children in mathematics: Standards for early childhood mathematics
education (pp. 105–148). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Fuson, K. C. (2009). Mathematically-desirable and accessible whole-number algorithms: Achieving understanding and fluency for all
students. Chicago, IL: Northwestern University.
Fuson, K. C., & Abrahamson, D. (2009). Word problem types, numerical situation drawings, and a conceptual -phase model to
implement an algebraic approach to problem-solving in elementary classrooms. Chicago, IL: Northwestern University.
Fuson, K. C., & Briars, D. J. (1990). Using a base-ten blocks learning/teaching approach for first- and second-grade place-value and
multidigit addition and subtraction. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 21, 180–206.
Fuson, K. C., Perry, T., & Kwon, Y. (1994). Latino, Anglo, and Korean children’s finger addition methods. In J. E. H. Van Luit (Ed.),
Research on learning and instruction of mathematics in kindergarten and primary school (pp. 220–228). Doetinchem, the
Netherlands: Graviant.
Fuson, K. C., Smith, S. T., & Lo Cicero, A. (1997). Supporting Latino first graders’ ten-structured thinking in urban classrooms.
Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 28, 738–760.
Fuson, K. C., Wearne, D., Hiebert, J. C., Murray, H. G., Human, P. G., Olivier, A. I. et al. (1997). Children’s conceptual structures
for multidigit numbers and methods of multidigit addition and subtraction. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education,
28, 130–162.
Gadanidis, G., Hoogland, C., Jarvis, D., & Scheffel, T.-L. (2003). Mathematics as an aesthetic experience. In Proceedings of the 27th
Conference of the International Group for the Psychology in Mathematics Education (Vol. 1, pp. 250). Honolulu, HI: University
of Hawai’i.
Gagatsis, A., & Elia, I. (2004). The effects of different modes of representation on mathematical problem solving. In M. J. Høines
& A. B. Fuglestad (Eds.), Proceedings of the 28th Conference of the International Group for the Psychology in Mathematics
Education (Vol. 2, pp. 447–454). Bergen, Norway: Bergen University College.
References • 345

Galen, F. H. J. v., & Buter, A. (1997). De rol van interactie bij leren rekenen met de computer [Computer tasks and classroom
discussions in mathematics]. Panama-Post. Tijdschrift voor nascholing en onderzoek van het reken-wiskundeonderwijs, 16(1),
11–18.
Gallou-Dumiel, E. (1989). Reflections, point symmetry and Logo. In C. A. Maher, G. A. Goldin, & R. B. Davis (Eds.), Proceedings of
the eleventh annual meeting, North American Chapter of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education
(pp. 149–157). New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University.
Gamel-McCormick, M., & Amsden, D. (2002). Investing in better outcomes: The Delaware early childhood longitudinal study:
Delaware Interagency Resource Management Committee and the Department of Education.
Gathercole, S. E., Tiffany, C., Briscoe, J., Thorn, A., & The, A. T. (2005). Developmental consequences of poor phonological short-
term memory function in childhood: a longitudinal study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46(6), 598–611. doi:
10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00379.x
Gavin, M. K., Casa, T. M., Adelson, J. L., & Firmender, J. M. (2013). The impact of challenging geometry and measurement units
on the achievement of grade 2 students. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 44(3), 478–509.
Geary, D. C. (1990). A componential analysis of an early learning deficit in mathematics. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,
49, 363–383.
Geary, D. C. (1994). Children’s mathematical development: Research and practical applications. Washington, D.C.: American
Psychological Association.
Geary, D. C. (2003). Learning disabilities in arithmetic: Problem solving differences and cognitive deficits. In H. L. Swanson, K.
Harris, & S. Graham (Eds.), Handbook of learning disabilities. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Geary, D. C. (2004). Mathematics and learning disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 37, 4–15.
Geary, D. C. (2006). Development of mathematical understanding. In D. Kuhn, R. S. Siegler, W. Damon, & R. M. Lerner
(Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Volume 2—Cognition, perception, and language (6th ed.) (pp. 777–810). Hoboken, NJ:
Wiley.
Geary, D. C. (2011). Cognitive predictors of achievement growth in mathematics: A 5-year longitudinal study. Developmental
Psychology, 47(6), 1539–1552. doi: 10.1037/a0025510
Geary, D. C. (2013). Early foundations for mathematics learning and their relations to learning disabilities. Current Directions in
Psychological Science, 22(1), 23–27. doi: 10.1177/0963721412469398
Geary, D. C., & Liu, F. (1996). Development of arithmetical competence in Chinese and American children: Influence of age,
language, and schooling. Child Development, 67(5), 2022–2044.
Geary, D. C., Bow-Thomas, C. C., Fan, L., & Siegler, R. S. (1993). Even before formal instruction, Chinese children outperform
American children in mental addition. Cognitive Development, 8, 517–529.
Geary, D. C., Bow-Thomas, C. C., & Yao, Y. (1992). Counting knowledge and skill in cognitive addition: A comparison of normal
and mathematically disabled children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 54, 372–391.
Geary, D. C., Brown, S. C., & Samaranayake, V. A. (1991). Cognitive addition: A short longitudinal study of strategy choice
and speed-of-processing differences in normal and mathematically disabled children. Developmental Psychology, 27(5),
787–797.
Geary, D. C., Hamson, C. O., & Hoard, M. K. (2000). Numerical and arithmetical cognition: A longitudinal study of process and
concept deficits in children with learning disability. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 77, 236–263.
Geary, D. C., Hoard, M. K., Byrd-Craven, J., Nugent, L., & Numtee, C. (2007). Cognitive mechanisms underlying achievement
deficits in children with mathematical learning disability. Child Development, 78, 1343–1359.
Geary, D. C., Hoard, M. K., & Hamson, C. O. (1999). Numerical and arithmetical cognition: Patterns of functions and deficits in
children at risk for a mathematical disability. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 74, 213–239.
Geary, D. C., Hoard, M. K., & Nugent, L. (2012). Independent contributions of the central executive, intelligence, and in-class
attentive behavior to developmental change in the strategies used to solve addition problems. Journal of Experimental Child
Psychology, 113(1), 49–65. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2012.03.003
Gelman, R. (1994). Constructivism and supporting environments. In D. Tirosh (Ed.), Implicit and explicit knowledge: An educa-
tional approach (Vol. 6, pp. 55–82). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
Gelman, R., & Williams, E. M. (1997). Enabling constraints for cognitive development and learning: Domain specificity and epi-
genesis. In D. Kuhn & R. Siegler (Eds.), Cognition, perception, and language. Vol. 2: Handbook of Child Psychology (5th ed.,
pp. 575–630). New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons.
Gersten, R. (1986). Response to “consequences of three preschool curriculum models through age 15.” Early Childhood Research
Quarterly, 1, 293–302.
Gersten, R., & White, W. A. T. (1986). Castles in the sand: Response to Schweinhart and Weikart. Educational Leadership, 44(3)
19–20.
Gersten, R., Chard, D. J., Jayanthi, M., Baker, M. S., Morpy, S. K., & Flojo, J. R. (2008). Teaching mathematics to students with
learning disabilities: A meta-analysis of the intervention research. Portsmouth, NH: RMC Research Corporation, Center on
Instruction.
Gersten, R., Jordan, N. C., & Flojo, J. R. (2005). Early identification and interventions for students with mathematical difficulties.
Journal of Learning Disabilities, 38, 293–304.
Gervasoni, A. (2005). The diverse learning needs of children who were selected for an intervention program. In H. L. Chick & J.
L. Vincent (Eds.), Proceedings of the 29th Conference of the International Group for the Psychology in Mathematics Education
(Vol. 3, pp. 33–40). Melbourne, Australia: PME.
Gervasoni, A., Hadden, T., & Turkenburg, K. (2007). Exploring the number knowledge of children to inform the development of
a professional learning plan for teachers in the Ballarat Diocese as a means of building community capacity. In J. Watson
& K. Beswick (Eds.), Mathematics: Essential research, essential practice (Proceedings of the 30th Annual Conference of the
Mathematics Education Research Group of Australasia) (Vol. 3, pp. 305–314). Hobart, Australia: MERGA.
Gervasoni, A., Parish, L., Hadden, T., Livesey, C., Bevan, K., Croswell, M. et al. (2012). The progress of grade 1 students who
participated in an extending mathematical understanding intervention program. Paper presented at the Mathematics Educa-
tion Research Group of Australasia, Singapore.
Gibson, E. J. (1969). Principles of perceptual learning and development. New York, NY: Appleton-Century-Crofts, Meredith
Corporation.
346 • References

Gilmore, C. K., & Papadatou-Pastou, M. (2009). Patterns of individual differences in conceptual understanding and arithmetical
skill: A meta-analysis. Mathematical Thinking and Learning, 10, 25–40.
Ginsburg, H. P. (1977). Children’s arithmetic. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
Ginsburg, H. P. (1997). Mathematics learning disabilities: A view from developmental psychology. Journal of Learning Disabilities,
30, 20–33.
Ginsburg, H. P. (2008). Mathematics education for young children: What it is and how to promote it. Social Policy Report, 22(1),
1–24.
Ginsburg, H. P., & Russell, R. L. (1981). Social class and racial influences on early mathematical thinking. Monographs of the Society
for Research in Child Development, 46(6, Serial No. 193).
Ginsburg, H. P., Choi, Y. E., Lopez, L. S., Netley, R., & Chi, C.-Y. (1997). Happy birthday to you: The early mathematical think-
ing of Asian, South American, and U.S. children. In T. Nunes & P. Bryant (Eds.), Learning and teaching mathematics: An
international perspective (pp. 163–207). East Sussex, England: Psychology Press.
Ginsburg, H. P., Duch, H., Ertle, B., & Noble, K. G. (2012). How can parents help their children learn math? In B. H. Wasik (Ed.),
Handbook of family literacy (2nd ed., pp. 496). New York, NY: Routledge.
Ginsburg, H. P., Inoue, N., & Seo, K.-H. (1999). Young children doing mathematics: Observations of everyday activities. In
J. V. Copley (Ed.), Mathematics in the early years (pp. 88–99). Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
Ginsburg, H. P., Klein, A., & Starkey, P. (1998). The development of children’s mathematical thinking: Connecting research with
practice. In W. Damon, I. E. Sigel, & K. A. Renninger (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology. Volume 4: Child psychology in
practice (pp. 401–476). New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons.
Ginsburg, H. P., Ness, D., & Seo, K.-H. (2003). Young American and Chinese children’s everyday mathematical activity.
Mathematical Thinking and Learning, 5, 235–258.
Gormley, W. T., Jr., Gayer, T., Phillips, D., & Dawson, B. (2005). The effects of universal pre-K on cognitive development. Devel-
opmental Psychology, 41, 872–884.
Graham, T. A., Nash, C., & Paul, K. (1997). Young children’s exposure to mathematics: The child care context. Early Childhood
Education Journal, 25, 31–38.
Grant, S. G., Peterson, P. L., & Shojgreen-Downer, A. (1996). Learning to teach mathematics in the context of system reform.
American Educational Research Journal, 33(2), 509–541.
Gravemeijer, K. P. E. (1990). Realistic geometry instruction. In K. P. E. Gravemeijer, M. van den Heuvel, & L. Streefland (Eds.),
Contexts free productions tests and geometry in realistic mathematics education (pp. 79–91). Utrecht, the Netherlands:
OW&OC.
Gravemeijer, K. P. E. (1991). An instruction-theoretical reflection on the use of manipulatives. In L. Streefland (Ed.),
Realistic mathematics education in primary school (pp. 57–76). Utrecht, the Netherlands: Freudenthal Institute, Utrecht
University.
Gray, E. M., & Pitta, D. (1997). Number processing: Qualitative differences in thinking and the role of imagery. In L. Puig & A.
Gutiérrez (Eds.), Proceedings of the 20th Annual Conference of the Mathematics Education Research Group of Australasia
(Vol. 3, pp. 35–42).
Gray, E. M., & Pitta, D. (1999). Images and their frames of reference: A perspective on cognitive development in elementary arith-
metic. In O. Zaslavsky (Ed.), Proceedings of the 23rd Conference of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics
Education (Vol. 3, pp. 49–56). Haifa, Israel: Technion.
Greabell, L. C. (1978). The effect of stimuli input on the acquisition of introductory geometric concepts by elementary school
children. School Science and Mathematics, 78(4), 320–326.
Green, J. A. K., & Goswami, U. (2007). Synaesthesia and number cognition in children. Paper presented at the Society for Research
in Child Development.
Greeno, J. G., & Riley, M. S. (1987). Processes and development of understanding. In R. E. Weinert & R. H. Kluwe (Eds.), Metacog-
nition, motivation, and understanding (pp. 289–313): Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Greenwood, C. R., Delquadri, J. C., & Hall, R. V. (1989). Longitudinal effects of classwide peer tutoring. Journal of Educational
Psychology, 81, 371–383.
Griffin, S. (2004). Number Worlds: A research-based mathematics program for young children. In D. H. Clements, J. Sarama,
& A.-M. DiBiase (Eds.), Engaging young children in mathematics: Standards for early childhood mathematics education
(pp. 325–342). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Griffin, S., & Case, R. (1997). Re-thinking the primary school math curriculum: An approach based on cognitive science. Issues
in Education, 3(1), 1–49.
Griffin, S., Case, R., & Capodilupo, A. (1995). Teaching for understanding: The importance of the Central Conceptual Structures
in the elementary mathematics curriculum. In A. McKeough, J. Lupart, & A. Marini (Eds.), Teaching for transfer: Fostering
generalization in learning (pp. 121–151). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Griffin, S., Case, R., & Siegler, R. S. (1994). Rightstart: Providing the central conceptual prerequisites for first formal learning
of arithmetic to students at risk for school failure. In K. McGilly (Ed.), Classroom lessons: Integrating cognitive theory and
classroom practice (pp. 25–49). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Grissmer, D., Grimm, K. J., Aiyer, S. M., Murrah, W. M., & Steele, J. S. (2010). Fine motor skills and early comprehension of the
world: Two new school readiness indicators. Developmental Psychology, 46(5), 1008–1017. doi: 10.1037/a0020104.supp
Grupe, L. A., & Bray, N. W. (1999). What role do manipulatives play in kindergartners’ accuracy and strategy use when solving
simple addition problems? Albuquerque, NM: Society for Research in Child Development.
Guarino, C., Dieterle, S. G., Bargagliotti, A. E., & Mason, W. M. (2013). What can we learn about effective early mathemat-
ics teaching? A framework for estimating causal effects using longitudinal survey data. Journal of Research on Educational
Effectiveness, 6, 164–198.
Gunderson, E. A., & Levine, S. C. (2011). Some types of parent number talk count more than others: Relation between
parents’ input and children’s number knowledge. Developmental Science, 14(5), 1021–1032. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-
7687.2011.01050.x
Gunderson, E., Ramirez, G., Levine, S., & Beilock, S. (2012). The role of parents and teachers in the development of gender-related
math attitudes. Sex Roles, 66(3–4), 153–166. doi: 10.1007/s11199-011-9996-2
References • 347

Halle, T. G., Kurtz-Costes, B., & Mahoney, J. L. (1997). Family influences on school achievement in low-income, African American
children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 527–537.
Hamre, B. K., & Pianta, R. C. (2001). Early teacher-child relationships and the trajectory of children’s school outcomes through
eighth grade. Child Development, 72, 625–638.
Hancock, C. M. (1995). Das Erlernen der Datenanalyse durch anderweitige Beschäftigungen: Grundlagen von Datencompetenz
bei Schülerinnen und Schülern in den klassen 1 bis 7. [Learning data analysis by doing something else: Foundations of data
literacy in grades 1–7]. Computer und Unterricht, 17(1).
Hanich, L. B., Jordan, N. C., Kaplan, D., & Dick, J. (2001). Performance across different areas of mathematical cognition in
children with learning difficulties. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93, 615–626.
Hannula, M. M. (2005). Spontaneous focusing on numerosity in the development of early mathematical skills. Turku, Finland:
University of Turku.
Hannula, M. M., Lepola, J., & Lehtinen, E. (2007). Spontaneous focusing on numerosity at Kindergarten predicts arithmetical but not
reading skills at grade 2. Paper presented at the Society for Research in Child Development.
Harris, L. J. (1981). Sex-related variations in spatial skill. In L. S. Liben, A. H. Patterson, & N. Newcombe (Eds.), Spatial representation
and behavior across the life span (pp. 83–125). New York, NY: Academic Press.
Harrison, C. (2004). Giftedness in early childhood: The search for complexity and connection. Roeper Review, 26(2), 78–84.
Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. Baltimore, MD: Paul
H. Brookes.
Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1999). The social world of children: Learning to talk. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes.
Hasselbring, T. S., Goin, L. I., & Bransford, J. (1988). Developing math automaticity in learning handicapped children: The role of
computerized drill and practice. Focus on Exceptional Children, 20(6), 1–7.
Hasselhorn, M., & Linke-Hasselhorn, K. (2013). Fostering early numerical skills at school start in children at risk for
mathematical achievement problems: A small sample size training study. International Educational Studies, 6(3), 213–220.
doi: doi:10.5539/ies.v6n3p213
Hatano, G., & Sakakibara, T. (2004). Commentary: Toward a cognitive-sociocultural psychology of mathematical and analogical
development. In L. D. English (Ed.), Mathematical and analogical reasoning of young learners (pp. 187–200). Mahwah, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Hattikudur, S., & Alibali, M. (2007). Learning about the equal sign: Does contrasting with inequalities help? Paper presented at the
Society for Research in Child Development.
Haugland, S. W. (1992). Effects of computer software on preschool children’s developmental gains. Journal of Computing in
Childhood Education, 3(1), 15–30.
Hausken, E. G., & Rathbun, A. (2004). Mathematics instruction in kindergarten: Classroom practices and outcomes. Paper presented
at the American Educational Research Association.
Hegarty, M., & Kozhevnikov, M. (1999). Types of visual-spatial representations and mathematical problems-solving. Journal of
Educational Psychology, 91, 684–689.
Hemmeter, M. L., Ostrosky, M. M., & Fox, L. (2006). Social emotional foundations for early learning: A conceptual model for
intervention. School Psychology Review, 35, 583–601.
Hemphill, J. A. R. (1987). The effects of meaning and labeling on four-year-olds’ ability to copy triangles. Columbus, OH: The Ohio
State University.
Henry, V J., & Brown, R. S. (2008). First-grade basic facts: An investigation into teaching and learning of an accelerated,
high-demand memorization standard. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 39, 153–183.
Heuvel-Panhuizen, M. v. d. (1996). Assessment and realistic mathematics education. Utrecht, the Netherlands: Freudenthal
Institute, Utrecht University.
Hiebert, J. C. (1999). Relationships between research and the NCTM Standards. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education,
30, 3–19.
Hiebert, J. C., & Grouws, D. A. (2007). The effects of classroom mathematics teaching on students’ learning. In F. K. Lester, Jr.
(Ed.), Second handbook of research on mathematics teaching and learning (pp. 371–404). New York, NY: Information Age
Publishing.
Hiebert, J. C., & Wearne, D. (1992). Links between teaching and learning place value with understanding in first grade. Journal for
Research in Mathematics Education, 23, 98–122.
Hiebert, J. C., & Wearne, D. (1993). Instructional tasks, classroom discourse, and student learning in second-grade classrooms.
American Educational Research Journal, 30, 393–425.
Hiebert, J. C., & Wearne, D. (1996). Instruction, understanding, and skill in multidigit addition and subtraction. Cognition and
Instruction, 14, 251–283.
Hinkle, D. (2000). School involvement in early childhood. Washington, D.C.: National Institute on Early Childhood Development
and Education, U.S. Department of Education, Office of Educational Research and Improvement.
Hitch, G. J., & McAuley, E. (1991). Working memory in children with specific arithmetical learning disabilities. British Journal of
Psychology, 82, 375–386.
Holloway, S. D., Rambaud, M. F., Fuller, B., & Eggers-Pierola, C. (1995). What is “appropriate practice” at home and in child care?:
Low-income mothers’ views on preparing their children for school. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 10, 451–473.
Holt, J. (1982). How children fail. New York, NY: Dell.
Holton, D., Ahmed, A., Williams, H., & Hill, C. (2001). On the importance of mathematical play. International Journal of Math-
ematical Education in Science and Technology, 32, 401–415.
Hopkins, S. L., & Lawson, M. J. (2004). Explaining variability in retrieval times for addition produced by students with math-
ematical learning difficulties. In M. J. Høines & A. B. Fuglestad (Eds.), Proceedings of the 28th Conference of the International
Group for the Psychology in Mathematics Education (Vol. 3, pp. 57–64). Bergen, Norway: Bergen University College.
Horne, M. (2004). Early gender differences. In M. J. Høines & A. B. Fuglestad (Eds.), Proceedings of the 28th Conference of the Inter-
national Group for the Psychology in Mathematics Education (Vol. 3, pp. 65–72). Bergen, Norway: Bergen University College.
Horne, M. (2005). The effects of number knowledge at school entry on subsequent number development: A five-year longitudinal
study. In P. Clarkson, A. Downton, D. Gronn, M. Horne, A. McDonough, R. Pierce, & A. Roche, (Eds.). Building connections:
348 • References

Research, theory and practice (Proceedings of the 28th annual conference of the Mathematics Education Research Group of
Australasia) (pp. 443–450). Melbourne, Australia: MERGA.
Howard, J. R., Watson, J. A., & Allen, J. (1993). Cognitive style and the selection of Logo problem-solving strategies by young black
children. Journal of Educational Computing Research, 9, 339–354.
Howes, C., Fuligni, A. S., Hong, S. S., Huang, Y. D., & Lara-Cinisomo, S. (2013). The preschool instructional context and
child–teacher relationships. Early Education & Development, 24(3), 273–291. doi: 10.1080/10409289.2011.649664
Hsieh, W.-Y., Hemmeter, M. L., McCollum, J. A., & Ostrosky, M. M. (2009). Using coaching to increase preschool teachers’ use of
emergent literacy teaching strategies. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 24, 229–247.
Hudson, T. (1983). Correspondences and numerical differences between disjoint sets. Child Development, 54, 84–90.
Hughes, M. (1981). Can preschool children add and subtract? Educational Psychology, 1, 207–219.
Hughes, M. (1986). Children and number: Difficulties in learning mathematics. Oxford, England: Basil Blackwell.
Hungate, H. (1982, January). Computers in the kindergarten. The Computing Teacher, 9, 15–18.
Hunting, R., & Pearn, C. (2003). The mathematical thinking of young children: Pre-K–2. In N. S. Pateman, B. J. Dougherty, &
J. Zilliox (Eds.), Proceedings of the 27th Conference of the International Group for the Psychology in Mathematics Education
(Vol. 1, p. 187). Honolulu, HI: University of Hawai’i.
Hutinger, P. L., & Johanson, J. (2000). Implementing and maintaining an effective early childhood comprehensive technology
system. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 20(3), 159–173.
Hutinger, P. L., Bell, C., Beard, M., Bond, J., Johanson, J., & Terry, C. (1998). The early childhood emergent literacy technology re-
search study. Final report. Macomb, IL: Western Illinois University.
Huttenlocher, J., Jordan, N. C., & Levine, S. C. (1994). A mental model for early arithmetic. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
General, 123, 284–296.
Huttenlocher, J., Levine, S. C., & Ratliff, K. R. (2011). The development of measurement: From holistic perceptual comparison to
unit understanding. In N. L. Stein & S. Raudenbush (Eds.), Developmental science goes to school: Implications for education
and public policy research. New York, NY: Taylor and Francis.
Hyde, J. S., Fennema, E. H., & Lamon, S. J. (1990). Gender differences in mathematics performance: A meta-analysis. Psychological
Bulletin, 107, 139–155.
Irwin, K. C., Vistro-Yu, C. P., & Ell, F. R. (2004). Understanding linear measurement: A comparison of Filipino and New Zealand
children. Mathematics Education Research Journal, 16(2), 3–24.
Ishigaki, E. H., Chiba, T., & Matsuda, S. (1996). Young children’s communication and self expression in the technological era. Early
Childhood Development and Care, 119, 101–117.
James, W. (1892/1958). Talks to teachers on psychology: And to students on some of life’s ideas. New York, NY: Norton.
Janzen, J. (2008). Teaching English language learners. Review of Educational Research, 78, 1010–1038.
Jayanthi, M., Gersten, R., & Baker, S. (2008). Mathematics instruction for students with learning disabilities or difficulty learning
mathematics: A guide for teachers. Portsmouth, NH: RMC Research Corporation, Center on Instruction.
Jenks, K. M., van Lieshout, E. C. D. M., & de Moor, J. M. H. (2012). Cognitive correlates of mathematical achievement in
children with cerebral palsy and typically developing children. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 82(1), 120–135. doi:
10.1111/j.2044-8279.2011.02034.x
Jimerson, S., Egeland, B., & Teo, A. (1999). A longitudinal study of achievement trajectories: Factors associated with change.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 116–126.
Johnson, D. W., & Johnson, R. T. (2009). An educational psychology success story: Social interdependence theory and cooperative
learning. Educational Researcher, 38(5), 365–379.
Johnson, M. (1987). The body in the mind. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.
Johnson, V. M. (2000). An investigation of the effects of instructional strategies on conceptual understanding of young children in
mathematics. New Orleans, LA: American Educational Research Association.
Johnson-Gentile, K., Clements, D. H., & Battista, M. T. (1994). The effects of computer and noncomputer environments on stu-
dents’ conceptualizations of geometric motions. Journal of Educational Computing Research, 11, 121–140.
Jordan, J.-A., Wylie, J., & Mulhern, G. (2007). Ability profiles of five to six-year-olds with mathematical learning difficulties. Paper
presented at the Society for Research in Child Development.
Jordan, K. E., Suanda, S. H., & Brannon, E. M. (2008). Intersensory redundancy accelerates preverbal numerical competence.
Cognition, 108, 210–221.
Jordan, N. C., & Montani, T. O. (1997). Cognitive arithmetic and problem solving: A comparison of children with specific and
general mathematics difficulties. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 30, 624–634.
Jordan, N. C., Glutting, J., & Ramineni, C. (2009). The importance of number sense to mathematics achievement in first and third
grades. Learning and Individual Differences, 22(1), 82–88.
Jordan, N. C., Glutting, J., Ramineni, C., & Watkins, M. W. (2010). Validating a number sense screening tool for use in
kindergarten and first grade: Prediction of mathematics proficiency in third grade. School Psychology Review, 39(2),
181–195.
Jordan, N. C., Hanich, L. B., & Kaplan, D. (2003). A longitudinal study of mathematical competencies in children with
specific mathematics difficulties versus children with comorbid mathematics and reading difficulties. Child Development,
74, 834–850.
Jordan, N. C., Hanich, L. B., & Uberti, H. Z. (2003). Mathematical thinking and learning difficulties. In A. J. Baroody & A. Dowk-
er (Eds.), The development of arithmetic concepts and skills: Constructing adaptive expertise (pp. 359–383). Mahwah, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Jordan, N. C., Huttenlocher, J., & Levine, S. C. (1992). Differential calculation abilities in young children from middle- and low-
income families. Developmental Psychology, 28, 644–653.
Jordan, N. C., Huttenlocher, J., & Levine, S. C. (1994). Assessing early arithmetic abilities: Effects of verbal and nonverbal response
types on the calculation performance of middle- and low-income children. Learning and Individual Differences, 6, 413–432.
Jordan, N. C., Kaplan, D., & Hanich, L. B. (2002). Achievement growth in children with learning difficulties in mathematics: Find-
ings of a two-year longitudinal study. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94, 586–597.
Jordan, N. C., Kaplan, D., Locuniak, M. N., & Ramineni, C. (2006). Predicting first-grade math achievement from developmental
number sense trajectories. Learning Disabilities Research and Practice, 22(1), 36–46.
References • 349

Jordan, N. C., Kaplan, D., Oláh, L. N., & Locuniak, M. N. (2006). Number sense growth in kindergarten: A longitudinal investiga-
tion of children at risk for mathematics difficulties. Child Development, 77, 153–175.
Kamii, C. (1973). Pedagogical principles derived from Piaget’s theory: Relevance for educational practice. In M. Schwebel & J.
Raph (Eds.), Piaget in the classroom (pp. 199–215). New York, NY: Basic Books.
Kamii, C. (1985). Young children reinvent arithmetic: Implications of Piaget’s theory. New York, NY: Teaching College Press.
Kamii, C. (1986). Place value: An explanation of its difficulty and educational implications for the primary grades. Journal of
Research in Childhood Education, 1, 75–86.
Kamii, C. (1989). Young children continue to reinvent arithmetic: 2nd grade. Implications of Piaget’s theory. New York, NY: Teaching
College Press.
Kamii, C., & DeVries, R. (1980). Group games in early education: Implications of Piaget’s theory. Washington, D.C.: National As-
sociation for the Education of Young Children.
Kamii, C., & Dominick, A. (1997). To teach or not to teach algorithms. Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 16, 51–61.
Kamii, C., & Dominick, A. (1998). The harmful effects of algorithms in grades 1–4. In L. J. Morrow & M. J. Kenney (Eds.), The
teaching and learning of algorithms in school mathematics (pp. 130–140). Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of Math-
ematics.
Kamii, C., & Housman, L. B. (1999). Young children reinvent arithmetic: Implications of Piaget’s theory (2nd ed.). New York, NY:
Teachers College Press.
Kamii, C., & Kato, Y. (2005). Fostering the development of logico-mathematical knowledge in a card game at ages 5–6. Early
Education & Development, 16, 367–383.
Kamii, C., & Russell, K. A. (2012). Elapsed time: Why is it so difficult to teach? Journal for Research in Mathematics Education,
43(3), 296–315.
Kamii, C., Rummelsburg, J., & Kari, A. R. (2005). Teaching arithmetic to low-performing, low-SES first graders. Journal of
Mathematical Behavior, 24, 39–50.
Kaput, J. J., Carraher, D. W., & Blanton, M. L. (Eds.). (2008). Algebra in the early grades. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1990). Constraints on representational change: Evidence from children’s drawing. Cognition, 34, 57–83.
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1992). Beyond modularity: A developmental perspective on cognitive science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Karoly, L. A., Greenwood, P. W., Everingham, S. S., Houbé, J., Kilburn, M. R., Rydell, C. P., et al. (1998). Investing in our
children: What we know and don’t know about the costs and benefits of early childhood interventions. Santa Monica, CA: Rand
Education.
Kawai, N., & Matsuzawa, T. (2000). Numerical memory span in a chimpanzee. Nature, 403, 39–40.
Keller, S., & Goldberg, I. (1997). Let’s Learn Shapes with Shapely-CAL. Great Neck, NY: Creative Adaptations for Learning, Inc.
Kersh, J., Casey, B. M., & Young, J. M. (2008). Research on spatial skills and block building in girls and boys: The relationship to
later mathematics learning. In B. Spodek & O. N. Saracho (Eds.), Contemporary perspectives on mathematics in early child-
hood education (pp. 233–251). Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishing.
Kidd, J. K., Carlson, A. G., Gadzichowski, K. M., Boyer, C. E., Gallington, D. A., & Pasnak, R. (2013). Effects of patterning instruc-
tion on the academic achievement of 1st-grade children. Journal of Research in Childhood Education, 27(2), 224–238. doi:
10.1080/02568543.2013.766664
Kieran, C. (1986). Logo and the notion of angle among fourth and sixth grade children. In C. Hoyles & L. Burton (Eds.), Pro-
ceedings of the tenth annual meeting of the International Group for the Psychology in Mathematics Education (pp. 99–104).
London, England: City University.
Kieran, C., & Hillel, J. (1990). “It’s tough when you have to make the triangles angles”: Insights from a computer-based geometry
environment. Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 9, 99–127.
Kilpatrick, J. (1987). Problem formulating: Where do good problems come from? In A. H. Schoenfeld (Ed.), Cognitive science and
mathematics education (pp. 123–147). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Kilpatrick, J., Swafford, J., & Findell, B. (Eds.) (2001). Adding it up: Helping children learn mathematics. Washington, D.C.: Math-
ematics Learning Study Committee, National Research Council; National Academies Press.
Kim, S.-Y. (1994). The relative effectiveness of hands-on and computer-simulated manipulatives in teaching seriation, classifica-
tion, geometric, and arithmetic concepts to kindergarten children. Dissertation Abstracts International, 54/09, 3319.
King, J. A., & Alloway, N. (1992). Preschooler’s use of microcomputers and input devices. Journal of Educational Computing Re-
search, 8, 451–468.
Kleemans, T., Segers, E., & Verhoeven, L. (2013). Relations between home numeracy experiences and basic calculation skills of
children with and without specific language impairment. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 28(2), 415–423. doi: http://
dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2012.10.004
Klein, A., & Starkey, P. (2004). Fostering preschool children’s mathematical development: Findings from the Berkeley Math Readi-
ness Project. In D. H. Clements, J. Sarama, & A.-M. DiBiase (Eds.), Engaging young children in mathematics: Standards for
early childhood mathematics education (pp. 343–360). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Klein, A., Starkey, P., & Ramirez, A. B. (2002). Pre-K mathematics curriculum. Glenview, IL: Scott Foresman.
Klein, A., Starkey, P., & Wakeley, A. (1999). Enhancing pre-kindergarten children’s readiness for school mathematics. Paper presented
at the American Educational Research Association.
Klein, A. S., Beishuizen, M., & Treffers, A. (1998). The empty number line in Dutch second grades: Realistic versus gradual pro-
gram design. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 29, 443–464.
Klibanoff, R. S., Levine, S. C., Huttenlocher, J., Vasilyeva, M., & Hedges, L. V. (2006). Preschool children’s mathematical knowledge:
The effect of teacher “math talk”. Developmental Psychology, 42, 59–69.
Klinzing, D. G., & Hall, A. (1985). A study of the behavior of children in a preschool equipped with computers. Chicago: American
Educational Research Association.
Knapp, M. S., Shields, P. M., & Turnbull, B. J. (1992). Academic challenge for the children of poverty. Washington, D.C.: U.S. Depart-
ment of Education.
Kolkman, M. E., Kroesbergen, E. H., & Leseman, P. P. M. (2013). Early numerical development and the role of non-symbolic and
symbolic skills. Learning and Instruction, 25(165), 95–103. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2012.12.001
Konold, C., & Pollatsek, A. (2002). Data analysis as the search for signals in noisy processes. Journal for Research in Mathematics
Education, 33, 259–289.
350 • References

Konold, T. R., & Pianta, R. C. (2005). Empirically-derived, person-oriented patterns of school readiness in typically-developing
children: Description and prediction to first-grade achievement. Applied Developmental Science, 9, 174–187.
Koponen, T., Aunola, K., Ahonen, T., & Nurmi, J.-E. (2007). Cognitive predictors of single-digit and procedural calculation and
their covariation with reading skill. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 97, 220–241.
Koponen, T., Salmi, P., Eklund, K., & Aro, T. (2013). Counting and RAN: Predictors of arithmetic calculation and reading fluency.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 105(1), 162–175. doi: 10.1037/a0029285
Kostos, K., & Shin, E.-K. (2010). Using math journals to enhance second graders’ communication of mathematical thinking. Early
Childhood Education Journal, 38(3), 223–231.
Kovas, Y., Haworth, C. M. A., Dale, P. S., & Plomin, R. (2007). The genetic and environmental origins of learning abilities and
disabilities in the early school years. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 72, whole number 3, Serial
No. 188, 1–144.
Krajewski, K. (2005) “Prediction of mathematical (dis-)abilities in primary school: A 4-year German longitudinal study from
kindergarten to grade 4.” Paper presented at the Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Atlanta,
GA, April 2005.
Krajewski, K., & Schneider, W. (2009). Exploring the impact of phonological awareness, visual–spatial working memory, and
preschool quantity–number competencies on mathematics achievement in elementary school: Findings from a 3-year
longitudinal study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 103(4), 516–531. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2009.03.009
Kretlow, A. G., Wood, C. L., & Cooke, N. L. (2011). Using in-service and coaching to increase kindergarten teachers’ accurate
delivery of group instructional units. The Journal of Special Education, 44(4), 234–246.
Kull, J. A. (1986). Learning and Logo. In P. F. Campbell & G. G. Fein (Eds.), Young children and microcomputers (pp. 103–130).
Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Kutscher, B., Linchevski, L., & Eisenman, T. (2002). From the Lotto game to subtracting two-digit numbers in first-graders. In A.
D. Cockburn & E. Nardi (Eds.), Proceedings of the 26th Conference of the International Group for the Psychology in Mathemat-
ics Education (Vol. 3, pp. 249–256).
Lamon, W. E., & Huber, L. E. (1971). The learning of the vector space structure by sixth grade students. Educational Studies in
Mathematics, 4, 166–181.
Lamy, C. E., Frede, E., Seplocha, H., Strasser, J., Jambunathan, S., Juncker, J. A., et al. (2004). Inch by inch, row by row, gonna
make this garden grow: Classroom quality and language skills in the Abbott Preschool Program [Publication]. Retrieved
September 29, 2007, from http://nieer.org/docs/?DocID=94
Lan, X., Legare, C. H., Ponitz, C. C., Li, S., & Morrison, F. J. (2011). Investigating the links between the subcomponents of executive
function and academic achievement: A cross-cultural analysis of Chinese and American preschoolers. Journal of Experimen-
tal Child Psychology, 108, 677–692. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2010.11.001
Landau, B. (1988). The construction and use of spatial knowledge in blind and sighted children. In J. Stiles-Davis, M. Kritchevsky,
& U. Bellugi (Eds.), Spatial cognition: Brain bases and development (pp. 343–371). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Landerl, K., Bevan, A., & Butterworth, B. (2004). Developmental dyscalculia and basic numerical capacities: A study of
8–9-year-old children. Cognition, 93(99–125).
Lane, C. (2010). Case study: The effectiveness of virtual manipulatives in the teaching of primary mathematics. (Master thesis),
University of Limerick, Limerick, UK. Retrieved from http://digitalcommons.fiu.edu/etd/229
Langhorst, P., Ehlert, A., & Fritz, A. (2012). Non-numerical and numerical understanding of the part–whole concept of children
aged 4 to 8 in word problems. Journal für Mathematik-Didaktik, 33(2), 233–262. doi: 10.1007/s13138-012-0039-5
Lansdell, J. M. (1999). Introducing young children to mathematical concepts: Problems with “new” terminology. Educational
Studies, 25, 327–333.
Lara-Cinisomo, S., Pebley, A. R., Vaiana, M. E., & Maggio, E. (2004). Are L.A.’s children ready for school? Santa Monica, CA:
Rand.
Laski, E. V., Casey, B. M., Yu, Q., Dulaney, A., Heyman, M., & Dearing, E. (2013). Spatial skills as a predictor of first grade girls’ use
of higher level arithmetic strategies. Learning and Individual Differences, 23(1), 123–130. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.
lindif.2012.08.001
Laurillard, D., & Taylor, J. (1994). Designing the Stepping Stones: An evaluation of interactive media in the classroom. Journal of
Educational Television, 20, 169–184.
Lavin, R. J., & Sanders, J. E. (1983). Longitudinal evaluation of the C/A/I Computer Assisted Instruction Title 1 Project: 1979–82.
Chelmsford, MA: Merrimack Education Center.
Lebens, M., Graff, M., & Mayer, P. (2011). The affective dimensions of mathematical difficulties in schoolchildren. Education
Research International, 2011, 1–13.
Lebron-Rodriguez, D. E., & Pasnak, R. (1977). Induction of intellectual gains in blind children. Journal of Experimental Child
Psychology, 24, 505–515.
Lee, J. (2002). Racial and ethnic achievement gap trends: Reversing the progress toward equity? Educational Researcher, 31,
3–12.
Lee, J. (2004). Correlations between kindergarten teachers’ attitudes toward mathematics and teaching practice. Journal of Early
Childhood Teacher Education, 25(2), 173–184.
Lee, J. S., & Ginsburg, H. P. (2007). What is appropriate mathematics education for four-year-olds? Journal of Early Childhood
Research, 5(1), 2–31.
Lee, K., Ng, S. F., Pe, M. L., Ang, S. Y., Hasshim, M. N. A. M., & Bull, R. (2012). The cognitive underpinnings of emerging math-
ematical skills: Executive functioning, patterns, numeracy, and arithmetic. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 82(1),
82–99. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8279.2010.02016.x
Lee, S. A., Spelke, E. S., & Vallortigara, G. (2012). Chicks, like children, spontaneously reorient by three-dimensional environmen-
tal geometry, not by image matching. Biology Letters, 8(4), 492–494. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2012.0067
Lee, V. E., & Burkam, D. T. (2002). Inequality at the starting gate. Washington, D.C.: Economic Policy Institute.
Lee, V. E., Brooks-Gunn, J., Schnur, E., & Liaw, F.-R. (1990). Are Head Start effects sustained? A longitudinal follow-up compari-
son of disadvantaged children attending Head Start, no preschool, and other preschool programs. Child Development, 61,
495–507.
References • 351

Lee, V. E., Burkam, D. T., Ready, D. D., Honigman, J. J., & Meisels, S. J. (2006). Full-day vs. half-day kindergarten: In which program
do children learn more? American Journal of Education, 112, 163–208.
Leeson, N. (1995). Investigations of kindergarten students’ spatial constructions. In B. Atweh & S. Flavel (Eds.), Proceedings of
18th Annual Conference of Mathematics Education Research Group of Australasia (pp. 384–389). Darwin, AU: Mathematics
Education Research Group of Australasia.
Leeson, N., Stewart, R., & Wright, R. J. (1997). Young children’s knowledge of three-dimensional shapes: Four case studies. In
F. Biddulph & K. Carr (Eds.), Proceedings of the 20th Annual Conference of the Mathematics Education Research Group of
Australasia (Vol. 1, pp. 310–317). Hamilton, New Zealand: MERGA.
LeFevre, J.-A., Berrigan, L., Vendetti, C., Kamawar, D., Bisanz, J., Skwarchuk, S.-L. et al. (2013). The role of executive attention in
the acquisition of mathematical skills for children in grades 2 through 4. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 114(2),
243–261. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2012.10.005
LeFevre, J.-A., Polyzoi, E., Skwarchuk, S.-L., Fast, L., & Sowinskia, C. (2010). Do home numeracy and literacy practices of Greek
and Canadian parents predict the numeracy skills of kindergarten children? International Journal of Early Years Education,
18(1), 55–70.
Lehrer, R. (2003). Developing understanding of measurement. In J. Kilpatrick, W. G. Martin, & D. Schifter (Eds.), A Research
companion to Principles and Standards for School Mathematics (pp. 179–192). Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of
Mathematics.
Lehrer, R., & Pritchard, C. (2002). Symbolizing space into being. In K. P. E. Gravemeijer, R. Lehrer, B. Van Oers, & L. Verschaffel
(Eds.), Symbolizing, modeling and tool use in mathematics education (pp. 59–86). Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
Lehrer, R., Harckham, L. D., Archer, P., & Pruzek, R. M. (1986). Microcomputer-based instruction in special education. Journal of
Educational Computing Research, 2, 337–355.
Lehrer, R., Jacobson, C., Thoyre, G., Kemeny, V., Strom, D., Horvarth, J., et al. (1998). Developing understanding of geometry and
space in the primary grades. In R. Lehrer & D. Chazan (Eds.), Designing learning environments for developing understanding
of geometry and space (pp. 169–200). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Lehrer, R., Jenkins, M., & Osana, H. (1998). Longitudinal study of children’s reasoning about space and geometry. In R. Lehrer
& D. Chazan (Eds.), Designing learning environments for developing understanding of geometry and space (pp. 137–167).
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Lehrer, R., Strom, D., & Confrey, J. (2002). Grounding metaphors and inscriptional resonance: Children’s emerging understand-
ings of mathematical similarity. Cognition and Instruction, 20(3), 359–398.
Lehtinen, E., & Hannula, M. M. (2006). Attentional processes, abstraction and transfer in early mathematical development. In L.
Verschaffel, F. Dochy, M. Boekaerts, & S. Vosniadou (Eds.), Instructional psychology: Past, present and future trends. Fifteen
essays in honour of Erik De Corte (Vol. 49, pp. 39–55). Amsterdam, the Netherlands: Elsevier.
Lembke, E. S., & Foegen, A. (2008). Identifying indicators of performance in early mathematics for kindergarten and grade
1 students. Submitted for publication.
Lembke, E. S., Foegen, A., Whittake, T. A., & Hampton, D. (2008). Establishing technically adequate measures of progress in early
numeracy. Assessment for Effective Intervention, 33(4), 206–210.
Leonard, J. (2008). Culturally specific pedagogy in the mathematics classroom: strategies for teachers and students. New York, NY:
Routledge.
Lepola, J., Niemi, P., Kuikka, M., & Hannula, M. M. (2005). Cognitive-linguistic skills and motivation as longitudinal predictors of
reading and arithmetic achievement: A follow-up study from kindergarten to grade 2. International Journal of Educational
Research, 43, 250–271.
Lerkkanen, M.-K., Kiuru, N., Pakarinen, E., Viljaranta, J., Poikkeus, A.-M., Rasku-Puttonen, H. et al. (2012). The role of teaching
practices in the development of children’s interest in reading and mathematics in kindergarten. Contemporary Educational
Psychology, 37(4), 266–279. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2011.03.004
Lerkkanen, M.-K., Rasku-Puttonen, H., Aunola, K., & Nurmi, J.-E. (2005). Mathematical performance predicts progress in
reading comprehension among 7-year-olds. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 20(2), 121–137.
Lerner, J. (1997). Learning disabilities. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin Company.
Lesh, R. A. (1990). Computer-based assessment of higher order understandings and processes in elementary mathematics. In G.
Kulm (Ed.), Assessing higher order thinking in mathematics (pp. 81–110). Washington, D.C.: American Association for the
Advancement of Science.
Lester, F. K., Jr., & Wiliam, D. (2002). On the purpose of mathematics education research: Making productive contributions to
policy and practice. In L. D. English (Ed.), Handbook of international research in mathematics education (pp. 489–506).
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Levesque, A. (2010). An investigation of the conditions under which procedural content enhances conceptual self-explanations in
mathematics. Master’s thesis, Concordia University. Available from ProQuest Dissertations and Theses database (UMI no.
MR67234). Retrieved from http://proquest.umi.com/pqdlink?did=2191474161&Fmt=7&clientId=39334&RQT=309&VN
ame=PQD
Levine, S. C., Gunderson, E., & Huttenlocher, J. (2011). Mathematical development during the preschool years in context: Home
and school input variations. In N. L. Stein & S. Raudenbush (Eds.), Developmental Science Goes to School: Implications for
Education and Public Policy Research. New York, NY: Taylor and Francis.
Levine, S. C., Huttenlocher, J., Taylor, A., & Langrock, A. (1999). Early sex differences in spatial skill. Developmental Psychology,
35(4), 940–949.
Levine, S. C., Jordan, N. C., & Huttenlocher, J. (1992). Development of calculation abilities in young children. Journal of
Experimental Child Psychology, 53, 72–103.
Levine, S. C., Ratliff, K. R., Huttenlocher, J., & Cannon, J. (2012). Early puzzle play: A predictor of preschoolers’ spatial transforma-
tion skill. Developmental Psychology, 48(2), 530–542. doi: 10.1037/a0025913
Levine, S. C., Suriyakham, L. W., Rowe, M. L., Huttenlocher, J., & Gunderson, E. A. (2010). What counts in the development of
young children’s number knowledge? Developmental Psychology, 46(5), 1309–1319. doi: 10.1037/a0019671
Li, Z., & Atkins, M. (2004). Early childhood computer experience and cognitive and motor development. Pediatrics, 113,
1715–1722.
352 • References

Liaw, F.-r., Meisels, S. J., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (1995). The effects of experience of early intervention on low birth weight, premature
children: The Infant Health and Development program. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 10, 405–431.
Liben, L. S. (2008). Understanding maps: Is the purple country on the map really purple? Knowledge Question, 36, 20–30.
Libertus, M. E., Feigenson, L., & Halberda, J. (2011a). Preschool acuity of the Approximate Number System correlates with math
abilities. Developmental Science, 14(6), 1292–1300. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.080100x
Libertus, M. E., Feigenson, L., & Halberda, J. (2011b). Effects of approximate number system training for numerical approximation
and school math abilities. Poster presented at NICHD Math Cognition Conference, Bethesda, MD.
Lieber, J., Horn, E., Palmer, S., & Fleming, K. (2008). Access to the general education curriculum for preschoolers with disabilities:
Children’s School Success. Exceptionality, 16(1), 18–32. doi: 10.1080/09362830701796776
Light, R. J., & Pillemer, D. B. (1984). Summing up: The science of reviewing research. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Lillard, A. S., & Else-Quest, N. (2007). Evaluating Montessori education. Science, 313, 1893–1894.
Linnell, M., & Fluck, M. (2001). The effect of maternal support for counting and cardinal understanding in pre-school children.
Social Development, 10, 202–220.
Lipinski, J. M., Nida, R. E., Shade, D. D., & Watson, J. A. (1986). The effects of microcomputers on young children: An examination
of free-play choices, sex differences, and social interactions. Journal of Educational Computing Research, 2, 147–168.
Loeb, S., Bridges, M., Bassok, D., Fuller, B., & Rumberger, R. (2007). How much is too much? The influence of preschool centers
on children’s development nationwide. Economics of Education Review, 26, 52–56.
Lonigan, C. J. (2003). Comment on Marcon (ECRP, Vol. 4, No. 1, Spring 2002): Moving up the grades: Relationship between
preschool model and later school success. Early Childhood Research & Practice, 5(1). Available at: http://ecrp.illinois.edu/
v5n1/lonigan.html
Lüken, M. M. (2012). Young children’s structure sense. Journal für Mathematik-Didaktik, 33(2), 263–285. doi: 10.1007/s13138-
012-0036-8
Lutchmaya, S., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2002). Human sex differences in social and non-social looking preferences, at 12 months of
age. Infant Behavior and Development, 25, 319–325.
Magnuson, K. A., & Waldfogel, J. (2005). Early childhood care and education: Effects on ethnic and racial gaps in school readiness.
The Future of Children, 15, 169–196.
Magnuson, K. A., Meyers, M. K., Rathbun, A., & West, J. (2004). Inequality in preschool education and school readiness. American
Educational Research Journal, 41, 115–157.
Maier, M. F., & Greenfield, D. B. (2008). The differential role of initiative and persistence in early childhood. Paper presented at the
Institute of Education Science 2007 Research Conference.
Malaguzzi, L. (1997). Shoe and meter. Reggio Emilia, Italy: Reggio Children.
Malmivuori, M.-L. (2001). The dynamics of affect, cognition, and social environment in the regulation of personal learning processes:
The case of mathematics. Helsinki, Finland: University of Helsinki.
Malofeeva, E., Day, J., Saco, X., Young, L., & Ciancio, D. (2004). Construction and evaluation of a number sense test with Head
Start children. Journal of Education Psychology, 96, 648–659.
Mandler, J. M. (2004). The foundations of mind: Origins of conceptual thought. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Marcon, R. A. (1992). Differential effects of three preschool models on inner-city 4-year-olds. Early Childhood Research Quarterly,
7, 517–530.
Marcon, R. A. (2002). Moving up the grades: Relationship between preschool model and later school success. Early Childhood
Research & Practice. Retrieved from http://ecrp.uiuc.edu/v4n1/marcon.html
Markovits, Z., & Hershkowitz, R. (1997). Relative and absolute thinking in visual estimation processes. Educational Studies in
Mathematics, 32, 29–47.
Martin, T., Lukong, A., & Reaves, R. (2007). The role of manipulatives in arithmetic and geometry tasks. Journal of Education and
Human Development, 1(1).
Mason, M. M. (1995). Geometric knowledge in a deaf classroom: An exploratory study. Focus on Learning Problems in Mathemat-
ics, 17(3), 57–69.
Mayfield, W. A., Morrison, J. W., Thornburg, K. R., & Scott, J. L. (2007). Project Construct: Child outcomes based on curriculum
fidelity. Paper presented at the Society for Research in Child Development.
Mazzocco, M. M. M., & Myers, G. F. (2003). Complexities in identifying and defining mathematics learning disability in the
primary school-age years. Annals of Dyslexia, 53, 218–253.
Mazzocco, M. M. M., & Thompson, R. E. (2005). Kindergarten predictors of math learning disability. Quarterly Research and
Practice, 20, 142–155.
Mazzocco, M. M. M., Feigenson, L., & Halberda, J. (2011). Preschoolers’ precision of the approximate number system predicts
later school mathematics performance. PLoS ONE, 6(9), e23749. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0023749.t001
McClain, K., Cobb, P., Gravemeijer, K. P. E., & Estes, B. (1999). Developing mathematical reasoning within the context of meas-
urement. In L. V. Stiff & F. R. Curcio (Eds.), Developing mathematical reasoning in grades K–12 (pp. 93–106). Reston, VA:
National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
McDermott, P. A., Fantuzzo, J. W., Warley, H. P., Waterman, C., Angelo, L. E., Gadsden, V. L. et al. (2010). Multidimensionality
of teachers’ graded responses for preschoolers’ stylistic learning behavior: The learning-to-learn scales. Educational and
Psychological Measurement, 71(1), 148–169. doi: 10.1177/0013164410387351
McFadden, K. E., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., & Cabrera, N. J. (2011). Quality matters: Low-income fathers’ engagement in learning
activities in early childhood predict children’s academic performance in fifth grade. Family Science, 2, 120–130.
McGee, M. G. (1979). Human spatial abilities: Psychometric studies and environmental, genetic, hormonal, and neurological
influences. Psychological Bulletin, 86, 889–918.
McGuinness, D., & Morley, C. (1991). Gender differences in the development of visuospatial ability in pre-school children. Journal
of Mental Imagery, 15, 143–150.
McKelvey, L. M., Bokony, P. A., Swindle, T. M., Conners-Burrow, N. A., Schiffman, R. F., & Fitzgerald, H. E. (2011). Father teaching
interactions with toddlers at risk: Associations with later child academic outcomes. Family Science, 2, 146–155.
McLeod, D. B., & Adams, V. M. (Eds.). (1989). Affect and mathematical problem solving. New York, NY: Springer-Verlag.
McNeil, N. M. (2008). Limitations to teaching children 2 + 2 = 4: Typical arithmetic problems can hinder learning of mathemati-
cal equivalence. Child Development, 79(5), 1524–1537.
References • 353

McTaggart, J., Frijters, J., & Barron, R. (2005, April). Children’s interest in reading and math: A longitudinal study of motivational
stability and influence on early academic skills. Paper presented at the Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child
Development, Atlanta, GA.
Methe, S., Kilgus, S., Neiman, C., & Chris Riley-Tillman, T. (2012). Meta-analysis of interventions for basic mathematics computa-
tion in single-case research. Journal of Behavioral Education, 21(3), 230–253. doi: 10.1007/s10864-012-9161-1
Metz, K. E. (1995). Reassessment of developmental constraints on children’s science instruction. Review of Educational Research,
65, 93–127.
Middleton, J. A., & Spanias, P. (1999). Motivation for achievement in mathematics: Findings, generalizations, and criticisms of the
research. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 30, 65–88.
Milesi, C., & Gamoran, A. (2006). Effects of class size and instruction on kindergarten achievement. Education Evaluation and
Policy Analysis, 28(4), 287–313.
Millar, S., & Ittyerah, M. (1992). Movement imagery in young and congenitally blind children: Mental practice without visuospa-
tial information. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 15, 125–146
Miller, K. F. (1984). Child as the measurer of all things: Measurement procedures and the development of quantitative concepts. In
C. Sophian (Ed.), Origins of cognitive skills: The eighteenth annual Carnegie symposium on cognition (pp. 193–228). Hillsdale,
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Miller, K. F. (1989). Measurement as a tool of thought: The role of measuring procedures in children’s understanding of quantita-
tive invariance. Developmental Psychology, 25, 589–600.
Miller, K. F., Kelly, M., & Zhou, X. (2005). Learning mathematics in China and the United States: Cross-cultural insights into the
nature and course of preschool mathematical development. In J. I. D. Campbell (Ed.), Handbook of mathematical cognition
(pp. 163–178). New York, NY: Psychology Press.
Miller, K. F., Smith, C. M., Zhu, J., & Zhang, H. (1995). Preschool origins of cross-national differences in mathematical
competence: The role of number-naming systems. Psychological Science, 6, 56–60.
Mitchelmore, M. C. (1989). The development of children’s concepts of angle. In G. Vergnaud, J. Rogalski, & M. Artique (Eds.),
Proceedings of the 13th Annual Conference of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education (Vol. 2).
Paris, France: City University.
Mitchelmore, M. C. (1992). Children’s concepts of perpendiculars. In W. Geeslin & K. Graham (Eds.), Proceedings of the 16th An-
nual Conference of the International Group for the Psychology in Mathematics Education (Vol. 2, pp. 120–127). Durham, NH:
Program Committee of the 16th PME Conference.
Moeller, K., Fischer, U., Cress, U., & Nuerk, H.-C. (2012). Diagnostics and intervention in developmental dyscalculia: Current
issues and novel perspectives. In Z. Breznitz, O. Rubinsten, V. J. Molfese, & D. L. Molfese (Eds.), Reading, writing,
mathematics and the developing brain: Listening to many voices (Vol. 6, pp. 233–275). The Netherlands: Springer.
Molfese, V. J., Brown, T. E., Adelson, J. L., Beswick, J., Jacobi-Vessels, J., Thomas, L. et al. (2012). Examining associations between
classroom environment and processes and early mathematics performance from pre-kindergarten to kindergarten. Gifted
Children, 5(2), article 2. Retrieved from http://docs.lib.purdue.edu/giftedchildren/vol5/iss2/2
Moll, L. C., Amanti, C., Neff, D., & Gonzalez, N. (1992). Funds of knowledge for teaching: Using a qualitative approach to connect
homes and classrooms. Theory into Practice, 31, 132–141.
Monighan-Nourot, P., Scales, B., Van Hoorn, J., & Almy, M. (1987). Looking at children’s play: A bridge between theory and practice.
New York, NY: Teachers College.
Montessori, M. (1964). The Montessori method. New York, NY: Schocken Books.
Montie, J. E., Xiang, Z., & Schweinhart, L. J. (2006). Preschool experience in 10 countries: Cognitive and language performance at
age 7. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 21, 313–331.
Mooij, T., & Driessen, G. (2008). Differential ability and attainment in language and arithmetic of Dutch primary school pupils.
British Journal of Educational Psychology, 78(Pt 3), 491–506. doi: 10.1348/000709907X235981
Morgenlander, M. (2005). Preschoolers’ understanding of mathematics presented on Sesame Street. Paper presented at the American
Educational Research Association, New Orleans, LA.
Morrongiello, B. A., Timney, B., Humphrey, G. K., Anderson, S., & Skory, C. (1995). Spatial knowledge in blind and sighted
children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 59, 211–233.
Moseley, B. (2005). Pre-service early childhood educators’ perceptions of math-mediated language. Early Education & Develop-
ment, 16(3), 385–396.
Moyer, P. S. (2000). Are we having fun yet? Using manipulatives to teach “real math”. Educational Studies in Mathematics: An
International Journal, 47(2), 175–197.
Moyer, P. S., Niezgoda, D., & Stanley, J. (2005). Young children’s use of virtual manipulatives and other forms of mathematical rep-
resentations. In W. Masalski & P. C. Elliott (Eds.), Technology-supported mathematics learning environments: 67th Yearbook
(pp. 17–34). Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
Mullet, E., & Miroux, R. (1996). Judgment of rectangular areas in children blind from birth. Cognitive Development, 11, 123–139.
Mulligan, J., English, L. D., Mitchelmore, M. C., Welsby, S., & Crevensten, N. (2011a). An evaluation of the pattern and structure
mathematics awareness program in the early school years. In J. Clark, B. Kissane, J. Mousley, T. Spencer & S. Thornton (Eds.),
Proceedings of the AAMT-MERGA Conference 2011, The Australian Association of Mathematics Teachers Inc. & Mathematics
Education Research Group of Australasia (pp. 548–556). Alice Springs, Australia.
Mulligan, J. T., English, L. D., Mitchelmore, M. C., Welsby, S. M., & Crevensten, N. (2011b). Developing the Pattern and Structure
Assessment (PASA) interview to inform early mathematics learning. Paper presented at the AAMT-MERGA Conference
2011, Alice Springs, Australia.
Mulligan, J., Mitchelmore, M., English, L. D., & Crevensten, N. (2012). Evaluation of the “reconceptualising early mathematics learn-
ing” project. Paper presented at the AARE APERA International Conference, Sydney.
Mulligan, J., Prescott, A., Mitchelmore, M. C., & Outhred, L. (2005). Taking a closer look at young students’ images of area mea-
surement. Australian Primary Mathematics Classroom, 10(2), 4–8.
Mullis, I. V. S., Martin, M. O., Foy, P., & Arora, A. (2012). TIMSS 2011 International Results in Mathematics. Chestnut Hill, MA:
TIMSS & PIRLS International Study Center, Lynch School of Education, Boston College.
Mullis, I. V. S., Martin, M. O., Gonzalez, E. J., Gregory, K. D., Garden, R. A., O’Connor, K. M. et al. (2000). TIMSS 1999 international
mathematics report. Boston: The International Study Center, Boston College, Lynch School of Education.
354 • References

Munn, P. (1998). Symbolic function in pre-schoolers. In C. Donlan (Ed.), The development of mathematical skills (pp. 47–71). East
Sussex, England: Psychology Press.
Murata, A. (2004). Paths to learning ten-structured understanding of teen sums: Addition solution methods of Japanese Grade 1
students. Cognition and Instruction, 22, 185–218.
Murata, A. (2008). Mathematics teaching and learning as a mediating process: The case of tape diagrams. Mathematical Thinking
and Learning, 10, 374–406.
Nasir, N. i. S., & Cobb, P. (2007). Improving access to mathematics: Diversity and equity in the classroom. New York, NY: Teachers
College Press.
Nastasi, B. K., & Clements, D. H. (1991). Research on cooperative learning: Implications for practice. School Psychology Review,
20, 110–131.
Nastasi, B. K., Clements, D. H., & Battista, M. T. (1990). Social-cognitive interactions, motivation, and cognitive growth in Logo
programming and CAI problem-solving environments. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 150–158.
Natriello, G., McDill, E. L., & Pallas, A. M. (1990). Schooling disadvantaged children: Racing against catastrophe. New York, NY:
Teachers College Press.
Navarro, J. I., Aguilar, M., Marchena, E., Ruiz, G., Menacho, I., & Van Luit, J. E. H. (2012). Longitudinal study of low and
high achievers in early mathematics. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 82(1), 28–41. doi: 10.1111/j.
2044-8279.2011.02043.x
NCES. (2000). America’s kindergartners (NCES 2000070). Washington, D.C.: National Center for Education Statistics, U.S.
Government Printing Office.
NCTM. (2000). Principles and standards for school mathematics. Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
NCTM. (2006). Curriculum focal points for prekindergarten through grade 8 mathematics: A quest for coherence. Reston, VA:
National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
Nes, F. T. v. (2009). Young children’s spatial structuring ability and emerging number sense. Doctoral dissertation, de Universtiteit
Utrecht, Utrecht, the Netherlands.
Neuenschwander, R., Röthlisberger, M., Cimeli, P., & Roebers, C. M. (2012). How do different aspects of self-regulation predict
successful adaptation to school? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 113(3), 353–371. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.
jecp.2012.07.004
Neville, H., Andersson, A., Bagdade, O., Bell, T., Currin, J., Fanning, J. et al. (2008). Effects of music training on brain and cogni-
tive development in under-privileged 3- to 5-year-old children: Preliminary results. In C. Asbury & B. Rich (Eds.), Learning,
Arts, & the Brain. New York/Washington D.C.: Dana Press.
Newcombe, N. S., & Huttenlocher, J. (2000). Making space: The development of spatial representation and reasoning. Cambridge,
MA: MIT Press.
Ng, S. N. S., & Rao, N. (2010). Chinese number words, culture, and mathematics learning. Review of Educational Research, 80(2),
180–206.
Niemiec, R. P., & Walberg, H. J. (1984). Computers and achievement in the elementary schools. Journal of Educational Computing
Research, 1, 435–440.
Niemiec, R. P., & Walberg, H. J. (1987). Comparative effects of computer-assisted instruction: A synthesis of reviews. Journal of
Educational Computing Research, 3, 19–37.
Nishida, T. K., & Lillard, A. S. (2007a, April). From flashcard to worksheet: Children’s inability to transfer across different formats.
Paper presented at the Society for Research in Child Development, Boston, MA.
Nishida, T. K., & Lillard, A. S. (2007b, April). Fun toy or learning tool?: Young children’s use of concrete manipulatives to learn about
simple math concepts. Paper presented at the Society for Research in Child Development, Boston, MA.
NMP. (2008). Foundations for success: The final report of the National Mathematics Advisory Panel. Washington D.C.: U.S.
Department of Education, Office of Planning, Evaluation and Policy Development.
Nomi, T. (2010). The effects of within-class ability grouping on academic achievement in early elementary years. Journal of
Research on Educational Effectiveness, 3, 56–92.
Northcote, M. (2011). Step back and hand over the cameras! Using digital cameras to facilitate mathematics learning with young
children in K–2 classrooms. Australian Primary Mathematics Classroom, 16(3), 29–32.
NRC. (2004). On evaluating curricular effectiveness: Judging the quality of K–12 mathematics evaluations. Washington, D.C.:
Mathematical Sciences Education Board, Center for Education, Division of Behavioral and Social Sciences and Education,
National Academies Press.
NRC. (2009). Mathematics in early childhood: Learning paths toward excellence and equity. Washington, D.C.: National Academy
Press.
Nührenbörger, M. (2001). Insights into children’s ruler concepts—Grade-2 students’ conceptions and knowledge of length
measurement and paths of development. In M. v. d. Heuvel-Panhuizen (Ed.), Proceedings of the 25th Conference of the
International Group for the Psychology in Mathematics Education (Vol. 3, pp. 447–454). Utrecht, the Netherlands: Freuden-
thal Institute.
Nunes, T., & Moreno, C. (1998). Is hearing impairment a cause of difficulties in learning mathematics? In C. Donlan (Ed.), The
development of mathematical skills (Vol. 7, pp. 227–254). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.
Nunes, T., & Moreno, C. (2002). An intervention program for promoting deaf pupils’ achievement in mathematics. Journal of Deaf
Studies and Deaf Education, 7(2), 120–133.
Nunes, T., Bryant, P., Evans, D., & Bell, D. (2010). The scheme of correspondence and its role in children’s mathematics. British
Journal of Educational Psychology, 2(7), 83–99. doi: 10.1348/97818543370009x12583699332537
Nunes, T., Bryant, P., Evans, D., Bell, D., & Barros, R. (2011). Teaching children how to include the inversion principle in their
reasoning about quantitative relations. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 79(3), 371–388. doi: 10.1007/s10649-011-9314-5
Nunes, T., Bryant, P., Evans, D., Bell, D., Gardner, S., Gardner, A. et al. (2007). The contribution of logical reasoning to the learning of
mathematics in primary school. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 25(1), 147–166. doi: 10.1348/026151006x153127
Nunes, T., Bryant, P. E., Barros, R., & Sylva, K. (2012). The relative importance of two different mathematical abilities to math-
ematical achievement. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 82(1), 136–156. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8279.2011.02033.x
Nunes, T., Bryant, P. E., Burman, D., Bell, D., Evans, D., & Hallett, D. (2009). Deaf children’s informal knowledge of multiplicative
reasoning. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 14(2), 260–277.
References • 355

Núñez, R., Cooperrider, K., Doan, D., & Wassmann, J. (2012). Contours of time: Topographic construals of past, present, and future
in the Yupno valley of Papua New Guinea. Cognition, 124(1), 25–35. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2012.03.007
Núñez, R., Doan, D., & Nikoulina, A. (2011). Squeezing, striking, and vocalizing: Is number representation fundamentally spatial?
Cognition, 120(2), 225–235. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2011.05.001
Núñez, R., Cooperrider, K., & Wassmann, J. (2012). Number concepts without number lines in an indigenous group of Papua
New Guinea. PLoS ONE, 7(4), 1–8. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0035662
Núñez, R. E. (2011). No innate number line in the human brain. Journal of Cross-cultural Psychology, 42(4), 651–668. doi:
10.1177/0022022111406097
O’Neill, D. K., Pearce, M. J., & Pick, J. L. (2004). Preschool children’s narratives and performance on the Peabody Individualized
Achievement Test Revised: Evidence of a relation between early narrative and later mathematical ability. First Language,
24(2), 149–183.
Oakes, J. (1990). Opportunities, achievement, and choice: Women and minority students in science and mathematics. In C. B.
Cazden (Ed.), Review of research in education (Vol. 16, pp. 153–222). Washington, D.C.: American Educational Research
Association.
Obersteiner, A., Reiss, K., & Ufer, S. (2013). How training on exact or approximate mental representations of number can enhance
first-grade students’ basic number processing and arithmetic skills. Learning and Instruction, 23(1), 125–135. doi: http://
dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2012.08.004
Olive, J., Lankenau, C. A., & Scally, S. P. (1986). Teaching and understanding geometric relationships through Logo: Phase II. Interim
Report: The Atlanta–Emory Logo Project. Atlanta, GA: Emory University.
Olson, J. K. (1988). Microcomputers make manipulatives meaningful. Budapest, Hungary: International Congress of Mathematics
Education.
Ostad, S. A. (1998). Subtraction strategies in developmental perspective: A comparison of mathematically normal and mathemat-
ically disabled children. In A. Olivier & K. Newstead (Eds.), Proceedings of the 22nd Conference for the International Group
for the Psychology of Mathematics Education (Vol. 3, pp. 311–318). Stellenbosch, South Africa: University of Stellenbosch.
Outhred, L. N., & Sardelich, S. (1997). Problem solving in kindergarten: The development of representations. In F. Biddulph &
K. Carr (Eds.), People in Mathematics Education. Proceedings of the 20th Annual Conference of the Mathematics Education
Research Group of Australasia (Vol. 2, pp. 376–383). Rotorua, New Zealand: Mathematics Education Research Group of
Australasia.
Owens, K. (1992). Spatial thinking takes shape through primary-school experiences. In W. Geeslin & K. Graham (Eds.), Proceed-
ings of the 16th Conference of the International Group for the Psychology in Mathematics Education (Vol. 2, pp. 202–209).
Durham, NH: Program Committee of the 16th PME Conference.
Pagani, L., & Messier, S. (2012). Links between motor skills and indicators of school readiness at kindergarten entry in urban
disadvantaged children. Journal of Educational and Developmental Psychology, 2(1), 95. doi: 10.5539/jedp.v2n1p95
Pagani, L. S., Fitzpatrick, C., & Parent, S. (2012). Relating kindergarten attention to subsequent developmental pathways of
classroom engagement in elementary school. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 40(5), 715–725. doi: 10.1007/s10802-
011-9605-4
Pagliaro, C. M., & Kritzer, K. L. (2013). The math gap: A description of the mathematics performance of preschool-aged deaf/
hard-of-hearing children. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 18(2), 139–160. doi: 10.1093/deafed/ens070
Pakarinen, E., Kiuru, N., Lerkkanen, M.-K., Poikkeus, A.-M., Ahonen, T., & Nurmi, J.-E. (2010). Instructional support predicts
children’s task avoidance in kindergarten. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 26(3), 376–386. doi: 10.1016/j.
ecresq.2010.11.003
Palardy, G., & Rumberger, R. (2008). Teacher effectiveness in first grade: The importance of background qualifications, attitudes,
and instructional practices for student learning. Educational Evaluation and Policy Analysis, 30, 111–140.
Pan, Y., & Gauvain, M. (2007). Parental involvement in children’s mathematics learning in American and Chinese families during two
school transitions. Paper presented at the Society for Research in Child Development.
Pan, Y., Gauvain, M., Liu, Z., & Cheng, L. (2006). American and Chinese parental involvement in young children’s mathematics
learning. Cognitive Development, 21, 17–35.
Papert, S. (1980). Mindstorms: Children, computers, and powerful ideas. New York, NY: Basic Books.
Papic, M. M., Mulligan, J. T., & Mitchelmore, M. C. (2011). Assessing the development of preschoolers’ mathematical patterning.
Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 42(3), 237–269. doi: 10.5951/jresematheduc.42.3.0237
Paris, C. L., & Morris, S. K. (1985). The computer in the early childhood classroom: Peer helping and peer teaching. Cleege Park, MD:
MicroWorld for Young Children Conference.
Parker, T. H., & Baldridge, S. J. (2004). Elementary mathematics for teachers. Quebecor World, MI: Sefton-Ash Publishing.
Pasnak, R. (1987). Accelerated cognitive development of kindergartners. Psychology in the Schools, 28, 358–363.
Pasnak, R., Kidd, J. K., Gadzichowski, M., Gallington, D. A., McKnight, P., Boyer, C. E. et al. (2012). An efficacy test of patterning
instruction for first grade. Fairfax, VA: George Mason University.
Passolunghi, M. C., & Lanfranchi, S. (2012). Domain-specific and domain-general precursors of mathematical achieve-
ment: A longitudinal study from kindergarten to first grade. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 82(1), 42–63. doi:
10.1111/j.2044-8279.2011.02039.x
Passolunghi, M. C., Vercelloni, B., & Schadee, H. (2007). The precursors of mathematics learning: Working memory, phonological
ability and numerical competence. Cognitive Development, 22(2), 165–184. doi: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2006.09.001
Peisner-Feinberg, E. S., Burchinal, M. R., Clifford, R. M., Culkins, M. L., Howes, C., Kagan, S. L., et al. (2001). The relation
of preschool child-care quality to children’s cognitive and social developmental trajectories through second grade. Child
Development, 72, 1534–1553.
Perlmutter, J., Bloom, L., Rose, T., & Rogers, A. (1997). Who uses math? Primary children’s perceptions of the uses of mathematics.
Journal of Research in Childhood Education, 12(1), 58–70.
Perry, B., & Dockett, S. (2002). Young children’s access to powerful mathematical ideas. In L. D. English (Ed.), Handbook of Inter-
national Research in Mathematics Education (pp. 81–111). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Perry, B., & Dockett, S. (2005). “I know that you don’t have to work hard”: Mathematics learning in the first year of primary
school. In H. L. Chick & J. L. Vincent (Eds.), Proceedings of the 29th Conference of the International Group for the Psychology
in Mathematics Education (Vol. 4, pp. 65–72). Melbourne, Australia: PME.
356 • References

Perry, B., Young-Loveridge, J. M., Dockett, S., & Doig, B. (2008). The development of young children’s mathematical understand-
ing. In H. Forgasz, A. Barkatsas, A. Bishop, B. A. Clarke, S. Keast, W. T. Seah et al. (Eds.), Research in mathematics education
in Australasia 2004–2007 (pp. 17–40). Rotterdam, the Netherlands: Sense Publishers.
Phillips, D., & Meloy, M. (2012). High-quality school-based pre-K can boost early learning for children with special needs.
Exceptional Children, 78(4), 471–490.
Piaget, J. (1962). Play, dreams and imitation in childhood. New York, NY: W. W. Norton.
Piaget, J. (1971/1974). Understanding causality. New York, NY: W. W. Norton.
Piaget, J., & Inhelder, B. (1967). The child’s conception of space. New York, NY: W. W. Norton.
Pianta, R. C., Howes, C., Burchinal, M. R., Bryant, D., Clifford, R., Early, D., et al. (2005). Features of pre-kindergarten programs,
classrooms, and teachers: Do they predict observed classroom quality and child–teacher interactions? Applied Developmental
Science, 9, 144–159.
Pollio, H. R., & Whitacre, J. D. (1970). Some observations on the use of natural numbers by preschool children. Perceptual and
Motor Skills, 30, 167–174.
Ponitz, C. C., McClelland, M. M., Matthews, J. S., & Morrison, F. J. (2009). A structured observation of behavioral self-regulation
and its contribution to kindergarten outcomes. Developmental Psychology, 45(3), 605–619.
Porter, J. (1999). Learning to count: A difficult task? Down Syndrome Research and Practice, 6(2), 85–94.
Powell, S. R., & Fuchs, L. S. (2010). Contribution of equal-sign instruction beyond word-problem tutoring for third-grade
students with mathematics difficulty. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102(2), 381–394.
Powell, S. R., Fuchs, L. S., & Fuchs, D. (2013). Reaching the mountaintop: Addressing the common core standards in
mathematics for students with mathematical disabilities. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 28(1), 38–48.
doi: 10.1111/ldrp.12001
Pratt, C. (1948). I learn from children. New York, NY: Simon and Schuster.
Primavera, J., Wiederlight, P. P., & DiGiacomo, T. M. (2001, August). Technology access for low-income preschoolers: Bridging the
digital divide. Paper presented at the American Psychological Association, San Francisco, CA.
Pruden, S. M., Levine, S. C., & Huttenlocher, J. (2011). Children’s spatial thinking: Does talk about the spatial world matter? De-
velopmental Science, 14(6), 1417–1430. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01088.x
Purpura, D. J., Hume, L. E., Sims, D. M., & Lonigan, C. J. (2011). Early literacy and early numeracy: The value of including early
literacy skills in the prediction of numeracy development. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 110, 647–658. doi:
10.1016/j.jecp.2011.07.004
Ragosta, M., Holland, P., & Jamison, D. T. (1981). Computer-assisted instruction and compensatory education: The ETS/LAUSD
study. Princeton, NJ: Educational Testing Service.
Ramani, G. B., Siegler, R. S., & Hitti, A. (2012). Taking it to the classroom: Number board games as a small group learning activity.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 104(3), 661–672. doi: 10.1037/a0028995.supp
Ramey, C. T., & Ramey, S. L. (1998). Early intervention and early experience. American Psychologist, 53, 109–120.
Raphael, D., & Wahlstrom, M. (1989). The influence of instructional aids on mathematics achievement. Journal for Research in
Mathematics Education, 20, 173–190.
Rathbun, A., & West, J. (2004). From kindergarten through third grade: Children’s beginning school experiences. Washington, D.C.:
U.S. Department of Education, National Center for Education Statistics.
Raver, C. C., Jones, S. M., Li-Grining, C., Zhai, F., Bub, K., & Pressler, E. (2011). CSRP’s impact on low-income preschoolers’
preacademic skills: Self-regulation as a mediating mechanism. Child Development, 82(1), 362–378. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-
8624.2010.01561.x
Raver, C. C., Jones, S. M., Li-Grining, C., Zhai, F., Metzger, M. W., & Solomon, B. (2009). Targeting children’s behavior problems in
preschool classrooms: A cluster-randomized controlled trial. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 77(2), 302–316.
doi: 10.1037/a0015302
Razel, M., & Eylon, B.-S. (1986). Developing visual language skills: The Agam program. Journal of Visual Verbal Languaging, 6(1),
49–54.
Razel, M., & Eylon, B.-S. (1990). Development of visual cognition: Transfer effects of the Agam program. Journal of Applied
Developmental Psychology, 11, 459–485.
Razel, M., & Eylon, B.-S. (1991, July). Developing mathematics readiness in young children with the Agam program. Genoa, Italy.
Reardon, S. F. (2011). The widening academic achievement gap between the rich and the poor: New evidence and possible explan-
ations. In G. J. Duncan & R. Murnane (Eds.), Whither Opportunity? Rising Inequality, Schools, and Children’s Life Chances
(pp. 91–116). New York, NY: Russell Sage Foundation.
Reikerås, E., Løge, I. K., & Knivsberg, A.-M. (2012). The mathematical competencies of toddlers expressed in their play and daily
life activities in Norwegian kindergartens. International Journal of Early Childhood, 44(1) 91–114. doi: 10.1007/s13158-011-
0050-x
Resnick, L. B., & Omanson, S. (1987). Learning to understand arithmetic. In R. Glaser (Ed.), Advances in instructional psychology
(pp. 41–95). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Rhee, M. C., & Chavnagri, N. (Cartographer). (1991). Four-year-old children’s peer interactions when playing with a computer. ERIC
Document No. ED342466.
Richardson, K. (2004). Making sense. In D. H. Clements, J. Sarama, & A.-M. DiBiase (Eds.), Engaging young children in mathemat-
ics: Standards for early childhood mathematics education (pp. 321–324). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Riel, M. (1985). The Computer Chronicles Newswire: A functional learning environment for acquiring literacy skills. Journal of
Educational Computing Research, 1, 317–337.
Ritter, S., Anderson, J. R., Koedinger, K. R., & Corbett, A. (2007). Cognitive Tutor: Applied research in mathematics education.
Psychonomics Bulletin & Review, 14(2), 249–255.
Robinson, G. E. (1990). Synthesis of research on effects of class size. Educational Leadership, 47(7), 80–90.
Robinson, N. M., Abbot, R. D., Berninger, V. W., & Busse, J. (1996). The structure of abilities in math-precocious young children:
Gender similarities and differences. Journal of Educational Psychology, 88(2), 341–352.
Rogers, A. (2012). Steps in developing a quality whole number place value assessment for years 3-6: Unmasking the “experts.” Paper
presented at the Mathematics Education Research Group of Australasia, Singapore.
References • 357

Romano, E., Babchishin, L., Pagani, L. S., & Kohen, D. (2010). School readiness and later achievement: Replication and extension
using a nationwide Canadian survey. Developmental Psychology, 46(5), 995–1007. doi: 10.1037/a0018880
Rosengren, K. S., Gross, D., Abrams, A. F., & Perlmutter, M. (1985). An observational study of preschool children’s computing
activity. Austin, TX: “Perspectives on the Young Child and the Computer” conference, University of Texas at Austin.
Rosser, R. A. (1994). The developmental course of spatial cognition: Evidence for domain multidimensionality. Child Study
Journal, 24, 255–280.
Rosser, R. A., Ensing, S. S., Glider, P. J., & Lane, S. (1984). An information-processing analysis of children’s accuracy in predicting
the appearance of rotated stimuli. Child Development, 55, 2204–2211.
Rosser, R. A., Horan, P. F., Mattson, S. L., & Mazzeo, J. (1984). Comprehension of Euclidean space in young children: The early
emergence of understanding and its limits. Genetic Psychology Monographs, 110, 21–41.
Roth, J., Carter, R., Ariet, M., Resnick, M. B., & Crans, G. (2000, April). Comparing fourth-grade math and reading achievement of
children who did and did not participate in Florida’s statewide Prekindergarten Early Intervention Program. Paper presented at
the American Educational Research Association, New Orleans, LA.
Rourke, B. P., & Finlayson, M. A. J. (1978). Neuropsychological significance of variations in patterns of academic performance:
Verbal and visual-spatial abilities. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 6, 121–133.
Rouse, C., Brooks-Gunn, J., & McLanahan, S. (2005). Introducing the issue. The Future of Children, 15, 5–14.
Rousselle, L., & Noël, M.-P. (2007). Basic numerical skills in children with mathematics learning disabilities: A comparison of
symbolic vs. non-symbolic number magnitude processing. Cognition, 102, 361–395.
Russell, K. A., & Kamii, C. (2012). Children’s judgements of durations: A modified republication of Piaget’s study. School Science
and Mathematics, 112(8), 476–482.
Russell, S. J. (1991). Counting noses and scary things: Children construct their ideas about data. In D. Vere-Jones (Ed.),
Proceedings of the Third International Conference on Teaching Statistics. Voorburg, the Netherlands: International Statistical
Institute.
Sandhofer, C. M., & Smith, L. B. (1999). Learning color words involves learning a system of mappings. Developmental Psychology,
35, 668–679.
Sarama, J. (1995). Redesigning Logo: The turtle metaphor in mathematics education. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, State
University of New York at Buffalo.
Sarama, J. (2002). Listening to teachers: Planning for professional development. Teaching Children Mathematics, 9, 36–39.
Sarama, J. (2004). Technology in early childhood mathematics: “Building Blocks” as an innovative technology-based curriculum.
In D. H. Clements, J. Sarama, & A.-M. DiBiase (Eds.), Engaging young children in mathematics: Standards for early childhood
mathematics education (pp. 361–375). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Sarama, J., & Clements, D. H. (2002a). Building Blocks for young children’s mathematical development. Journal of Educational
Computing Research, 27(1&2), 93–110.
Sarama, J., & Clements, D. H. (2002b). Learning and teaching with computers in early childhood education. In O. N. Saracho
& B. Spodek (Eds.), Contemporary Perspectives on Science and Technology in Early Childhood Education (pp. 171–219).
Greenwich, CT: Information Age Publishing, Inc.
Sarama, J., & Clements, D. H. (2009). Early childhood mathematics education research: Learning trajectories for young children. New
York, NY: Routledge.
Sarama, J., & Clements, D. H. (2011). Mathematics knowledge of low-income entering preschoolers. Far East Journal of
Mathematical Education 6(1), 41–63.
Sarama, J., & Clements, D. H. (2012). Mathematics for the whole child. In S. Suggate & E. Reese (Eds.), Contemporary debates in
childhood education and development (pp. 71–80). New York, NY: Routledge.
Sarama, J., & DiBiase, A.-M. (2004). The professional development challenge in preschool mathematics. In D. H. Clements, J.
Sarama, & A.-M. DiBiase (Eds.), Engaging young children in mathematics: Standards for early childhood mathematics educa-
tion (pp. 415–446). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Sarama, J., Clements, D. H., Swaminathan, S., McMillen, S., & González Gómez, R. M. (2003). Development of
mathematical concepts of two-dimensional space in grid environments: An exploratory study. Cognition and Instruction,
21, 285–324.
Sarama, J., Clements, D. H., & Vukelic, E. B. (1996). The role of a computer manipulative in fostering specific psychological/
mathematical processes. In E. Jakubowski, D. Watkins, & H. Biske (Eds.), Proceedings of the 18th Annual Meeting of the North
America Chapter of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education (Vol. 2, pp. 567–572). Columbus,
OH: ERIC Clearinghouse for Science, Mathematics, and Environmental Education.
Sarama, J., Clements, D. H., Wolfe, C. B., & Spitler, M. E. (2012). Longitudinal evaluation of a scale-up model for teaching
mathematics with trajectories and technologies. Journal of Research on Educational Effectiveness, 5(2), 105–135.
Sarama, J., Lange, A., Clements, D. H., & Wolfe, C. B. (2012). The impacts of an early mathematics curriculum on emerging
literacy and language. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 27, 489–502. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2011.12.002
Saxe, G. B., Guberman, S. R., & Gearhart, M. (1987). Social processes in early number development. Monographs of the Society for
Research in Child Development, 52(2, Serial #216).
Schery, T. K., & O’Connor, L. C. (1997). Language intervention: Computer training for young children with special needs. British
Journal of Educational Technology, 28, 271–279.
Schliemann, A. c. D., Carraher, D. W., & Brizuela, B. M. (2007). Bringing out the algebraic character of arithmetic. Mahwah, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Schoenfeld, A. H. (2008). Early algebra as mathematical sense making. In J. J. Kaput, D. W. Carraher, & M. L. Blanton (Eds.),
Algebra in the early grades (pp. 479–510). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Schumacher, R. F., & Fuchs, L. S. (2012). Does understanding relational terminology mediate effects of intervention on compare
word problems? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 111(4), 607–628. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2011.12.001
Schwartz, S. (2004). Explorations in graphing with prekindergarten children. In B. Clarke (Ed.), International perspectives on
learning and teaching mathematics (pp. 83–97). Gothenburg, Sweden: National Centre for Mathematics Education.
Schweinhart, L. J., & Weikart, D. P. (1988). Education for young children living in poverty: Child-initiated learning or teacher-
directed instruction? The Elementary School Journal, 89, 212–225.
358 • References

Schweinhart, L. J., & Weikart, D. P. (1997). The High/Scope curriculum comparison study through age 23. Early Childhood
Research Quarterly, 12, 117–143.
Scott, L. F., & Neufeld, H. (1976). Concrete instruction in elementary school mathematics: Pictorial vs. manipulative. School
Science and Mathematics, 76, 68–72.
Secada, W. G. (1992). Race, ethnicity, social class, language, and achievement in mathematics. In D. A. Grouws (Ed.), Handbook of
research on mathematics teaching and learning (pp. 623–660). New York, NY: Macmillan.
Senk, S. L., & Thompson, D. R. (2003). Standards-based school mathematics curricula. What are they? What do students learn?
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Seo, K.-H., & Ginsburg, H. P. (2004). What is developmentally appropriate in early childhood mathematics education? In
D. H. Clements, J. Sarama, & A.-M. DiBiase (Eds.), Engaging young children in mathematics: Standards for early childhood
mathematics education (pp. 91–104). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Shade, D. D. (1994). Computers and young children: Software types, social contexts, gender, age, and emotional responses. Journal
of Computing in Childhood Education, 5(2), 177–209.
Shade, D. D., Nida, R. E., Lipinski, J. M., & Watson, J. A. (1986). Microcomputers and preschoolers: Working together in a
classroom setting. Computers in the Schools, 3, 53–61.
Shamir, A., & Lifshitz, I. (2012). E-books for supporting the emergent literacy and emergent math of children at risk for learning
disabilities: can metacognitive guidance make a difference? European Journal of Special Needs Education, 28(1), 33–48. doi:
10.1080/08856257.2012.742746
Shaw, K., Nelsen, E., & Shen, Y.-L. (2001, April). Preschool development and subsequent school achievement among Spanish-speaking
children from low-income families. Paper presented at the American Educational Research Association, Seattle, WA.
Shaw, R., Grayson, A., & Lewis, V. (2005). Inhibition, ADHD, and computer games: The inhibitory performance of children with
ADHD on computerized tasks and games. Journal of Attention Disorders, 8, 160–168.
Shayer, M., & Adhami, M. (2010). Realizing the cognitive potential of children 5–7 with a mathematics focus: Post-test and long-
term effects of a 2-year intervention. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 80(3), 363–379.
Shepard, L. (2005). Assessment. In L. Darling-Hammond & J. Bransford (Eds.), Preparing teachers for a changing world
(pp. 275–326). San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.
Sherman, J., Bisanz, J., & Popescu, A. (2007, April). Tracking the path of change: Failure to success on equivalence problems. Paper
presented at the Society for Research in Child Development, Boston, MA.
Shiffrin, R. M., & Schneider, W. (1984). Controlled and automatic human information processing: II. Perceptual learning,
automatic attending, and a general theory. Psychological Review, 84, 127–190.
Shonkoff, J. P., & Phillips, D. A. (Eds.). (2000). From neurons to neighborhoods: The science of early childhood development.
Washington, D.C.: National Academy Press.
Shrock, S. A., Matthias, M., Anastasoff, J., Vensel, C., & Shaw, S. (1985). Examining the effects of the microcomputer on a real world
class: A naturalistic study. Anaheim, CA: Association for Educational Communications and Technology.
Sicilian, S. P. (1988). Development of counting strategies in congenitally blind children. Journal of Visual Impairment & Blindness,
331–335.
Siegler, R. S. (1993). Adaptive and non-adaptive characteristics of low income children’s strategy use. In L. A. Penner, G. M.
Batsche, H. M. Knoff, & D. L. Nelson (Eds.), Contributions of psychology to science and mathematics education (pp. 341–366).
Washington, D.C.: American Psychological Association.
Siegler, R. S. (1995). How does change occur: A microgenetic study of number conservation. Cognitive Psychology, 28, 255–273.
Siegler, R. S., & Booth, J. L. (2004). Development of numerical estimation in young children. Child Development, 75, 428–444.
Silverman, I. W., York, K., & Zuidema, N. (1984). Area-matching strategies used by young children. Journal of Experimental Child
Psychology, 38, 464–474.
Silvern, S. B., Countermine, T. A., & Williamson, P. A. (1988). Young children’s interaction with a microcomputer. Early Child
Development and Care, 32, 23–35.
Simmons, F. R., Willis, C., & Adams, A.-M. (2012). Different components of working memory have different relationships with
different mathematical skills. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 111(2), 139–155. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2011.08.011
Skoumpourdi, C. (2010). Kindergarten mathematics with ‘Pepe the Rabbit’: how kindergartners use auxiliary means to solve
problems. European Early Childhood Education Research Journal, 18(3), 149–157.
Slovin, H. (2007, April). Revelations from counting: A window to conceptual understanding. Paper presented at the Research Pre-
session of the 85th Annual Meeting of the National Council of Teachers of Mathematics, Atlanta, GA.
Sonnenschein, S., Baker, L., Moyer, A., & LeFevre, S. (2005, April). Parental beliefs about children’s reading and math development
and relations with subsequent achievement. Paper presented at the Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child
Development, Atlanta, GA.
Sophian, C. (2002). Learning about what fits: Preschool children’s reasoning about effects of object size. Journal for Research in
Mathematics Education, 33, 290–302.
Sophian, C. (2004a). A prospective developmental perspective on early mathematics instruction. In D. H. Clements,
J. Sarama, & A.-M. DiBiase (Eds.), Engaging young children in mathematics: Standards for early childhood mathematics educa-
tion (pp. 253–266). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Sophian, C. (2004b). Mathematics for the future: Developing a Head Start curriculum to support mathematics learning. Early
Childhood Research Quarterly, 19, 59–81.
Sophian, C. (2013). Vicissitudes of children’s mathematical knowledge: Implications of developmental research for early child-
hood mathematics education. Early Education & Development, 24(4), 436–442. doi: 10.1080/10409289.2013.773255
Sophian, C., & Adams, N. (1987). Infants’ understanding of numerical transformations. British Journal of Educational Psychology,
5, 257–264.
Sowder, J. T. (1992a). Estimation and number sense. In D. A. Grouws (Ed.), Handbook of research on mathematics teaching and
learning (pp. 371–389). New York, NY: Macmillan.
Sowder, J. T. (1992b). Making sense of numbers in school mathematics. In G. Leinhardt, R. Putman, & R. A. Hattrup (Eds.), Analy-
sis of arithmetic for mathematics teaching. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Sowell, E. J. (1989). Effects of manipulative materials in mathematics instruction. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education,
20, 498–505.
References • 359

Spaepen, E., Coppola, M., Spelke, E. S., Carey, S. E., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2011). Number without a language model. Proceedings
of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(8), 3163–3168. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1015975108
Spelke, E. S. (2003). What makes us smart? Core knowledge and natural language. In D. Genter & S. Goldin-Meadow (Eds.),
Language in mind (pp. 277–311). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Spelke, E. S. (2008). Effects of music instruction on developing cognitive systems at the foundations of mathematics and science.
In C. Asbury & B. Rich (Eds.), Learning, Arts, & the Brain. New York/Washington D.C.: Dana Press.
Spitler, M. E., Sarama, J., & Clements, D. H. (2003). A preschooler’s understanding of “Triangle”: A case study. Paper presented at the
81st Annual Meeting of the National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
Starkey, P., & Klein, A. (1992). Economic and cultural influence on early mathematical development. In F. L. Parker, R. Robinson,
S. Sombrano, C. Piotrowski, J. Hagen, S. Randoph, & A. Baker (Eds.), New directions in child and family research: Shaping
Head Start in the 90s (p. 440). New York, NY: National Council of Jewish Women (NCJW Center for the Child).
Starkey, P., Klein, A., Chang, I., Qi, D., Lijuan, P., & Yang, Z. (1999, April). Environmental supports for young children’s mathematical de-
velopment in China and the United States. Paper presented at the Society for Research in Child Development, Albuquerque, NM.
Starkey, P., Klein, A., & Wakeley, A. (2004). Enhancing young children’s mathematical knowledge through a pre-kindergarten
mathematics intervention. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 19, 99–120.
Starr, A., Libertus, M. E., & Brannon, E. M. (2013). Infants show ratio-dependent number discrimination regardless of set size.
Infancy, 18(6), 927–941. doi: 10.1111/infa.12008
Steen, L. A. (1988). The science of patterns. Science, 240, 611–616.
Steffe, L. P. (1991). Operations that generate quantity. Learning and Individual Differences, 3, 61–82.
Steffe, L. P. (2004). PSSM from a constructivist perspective. In D. H. Clements, J. Sarama, & A.-M. DiBiase (Eds.), Engaging
young children in mathematics: Standards for early childhood mathematics education (pp. 221–251). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates.
Steffe, L. P., & Cobb, P. (1988). Construction of arithmetical meanings and strategies. New York, NY: Springer-Verlag.
Steffe, L. P., & Olive, J. (2002). Design and use of computer tools for interactive mathematical activity (TIMA). Journal of Educa-
tional Computing Research, 27(1&2), 55–76.
Steffe, L. P., Thompson, P. W., & Richards, J. (1982). Children’s counting in arithmetical problem solving. In T. P. Carpenter,
J. M. Moser, & T. A. Romberg (Eds.), Addition and subtraction: A cognitive perspective. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates.
Steffe, L. P., & Wiegel, H. G. (1994). Cognitive play and mathematical learning in computer MicroWorlds. Journal of Research in
Childhood Education, 8(2), 117–131.
Steinke, D. (2013) Rhythm and number sense: How music teaches math. Lafayette, CO: NumberWorks.
Stenmark, J. K., Thompson, V., & Cossey, R. (1986). Family math. Berkeley, CA: Lawrence Hall of Science, University of California.
Stephan, M., & Clements, D. H. (2003). Linear, area, and time measurement in prekindergarten to grade 2. In D. H. Clements (Ed.),
Learning and teaching measurement: 65th Yearbook (pp. 3–16). Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
Stevenson, H. W., & Newman, R. S. (1986). Long-term prediction of achievement and attitudes in mathematics and reading. Child
Development, 57, 646–659.
Stewart, R., Leeson, N., & Wright, R. J. (1997). Links between early arithmetical knowledge and early space and measurement
knowledge: An exploratory study. In F. Biddulph & K. Carr (Eds.), Proceedings of the Twentieth Annual Conference of the
Mathematics Education Research Group of Australasia (Vol. 2, pp. 477–484). Hamilton, New Zealand: MERGA.
Stigler, J. W., Fuson, K. C., Ham, M., & Kim, M. S. (1986). An analysis of addition and subtraction word problems in American and
Soviet elementary mathematics textbooks. Cognition and Instruction, 3, 153–171.
Stiles, J., & Nass, R. (1991). Spatial grouping activity in young children with congenital right or left hemisphere brain injury. Brain
and Cognition, 15, 201–222.
Stipek, D. J., & Ryan, R. H. (1997). Economically disadvantaged preschoolers: Ready to learn but further to go. Developmental
Psychology, 33, 711–723.
Stone, T. T., III. (1996). The academic impact of classroom computer usage upon middle-class primary grade level elementary
school children. Dissertation Abstracts International, 57–06, 2450.
Sun Lee, J., & Ginsburg, H. P. (2009). Early childhood teachers’ misconceptions about mathematics education for young children
in the United States. Australasian Journal of Early Childhood, 34(4), 37–45.
Suydam, M. N. (1986). Manipulative materials and achievement. Arithmetic Teacher, 33(6), 10, 32.
Swigger, K. M., & Swigger, B. K. (1984). Social patterns and computer use among preschool children. AEDS Journal, 17, 35–41.
Sylva, K., Melhuish, E., Sammons, P., Siraj-Blatchford, I., & Taggart, B. (2005). The effective provision of pre-school education
[EPPE] project: A longitudinal study funded by the DfEE (1997–2003). London, England: EPPE Project, Institute of Educa-
tion, University of London.
Tharp, R. G., & Gallimore, R. (1988). Rousing minds to life: Teaching, learning, and schooling in social contexts. New York, NY:
Cambridge University Press.
Thirumurthy, V. (2003). Children’s cognition of geometry and spatial thinking—A cultural process. Unpublished doctoral disserta-
tion, University of Buffalo, State University of New York.
Thomas, B. (1982). An abstract of kindergarten teachers’ elicitation and utilization of children’s prior knowledge in the teaching of
shape concepts. Unpublished manuscript, School of Education, Health, Nursing, and Arts Professions, New York University.
Thomas, G., & Tagg, A. (2004). An evaluation of the Early Numeracy Project 2003. Wellington, Australia: Ministry of Education.
Thomas, G., & Ward, J. (2001). An evaluation of the Count Me In Too pilot project. Wellington, New Zealand: Ministry of Education.
Thommen, E., Avelar, S., Sapin, V. r. Z., Perrenoud, S., & Malatesta, D. (2010). Mapping the journey from home to school: A study
on children’s representation of space. International Research in Geographical and Environmental Education, 19(3), 191–205.
Thompson, A. C. (2012). The effect of enhanced visualization instruction on first grade students’ scores on the North Carolina stan-
dard course assessment. (Dissertation), Liberty University, Lynchburg, VA.
Thompson, P. W. (1992). Notations, conventions, and constraints: Contributions to effective use of concrete materials in elemen-
tary mathematics. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 23, 123–147.
Thompson, P. W., & Thompson, A. G. (1990). Salient aspects of experience with concrete manipulatives. In F. Hitt (Ed.), Proceed-
ings of the 14th Annual Meeting of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics (Vol. 3, pp. 337–343). Mexico
City, Mexico: International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education.
360 • References

Thomson, S., Rowe, K., Underwood, C., & Peck, R. (2005). Numeracy in the early years: Project Good Start. Camberwell, Victoria,
Australia: Australian Council for Educational Research.
Thorton, C. A., Langrall, C. W., & Jones, G. A. (1997). Mathematics instruction for elementary students with learning disabilities.
Journal of Learning Disabilities, 30, 142–150.
Toll, S. W. M., Van der Ven, S., Kroesbergen, E., & Van Luit, J. E. H. (2010). Executive functions as predictors of math learning
disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 20(10), 1–12. doi: 10.1177/0022219410387302
Torbeyns, J., van den Noortgate, W., Ghesquière, P., Verschaffel, L., Van de Rijt, B. A. M., & van Luit, J. E. H. (2002). Development
of early numeracy in 5- to 7-year-old children: A comparison between Flanders and the Netherlands. Educational Research
and Evaluation. An International Journal on Theory and Practice, 8, 249–275.
Touchette, E., Petit, D., Séguin, J. R., Boivin, M., Tremblay, R. E., & Jacques Y. Montplaisir. (2007). Associations between sleep
duration patterns and behavioral/cognitive functioning at school entry. Sleep, 30, 1213–1219.
Tournaki, N. (2003). The differential effects of teaching addition through strategy instruction versus drill and practice to students
with and without learning disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 36(5), 449–458.
Tudge, J. R. H., & Doucet, F. (2004). Early mathematical experiences: Observing young Black and White children’s everyday
activities. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 19, 21–39.
Turkle, S. (1997). Seeing through computers: Education in a culture of simulation. The American Prospect, 31, 76–82.
Turner, E. E., & Celedón-Pattichis, S. (2011). Problem solving and mathematical discourse among Latino/a kindergarten students:
An analysis of opportunities to learn. Journal of Latinos and Education, 10(2), 146–169.
Turner, E. E., Celedón-Pattichis, S., & Marshall, M. E. (2008). Cultural and linguistic resources to promote problem solving and
mathematical discourse among Hispanic kindergarten students. In R. S. Kitchen & E. A. Silver (Eds.), Promoting high par-
ticipation and success in mathematics by Hispanic students: Examining opportunities and probing promising practices (Vol. 1,
pp. 19–42). Tempe, AZ: TODOS: Mathematics for ALL.
Turner, R. C., & Ritter, G. W. (2004, April). Does the impact of preschool childcare on cognition and behavior persist throughout the
elementary years? Paper presented at the American Educational Research Association, San Diego, CA.
Tyler, R. W. (1949). Basic principles of curriculum and instruction. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Tymms, P., Jones, P., Albone, S., & Henderson, B. (2009). The first seven years at school. Educational Assessment and Evaluation
Accountability, 21, 67–80.
Tzur, R., & Lambert, M. A. (2011). Intermediate participatory stages as zone of proximal development correlate in construct-
ing counting-on: A plausible conceptual source for children’s transitory “regress” to counting-all. Journal for Research in
Mathematics Education, 42, 418–450.
Ungar, S., Blades, M., & Spencer, C. (1997). Teaching visually impaired children to make distance judgments from a tactile map.
Journal of Visual Impairment and Blindness, 91, 163–174.
Uttal, D. H., Marzolf, D. P., Pierroutsakos, S. L., Smith, C. M., Troseth, G. L., Scudder, K. V., et al. (1997). Seeing through symbols:
The development of children’s understanding of symbolic relations. In O. N. Saracho & B. Spodek (Eds.), Multiple perspec-
tives on play in early childhood education (pp. 59–79). Albany, NY: State University of New York Press.
Uttal, D. H., Scudder, K. V., & DeLoache, J. S. (1997). Manipulatives as symbols: A new perspective on the use of concrete objects
to teach mathematics. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 18, 37–54.
Valiente, C., Eisenberg, N., Haugen, R., Spinrad, T. L., Hofer, C., Liew, J. et al. (2011). Children’s effortful control and aca-
demic achievement: mediation through social functioning. Early Education & Development, 22(3), 411–433. doi:
10.1080/10409289.2010.505259
Vallortigara, G. (2012). Core knowledge of object, number, and geometry: a comparative and neural approach. Cognitive Neuro-
psychology, 29(1–2), 213–236. doi: 10.1080/02643294.2012.654772
Vallortigara, G., Sovrano, V. A., & Chiandetti, C. (2009). Doing Socrates [sic] experiment right: controlled rearing studies of
geometrical knowledge in animals. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 19(1), 20–26. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2009.02.002
van Baar, A. L., de Jong, M., & Verhoeven, M. (2013). Moderate preterm children born at 32–36 weeks gestational age around
8 years of age: Differences between children with and without identified developmental and school problems. In O. Erez
(Ed.), Preterm Birth (pp. 175–189). Rijeka, Croatia: InTech Europe.
Van de Rijt, B. A. M., & Van Luit, J. E. H. (1999). Milestones in the development of infant numeracy. Scandinavian Journal of
Psychology, 40, 65–71.
Van de Rijt, B. A. M., Van Luit, J. E. H., & Pennings, A. H. (1999). The construction of the Utrecht early mathematical competence
scales. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 59, 289–309.
Van der Ven, S. H. G., Kroesbergen, E. H., Boom, J., & Leseman, P. P. M. (2012). The development of executive functions and
early mathematics: A dynamic relationship. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 82(1), 100–119. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-
8279.2011.02035.x
van Oers, B. (1994). Semiotic activity of young children in play: The construction and use of schematic representations. European
Early Childhood Education Research Journal, 2, 19–33.
van Oers, B. (1996). Are you sure? Stimulating mathematical thinking during young children’s play. European Early Childhood
Education Research Journal, 4, 71–87.
van Oers, B. (2003). Learning resources in the context of play. Promoting effective learning in early childhood. European Early
Childhood Education Research Journal, 11, 7–25.
van Oers, B., & Poland, M. (2012). Promoting abstract thinking in young children’s play. In B. van Oers (Ed.), Developmental
Education for Young Children (Vol. 7, pp. 121–136). The Netherlands: Springer.
Vanbinst, K., Ghesquiere, P., & Smedt, B. D. (2012). Numerical magnitude representations and individual differences in children’s
arithmetic strategy use. Mind, Brain, and Education, 6(3), 129–136. doi: 10.1111/j.1751-228X.2012.01148.x
Vandermaas-Peeler, M., Boomgarden, E., Finn, L., & Pittard, C. (2012). Parental support of numeracy during a cooking activity
with four-year-olds. International Journal of Early Years Education, 20(1), 78–93. doi: 10.1080/09669760.2012.663237
Varela, F. J. (1999). Ethical know-how: Action, wisdom, and cognition. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
Vergnaud, G. (1978). The acquisition of arithmetical concepts. In E. Cohors-Fresenborg & I. Wachsmuth (Eds.), Proceedings of the
2nd Conference of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education (pp. 344–355). Osnabruck, Germany.
Verschaffel, L., Greer, B., & De Corte, E. (2007). Whole number concepts and operations. In F. K. Lester, Jr. (Ed.), Second handbook
of research on mathematics teaching and learning (pp. 557–628). New York, NY: Information Age Publishing.
References • 361

Vitiello, V. E., Greenfield, D. B., Munis, P., & George, J. L. (2011). Cognitive flexibility, approaches to learning, and aca-
demic school readiness in head start preschool children. Early Education & Development, 22(3), 388–410. doi:
10.1080/10409289.2011.538366
Vogel, C., Brooks-Gunn, J., Martin, A., & Klute, M. M. (2013). Impacts of early Head Start participation on child and parent
outcomes at ages 2, 3, and 5. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 78(1), 36–63. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-
5834.2012.00702.x
Votruba-Drzal, E., & Chase, L. (2004). Child care and low-income children’s development: Direct and moderated effects. Child
Development, 75, 296–312.
Vukovic, R. K. (2012). Mathematics difficulty with and without reading difficulty: Findings and implications from a four-year
longitudinal study. Exceptional Children, 78, 280–300.
Vukovic, R. K., & Lesaux, N. K. (2013). The language of mathematics: Investigating the ways language counts for children’s
mathematical development. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 115(2), 227–244. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.
jecp.2013.02.002
Vukovic, R. K., Lesaux, N. K., & Siegel, L. S. (2010). The mathematics skills of children with reading difficulties. Learning and
Individual Differences, 20(6), 639–643.
Vurpillot, E. (1976). The visual world of the child. New York, NY: International Universities Press.
Vygotsky, L. S. (1934/1986). Thought and language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Internalization of higher psychological functions. In M. Cole, V. John-Steiner, S. Scribner, & E. Souberman
(Eds.), Mind in society (pp. 52–57). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Waber, D. P., de Moor, C., Forbes, P., Almli, C. R., Botteron, K., Leonard, G., et al. (2007). The NIH MRI study of normal brain
development: Performance of a population based sample of healthy children aged 6 to 18 years on a neuropsychological
battery. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 13(5), 729–746.
Waddell, L. R. (2010). How do we learn? African American elementary students learning reform mathematics in urban class-
rooms. Journal of Urban Mathematics Education, 3(2), 116–154.
Wadlington, E., & Burns, J. M. (1993). Instructional practices within preschool/kindergarten gifted programs. Journal for the
Education of the Gifted, 17(1), 41–52.
Wagner, S. W., & Walters, J. (1982). A longitudinal analysis of early number concepts: From numbers to number. In G. E. Forman
(Ed.), Action and Thought (pp. 137–161). New York, NY: Academic Press.
Wakeley, A. (2005, April). Mathematical knowledge of very low birth weight pre-kindergarten children. Paper presented at the
Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Atlanta, GA.
Walshaw, M., & Anthony, G. (2008). The teacher’s role in classroom discourse: A review of recent research into mathematics
classrooms. Review of Educational Research, 78, 516–551.
Walston, J. T., & West, J. (2004). Full-day and half-day kindergarten in the United States: Findings from the “Early childhood
longitudinal study, kindergarten class 1998–99” (NCES 2004-078). Washington, D.C.: U.S. Department of Education,
Institute of Education Sciences, National Center for Education Statistics.
Walston, J. T., West, J., & Rathbun, A. H. (2005). Do the greater academic gains made by full-day kindergarten children persist
through third grade? Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Educational Research Association, Montreal,
Canada.
Wang, J., & Lin, E. (2005). Comparative studies on U.S. and Chinese mathematics learning and the implications for standards-
based mathematics teaching reform. Educational Researcher, 34(5), 3–13.
Wang, M., Resnick, L. B., & Boozer, R. F. (1971). The sequence of development of some early mathematics behaviors. Child
Development, 42, 1767–1778.
Warren, E., & Cooper, T. (2008). Generalising the pattern rule for visual growth patterns: Actions that support 8 year olds’ think-
ing. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 67, 171–185. doi: 10.1007/sl0649-007-9092-2
Warren, E., & Miller, J. (2010). Exploring four year old Indigenous students’ ability to pattern. International Research in Early
Childhood Education, 1(2), 42–56.
Watson, J. A., & Brinkley, V. M. (1990/91). Space and premathematic strategies young children adopt in initial Logo problem
solving. Journal of Computing in Childhood Education, 2, 17–29.
Watson, J. A., Lange, G., & Brinkley, V. M. (1992). Logo mastery and spatial problem-solving by young children: Effects of Logo
language training, route-strategy training, and learning styles on immediate learning and transfer. Journal of Educational
Computing Research, 8, 521–540.
Weiland, C., & Yoshikawa, H. (2012). Impacts of BPS K1 on children’s early numeracy, language, literacy, executive functioning, and
emotional development. Paper presented at the School Committee, Boston Public Schools, Boston, MA.
Weiland, C., Ulvestad, K., Sachs, J., & Yoshikawa, H. (2013). Associations between classroom quality and children’s vocabulary
and executive function skills in an urban public prekindergarten program. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 28(2),
199–209.
Wellman, H. M., & Miller, K. F. (1986). Thinking about nothing: Development of concepts of zero. British Journal of Developmen-
tal Psychology, 4, 31–42.
Welsh, J. A., Nix, R. L., Blair, C., Bierman, K. L., & Nelson, K. E. (2010). The development of cognitive skills and gains in academic
school readiness for children from low-income families. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102(1), 43–53.
West, J., Denton, K., & Reaney, L. (2001). The kindergarten year: Findings from the “Early childhood longitudinal study,
kindergarten class of 1998–99.” Retrieved from http://nces.ed.gov/pubsearch/pubsinfo.asp?pubid=2002125
What Works Clearinghouse. (2013a). Bright beginnings WWC Intervention Report. Princeton, NJ: What Works Clearinghouse.
What Works Clearinghouse. (2013b). The creative curriculum for preschool WWC Intervention Report. Princeton, NJ: What Works
Clearinghouse.
Wheatley, G. (1996). Quick draw: Developing spatial sense in mathematics. Tallahassee, FL: Mathematics Learning.
Whitin, P., & Whitin, D. J. (2011, May). Mathematical pattern hunters. Young Children, 66(3), 84–90.
Wiegel, H. G. (1998). Kindergarten students’ organizations of counting in joint counting tasks and the emergence of cooperation.
Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 29, 202–224.
Wilensky, U. (1991). Abstract mediations on the concrete and concrete implications for mathematics education. In I. Harel & S.
Papert (Eds.), Constructionism (pp. 193–199). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
362 • References

Wilkinson, L. A., Martino, A., & Camilli, G. (1994). Groups that work: Social factors in elementary students mathematics problem
solving. In J. E. H. van Luit (Ed.), Research on learning and instruction of mathematics in kindergarten and primary school
(pp. 75–105). Doetinchem, the Netherlands: Graviant Publishing Company.
Williams, R. F. (2008). Guided conceptualization? Mental spaces in instructional discourse. In T. Oakley & A. Hougaard (Eds.),
Mental spaces in discourse and interaction (pp. 209–234). Amsterdam, the Netherlands: John Benjamins Publishing
Company.
Wilson, A. J., Dehaene, S., Pinel, P., Revkin, S. K., Cohen, L., & Cohen, D. K. (2006). Principles underlying the design of “The
Number Race”, an adaptive computer game for remediation of dyscalculia. Behavioral and Brain Functions, 2, 19.
Wilson, A. J., Revkin, S. K., Cohen, D. K., Cohen, L., & Dehaene, S. (2006). An open trial assessment of “The number race,” an
adaptive computer game for remediation of dyscalculia. Behavioral and Brain Functions, 2, 20.
Winton, P. J., Buysse, V., Bryant, D., Burchinal, M., Barbarin, O., Clifford, D., et al. (2005). National Center for Early Development
and Learning multi-state study of pre-kindergarten, 2001–2003. Ann Arbor, MI: Inter-university Consortium for Political and
Social Research.
Wolfgang, C. H., Stannard, L. L., & Jones, I. (2001). Block play performance among preschoolers as a predictor of later school
achievement in mathematics. Journal of Research in Childhood Education, 15, 173–180.
Wong, V. C., Cook, T. D., Barnett, W. S., & Jung, K. (2008). An effectiveness-based evaluation of five state pre-kindergarten
programs. Journal of Policy Analysis and Management, 27(1), 122–154.
Wood, K., & Frid, S. (2005). Early childhood numeracy in a multiage setting. Mathematics Education Research Journal, 16(3),
80–99.
Woodward, J. (2004). Mathematics education in the United States: Past to present. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 37, 16–31.
Wright, A. (1987). The process of microtechnological innovation in two primary schools: A case study of teachers’ thinking.
Educational Review, 39(2), 107–115.
Wright, B. (1991). What number knowledge is possessed by children beginning the kindergarten year of school? Mathematics
Education Research Journal, 3(1), 1–16.
Wright, R. J. (2003). A mathematics recovery: Program of intervention in early number learning. Australian Journal of Learning
Disabilities, 8(4), 6–11.
Wright, R. J., Martland, J., Stafford, A. K., & Stanger, G. (2002). Teaching number: Advancing children’s skills and strategies. London,
England: Paul Chapman Publications/Sage Publications.
Wright, R. J., Stanger, G., Cowper, M., & Dyson, R. (1994). A study of the numerical development of 5-year-olds and 6-year-olds.
Educational Studies in Mathematics, 26, 25–44.
Wright, R. J., Stanger, G., Cowper, M., & Dyson, R. (1996). First-graders’ progress in an experimental mathematics recovery
program. In J. Mulligan & M. Mitchelmore (Eds.), Research in early number learning (pp. 55–72). Adelaide, Australia:
AAMT.
Wright, R. J., Stanger, G., Stafford, A. K., & Martland, J. (2006). Teaching number in the classroom with 4–8 year olds. London,
England: Paul Chapman Publications/Sage Publications.
Wu, H.-H. (2011). Understanding numbers in elementary school mathematics. Providence, RI: American Mathematical Society.
Wu, S. S., Barth, M., Amin, H., Malcarne, V., & Menon, V. (2012). Math anxiety in second and third graders and its relation to
mathematics achievement. Frontiers in Psychology, 3(162), 1–11. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00162
Wynn, K. (1992). Addition and subtraction by human infants. Nature, 358, 749–750.
Xin, J. F. (1999). Computer-assisted cooperative learning in integrated classrooms for students with and without disabilities.
Information Technology in Childhood Education Annual, 1(1), 61–78.
Yackel, E., & Wheatley, G. H. (1990). Promoting visual imagery in young pupils. Arithmetic Teacher, 37(6), 52–58.
Yost, N. J. M. (1998). Computers, kids, and crayons: A comparative study of one kindergarten’s emergent literacy behaviors.
Dissertation Abstracts International, 59–08, 2847.
Young-Loveridge, J. M. (1989a). The development of children’s number concepts: The first year of school. Australian Journal of
Early Childhood, 21, 16–20.
Young-Loveridge, J. M. (1989b). The development of children’s number concepts: The first year of school. New Zealand Journal
of Educational Studies, 24(1), 47–64.
Young-Loveridge, J. M. (1989c). The relationship between children’s home experiences and their mathematical skills on entry to
school. Early Child Development and Care, 43, 43–59.
Young-Loveridge, J. M. (2004). Effects on early numeracy of a program using number books and games. Early Childhood Research
Quarterly, 19, 82–98.
Ysseldyke, J., Spicuzza, R., Kosciolek, S., Teelucksingh, E., Boys, C., & Lemkuil, A. (2003). Using a curriculum-based instructional
management system to enhance math achievement in urban schools. Journal of Education for Students Placed at Risk, 8(2),
247–265.
Zacharos, K., & Kassara, G. (2012). The development of practices for measuring length in preschool education. Skholê, 17, 97–103.
Zelazo, P. D., Reznick, J. S., & Piñon, D. E. (1995). Response control and the execution of verbal rules. Developmental Psychology,
31, 508–517.
Zur, O., & Gelman, R. (2004). Young children can add and subtract by predicting and checking. Early Childhood Research
Quarterly, 19, 121–137.

You might also like

pFad - Phonifier reborn

Pfad - The Proxy pFad of © 2024 Garber Painting. All rights reserved.

Note: This service is not intended for secure transactions such as banking, social media, email, or purchasing. Use at your own risk. We assume no liability whatsoever for broken pages.


Alternative Proxies:

Alternative Proxy

pFad Proxy

pFad v3 Proxy

pFad v4 Proxy