100% found this document useful (1 vote)
2K views183 pages

Understanding AUTISM

Understanding_autism_printable

Uploaded by

André Silva
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
100% found this document useful (1 vote)
2K views183 pages

Understanding AUTISM

Understanding_autism_printable

Uploaded by

André Silva
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
You are on page 1/ 183

Understanding autism

About this free course


Find out more about studying with The Open University by visiting our online prospectus.
This version of the content may include video, images and interactive content that may not be optimised
for your device.
You can experience this free course as it was originally designed on OpenLearn, the home of free
learning from The Open University: .
There you’ll also be able to track your progress via your activity record, which you can use to
demonstrate your learning.
Copyright © 2017 The Open University
Intellectual property
Unless otherwise stated, this resource is released under the terms of the Creative Commons Licence
v4.0 http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/deed.en_GB. Within that The Open University
interprets this licence in the following way:
www.open.edu/openlearn/about-openlearn/frequently-asked-questions-on-openlearn. Copyright and
rights falling outside the terms of the Creative Commons Licence are retained or controlled by The Open
University. Please read the full text before using any of the content.
We believe the primary barrier to accessing high-quality educational experiences is cost, which is why
we aim to publish as much free content as possible under an open licence. If it proves difficult to release
content under our preferred Creative Commons licence (e.g. because we can’t afford or gain the
clearances or find suitable alternatives), we will still release the materials for free under a personal end-
user licence.
This is because the learning experience will always be the same high quality offering and that should
always be seen as positive – even if at times the licensing is different to Creative Commons.
When using the content you must attribute us (The Open University) (the OU) and any identified author in
accordance with the terms of the Creative Commons Licence.
The Acknowledgements section is used to list, amongst other things, third party (Proprietary), licensed
content which is not subject to Creative Commons licensing. Proprietary content must be used (retained)
intact and in context to the content at all times.
The Acknowledgements section is also used to bring to your attention any other Special Restrictions
which may apply to the content. For example there may be times when the Creative Commons Non-
Commercial Sharealike licence does not apply to any of the content even if owned by us (The Open
University). In these instances, unless stated otherwise, the content may be used for personal and non-
commercial use.
We have also identified as Proprietary other material included in the content which is not subject to
Creative Commons Licence. These are OU logos, trading names and may extend to certain
photographic and video images and sound recordings and any other material as may be brought to your
attention.
Unauthorised use of any of the content may constitute a breach of the terms and conditions and/or
intellectual property laws.
We reserve the right to alter, amend or bring to an end any terms and conditions provided here without
notice.
All rights falling outside the terms of the Creative Commons licence are retained or controlled by The
Open University.
Head of Intellectual Property, The Open University

2 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Contents
Overview and guidance 8
The autism spectrum 8
Moving around the course 10
What is a badged course? 10
How to get a badge 11

Week 1: Introducing the autism spectrum 13


Introduction 13
1 Your understanding of autism 13
2 What is autism? 15
2.1 Main characteristics 15
2.2 The autism spectrum 15
3 Sources of knowledge 18
4 Methods for understanding autism 20
4.1 Case studies and observational methods 20
4.2 Experiments 20
4.3 Brain imaging 21
4.4 Longitudinal studies 21
4.5 Twin studies 21
4.6 Surveys and questionnaires 22
5 Personal testimonies 23
6 Brief history of autism: key players and milestones 25
6.1 1940s: the pioneers 25
6.2 Asperger revisited 27
6.3 1960s: biological and socio-emotional theories of autism 28
6.4 1960s: developments in the UK 28
6.5 1970s: early research milestones 28
6.6 1980s: an intervention to help children with autism 29
6.7 1980s–90s: a new theory of autism 30
6.8 1960s–2010s: prevalence of autism in the population 31
6.9 1986 onwards: autistic people speak for themselves 32
6.10 1990s onwards: the neurodiversity movement 32
6.11 The autism spectrum in the 21st century 32
7 This week’s quiz 34
8 Summary 35

Week 2: What is autism like? 37


Introduction 37
1 Autistic traits and neurotypicality 37
2 Social characteristics 38

3 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


2.1 Interacting and communicating non-verbally 38
2.2 Communicating with language 39
2.3 Taking things literally 40
2.4 Socialising 41
3 Non-social differences 42
3.1 Repetitive behaviour and routines 42
3.2 Special interests 42
3.3 Unusual sensory responses 43
4 Reactions to stress 45
5 Skills and talents 46
5.1 Skills 46
5.2 Exceptional talents 46
5.3 Creativity 47
5.4 Managing exceptionality 49
6 Further dimensions of autism 51
6.1 Intellectual ability 51
6.2 Accompanying medical and psychological difficulties 52
7 This week’s quiz 53
8 Summary 54

Week 3: Identifying and diagnosing autism 56


Introduction 56
1 Early clues to autism 56
1.1 Birth to 12 months 57
1.2 12 to 24 months 57
1.3 Can typical development plateau out? 58
1.4 Parents’ reflections on their children’s behaviour 58
2 What is diagnosis? 60
2.1 The role of diagnosis 60
2.2 DSM-5, ICD-10 and ICD-11 60
2.3 How is diagnosis carried out? 63
3 Experiences of diagnosis 65
3.1 The first diagnoses 65
3.2 Parental blame: fighting back 65
3.3 Experiences of diagnosis: 1990s to now 66
3.4 Experiencing diagnosis in adulthood 66
3.5 After diagnosis 67
4 Challenges for diagnosis 69
4.1 Autism in females 69
4.2 Diagnosing autism in different cultures 70
4.3 The effects of stigma 71
5 This week’s quiz 72
6 Summary 73

Week 4: Explaining autism: mind and brain 75


Introduction 75

4 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


1 The psychology of autism: explaining social characteristics 75
1.1 Theory of Mind 76
1.2 False belief 76
1.3 The Sally–Anne false belief task 76
1.4 Theory of Mind and thinking literally 79
2 Psychology of autism: explaining non-social characteristics 81
2.1 Executive function 81
2.2 Attention to detail 82
3 Psychology of autism: an integrative explanation? 84
3.1 Recognising emotions 84
3.2 Empathising and systemising 85
3.3 What do psychological theories tell us? 86
4 The neurobiology of autism 87
4.1 Brain structure and function 87
4.2 Neurons, neurotransmitters and hormones 89
5 The genetics of autism 91
5.1 Autism in families 91
5.2 Genes and chromosomes 91
5.3 Autism genetics are complex 93
This week's quiz 95
6 Summary 96

Week 5: Addressing challenges: approaches to intervention 9-


8
Introduction 98
1 Perspectives on recovery and help 98
1.1 Optimal outcomes 99
1.2 Contrasting views on acceptance 99
2 Interventions 100
2.1 The importance of evidence for interventions 100
2.2 Intervening without evidence 100
3 Establishing the evidence base for interventions 102
3.1 Principles of evaluation 102
3.2 Pilot and small-scale studies 103
3.3 Controlled studies and randomised control trials 104
3.4 Problems in evaluating autism interventions 105
3.5 The Research Autism database 105
4 TEACCH 107
4.1 Principles of TEACCH 107
4.2 Evidence base for TEACCH 108
5 The behavioural approach to intervention 109
5.1 Applied behavioural analysis 109
5.2 Evaluations and views of ABA 109
6 Naturalistic interventions 111
6.1 Picture Exchange Communication System (PECS) 111
6.2 Evaluations of PECS 111

5 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


7 Recent developments in intervention 113
7.1 Pre-school Autism Communication Therapy (PACT) 113
7.2 Evaluations 113
7.3 Assistive technology 113
7.4 Animal-assisted interventions 114
7.5 Evaluations of animal-assisted interventions 114
8 This week’s quiz 116
9 Summary 117

Week 6: Family life and education 119


Introduction 119
1 Family life 119
1.1 Sources of stress in families 120
1.2 Multiplex families 121
1.3 Adolescence 122
1.4 Adulthood 122
1.5 Family resilience 123
2 Support for families 124
2.1 NAS services 124
2.2 Problems in accessing support 125
3 Education 126
3.1 Challenges in educational settings 126
4 Educational support and choices: the mainstream and other options 1-
29
4.1 Educational plans and statements 129
4.2 Mainstream and other educational choices 130
4.3 Educational provision in regional and international context 130
4.4 Home education 131
5 This week’s quiz 133
6 Summary 134

Week 7: Adulthood 136


Introduction 136
1 The transition to adulthood 136
1.1 The importance of ongoing education 137
1.2 Higher education and beyond 137
1.3 Education, Health and Care Plan 137
2 Living arrangements 138
2.1 Residential support 138
2.2 Support for independence 138
2.3 Adult support in international perspective 139
3 Adult outcomes 140
3.1 Cognition, language and adaptive functioning 140
3.2 Social life, independence and mental health 140
3.3 What factors influence outcomes? 140
4 Outcomes across the spectrum 142

6 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


4.1 Timothy Baron 142
4.2 Donald Triplett 142
4.3 Wenn Lawson 143
4.4 Optimal outcomes 143
5 Addressing challenges in adult life: employment and relationships 145
5.1 Employment 145
5.2 Support for employment 146
5.3 Relationships 146
6 Addressing challenges in adult life: legal issues, health and ageing 14-
9
6.1 The criminal justice system 149
6.2 Health and ageing 149
7 The Autism Act and related legislation 151
8 This week’s quiz 152
9 Summary 153

Week 8: Reflecting back, looking forward 155


Introduction 155
1 Autism: what have you learned? 155
2 The autism spectrum in the 21st century 157
2.1 ‘When you’ve met one autistic person, you’ve met one autistic person’ 157
2.2 Neurodiversity 157
2.3 One autism or several autisms? 158
3 Future directions for autism research 160
3.1 What are the priorities? 160
3.2 Doing research well 161
4 Autism in society 162
4.1 Media portrayals of autism 162
4.2 ‘Coming out’ 163
4.2 Making society autism-friendly 163
5 Autism in a global perspective 166
5.1 Autism in the 21st century in the UK 166
5.2 Autism in Lower and Middle Income Countries 166
5.3 Autism in Ethiopia 167
5.4 Mental Health Pocket Guide and training videos 168
5.5 Pooling resources and practices 169
6 End of course quiz 170
7 End of course summary 171
References 171
Acknowledgements 180

7 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Overview and guidance
The autism spectrum

Overview and guidance


The autism spectrum
Understanding autism is a free badged course which lasts 8 weeks, with approximately 3
hours’ study time each week. You can work through the course at your own pace, so if you
have more time one week there is no problem with pushing on to complete another week’s
study.
Across the 8 weeks of Understanding autism, you will engage with key topics including
how autism affects children, adults and families, how the condition was first identified and
how ideas and understanding have evolved in the decades since. You will consider
current ideas about what causes autism, the challenges faced by autistic individuals and
their families, and what forms of help and support are available. You will learn that no two
cases of autism are completely alike, giving rise to the concept of an autism spectrum.
The course includes authoritative overviews of what is known, and equally important,
highlights the significant gaps in our knowledge. The material includes the work of autism
professionals – researchers and clinicians for instance, and also the ‘inside’ perspectives
of autistic individuals and family members: all these viewpoints play a role in
understanding autism.
Knowledge about autism and provision of services and support have tended to advance
more rapidly in countries such as the UK and the US where resources are more plentiful.
The course therefore draws extensively on the insights and provision developed in such
settings. But autism is a global concern, and it is equally important to consider how
cultural differences may affect awareness and understanding of autism, and the particular
challenges it poses in low income countries where resources are scarce.

Tell us a little about yourself


Before reading further, please take a few minutes to tell us about yourself and why you are
studying the course via this survey.

Terminology in the autism field


Throughout the course, you will engage with a range of different ideas about what autism
means to autistic individuals and their families, and how it should be approached by
society. This includes a range of views on the most appropriate language for this field.
Some key points are noted here:
Autism or autism spectrum?
In this course the terms ‘autism’ and ‘autism spectrum’ will be used more or less
interchangeably.
‘Person with autism’ or ‘autistic person’?

8 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Overview and guidance
The autism spectrum

Some years ago, the National Autistic Society of the UK recommended ‘person with
autism’ arguing that ‘autistic person’ and similar phrases were demeaning to the
individual. Yet, as it turns out, many adults on the spectrum prefer the latter phrase. This
course predominantly adopts the second usage, but recognises the range of views on
these language choices
Autism Spectrum Disorder or Autism Spectrum Condition?
Though the formal diagnostic criteria use the phrase ‘Autism Spectrum Disorder’ (ASD),
many people reject the medical and disability implications of this phrase. The more neutral
phrase ‘Autism Spectrum Condition’ or ASC will be used in this course. We recognise that
for some even the term ‘condition’ may seem an unacceptable label for a way of engaging
with the world which is just an aspect of human diversity. You will encounter some
different views on this during the course.
Difficulty, disability or difference?
Autism involves characteristic traits – ways of behaving and interacting with the world –
which differ from those of others in the population. Many of these differences are
undoubtedly challenging for the individual and their family, and ‘difficulty’ is therefore a
reasonable term to use. Some of these difficulties may also be disabling. But seeing
autism characteristics purely as ‘difficulties’ or ‘disabilities’ is an oversimplification. What
other people perceive as ‘difficult behaviour’ may be unproblematic or fulfilling for the
person with autism themselves. What is disabling for one autistic individual may not be
experienced as such by another. Differences may also take the form of very positive traits.
Where possible, then, the term ‘difference’ will be employed in this course, rather than
difficulty or disability. But it is nonetheless important to acknowledge the serious
challenges that certain differences present for people with autism.
As you can begin to see, the issues underlying different uses of language in the autism
field are controversial, and have no easy or universally accepted answers. You will
encounter these issues again at points during the course, and we will also give you
suggestions for further reading.
Acronyms
Acronyms are small groups of letters used to stand for longer phrases or descriptions
e.g. U.K. stands for United Kingdom. You will encounter quite a lot of acronyms in the
autism field e.g. NAS for National Autistic Society. All are fully introduced, usually with an
accompanying glossary entry. Try to familiarise yourself with these acronyms, some of
which are tested in quiz questions.
Special study features:

● Each week opens with five specific Learning Outcomes and a brief video overview of
the week’s work from course author, Dr Ilona Roth.
● Regular text activities encourage your active engagement with video clips and other
materials. You can record your answers in the text boxes provided and refer back to
them at any time.
● The interactive glossary will help you with new and unfamiliar terms. If you click on
any term that is in bold in the text, a definition or explanation will pop up. You can also
search through the whole glossary, which you will find here.
● An interactive quiz at the end of each week is designed for you to test your learning.
Passing the quizzes for weeks 4 and 8 will enable you to gain a badge for the course
– see the next page of this introduction for further information.

9 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Overview and guidance
Moving around the course

● Key articles and resources cited in the text are listed as references at the end of the
week. A list of further recommended reading and other resources is provided at the
end of the course.

Learning outcomes for the course:


After completing this course, you will be able to:

● outline what is meant by autism, why it is considered a spectrum, and how it affects
different individuals and families
● appreciate different approaches to understanding autism, including theoretical and
clinical perspectives and personal accounts
● outline key features of psychological, neurobiological and genetic explanations of
autism
● explain key aspects of diagnosis, intervention, education and life-span development
● understand topical issues including neurodiversity perspectives, autism prevalence
in women and autism in global context.

Moving around the course


In the ‘Summary’ at the end of each week, you can find a link to the next week. If at any
time you want to return to the start of the course, click on the ‘Course content’ link
displayed in the sidebar on any page. This page lists all weeks of the course, showing
how much of each week you have completed. Click on the arrows beside each week to
display a more detailed breakdown of your progress through that week – green ticks
indicating the pages you have already visited. You can use this to navigate to any part of
the course.
Alternatively, you can use the numbered week links at the top of every page of the course.
It's also good practice, if you access a link from within a course page (including links to the
quizzes), to open it in a new window or tab. That way you can easily return to where
you’ve come from without having to use the back button on your browser.

What is a badged course?


While studying Understanding autism you have the option to work towards gaining a
digital badge.
Badged courses are a key part of The Open University’s mission to promote the
educational well-being of the community. The courses also provide another way of helping
you to progress from informal to formal learning.
To complete a course you need to be able to find about 24 hours of study time, over a
period of about 8 weeks. However, it is possible to study them at any time, and at a pace
to suit you.
Badged courses are all available on The Open University’s OpenLearn website and do
not cost anything to study. They differ from Open University courses because you do not
receive support from a tutor. But you do get useful feedback from the interactive quizzes.

10 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Overview and guidance
Moving around the course

What is a badge?
Digital badges are a new way of demonstrating online that you have gained a skill.
Schools, colleges and universities are working with employers and other organisations to
develop open badges that help learners gain recognition for their skills, and support
employers to identify the right candidate for a job.
Badges demonstrate your work and achievement on the course. You can share your
achievement with friends, family and employers, and on social media. Badges are a great
motivation, helping you to reach the end of the course. Gaining a badge often boosts
confidence in the skills and abilities that underpin successful study. So, completing this
course should encourage you to think about taking other courses.

How to get a badge


Getting a badge is straightforward! Here’s what you have to do:

● read each week of the course


● score 50% or more in the two badge quizzes in Week 4 and Week 8.

For all the quizzes, you can have three attempts at most of the questions (for true or false
type questions you usually only get one attempt). If you get the answer right first time you
will get more marks than for a correct answer the second or third time. Therefore, please
be aware that for the two badge quizzes it is possible to get all the questions right but not
score 50% and be eligible for the badge on that attempt. If one of your answers is
incorrect you will often receive helpful feedback and suggestions about how to work out
the correct answer.
For the badge quizzes, if you’re not successful in getting 50% the first time, after 24 hours
you can attempt the whole quiz, and come back as many times as you like.
Please note that quiz questions using drag and drop do not work on a mobile device.
Instead, you should use a tablet or desktop.
We hope that as many people as possible will gain an Open University badge – so you
should see getting a badge as an opportunity to reflect on what you have learned rather
than as a test.
If you need more guidance on getting a badge and what you can do with it, take a look at
the OpenLearn FAQs. When you gain your badge you will receive an email to notify you
and you will be able to view and manage all your badges in My OpenLearn within 24 hours
of completing the criteria to gain a badge.
Get started with Week 1: Introducing the autism spectrum.

11 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Overview and guidance
Moving around the course

12 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 1: Introducing the autism spectrum
Introduction

Week 1: Introducing the


autism spectrum
Introduction
By the end of this week you should be able to:

● evaluate and update your own knowledge of autism


● express a general overview of autism and why it is considered a spectrum
● understand different ways of gaining evidence and insights into autism
● appreciate some different experiences of autism
● understand key milestones in autism history.

Now watch the video in which Dr Ilona Roth introduces this week’s work.

Video content is not available in this format.

1 Your understanding of autism


In the following activities you will evaluate your own knowledge of autism.

Activity 1 What do you know about autism?


Allow about 10 minutes

To start with, spend about 10 minutes writing some quick notes summarising your
current knowledge and understanding of autism. Maybe you already know quite a bit,
or maybe you know very little at all, but you will almost certainly come to this course
with some ideas.

Provide your answer...

13 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 1: Introducing the autism spectrum
1 Your understanding of autism

Activity 2 How accurate is your current knowledge?


Allow about 10 minutes

Now take the interactive quiz at this link:


‘How much do you know about autism?’
As you work through it, compare your own first ideas about autism with the questions
and feedback. Some of the statements are known to be true or false, while for others
the answer is uncertain even to the experts. Some information in the quiz is adapted
from Roth et al., 2010.

Activity 3 Updating your knowledge


Allow about 15 minutes

Now revise your notes from Activity 1 in light of the quiz feedback. Keep your updated
notes to hand as you read the next section.

14 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 1: Introducing the autism spectrum
2 What is autism?

2 What is autism?
This section gives an overview of key facts about autism. Read it through, and add to your
notes again where necessary.
Autism is considered to be a lifelong, developmental condition that affects how a person
thinks, communicates with and relates to other people, and interacts with the world
around them.
Autism is much more common than was previously thought. About one per cent (or one in
a hundred) of people in the UK are thought to be on the autism spectrum. You may know
someone with autism, or be on the autism spectrum yourself.
The exact causes are still unknown, but there is good evidence that subtly atypical brain
function may lie behind the differences in thinking and behaviour. It is also clear that
autism is highly genetic. Scientific evidence suggests that multiple genes are involved,
and different combinations of genes in different families or individuals.

2.1 Main characteristics


The current framework for diagnosing autism identifies two main groups of characteristics.
The first concerns social communication and interaction: autistic people find it hard to
interact socially with others or to make friends. They have communication difficulties –
some can’t speak at all; others develop speech later than usual; others can speak
perfectly well, but have problems with the social aspects of communication. For example,
they don’t understand when a listener is getting bored by their stories; they may take
language very literally or find it hard to get the point of a joke.
The second group of characteristics is ‘non-social’: autistic people tend to have narrow or
unusual interests, such as acquiring lots of information about just one type of dinosaur.
They often repeat the same activity, ranging from constantly rocking backwards and
forwards or flicking the fingers, to always eating the same foods, or repeatedly watching
the same video. These traits are collectively known as Restricted and Repetitive
Behaviours and Interests (RRBIs). Finally, most autistic people also have unusual
sensory responses, such as being overly sensitive to particular sounds, sights or smells,
or quite the opposite, for instance being insensitive to sensory inputs such as pain.
While diagnosis is based on these social and non-social difficulties, many autistic people
also have enhanced skills such as good attention to and memory for detail, or natural
ability with numbers or IT. A very small proportion of individuals on the spectrum have
outstanding talents in fields including art and music.
While social, language and sensory challenges may mean that an autistic person finds it
hard to function in a mainstream school or workplace environment, with the right support
they can flourish. Employers are beginning to realise the benefit to the workplace of
attributes often associated with autism, such as mathematical and IT skills, persistence
and attention to detail.

2.2 The autism spectrum


Because of the striking differences among individuals with autism, researchers and
practitioners usually talk of the ‘autism spectrum’. This emphasises that autistic

15 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 1: Introducing the autism spectrum
2 What is autism?

individuals may have very different profiles of strengths and weaknesses. Autism
Spectrum Disorder (ASD) is the term used within formal autism diagnosis. ASD is also
used by some researchers and practitioners; however, many prefer the more neutral term
Autism Spectrum Condition (ASC).
A proportion of people on the spectrum (up to about 50%) have profound social and
communication difficulties often including little or no speech, markedly restricted and
repetitive behaviour and interests, and intellectual disabilities. This variant of autism has
sometimes been known as classic or Kanner’s autism, after Kanner, who described
autism in 1944 (there will be more on Kanner in Section 6).
Another major group are those who have no obvious language problems and are
intellectually capable or even exceptionally bright, while remaining inflexible, bound by
routines, struggling to interact socially and communicate effectively. This variant has until
recently been diagnosed as Asperger syndrome (after the other main autism pioneer).
However, sub-types such as Asperger syndrome are being phased out in contemporary
approaches to diagnosis. One contemporary approach treats the spectrum essentially as
a continuum on which all autistic individuals have their own specific profile of strengths
and challenges. Another approach maintains the notion of a spectrum, but allocates
autistic profiles of strengths and difficulties into some newly defined sub-types. You will
read more about this in Week 3.
Autism spectrum conditions can occur alongside other psychological and physical
conditions (sometimes known as co-morbidity). Epilepsy is fairly common, especially in
those with ‘classic’ autism. Depression is also very common, as is dyslexia, although it is
not clear if the incidence of these is greater than in the non-autistic population.

Activity 4 Why is autism a spectrum?


Allow about 15 minutes

Now that you have been introduced to some key facts about autism, write a few notes
commenting on why it is considered to be a spectrum. For instance, how much does
autism vary between people?

Provide your answer...

16 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 1: Introducing the autism spectrum
2 What is autism?

Answer
How do your notes compare with our feedback below? Have you included some of the
same ideas?
Although people meeting the diagnostic criteria for autism share characteristic
difficulties in social interaction, and the tendency to repetitive or restricted behaviours
and interests, these are expressed in different ways and to different extents. One
person may speak very little, while another speaks a lot, perhaps not taking the
interests of listeners into account; one person may be particularly sensitive to loud
sounds, while another has heavy metal music as a special interest. This variety in
behaviour, and the genetic and biological variability which underlies it, has meant that
the concept of autism has evolved from the original idea of a specific syndrome, to that
of a spectrum.

Note that the concept of a spectrum derives from the physics of white light which is made
up of an array of colours ordered from low frequencies (red) to higher frequencies (violet)
as can be seen in a rainbow. But borrowing this concept has led to the misleading idea
that autism is a linear scale with profoundly affected individuals at the 'low' end and less
affected individuals at the 'high' end. In practice, a person with good intellectual and
language skills, often known as high-functioning, may nevertheless be profoundly
disabled by repetitive behaviours and routines which challenge daily living skills. So the
autism spectrum needs to be thought of in more complex terms. In this 'infinity spectrum'
one individual could be in the 'high' or violet area for intellectual skills, have moderately
good language and communication skills (green) but be in the 'low' or red area for daily
living skills.

Figure 1 Autism spectrum infinity awareness symbol.

17 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 1: Introducing the autism spectrum
3 Sources of knowledge

3 Sources of knowledge
How have your initial ideas about autism compared to what you have read so far? You
may have been pleased to have your ideas confirmed, or surprised by what you didn’t
know. As you will see, it is also important to think about where your initial ideas came
from.

Activity 5 Information about autism: reliable or not?


Allow about 15 minutes

Think now about the sources of your ideas about autism when you started the BOC.
Did they come from articles or books you have read, films, the internet, or other
sources? Spend a few minutes noting what your sources were, and for each source,
how reliable you think the information would be.

Provide your answer...

Answer
Did you mention any of the following?

● the internet
● other media such as newspapers/radio
● fictional stories and films
● theatre
● television documentary
● factual books
● personal acquaintance
● published autobiographies and parental accounts of autism
● academic articles
● conferences and lectures.

18 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 1: Introducing the autism spectrum
3 Sources of knowledge

Although ‘popular’ sources such as the internet, newspapers and radio can be useful,
the claims they make are not subject to the same standards of verification that are
required for claims based on research or clinical practice. Fictional sources such as
the theatre, books and films can also be helpful in bringing conditions like autism to
public attention. But there is no requirement to be ‘true’ to the condition, and directors
may choose to emphasise or exaggerate particular aspects for dramatic effect.
Knowing an autistic person is obviously an extremely good way to gain understanding,
but since each autistic person is different, knowledge derived in this way may not be
representative of everyone on the spectrum.
Much of the reliable information that we have about how and why autistic people differ
from the ‘neurotypical’ (non-autistic) population comes from clinical and research
work, which is spread through academic conferences and lectures and published in
academic journals and books. These sorts of sources were used for the quiz answers
earlier. Two more very important sources for understanding what autism is like are
personal testimonies by individuals with autism and their parents and carers. Again,
however, individual or parental accounts may not be representative of everyone on the
spectrum.

In the next section you will read about the different methods used by researchers and
clinicians to derive evidence about autism. Following that there will be some personal
testimonies from autistic people and from parents.

19 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 1: Introducing the autism spectrum
4 Methods for understanding autism

4 Methods for understanding autism


Claims about autism should be based on the careful and balanced evaluation of evidence
derived from reliable sources. There are many different ways of obtaining evidence, and
no one method can provide all the answers. Key methods, as discussed in this section,
typically involve studying autism from an ‘outside perspective’– that is from the stance of
the clinician or researcher. In the next section you will consider insights from the ‘inside
perspective’ – from personal accounts by autistic people or close family members.
Clinical and research methods may also involve working closely with autistic individuals
and their families to gain more of an ‘inside view’. Consequently ‘outsider’ and ‘insider’
approaches are not always clearly differentiated.

4.1 Case studies and observational methods


The case study method, most often used by clinicians, involves careful, detailed
observation of individuals (autistic children in this case) over a period of time, together
with in-depth interview with the individuals, or with other family members. The clinician
uses their expertise to interpret observed behaviour and other signs as carefully and
objectively as possible in order to formulate detailed individual reports. But the choice of
which individuals and which behaviours to observe, and the clinician’s own assumptions
and expectations may nonetheless serve as sources of bias. Fully fledged observational
methods, often used in research studies, may involve further steps to control or minimise
such biases.
In a typical investigation, observations of a group of autistic individuals will be compared
with observations of a comparable group of neurotypical people (people without autism).
The neurotypical comparison group, known as a control group, is matched as closely as
possible to the autistic group in terms of factors such as age, intellectual level and
language ability. Thus any differences that emerge between the autistic and control
groups can be attributed to autism, not some other group difference, such as age.
Matching by age might seem straightforward, but is usually not sufficient to make a fair
comparison, since autistic people may have language difficulties or be intellectually less
able than neurotypical people of the same age. Specialised tests of language ability and/
or intellectual ability (IQ), part of a systematic method known as psychometrics, are used
to measure the functioning of those in each group. Psychometrics means the objective
measurement of psychological abilities and traits, and includes tests of skills and
knowledge, abilities, attitudes, personality traits and so on.
Other means to avoid observational bias may include an agreed observation schedule to
avoid the researcher picking out just what appear to be the most significant behaviours,
and ‘blinding’, a procedure in which those making and interpreting the observations are
not told which participants have autism and which are neurotypical.

4.2 Experiments
The experiment is a method which builds further on the systematic, controlled approach
just described. Experiments are typically used to gain insights into how people deal with
the world around them – for instance, how they remember information, attend to things, or

20 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 1: Introducing the autism spectrum
4 Methods for understanding autism

recognise people’s faces. Experiments put claims such as ‘individuals with autism have
very good memories’ to a critical test, so countering false or misguided opinions.
Say that a researcher sets out to explore memory ability in autistic people. Firstly a
hypothesis is formulated. This might state, for instance, that memory for words or faces
will differ between autistic and neurotypical people. An experimental group of autistic
people and a matched neurotypical control group will take part in a memory test: for
instance, seeing or hearing a list of words and recalling them after an interval. The
number of words recalled by each participant would be one simple measure of their
memory in this task. From this the researcher calculates the average score for each group
and compares them. Statistical tests are used to evaluate the probability that any
apparent difference between the groups has occurred by chance. A result that is highly
unlikely to have occurred by chance is said to be statistically significant, enabling a
reasonably confident conclusion that the hypothesis has been confirmed.
The same basic framework can also help determine whether or not an intervention
(treatment) is useful, by comparing changes in a group of autistic children receiving the
intervention with an age- and intellectually-matched group of children also with autism, but
not receiving the intervention.

4.3 Brain imaging


Much of the evidence for differences in the brain in autistic people comes from brain
imaging methods. Some – for instance Magnetic Resonance Imaging (MRI) – are used
to explore whether there are differences in brain structure. Others, for instance functional
Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI), are used to explore brain activity while a person
performs experimental tasks such as recognising faces.

4.4 Longitudinal studies


All the methods mentioned so far – case studies, observational studies, experiments and
brain imaging – can be repeated at intervals. So, for instance, behaviours in a particular
group of children may be tracked over a number of months or years. Following the
progress of individuals or groups over a time period like this is called a longitudinal
study.

4.5 Twin studies


Another important method for autism research is the twin study. This involves comparing
identical twins, who originate from a single fertilised egg and have identical genes, with
non-identical twins, who come from two different eggs and who, on average, share 50% of
their genes – just as siblings do. This type of study regularly finds that when one member
of an identical twin pair has autism, the second twin is more likely to have some form of
autism than when the twins are non-identical, providing evidence of a strong genetic
influence. Studies of both twins and wider family members have shown that autism is
highly heritable, meaning that genetic factors play a major role in determining whether
individuals within a population will develop autism. However, this heritability is very
complex. For instance, while some genetic variations linked to autism are ones that
offspring inherit from their parents, others may arise afresh, affecting just one individual or

21 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 1: Introducing the autism spectrum
4 Methods for understanding autism

a pair of identical twins within a family. Some non-genetic factors may also contribute to
causing autism. These complex factors are further discussed in section 5.3.

4.6 Surveys and questionnaires


Surveys and questionnaires are ways to obtain a relatively large sample of opinions,
attitudes or preferences. The methods involve standardised questions (written or verbal)
exploring topics such as what parents and autistic people think about the support which is
available to them. This information is then analysed statistically to obtain a representative
picture of the responses.

22 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 1: Introducing the autism spectrum
5 Personal testimonies

5 Personal testimonies
While not everyone with autism is able to describe their experiences, an increasing
number of personal accounts provide insights which both complement and differ from
‘outsider’ insights. Parents may also provide deep insights which are not available without
these close bonds. Remember, though, that individual accounts of autism are just that.
For instance, some autistic individuals describe their thinking style as highly visual. But
this does not necessarily mean that visual thinking is a general trait in autism. Next you
will consider some parental and personal accounts.

Activity 6 Learning from personal accounts


Allow about 20 minutes

Read these two passages and listen to the interview extract. Make a few notes on
similarities and differences between the accounts. For instance, are the accounts
given by autistic individuals or by parents? What interests and difficulties do they talk
about?

Jessy’s social understanding remained, and remains, radically incomplete.


Such simple lessons. ‘We can’t ask them to move because they were there
first.’ The difference between irritation and hurt feelings. Making sense of
people, ‘grasping the general significance of situations’. What the autistic
adult, like the autistic child, finds hardest of all.
What is it like to have a mind that picks ‘remembrance’ out of the newspaper
yet must struggle to comprehend the most ordinary vocabulary of social
experience? What is it like to have to learn the myriad rules of human
interaction by rote, one by one? By rote, because the criterion of ‘how would
I feel if’ is unavailable, since so much of what pleases (or distresses) her,
does not please others, so little of what pleases (or distresses) others
pleases her.
Clara Claiborne Park writing about Jessy, aged 42 (Claiborne Park, 2001, pp. 16–7)

I must mention that the boy loved to watch the different calendars of
different rooms and then recall the numbers. He also compared them. He
thus spent a lot of time, gazing at the numbers. He wanted to know what
they meant. He found a kind of pattern in them. He wondered how the
figures bent and straightened up, curled and sometimes broke!
Tito Mukhopadhyay aged 8, writing about himself as an infant (Mukhopadhyay,
2000, p. 19)

Listen to the following extract from a recorded interview with Dr Wenn Lawson
discussing his autism with Dr Ilona Roth (Lawson and Roth, 2011). Note that Wenn
was living as a woman at the time of this interview.

Audio content is not available in this format.

23 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 1: Introducing the autism spectrum
5 Personal testimonies

Answer
Tito Mukhopadhyay (a young boy in 2000) and Wenn Lawson (an adult) contribute
their personal reflections, while Clara Claiborne Park speaks for her adult daughter
Jessy.
Tito and Wenn Lawson mention passionate engagement with particular topics
(calendars, numbers, insects, machines) while Clara mentions Jessy’s attention to
detail.
Clara Claiborne Park and Wenn Lawson both mention problems and frustrations of
understanding the subtleties of language, the rules governing social situations, and the
difficulty of taking other people’s feelings into account.

Finally, in this clip meet Alex, who will describe his experiences of autism at various points
in the module.

Video content is not available in this format.

24 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 1: Introducing the autism spectrum
6 Brief history of autism: key players and milestones

6 Brief history of autism: key players and


milestones
We will end this introduction to autism with some historical background. Autism was first
described in the early 1940s, and with developments in knowledge, the concept has been
evolving ever since. To understand where we are now, it is important to know something
about the key players and historical milestones of the last 8 decades.
Some of the language and concepts for describing and discussing autistic people
throughout history may now seem odd or completely unacceptable. Remember, though,
that ideas about autism arose within the context of psychiatric thinking and social attitudes
of the time. Even if some of these ideas now seem misguided or wrong, their role in the
history of autism must be understood and acknowledged.

6.1 1940s: the pioneers


Leo Kanner, an Austrian-born immigrant to the USA, pioneered the application of
psychiatry (the branch of medicine that deals with the mind) to psychological problems in
children. In 1943, he published one of the first known accounts of autism, based on his
case studies of 11 children referred to his clinic (Kanner, 1943). He identified language
problems; atypical use of non-verbal communication, such as eye gaze and gesture;
narrowly restricted interests and a desire for sameness; and atypical reactions to sensory
stimuli. The children’s apparent aloofness and isolation from the human world led him to
coin the phrase ‘autistic aloneness’. ‘Autism’ comes from the Greek word ‘autos’ for self,
and means being absorbed into oneself. To Kanner these shared features of behaviour
suggested a syndrome, a specific disorder with a characteristic set of symptoms
(observable characteristics that may indicate a clinical problem or disorder). Even today
Kanner’s descriptions of autism remain remarkably fresh and up to date. However, these
days the varied profile and severity of key symptoms between individuals is considered to
represent a spectrum of problems rather than a single unified syndrome

25 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 1: Introducing the autism spectrum
6 Brief history of autism: key players and milestones

Figure 2 Leo Kanner 1894-1981


The second autism pioneer was Hans Asperger, a paediatrician working in Vienna during
the same period as Kanner was active in the USA. In a paper published in German,
Asperger described a behaviour pattern very similar to Kanner’s description, which he
called ‘autistic psychopathy’ (Asperger, 1944). ‘Autistic’, as for Kanner, referred to the
children’s aloneness and self-absorption, while by ‘psychopathy’ Asperger meant a
psychiatric disorder affecting the personality. Asperger highlighted important features in
common with Kanner’s case studies, yet the children he described seemed less disabled
than Kanner’s group, generally having fluent speech and vocabulary, even if they used it
oddly. They tended to talk at length about their favourite topic or interest. Rather than
seeming unaware of the existence of others, their reactions to others appeared strange
and antisocial. Asperger’s work was for many years not widely known outside Austria. It
was only in the 1980s that psychiatrist Lorna Wing highlighted the remarkable similarity
between Asperger’s and Kanner’s clinical observations and ‘Asperger syndrome’ was
recognised as a diagnostic sub-type of autism. Wing also introduced the term ‘autism
spectrum’ to reflect the variation on the core symptom profile.

26 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 1: Introducing the autism spectrum
6 Brief history of autism: key players and milestones

Figure 3 Hans Asperger 1906-1980

6.2 Asperger revisited


Until very recently, many believed that Asperger shielded children in his clinic from Nazi
programmes designed to ‘cleanse’ the Austrian and German population of individuals
deemed to be weak, burdensome or ethnically non-Aryan (e.g. Jewish). However, very
recently two researchers have shed new light on Asperger’s war-time activities (Czech
2018; Sheffer 2018). They provide convincing evidence that Asperger collaborated with
the Nazi regime, despatching some children in his care to a ‘euthanasia clinic’ where they
met their death. These revelations have caused widespread shock and revulsion, and at
the time of launching this course, Asperger’s standing and contributions to the autism field
are under searching review. There is no simple way forward. For instance, although
Asperger syndrome is, for other reasons, ceasing to be a formal sub-diagnosis, for
several decades, many people on the autism spectrum have accepted Asperger
syndrome as their diagnosis, and embraced it as an identity. A range of views is beginning
to emerge: some are arguing that Asperger syndrome should be renamed; others point
out that history cannot be completely rewritten and terminology should be considered
distinct from the individuals who may have originated it.
You can read more about the researchers’ findings and about different ideas for the way
forward here:

27 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 1: Introducing the autism spectrum
6 Brief history of autism: key players and milestones

https://www.spectrumnews.org/news/new-evidence-ties-hans-asperger-nazi-eugenics-
program/

6.3 1960s: biological and socio-emotional theories


of autism
Kanner initially favoured a biological explanation of autism (an atypicality in brain
function). However, he began to consider autism as a form of withdrawal from the
emotional coldness he had perceived in some mothers. This socio-emotional
explanation of autism was probably influenced by the early 20th century popularity of
Freudian psychoanalysis in the USA, which saw children’s personalities as strongly
influenced by their early experiences with their parents. Although Kanner later retracted
this idea, it was enthusiastically promoted by the psychoanalyst Bruno Bettelheim who, in
the 1960s, practised a treatment in which children with autism were separated from their
parents to live in a special ‘therapeutic’ environment (Bettelheim, 1967). He described
apparently dramatic improvements in the emotional adjustment, speech and behaviour of
children treated in this way, but his claims were subsequently discredited.
Many parents were extremely upset by the poor parenting theory. The psychologist
Bernard Rimland noted that while his wife was an affectionate mother, their son screamed
constantly and inconsolably from an early age. Rimland began to collect scientific and
medical evidence to challenge Bettelheim’s approach, publishing his own biological
theory of causation (Rimland, 1964). He devoted his life’s work to autism, acting as an
advocate for children with autism and founding the Autism Society of America in 1965.

6.4 1960s: developments in the UK


In 1962, a group of pioneering UK parents founded the National Autistic Society (NAS)
to campaign for better services and support for autistic children. Their first school, run by
Sybil Elgar, was established in 1965. Elgar pioneered a teaching method using a
structured environment, clear and straightforward communication and visual aids. The
NAS has since become a major source of information, advice, services and resources,
and notably campaigns for the needs of adults with autism to be recognised and
addressed.

6.5 1970s: early research milestones


Some of the first experimental studies of autism were conducted in the 1970s by the
psychologists Beate Hermelin and Neil O’Connor, who developed a framework for
studying many aspects of sensory, perceptual and memory processing (Hermelin and
O’Connor, 1970).
Later in the 70s, Susan Folstein and Michael Rutter carried out their pioneering twin study
(Folstein and Rutter, 1977). Folstein and Rutter found that when one member of an
identical twin pair had autism, the second twin was more likely to have some form of
autism than when the twins were non-identical. The researchers interpreted this as strong
evidence that autism has a genetic basis.

28 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 1: Introducing the autism spectrum
6 Brief history of autism: key players and milestones

Figure 4 Sir Michael Rutter b.1933

6.6 1980s: an intervention to help children with


autism
An influential early development in therapeutic interventions adopted the idea that
autistic people have acquired, through learning, a set of ‘maladaptive’ or inappropriate
behaviours, which can be eradicated or ‘retrained’. The psychologist Ivar Lovaas
pioneered behaviour modification techniques for children with autism, using rewards to
encourage ‘desired’ behaviour such as language and to discourage ‘difficult’ behaviours
(Lovaas, 1987). While these days some practitioners are highly critical of behaviour
modification, arguing that it does not address the underlying psychological difficulties,
approaches based on Lovaas’ ideas are still in use. Other approaches which aim to tackle
the core problems more directly may nonetheless incorporate the basic principles of
behaviour shaping.

29 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 1: Introducing the autism spectrum
6 Brief history of autism: key players and milestones

Figure 5 Ivar Lovaas 1928-2010

6.7 1980s–90s: a new theory of autism


In 1985 three psychologists, Simon Baron-Cohen, Uta Frith and Alan Leslie, published an
experimental study suggesting that children with autism have difficulty understanding
what others are thinking and feeling – a ‘Theory of Mind (ToM)’ difficulty (Baron-Cohen,
Leslie and Frith, 1985). This finding stimulated a huge wave of further research,
throughout the 1990s and to the present day. Though by no means the only new theory
proposed during this period, and much modified, it is still influential. You will come across
it later in the course.

Figure 6 Simon Baron-Cohen b.1958

30 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 1: Introducing the autism spectrum
6 Brief history of autism: key players and milestones

Figure 7 Uta Frith b.1941

6.8 1960s–2010s: prevalence of autism in the


population
Prevalence means the number of individuals affected by a condition at a given time.
Measuring autism prevalence depends on estimating how many individuals in a
population have a symptom profile that fits the diagnosis. When autism was first identified
it seemed to be rare. In 1966, Lotter estimated that childhood autism affected 4 children in
10,000. In the late 1970s, and adopting a wider definition of autism, Lorna Wing and
Judith Gould (Wing and Gould, 1979) scrutinised the medical and social services records
of 35,000 children in Camberwell for likely cases of autism. Their prevalence estimate was
21 children in 10,000, of whom 5 would have ‘classic autism’, while the rest were
somewhere on a ‘spectrum’ – which was becoming an increasingly relevant concept to all
working on autism. Notice how prevalence estimates changed as the definition of autism
(i.e. the inclusion criteria) was broadened. With further research and clinical advances
leading to even broader criteria and more widespread diagnosis, it is currently estimated
that at least 1 in 100 individuals in the UK may be on the autism spectrum. A recent
worldwide estimate, based on prevalence studies in different countries, is somewhat

31 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 1: Introducing the autism spectrum
6 Brief history of autism: key players and milestones

lower. But where awareness of autism and diagnostic services are limited, estimates of
prevalence are inevitably lower. For some parts of the world (for instance many African
countries) where diagnosis is barely available, prevalence cannot be reliably estimated.
You will read more about this in Weeks 3 and 8.

6.9 1986 onwards: autistic people speak for


themselves
The dominant image of autism up to the 1980s was that of a person lacking the self-
insight and communicative skills necessary to tell others what it is like to be autistic. This
perception was challenged when an American professor called Temple Grandin published
a book about her experience of autism (Grandin and Scariano, 1986). As a child in the
1950s, Temple’s delayed speech development and odd behaviour were attributed to brain
damage. Her mother resisted attempts to have her institutionalised and hired a speech
therapist. At school Temple benefitted from the encouragement of some wise mentors.
When she was 18, her mother happened upon Bernard Rimland’s work and realised that
her daughter was autistic. Temple nonetheless went from strength to strength, studying at
university and becoming a professor and world expert on livestock handling, as well as a
spokesperson on autism. Since Temple’s pioneering effort, many other personal accounts
of autism have been published.

6.10 1990s onwards: the neurodiversity movement


Once people like Temple Grandin had found a voice and an audience, other challenges to
the image of autism as a disabling medical condition started to occur. Neurodiversity
refers to the idea that a neurological condition such as autism is simply one form of
variation between individuals, not a fundamentally different or pathological category
(Blume, 1998). This goes hand in hand with the idea that autism does not need treatment
or a cure. Not everyone agrees with this view: many families where an individual is
profoundly affected by autism do see autism as disabling, seek treatment, and argue that
those advocating neurodiversity do not speak for everyone on the autism spectrum.

6.11 The autism spectrum in the 21st century


The many developments in the autism field since the year 2000 make it hard to select the
most important ‘players’ and milestones. You will learn more during the rest of this course.
In research, advances in brain imaging and human genetics have shed new light on the
brain systems and genetic mechanisms that may be involved. There is no ‘cure’ for
autism, and some reject the very idea of a cure. But a range of helpful interventions is
available, thought to be especially effective if started early. Revised diagnostic criteria for
autism published by the American Psychiatric Association (American Psychiatric
Association, 2013) addressed some of the inadequacies of earlier versions. Yet there is
growing concern that autism in women may present in atypical ways and is going
undiagnosed. Progress in autism rights includes, in the UK, the 2009 Autism Act,
responding to an NAS campaign to protect the interests and address the needs of autistic
adults. In 2014 the 67th World Health Assembly of the World Health Organisation passed

32 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 1: Introducing the autism spectrum
6 Brief history of autism: key players and milestones

a resolution supported by 60 member states to coordinate their efforts in addressing


autism worldwide.

33 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 1: Introducing the autism spectrum
7 This week’s quiz

7 This week’s quiz


Check what you’ve learned this week by taking the end-of-week quiz.
Week 1 quiz
Open the quiz in a new window or tab then return here once you’ve finished it.

34 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 1: Introducing the autism spectrum
8 Summary

8 Summary
This introductory week of the course has provided you with an overview of autism,
highlighting key facts, and also just how much remains to be discovered. You have seen
how the concept of autism has evolved since Kanner's and Asperger's early work, and in
light of the variability which has emerged, how autism has come to be known as a
spectrum. The importance of basing claims about autism on reliable sources of
information has been demonstrated. Systematic methods such as experiments and
surveys play a key role in furthering knowledge; insider accounts by people on the
spectrum are also crucial in enhancing understanding. You will have noted that some
ways of discussing, explaining and engaging with autism are highly contested, especially
concerning whether it is a disorder, condition, or just a form of neurodiversity, and the
related issue of whether autistic people need to be ‘cured’.
You should now be able to:

● evaluate and update your own knowledge of autism


● express a general overview of autism and why it is considered a spectrum
● understand different ways of gaining evidence and insights into autism
● appreciate some different experiences of autism
● understand key milestones in autism history.

Next week you will look at the different features of autism in more detail, giving particular
attention to characteristic profiles of difficulties and strengths, to difficulties which may
accompany autism, and to how things may change as children develop.
Now you can go to Week 2.

35 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 1: Introducing the autism spectrum
8 Summary

36 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 2: What is autism like?
Introduction

Week 2: What is autism like?


Introduction
By now you should begin to have a fair picture of autism. This week looks in more detail at
key characteristics, giving particular attention to difficulties and challenges, and also to
strengths, and to how these may vary between individuals and groups.
Now watch the video in which Dr Ilona Roth introduces this week’s work.

Video content is not available in this format.

By the end of this week you should be able to:

● formulate a more detailed picture of autism characteristics and the challenges they
may pose for the individual and their family
● have insights into autistic strengths, including special skills and outstanding talents
● appreciate ways in which the profile of difficulties and strengths varies between
different individuals
● understand that the autism spectrum encompasses striking IQ differences between
individuals
● recognise the presence of accompanying problems such as epilepsy or dyslexia in
some cases.

1 Autistic traits and neurotypicality


As you saw in Week 1, autism involves characteristic traits – ways of behaving and
interacting with the world – which differ from those of the neurotypical population.
Given the varying pattern and impact of these differences in autism, specialists need clear
and explicit criteria to evaluate whether, say, one person’s limited use of spoken language
and another person’s excessively verbose speech both ‘tick the box’ for an autism-related
symptom. Diagnostic criteria are developed, piloted and refined over a period of years by
specialist working groups. You will explore them in more detail next week. Meanwhile,
watch this clip in which Arabella describes how she came to realise that her daughter, Iris
Grace, might be different from other children.

Video content is not available in this format.

37 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 2: What is autism like?
2 Social characteristics

2 Social characteristics
Common social differences between young children with autism and typically
developing (TD) children might include a lack of the spontaneous showing and pointing
by which TD children share their interests with others, or not following what others are
attending to, known as joint attention.

2.1 Interacting and communicating non-verbally

Figure 1 This small boy points to indicate something in the sky that interests him, and to
engage his mother’s attention in the same object. Children with autism often lack this kind
of pointing and drive for joint attention.
At the age when typically developing children start to play together and make friends, an
autistic child may prefer to play alone; the play may seem rigid – for instance a child may
prefer to line up his play figures rather than using them for a pretend game, such as a tea
party.

38 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 2: What is autism like?
2 Social characteristics

Figure 2 Children with autism often seem happiest playing alone, rather than joining in
with other children.
Further differences that may impact on social interaction at any age include problems with
non-verbal communication, such as using or understanding gestures and facial
expressions, making eye contact with others, and judging subtle aspects of body
language such as how close it is acceptable to stand to a person you don’t know. There
may also be a lack of emotional ‘give and take’, such as not responding to the changing
emotions of other people.

2.2 Communicating with language


In autism, difficulties in using language for communication range from the total absence of
speech (without the use of gestures to compensate) to the use of language that is
excessively repetitive or unusually stereotyped. Some may only speak in certain
circumstances, such as with people they know very well. This is known as selective
mutism. Speech may also seem unusual to others in terms of qualities such as
intonation (the way it rises and falls), pitch (whether the speech is high or low) and
stress (the use of emphasis to mark particular words or phrases).
Even when an autistic person has fluent language, there are likely to be other problems of
communication, such as difficulty in taking turns when talking to others. Turn-taking –

39 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 2: What is autism like?
2 Social characteristics

where one person speaks and then listens to what another has to say in response – is
fundamental for dialogue or two-way conversation. Without this skill, an autistic person
may constantly interrupt when others are talking, or deliver long monologues on a subject
which fascinates them, without noticing whether this is of interest to those listening.

2.3 Taking things literally


Another noticeable language difference is taking the meaning of words, phrases and
sentences literally – for instance a child told ‘pull your socks up’, meaning ‘try harder’, may
assume that it is their socks which need attention. When an autistic boy called Michael
Barton was at junior school, he devised a strategy to help him decode the non-literal
expressions that he found so strange. He would note the expression and draw a picture of
it, followed by a sentence explaining what it meant. As a young adult he has published his
delightful drawings to help others on the spectrum (Barton, 2012).

Figure 3 'His head is in the clouds' by Michael Barton.

Activity 1 Misunderstanding what people say


Allow about 5 minutes

In this clip a young man explains why taking things literally can make it difficult to
understand jokes. Watch the clip and then write a few notes explaining how an ironic or
sarcastic comment might lead to a similar misunderstanding (hint: think about the
words and the tone of voice when a person says something sarcastic).

Video content is not available in this format.

40 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 2: What is autism like?
2 Social characteristics

Provide your answer...

Discussion
When a person speaks ironically or sarcastically, they may say one thing, but in a tone
of voice which indicates that they mean something else. For instance ‘It’s a really nice
day today’ when it is actually pouring with rain, or ‘You are so good at English’ when it
is a field that the person struggles in. An autistic person listening to such a comment
may take it literally, and not notice or understand the non-verbal cues provided by the
tone of voice, which indicate that the speaker means something different from what he
or she says.

Literal-mindedness can also mean that an autistic person says things which others find
rude or hurtful, because they don’t realise that being completely truthful and candid isn't
always polite. For instance, telling someone who has just cooked you a meal that you
don’t like their food is not usually the best approach if you are invited for dinner. In general,
autistic people tend to lack intuitive understanding of the unspoken social rules that apply
to different situations, leading to social ‘faux pas’ or blunders. This lack of insight into other
people’s thoughts, feelings and points of view is often thought of as a ‘Theory of Mind’
failure, a psychological concept which will be covered in Week 4.
To gain an insight into how an autistic person may quite unintentionally upset others by
failing to understand social rules, watch the two video clips below.

Video content is not available in this format.

Video content is not available in this format.

2.4 Socialising
Finally, autistic people may not always value socialising as much as others. Children in
particular may react strangely to things like birthday parties and Christmas gatherings of
family, finding the changes to normal routine, the comings and goings and the surprise of
presents overwhelming. Yet autistic people can still experience loneliness. It should not
be assumed that they want to be left alone, but they may need help to socialise, and also
‘recovery time’ from socialising, since constantly trying to work out the meaning of what
others say and do can be exhausting and stressful.
For further discussion of social and communication characteristics in autism, see Roth
et al. (2010), chapter 3.

41 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 2: What is autism like?
3 Non-social differences

3 Non-social differences

3.1 Repetitive behaviour and routines


Some children incessantly twirl their fingers or flap their hands. Such movements are
often known as stimming, because they may play a role in changing sensory stimulation.
For instance, moving the hand up and down in front of the face may cause a pleasing
change in the pattern of light reaching the eyes. Children may also develop intense
preoccupations and obsessions with specific objects or textures, such as running their
hand up and down a velvet curtain. They may line up their toys in a repetitive or obsessive
way, or insist on wearing the same clothes or eating the same food every day. They may
become anxious and distressed if a familiar routine, such as going to school by bus, is
disrupted.

3.2 Special interests


Many autistic people, both children and adults, develop an extremely intense special
interest in a particular topic, sometimes becoming so absorbed that they fail to do other
things, including eating and sleeping! Sometimes, the interest topic itself may not seem
unusual, but what sets it apart is how strongly and for how long it is pursued. For instance,
many young children love Thomas the Tank Engine, but they may not watch their favourite
episodes incessantly and to the exclusion of any other children’s programmes, or still
watch them when adolescent. Some autistic special interests are markedly different from
those that most people favour as hobbies and leisure pursuits, focusing on unusual topics
such as washing machines, or the Titanic passenger list, for instance.
Neurotypical people may struggle to understand the attraction of these unusual special
interests, and parents may express frustration at the amount of time their child spends on
their interest to the exclusion of other activities, and at the incessant questioning that may
accompany it. Some autism practitioners argue that special interests are detrimental
because they exacerbate social isolation and suppress other opportunities for learning.
Yet the individual with the interest may find their chosen pursuit fulfilling and comforting
(Grove, Roth and Hoekstra, 2016), and other practitioners believe that special interests
can serve as effective building blocks for learning. For instance, teachers can make use of
the special interest to develop reading, writing and other skills, or simply as rewards for
attending to other tasks. Finding others who share the same interest may also lead to
developing a circle of friends. There is much still to be explored in this field.

Activity 2 Special interests


Allow about 10 minutes

Read the two extracts in which autistic people describe their special interests. Do you
find anything unusual about these interests – either the topics, or the way they are
pursued? Make a few notes.

In fourth grade, I was … interested in both dinosaurs and astronomy,


especially since this was the time of the Voyager flybys of Jupiter and

42 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 2: What is autism like?
3 Non-social differences

Saturn. My appetite for information was voracious and I would clip or


photocopy everything I could find on the subject in the newspaper,
magazines, academic journals and books. I think my interest in dinosaurs
waned at this point, though I remember an occasion when I went to the
neighbourhood pool and I went up to total strangers asking them to ask me
any question about dinosaurs because I felt I knew everything about them.
Sarah quoted in Sainsbury, 2000, p. 68

My parents and my family weren’t really into reading and the sorts of things I
was interested in so it was difficult and it was hard for my family to
appreciate the passionate way that I got involved with things. They didn’t
understand why anyone would want 100 mice, for example, little white mice
with purple eyes that I bred in Smiths Crisp tins covered with chicken wire in
the garage, and they didn’t understand why I collected beetles or why I
would line up my insects and race them. My sisters wouldn’t do those sorts
of games, they played tea parties and dolls houses and I wasn’t interested in
those sorts of things.
Lawson and Roth, 2011

Provide your answer...

Answer
The first extract describes special interests which many other people share. But the
engagement with the interest is very intense, and the attempt to involve strangers in
‘quizzing’ the writer is perhaps unusual.
The second extract describes passionate involvement in various interests, including
mice. Keeping one or more mice as pets would not be that unusual as a childhood
interest. But Wenn’s interest focused on one particular kind of mouse, and breeding
lots of them suggests a strong drive to collect things, which Wenn acknowledges in his
reference to beetles. Again, there is an intense and somewhat unusual way of
engaging with several topics of interest.

3.3 Unusual sensory responses


For many people on the autism spectrum, sensory information (received via eyes, ears,
touch etc.) evokes either stronger or reduced responses compared to neurotypical
individuals. For instance, autistic people may dislike fluorescent lighting because they can
perceive the flicker. This dislike can be so intense that they will refuse to enter a room with
that type of lighting. They may need labels cut off clothes as they find the sensation
unbearably irritating. One of the reasons Temple Grandin gives for wearing her distinctive
cowboy-style shirts is that they are made in very soft cotton, the only texture she says she
can tolerate next to the skin. Such accentuated reactions are known as sensory
hypersensitivity.

43 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 2: What is autism like?
3 Non-social differences

Figure 4 Temple Grandin wearing one of her soft cotton cowboy shirts.
Profound aversion to the taste or smell of particular foods is also common, and yet some
autistic children seem to crave particular tastes such as sugar. Similarly, when it comes to
sound, one person may find the noise of traffic in the street unbearable, but another may
seem immune to the noise. Apparently lowered responsivity to sensory stimuli is known
as sensory hyposensitivity. For instance, an autistic person may tolerate or enjoy the
sound of vacuum cleaners, or heavy metal music played at exceptionally high volume,
oblivious to the disagreeable effect on others, or the possible damage to their own
hearing. The pattern of these sensory differences may also change over time.
Listen to this clip of Arabella, mother of Iris Grace, discussing how Iris Grace’s sensory
responses fluctuate and change over time.

Video content is not available in this format.

44 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 2: What is autism like?
4 Reactions to stress

4 Reactions to stress
Autistic people may experience enormous stress and anxiety as a result of any of the
traits just described. Social situations, the disruption of familiar routines and activities, or
exposure to aversive sensory stimuli such as textures, smells and sounds, may be
confusing, overwhelming or even frightening. In such situations, both children and adults
with autism may resort to activities or behaviours which seem particularly unusual to
others, but which help the person to manage and reduce the stress they are feeling.
Listen to Arabella again:

Video content is not available in this format.

Sometimes, in response to an unbearable level of stress, an autistic person may have a


‘meltdown’. This may look like a temper tantrum, but is an expression of distress and
overwhelm, with the added frustration that the person may be unable to communicate this.

During our first hour on the road, Elijah rifled through hundreds of stickers I had
brought along to keep him busy in the car. He feverishly peeled them and
pasted them onto a large piece of cardboard like a small machine with his strict
and narrow concentration. In the rear-view mirror, I saw the waxy paper
backings of the stickers piling up in the back seat like fluffy patches of snow
surrounding him. When he had peeled the very last sticker from its paper he let
out a screech. Quickly, I popped the Pinocchio soundtrack into the tape player
to redirect him, but to my dismay, I had forgotten to rewind it.
… ‘REEE…WIND’ he bellowed when he suddenly heard Pinocchio’s voice
singing mid-song.
Valerie writing about her son, aged 5 (Paradiž, 2002, p. 132)

Stress reactions are likely to happen regardless of the person’s level of functioning. For
instance, high-functioning teenagers with all the intellectual skills necessary to attend
university, are quite likely to struggle with living away from home, dealing with personal
care and the constant pressure to socialise. It is important that their tutors or mentors are
aware of the additional emotional strains they are under, and that the university has
support strategies in place.

45 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 2: What is autism like?
5 Skills and talents

5 Skills and talents


While some aspects of autism undoubtedly present difficulties or challenges for the
individual, you have seen that others may simply be unusual such as an eccentric special
interest. Some autistic people also have enhanced skills, such as very good numerical or
mathematical skills, excellent memory for names or facts, or enhanced visual and spatial
skills. Since such skills often exist alongside marked difficulties, they are sometimes
called 'splinter skills', meaning that they do not generalise well to other tasks.

5.1 Skills
Often the same autistic characteristic which can make life difficult in some situations – for
instance, the tendency for attention to detail – has positive sides in others. An autistic
person trying to describe the main features of a picture or diagram, or to summarise the
general storyline of a film, may go into all the details, and thus be unable to give a concise
overview. This can be problematic, especially in education, where the skill of summarising
key points is important. However, noticing details can also be a valuable skill. For
instance, a recent report describes how autistic employees are helping banks to detect
fraud and money-laundering, with their ability to detect subtly unusual patterns in large
data sets (Hickey, 2015).
Another example is the need for structure, routine and repetition. Difficulty adapting to
change may go with the capacity to persist in tasks for which others would not have
sufficient patience or attention span. Again, this is proving invaluable in some industrial
jobs.
The same social naivety which, as we saw earlier, may lead a person into awkward social
situations, means that autistic people tend to speak their mind with great honesty. In a
world where some people resort to dishonesty and deception to get what they want, such
honesty can and should be highly valued. An employer, for instance, may place particular
trust in autistic staff members.

5.2 Exceptional talents


While pursuing their special interest (see the previous section), an autistic person is likely
to develop an exceptional knowledge or grasp of their favoured topic and so become an
expert. A minority of autistic people show truly exceptional talents, which typically
appear in early childhood, often without prior learning or practice.
For instance, Stephen Wiltshire did not speak until he was 5 years old, and was
diagnosed with autism. But he showed an outstanding talent for drawing from an early
age, and without being taught. Ever since then he has been drawing complex cityscapes
such as Canary Wharf, producing impressive and astonishingly accurate works after just
a few minutes studying the subject matter.

46 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 2: What is autism like?
5 Skills and talents

Figure 5 Canary Wharf by Stephen Wiltshire, 2006.


Watch these clips to learn more about the work of talented autistic artists.

1. Stephen Wiltshire and his sister talk us through as he makes a drawing of New York
City.
https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=bsJbApZ5GF0
2. Iris Grace is another extremely talented artist, whose talent just took off when her
mother bought her some paints. Here she is shown aged 3, working on one of her
colourful paintings.
https://vimeo.com/134532771
3. Derek Paravicini displayed a similar early and self-taught talent for the piano. As an
adult he has an impressive musical repertoire, is an accomplished jazz pianist and
has played with Jools Holland among others. Here he improvises on a well-known
Brazilian melody.
https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=Ti2xi48WCMQ

Such exceptional talent, surpassing that of most neurotypical people and coupled with
fairly profound difficulties, is often known as savant talent. As in these examples, it often
involves visual art or music, though savant talent may also involve domains such as
calculation, as portrayed in the film Rain Man. Note that Dustin Hoffman’s character
Raymond had difficulty with everyday transactions despite his specific calculating skills.

5.3 Creativity
Researchers seeking to explain the basis of exceptional savant talents have suggested
that the key underlying abilities are exceptionally accurate memory and attention to detail
rather than the ability to generate truly original outputs (Treffert, 2006). This is in keeping
with a traditional view, emphasised by the diagnostic criteria, that autistic people lack

47 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 2: What is autism like?
5 Skills and talents

creativity. However, it is becoming clear that autism is compatible with creativity and may
even promote it (Roth, 2007; Treffert, 2009). The American artist Jessica Park makes
pictures of well-known buildings which are accurate and yet highly original in their
imaginative use of colour.

Figure 6 Jessica Park’s picture of the Chrysler Building, New York.


The autistic artist Jon Adams, who was diagnosed with Asperger syndrome as an adult,
reflects on what he sees as the link between his autism and his creativity:

As I viewed the world with a different lens, a differing perspective, the influence
on my creativity and making is not surprising. I don’t think there has been a day
where creativity hasn’t been the major part of my life. As a child, I was always
assembling, collecting and drawing – never letting go of those desires or a
pencil ever since. At 6 years old, when asked what I wanted, I said ‘to be an
artist’. It seemed the most honest, logical and heartfelt answer I could give.
Jon Adams, 2016

48 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 2: What is autism like?
5 Skills and talents

Figure 7 Landscape A by Jon Adams.

5.4 Managing exceptionality


The success and public interest enjoyed by exceptional autistic artists is as fulfilling and
well-deserved as that of any gifted artist. Stephen Wiltshire has his own gallery in the Mall
in London, where people can watch him creating his drawings and buy items from his
extensive collection of works. Family members run the commercial side of his business.

Activity 3 Exceptional talent: positives and pitfalls


Allow about 10 minutes

In what ways do you think exceptional talent might benefit an autistic person and their
family? What drawbacks might there be for the individual, and indeed for other autistic
individuals and their families? Make a few notes.

Provide your answer...

Discussion
Working in a field that you enjoy and excel at is likely to be a source of well-being, self-
esteem and income. Clearly, exceptionality must be managed so that the gifted autistic
person is not exploited or treated as a spectacle.
Publicity for exceptional autistic talent could promote the idea that everyone on the
autism spectrum has exceptional savant-type skills, such that autistic people without
notable special skills and their parents may feel that everyone expects them to do

49 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 2: What is autism like?
5 Skills and talents

something exceptional. It is important to recognise and celebrate the exceptional


individuals, but not to overlook the needs and difficulties of the majority with autism
without extraordinary skills.

Arabella, Iris Grace’s mother, discusses some of the pros and cons of her daughter’s
talent in the following clip:

Video content is not available in this format.

50 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 2: What is autism like?
6 Further dimensions of autism

6 Further dimensions of autism


To conclude this week, two further sources of variation between autistic individuals will be
considered.

6.1 Intellectual ability


Differences in cognition, that is, in thinking, learning and problem-solving skills are often
represented in terms of Intelligence Quotient (IQ). This is a standardised way of
measuring intelligence using tests of both verbal and non-verbal ability. The IQ of people
with autism ranges from scores of 70 and below, which corresponds to a profound
intellectual disability, to 100, which is the average IQ for any population, to 130 and above,
which corresponds to high or extremely high IQ.

Figure 8 The characteristic bell-shaped distribution of IQ scores within the general


population. Most people’s IQ scores fall within the range 70 to 130. A minority of
individuals have scores lower than 70 or higher than 130.
When a person has profound intellectual abilities, as defined by an IQ of less than 70,
their autism has sometimes been described as ‘low-functioning’. This indicates that the
person is profoundly affected, with overt speech and language problems, possibly being
non-verbal even into adulthood. Autism in a person with an IQ of 70 or above has often
been described as ‘high-functioning’. But this is misleading in several ways. Firstly, IQ
scores in the range 70–99 are still below average; a person with an IQ of 85, for instance,
may struggle with academic tasks. Secondly, ‘high-functioning’ might suggest that the
person is not profoundly affected by their autistic symptoms. But social difficulties and
repetitive behaviours can be profoundly disabling at any level of intellectual ability. For
instance, an academically able autistic person may struggle to understand instructions or
questions from teachers, especially if these are phrased in abstract language. They may
find their learning environment stressful due to loud noise, bright lights and unusual
smells, and they may expend considerable cognitive resources in ‘pretending to be
normal’. Consequently, even with a high IQ, a person’s capacity to learn and thrive
academically may be hampered by these additional factors. Alternatively, an academically
successful person may find everyday tasks very challenging, as expressed here by Dr
Wenn Lawson, writer, psychologist and autism advocate:

I have very uneven skills. This is another one of those enigmas. I have
University degrees, I am married and I have three grown children. However, I
have huge problems with being disorganised, getting lost, using public
transport, understanding others, and just the practical interactions of social
situations. I think many of you might be saying ‘So what, I do as well.’ I know
that neural-typical individuals might have issues in these areas but I would
suggest to you that it is the degree of the ‘issue’ that separates us. How many

51 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 2: What is autism like?
6 Further dimensions of autism

of you need to sit down on the path outside of a supermarket and do breathing
exercises because they have changed the tinned soup isle?!
Lawson, 2018

6.2 Accompanying medical and psychological


difficulties
In some individuals, autism goes together with other physical or psychological health
problems. Perhaps the most common of these is epilepsy. It is estimated that up to one
third of people on the autism spectrum may be prone to epilepsy, which is more common
in ‘low-functioning’ autism (Viscidi et al., 2013). Some researchers believe that there are
common sources for the atypical brain activity associated with autism and epilepsy.
Fortunately there is a range of medications and treatment which can help to prevent
seizures occurring, but sadly this is not always the case.
Other common problems that co-occur in some cases of autism are dyslexia, Attention
Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) and Obsessive Compulsive Disorder (OCD).
Here Alex talks about his obsession with handwashing, and how he overcame it.

Video content is not available in this format.

Depression and anxiety are also common in autistic people, and there may be many
reasons for this. At school, young people with autism often experience bullying, because
others perceive them as different or eccentric, and this may lead to low self-esteem and
social isolation. Similarly at college, university or in the workplace, autistic people may find
it hard to fit in and make friends, suffering all the more because they don’t
understand why.
Here Alex talks about his experiences of bullying at school.

Video content is not available in this format.

The NAS (National Autistic Society 2018a; 2018b) has further information about all the
problems discussed in this section.

52 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 2: What is autism like?
7 This week’s quiz

7 This week’s quiz


Check what you’ve learned this week by taking the end-of-week quiz.
Week 2 quiz
Open the quiz in a new window or tab then return here once you’ve finished it.

53 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 2: What is autism like?
8 Summary

8 Summary
This week has provided an in-depth look at social and non-social characteristics of
autism, and how they differ in their expression among autistic individuals. Some
characteristics present difficulties or challenges for the individual and family members,
while others are just unusual ways of engaging with the world and other people. Some
traits, such as attention to detail, may be problematic in some situations, but highly
beneficial in others. A minority of autistic people have exceptional talents, which may
develop early and apparently without learning. Differences in IQ, and in the presence of
accompanying problems like epilepsy, is another major source of variation across the
spectrum.
You should now be able to:

● formulate a more detailed picture of autism characteristics and the challenges they
may pose for the individual and their family
● have insights into autistic strengths, including special skills and outstanding talents
● appreciate ways in which the profile of difficulties and strengths varies between
different individuals
● understand that the autism spectrum encompasses striking IQ differences between
individuals
● recognise the presence of accompanying problems such as epilepsy or dyslexia in
some cases.

Next week considers how the characteristics of autism are used within diagnosis
Now you can go to Week 3.

54 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 2: What is autism like?
8 Summary

55 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 3: Identifying and diagnosing autism
Introduction

Week 3: Identifying and


diagnosing autism
Introduction
This week starts with the earliest years of a child’s life, when there may be subtle clues
that a child is developing differently. Video clips illustrate what parents notice, and include
some parents’ reflections on getting their children diagnosed in the 1960s. The two main
current diagnostic systems are introduced, followed by clips providing parental and
personal experiences of diagnosis. The week ends with a look at two key challenges for
diagnosis: different presentation of autism in females, and diagnosis in world cultures with
differing expectations about typical behaviour.
Now watch the video in which Dr Ilona Roth introduces this week’s work.

Video content is not available in this format.

By the end of this week you should be able to:

● describe behaviour in the first 2 years which may provide early indicators of autism
● appreciate the contributions made by parental accounts
● recognise internationally used diagnostic criteria and broad principles of diagnostic
assessment
● appreciate different reactions to diagnosis
● understand challenges to diagnosis posed by gender and cross-cultural differences.

1 Early clues to autism


It is generally considered that autism cannot be reliably diagnosed before the age of 2
years. However, for some children later diagnosed with autism, parents report subtle
differences from typical developmental milestones during the first year of life, such as
delays in sitting up or walking (Chawarska et.al., 2007), or say that their child did not enjoy
cuddles, or was difficult to feed or to comfort (Young, Brewer and Pattison, 2003).

56 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 3: Identifying and diagnosing autism
1 Early clues to autism

1.1 Birth to 12 months


Where parents are recalling their child’s first months, their memories may well be
influenced by the child’s subsequent diagnosis. They may reinterpret particular
behaviours that did not cause concern at the time. For this reason, a family’s home videos
of their child’s first months are invaluable for retrospective research, as direct
observations of the child can be made. In one such study, researchers applied an
observational method (see Week 1) to extracts from home videos loaned by parents,
exploring whether infants later diagnosed with autism showed as much interest in people
as a typically developing (TD) control group of children (Maestro and Muratori, 2008).
Infants aged 0–6 months who were later diagnosed were less likely than TD children to
look, smile or make sounds to people. From 6–12 months the infants later diagnosed
became more likely to look, smile and talk to objects than to other people, and although
they did also show increased reactions to people, this remained at a lower level than the
TD infants.

1.2 12 to 24 months
It is often in the second year of life (12–24 months), when language, communication and
play are beginning to take off in TD children, that important differences start to emerge,
and are picked up by parents. They may notice difficulties with speech and language
development, apparent indifference to others, dislike of change, or eating and sleeping
issues. They may also notice that the child plays unusually, for instance repeatedly tipping
bricks out of their container and then putting them back, rather than building with them.
These possible signs may be particularly evident in children later diagnosed as ‘lower-
functioning’. The more subtle symptoms of ‘high-functioning’ autism may go unnoticed for
much longer, especially if, rather than showing developmental delays, a child seems
particularly precocious. For instance, some parents report that their child showed
strikingly early skills in reading or naming things.
Between 12 to 24 months, children subsequently diagnosed with autism may show little
response to what is said to them (known as difficulty with receptive language), and may
not use their few words in a meaningful way (known as difficulty with expressive
language). Often this will lead to a hearing check before autism is considered. Children
may also exhibit echolalia – simply repeating what has been said to them instead of
responding in a typical way. For instance if asked ‘Do you want a drink?’, a child may just
repeat that rather than saying ‘Yes please’. This may indicate a lack of reciprocity, the
two-way use of language. Another example of poor reciprocity is difficulty taking turns in
conversation, such that the person may seem to be in a monologue rather than a
dialogue. Even if a child shows no delay or difficulty in developing grammar, vocabulary
and other language features, this difficulty in turn-taking may be indicative.
Also in this second year, children subsequently diagnosed with autism may show little eye
contact (looking directly at another person), or joint attention, whether looking towards
something that another person is looking at, or engaging another person to look where
they are looking. One way that TD children do this is by protodeclarative pointing, that
is, pointing simply to indicate or share with others their interest in something. Another
noticeable difference in an autistic toddler is absence of pretend play, such as ‘driving a
vehicle’ consisting of a large cardboard box (Barbaro and Dissanayake, 2013).

57 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 3: Identifying and diagnosing autism
1 Early clues to autism

1.3 Can typical development plateau out?


Some parents report that their baby seemed to be developing perfectly normally, until at
some point (typically during the second year) this development seemed to ‘plateau out’ or
even go backwards. In the past this pattern was thought to reflect a specific ‘regressive’
form of autism. Some experts no longer accept this view, arguing instead that most infants
later diagnosed with autism undergo early subtle changes in the speed and direction of
development, which may include the loss of previously acquired skills (Dobbs, 2017).
These different views are still not reconciled.

1.4 Parents’ reflections on their children’s behaviour


In the following activity, video clips will show parents' observations of their children's
behaviour.

Activity 1 What parents noticed


Allow about 20 minutes

Watch the following video clips from The Autism Puzzle (2003), in which parents and
family members, some whose children were diagnosed in the 1960s, and others from
recent years, talk about their children’s behaviour in the first few years. List some key
behavioural features mentioned for each child.
Michael Baron on his son Timothy

Video content is not available in this format.

Provide your answer...

Saskia Baron on Timothy

Video content is not available in this format.

Provide your answer...

Lorna Wing on her daughter Susan

Video content is not available in this format.

Provide your answer...

Hannelore Braunsberg on her son David

Video content is not available in this format.

58 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 3: Identifying and diagnosing autism
1 Early clues to autism

Provide your answer...

Charlotte Moore on her son George

Video content is not available in this format.

Provide your answer...

Acis Peters and his mother Samantha

Video content is not available in this format.

Provide your answer...

Harry Peters and his mother Samantha

Video content is not available in this format.

Provide your answer...

Discussion
Key behaviours and problems noted by parents include:

Timothy Baron: initial pre-speech sounds disappeared; odd way of looking at


mother.
Susan Wing: socially remote; did not look straight at people or pay attention to
them; screamed, was ‘difficult’ and did not sleep; repetitive behaviours.
David Braunsberg: no communication; rejected solid foods; active and ‘difficult’;
still in nappies at 4.
George Moore: initially seemed bright, advanced and attentive to everything; was
early to smile, speak, stand and walk; liked stories, had excellent memory for
words and phrases; language use quaint or unusual; sleep problems.
Acis: language developed late; would not look at his Mum or point; appeared
indifferent to sounds including others talking to him.
Harry: obsession with wearing the same pyjamas; wouldn’t tolerate anything
different.

As this activity shows, there are both similarities and differences in what parents
notice. For some parents sleep problems, inflexibility or just ‘difficult behaviour’ stood
out, while for others, it was differences in attention, looking or language. Two parents,
Michael and Charlotte, thought that their child had regressed. Notice that despite his
earlier language delay, 8 year old Acis is a very verbal child.

59 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 3: Identifying and diagnosing autism
2 What is diagnosis?

2 What is diagnosis?
As you have seen, some parents develop early and well-founded concerns about their
infant’s development. Specialist assessment is important, even if this serves to rule out
autism. The fact that a child is, for instance, late in developing language, shy, or plays in
an unusual way does not necessarily mean that the child has autism. By contrast, some
parents may notice nothing unusual about their child’s development until he or she goes
to school, when teachers report that the child is troubled by the presence of other children,
by the physical environment of the school, or in other ways. Parents in this situation may
be surprised and shocked that autism is suggested as a possible basis for their child’s
difficulties. Some individuals are well into adulthood, feeling perhaps different from others,
but not knowing why, before they are formally diagnosed. Finally, it is thought that many
individuals in countries like the UK have undiagnosed autism, and in many Lower and
Middle Income Countries (LMIC) around the world, this is sadly the rule rather than the
exception. In all these different situations, formal diagnosis is the ‘gold standard’ for
deciding whether or not an individual meets the criteria for an autism spectrum diagnosis,
but is not always readily available.

2.1 The role of diagnosis


Early diagnosis is highly desirable, as early intervention has been shown to help improve
social and communication skills. However, even at 24 months, when some parents can be
fairly certain that their child is developing differently, healthcare professionals may not
give parents’ concerns the importance they deserve. And when referrals are made there
is often, unfortunately, a long wait before assessment and diagnosis.

2.2 DSM-5, ICD-10 and ICD-11


Diagnosis, whether of a physical or psychological condition, involves comparing an
individual’s symptoms with diagnostic criteria which specify which symptoms must be
present for a specific diagnosis to be appropriate, together with any criteria that rule out
this particular diagnosis.
There are two main official sources for autism diagnosis, the Diagnostic and Statistical
Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM) (published by the American Psychiatric Association),
and the International Classification of Diseases (ICD) (published by the World Health
Organisation). DSM is favoured in the USA while ICD is widely used in Europe, although
the DSM criteria are also influential in Europe. Both systems are also used in many other
parts of the world. However, the appropriateness of western-oriented criteria for diagnosis
in different cultures is questionable, as will be discussed later on.
As you learned in Weeks 1 and 2, autism involves ‘social’ and ‘non-social’ characteristics.
How these are used as the basis for diagnosis depends on which diagnostic system is
used. ICD-10 (the tenth version of the ICD) was introduced in 1992 (WHO, 1992) and
reflects thinking about the autism spectrum at that time, including some ideas that may be
familiar, such as considering Asperger syndrome a separable sub-type of autism. DSM-5
(the fifth version of the manual) was introduced in 2013 and reflects more recent thinking,
including the removal of Asperger syndrome as a separable sub-type. It is important to

60 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 3: Identifying and diagnosing autism
2 What is diagnosis?

understand the evolution from ICD-10 to DSM-5. At the time of finalising this module, both
remain in operation. The details of ICD-11 were released in June 2018. After presentation
to WHO member states in 2019, it will come into use in 2022. It will mirror DSM-5 in most
key respects but includes some different features which are being viewed as
improvements (Zeldovich, 2017). Table 1 summarises the key differences between ICD-
10 and DSM-5, with some additional notes on ICD-11. For all autism diagnoses, an
overarching criterion is whether the observed symptoms are sufficient to undermine the
individual’s functioning in everyday life. A clinician may decide, for instance, that a
person’s mild problems of social communication, and slightly repetitive behaviours and
interests are not sufficiently disruptive to merit diagnosis.

Differences between ICD-10 and DSM-5, with explanatory notes


ICD-10 DSM-5 Comments
What are the Three different autism A single diagnosis: In ICD-10, the
possible subtypes: ‘spectrum’ is divided
diagnoses? Autism spectrum dis- into three subtypes.
Childhood autism; As- order. DSM-5 has
perger syndrome; Per- relinquished subtypes
vasive developmental such as Asperger in
disorder – unspecified favour of a single
(information insuffi- continuous spectrum,
cient to diagnose aut- reflecting the variability
ism or Asperger syn- of symptoms and how
drome). they are expressed.
ICD-11 mirrors this
DSM-5 approach, but
does differentiate
autism with and
without intellectual
disability.
What are the main Three: Two: DSM-5 and ICD-11
types of symptom? both merge
Problems in: Problems in: communication and
1) communication 1) social communica- social interaction into
tion one social
2) social interaction communication
2) restrictive, repetitive symptom cluster.
3) restrictive, repetitive activities and interests;
Clinicians had found it
activities and interests sensory difficulties.
hard to categorise
(RRBIs).
symptoms as either, as
the difficulties are
interrelated. For
instance, if a child has
limited language (a
communication
problem) this will
almost inevitably limit
social interaction.
By what age must For childhood autism Social communication DSM-5 criteria
symptoms have (but not for Asperger difficulties and RRBIs accommodate cases
appeared for syndrome) ‘functional must have been where early childhood
diagnostic criteria to impairment’ in social present in early symptoms only
be met? interaction, or in childhood; however, become apparent later
language use for ‘functional impairment’ than 3 years of age.
communication, must need not be apparent This allows for what
till later. was formerly Asperger

61 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 3: Identifying and diagnosing autism
2 What is diagnosis?

have appeared by age syndrome within a


3 years. continuum of different
developmental
profiles.
Sensory problems The ICD-10 criteria do DSM-5 includes The evidence that
are common in not include sensory sensory hyper/ sensory difficulties
autism: how are problems as a formal hyposensitivities as occur in a majority of
these represented criterion. part of the ‘non-social’ autistic people,
in the criteria? RRBI cluster of convinced the DSM-5
symptoms. working groups to
include them as a
diagnostic criterion.
ICD-11 has done the
same.
How are differences Mainly through use of Evaluation of severity The DSM-5 severity
in symptoms and the three sub- (level 1, 2 and 3) is an scores should help
severity diagnoses. integral part of the clinicians to avoid the
represented in diagnosis. confusing informal
diagnosis? terms ‘high-
functioning’ and ‘low-
functioning’.
How are additional In ICD-10, problems In DSM-5, problems The aim of the DSM-5
problems beyond such as epilepsy or such as epilepsy, specifiers is to make
the main symptom dyslexia would be dyslexia or intellectual each diagnosis as
clusters noted as clinical disability are combined precise and specific as
represented? features beyond the with the individual’s possible to the
main diagnosis. diagnosis as individual person. ICD-
‘specifiers’ – additional 11 adopts similar
problems which help to principles, but treats
characterise the autism with and
individual's case. without intellectual
disability as distinct
sub-diagnoses.

Activity 2 Additional symptoms with significance


Allow about 5 minutes

From the feedback to Activity 1, can you identify one or more behaviours highlighted
by parents but not included within the ICD-10 or DSM-5 core diagnostic criteria?

Provide your answer...

62 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 3: Identifying and diagnosing autism
2 What is diagnosis?

Answer
Several parents report their children’s sleep problems. Charlotte, George’s mother,
mentions his precocious motor development and exceptional memory for words and
phrases.

2.3 How is diagnosis carried out?


Diagnostic practice depends on the professionals involved, how they work, geographical
area and country. In the UK, a school educational psychologist, GP or paediatrician may
be involved in the early stages; a clinical psychologist or psychiatrist may make the
diagnosis alone, but diagnosis will typically involve different specialists working as a team.
An individual’s journey to diagnosis can involve problems, delays, distress and conflicting
information. In the UK, most local authorities have defined procedures or ‘pathways’, that
specify the process by which children suspected of having a special educational need are
referred for specialist assessment and/or diagnostic evaluation. The National Autism
Plan for Children (NAPC) is a UK framework containing guidelines and recommenda-
tions for good practice in relation to the identification and diagnosis of children with autism
(NIASA, 2003). Pathways for adult diagnosis are less well-developed but the Autism Act
(2009) has required all local authorities to develop them.
The process of assessing a child or adult usually involves a diagnostic instrument or
tool, designed to ensure consistent application of the diagnostic criteria themselves. One
is the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS), comprising ‘modules’ for
assessing children of different age groups. A trained practitioner selects the appropriate
module (e.g. toys and games for age 2) and uses it to assess whether the individual has
‘age appropriate skills and behaviour’ – for instance, for a 2 year old, these will involve
play, joint attention and language. The Autism Diagnostic Interview - Revised (ADI-R)
comprises questions for parents about their child’s current skills and past behaviour.
Alternative instruments include the Diagnostic Interview for Social and Communica-
tion Disorders (DISCO), and the Developmental, Dimensional and Diagnostic Interview
(3di). Multiple tools may be used in an assessment.

Figure 1 Equipment used in the ADOS.

Activity 3 A diagnostic tool in action


Allow about 10 minutes

The following two clips feature expert Dr Amitta Shah using the DISCO in a diagnostic
session with a young boy and his parents. Attending the session and providing
commentary is clinical psychologist, Dr Laverne Antrobus. The interview itself focuses
on the parents’ recall of their child’s early behaviour and their current concerns. As you
watch the clips, note three other sources of evidence that Dr Shah says she has used
in making the diagnosis.

63 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 3: Identifying and diagnosing autism
2 What is diagnosis?

Video content is not available in this format.

Video content is not available in this format.

Provide your answer...

Discussion
Dr Shah mentions reports from the paediatrician, DVDs and also meeting the child
himself, which has enabled her to observe him directly. This integration of different
evidence sources is typical of diagnostic tools for autism and is a key feature of the
DISCO.

64 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 3: Identifying and diagnosing autism
3 Experiences of diagnosis

3 Experiences of diagnosis
Families and individuals vary in how they react to an autism spectrum diagnosis.
Sometimes it comes as a relief, with parents having an explanation for the differences
they see in their children, and which others may comment on. Other reactions may include
grief, surprise, devastation, disbelief, anger, guilt or helplessness, sometimes together
with depression. While parents may come to terms with the diagnosis, other family
members may find it very difficult. For people diagnosed in adulthood, diagnosis often
helps to explain years of struggling with a feeling of difference.
The following clips illustrate the reactions of parents and individuals over the years.

3.1 The first diagnoses


Back in the 1960s, parents of some of the first UK children to be diagnosed had to
contend with the view that the autism was ‘their fault’. In the pioneering group of parents
featured here, this stigmatising perception engendered disbelief and a determination to
challenge the prevailing attitudes.
Timothy Baron was one of the first children to be diagnosed in the UK. His difficulties
became apparent quite early – at 15 months – when his initial babbling began to
disappear. However, his parents ignored advice to put him in an institution. Michael, his
father, was one of the pioneering group who established the National Autistic Society, and
its founding chair. Here first is Timothy's sister, filmmaker Saskia Baron:

Video content is not available in this format.

Video content is not available in this format.

Note that the paediatrician who diagnosed Timothy had apparently picked up new
information about autism while in the States. The diagnosis given was ‘childhood
psychosis’. The term autism did not yet appear in diagnostic classifications, and clinicians
often used ‘childhood psychosis’ and ‘childhood schizophrenia’ interchangeably with it –
though it was later demonstrated that autism had distinct symptoms. Nowadays many
more professionals have knowledge of autism, and diagnosis is more likely to be carried
out by a multidisciplinary team.

3.2 Parental blame: fighting back


Here are two more pioneering parents: Lorna Wing, who became a leading autism expert,
followed by Hannelore Braunsberg. These days, Hannelore’s son David, who appears in
the clip, is a talented artist. Both these parents contested the stigmatising explanation for
their children’s condition.

Video content is not available in this format.

65 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 3: Identifying and diagnosing autism
3 Experiences of diagnosis

3.3 Experiences of diagnosis: 1990s to now


Despite huge progress in diagnostic practices since the 1960s, parents’ reactions still vary
widely, depending on what they were expecting, and also how the diagnoses were given.
In these clips, note the efforts of all three parents to find positives in their child’s diagnosis.
Charlotte Moore had been relieved to have an explanation of why her son George, born in
1990, was still in nappies at age 4. Her second son, Sam, born in 1992, was also
diagnosed with autism.

Video content is not available in this format.

Mark and his wife had imagined that Zack’s problem was primarily a failure to develop
speech, and had to come to terms with the diagnosis of a lifelong developmental
condition. Mark talks of profound shock, and emotions similar to bereavement and
mourning. Yet he describes with warmth the realisation that Zack was just the same
person as before the diagnosis, and of the progress they have both helped Zack to make.

Video content is not available in this format.

Arabella was not comfortable with the way diagnosis of her daughter Iris Grace, born in
2009, was delivered. She stresses the need for parents to receive positive messages and
a sense of empowerment.

Video content is not available in this format.

3.4 Experiencing diagnosis in adulthood


Nowadays it is not uncommon for an adult to receive a diagnosis after a younger family
member has been diagnosed. John Peters was born in the 1940s. He is articulate and
sociable, with an obsessive interest in collecting and hoarding objects. He was diagnosed
with Asperger syndrome around two decades ago, after specialists assessing his two
grandchildren, Acis and Harry, noticed his unusual behaviour. His Asperger syndrome
diagnosis implies that he showed no delay in language development as an infant. John’s
teenage behaviour was probably seen as eccentric, but in the period when he was
growing up, a profile of skills and behaviour like his did not match the symptoms of autism
as then described, and Asperger syndrome was not recognised.

Activity 4 On being diagnosed as an adult


Allow about 10 minutes

Watch these clips about John Peters. Note one positive and one negative feeling that
John experienced on being diagnosed.

Video content is not available in this format.

Video content is not available in this format.

66 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 3: Identifying and diagnosing autism
3 Experiences of diagnosis

Video content is not available in this format.

Positive:

Negative:

Discussion
John's diagnosis has helped him to explain feeling different, and other problems that
he has suffered with all his life. However, he also refers to a deep sense of guilt. This
may be partly because he now realises that he was difficult to live with, and also
because he has passed ‘autism genes’ to his grandchildren. He feels that if his own
diagnosis had been known, Acis and Harry’s difficulties would have been explained
earlier.

3.5 After diagnosis


Diagnosis is merely the beginning of a long journey. For parents, it can mean struggling to
access appropriate education (both pre-school and school) and worthwhile interventions.
You will read more about these in later weeks. Families are usually keen to know the
prognosis, that is, the likely outcome in the longer term. This however is very difficult to
predict. Some individuals may need a high level of care and support all their lives, some
may need a moderate amount of help with daily living skills and may be able to work part-
time, whereas others are able to live independently, hold down a full-time job and get
married.

Activity 5 The benefits and drawbacks of diagnosis


Allow about 15 minutes

You have seen that coming to terms with diagnosis is a challenge for any family. Some
parents who are aware their child has difficulties may even avoid formal diagnosis,
fearing that the ‘autism’ label will be detrimental.
Reflecting on what you have read and the video clips you have viewed, note three
positive consequences of diagnosis and one potentially negative consequence.

Positive:



Negative:


67 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 3: Identifying and diagnosing autism
3 Experiences of diagnosis

Answer
Positives
Diagnosis:

● helps parents to understand behavioural differences that have concerned them


● explains why a person is very good at some things and struggles with others
● may help other people to accept why a child or adult behaves unusually
● may, particularly for a child, enable access to services and interventions, as well
as appropriate support at school and/or social care services
● may, particularly for an adult, provide self-understanding, enabling the person to
accept ‘who they are’.

Negatives
Diagnosis may:

● be hard for some family members to accept, leading to rifts


● serve as a stigmatising label
● cause uncomfortable emotions such as guilt.

Now watch this clip in which Arabella concludes in favour of diagnosis, despite the ups
and downs:

Video content is not available in this format.

68 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 3: Identifying and diagnosing autism
4 Challenges for diagnosis

4 Challenges for diagnosis


The prevalence of autism, an estimate of the number of individuals in a population
identified as matching the autism diagnosis, is currently quoted as just over 1 in 100
children UK-wide (NAS, n.d.). But this figure is not consistent across all UK subgroups of
people, let alone across different countries of the world. Here we consider two reasons for
these differences.

4.1 Autism in females


Prevalence estimates for autism in males are consistently much higher than for autism in
females, suggesting that four times as many males are diagnosed as females. This male/
female disparity may even be as high as 10:1 if considering only males and females with
high-functioning autism. There is much ongoing research into genetic and other biological
factors which might make boys more susceptible to autism than girls. But there is also
increasing evidence that autism diagnoses in girls are being missed.
Experts believe that autism characteristics in girls and women do not necessarily match
the stereotype of obsessive interest in machines and other physical systems, coupled with
obvious social withdrawal. Girls on the autism spectrum may have more ‘typically female’
interests, such as fashion or fiction, such that an unusually obsessive focus on the
interest, or an unusual way of engaging with it, goes unnoticed by parents and peers,
especially in intellectually able girls. Through an interest in other people’s behaviour, and/
or a strong desire to conform, girls may strive hard to appear sociable, emulating the
behaviour of their peers. This is well illustrated in this extract from Liane Holliday Willey’s
autobiography:

My mother tells me I was very good at capturing the essence and persona of
people. At times I literally copied someone’s looks and their actions. I was
uncanny in my ability to copy accents, vocal inflections, facial expressions,
hand movements, gaits and tiny gestures. It was as if I became the person I
was emulating.
(Willey, 1999, p. 22)

Girls may work hard to camouflage their autism, struggling for years with an underlying
sense of isolation and difference. Even where parents express concerns to professionals,
for instance because their daughter is extremely ‘faddish’ about food, autism may be
dismissed because other areas of the child’s behaviour ‘don’t fit’. An increasing number of
females are receiving diagnosis in adulthood, sometimes after treatment for an
accompanying condition such as depression or anorexia. Experts are considering
whether diagnostic tools need adjustment to be more ‘gender neutral’. Nonetheless,
underdiagnosis alone is unlikely to explain all of the marked excess of males over females
on the autism spectrum, suggesting that there is also increased biological risk in males.
See Lai et al. (2015) for a review of the evidence and ideas covered in this section.

69 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 3: Identifying and diagnosing autism
4 Challenges for diagnosis

4.2 Diagnosing autism in different cultures


In many developing countries, access to diagnosis is extremely limited, which contributes
to strikingly lower prevalence estimates (Elsabbagh et al., 2012). There is also evidence
for variations in diagnosis rates between different ethnic communities within a given
country (Begeer et al., 2009).
Most diagnostic criteria and tools have been developed in the UK and US and reflect the
majority Western understanding of what is typical behaviour and what constitutes
significant difference. Beyond the challenge of making diagnosis available wherever it is
needed, there is the issue of what diagnostic criteria and instruments are appropriate to
use; cultural norms for behaviour must be considered (Norbury and Sparks, 2013).

Activity 6 What factors affect varying prevalence estimates across cultures?


Allow about 10 minutes

In these clips, Dr Prithvi Perepa, of Northampton University, draws on his own


research to consider the implications of cross-cultural factors for diagnosis of autism.
As you watch the clips, note the main factors he mentions.

Clips 1 and 2

Video content is not available in this format.

Video content is not available in this format.

Provide your answer...

Discussion
Prithvi acknowledges that limited access to diagnostic services will have an impact on
prevalence estimates: fewer diagnosed cases means lower prevalence. However, he
also stresses that culturally different understandings of autism, may affect whether
particular behavioural traits are seen as atypical or not.

Clip 3

Video content is not available in this format.

Provide your answer...

Discussion
Culturally different expectations may be particularly marked concerning children’s
early developmental milestones. For example, whereas imaginative pretend play is
considered an important milestone in the West, some cultures prefer their children’s
play to be ‘functional’ e.g. the ability to stack toy bricks would be considered more
important than using them as ‘cups’ for pretending to drink. In certain African cultures,

70 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 3: Identifying and diagnosing autism
4 Challenges for diagnosis

children are not expected to refer to themselves with personal pronouns such as ‘I’,
and in India, boys are not expected to develop speech early. Such differences are
likely to affect whether and when a parent forms concerns about their child’s
development.

Clip 4
Besides differences in cultural expectations for developmental milestones, there may
be differences concerning acceptable social behaviour. Such cultural differences may
exist in UK-based ethnic minorities, as well as across different world cultures, as Dr
Perepa explains.

Video content is not available in this format.

Provide your answer...

Discussion
Besides different expectations for development, acceptable social behaviour may
differ. For instance, in some countries, it is considered disrespectful for a child to
engage in eye contact with an adult. So lack of eye contact would not give a parent
cause for concern. Such cultural differences may exist in UK-based ethnic minorities,
as well as across different world cultures

4.3 The effects of stigma


Even when parents are aware that their child has a problem, shame or fear of societal
stigma may deter them from seeking appropriate help. For instance, in South Korean
culture, some consider autism to be a ‘genetic taint’, which diminishes the marriage
prospects of other children in the family. Parents may be reluctant to come forward, or
may seek a diagnosis of ‘Reactive attachment disorder’, which implicates the mother’s
behaviour in causing the child’s difficulties (Grinker et al., 2011).
Yet, as Dr Perepa explains, the role of stigma in explaining different rates of diagnosis
across ethnic groups is complex and requires more research.

Video content is not available in this format.

We will return to the global perspective in Week 8.

71 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 3: Identifying and diagnosing autism
5 This week’s quiz

5 This week’s quiz


Check what you’ve learned this week by taking the end-of-week quiz.
Week 3 quiz
Open the quiz in a new window or tab then return here once you’ve finished it.

72 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 3: Identifying and diagnosing autism
6 Summary

6 Summary
This week has considered clues to autism in early childhood, highlighting subtle
differences from typical development which parents tend to notice. After introducing the
diagnostic criteria and diagnostic process, the focus moved to parents’ and individuals’
experiences of diagnosis, considering both the positives and the negatives. Finally, the
week considered the likelihood that girls are being underdiagnosed because their
behaviour does not match autism stereotypes, and the challenges of diagnosis in cross-
cultural settings.
You should now be able to:

● describe behaviour in the first 2 years which may provide early indicators of autism
● appreciate the contributions made by parental accounts
● recognise internationally used diagnostic criteria and broad principles of diagnostic
assessment
● appreciate different reactions to diagnosis
● understand challenges to diagnosis posed by gender and cross-cultural differences.

Next week you will look at psychological and biological explanations for autism.
Now you can go to Week 4.

73 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 3: Identifying and diagnosing autism
6 Summary

74 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 4: Explaining autism: mind and brain
Introduction

Week 4: Explaining autism:


mind and brain
Introduction
In Weeks 1 to 3, you gained a picture of how the behaviour and thinking style of autistic
people may differ from that of neurotypical people, and you have learned which of these
key differences form the basis for diagnosis. But how and why do such differences come
about? This is a question that scientists have tried to answer, offering explanations or
theories about the psychology of autism (how the mind works), the neurobiology
(structure and function of the brain and nervous system), and genetics (the influence of
genes in a person’s physical and psychological traits, in this case making autism more
likely to occur in some people than others). This week we will consider selected highlights
of this scientific work. Notice that some of these studies consider relationships between
different levels (psychology, neurobiology and genetics).
Now watch the following video in which Dr Ilona Roth introduces this week’s work.

Video content is not available in this format.

By the end of this week you should be able to:

● understand key approaches to explaining autism


● differentiate three levels of explanation: psychological, neurobiological and genetic
● understand key psychological accounts of the autistic ‘thinking style’ and identify
implications for everyday life
● appreciate key ideas about brain and nervous system function in autism
● appreciate the complex role of genetic influences in autism.

1 The psychology of autism: explaining


social characteristics
Psychological theories of autism seek to explain the characteristic behaviour and thinking
style in terms of underlying psychological processes, that is, how autistic people
process information about other people and the environment. Some theories have a
primary focus on either the social traits or the non-social traits, so these theories are not

75 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 4: Explaining autism: mind and brain
1 The psychology of autism: explaining social characteristics

mutually exclusive. Further theories have tried to bring the social and non-social areas of
difference together within a single framework.
We will start with the theory known, confusingly, as ‘Theory of Mind’ theory, which is
probably the most influential explanation of the social challenges in autism.

1.1 Theory of Mind


Back in the 1980s, autism researchers Simon Baron-Cohen, Uta Frith and Alan Leslie set
out to investigate why children and adults with autism seemed prone to misunderstanding
social situations, and were claimed to be unaware of other people’s feelings. They
devised an elegant psychological test which suggested that most children with autism
have great difficulty in ‘putting themselves in another person’s shoes’, that is,
understanding that others have thoughts, knowledge, beliefs, desires and goals which
may differ from their own. This difficulty in understanding other people’s thoughts and
points of view is known as a Theory of Mind (ToM) or ‘mindreading’ problem (Baron-
Cohen, Leslie and Frith, 1983).

1.2 False belief


The task, developed by Baron-Cohen and his colleagues and used frequently in
subsequent studies, is known as the Sally–Anne false belief task. Before watching the
animation illustrating the task (Activity 1), consider the following imaginary scenario, an
everyday example of the kind of skill that Baron-Cohen was exploring.
You and a friend, Kelly, drive to the shops in your car. You park in a particular street
(Mount Street) and as you both have different shops to visit, you arrange to meet back at
the car in an hour’s time. Shortly after parting from your friend, you realise that you have
left your wallet at home, so you drive home to fetch it. When you get back to where you
parked before, the parking spaces are full, so you have to park in a different street (Park
Street). You know that when Kelly goes to meet you she will have the false belief that the
car is where you originally parked it. Unless you can contact her first, she will go to meet
you in Mount Street, not in Park Street.
Of course these days, mobile phones offer a ready solution to problems like this. The point
is to illustrate what neurotypical people routinely understand or figure out about what
another person is thinking. Without an understanding that Kelly would hold a false belief
about your meeting place, you would not even realise that it was necessary to redirect
her! So the ability to understand false belief is an important aspect of understanding other
people’s thoughts and beliefs – that is, theory of mind.

1.3 The Sally–Anne false belief task

Activity 1 Try out the Sally–Anne false belief task


Allow about 10 minutes

Simon Baron-Cohen used the Sally–Anne task to investigate whether autistic children
could understand false belief. The following download contains an animation that

76 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 4: Explaining autism: mind and brain
1 The psychology of autism: explaining social characteristics

illustrates this test and his results. After watching the animation, answer the three
questions that follow it, and pay careful attention to the feedback.
You can find the downloadable Sally–Anne task at this link.
You can find instructions for downloading and using the Sally–Anne task at this link.
If you can't use this download, or prefer not to, here is an image showing the key
contents of the animation:

Figure 1 The Sally–Anne false belief task.

When this task is used with typically developing children, it is found that over the age of 4–
5 years, most are able to correctly identify that Sally has a false belief about the location of
the marble.

77 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 4: Explaining autism: mind and brain
1 The psychology of autism: explaining social characteristics

Activity 2 Children taking the Sally–Anne task


Allow about 10 minutes

Now watch a short video, in which Baron-Cohen first tested two children with autism
and then a younger neurotypical child on the task. Notice that most children with
autism (around 80%) fail on the ‘belief’ question ‘Where will Sally look for her marble?’,
while children in the two control groups mostly pass. What does failure on the belief
question suggest? Note down your explanation.

Video content is not available in this format.

Provide your answer...

Discussion
Baron-Cohen and his colleagues argued that instead of 'putting themselves in Sally's
shoes', the autistic children assume that Sally’s belief about where she will find her
marble is the same as their own knowledge of where the marble really is. In short, the
study suggests that children with autism have difficulty understanding another
person's thoughts, which in this case are different from their own.

The basic finding of the Sally–Anne task has been replicated (repeated with the same
outcome) many times, with numerous variants of the task. However, the number of autistic
children failing the Sally–Anne task does vary from one study to another.
Note that the task does not offer a way of diagnosing autism. Not all autistic people fail it,
and some neurotypical people may also find it difficult.
Over the decades since the Sally-Anne false belief task findings were first reported, their
implications have been widely questioned and qualified. However, a quite common
occurrence in autism is that a person fails to give some crucial information to another
person. This could well reflect a problem in understanding other people's knowledge of a
situation.

Activity 3 A real-life ToM problem?


Allow about 5 minutes

Read the following extract, and explain how it fits with the idea that ToM is a challenge
for autistic people:

One of the most recurrent problems throughout middle childhood was my


constant failure to distinguish between my knowledge and that of others.
Very often my parents would miss deadlines or appointments because I
failed to tell them of these matters. For instance, my parents missed the
school’s Open House in my fifth grade and my mom asked me afterward
‘why didn’t you tell us about it?’ ‘I thought you knew it’, I replied.
Sarah quoted in Sainsbury, 2000, p. 60

Provide your answer...

78 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 4: Explaining autism: mind and brain
1 The psychology of autism: explaining social characteristics

Discussion
Sarah seems not to understand that her parents don’t have exactly the same thoughts
as she does. Because she has not told them about the Open House at school, they
have no knowledge that it is due to happen.

1.4 Theory of Mind and thinking literally


ToM is about understanding other people’s mental states, that is their beliefs, intentions,
feelings and so on. Some researchers have suggested a link between ToM difficulties and
‘literal-mindedness’ in autism. As you learned in Week 2, in everyday situations, people
often say one thing while actually meaning or intending something else. For instance,
when people speak ironically or sarcastically, understanding what they really mean
depends on ‘reading behind’ what they say to their intentions given the context. To test
how autistic people interpret non-literal utterances, Francesca Happé devised the
‘Strange stories’ test (Happé, 1994).
Participants in the test were presented with stories like this one which contains an
example of irony:

Ann’s mother has spent a long time cooking Ann’s favourite meal: fish and
chips. But when she brings it in, Ann is watching TV, and she doesn’t even look
up or say thank you. Ann’s mother is cross and says ‘Well that’s very nice isn’t
it! That’s what I call politeness!’

The participants were asked:

Question 1: Is it true what Ann’s mother says?


Question 2: Why does Ann’s mother say this?

79 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 4: Explaining autism: mind and brain
1 The psychology of autism: explaining social characteristics

Figure 2 Ann and her mother.


While autistic participants were able to identify that what Ann’s mother says is not true,
most struggle to identify why she might say it, suggesting, for instance, that she was
‘having a joke’. A person who has difficulty in reading the meanings and intentions behind
other people’s utterances may find all such expressions, interpreted literally, really
puzzling or disconcerting. The consequences can sometimes be really profound.
In this extract, Wenn Lawson describes how, years ago when autism was less well known,
his literal interpretation of questions from a psychiatrist led him to be misdiagnosed with
schizophrenia (Lawson and Roth, 2011).

Audio content is not available in this format.

In Week 2 you viewed two video clips titled ‘Socially awkward’ and 'Misunderstanding',
which you might like to watch again, considering how ToM difficulties could affect the
behaviour of the young man in the clip.
Just how ToM and language skills are linked is debatable (de Villiers, 2000). Autistic
people with pronounced language problems are more likely to fail false belief tasks,
possibly suggesting that language difficulties cause ToM difficulties rather than the other
way round. With much greater awareness of literal-mindedness these days, organisations
like the National Autistic Society advocate clear, straightforward language for commu-
nicating with people in the autistic community.

80 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 4: Explaining autism: mind and brain
2 Psychology of autism: explaining non-social characteristics

2 Psychology of autism: explaining non-


social characteristics
You will recall from Week 2 that ‘non-social’ features of autism include the tendency to
repeat particular movements or activities, to be stuck with familiar routines and to be
resistant to anything new or unfamiliar, however insignificant the change might seem to
others. Here we look at two theories which focus on these traits.

2.1 Executive function


Some experimental tests suggest that the profile just described reflects problems with
executive function (Demetriou et al., 2017). This means the mental capacity to organise
thoughts and actions to meet goals, for instance completing a task, shifting flexibly from
one task to another, or thinking up new ideas for things to do. Executive function
difficulties are not unique to autism – for instance, they occur in ADHD.One test of
executive function in which children and adults with autism may have difficulty is the
Tower of Hanoi puzzle, illustrated below.

Tower of Hanoi
The puzzle consists of three pegs, A, B and C, and a set of rings that vary in size. At the
start of the test, the rings are arranged in order of size on peg A (see Figure 2). The aim is
to move all the rings, one at a time and in as few moves as possible, to peg C, with the
constraint that a larger ring can never be placed on top of a smaller ring. To succeed at this
task the participant must work out an overall strategy or plan for transferring the rings – the
secret is in the way all three pegs, including peg B, are used as ‘staging posts’.

Figure 3 The Tower of Hanoi puzzle.


You might like to find an online version of the Tower of Hanoi puzzle (such as this one) and
try it for yourself. (Note: neurotypical people as well as autistic people may find this task
difficult. No conclusions can be drawn from finding the task challenging.)

Other executive function tasks test flexibility and the ability to generate new ideas.
Watch this video clip, in which Dr Jamie Craig asks first a child with autism, and then a
typically developing child to suggest new uses for a piece of foam. You will notice that

81 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 4: Explaining autism: mind and brain
2 Psychology of autism: explaining non-social characteristics

while both children come up with some ideas, the typically developing child offers a
greater and richer range of suggestions (Craig and Baron-Cohen, 1999).

Video content is not available in this format.

Executive function difficulties may be one reason why even able autistic people can find
everyday life challenging. Difficulties with everyday organisational tasks are well
illustrated in this interview extract with Wenn Lawson (Lawson and Roth, 2011):

Audio content is not available in this format.

2.2 Attention to detail


As you saw in Week 2, people on the autism spectrum often have a very good eye for
detail, coupled with difficulty in ‘seeing the wood for the trees’, that is, in grasping the most
salient aspects of a concept or idea. This thinking style is sometimes known as weak
central coherence. Attention to visual detail has been studied with the embedded
figures test, where the task is to find a simple shape such as a triangle, embedded in a
larger figure. Some people on the autism spectrum find the embedded shape more easily
and quickly than neurotypical people, suggesting that they are focusing on the details, not
on the overall shape and identity of the figure (Happé and Booth, 2008).

Figure 4 Example of the embedded figures test. Can you pick out a shape within the pram
figure that exactly matches the separate triangle?
This kind of visual eye for detail could be very useful in jobs such as quality control on a
production line, where picking up subtle flaws in a product is crucial. Conversely, an
autistic person may find it hard to work out what a whole object is from drawings of parts,
or be unable to arrange apparently random sentences into a coherent story. This could,
for instance, put a student at a disadvantage when trying to assemble information for an
essay.
Weak central coherence could help to explain the narrow, specific focus of special
interests and adherence to familiar routines in people with autism. Although changes to
routines may seem minor and unimportant to a neurotypical person, for an autistic person,
the feeling that everything is not exactly how they expect it and prefer it to be may provoke
extreme anxiety.
Watch these video clips in which two autistic people describe their perfectionist
tendencies.

Video content is not available in this format.

Video content is not available in this format.

82 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 4: Explaining autism: mind and brain
2 Psychology of autism: explaining non-social characteristics

Attention to detail by autistic people does not invariably show up in experimental tests.
However, the theory does resonate with what a lot of autistic people experience, and also
seeks to address strengths as well as challenges in the autistic thinking style. Next we will
consider one more recent theory which aims to integrate some features of the approaches
discussed so far.

83 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 4: Explaining autism: mind and brain
3 Psychology of autism: an integrative explanation?

3 Psychology of autism: an integrative


explanation?
As you have seen, the ToM approach primarily focuses on social challenges in autism,
while executive function and weak central coherence focus primarily on non-social
aspects. In the early 2000s, Baron-Cohen proposed a new theory which combined
revised ideas about the social difficulties with a new approach to the non-social
differences. This is known as the empathising–systemising theory.

3.1 Recognising emotions


One stimulus for Baron-Cohen's new approach was some research suggesting that
autistic people may struggle to understand other people’s emotions or feelings. In one
experimental test known as ‘Reading the Mind in the Eyes’, participants looked at images
such as the one below, and had to choose which of the emotions mentioned was being
portrayed (Baron-Cohen et al., 2001). Autistic adults had more difficulty than control
participants, and often made the wrong choice.

Figure 5 Image from the ‘Reading the Mind in the Eyes’ test.

84 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 4: Explaining autism: mind and brain
3 Psychology of autism: an integrative explanation?

3.2 Empathising and systemising


Bringing earlier ToM work together with research on emotion recognition, Baron-Cohen
proposed that autistic people may have difficulty with empathising – recognising or
understanding other people’s emotions, and reacting appropriately, leading to their
difficulties in interacting with other people, making friends, and so on. At the same time,
they may be strongly drawn to subject matter governed by systems or rules, leading to an
interest in fields like physics, mathematics and technology, and in fact any domain which
can be approached in a systematic rule-like way, which Baron-Cohen termed system-
ising (Baron-Cohen, 2009).
This quote from Luke Jackson, who wrote his own guide to Asperger syndrome when a
teenager, illustrates a systemising approach in his fascination with chain reactions and
springs.

I like the idea of chain reactions – one thing happening which triggers off
another, which triggers off another and so on and so on. I used to put string
round a dozen objects and watch them all fall down at once. That’s why I love
slinkies (coiled springs) so much. When you wind one round loads of things and
then let go, it pulls itself through all of them.
Jackson 2002, p. 52

To provide evidence for the ‘low empathising/high systemising profile’, Baron-Cohen


devised questionnaires – the empathy quotient (EQ) and the systemising quotient
(SQ). People were asked to evaluate how strongly they agreed or disagreed with
statements such as ‘I find it easy to put myself in someone else's shoes’.

Activity 4 Empathy Quotient and Systemising Quotient


Allow about 5 minutes

Here are some items from updated versions of the EQ and SQ. How do you think a
person who was low on empathising and high on systemising might answer each of
these questions? Choose ‘Strongly agree’ or ‘Strongly disagree’ for each.

Empathy quotient
1. I can't always see why someone should have felt offended by a remark
¡ Strongly agree
¡ Strongly disagree
2. I can pick up quickly if someone says one thing but means another
¡ Strongly agree
¡ Strongly disagree

Systemising quotient
3. I am fascinated by how machines work
¡ Strongly agree
¡ Strongly disagree
4. I rarely read articles or web pages about new technology
¡ Strongly agree

85 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 4: Explaining autism: mind and brain
3 Psychology of autism: an integrative explanation?

¡ Strongly disagree
These items are from the Empathy Quotient (EQ) and Systemising Quotient (SQ) for
Adults (Autism Research Centre, 2018)

From people’s total questionnaire scores, Baron-Cohen reported that autistic respondents
tended to score high on systemising and low on empathising, whereas few of the typically
developed respondents tested showed the same pattern (Baron-Cohen et al., 2014).
According to this profile, autistic people have particular interests and skills in ‘systematic’
subjects such as engineering, science and computing, and are less interested or skilled in
dealing with people and social relationships. This profile does seem broadly consistent
with the diagnostic criteria, and the theory has the merit of attempting to integrate social
and non-social characteristics. However, the approach has been strongly questioned
(Subbaraman, 2014). Firstly, since the questionnaires are ‘self-report’, participants may
choose their answers to fit a certain self-image, rather than their true preferences.
Secondly, the overall score differences between autistic and control groups of participants
are small. Thirdly, the theory plays to a predominantly male stereotype of the autistic
person as socially insensitive and obsessed with machines. But as you saw in Week 2,
autistic people may have skills in many areas besides engineering, science and
computing. They cannot be assumed to conform neatly to the empathising–systemising
profile, and the way autism is expressed in women may be particularly far from this
account.

3.3 What do psychological theories tell us?


Each of the psychological theories outlined this week is based on research, and offers
possible insights into the thinking processes and experiences of individuals with autism. A
serious limitation to all the theories is that the experimental evidence tends to come from
‘high-functioning’ individuals, who are able to understand and comply with task
instructions. Even within this group, there are individual differences in the pattern of
responses, highlighting once again the heterogeneity of autism. Also, autistic females are
often under-represented in psychological tests, and when they are tested, there are some
differences in how they respond (Mandy et al., 2012).
None of the approaches considered offers insights into unusual sensory responses, such
as hyper- and hyposensitivity to sounds and other sensory stimuli. These affect a majority
of people on the spectrum, but in different ways, which makes it hard for researchers to
identify common underlying factors. Some recent research in this field focuses on
differentiating the sensory issues in autism into different profiles, as a first step towards
explaining underlying causes (Tomchek et al., 2018).
Psychological theories and tasks do provide a useful reference point for research into how
underlying neurobiological differences relate to the behavioural characteristics of autism.
We turn to neurobiology next.

86 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 4: Explaining autism: mind and brain
4 The neurobiology of autism

4 The neurobiology of autism


Neurobiological research covers a range of levels from the structure and function of brain
areas, to the way nerve cells communicate with one another, to the role of ‘chemical
messengers’ such as hormones.

4.1 Brain structure and function


Research into the structure and function of the brain draws extensively on a range of
brain imaging techniques. Magnetic Resonance Imaging (MRI) suggests that key brain
structures may have a slightly different size or shape in autistic people. For instance,
studies suggest that the brains of some young autistic children are 5–10% bigger than
those of typically developing children, although this difference disappears by adoles-
cence. Another area where increased size has been observed is the amygdala, a brain
region involved in evaluating the emotional significance of external events. Overgrowth of
the amygdala in children with autism is related to the severity of their social and
communication difficulties, – greater overgrowth tends to go with more severe difficulties –
but again this disparity of size compared with typical development disappears in
adolescence.
Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI) monitors brain activity while a person is
performing psychological tests, such as recognising faces, responding to emotional
stimuli or understanding language.

87 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 4: Explaining autism: mind and brain
4 The neurobiology of autism

Figure 6 Image of Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI) scanning. The


participant in an fMRI study responds to images, sounds or other stimuli while lying in a
scanner. Use of magnetic fields to monitor blood flow in the brain yields information about
which brain regions are active.
The patterns of brain activity revealed by fMRI may differ in people with autism, compared
to the neurotypical population. For instance, there may be reduced activity in a brain
region called the fusiform gyrus, which has a specialised role in face recognition, linking
with the observation that autistic people find it hard to recognise faces which they have
seen before.

Figure 7 Images of fMRI scans of an adolescent male on the autism spectrum (right)
compared with an age- and IQ-matched typically developing control (left).
Atypical patterns of brain activity are also observed when autistic people perform tasks
such as the ‘Reading the Mind in the Eyes’ test illustrated earlier.
(See Lai, Lombardo and Baron-Cohen, 2013, for an overview of findings like those
discussed in this section.)

88 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 4: Explaining autism: mind and brain
4 The neurobiology of autism

4.2 Neurons, neurotransmitters and hormones


Other brain studies focus at the level of nerve cells or neurons and other microscopic
components of the nervous system. The millions of neurons which transmit messages
within the brain and nervous system form a dense network of connected fibres. One
current idea is that the overall pattern of this connectivity is different in the autistic brain,
with some areas being unusually densely connected, and others sparsely connected
(Wolff, 2012, cited in Hughes, 2012).

Figure 8 Different areas of the brain are profusely connected by complex networks of
neurons. The pattern of connectivity may be different in autism.
There is also some evidence that the structure and functioning of synapses (the
miniscule gaps between neurons) differs in autism. Messages are transmitted along nerve
fibres by minute electric currents, but crossing the synaptic gaps involves chemical
messengers called neurotransmitters.

Figure 9 Schematic image of a synapse with molecules of neurotransmitter carrying


messages across the synaptic gap.

89 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 4: Explaining autism: mind and brain
4 The neurobiology of autism

Some studies suggest that some autistic people have higher than normal levels of a
neurotransmitter called serotonin in their blood, suggesting an overproduction within the
brain. Medical drugs which are known to influence serotonin uptake in the brain can have
an impact on anger and repetitive behaviour in autism.
Finally, different levels of certain hormones have been reported. Hormones are another
type of chemical messenger, which play an important role in bodily and brain function. For
instance, when you experience a stressful situation, adrenaline is released which causes
perspiration, raised heart rate and other ‘fight or flight’ reactions. Oxytocin is a hormone
which is known to be important in social relations, and some studies report lower levels in
children with autism. Some studies suggest that administering extra oxytocin to autistic
people via a nasal spray may help with emotion recognition skills.
If you are interested to learn more about the brain and nervous system and how they are
implicated in autism, you may like to look at parts of this optional interactive activity:
You can find an downloadable interactive brain activity at this link.
You can find instructions for downloading and using the interactive brain activity at this
link.

90 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 4: Explaining autism: mind and brain
5 The genetics of autism

5 The genetics of autism


Both the psychological characteristics of autism and underlying neurobiological
atypicalities are thought to be linked to genetic influences.

5.1 Autism in families


As you learned in Week 1, twin studies provide evidence for a strong genetic factor in
autism. When one twin of a pair is on the autism spectrum, the chance of the other twin
also being on the spectrum (known as concordance) is much higher if these twins are
identical than if they are fraternal. Identical twins have identical genes, whereas fraternal
twins are no more alike genetically than, say, two brothers or two sisters. Identical twins
and fraternal twins are likely to be very similar in their experiences of the environment.
Therefore, the higher concordance for autism in identical twins suggests that the
predisposition to develop autism is strongly genetic.
Even in non-identical twins or in siblings, concordance for autism is higher than in the
neurotypical population. Twin and sibling concordance findings together suggest that
autism can be passed down (inherited) from one generation to another, and affect multiple
members of the same family. This was illustrated in video clips in Week 3: brothers Acis
and Harry and their grandfather John are all on the autism spectrum.

5.2 Genes and chromosomes


Genes are commonly referred to as the ‘blueprints’ for life – the basic units of heredity,
which means the passing on of physical and behavioural traits from one generation to the
next. Differences in, for example, our eye colour or hair structure are due to differences in
genes we inherit from our parents. Genes are small sections of very long molecular
structures called deoxyribonucleic acid (DNA). DNA has a precise sequence of units,
with a section of these units together constituting a gene.
Each gene contains the instructions for making a specific protein which in turn instructs
our cells and tissues how to interact, grow or respond to damage and diseases. For
example, there is a gene containing instructions for making the hormone insulin, a
substance with an important role in regulating our blood sugar level. While we each have
a gene that codes for insulin, the precise sequence of units within that gene can vary
between individuals. Such differences, known as DNA variants, may cause differences in
the way a protein functions
Genes are organised into 23 distinctive pairs of structures called chromosomes, carried
within the cells of our body, and visible down a microscope (Fig. 10). Of each pair of
chromosomes, one is inherited from the mother and one is inherited from the father. The
first 22 pairs of chromosomes look the same down the microscope for men and women.
The last pair are the sex chromosomes. The body cells of males have one copy each of
the X chromosome and the (much smaller) Y chromosome, while the cells of females
typically have two X chromosomes.

91 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 4: Explaining autism: mind and brain
5 The genetics of autism

Figure 10 Photograph of the 23 pairs of chromosomes of a human male, from a light


microscope image, magnified approximately 1000 times.
In humans, the 23 chromosome pairs hold tens of thousands of genes that together are
known as the human genome.
Each of us inherits one member of each chromosome pair from each of our parents – but
before they are passed on during sexual reproduction, material within each of these
chromosome pairs crosses over during the formation of egg or sperm, part of a process
known as meiosis (Fig. 11). Natural breakages occur on each paired chromosome, shown
here at the white line two thirds of the way down, and a section of genetic material is
exchanged such that novel combinations are formed. Each egg or sperm inherits just one
of these paired chromosomes which includes a novel combination of material from each
parental chromosome.

Figure 11 A chromosome pair before, during and after the cross-over of genetic material
that occurs during production of eggs and sperm.
The result is that offspring inherit combinations of each of their parents' chromosomes,
and can therefore also exhibit characteristics of each parent, and of earlier generations
(Fig. 12).

92 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 4: Explaining autism: mind and brain
5 The genetics of autism

Figure 12 The effect of crossing over on the arrangement of genetic material along
chromosomes, shown across three generations. The three colours represent the different
origins of the genetic material in the chromosomes of the grandparents, and how a
grandchild thus inherits a mix of genetic material from both grandparents.
During egg and sperm formation, and during breakage and rejoining of the parental
chromosomes, additional changes to the genetic sequence may occur. Such ‘mutations’
give rise to new DNA variants which may contain altered instructions for protein
development. This may in turn trigger differences in the way the brain and nervous system
develop, which are then passed on to further generations

5.3 Autism genetics are complex


In autism, links are proposed between particular genetic variants, atypical development of
the nervous system including the brain, and behavioural differences such as theory of
mind difficulty and repetitive tendencies.
However, this is a complex and speculative field. Whereas certain conditions (e.g. cystic
fibrosis) result from mutation of just a single gene, autism (except in fairly rare cases)
involves the combined effects of variants in many different genes – it is said to be

93 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 4: Explaining autism: mind and brain
5 The genetics of autism

polygenic. Also, this combination of genes and variants may vary from one person or
family to another, so autism is said to be heterogeneous. Researchers have found
candidate genes (genes that may potentially transmit susceptibility to autism) on a very
large number of chromosomes.
Besides this complex pattern of genetic influences, the heritability of autism (the extent
to which it can be attributed to genetic factors) is not 100%. A parent may be on the autism
spectrum without his or her children inevitably having autism. A child may develop autism
without a family history – their genome may be altered by a new mutation, for instance
arising during egg or sperm production, or by epigenetic influences, which control the
action of certain genes. Other non-genetic factors may also influence the development of
autism. For instance, exposure in the womb to Valproate, taken by a mother as epilepsy
medication, may increase a child’s risk of developing autism (Christensen et al., 2013).
Influences such as these, thought to affect the prenatal environment of the developing
foetus, are not well understood at present.
(See Lai, Lombardo and Baron-Cohen, 2013 for an overview of autism findings like those
discussed in this section)

94 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 4: Explaining autism: mind and brain
This week's quiz

This week's quiz


Now it’s time to complete the Week 4 badge quiz. It is similar to previous quizzes, but this
time instead of answering 5 questions there will be 15.
Remember, this quiz counts towards your badge. If you’re not successful the first time,
you can attempt the quiz again in 24 hours.
Week 4 badge quiz.
Open the quiz in a new window or tab then come back here when you’re done.

95 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 4: Explaining autism: mind and brain
6 Summary

6 Summary
This week has looked at key explanations of autism at three different levels: psychology,
neurobiology and genetics. Psychological research has highlighted processes (theory of
mind, executive function, etc.) which may help to explain observed behaviour and thinking
style in autism. But no theory is conclusive, the findings vary, and in particular sensory
differences have proved difficult to explain. There is copious research into brain and
nervous system differences and genetic influences related to autism, but again, no firm
conclusions can be drawn.
Next week deals with the very different question of how autistic people can be helped.
You should now be able to:

● understand key approaches to explaining autism


● differentiate three levels of explanation: psychological, neurobiological and genetic
● understand key psychological accounts of the autistic ‘thinking style’ and identify
implications for everyday life
● appreciate key ideas about brain and nervous system function in autism
● appreciate the complex role of genetic influences in autism.

Now you can go to Week 5.

96 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 4: Explaining autism: mind and brain
6 Summary

97 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 5: Addressing challenges: approaches to intervention
Introduction

Week 5: Addressing
challenges: approaches to
intervention
Introduction
This week opens by considering the goal, sought by some people and vehemently
rejected by others, of a ‘cure’ for autism. Interventions, the main focus of this week, are
presented not as ‘cures’, but as procedures to help autistic people overcome challenges
in order to promote development and quality of life. The framework for evaluating
interventions in relation to evidence is explained. A small selection of key interventions
are outlined, including some recent developments in the field.
Now watch the following video in which Dr Ilona Roth introduces this week’s work.

Video content is not available in this format.

By the end of this week you should be able to:

● appreciate contrasting views on ‘curing’ autism


● understand what is meant by ‘intervention’
● appreciate the importance of interventions being evidence-based
● understand broad principles for evaluating interventions
● be familiar with key interventions and recent developments in the field.

1 Perspectives on recovery and help


The first priority for many parents of an autistic child or adult is to secure help and support.
Some parents, and clinicians too, go further, seeing autism as something to be ‘cured’ or
eradicated. An objection to this idea, from a neurodiversity perspective, is that autism
should be seen as a difference, not as an illness or disability which needs ‘curing’. Even if
aiming for a cure is seen as acceptable by some, since autistic children’s communication,
social and thinking skills start to diverge from the typical developmental pathway at an
early stage, reversing these changes poses a big challenge.

98 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 5: Addressing challenges: approaches to intervention
1 Perspectives on recovery and help

1.1 Optimal outcomes


Some recent research does provide provisional evidence that a small proportion of
autistic children achieve an ‘optimal outcome’ in adulthood (Fein et al., 2013). This
means that their symptoms have modified to such an extent that they no longer meet the
diagnostic criteria for autism, though they may retain subtle differences in thinking style.
The research suggests that such an outcome is most likely following early intensive
treatment or intervention. However, it is not yet clear what characteristics distinguish
those who achieve an optimal outcome from those who do not. You will read more about
this in Week 7.
Of course, the idea of an optimal outcome could also be seen as disrespectful to autistic
people – who is it that decides what an ‘optimal’ outcome is? Alex, whom you have met in
earlier weeks, has overcome many challenges while growing up, and leads a full life,
studying, working and driving his own car. In this clip, he reflects on strengths that are part
of his identity as an autistic person.

Video content is not available in this format.

1.2 Contrasting views on acceptance


More able autistic individuals may be more likely to oppose the idea that the differences
which make them who they are, need to be removed. And some may even reject the need
for any form of help to address their challenges. Parents of more severely affected
children, who face profound challenges and difficulties for themselves and their family, are
more likely to seek help, and perhaps even to pursue recovery. However, this
differentiation is not always correct. The journalist Charlotte Moore, who has two children
with a severe form of autism, wrote:

I hope I was never looking for a cure; now, I’m sure I’m not. I want Sam to stop
scattering his food and biting his hands, but I don’t dream of a neurotypical Sam
with the usual emotional and intellectual range of a boy of 11, because no such
Sam could possibly exist.
(Moore, 2003)

Charlotte prefers to accept her sons’ autism as a different way of being, while
acknowledging that there are challenges which it would be desirable to overcome.
Arabella, mother of Iris Grace, expresses her views in this clip.

Video content is not available in this format.

99 of 183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


Week 5: Addressing challenges: approaches to intervention
2 Interventions

2 Interventions
Even without help, an individual’s profile of skills and behaviours is likely to change over
time, and in adulthood some difficulties may become less severe or apparent. However,
key symptoms and difficulties typically persist and may even become more marked, with
an enduring impact on the person’s lifestyle and well-being. It is generally believed that
identifying autism and intervening as early as possible is most likely to have a beneficial
effect (Howlin et al., 2009). An intervention is a technique or procedure to support and
help children or adults with autism to engage with others and to thrive, for instance by
helping a non-verbal person to communicate, or a highly anxious person to reduce their
anxiety. While this means adapting to the neurotypical world, it is equally important that
the neurotypical world becomes more accepting and tolerant of autistic behaviour, and
better adapted to autistic needs. Organisations like the National Autistic Society (NAS)
are active in promoting this goal. You will read more in Week 8.
A range of interventions has been developed, targeted at different problems and different
groups. Many of these interventions are for children, especially those with low-functioning
autism including marked language and intellectual disabilities, and are designed for use in
home and educational settings. Although individuals with high-functioning autism or
Asperger syndrome have better language and intellectual skills, they may face challenges
in interacting, behaving flexibly, and in other everyday skills, which also call for support
throughout life. Some interventions lend themselves well to supporting these more able
groups, and also the needs of adults.

2.1 The importance of evidence for interventions


Before they are tried out, interventions should be evaluated in terms of their rationale,
safety and likely chances of success. Careful evaluation of evidence is fundamental to
the framework of sound clinical practice known as evidence-based practice. Whether
altered behaviour or improved skills learned through an intervention will readily
generalise i.e. carry over to contexts different from those where they were acquired, and
whether they will apply to others with autism too, are further important considerations.
Before looking at what goes into an evaluation, we will briefly consider the risks of
intervening without adequate rationale or evidence.

2.2 Intervening without evidence


From time to time, interventions for autism are proposed with an odd or unconvincing
rationale, little or no evidence, and often with grossly exaggerated claims for success.
Parents of autistic children may be highly vulnerable to claims for a ‘cure’, ‘recovery’ or
dramatic alleviation of symptoms, and understandably, they also feel empowered by
doing anything they can to help their child, even if only on the principle of ‘try anything if it
might help’.
One such approach, known as packing therapy, has been promoted in France, where
some ideas about autism still remain far from those that are widely accepted elsewhere.
The child is first wrapped in towels soaked with cold water, and then with blankets to warm
the body. The claim is that this develops consciousness of bodily limits, which, according

100 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183
Week 5: Addressing challenges: approaches to intervention
2 Interventions

to proponents of the therapy, is lacking in autism. There has been widespread


condemnation of this ill-founded practice, which has been described as ‘a form of child
abuse and a gross violation of basic human rights’ (Research Autism, 2018).
Another ill-conceived approach assumes that autism is caused by excessive levels of
toxins such as mercury and lead in the blood. The ‘therapy’, known as chelation, involves
administering substances to eliminate the ‘excess toxic substances’ from the blood-
stream. However, chelation has proved to have extremely harmful side effects and in
2005, an autistic child who was being treated in this way died of a cardiac arrest. In 2013,
the National Institute for Health and Care Excellence (NICE) published the following
statement:

Do not use [chelation] to manage autism in any context in children and young
people
(National Institute for Health and Care Excellence, 2013)

These two techniques provide just two examples of highly dubious interventions. Imagine
you had a child with diabetes, and someone offered you a previously unknown medication
which they claimed provided a highly effective cure. Before trying out such medication,
you would want to know how and why the substance was supposed to work, you would
need evidence that the substance actually worked, and assurance that it did not have
harmful or dangerous side effects. You would also want to know more about the person
promoting the cure, such as whether they had a financial interest in it. The importance of
addressing questions like these applies equally to autism as to treatments for medical
ailments. Interventions for which the rationale is unclear, and which lack proper evaluation
or ethical screening are quite rightly avoided or treated with great caution.

101 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183
Week 5: Addressing challenges: approaches to intervention
3 Establishing the evidence base for interventions

3 Establishing the evidence base for


interventions
Deciding whether an intervention works might seem like a relatively straightforward
process of trying the approach with one or more autistic participants, to see if it helps to
address particular challenges or difficulties, or promotes ‘positive’ aspects of behaviour.
However, evaluation is never this simple or straightforward.

3.1 Principles of evaluation

Activity 1 Reflecting on the principles


Allow about 10 minutes

Imagine that you are a practitioner considering a new intervention developed by a


team of researchers and practitioners, who claim successful evaluation of their
procedure. What questions would you want to be sure had been addressed? Drawing
on the earlier discussion in Section 2, note three or four key points.

Provide your answer...

102 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183
Week 5: Addressing challenges: approaches to intervention
3 Establishing the evidence base for interventions

Answer
Here is a list of major considerations:
Rationale
Does the suggested intervention draw upon theory and/or previous research about
autism which helps to explain how and why it might work?
Focus
What specific problems, behaviours or skills is the intervention supposed to target?
(e.g. if communication, which aspects?)
Ethics
Does the suggested intervention avoid unpleasant or dangerous side effects? Does it
safeguard the well-being of participants?
Were the researchers who conducted the evaluation independent of the group
involved in developing and promoting it?
Method
Participants: has the procedure been tested with an adequately sized group of
participants, all with a verified autism diagnosis?
Have specific procedures for the intervention been clearly defined, and rigorously
followed during the study?
Criteria for ‘success’: what measures or findings indicate that targeted behaviour has
been reduced or enhanced?
Outcomes
Do any changes which come about actually result from the procedure? (They could
occur by chance or due to some other factor.)
Would the intervention generalise, that is be effective long-term and if administered in
‘real-life’ settings?

There has been increasing emphasis in recent years on the need for interventions to be
fully evaluated, to avoid harm, and to comply with the principles of evidence-based
practice. In an ideal situation, evaluation should commence with relatively small-scale
informal tests, and build up to more formal and wide-ranging evaluation, as outlined next.

3.2 Pilot and small-scale studies


The initial stage of evaluation is an exploratory or pilot study in which the proposed
intervention is tried with just a few autistic individuals. Of course, full ethical safeguards
must be ensured, and the participants (or parents on participants’ behalf) must give their
consent to participation.
If a pilot study suggests a positive effect, a more formal, small-scale evaluation can be
carried out. Again this may involve just a small group of participants, who are observed for
a period before the intervention, while it is carried out, and for a period of time afterwards.
A common procedure for this is a multiple-baseline study. Each participant is observed
for a period before the intervention to establish a baseline for the behaviour to be targeted
by the intervention e.g. how often the child communicates verbally. The length of the
observation period is varied such that the intervention start date is staggered across
participants. This makes it easier to ascertain that any change, such as more verbal
communications, is due to the intervention itself. If all children started the intervention

103 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183
Week 5: Addressing challenges: approaches to intervention
3 Establishing the evidence base for interventions

simultaneously, they might all experience another new factor (e.g. a change of teacher)
which could be causing any changed behaviour.

3.3 Controlled studies and randomised control trials


A controlled study is a more rigorous and formal evaluation of an intervention similar to a
research experiment (see Week 1). The study typically involves two groups of participants
on the autism spectrum, both larger than those used in pilot and small-scale evaluations.
One group (the experimental or intervention group) receives the intervention, and the
other (the control group) does not, or they receive another intervention already known to
have some effect. The latter procedure is known as ‘treatment as usual’.
Ideally the two participant groups in the study should be matched on criteria such as age,
IQ or severity of symptoms, before the intervention begins. After the intervention period,
the two groups are compared to see if there are any differences. This comparison requires
a specific measure of the skills or behaviours that the intervention is designed to target,
known as the outcome measure.

Activity 2 Principles of controlled evaluation


Allow about 5 minutes

In a controlled study, one possible way to evaluate the effect of the intervention is for
the control group to receive no intervention at all. However, the ‘treatment as usual’
procedure just outlined is more usual. Suggest one or more reasons for this.

Provide your answer...

Answer
Offering an alternative ‘treatment as usual’:

● reduces the likelihood that parents, or control participants themselves, will lack
motivation to be involved. This could affect recruitment, and/or the outcomes in
the control group
● helps to address ethical issues: there is a good chance that everyone will gain
some benefit from the study, whether in the new intervention or in the ‘treatment
as usual’ group
● enables the new intervention to be compared with existing interventions, to see if
it is more or less effective.

Another ideal procedure for a controlled study is that participants are assigned randomly
to treatment and control groups – much like drawing numbers out of a hat. This avoids
factors which might bias the outcomes, for instance, that the participants in the
intervention group are more high-functioning than those in the control group. The term
randomised control trial (RCT) refers to a full-scale controlled evaluation involving
random allocation to intervention and ‘treatment as usual’ groups, and a relatively large
number of participants. RCTs are widely used to evaluate new medical interventions, and
are also considered the ‘gold standard’ in the autism field. But to date, few evaluations of

104 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183
Week 5: Addressing challenges: approaches to intervention
3 Establishing the evidence base for interventions

autism interventions have been evaluated as far as the RCT stage. We will consider why
next.

3.4 Problems in evaluating autism interventions


Taking a new intervention through a rigorous series of evaluations is usually lengthy and
costly – to borrow another example from medicine, think of how long a malaria vaccination
has been in development. In the knowledge that, in autism, intervening early in
development is best, parents, teachers and even clinicians may feel that they cannot wait
for the full outcomes of clinical trials. They may therefore resort to procedures known to be
broadly helpful and not actively harmful. Unfortunately, some parents may also feel driven
to try extremely risky procedures.
Another problem is the heterogeneity of autism. It may genuinely be the case that an
intervention works for some individuals but not for others. In a formal control study or RCT,
where relatively large participant groups are compared, there is the risk that benefits
experienced by a small subset of the participants receiving the intervention are ‘averaged
out’, such that the intervention does not appear to be beneficial overall.
As you will see, some well-established treatments depend crucially on adapting the goals
and procedures to the needs of the individual child, making it very difficult to specify
standard group procedures and outcome measures as required for an RCT.

3.5 The Research Autism database


A UK information service, Research Autism, has a database offering accessible and
impartial guidance on as many as possible of the known interventions for autism,
including a description of each approach, and a grading across the range from strongly
positive to a warning that the intervention could be harmful (Research Autism, 2018). The
evaluations, made by experts commissioned by Research Autism, are a valuable and
authoritative source, whether for researchers and practitioners seeking a quick update, or
for parents and individuals with autism, who may not have access to the full academic
literature concerning evidence for interventions.

Activity 3 Getting to know the Research Autism website


Allow about 10 minutes

Use the following link to access the list of interventions which have been evaluated by
Research Autism. Have a look at the description and evaluation of the different
interventions and locate ones which have these symbols next to them:

105 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183
Week 5: Addressing challenges: approaches to intervention
3 Establishing the evidence base for interventions

Hover over the symbols on the website and note down what they mean. Here's the
link:
Our Evaluations of Autism Interventions, Treatments and Therapies

Answer
Two ticks next to an intervention indicates that there is ‘strong positive evidence’ to
suggest the intervention is effective. If you click on the specific intervention, you will
find out more about that intervention and its benefits.
A question mark means that the evidence for the intervention is mixed or insufficient.
An exclamation mark indicates that the intervention risks physical and/or psychological
harm to people who experience it. Research Autism advises that the procedure should
not be used.

The next sections outline some well-known and widely used interventions for autism,
followed by some recent developments in the intervention field. Note that while none of
these interventions is considered risky in application, the level of evidence varies, and at
least one of these approaches attracts widely polarised views among practitioners. Most
of these interventions are included in the Research Autism evaluation list, and you will find
reports on the others elsewhere on the Research Autism website.

106 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183
Week 5: Addressing challenges: approaches to intervention
4 TEACCH

4 TEACCH
TEACCH is the acronym for the Treatment and Education of Autistic and Related
Communication Handicapped Children. The approach was introduced in the 1970s when
the term ‘handicapped’ was not deemed unacceptable as it is now. TEACCH has been
effectively used in many countries since then and is recommended by the NAS and the
Autism Society of America (ASA). It is a ‘whole person’ approach in that it aims to support
many aspects of functioning, including learning, behaviour, social and communication
needs, and may be used across different settings (at home, school, in respite services
and other locations) as well as across the person's lifespan. It can also be successfully
adapted for more intellectually able individuals, and used in adult settings.

4.1 Principles of TEACCH


The TEACCH framework aims to maximise an individual's strengths, drawing on how
autistic people tend to think and to engage with the world. Here are some key principles:

● emphasis on structure, including physical structure of the person’s environment and


clearly defined routines
● harnessing skills; for instance visual skills and good memory for factual information
mean that a visual schedule indicating what activities will happen, where and when,
is likely to be an effective tool
● evaluation of individual support needs on a regular basis, including use of other
interventions where necessary to complement the approach
● empowering parents by encouraging their full participation
● emphasis on developing independence, and generalising from learning experiences
in order to master a range of everyday situations.

107 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183
Week 5: Addressing challenges: approaches to intervention
4 TEACCH

Figure 1 Clear structure and visual information provide the basis for a planner to help a
child with their day at school.

4.2 Evidence base for TEACCH


TEACCH has a clear and appropriate rationale, in that it addresses the characteristic
need for structure and routine in autism. Research Autism reports a ‘small amount of high
quality research evidence for TEACCH’, pointing to an impact on social communication,
cognitive and motor skills. However, to date there have been no RCTs or large-scale
evaluations. The fact that it is specifically tailored to the challenges experienced by an
individual child or adult means that it is not easy to identify outcome measures appropriate
for all participants in a study, nor to set up control groups. It is likely that outcomes will be
affected by the skill and expertise of the professionals involved. There is also evidence
that when or if TEACCH is withdrawn, an autistic person may regress in their behaviour
and anxiety levels, or be unable to generalise what they have learned to new contexts.

108 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183
Week 5: Addressing challenges: approaches to intervention
5 The behavioural approach to intervention

5 The behavioural approach to


intervention
A quite different approach to intervention originated in the work of Ivor Lovaas, described
in Week 1. He in turn drew on the work of mid-twentieth century psychologists such as B.
F. Skinner, who proposed that in animals, including humans, the repertoire of behaviour
can be modified and new behaviours learned, using operant conditioning. Operant
conditioning assumes that active behaviour 'operates' on the environment, with
consequences that modify whether the behaviour is repeated in the future. If the
consequences are rewarding, the behaviour is more likely to be repeated – known as
reinforcement.

5.1 Applied behavioural analysis


Lovaas’ approach evolved to become Applied Behavioural Analysis (ABA), an
intervention for autism in which operant conditioning principles are used to reinforce
‘desirable’ behaviours (e.g. more speech and communication) and reduce ‘undesirable’
behaviours (e.g. aggressive or repetitive behaviours that may be injurious or considered
antisocial, such as head banging, destruction of objects or taking clothes off in public).
In ABA the child’s behaviour is analysed into components that are tackled one at a time. If
the target behaviour is one that it is identified for the child to learn, for instance uttering a
word or phrase, the reinforcement needs to be a positive outcome for the child, such as a
favourite snack or some non-edible reward. If the behaviour is something that it is thought
should be discouraged, for instance a tantrum, then withholding reinforcement (e.g. by
ignoring the behaviour, saying ‘no’ loudly and firmly, removing the child from the context or
reinforcing alternative behaviour) should eventually result in the behaviour disappearing.
The first stages of ABA may focus on teaching self-help and receptive language skills
(i.e. language understanding), non-verbal and verbal imitation, and the foundations of
appropriate play. The second stage emphasises the teaching of expressive language and
interactive play with peers. Advanced stages involve the learning of early academic tasks,
socialisation skills, cause and effect relationships, and learning by observation. The
intervention might seem particularly relevant for young children who are lacking basic
communication skills and everyday capabilities. However, children with higher initial
intellectual ability and less pronounced autism are also said to make good progress,
especially following early behavioural intervention. ABA is also used with some autistic
adults.
If at all possible parents are trained to carry out ABA at home, albeit supported by trained
therapists and consultants. Classic ABA approaches stipulate that 40 hours per week
should be spent on ‘shaping’ the child, making it expensive for parents or local service
providers to employ trainers. The commitment for parents is also substantial.

5.2 Evaluations and views of ABA


ABA has been difficult to formally evaluate for two reasons. Firstly, like TEACCH, it is a
highly individualised approach, with outcomes that are tailored to the behaviour of an

109 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183
Week 5: Addressing challenges: approaches to intervention
5 The behavioural approach to intervention

individual child. Secondly, ABA is no longer a single unified approach, but rather a set of
varying procedures, some of which have also been incorporated into other types of
intervention.
Attitudes to ABA among families and professionals are sharply divided. Proponents of the
approach argue that it is one of the few really effective treatments, which can make
dramatic improvements including the potential to develop language skills in non-verbal
children. An early intervention based on ABA principles, known as Early Intensive
Behavioural Intervention (EIBI), has been evaluated as particularly effective (Peters-
Scheffer et al., 2011). One criticism of ABA claims that it is a simplistic tool, which changes
‘surface’ behaviour rather than underlying thought processes. The fact that parents and
therapists decide what behaviour should be shaped has led to the further criticism that
ABA is ‘adult-directed’, taking away the child’s autonomy, choice and dignity (Devita-
Raeburn, 2016). ABA also requires a level of dedication and expense that makes it
inaccessible to some families.
Bear these contrasting viewpoints in mind while watching this clip illustrating how a young
boy called Joe is helped with his communication skills by therapists using Applied
Behavioural Analysis. Watch this clip now.

Video content is not available in this format.

110 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183
Week 5: Addressing challenges: approaches to intervention
6 Naturalistic interventions

6 Naturalistic interventions
In contrast to how adults identify the ‘desirable’ behaviour within ABA, approaches have
been developed which build upon the child’s own interests and preferences. The role of
the teacher or therapist is to prompt or encourage natural communicative behaviours
which are initiated by the child. These approaches are known as naturalistic
interventions, and they are all, to a greater or lesser degree, child-centred.

6.1 Picture Exchange Communication System


(PECS)
The best known and most widely used naturalistic intervention is the Picture Exchange
Communication System (PECS).This was devised in the 1990s with the aim of teaching
functional communication skills, especially to autistic children with little or no language.
Each child is individually assessed to identify objects and activities that he or she finds
rewarding, and a booklet or board of small pictures is compiled matching the child's
preferences.
The child is then guided through different stages towards the goal of making spontaneous
requests for these items, one adult acting as a communication partner and a second adult
as the child's physical prompter. As the child reaches for a desired object, the physical
prompter then physically guides the child to pick up a picture of the object and release it
into the communication partner’s hand. The physical prompter gradually reduces the
prompting as the child becomes more independent in selecting pictures of what he or she
wants, and exchanging them for the object itself.
Once the child is using pictures in spontaneous communicative exchanges, PECS
intervention aims to strengthen this spontaneity and to enhance the child’s ability to
distinguish between pictures, increasing the number available to them, firstly from a board
and then from a folder. The child is also encouraged to generalise his/her new-found
communication skills to different settings and communication partners, to produce more
complex communications, and eventually make comments about things they see rather
than just requesting things they want.
Watch the following video clip which illustrates the use of PECS in classrooms Queensmill
School in Hammersmith. Notice that these children have reached the stage of combining
pictorial symbols with phrases such as 'I want', and sometimes saying the sentences out
loud. Look carefully at how the communication partners are using PECS in these clips,
and keep this in mind for Activity 4, coming up in the next section.

Video content is not available in this format.

6.2 Evaluations of PECS


PECS is one of the few autism-specific interventions to have been evaluated through all of
the stages described earlier, including some RCTs. For younger children with little
functional speech there are positive findings, especially increased social communication.

111 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183
Week 5: Addressing challenges: approaches to intervention
6 Naturalistic interventions

PECS is widely used, although further research is needed to establish which children on
the spectrum are most likely to gain maximum benefit from using it. Unlike ABA, PECS is
easy and cost-effective to administer. However, Research Autism stresses the importance
of training to ensure correct implementation.

Activity 4 PECS in action


Allow about 3 minutes

In the clip you just watched, was there anything that didn't match the earlier decription
of how children are taught to use PECS?

Provide your answer...

Discussion
The teachers were serving as communication partners, but they were also prompting
children who already had PECS skills. This may have been detrimental to developing
the children's spontaneity. The challenge is to manage any child-specific adaptation of
an intervention like PECS without losing the core procedures which make it effective.

112 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183
Week 5: Addressing challenges: approaches to intervention
7 Recent developments in intervention

7 Recent developments in intervention


In the last few years, new approaches to intervention have emerged, some building on the
classic approaches already described, and others completely novel. A small selection is
featured here.

7.1 Pre-school Autism Communication Therapy


(PACT)
The PACT approach trains parents to work on enhancing the communication and
language development skills of their own children, commencing as early as possible after
diagnosis (Green et al., 2010). Parents view video footage of their interactions with their
child and are taught to adapt their own communications in order to respond with enhanced
sensitivity and responsiveness, for instance using eye gaze, sharing, showing and giving
to increase shared attention. Parents learn to tailor their language use to the child’s level
of understanding and to facilitate child communication and participation, for instance
through sequences of actions, repeated verbal scripts and the use of elaborations,
pauses, and playful teasing.

7.2 Evaluations
In 2010, an RCT (also known as PACT, the Pre-school Autism Communication Trial)
reported that children whose parents received the training were using words and gestures
more often than those in the control group. In 2017, a ‘follow-up’ study showed that these
benefits had persisted. Children also showed enhanced social skills and fewer repetitive
behaviours.
The approach is attracting considerable interest because of the substantial number of
children involved in the RCT, and the retention of positive outcomes over the longer term.
The successful use of parents as therapists also has the benefit of being naturalistic and
cost-effective, making the intervention a good choice for low and middle income countries.
A culturally appropriate version of PACT is being trialled in India (Divan, 2017). Given the
dearth of available specialists in India, the approach was adapted to enable lay people to
train parents. Though viewing videos of themselves was challenging for some parents,
they came to value the guidance. One mother commented that the approach was ‘making
a change in my child, but also making a change in me’ (Divan, 2017). RCTs of the Indian
initiative are underway.

7.3 Assistive technology


There is rapid growth in assistive technology, the use of technology, and especially IT, to
help autistic people. This is a massive field, of which only a brief summary can be given
here. Smartphones, tablets and computers have all been harnessed to help autistic
people deal with problems of daily living, learn social and communicative skills, and in
other ways. One important strand is the development of smartphone ‘apps’ designed to
help autistic people with daily living skills. For instance, an able young person may

113 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183
Week 5: Addressing challenges: approaches to intervention
7 Recent developments in intervention

become anxious and confused if the bus they always catch to school is late or fails to
come. A smartphone app could offer practical actions for such a situation, for example:
wait 20 minutes and then call parents at home; check the bus timetable for other possible
buses. In video modelling a child or adult learns a new behaviour, such as how to greet a
friend, by watching it being modelled by someone on a computer screen. Some more
traditional approaches, such as the visual schedule shown in Fig. 1, are now available in
computerised versions. Finally, researchers have started investigating the use of robots to
teach new skills, on the view that robots provide a particularly accessible medium for an
autistic person to learn from. You will find more information about these approaches on
the Research Autism website.

7.4 Animal-assisted interventions


There have been some enthusiastic media reports and documentaries about animal-
assisted interventions, for instance horseback riding or having a pet in the family home.
Advocates claim that contact with animals reduces stress, helping children to focus their
attention and communicate better, promoting learning and increased sociability. Arabella,
mother of Iris Grace, reports that acquiring a Maine Coon cat, Thula, played a remarkable
role in helping Iris to overcome hypersensitivities and anxiety, such as a sensory aversion
to certain clothes and fear of water. In this clip, Arabella talks about how Iris changed and
developed when Thula joined the family.

Video content is not available in this format.

7.5 Evaluations of animal-assisted interventions


A recent scientific overview of animal-based interventions in autism, while recognising
that they may be beneficial for some children, concluded that the evidence is as yet
unclear. Interpretation of studies is complicated by factors such as small participant
numbers, and variations in the animals, procedures, duration of therapy and outcome
measures (O’Haire, 2013).

Activity 5 Considering animal-assisted interventions


Allow about 15 minutes

Draw on the interview with Arabella and the Research Autism website to note two
arguments in favour of animal-based intervention and two against. You should look up
Research Autism entries for equine and dolphin therapies and assistance dogs.

For:
1.
2.
Against:
1.
2.

114 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183
Week 5: Addressing challenges: approaches to intervention
7 Recent developments in intervention

Answer
Points in favour:

1. Arabella suggests that contact with a cat helped Iris to overcome some sensory
problems and other anxieties e.g. about travel.
2. The good relationship between Thula and Iris provided important respite for her
parents.

Points against:

1. Some animal-based approaches such as swimming with dolphins could be


harmful for the animal and harmful or frightening for the child.
2. Some improvements attributed to an animal like Thula may have taken place
anyway. Since supportive evidence is so far sparse, the expense and effort of
animal-based intervention may not be justified.

115 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183
Week 5: Addressing challenges: approaches to intervention
8 This week’s quiz

8 This week’s quiz


Check what you’ve learned this week by taking the end-of-week quiz.
Week 5 quiz
Open the quiz in a new window or tab then return here once you’ve finished it.

116 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183
Week 5: Addressing challenges: approaches to intervention
9 Summary

9 Summary
One message from this week is that no ‘one size fits all’, due to the diverse nature of
autism. Increasingly psychologists are advocating a combination of methods, individually
tailored to the person’s skills and needs. While many interventions do require training or
specialist therapists, some adjustments can be implemented easily by families, schools or
workplaces. For instance: removing, as far as possible, sources of sensory distress, such
as fluorescent lights or noisy air conditioning; providing a secure place for when the
person feels overwhelmed, or a ‘traffic light’ card system so that, at times when they feel
unable to speak, they can signal positive or negative feelings with a green, amber or red
card.
This clip from Arabella describes some of the strategies she has used to help Iris.

Video content is not available in this format.

You should now be able to:

● appreciate contrasting views on ‘curing’ autism


● understand what is meant by ‘intervention’
● appreciate the importance of interventions being evidence-based
● understand broad principles for evaluating interventions
● be familiar with key interventions and recent developments in the field.

Now you can go to Week 6.

117 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183
Week 5: Addressing challenges: approaches to intervention
9 Summary

118 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183
Week 6: Family life and education
Introduction

Week 6: Family life and


education
Introduction
This week considers the experiences and challenges of family life when one or more
family members is autistic. The discussion starts with autistic children, considering initial
experiences within the family, as well as the changes that occur as children grow and
develop. The focus then moves to support for families and options for education.
However, as you will see, access to these options is not evenly distributed across all
families and groups. And beyond countries such as the UK, especially in low and middle
income countries (LMICs), families may struggle to get any support or education for their
child.
Now watch the video in which Dr Ilona Roth introduces this week’s work.

Video content is not available in this format.

By the end of this week you should be able to:

● appreciate different ways in which family life may be affected when one or more
family members is autistic
● understand how challenges for the family and for the individual with autism change
over the course of development
● be aware of some of the sources of support available to families in the UK
● appreciate the psychological, academic and social challenges posed by education,
and the main choices available in terms of schooling
● understand the rationale for different educational choices, including home education.

1 Family life
All members of a family are likely to be affected when a child receives a diagnosis of an
autism spectrum condition. Of course, families may have concerns even before diagnosis,
and may have developed ways of coping with a child whose behaviour does not fit typical
expectations. On diagnosis, close family members are typically affected the most,
because they engage with their autistic family member on a daily basis.
Parents may experience a range of emotions, including initial grief that the child is not
neurotypical, but perspectives change. You may recall the video clip from Week 3: after

119 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183
Week 6: Family life and education
1 Family life

the shock and grief of discovering that his son Zack was autistic, Mark realised that Zack
was just the same person as he had been before diagnosis. Another parent describes her
primary feeling as relief. Her son Louis was finally diagnosed with Asperger syndrome,
ADHD and learning difficulties at the age of 6, after years in which Melanie had struggled
to have her concerns taken seriously:

My feelings were of total relief … after years of being told he was perfectly
normal and behavioural management tactics would solve it. He was described
as a 'whirling dervish' by the local paediatrician! Impossible to control his
behaviour in any way. Diagnosis meant I could get a Statement at school.
Phew! But I had to fight for it.
(Melanie, 2017, pers. comm.)

Siblings may also experience a variety of feelings, including resentment, embarrassment


and guilt. However, research shows that siblings also come to see autism in the family as
‘normal’, developing close relationships with their autistic sibling(s) (Preece, 2014).
Members of the wider family such as grandparents may be a source of support, but also
conflict (Hillman, 2007). For instance, they may have difficulty accepting the diagnosis,
insisting that their grandchild will grow out of their difficulties. Some grandparents may
even believe that the parents are responsible for their child’s ‘difficult’ behaviour. In
Melanie’s case, her father’s attitudes to Louis evolved from traditional ideas about ‘dealing
with bad behaviour’ to providing support:

Grandpa (my Dad and the only grandparent on my side) took a while to
understand that a good hiding wasn't going to work and finally rose to the
challenge – [he was] enormously supportive to me. Most others in the family
accepted it well. At last! An explanation!
(Melanie, 2017, pers. comm.)

1.1 Sources of stress in families


Parents are likely to experience substantial stress. For instance, they may have to
manage difficult behaviour, such as their child’s anger outbursts, self-injury or insistence
on wearing the same clothes every day. Sometimes the desperation to find immediate
solutions for young children’s behaviour may not have the best long-term consequences,
as Charlotte Moore, mother of George and Sam, freely admits:

To start with the basics – food. It’s taken 20 years to get George and Sam to eat
anything like a normal diet. Most autists get stuck on a tiny range of trusted
foods – I wish I’d kept all junk at bay, so that the boys would have had to obsess
over, say, chicken, brown rice and carrots rather than chocolate and HP sauce
(George) or crisps and icing (Sam). I used ‘treat’ food to help them cope with
stressful situations, such as travelling or going to a party. Once they knew such
treats existed they held out for them. Now, I would avoid such situations.
(Moore, 2012)

The challenges faced by families vary depending on the age of the autistic child and their
cognitive skills, such as whether they use spoken language. A child who has difficulty

120 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183
Week 6: Family life and education
1 Family life

communicating may appear angry towards others or seek isolation, when the underlying
problem is sensory overload, or frustration at not understanding what is going on around
them.
Some children are hyperactive and impulsive. Other family members may have to adapt
to not having spontaneous activities because of the autistic person’s need for structure
and sameness.

We started to avoid group activities as G couldn’t join in, not understanding how
to engage and interact. Swimming, music, even group speech therapy became
inaccessible. I will never forget the speech therapist leading the group, taking
me to one side and gently suggesting that G wasn’t benefitting and was
disruptive to the group. I left the room and we went to feed the ducks, which
ended up with him eating bread and me sobbing through my sense of
abandonment, hopelessness and isolation.
(N, 2017, pers. comm.)

Lack of sleep is also a significant issue. Children with autism often have disrupted sleep
patterns, and inevitably this affects parents too. Jacqui Jackson is a single parent of seven
sons and daughters, several of whom are on the autism spectrum. In 2006, when her
family was still very young, Jacqui commented that she often went for several nights
without sleep, and only took cat-naps on other nights. She was trying to write her PhD
thesis at the time.

Physically it does take its toll … It gets you down and it affects how you work.
(Elliott, 2006)

1.2 Multiplex families


Given the strong genetic influences in autism, it is not unusual to find families like
Charlotte Moore’s or Jacqui Jackson’s, where more than one offspring has an ASC
diagnosis. These are known as multiplex families.
Sometimes, when a child is being diagnosed, a parent, or even a grandparent, realises
that they too are ‘on the spectrum’. You may recall from Week 3 that John Peters received
a diagnosis at the same time as his grandsons were being assessed, which helped him to
find an explanation for the problems he had struggled with all his life.
Knowing that there is more than one person with autism can make family life less difficult,
as the whole family can more easily adjust to living in an ‘autism-friendly’ way. Jacqui
Jackson comments on the loyalty that her three teenage daughters show to her sons:

The boys are their yardstick. If someone can't accept them, then that person's
not worth bothering with. Rachel had a boyfriend who called them ‘spackers’
and she dumped him pretty quick. The lad she’s seeing at the moment loves
Ben, in fact they fall out over him: ‘You come to see Ben, not me!’
(Moore, 2003)

121 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183
Week 6: Family life and education
1 Family life

1.3 Adolescence
Adolescence often presents new challenges for young people with autism and their
families. Hormonal changes can be difficult for any teenager, but especially so for the
young person with autism, who may struggle to understand their changing body and
emotions. They may have difficulties understanding their own sexual feelings, and
misunderstand social norms concerning the development of friendships and romantic
relationships. Neurotypical teenagers can be quite ‘cliqueish’, excluding anyone who
doesn’t conform to their way of doing things. Consequently, this is a life-stage when
experiences of rejection, isolation or bullying are likely to be particularly prevalent, and is
possibly why young autistic people are especially prone to mental health problems such
as depression and anxiety (Picci and Scherf, 2014).
Picci and Scherf argue that the problems facing the autistic adolescent concern more than
the challenges of social relationships. They suggest that around 30 per cent of autistic
adolescents show an overall deterioration in their level of functioning in adolescence, and
attribute this to a ‘second hit’ of difficulties affecting the brain and cognitive processes,
accentuating weak central coherence and executive function problems (see Week 4).
One consequence is that young people with autism often have difficulty with autonomy –
that is, making decisions and performing everyday tasks. Despite these challenges, Claire
Bachman, an American student with Asperger syndrome, is living independently from her
parents and attending college:

As I transition to adulthood, I find it particularly challenging to do things such as


daily chores on my own. I struggle to remember to take care of myself by
bathing, brushing my teeth and the like. I was so used to my parents either
doing things for me or reminding me to do them. Getting myself prepared for
the day, looking for what to wear to school or to meetings, activities or events
has been challenging. I am often not sure which outfit is appropriate,
professional or acceptable.
(Bachman, 2016)

1.4 Adulthood
A lack of autonomy is especially prevalent in autism compared with other developmental
conditions such as Down syndrome (Esbensen et al., 2010). While more profoundly
affected adults may gain a residential placement, many autistic adults continue to live with
their parents.
Since adult-focused services are very sparsely available, parents and/or other family
members typically find themselves with multiple responsibilities, ranging from everyday
care to negotiating benefits and seeking employment opportunities for their autistic family
member. Even the most able autistic adult is likely to struggle with tasks such as form-
filling. A family will need to deal with such administrative tasks, as well as acting as
advocate in relation to officialdom. Parents may find themselves torn between caring for
their own ageing parents and their adult child. As time passes, parents themselves are
likely to become less able to care. This may then impact on siblings, who by this time may
have children of their own.
You will learn more about the situation of autistic adults in Week 7.

122 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183
Week 6: Family life and education
1 Family life

1.5 Family resilience


Families with one or more autistic members have often been noted as developing
resilience, becoming stronger and more resourceful. This comes from flexibility in
accommodating the autistic person’s needs and from good communication with each
other. While every family’s journey with autism is different, they have in common learning
to appreciate the smallest of achievements and developing greater awareness of, and
compassion towards, individual differences.

Life has never been the same since Matthew made his appearance in 2000.
Everyday activities that people take for granted, are a challenge in my house …
Despite the pressure put onto us as a family, this has now become a way of life
and we know no different. We feel that ignorance lies with other people and
until they re-educate themselves into accepting diversity then life for us will
remain quite a challenge! Life with Matthew is wonderful and marvellous!
Zöe Wood

Figure 1 Zoe Wood's son Matthew.

123 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183
Week 6: Family life and education
2 Support for families

2 Support for families


In the UK, support from the NHS and social services tends to focus on diagnosis and the
needs of the autistic child, rather than on the family as a whole. The sparse family support
that is available tends to be for parents, such as respite breaks for those with a severely
affected child.
However, there are many volunteer-run support groups, some of them provided by people
who themselves have an autistic child or adult in the family. Additionally, there are groups
on social media and email/online bulletin boards that people can subscribe to.

2.1 NAS services


The National Autistic Society (NAS) has assembled a directory of support services for
families with an autistic member. It includes a wide range of service providers as well as
services offered by the NAS itself, two of which are outlined below.

EarlyBird
Besides the many services that the NAS offers to autistic people, it also offers some
excellent services for parents and families. Parents of younger children (up to age 8) may
be able to access an EarlyBird or EarlyBird Plus support programme.
EarlyBird is a three-month programme that provides group training sessions for parents
with individual home visits. Video feedback is used to help parents apply what they learn
as they work with their child in the familiar home setting.

Figure 2 Photos of an EarlyBird session, each featuring parent and child with an EarlyBird
trainer who is recording the session.
EarlyBird programmes are run by trained and licensed professionals with the aim of:

● supporting parents in the period between diagnosis and school placement


● empowering parents to facilitate their child’s communication skills
● supporting parents to deal resourcefully and positively with their child’s behaviour,
including more challenging aspects.

EarlyBird Plus is an enhanced form of the service that invites a professional who works
with the child (such as a teacher at school) to attend the sessions, so that the child
receives consistent support.
Evaluations of both EarlyBird and EarlyBird Plus programmes are positive (Stevens and
Shields, 2013). Parents who have participated show reduced stress, more positive
perceptions of their children and enhanced communication with them.
The two EarlyBird services are here:
http://www.autism.org.uk/earlybird

124 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183
Week 6: Family life and education
2 Support for families

Parent to Parent Service


‘I cannot thank the volunteer I spoke to enough. For the first time as a parent I
felt understood. I have felt so isolated for such a long time’.
Anonymous comment from a parent (NAS)

The NAS also offers a telephone service for parents seeking advice, support or just
someone to share things with. The Parent to Parent Service gives parents a chance to
talk to other parents of children on the spectrum, trained to provide this support. This
service has proved a popular and effective way of reducing the isolation that parents often
feel. The service is here:
http://www.autism.org.uk/services/helplines/parent-to-parent.aspx

2.2 Problems in accessing support


A big problem for many parents is finding out what support is available and how to access
it. With so much information available online, this might seem surprising. But not everyone
has the time, money or skills to seek the help that they need, and there is also the difficulty
of deciding which of the many services offered are reputable and risk-free. These
difficulties may be particularly salient in ethnic minority communities, accentuated by
cultural reservations about using the forms of support that are offered, as Dr Prithvi
Perepa explains in these video clips:

Video content is not available in this format.

Yet Prithvi also emphasises that different cultural attitudes to autism may help ethnic
families to view autism positively and to seek the support they need within their own
community:

Video content is not available in this format.

125 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183
Week 6: Family life and education
3 Education

3 Education
Decisions about nursery and school often pose huge challenges for autistic children and
their families. Ensuring that an autistic child is in the right environment is further
complicated by the fact that a substantial number of children arrive at school with no
diagnosis – their difficulties are first picked up by a teacher. Asperger syndrome or high-
functioning autism may only become apparent in the teenage years.
Even if the diagnosis is known, teachers with more able autistic pupils in their class may
not be able to reconcile their academic abilities with their communication difficulties and
the difficulties experienced in navigating the social world. As Luke Beardon stated:

The fact that I am well-spoken, and my condition isn’t obvious worked against
me being understood, when my behaviour may have been put down to
obstinacy, deviancy, pure eccentricity, or by school peers as being ‘mental’.
(Beardon and Edmonds, 2007)

Activity 1 Autism and mainstream school


Allow about 15 minutes

Drawing on your knowledge of autism characteristics outlined in Week 2 and the


psychological theories outlined in Week 4, use the space below to note down issues
that an autistic child might have in a mainstream school setting. Base your notes on a
child who has language skills and is academically able.

Provide your answer...

Discussion
Even a child with good language and academic skills is likely to encounter challenges.
The main areas of challenge are likely to include:

● the physical and sensory environment, e.g. sounds, colours and smells to which
the child is hypersensitive
● cognition and the curriculum, e.g. the child may have difficulty overriding their
focus on a particular topic of interest, or organising their activities through the
school day
● the social environment of the school, e.g. the child may find proximity with other
children difficult, or may try to make friends and feel excluded.

More detail of these challenges is given in the following section.

3.1 Challenges in educational settings


Now find out more about the challenges explored in the previous activity.

126 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183
Week 6: Family life and education
3 Education

Physical and sensory environment


Classrooms in the western world are typically busy, colourful and information-rich, all of
which may overload the senses of an autistic child. Bright colours, patterns, bright lighting,
movement and the chatter of other pupils may distract or confuse them, as can examples
of work displayed around the room.
Relating this also to the psychological theories encountered in Week 4, a child’s
excessive attention to detail may mean that they are not clear what needs their focus and
attention.

Cognition and the curriculum


An autistic child may have an especially uneven profile of academic strengths and
weaknesses, coupled with a strong preference for particular areas of interest. Getting any
child to engage with areas of the curriculum that they don’t like can be difficult, but in the
case of an autistic child, it may be necessary to adapt the curriculum. Executive function
issues may affect many areas of school life: getting ready for school, organising the
materials needed for a lesson, knowing how to start a task and determining when it is
finished, and navigating around the school from one classroom to another. A planner
detailing the order of activities during the day may be helpful.
Theory of mind difficulties may make more abstract areas of the curriculum, such as
reading or creative writing, a particular challenge to the autistic student. For instance, a
child may have difficulty imagining the emotions or actions of the characters. The
empathising-systemising model (Week 4) suggests that autistic children are likely to be
drawn to systematic subjects like science and mathematics, although this is undoubtedly
an oversimplification. Some autistic children particularly enjoy creative subjects such as
drama and music, where emotional understanding and expression is called for.
Theory of mind difficulties may also mean that the autistic pupil may not understand
instructions or takes them literally, leading to accusations of being cheeky or disobedient.
For instance, an instruction phrased as ‘Would you like to turn to page 20’ may not be
followed, because the pupil doesn’t realise this is a command rather than a question with
optional answers (‘Yes, I would like to’, or ‘No, I would not like to’).
Parts of instructions or group discussions may be missed due to slower processing of
verbal information. Even more able autistic students benefit from verbal communication
being supplemented by written or pictorial information.
Finally, the 21st century curriculum also includes areas such as physical, emotional and
sexual health, and understanding relationships. These may need to be presented in a
more concrete and explicit way so that the implications and consequences are clear to the
autistic young person.

The social environment


Unstructured periods such as breaks and mealtimes may be dreaded by the autistic
student, because their lack of social awareness and social motivation distances them
from the games and conversations of the other pupils. They find it difficult to join in
because of all the ‘unspoken’ rules and the (seemingly to them) invisible processes by
which others communicate.

127 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183
Week 6: Family life and education
3 Education

It is also common for autistic students to be teased by other children, either due to their
‘weird’ behaviour, the way they speak or because social naivety causes them to
misunderstand something. This teasing can very easily escalate into overt and covert
bullying, involving name-calling, physical violence or social isolation. Another way that
bullying can manifest is by manipulation, when an autistic pupil is coerced by another
pupil into breaking a rule or hurting someone.
Surveys suggest that at least 40 per cent of autistic children have experienced bullying,
and studies have shown that significantly more are bullied than are children with other
special educational needs or neurotypical children (Humphrey and Symes, 2010). As
David Hawker notes:

They called me the ‘sixth form punch bag’ as I was probably about the only kid
who got bullied in sixth form (A-levels).
(Sainsbury, 2000, p. 72)

Bullying can result in low self-esteem, mental health problems and poor academic
performance. Melanie said that her son Louis left mainstream school at 16 because of the
bullying and could not face further education. However, some young people find the
resilience to cope with bullying and emerge stronger as a result. Alex talked about his
experiences of bullying in Week 2. Here he maintains that his experience of mainstream
school was positive, despite what happened to him.

Video content is not available in this format.

128 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183
Week 6: Family life and education
4 Educational support and choices: the mainstream and other options

4 Educational support and choices: the


mainstream and other options
One of the greatest challenges that parents face is to secure appropriate support and the
right context for their child’s education. In countries such as the UK and the US, a
framework is in place for this. But parents’ experience of negotiating the framework is not
always positive and the range of educational choices varies widely from one place and
community to another. Parents may also find that the choice they have made – for
instance, for their child to go to a mainstream school – does not work out, leading them to
change their child’s schooling arrangements.

4.1 Educational plans and statements


To ensure that an autistic child has appropriate support for their education, it is important
that their needs are fully understood and documented at the outset. This may require, in
England, an Education and Health Care Plan (EHCP), in Scotland a Coordinated
Support Plan (CSP), or in Wales or Northern Ireland a statement of Special
Educational Needs (SEN). These are legal documents drawn up by the local authority to
specify the additional support needs of the child, objectives for them to achieve and the
help that they need in order to achieve those objectives. However, some parents report a
considerable battle to obtain the relevant statement for their child. Moreover, as in
Melanie’s case, they may find that additional funding allocated to the school for their own
child’s support is absorbed into a pool of resources for the support of a number of
children:

Louis’s 11K was put in the pool to provide teaching assistants (TAs) generally.
As a result he got no one-to-one support.
(Melanie, 2017, pers. comm.)

Another problem is that not all autistic children will receive a plan or statement, as their
needs are not considered severe enough. In these cases, parents may find themselves
trying to persuade the mainstream school that support is necessary:

In the case of my daughter, who was aged 13 at the point of diagnosis, she was
discharged from CAMHS* after we received the report as if the job was done. It
was left to us to liaise with her school about what this meant and what
adjustments, if any, were appropriate. Autism was an explanation for behaviour
deemed to be ‘socially inappropriate’ and for insecurities in coping with change,
anger management, sensory issues and the otherworldliness described as
daydreaming. She had developed coping and masking strategies to disguise
her sense of difference and to fit in with her peer group. I was told that she was
‘borderline’, ‘very high functioning’ due to her intelligence, vocabulary and
imagination. As she didn’t present as typically autistic in the way that her elder
brother [G] did, there seemed to be an assumption that no additional support
was needed.
(N, 2017, pers. comm.)

129 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183
Week 6: Family life and education
4 Educational support and choices: the mainstream and other options

*CAMHS stands for Child and Adolescent Mental Health Services. In the UK these are
services within the National Health Service that assess and treat young people with
emotional, behavioural or mental health difficulties.

4.2 Mainstream and other educational choices


Some mainstream schools have a unit attached that autistic pupils can attend for part of
the day. This may be for particular lessons or to provide a safe place during breaks and
lunchtimes.
If a child is receiving interventions such as TEACCH or PECS, then these need to be built
into the school day, to provide consistency. Autism units within mainstream schools may
not be appropriately resourced for such specialised support. This is easier to deliver if the
child attends a special school, as these will be geared to using such approaches and will
have staff who are trained in them.
Some children’s behaviour may require a highly structured environment throughout the
day, such that the only option is a residential school. Children attending residential
schools may return home for some or all weekends, or only during the holidays. Some
may not even be able to do this, although their families will visit them. For G, who is now
17 years old, special school has been a positive experience:

G is now 17 with very limited language and attends a special school. He seems
happy and well-liked by teachers and peers; he is sociable, affectionate and
has a sense of humour. He cannot live independently, but with support is able to
dress himself, shower, use the washing machine, dishwasher and help with
cooking – he loves food. He bounces like Tigger when we are out and about
(drawing attention to himself), but now goes to the gym and enjoys running and
swimming.
(N, 2017, pers. comm.)

Places at special schools and residential schools are limited and are only available to
those children with an EHCP, CSP or Statement of SEN. In the late 20th century there
was a move away from special schools, towards placing as many children as possible in
mainstream settings. The emphasis on ‘inclusion’ reflects the belief that it is better for
autistic children to be educated alongside neurotypical children in their community, thus
being involved in all the social and academic activities that other children experience.
Sadly, despite the advantages of mainstream school, many autistic children do not feel
included, lack friends and may not achieve their academic or vocational potential. As you
have learned throughout this module, autism is highly heterogeneous: in education (as
with so many other areas of life) a ‘one size fits all’ approach does not work. It is essential
that the individual needs of the child are considered, and that the right education context
is found.

4.3 Educational provision in regional and


international context
Many parents of autistic children in the UK feel that their choices are limited. However, the
range and quality of educational choices is substantial compared with provision in many

130 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183
Week 6: Family life and education
4 Educational support and choices: the mainstream and other options

other areas of the world, and especially in low income countries. For instance, Ethiopia
has a population of nearly 100 million people, of whom more than half are children – yet
until recently there have been only two schools for autistic children. These are in the
capital city, Addis Ababa, and were founded by two mothers, both of whom have taken
important steps in improving support and services for children with autism (Tekola
et al., 2016).

Activity 2 Educational provision for autism in your area


Allow about 15 minutes

If you are in the UK, explore the internet to find out about educational provision near to
you. You may find it useful to access the NAS Autism Services Directory, which you will
find here: http://www.autism.org.uk/earlybird
Check which of the following types of provision are available:

● mainstream school with an autism unit


● special school
● residential school.

If you are studying outside the UK, see if you can find out where the nearest autism-
friendly school is located.

4.4 Home education


Some parents choose to home-educate their children. They may have tried mainstream
and/or special school options, and found both to be unsuitable for their child. Their child
may have had bad experiences or failed to progress, or parents may be using
interventions such as Applied Behavioural Analysis (ABA), which cannot be readily
implemented in school. Receiving formal education at home is often a lot less stressful
and anxiety-provoking for the child because it is a familiar environment, and the day can
be structured to suit their routines and interests. However, it can be more stressful and
exhausting for parent(s), and is financially costly if paid employment has to be given up.
Parents may also find it hard to obtain the necessary teaching materials and they may
lack sufficient information about what the child should be learning. Parents undertaking
homeschooling are not obliged to follow the National Curriculum, and this may benefit the
child in the short to medium term, but difficulties may arise if the child ever wants or needs
to take formal qualifications such as GCSEs and A-levels. Despite his regrettable
experience of bullying, Alex is positive about his formal mainstream education, which has
equipped him for university studies. Home education may be the best choice for some
children, but it is not an easy option or one that all parents could manage.
In this video clip Arabella, mother of Iris Grace, explains why she took the decision to
educate Iris at home:

Video content is not available in this format.

131 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183
Week 6: Family life and education
4 Educational support and choices: the mainstream and other options

Activity 3 Home education


Allow about 5 minutes

Besides the potential drawbacks of home education just outlined, can you think of
another important developmental opportunity that home-educated children are likely to
miss out on?

Provide your answer...

Discussion
All home-educated children, whether autistic or not, may lack opportunities for
interacting and learning to socialise with other children, an important part of their
learning and progression towards adulthood. However, it is possible to join a network
of other home-educating parents and participate in organised outings or joint learning
sessions. Some parents have even set up groups in their own homes to facilitate their
child’s interactions. In this video clip Arabella talks about the Little Explorers Activity
Club that she runs from home, bringing Iris Grace together with other autistic children
for informal learning activities:

Video content is not available in this format.

132 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183
Week 6: Family life and education
5 This week’s quiz

5 This week’s quiz


Check what you’ve learned this week by taking the end-of-week quiz.
Week 6 quiz
Open the quiz in a new window or tab then return here once you’ve finished it.

133 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183
Week 6: Family life and education
6 Summary

6 Summary
This week you have looked at key challenges facing families when one or more members
is autistic. This week’s material has outlined the forms of support that are available to
families, and touched on the difficulties that some families may have in accessing this
support. It has also highlighted the remarkable resilience often present in the face of
difficulties, including in multiplex families. One of the biggest challenges for parents is
finding the right school or other educational option for their child. The key features of the
main educational choices available in the UK were described, and the arguments for and
against different options were considered.
You should now be able to:

● appreciate different ways in which family life may be affected when one or more
family members is autistic
● understand how challenges for the family and for the individual with autism change
over the course of development
● be aware of some of the sources of support available to families in the UK
● appreciate the psychological, academic and social challenges posed by education,
and the main choices available in terms of schooling
● understand the rationale for different educational choices, including home education.

Next week focuses on what happens when a young person with autism leaves school and
enters adulthood.
Now you can go to Week 7.

134 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183
Week 6: Family life and education
6 Summary

135 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183
Week 7: Adulthood
Introduction

Week 7: Adulthood
Introduction
Most of what you have learned so far has focused on children. However, autism is usually
a lifelong condition with many core challenges persisting in adulthood – although there is
also change and development, just as for any neurotypical person growing up. Some
young autistic adults succeed in overcoming multiple challenges and adapt to
independent living, and as you saw in Week 5, a small proportion achieve ‘optimal
outcomes’, becoming free of their autistic symptoms. For others, serious challenges
affecting autonomy and well-being persist. Some people only discover their autism in
adulthood, often after years spent struggling with a sense of being different. Across these
different groups, several things are clear:

● less is known about how autism affects people in adulthood


● the factors that lead to different outcomes in adulthood are not fully understood
● less help and support is available for adults than for children.

This week discusses what is known about autism during and after the transition to
adulthood and in older age. It describes some different life scenarios for autistic adults
and the challenges that they may face, for instance in employment and relationships. It
outlines some of the provision and support available, but also highlights the many gaps,
whether in the UK or worldwide.
Now watch the video in which Dr Ilona Roth introduces this week’s work:

Video content is not available in this format.

By the end of this week you should be able to:

● understand problems relating to the transition into adulthood for autistic people
● appreciate factors that may affect outcomes in adult life
● be aware of the range of outcomes that may result for autistic adults
● understand particular challenges in adult life concerning independence, employ-
ment, relationships, legal issues and health
● be familiar with forms of support available for adults and their limitations.

1 The transition to adulthood


The transition from childhood to legal adulthood at the age of 18 has been described as
‘falling off a cliff’ because there is generally very little provision for autistic adults. As for
any young person, the moment of leaving school marks the loss of a familiar social

136 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183
Week 7: Adulthood
1 The transition to adulthood

network. But whereas most neurotypical 18-year-olds can look forward to making new
friends at college or work, those with autism may find themselves isolated again,
augmenting the risk of depression and other mental health problems.
In this section we discuss some key considerations within the transition to adulthood.

1.1 The importance of ongoing education


Educational placements stop at 18 or 19. However, because autism can be associated
with delays in development, many would benefit from a more extended period of
education in a broad range of skills.
Skills learned at school may be lost if there is no post-school placement to reinforce and
practise them, and continuing education is needed both for learning daily living skills and
enabling autistic people to gain employment, where possible. Social and communication
skills can continue developing for many years. However, further education colleges may
not have the resources to support young people with autism, and specialist colleges are
sparse.

1.2 Higher education and beyond


Many children with good intellectual skills do well in public examinations such as the UK
system of GCSEs and A-levels, and some go on to university. Students within all four
countries of the UK can obtain an allowance for relevant ‘non-medical’ support, such as a
note-taker in lectures and a mentor to assist coping. However, such funding does not
continue after graduation.
The young person may be unable to negotiate the complexities of applying for jobs or
attending interviews. Even claiming out-of-work benefits may be impossible: forms are
often written in complex bureaucratic language that anyone can find a challenge. So even
young people with qualifications can find themselves returning to live with their parents or
struggling on their own. This may contribute to the high level of mental health issues noted
earlier.

1.3 Education, Health and Care Plan


The new Education, Health and Care Plan (EHCP) introduced in England in 2014 covers
the additional support needs for children and young people up to the age of 25. The EHCP
can fund a placement at a specialist college (if available), and can offer a personal budget
so that a young person or their designated carer can decide what support to buy.
However, according to an NAS survey (Moore, 2016), a majority of parents remain
dissatisfied with the new system, especially in relation to health and social care. It is
unclear how effective the EHCP system has been in securing services for young adults.
At present the Coordinated Support Plan (CSP) used in Scotland, and statements of
Special Educational Need (SEN) used in Northern Ireland and Wales do not provide the
continuity into adulthood that the EHCP is designed to provide; families have to negotiate
support after their children leave school.

137 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183
Week 7: Adulthood
2 Living arrangements

2 Living arrangements
In this section you will look at living arrangements for young people with autism.

2.1 Residential support


The parents who started the National Autistic Society (NAS) in the 1960s and set up the
first schools for children with autism all too soon found themselves addressing provision
for their adult offspring to experience the kind of structured environment that would enable
them to achieve their potential. Previously, offspring such as theirs may have been
committed to long-term institutions for those most unfortunately described at the time as
‘feeble-minded’.
This led some parents to purchase a property that could become a residential home for
their adult offspring: Somerset Court, near Burnham-on-Sea, was established in the
early 1970s. Subsequently the NAS has set up some other small residential homes
around the country. Each aims to help residents augment their social and communication
skills; learn some everyday life skills such as cleaning, shopping and cooking; and gain in
confidence, with the support of specialist staff. Approaches such as TEACCH, discussed
in Week 5, are used to create a structured environment. Residents may attend a local
college for day and evening classes, or learn some vocational skills. Somerset Court has
its own onsite resource centre.
Other local autism charities have similar facilities, with places funded by local authorities.
Another option is to attend a local daycare centre, although these may not be autism-
specific. A person must have been assessed by social services, and have had funding
agreed, in order to gain access to such services.

2.2 Support for independence


In some areas of the UK, independent living is facilitated by access to funded support.
Some adults obtain direct funding for a part-time support worker to help with areas of daily
living where they have difficulty, such as self-care or dealing with officialdom. Some NAS
adult services are directed towards more able adults, including small social groups that
aim to help with the continuing development of social skills. The success of such
arrangements depends on the individual case. Melanie’s son Louis lives with his father,
with frequent visits and input from Melanie herself, and support from other services:

Louis is on Direct Payments and pays helpers to take him out so this is good.
The social worker changes every six or 12 months so there is no continuity and
no help to get him moved into independent living. He finally has a local
psychiatrist and is on heavy medication to calm him down. [My hope for the
future is for Louis to be] independent and safe from eviction or life on the streets
after I and his father die. He'll never work. I pray the welfare state will always
exist to support him.
(Melanie, 2017, pers. comm.)

138 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183
Week 7: Adulthood
2 Living arrangements

Alex lives at home while studying for a degree and working part-time, although he hopes
to move into his own place eventually. In this clip he talks about his mentor and also the
social group he attends:

Video content is not available in this format.

2.3 Adult support in international perspective


As outlined in earlier weeks, services for autistic children in many lower and middle
income countries (LMICs) are very sparse. The gap is even greater for adults, as
illustrated by a recent interview study of South African professionals and parents (Meiring
et al., 2016). In some ways the fears about transition expressed by South African parents
mirror those of parents like Melanie, quoted earlier. But the situation is more extreme, as
these quotes from parents illustrate:

‘It’s a very, very scary thought, it’s a very sad situation, to be very honest, there
is nobody, after us [parents] there is really nobody …’
‘No I don’t know of any [transition arrangements] … I have ‘googled’, I have
looked, it’s only overseas where I see there is so much support systems …’
(Meiring et al., 2016)

139 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183
Week 7: Adulthood
3 Adult outcomes

3 Adult outcomes
Adult outcomes is the term for what happens to people with autism once they transition
into adulthood. One reason that relatively little is known about outcomes is that autism
was only identified in the 1940s, and it took several decades for the diagnosis to become
well-known.
So until recently there have been relatively few adults on whom to base outcome studies.
This situation is changing, and an increasing number of longitudinal studies have tracked
the progress of children with autism into adulthood. Key findings from 25 such studies
have been assembled and evaluated by Magiati et al. (2014).

3.1 Cognition, language and adaptive functioning


The researchers reported that cognitive function (for instance as measured by IQ scores)
remains stable for many adults on the autism spectrum.
However, there is much individual variation, with evidence that IQ improves for some
individuals and declines for others. Language function tends to improve in adulthood, but
in most cases significant difficulties remain. Adaptive functioning means an individual’s
capacity to deal with everyday tasks such as dressing and washing oneself, and
communicating personal needs.
On the whole, measures of both adaptive functioning and the severity of core autism
symptoms show improvements, but again there are individual variations.

3.2 Social life, independence and mental health


The most consistently negative results from Magiati’s report concern social life and
independence in adults on the spectrum. More than half of participants remained fully or
largely dependent on parents or carers in adulthood, and needed significant support for
education and living arrangements. Few have friendships, romantic relationships or
employment. Correspondingly, the NAS estimates that only 16 per cent of adults in the UK
are employed (National Autistic Society, 2016).
More than half of autistic adults are likely to have some mental health difficulties, most
commonly depression and anxiety. It is not clear to what extent mental health problems
are a feature linked to autism itself, or a consequence of social isolation and society’s
response to autism. The need to overcome rigid routines, make decisions or adapt to new
circumstances in order to engage with the world could provoke extreme anxiety. Behaving
and thinking ‘differently’ and being awkward around others could lead to the difficulty in
forming relationships and obtaining employment.
In general, the quality of life experienced by adults on the spectrum is lower than for
neurotypical adults (van Heijst and Geurts, 2015).

3.3 What factors influence outcomes?


IQ and language ability play a role in shaping adult outcomes. In general, those with a
higher IQ and better language skills are likely to have more favourable outcomes in

140 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183
Week 7: Adulthood
3 Adult outcomes

adulthood. But the pattern is very mixed: the quality of support available and the
individual’s own resilience are likely to play a role in adult well-being, and as we shall see,
what counts as a favourable outcome should be judged in relation to the individual, rather
than to some absolute standard. A successful individual outcome doesn’t necessarily
mean that the person has ‘outgrown’ their autism or that they are living independently,
with a job and so on.

141 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183
Week 7: Adulthood
4 Outcomes across the spectrum

4 Outcomes across the spectrum


The extent to which autistic people are capable of a ‘standard’ adult life varies greatly. In
general, those more profoundly affected are more likely to need care and support, either
at home or in a residential home. However, this group may also include high-functioning
adults, who experience disabling levels of anxiety, or find that adaptive functioning skills –
such as dressing, preparing a meal for themselves or paying bills – are too challenging.
On the other hand a substantial number of adults do live independently of their parents or
other forms of support. This group undoubtedly includes many people whose autism is
undiagnosed.
Finally, a small proportion of those on the spectrum do appear to lose their autistic
symptoms altogether, achieving what have been termed optimal outcomes. This section
considers a range of outcomes, incorporating individual life stories.

4.1 Timothy Baron


In earlier weeks, Michael Baron talked about his profoundly autistic son, Timothy, one of
the first children to be diagnosed in the UK. Timothy is in his 60s now. Despite remaining
in residential care for most of his life, Michael sees real progress in how Timothy
experiences and engages with the world:

What I think when I see him, and I saw him yesterday, is that the … rage and
the disturbance that was the hallmark of Timothy at the age of four … that’s all
sort of fallen away. … He isn't an angry middle-aged man. And in a way I think
that the autism in a sense has fallen away and you are left with, in his case, a
learning disabled, middle-aged man …
It’s just a theory that … he is not so, as it were, classically autistic as he used to
be. … He is calmer and he is more sociable, can live in a community, can do
things that he wouldn't have been able to do before and he is not so disabled by
his autism as he was.
(Baron and Roth, 2017)

Michael is saying that the outcome for Timothy is successful compared with other possible
eventualities for him as a profoundly affected person. This is a judgement in individual
terms, rather than in relation to society’s norms.

4.2 Donald Triplett


In the 1940s, Donald Triplett was one of the small group of children from whom Kanner
first formulated his ideas about autism. Among Kanner’s case notes at the time is this
quote from Donald’s mother:

Another of his recent hobbies is with old issues of Time magazine. He found a
copy of the first issue of March 3, 1923, and has attempted to make a list of the
dates of publication of each issue since that time. So far he has gotten to

142 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183
Week 7: Adulthood
4 Outcomes across the spectrum

April 1934. He has figured the number of issues in a volume and similar
nonsense.
(Kanner, 1943, p. 222)

You may note a hint of exasperation at Donald’s unusual special interest. His mother had
also concluded that he was mentally ill, and on the advice of psychiatrists, she and her
husband had Donald committed to an institution. But they regretted their decision and
brought him home again after a year. When he was growing up, Donald was described as
socially aloof and eccentric, but with a number of savant skills, such as his exceptional
memory for numbers. Despite his difficulties, he sustained employment in a bank, and
learned to drive, play golf and to travel independently.
Donald is in his eighties now, enjoys his life and remains a valued and cherished member
of his community in Forest, Mississippi. Factors that have clearly made a big difference to
this outcome are his parents’ positivity, their ability to ensure a secure future for him and
the positive social support he has received from the community. Although he remains a
loner, he meets others to play golf, and participates in church and other local activities
(Donvan and Zucker, 2010).

4.3 Wenn Lawson


Donald Triplett’s story is not just about the support of others, but also his own positivity
and resilience in the face of difficulties. These attributes feature strongly in the life story of
Wenn Lawson.
In an interview recorded for The Open University in 2011, Wenn described his difficult
relationship with his parents, misdiagnosis with schizophrenia aged 17, troubled marriage
and loss of a child in a car accident. But he also talked of his success as an academic,
writer and autism advocate, some of which he attributed to the love and support he found
with another partner after divorce. Wenn studied psychology with The Open University,
and considered distance study to be ideally suited to people on the autistic spectrum:

[The OU] enables us to not have to mix with lots other people who might
actually take away from our ability to learn. The fact that you can be in a
classroom where you have got people clicking pens, turning pages, all the
sensory overwhelming stuff that comes from just being with lots of people,
closes me down. Whereas distance education, like the Open University
courses, I can study at home, I can set up my study area, I can organise my
studies, all the materials are posted out to me, I’ve got time to process the
whole event of distance education in a much better way than I can if I had to go
to a typical university.
(Lawson and Roth, 2011)

4.4 Optimal outcomes


Longitudinal work by a few research groups suggests that a small group of children with
an autism spectrum diagnosis completely lose the symptoms consistent with this
diagnosis, though they may retain some subtle symptoms similar to ADHD (Suh
et al., 2016). Research into what factors might promote this outcome is still at an early

143 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183
Week 7: Adulthood
4 Outcomes across the spectrum

stage. Deborah Fein, a leading researcher in this field, suggests that a high proportion of
the optimal outcomes group have received intensive early ABA intervention (see Week 5),
but also speculates that biological factors may differentiate the group from others on the
autism spectrum
This kind of outcome is not common, and becoming free of autism should not be
considered the only good outcome. Some feel that their autism is integral to their identity
and would not wish to lose it. And as the lives of Timothy Baron, Donald Triplett and Wenn
Lawson all show, positive outcomes in adulthood should be evaluated in relation to the
individual.

144 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183
Week 7: Adulthood
5 Addressing challenges in adult life: employment and relationships

5 Addressing challenges in adult life:


employment and relationships
Sections 5 and 6 consider the major challenges faced by many adults on the spectrum,
whatever their setting.

5.1 Employment

Activity 1 Challenges at work


Allow about 15 minutes

Imagine that someone on the autism spectrum has started a new job based at a
company not far from their home. Bearing in mind what you have learned about autism
up to now, use the space below to identify two kinds of difficulties that this person may
find especially challenging in the work environment, and suggest a helpful adaptation
that an employer could make for each one. You may find it useful to refer back to
Week 6, where Activity 1 called for comparable reflections relating to education.

1.
2.

Discussion
You may have identified some of the following challenges:
Physical and sensory environment
The autistic employee may find the noise, smells and visual stimuli of the office
disturbing, especially if it is open plan.
An employer could help by providing the employee with a secluded work area, where
these stimuli are minimised.
The work day
Employees may be expected to carry out their work in a particular sequence and at a
particular rate, and to respond flexibly if new priorities unexpectedly crop up. Such
organisational matters may pose great challenges for autistic employees. For
instance, an autistic employee may tend to focus in great detail on one task to the
exclusion of others, and may have great difficulties in switching to something else as
required.
An employer could help by providing as much flexibility as possible, and by employing
the autistic person’s strengths, e.g. by allocating work that needs to be carried out with
extra precision and care, or giving responsibility in areas of work requiring particular
numerical or IT skills.
Communication
Autistic employees may struggle to understand or carry out instructions if these are
implicit or not delivered clearly enough.

145 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183
Week 7: Adulthood
5 Addressing challenges in adult life: employment and relationships

Employers and other staff can help by always choosing clear, direct language and
avoiding metaphors.
Social context
The autistic employee may be disturbed by close proximity to others (e.g. in an open
plan space), or by the expectation to socialise in lunch breaks or after work.
As for sensory stimuli, an employer could help by providing a secluded work area. He/
she could also seek the employee’s permission to explain their needs to other
employees.
Other measures that may help in the work setting include:

● allowing part-time working and/or ‘mental health days’ if the person becomes too
stressed by having to interact with colleagues
● permitting a mentor/advocate to act as an intermediary between the autistic
person and their colleagues or managers.

5.2 Support for employment


The NAS has some support centres across the UK and an outreach team that aims to
assist with preparing for work, for instance by building on social and practical skills,
confidence and experience, as well as CV writing. However, because it is a charity, such
support centres are few, and many people do not have access to them. The NAS also
offers advice and training for employers on understanding autism and how to manage
people with autism.
There are also commercial companies that help autistic people into meaningful and
productive employment. At Specialisterne, which started in Denmark and now operates in
11 different countries (although not the UK), employees are found work as business
consultants on tasks such as software testing, programming and data entry for the public
and private sectors. Auticon has bases in London and Munich, and works on similar
principles. Their employees are all on the autism spectrum and work as consultants for
various clients, with a job coach who gets to know the consultant and their capabilities,
assesses the work environment for any reasonable adjustments that could be made, acts
as a mediator and may initially help the autistic person with travelling to and from the job.
Autistic people have many skills to offer in the workplace, and simple respect and
friendship from fellow employees may be as important to well-being as specific support
measures. In this clip Alex talks about his very positive experiences of working part-time in
a bookmakers:

Video content is not available in this format.

5.3 Relationships
The lives of autistic adults can follow a neurotypical pattern such as gaining employment,
getting married and having children. Some may make great efforts to behave in a way that
means that, at most, they are considered a bit eccentric. There is growing evidence that

146 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183
Week 7: Adulthood
5 Addressing challenges in adult life: employment and relationships

females, in particular, may seek ways to mask their autism in order to fit in. Perhaps only
their close family sees their struggles and the impact of maintaining this façade.
Autistic adults may find relationships difficult, on many different levels, and many do not
enter into intimate relationships. In his account of Temple Grandin, the writer and
neurologist Oliver Sacks wrote that Temple had never dated or been in a relationship:

She found such interactions completely baffling, and too complex to deal with.
‘Have you cared for somebody else?’ I asked her. She hesitated for a moment
before answering, ‘I think lots of times there are things that are missing from my
life’.
(Sacks, 1995, p. 122)

Many feel lonely and would dearly like to have an intimate relationship with another adult,
but lack the social skills or social understanding to know how to even start the process, or
to know when it is appropriate to take things to a deeper level from ‘friendship’. If a
relationship does blossom, the partner/spouse can sometimes find life frustrating because
of the autistic person’s lack of understanding of their emotions:

Occasionally things do go wrong. Having explained carefully, calmly and


rationally why I feel upset, I will check that Chris has understood and when he
replies ‘Not really’, there is the old temptation to find something expendable in
the kitchen.
(Slater-Walker and Slater-Walker, 2002, p. 72)

Activity 2 Thinking about relationships


Allow about 15 minutes

What aspects of autism might pose challenges in relationships? Think of this in relation
to parents’ relationships with their adult offspring, or couples where one or both is
autistic. Use the space below to answer the following prompts:

1. Draw on what you have learned so far to suggest three challenges.


2. Note two autistic characteristics that could be beneficial in a relationship .

Provide your answer...

Discussion
1. You may have thought of some or all of the following:
○ Parents may find it frustrating or wearing to provide support for tasks that
most neurotypical adults can cope with, such as completing forms or
applying for jobs.
○ Some attributes, such as literal understanding and speaking, or fixations on
spinning objects or Thomas the Tank Engine, which may be endearing or
amusing in a child, may be harder to tolerate or understand in an adult.
○ The autistic person may be unaware of the stress that caring for them can
cause – often alongside other stressors, such as caring for elderly parents,
grandchildren or a spouse/partner.

147 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183
Week 7: Adulthood
5 Addressing challenges in adult life: employment and relationships

○ Social situations like parties or family get-togethers may be difficult. The


autistic person may come across as odd or rude in social settings, leading to
feelings of awkwardness in their parents or partner.
○ Autistic people may find it very hard to deal effectively with the usual conflicts
and disagreements that crop up in a relationship. Social communication
difficulties and a lack of emotional insight may impede resolving even small
problems and may affect the relationship more than they should.
○ An autistic partner who can’t anticipate their loved one’s thoughts, interests
and motivations may feel bemused or even annoyed by their actions.
2. The positive qualities that autistic people may bring to relationships include their
loyalty, commitment and honesty. Close attention to detail could be trying in some
circumstances, but could be helpful, for example, in planning a holiday or sticking
to a budget. It is important not to think exclusively in terms of challenges.

148 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183
Week 7: Adulthood
6 Addressing challenges in adult life: legal issues, health and ageing

6 Addressing challenges in adult life: legal


issues, health and ageing
An autistic person may come into contact with the criminal justice system, either as a
victim or witness of crime or as a suspect.

6.1 The criminal justice system


There are several reasons why autistic people may be especially vulnerable to crime. For
instance, unusual behavior in a public place may, sadly, attract physical bullying or other
hate crime. Naivety about other people’s motives may make the autistic person especially
vulnerable to fraud. It may even result in the autistic person being drawn into committing a
crime. Anecdotally, an autistic person has been caught as the perpetrator in several
recent cases of computer hacking, while other more devious members of a hacking group
have got away.
Autistic people are no more likely to be in prison than any other section of the population
(King and Murphy, 2014). However, as we have seen, they may have been manipulated
into engaging in criminal acts, or have missed social cues that would have otherwise
prevented them from acting this way. It is also possible that some are wrongfully
imprisoned because difficulty in understanding police procedure or questioning has led
them to admit to crimes they did not commit.
All of these situations require that police officers and court personnel are aware of a
person’s autism and follow correct procedures. Adjustments made by the police and
courts can include:

● an interview location where the lighting, chair coverings or officer’s clothes can be
adjusted so as not to cause sensory overload
● presence of a parent, carer, advocate or support person
● a registered intermediary to advise the police or court on how to phrase questions so
they are fully understood
● in court, the autistic person may be able to use the live video link or to have their
interviews and cross-examinations recorded.

As with other aspects of public life, the NAS is working towards awarding accreditation to
organisations that demonstrate a commitment to understanding autism, and making
appropriate accommodations. The UK Ministry of Justice is encouraging prisons and
young offender institutions to become accredited. The NAS also offers a free guide for
legal professionals and police officers who find themselves working with autistic people.

6.2 Health and ageing


Autistic adults may be susceptible to particular health problems and also to accidents for a
range of reasons.
As you have learned in earlier weeks, epilepsy, depression and other health problems are
‘comorbid’ with autism. In addition, narrow food preferences may lead to an unhealthy

149 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183
Week 7: Adulthood
6 Addressing challenges in adult life: legal issues, health and ageing

diet, which could put the individual at risk of obesity and heart problems. Some autistic
people are relatively insensitive to pain, which means that serious health problems such
as a broken bone go unreported. In emergency situations (e.g. on admission to hospital),
a person with autism may be unable to make decisions or insist on their treatment rights,
and in rehabilitation, they may not appreciate the importance of following medical
guidance.
Any of these factors could potentially affect life expectancy in autism, and there is some
evidence for raised mortality (Howlin and Moss, 2012), although more work is needed.
Research is also lacking into how autism impacts conditions of older adulthood such as
dementia, or whether older autistic adults have unique health needs. As more and more
diagnosed adults are becoming elderly, this is an area that needs urgent attention. For
instance, is dementia more common than in the general population, or might autism
provide a protective function?

150 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183
Week 7: Adulthood
7 The Autism Act and related legislation

7 The Autism Act and related legislation


A landmark piece of legislation in England was the Autism Act of 2009, which came out of
an NAS campaign to highlight the problems faced by autistic adults. It required all local
authorities to develop an autism strategy to provide relevant services, from the authority
themselves and from other bodies such as NHS trusts, in order to meet the needs of
autistic adults, including:

● clear and consistent pathways to diagnosis for adults


● identifying adults in the area already diagnosed as autistic and assessing their needs
● planning for children transitioning to adult services
● training for staff providing services
● providing help into employment, including developing skills and overcoming barriers
which may prevent the person accessing job opportunities.

Building on the first national strategy ‘Fulfilling and Rewarding Lives’, an updated strategy
called ‘Think Autism’ was introduced in 2014, with some government money to be used
for projects developing local services. There is also a commitment to training for GPs and
other healthcare professionals, as well as Disability Employment Advisors at Jobcentres.
The Northern Ireland Assembly passed an Autism Act in 2011, which has a similar focus
to the Act in England. Scotland has an autism strategy that covers both children and
adults, with aims including supporting autistic people through the many challenges that
they might face in their lives, improving people’s quality of life and supporting them into
employment where appropriate. Although Wales has an autism strategy, it is not backed
by legislation. There are gaps in service provision and diagnosis for adults can take up to
seven years in Wales. At the time of writing, the NAS is campaigning for an Autism Act in
Wales that is similar to the one in England.
Yet some bodies, such as the Shirley Foundation, have been very critical of the lack of
progress made UK-wide in recognition and support for autism since this legislation came
into being, and recommend further research into good practice, what is most effective and
where money should be spent (Iemmi et al., 2017).

151 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183
Week 7: Adulthood
8 This week’s quiz

8 This week’s quiz


Check what you’ve learned this week by taking the end-of-week quiz.
Week 7 quiz
Open the quiz in a new window or tab then return here once you’ve finished it.

152 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183
Week 7: Adulthood
9 Summary

9 Summary
This week has painted a sometimes sombre picture of the many difficulties and
challenges that confront autistic adults in all aspects of their lives. Yet we have also
highlighted positive outcomes, individual resilience and the benefits that autistic
characteristics may offer at work and in relationships. We will end on a similarly optimistic
note with a comment from Alex about his achievements so far and hopes for the future:

Video content is not available in this format.

You should now be able to:

● understand problems relating to the transition into adulthood for autistic people
● appreciate factors that may affect outcomes in adult life
● be aware of the range of outcomes that may result for autistic adults
● understand particular challenges in adult life concerning independence, employ-
ment, relationships, legal issues and health
● be familiar with forms of support available for adults and their limitations.

Now you can go to Week 8, the final week of the course.

153 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183
Week 7: Adulthood
9 Summary

154 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183
Week 8: Reflecting back, looking forward
Introduction

Week 8: Reflecting back,


looking forward
Introduction
This week you will think about what you have learned so far, where we are today with our
understanding of autism and what the future might hold. Attitudes to and provision for
autism are changing, although at different rates across the globe. You will reflect on the
changes in countries such as the UK, and consider priorities and challenges for the future,
including for lower and middle income countries (LMICs), with a special focus on Ethiopia.
Now watch the video in which Dr Ilona Roth introduces this week’s work:

Video content is not available in this format.

By the end of this week you should be able to:

● evaluate how your own knowledge and understanding has increased while studying
this course
● appreciate both shared problems and differences in the lived experience and needs
of autistic people
● discuss trends and priorities in autism research and support
● identify wider societal issues that affect the lives of those with autism and their
families
● understand some of the priorities for autism in low resource settings.

1 Autism: what have you learned?


Now try the following activity.

Activity 1 Reflect on what you have learned


Allow about 10 minutes

Use the space below to note key things that you have learned about autism so far in
the course. You might want to check back to the notes you made for the same exercise
in Week 1, Activity 1. In what ways has your understanding changed most? What do
you consider to be the most important insights you have gained?

155 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183
Week 8: Reflecting back, looking forward
1 Autism: what have you learned?

Display of content entered previously

Provide your answer...

Discussion
Here are some key points you may have noted:
Understanding autism should have helped you to gain a wider knowledge of what
autism is, and how it affects children and adults across the lifespan. You have learned
how the concept of autism has changed, and with it the tools used to diagnose it. You
have considered how the impact and experience of autism varies between individuals,
and how perceptions of autism may be affected by cultural context. You have looked at
explanations of the causes of autism, and at the benefits and drawbacks of different
approaches to intervention and support. You have considered how autism may affect
families, and looked at the particular challenges of adulthood. You have also learned
that autistic people have strengths and skills, and need scope to fulfil their potential.

Now we will reflect further on some key themes, ongoing issues and implications for the
future.

156 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183
Week 8: Reflecting back, looking forward
2 The autism spectrum in the 21st century

2 The autism spectrum in the 21st century


As you learned in Week 1, knowledge about autism has come a long way in the last 8
decades – including recognition that it encompasses a great deal of diversity. The DSM-IV
diagnostic criteria attempted to encompass this diversity by distinguishing differentiating
sub-types such as childhood autism and Asperger syndrome (see Week 3, Section 2.2),
but this proved problematic because the overlapping symptoms meant that diagnosis
could not reliably differentiate between these categories.
The recent DSM-5 diagnostic criteria define autism as a single spectrum but also allow an
individual profile to be specified for each person diagnosed. The ICD-11 criteria published
in 2018 mirror DSM-5 in many respects, yet do recognise sub-types, differentiating autism
with and without intellectual disability. Debate continues about the best way to capture
both the shared features and the diversity of autism. Some different perspectives that
bear on this issue are discussed in the next section.

2.1 ‘When you’ve met one autistic person, you’ve


met one autistic person’
The above phrase is used by some autistic people and those who know them to
emphasise that each autistic person is uniquely different and needs to be understood
individually – within the broad framework of the social, communication and non-social
differences that are implied by an autism diagnosis.
As you have seen throughout this course, first-hand accounts from autistic people provide
important insights into the ways autistic individuals see the world, although inevitably
these are limited to those who are able to articulate their self-awareness and describe
their experiences. These accounts enable autistic people to voice their feelings and ideas,
highlighting the diversity of autism from an inside perspective and qualifying insights from
research and clinical practice. However, the individuality of these accounts means that no
single personal account can represent everyone on the spectrum. For instance, Temple
Grandin describes that she ‘thinks in pictures’ (Grandin, 1996); this may be true for others
on the spectrum, but it is not universal. Wenn Lawson has described his thought
processes as more verbally based (Lawson and Roth, 2011).

2.2 Neurodiversity
The relationship between the autism spectrum and the rest of the population is one with
important social and ethical implications. Traditionally, autism was seen as akin to a
medical problem – diagnosed by medical professionals and regarded as a disorder and/or
disability, with deficits that impair the individual’s functioning and depart from the ‘norm’.
Historically, this was the perspective most strongly associated with the idea of a
‘treatment’ or ‘cure’ aimed at returning the individual to ‘normality’.
Nowadays, many individuals with autism and their families prefer to think of autism as a
difference from neurotypicality (and note the use of this term in preference to ‘normality’).

157 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183
Week 8: Reflecting back, looking forward
2 The autism spectrum in the 21st century

Some, including members of the Autism Rights Movement, altogether reject the
concept of autism as a disability, arguing that being autistic is simply a valid alternative to
neurotypicality, and something to be celebrated.
Here Arabella reflects on coming to accept and celebrate Iris Grace’s unique qualities

Video content is not available in this format.

An associated perspective first introduced in Week 1 is ‘neurodiversity’. This extends the


‘difference not disorder’ perspective to include individuals with a range of cognitive
differences besides autism, such as dyslexia and ADHD. Neurodiversity advocates argue
that these conditions arise due to natural genetic variation and are not ‘pathological’
(medically disordered), but should be recognised as a social category like ethnicity or
sexual orientation. They suggest that it is society that needs to change, to accept autistic
people for who they are and to accommodate their behaviours.
Those in the ‘difference not disorder’ lobby are making valuable contributions to changing
perceptions about autism. However, their views are not universally shared. In particular,
some families where there is a profoundly affected autistic individual may feel that while
the neurodiversity view is appropriate for high-functioning individuals who lead relatively
independent lives, it fails to acknowledge the distress, suffering and disability of those
such as their own family member.

2.3 One autism or several autisms?


In a recent interview, Michael Baron, father of Timothy, echoed the concern that society’s
current image of ‘what autism is’ downplays or overlooks the difficulties of people like
Timothy:

On the one hand the increased awareness [of autism] is a good thing and can
only make things better. And on the other hand I'm no longer sure that the label
is right for all the people who say ‘oh I've just been diagnosed’ … I think the
situation has become very fragmented, very confused. The language is very
difficult and in a sense I don’t recognise it. And so when I look back the parents
I knew … of – let’s use that awful word – handicapped children, they were
children who were made dependent by their disorder and over the years they
have changed maybe from being the majority to being … not so big a majority,
maybe in fact a minority now. So it has changed enormously.
(Baron and Roth, 2017).

Michael Baron’s view is that the time may have come to reconsider – again – the idea of a
unified autism condition. Interestingly this is an ongoing topic of discussion among
clinicians (Boustead, 2015), and fits with suggestions by some geneticists. As outlined in
Week 4, genetic studies have shown that DNA variants on a large number of genes may
be involved – besides some rare cases, it is a polygenic condition. Moreover, autistic
people seem to have different combinations of candidate genes, meaning that it is a
heterogeneous condition. On the face of it, this genetic variation fits well with the idea of a
spectrum of heterogeneity and individuality at the level of symptoms and behavior.
Yet some experts maintain that the future lies in fragmenting autism into different
disorders or conditions. These would be defined in terms of which genes are affected and

158 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183
Week 8: Reflecting back, looking forward
2 The autism spectrum in the 21st century

how these relate to distinctive patterns of physical characteristics such as digestive


problems and traits such as intellectual disability (Deweerdt, 2014). Creating genetically
differentiated autism sub-groups has made it possible to link some families whose
children have characteristics in common, enabling the families to share information on
what interventions have worked for their children, to provide guidance for teachers, and to
inform research. However, given the vast range of possible genetic variants that have
been identified in autism, and the fact that some of these occur in conditions other than
autism, it may only be possible to group a small proportion of autistic people in this way.
The debate about whether and how autism can be subdivided is ongoing.
However we think of the autism spectrum, it is clear that some autistic individuals manage
and even thrive without support, while others need as much help as they can get. The
challenge to us all is to strike the right balance between respecting autistic people’s right
to autonomy and self-determination, while providing support wherever and whenever it is
needed, including in the areas where even quite able people struggle. Autistic individuals
should have as much input as they can into decisions about their care and support. This
person-centred planning (PCP) enables people to maintain dignity, while also helping to
identify the accommodations they may need to function at their best.

159 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183
Week 8: Reflecting back, looking forward
3 Future directions for autism research

3 Future directions for autism research


In Week 1 it was argued that some of the most reliable information about autism comes
from properly conducted research studies. But what questions should such research
focus upon? In the last few decades, funding agencies have injected huge sums of money
into research into explaining autism, including psychological, neurobiological and genetic
factors. But many, especially those in the autism community and their carers, believe that
other issues impinge more directly on their lives and well-being, and should take higher
priority in terms of funding.

3.1 What are the priorities?


In the following activity you will explore the priorities for the future directions in autism
research.

Activity 2 How should funding be allocated?


Allow about 20 minutes

Drawing on the knowledge you have gained throughout the course, use the space
below to list five fields in which you think funding for research would be most
worthwhile.

Provide your answer...

Discussion
In 2016, a UK survey by the research charity Autistica, in collaboration with the NAS,
Research Autism and others, asked participants to list their top priorities for research
funding (Autistica, 2016). The 1213 respondents included individuals with autism
(23%), family members and caregivers (52%), and clinicians and other professionals
(25%). The 25 most frequently listed choices were compiled and put to another panel,
comprised of equal numbers of people on the spectrum, parents and carers, and
clinicians and professionals. The top ten selected areas were as follows:

● Which interventions improve mental health or reduce mental health problems in


people with autism? How should mental health interventions be adapted for the
needs of people with autism?
● Which interventions are effective in the development of communication/language
skills in autism?
● What are the most effective ways to support/provide social care for autistic
adults?
● Which interventions reduce anxiety in autistic people?
● Which environments/supports are most appropriate in terms of achieving the best
education/life/social skills outcomes in autistic people?
● How can parents and family members be supported/educated to care for and
better understand an autistic relative?

160 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183
Week 8: Reflecting back, looking forward
3 Future directions for autism research

● How can autism diagnostic criteria be made more relevant for the adult
population? And how do we ensure that autistic adults are appropriately
diagnosed?
● How can we encourage employers to apply person-centred interventions and
support to help autistic people maximise their potential and performance in the
workplace?
● How can sensory processing in autism be better understood?
● How should service delivery for autistic people be improved and adapted in order
to meet their needs?

Are your five priority areas included in the ten listed above? Are there any important
priorities that you think are missing from the survey results?

Provide your answer...

Discussion
Obviously views on the priorities will vary. Beyond the ‘top ten’, other areas listed
included evaluating interventions for managing challenging behavior, improving social
skills and exploring the most effective early interventions. Research into sensory
processing was the only more theory-related item in the top ten. Researching the
causes of autism was number 20 on the list.
One topic not mentioned in the Autistica report, but much in the news at the time of
writing, is the finding that autism can manifest differently in females, leading to under-
diagnosis. You might want to re-read the discussion of this in Week 3 Section 4.1.
Also, because the Autistica survey was conducted in the UK, research priorities for
autism in other parts of the world were not identified.

3.2 Doing research well


Facilitating effective autism research is not just a question of funding the right areas; it is
also about how the research is done and who gets to participate. Scientific approaches
such as cognitive psychology and neuroscience traditionally follow a model in which one
group of people (research specialists in the field) carry out tests and gather information
from another group of people (the participants).
But there has been a growing call for autistic people to play a more integrated role
(Pellicano and Stears, 2011). One leading Canadian research team, headed by Professor
Laurent Mottron, has been following this approach for some years, with Michelle Dawson,
an autistic person, as a research team member. Dr Anna Remington, Director of the
Centre for Research in Autism and Education, University College London, leads on
several initiatives to ensure that autistic people inform the CRAE research activities not
just as participants, but in other roles (Zeliadt, 2017).
Of course, doing research this way is not straightforward. For instance, autistic people
may need to accept certain research findings which do not fit with their own personal
experience. Mutual respect, trust and candid dialogue between team members is an
important part of making such a process viable.

161 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183
Week 8: Reflecting back, looking forward
4 Autism in society

4 Autism in society
Bringing autistic people into the research process is part of a wider process of
accommodating autism within society. These days, autism is of wide public interest in
countries like the UK, and media portrayals have undoubtedly played a role in this. But
this doesn’t mean that autism is well understood: while media portrayals have enhanced
general public awareness, they are also responsible for some erroneous ideas that are
unhelpful when it comes to making the right accommodations for autistic people.

4.1 Media portrayals of autism


How is autism portrayed in the media? You can explore this question in the activity below.

Activity 3 Media portrayals of autism


Allow about 20 minutes

Think about the 1988 film Rain Man and two more recent media or fictional
representations of autism. These could be films, TV series, books, etc. How accurate
was Rain Man’s representation of autism? Do you think that media representations
have become more authentic in recent works? Can you think of any portrayals of
women? (Note: if you have not seen Rain Man, you can look up a synopsis on
Wikipedia).

Provide your answer...

Discussion
It is only possible to discuss one or two of the different representations of autism here.
In Rain Man, Dustin Hoffman plays the autistic character Raymond Babbitt with a
skilful and convincing portrayal of social detachment, naivety and rigid adherence to
structure and routine. Equally key to the plot, however, is Raymond’s exceptional
memory and powers of mental calculation, which his brother Charlie exploits to his
advantage in the Las Vegas casinos. For movie-makers and writers, special or savant
skills have the obvious attraction of making the character exciting, exceptional and
exotically different, but as you have learned in earlier weeks, such skills are by no
means representative.
In a recent portrayal of autism in fiction, Stieg Larsson, the author of The Girl with the
Dragon Tattoo, takes the unusual and important step of portraying a female character
who is strongly implied to have autism. The character Lisbeth Salander contrasts
strongly with Raymond Babbitt in her independence, autonomy and capacity for
deception. Yet the motif of special powers of memory and exceptional skills – IT skills,
as befits the era – still surfaces in this portrayal.
A recent study (Nordahl-Hansen et al., 2017) compared portrayals of the autism
spectrum in 26 films and four television series with the core symptoms in the DSM-5
criteria. Encouragingly, they found that most of the portrayals aligned well with the
diagnostic criteria. However, there was still an undue emphasis on savant
characteristics. The authors also expressed concern that the characters tended to be

162 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183
Week 8: Reflecting back, looking forward
4 Autism in society

stereotypically autistic, thus failing to portray the rich variation and individuality of
autism
A Guardian article about the BBC series The A Word offers an interesting critique by
parent Simon Hattenstone and his autistic daughter Maya (Hattenstone and
Hattenstone, 2016).
An important step towards giving autism an authentic voice in the media was taken by
the TV series Holby City. In 2016, the series introduced an autistic character, Jason
Haynes, played by the young autistic actor, Jules Robertson.

4.2 ‘Coming out’


There are other ways in which the media can help to present authentic and positive
images of autism. A small but growing number of personalities with successful media
careers have chosen to acknowledge their autism. One such person is TV presenter and
wildlife expert Chris Packham, who struggled for years with depression and a sense of
being different from others. As a child he became passionately attached to animals,
secretly hand-rearing a kestrel at home. The loss of this bird caused him trauma, and in
adult life he suffered a deep depression on the death of his dog. It was after therapy that
he was diagnosed with Asperger syndrome at the age of 42. He now says that although
he worked hard to acquire neurotypical traits, such as eye contact for the sake of his TV
appearances, he believes that Asperger syndrome is an important part of him which has
supported and enhanced his career:

Managing my autism on national television still requires an enormous effort.


Sometimes I fail, I do just go off on one. But I realise now there is no way I could
do my job without Asperger's.
What I do in terms of making programmes is afforded to me because of my
neurological differences. Being able to see things with perhaps a greater clarity,
being able to see the world in a very visual way.
(Chris Packham: Asperger’s and Me, 2017)

Susan Boyle, the Scottish singer who achieved fame on the TV show Britain’s Got Talent,
was diagnosed with Asperger syndrome in 2013. Like Chris Packham, she struggled with
many years of adversity before receiving her diagnosis, and like him, her willingness to
make her diagnosis public has offered a positive image of what people on the spectrum
can achieve.
Of course, many autistic people are achieving success and fulfilment in their own fields.
The ‘celebrity’ life stories mentioned above bring autism to wider public awareness, and
are helpful because they are honest and authentic.

4.2 Making society autism-friendly


In the following activity you will think about challenges that autistic people may face in
everyday life.

163 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183
Week 8: Reflecting back, looking forward
4 Autism in society

Activity 4 Challenges of autism in everyday life


Allow about 10 minutes

Think of the activities that many people engage in regularly in their daily lives. These
might include travelling on public transport for work or leisure, shopping for food, or
going to the cinema or a sports hall. List three kinds of challenges that an autistic child
or adult might experience in doing these or similar activities. If you are on the spectrum
yourself, or have autism in the family, this will not be difficult. If not, think about what
you have learned in previous weeks.

Provide your answer...

Discussion
Sensory issues
Travelling on public transport or visiting a cinema or sports hall may induce unfamiliar
or painfully loud noises. Shopping for oneself or with a parent may also involve noise
as well as unpalatable smells, bright lights or confusing visual displays.
Social and communication issues
On a train or bus, or in a cinema or sports facility, an autistic child or adult may find the
physical proximity to others anxiety-provoking and difficult to deal with. Instructions
(e.g. for purchasing travel or cinema tickets) may be confusing. The person may be too
shy to seek help from staff or other passengers when needed.
Structure, routine and decision-making
Travelling (e.g. to go on holiday) involves disruption to familiar routines. Shopping
involves planning and making decisions about products, all of which may seem
overwhelming to an autistic person.

Problems like these often serve to exclude autistic people from society: they may be too
anxious to go out, travel or shop. Parents may find the prospect of a holiday with an
autistic child daunting and end up staying at home. Even going shopping may be an
ordeal for both parent and child.
Several UK organisations work to overcome such barriers to inclusion. The National
Autistic Society (NAS) has established the Autism Friendly Award scheme. Those
managing public spaces and amenities are invited to submit evidence of their
‘friendliness’ to autistic people in terms of five criteria, including provision of clear
information accessible to autistic users of the service, well-trained staff and volunteers,
and a physical environment that minimises stressors such as a confusing layout and
noise.
Organisations that have received the Autism Friendly Award include several UK airports,
theatres, museums and sports stadia; a handful of opticians and supermarkets; and the
buildings of the Northern Ireland and Welsh Assemblies and Scottish Parliament.
A separate charity, The Autism Directory, operates its own listing of autism-friendly places,
including shops, hairdressers, pubs and restaurants (The Autism Directory, 2017). An
increasing number of cinema chains are providing autism-friendly screenings, for instance
without advertising or trailers, lighting kept on low, reduced volume and other
adjustments. Organisations such as Auticon and Specialisterne, which you read about in

164 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183
Week 8: Reflecting back, looking forward
4 Autism in society

Week 7, work specifically to place autistic people in appropriate jobs, and to ensure that
their working environment is autism-friendly.

165 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183
Week 8: Reflecting back, looking forward
5 Autism in a global perspective

5 Autism in a global perspective


There are big disparities between the current situation of autistic people in developed
countries and elsewhere. Despite major gaps and deficiencies in UK infrastructure and
services, there is much progress compared to several decades ago, and as outlined this
week many positive initiatives are ongoing. There follows a summary of some key
advances in the UK, before a discussion of priorities and initiatives for some LMICs where
resources are scarce.

5.1 Autism in the 21st century in the UK


These bullet points summarise some of the key advances in the autism field in the UK
during the 21st century:

● Autism diagnosis now follows internationally agreed criteria and standard procedures
– it did not do so in the 1960s. However, there are deficiencies and delays in the
accessibility of diagnosis.
● A range of educational strategies and interventions is in use, with some measure of
success. Again, though, access to schools and specialist centres with the resources
to implement best practice is often extremely patchy.
● Research has provided insights into cognition, behavior, brain function and genetics
in autism, as well as long-term outcomes. Yet there are huge gaps in this
understanding, concerns about funding priorities and calls to inform research from an
autistic perspective
● There is recognition that autism often goes undiagnosed, notably in females (Gould
and Ashton-Smith, 2011), and also more widely in the adult population. Rectifying
this problem, and meeting the needs of autistic adults generally, remain significant
challenges.
● There have been significant changes in public awareness and perceptions of autism
in which autistic people have played a key role. However, there is still ignorance and
stigma, and as you have seen, the media has played a mixed role.
● Autism legislation such as the UK Autism Act 2009 and the Autism Act (Northern
Ireland) 2011 are positive steps. However, the NAS and Autism NI joint report,
Broken Promises, highlights failure to implement the autism strategy and action plan
set out in legislation (Stewart, 2016). Similarly, The Autism Dividend, published by
the National Autism Project (Iemmi et al., 2017), documents the UK-wide failure to
deliver services and to source evidence-based, cost-effective interventions.

5.2 Autism in Lower and Middle Income Countries


The situation for autistic people and their families in many LMICs reflects, at considerably
greater scale, the problems experienced in developed areas of the world, but with
additional cultural, political and economic challenges. For instance, health priorities may
focus elsewhere on problems such as infant mortality, malaria and AIDS. Public
awareness and professional understanding of autism is usually limited. Even where
diagnosis is available, it may be hampered by inadequate professional training.

166 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183
Week 8: Reflecting back, looking forward
5 Autism in a global perspective

As you saw in Week 3, there are differences across cultures in expectations about
development and behavior. For instance, in parts of the world where eye contact between
children and adults is culturally inappropriate, a lack of eye contact is unlikely to cause
concern (Matson et al., 2011). This means that diagnostic criteria and tools based on
Western ‘norms’ of child behaviour may be inappropriate for different cultures. Prithvi
Perepa comments here on the importance of taking cultural context into account in
identifying autism:

Video content is not available in this format.

Another problem is that fear of stigma may cause parents in some societies to conceal
their children and avoid or delay seeking help (Kishore and Basu, 2011). The few
diagnosed cases in such settings tend to be the children of more affluent parents, and to
be ‘obvious’ or classic cases, while high-functioning children are likely to be overlooked.
This impacts on overall public understanding of autism in these societies: as in the UK in
the 1960s, autism tends to be known only as a profoundly disabling condition.
A recent worldwide autism prevalence estimate of 0.6 per cent, falling well below
estimates for developed countries, most probably reflects sparse diagnosis rather than
worldwide differences in the incidence of autism (Elsabbagh et al., 2012). Wallace et al.
(2012) advocated internationally coordinated efforts to tackle autism worldwide. Some
recent key milestones towards this global initiative include resolutions by the United
Nations (2007) and World Health Organisation (2014) and the Autism Speaks ‘Global
Autism Public Health’ (GAPH) initiative (2008).
To illustrate why such global initiatives are necessary, we will look briefly at autism in
Ethiopia, and at a collaborative research and training initiative involving an Open
University (OU) team including Ilona Roth, lead author of this course.

5.3 Autism in Ethiopia


Ethiopia is in sub-Saharan Africa, with a population of almost 100 million, of whom around
50 per cent are children, and 85 per cent based in rural areas. Despite strong growth in
the last decade, Ethiopia remains one of the poorest countries in the world.
In 2012, Dr Rosa Hoekstra (now at Kings College London) led a team of specialists from
the OU and Addis Ababa University (AAU) on the first research project on autism in
Ethiopia. To ensure practical benefits, development of training materials was integral to
the project. Previously the OU had provided health training resources for the rurally based
health extension workers (HEWs) who, for most Ethiopians in rural areas, provide the
only form of health service. These earlier training materials included a brief discussion of
mental health and just a few sentences about autism.
The team conducted interviews with the few service providers in the capital Addis Ababa
and looked at public documentation (Tekola et al., 2016). The emerging picture mirrored
the broad picture already outlined for LMICs, highlighting low levels of awareness and
high levels of stigma among the public, limited specialist training, sparse mental health
services, and inadequate funding in the context of other health priorities.
The two schools for autistic children were both set up in Addis Ababa by parents of autistic
boys, among the very few children to have been diagnosed. After finding that no school
would take her own autistic son, in 2003 Zemi Yenus established the Nia Foundation Joy

167 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183
Week 8: Reflecting back, looking forward
5 Autism in a global perspective

Center for Autism, which now provides for around 80 children and offers advice, support
and training to parents.

Figure 1 (left) Zemi Yenus; (right) The Joy Center for Autism.
Another parent, Rahel Abayneh, established the Nehemiah Center in 2010, also in Addis
Ababa and catering for around 60 children. Both parents campaign vigorously to raise
public awareness, combat stigma and mobilise practitioners and policy-makers. As of
2018, two more autism schools have opened outside the capital.

Figure 2 (left) Rahel Abayneh; (right) The Nehemiah Center.

5.4 Mental Health Pocket Guide and training videos


Continuing their research in rural areas, the team found that stigma is common, including
among HEWs themselves (Tilahun et al., 2016; 2017). Stigma may be directed at the
autistic individual and their family, or experienced by the family without the specific actions
of others – for instance, a family may feel shame or guilt at their child’s slow and atypical
development. In the majority of cases where autism remains undiagnosed and the label
itself is unfamiliar, stigma may arise nonetheless because a child’s slow development and
unusual behaviour marks them out as different. Certain traditional beliefs may have a
stigmatising effect: children may be thought possessed by a spirit, and parents may
believe they are being punished for a sin. One HEW gave the following account:

I have got one child in our survey; he does not talk. His parents were hiding
information about him. They thought that this type of disease is cured through
traditional or spiritual means. They said [his illness was] due to spirit
possession – likift – because someone had given him some potion. When I saw
the child he was very pale and […] chained.
(Tilahun et al., 2016, p. 7)

The research demonstrated that the HEWs would benefit from more targeted training
(Tilahun et al., 2017). In response, the team produced a guide to autism, intellectual
disability and other child and adult mental health problems, including tips on supporting
parents and families. In addition, the team produced five interview training videos for
autism and intellectual disability.
Versions of these materials now form a free OpenLearn Create resources pack on mental
health, available on this page: Mental Health: Resources for Community Health Workers.

168 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183
Week 8: Reflecting back, looking forward
5 Autism in a global perspective

Ongoing initiatives in Ethiopia include the development of a culturally appropriate


diagnostic instrument, and the trial of an intervention that can be implemented by parents
with their children.

5.5 Pooling resources and practices


Looking at autism in a less well-resourced country serves to emphasise that, despite
many outstanding problems and challenges, resources available in Western countries
such as the UK have facilitated progress. Building on this, international research teams
can share knowledge and skills, thus promoting global exchange of insights and
practices. As Prithvi Perepa emphasises, this is not a one-way process: despite low
resources, non-Western communities may offer progressive models from which much can
be learned:

Video content is not available in this format.

169 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183
Week 8: Reflecting back, looking forward
6 End of course quiz

6 End of course quiz


Now try the End of course quiz and earn yourself a digital badge.

170 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183
Week 8: Reflecting back, looking forward
7 End of course summary

7 End of course summary


The final week of Understanding autism has encouraged you to reflect on what you have
learned in previous weeks, using this knowledge to consider where we are now with
autism and what challenges and priorities there are for the future, whether in the UK or
globally.
The final words go to Alex:

Video content is not available in this format.

You should now be able to:

● evaluate how your own knowledge and understanding has increased while studying
this course
● appreciate both shared problems and differences in the lived experience and needs
of autistic people
● discuss trends and priorities in autism research and support
● identify wider societal issues that affect the lives of those with autism and their
families
● understand some of the priorities for autism in low resource settings.

Tell us what you think


Now you have completed the course, take a few moments to reflect on your experience of
working through this BOC and what you have learned. Please complete the optional survey
to tell us about your reflections. Your responses will allow us to gauge how useful you have
found the BOC and how effectively you have engaged with the content. We will also use
your feedback to better inform the design of future online experiences for our learners.
Many thanks for your help.

References
American Psychiatric Association (2013) Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental
disorders: DSM-5. Washington, D.C., American Psychiatric Association.
Asperger, A. (1944) ‘Die ‘Autistischen Psychopathen’ in Kindersalter’, Archive fűr
Psychiatrie und Nervenkrankheiten, vol. 117, pp. 76–136. Translated from German by U.
Frith (1991) ‘Autism and Asperger Syndrome’, Cambridge, Cambridge University Press.
Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M. and Frith, U. (1985) ‘Does the autistic child have a ‘theory
of mind’?’, Cognition, vol. 21, pp. 37–46.
Bettelheim, B. (1967) The Empty Fortress: Infantile Autism and the Birth of the Self, New
York, Free Press.

171 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183
Week 8: Reflecting back, looking forward
References

Blume, H. (1998) ‘Neurodiversity’, The Atlantic [Online]. Available at https://www.


theatlantic.com/magazine/archive/1998/09/neurodiversity/305909/ (Accessed 12 Feb-
ruary 2018).
Czech, H. (2018) ‘Hans Asperger, National Socialism and “race hygiene” in Nazi-era
Vienna’, Molecular Autism, vol. 9, no. 29 [Online]. Available at https://molecularautism.
biomedcentral.com/articles/10.1186/s13229-018-0208-6 (Accessed 31 May 2018).
Claiborne Park, C. (2001) Exiting Nirvana: A Daughter’s Life with Autism, Boston, Little,
Brown and Company.
Folstein, S. and Rutter, M. (1977) ‘Genetic influences and infantile autism’, Nature,
vol. 265, no. 5596, pp. 726–8.
Grandin, T. and Scariano, M. M. (1986) Emergence: Labeled Autistic, Novato, Arena
Press.
Hermelin, B. and O’Connor, N. (1970) Psychological Experiments with Autistic Children,
Oxford, Pergamon Press.
Kanner, L. (1943) ‘Autistic disturbances of affective contact’, The Nervous Child, vol. 2,
pp. 217–50.
Lawson, W. and Roth, I. (2011) ‘Wendy Lawson in conversation with Ilona Roth’, audio
recording from Open University module SK124 Understanding the autism spectrum. Full
transcript of conversation available here.
Lovaas, O. I. (1987) ‘Behavioural treatment and normal educational and intellectual
functioning in young autistic children’, Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
vol. 55, pp. 3–9.
Mukhopadhyay, T. R. (2000) Beyond the Silence: My Life, the World and Autism, London,
National Autistic Society.
Rimland, B. (1964) Infantile Autism: the Syndrome and Its Implications for a Neural
Theory of Behaviour, Edelson, S. (ed.), London, Jessica Kingsley Publishers (50th
anniversary edition 2014).
Roth, I., with Barson, C., Hoekstra, R. A., Pasco, G. and Whatson, T. (2010) The Autism
Spectrum in the 21st Century: Exploring Psychology, Biology and Practice, London,
Jessica Kingsley Publishers.
Sheffer, E. (2018) Asperger’s Children: The Origins of Autism in Nazi Vienna, New York,
W.W. Norton.
Wing, L. and Gould, J. (1979) ‘Severe impairments of social interaction and associated
abnormalities in children: epidemiology and classification’, Journal of Autism and
Developmental Disorders, vol. 9, pp. 11–29.
Adams, J. (2016) ‘Jon Adams on autism, diagnosis and creativity’ [Online]. Available at
http://network.autism.org.uk/knowledge/insight-opinion/jon-adams-autism-diagnosis-and-
creativity (Accessed 7 February 2018).
Barton, M. (2012) It's Raining Cats and Dogs, London, Jessica Kingsley Publishers.
Brooks-Kayal, A. (2010) ‘Epilepsy and autism spectrum disorders: are there common
developmental mechanisms?’, Brain and Development, vol. 32 (9), pp. 731–738.
Grove, R., Roth, I., and Hoekstra, R. A. (2016) ‘The motivation for special interests in
individuals with autism and controls: Development and validation of the special interest
motivation scale’, Autism Research, vol. 9 (2), pp. 677–688 [Online]. Available at https://
onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/full/10.1002/aur.1560 (Accessed 29 March 2018).
Hickey, S. (2015) ‘The innovators matching autistic people with jobs needing special
skills’, Guardian, 19 April [Online]. Available at https://www.theguardian.com/business/

172 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183
Week 8: Reflecting back, looking forward
References

2015/apr/19/the-innovators-matching-autistic-people-with-jobs-needing-special-skills
(Accessed 7 February 2018).
Lawson, W. and Roth, I. (2011) ‘In conversation’, audio recording from Open University
module SK124 Understanding the autism spectrum. Full transcript of conversation
available here.
Lawson, W. (2018) Dr. Wenn B. Lawson [Online]. Available at http://www.build-
somethingpositive.com/wenn/adulthood.html (Accessed 6 February 2018).
National Autistic Society (2018a) Related conditions [Online]. Available at http://www.
autism.org.uk/about/what-is/related-conditions.aspx (Accessed 8 February 2018).
National Autistic Society (2018b) Mental health and autism [Online]. Available at http://
www.autism.org.uk/about/health/mental-health.aspx (Accessed 8 February 2018).
Paradiž, V. (2002) Elijah’s Cup, New York, The Free Press.
Roth, I. (2007) ‘Autism and the imaginative mind’ in Roth, I. (ed.) Imaginative Minds:
Concepts, Controversies and Themes, Oxford, Oxford University Press.
Roth, I., with Barson, C., Hoekstra, R. A., Pasco, G. and Whatson, T. (2010) The Autism
Spectrum in the 21st Century: Exploring Psychology, Biology and Practice, London,
Jessica Kingsley Publishers.
Sainsbury, C. (2000) Martian in the Playground: Understanding the Schoolchild with
Asperger’s syndrome, Bristol, Lucky Duck Publishing.
Treffert, D. (2006) Extraordinary People: Understanding Savant Syndrome, Lincoln,
iUniverse.
Treffert, D. (2009) ‘The savant syndrome: an extraordinary condition. A synopsis: past,
present, future’, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, vol. 364, pp. 1351-58
[Online]. Available at http://rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org/content/364/1522/1351 (Ac-
cessed 8 February 2018).
Viscidi, E. W., Triche, E. W., Pescosolido, M. F., McLean, R. L., Joseph, R. M., Spence, S.
J., Morrow, E. M. (2013) ‘Clinical characteristics of children with autism spectrum disorder
and co-occurring epilepsy’, PLoS One, 4;8(7): e67797 [Online]. Available at http://
journals.plos.org/plosone/article?id=10.1371/journal.pone.0067797 (Accessed 8 Feb-
ruary 2018).
The Autism Puzzle (2003) BBC, 15 January. Available from director Saskia Baron at
https://vimeo.com/20748434 (Accessed 10 July 2018).
American Psychiatric Association (2013) Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental
disorders: DSM-5. Washington, D.C., American Psychiatric Association.
Barbaro, J., and Dissanyake, C. (2013) ‘Early markers of autism spectrum disorders in
infants and toddlers prospectively identified in the Social Attention and Communication
Study’, Autism, vol. 17(1), pp. 64–86.
Begeer, S. S., El Bouk, S. E., Boussaid, W., Terwoght, M. M. and Koot, H. M. (2009)
‘Underdiagnosis and referral bias of autism in ethnic minorities’, Journal of Autism and
Developmental Disorders, vol. 39, no. 1, pp. 142–8.
Chawarska, K., Rhea, P., Klin, A., Hannigen, S., Dichtel, L. E., Volkmar, F. (2007)
‘Parental recognition of developmental problems in toddlers with autism spectrum
disorders’, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, vol. 37, no. 1, pp. 62–72.
Dobbs, D. (2017) Rethinking regression in autism [Online]. Available at https://
spectrumnews.org/features/deep-dive/rethinking-regression-autism (Accessed 9 Feb-
ruary 2018).

173 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183
Week 8: Reflecting back, looking forward
References

Elsabbagh, M., Divan, G., Koh, Y.-J., Kim, Y. S., Kauchali, S., Marcín, C., Montiel-Nava,
C., Patel, V., Paula, C. S., Wang, C., Yasamy, M. T. and Fombonne, E. (2012), ‘Global
Prevalence of Autism and Other Pervasive Developmental Disorders’. Autism Research,
vol. 5, pp. 160–179 [Online]. Available at https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/full/10.1002/
aur.239 (Accessed 29 March 2018).
Lai, M.-C., Lombardo, M. V., Auyeung, B., Chakrabarti, B., Baron-Cohen, S. (2015) ‘Sex/
gender differences and autism: Setting the scene for future research’, Journal of the
American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, vol. 54(1), pp. 11–24.
Maestro, S. and Muratori, F. (2008) ‘How young children with autism treat objects and
people: some insight into autism in infancy from research in home movies’, in McGregor,
E., Núñez, M., Cebula, K. and Gómez, J. C. (eds) Autism: An integrated view from
neuroscience, clinical, and intervention research, Oxford, Blackwell Publishing, pp. 170-
–92.
National Autistic Society (n.d.) Autism facts and history [Online]. Available at http://www.
autism.org.uk/about/what-is/myths-facts-stats.aspx (Accessed 12 February 2018).
National Initiative for Autism: Screening and Assessment (NIASA) (2003) National Autism
Plan for Children: Plan for the identification, assessment, diagnosis and access to early
interventions for pre-school and primary school aged children with autism spectrum
disorders (ASD), London, National Autistic Society.
Norbury, C. F., and Sparks, A. (2013) ‘Difference or disorder? Cultural issues in
understanding neurodevelopmental disorders’, Developmental Psychology, vol. 49(1),
pp. 45-58.
Willey, L. H. (1999) Pretending to be Normal: Living with Asperger’s Syndrome, London,
Jessica Kingsley Publishers.
World Health Organisation (1992) ‘International Statistical Classification of Diseases and
Related Health Problems: ICD-10’, Geneva, World Health Organisation.
Young, R. L., Brewer, L., and Pattison, C. (2003) ‘Parental identification of early
behavioural abnormalities in children with autistic disorder’ Autism, vol. 7(2), pp. 125-143.
Zeldovich, L., (2017) New global diagnostic manual mirrors U.S. autism criteria [Online].
Available at https://spectrumnews.org/news/new-global-diagnostic-manual-mirrors-u-s-
autism-criteria/ (Accessed 11 February 2018).
Autism Research Centre (2018) Downloadable Tests [Online]. Available at https://www.
autismresearchcentre.com/arc_tests (Accessed 3 May 2018).
Baron-Cohen, S. (2009) ‘Autism: The Empathising-Systemising (E-S) Theory’, Annals of
the New York Academy of Science, vol. 1156, pp. 68–80.
Baron-Cohen, S., Cassidy, S., Auyeung, B., Allison, C., Achoukhi, M. et al. (2014)
‘Attenuation of Typical Sex Differences in 800 Adults with Autism vs. 3,900 Controls’,
PLoS ONE, vol. 9, no. 7: e102251 [Online]. Available at http://journals.plos.org/plosone/
article?id=10.1371/journal.pone.0102251 (Accessed 3 May 2018).
Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M. and Frith, U. (1985) ‘Does the autistic child have a ‘theory
of mind’?’ Cognition, vol. 21, pp. 37–46.
Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Hill, J., Raste, Y. and Plumb, I. (2001) ‘The “Reading
the Mind in the Eyes” Test Revised Version: A Study with Normal Adults, and Adults with
Asperger Syndrome or High-functioning Autism’, The Journal of Child Psychology and
Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, vol. 42, no. 2, pp. 241-251 [Online]. Available at https://
onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/abs/10.1111/1469-7610.00715 (Accessed 3 May 2018).

174 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183
Week 8: Reflecting back, looking forward
References

Christensen, J., Grønborg, T. K., Sørensen, M. J., Schendel, D., Parner, E. T., Pedersen,
L. H., & Vestergaard, M. (2013). Prenatal valproate exposure and risk of autism spectrum
disorders and childhood autism. JAMA, 309(16), 1696–1703. doi:10.1001/
jama.2013.2270.
Craig, J. and Baron-Cohen, S. (1999) ‘Creativity and imagination in autism and Asperger
syndrome’, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, vol. 29, no. 4, pp. 319–26.
Demetriou, E. A., Lampit, A., Quintana, D. S., Naismith, S. L., Song, Y. J. C., Pye, J. E.,
Hickie, I. and Guastella, A. J. (2017) ‘Autism spectrum disorders: a meta-analysis of
executive function’, Molecular Psychiatry, vol. 23, no. 5, pp. 1198–1204 [Online]. Available
at www.nature.com/articles/mp201775 (Accessed 3 May 2018).
de Villiers, J. (2000) ‘Language and theory of mind: What are the developmental
relationships?’, in S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, & D. J. Cohen (eds) Under-
standing other minds: Perspectives from developmental cognitive neuroscience, New
York, NY, US: Oxford University Press, pp 83–123.
Frith, U. (1989) Autism: Explaining the Enigma, Oxford: Blackwell.
Golan, O., Baron-Cohen, S., Hill, J. J. and Golan, Y. (2006) ‘The ‘Reading the Mind in
Films’ task: complex emotion recognition in adults with and without autism spectrum
conditions’, Social Neuroscience, vol. 1, pp. 111–23.
Happé, F. G. E. (1994) ‘An advanced test of theory of mind: understanding of
storycharacters’ thoughts and feelings by able autistic, mentally handicapped and
normalchildren and adults’, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, vol. 24, no.
2, pp. 129–54.
Happé, F. G. E. and Booth, R. D. L. (2008) ‘The Power of the Positive: Revisiting Weak
Coherence in Autism Spectrum Disorders’, Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,
vol. 61, no. 1, pp. 50–63.
Hughes, V. (2012) ‘Infants who develop autism show distinct brain connectivity’,
Spectrum, 20 February [Online]. Available at spectrumnews.org/news/infants-who-
develop-autism-show-distinct-brain-connectivity/ (Accessed 21 February 2018).
Jackson, L. (2002) Freaks, Geeks & Asperger Syndrome: A User Guide to Adolescence,
London, Jessica Kingsley Publishers.
Lai, M. C., Lombardo, M. V. and Baron-Cohen, S. (2013) ‘Autism’, Lancet, vol. 383, no.
9920, pp. 896–910 [Online]. Available at www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/
S0140673613615391?via%3Dihub (Accessed 19 February 2018).
Lawson, W. and Roth, I. (2011) ‘In conversation’, audio recording from Open University
module SK124 Understanding the autism spectrum. Full transcript of conversation
available here.
Mandy, W., Chilvers, R. and Chowdhury, U. (2012) ‘Sex Differences in Autism Spectrum
Disorder: Evidence from a Large Sample of Children and Adolescents’, Journal of Autism
and Developmental Disorders, vol. 42, pp. 1304-1313 [Online]. Available at https://link.
springer.com/article/10.1007%2Fs10803-011-1356-0 (Accessed 3 May 2018).
Sainsbury, C. (2000) The Martian in the Playground: Understanding the Schoolchild with
Asperger’s Syndrome, Bristol, Lucky Duck Publishing.
Subbaraman, N. (2014) ‘Study on ‘extreme male brain’ theory of autism draws critics’,
Spectrum, 25 August [Online]. Available at spectrumnews.org/news/study-on-extreme-
male-brain-theory-of-autism-draws-critics/ (Accessed 12 February 2018).
Tomchek, S. D., Lauren, M., Little, L. M., Myers, J. and Dunn, W. (2018) ‘Sensory
Subtypes in Preschool Aged Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder’, Journal of Autism

175 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183
Week 8: Reflecting back, looking forward
References

and Developmental Disorders, vol. 48, no. 6, pp. 2139–47 [Online]. Available at https://
link.springer.com/article/10.1007/s10803-018-3468-2 (Accessed 15 February 2018).
DeVita-Raeburn, E. (2016) The controversy over autism’s most common therapy [Online].
Available at https://spectrumnews.org/features/deep-dive/controversy-autisms-common-
therapy (Accessed 16 March 2018).
Divan, G. (2017) Novel autism treatment translates well to South Asian nations [Online].
Available at https://spectrumnews.org/opinion/viewpoint/novel-autism-treatment-trans-
lates-well-south-asian-nations (Accessed 15 March 2018).
Fein, D., Barton, M., Eigsti, I.-M., Kelley, E., Naigles, L., Schultz, R. T., Stevens, M., Helt,
M., Orinstein, A., Rosenthal, M., Troyb, E., Tyson, K. (2013) ‘Optimal outcome in
individuals with a history of autism’, Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, vol. 54,
no. 2 pp. 195–205 [Online]. Available at https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/full/10.1111/
jcpp.12037 (Accessed 16 March 2018).
Green, J., Charman, T., McConachie, H., Aldred, C., Slonims, V., Howlin, P., Le Couteur,
A., Leadbitter, K., Hudry, K., Byford, S., Barrett, B., Temple, K., Macdonald, W., Pickles, A.
(2010) ‘Parent-mediated communication-focused treatment in children with autism
(PACT): a randomised controlled trial’, Lancet, vol. 375 (9732), pp. 2152–2160 [Online].
Available at https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2890859/ (Accessed 16
March 2018).
Howlin, P., Magiati, I., and Charman, T. (2009) ‘Systematic review of early intensive
behavioral interventions for children with autism’, American Journal on Intellectual and
Developmental Disabilities, vol. 114, no. 1, pp. 23–41 [Online]. Available at https://www.
ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19143460 (Accessed 16 March 2018).
Moore, C. (2003) ‘Mind the gap’, Guardian, 14 May [Online]. Available at http://www.
guardian.co.uk/lifeandstyle/2003/may/14/familyandrelationships.features10 (Accessed 1
March 2018).
National Institute for Health and Care Excellence (2013) Autism spectrum disorder in
under 19s: support and management [Online]. Available at https://www.nice.org.uk/
guidance/cg170/chapter/1-Recommendations#interventions-for-autism-that-should-not-
be-used (Accessed 14 March 2018).
O’Haire, M. E. (2013) ‘Animal-Assisted Intervention for Autism Spectrum Disorder: A
Systematic Literature Review’, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, vol. 43,
no. 7, pp. 1606–1622 [Online]. Available at https://link.springer.com/article/10.1007%
2Fs10803-012-1707-5 (Accessed 16 March 2018).
Peters-Scheffer, N., Didden, R., Korzilius, H., Sturmey, P. (2011) ‘A meta-analytic study on
the effectiveness of comprehensive ABA-based early intervention programs for children
with Autism Spectrum Disorders’, Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, vol. 5.
pp. 60–69 [Online]. Available at https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/
S1750946710000498?via%3Dihub (Accessed 16 March 2018).
Research Autism (2018) Packing therapy [Online]. Available at www.researchautism.net/
glossary/541/packing-therapy (Accessed 31 August 2018).
Research Autism (2018) Research autism home page [Online]. Available at http://www.
researchautism.net (Accessed 15 March 2018).
Research Autism (2018) Our evaluations of autism interventions, treatments and
therapies [Online]. Available at http://www.researchautism.net/autism-interventions/our-
evaluations-interventions/ (Accessed 15 March 2018).

176 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183
Week 8: Reflecting back, looking forward
References

Bachman, C. (2016) ‘Young woman with autism asks: “Am I so different?”’, Spectrum, 26
December [Online]. Available at https://spectrumnews.org/opinion/viewpoint/young-wo-
man-autism-asks-different (Accessed 7 November 2017).
Beardon, L. and Edmonds, G. (2007) The ASPECT Consultancy Report: A national report
on the needs of adults with Asperger syndrome, Sheffield Hallam University, The Autism
Centre [Online]. Available at https://www.sheffield.ac.uk/polopoly_fs/1.34791!/file/AS-
PECT_Consultancy_report.pdf (Accessed 7 November 2017).
Esbensen, A. J., Bishop, S. L., Seltzer, M. M., Greenberg, J. S. and Taylor, J. L. (2010)
‘Comparisons between individuals with autism spectrum disorders and individuals with
Down syndrome in adulthood’, American Journal of Intellectual and Developmental
Disabilities, vol. 115, no. 4, pp. 277–90 [Online]. Available at https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/
pmc/articles/PMC2885822 (Accessed 20 April 2018).
Elliott, J. (2006) ‘“I have four autistic sons and no sleep”’, BBC News, 28 April [Online].
Available at http://news.bbc.co.uk/1/hi/health/4950668.stm (Accessed 7 No-
vember 2017).
Hillman, J. (2007) ‘Grandparents of Children with Autism: A Review with Recommenda-
tions for Education, Practice, and Policy’, Educational Gerontology, vol. 33 no. 6,
pp. 513-527 [Online]. Available at https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1080/
03601270701328425 (Accessed 20 April 2018).
Humphrey, N. and Symes, W. (2010) ‘Perceptions of social support and experience of
bullying among pupils with autistic spectrum disorders in mainstream secondary schools’,
European Journal of Special Needs Education, vol. 25, no. 1, pp. 77-91 [Online]. Available
at https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1080/08856250903450855 (Accessed 20
April 2018).
Moore, C. (2003) ‘“The amount of adult time I have is nil”’, The Guardian, 30 July [Online].
Available at https://www.theguardian.com/society/2003/jul/30/childrensservices.medici-
neandhealth (Accessed 7 November 2017).
Moore, C. (2012) ‘Our lives shaped by autism’, The Guardian, 24 March [Online].
Available at https://www.theguardian.com/lifeandstyle/2012/mar/24/charlotte-moore-au-
tistic-sons (Accessed 7 November 2017).
National Autistic Society (n.d.) ‘Autism services directory’ [Online]. Available at http://
www.autism.org.uk/directory.aspx (Accessed 7 November 2017).
National Autistic Society (n.d.) ‘Parent to Parent Service’ [Online]. Available at http://www.
autism.org.uk/services/helplines/parent-to-parent.aspx (Accessed 21 April 2018)
Preece, D. (2014) ‘A matter of perspective: the experience of daily life and support of
mothers, fathers and siblings living with children on the autism spectrum with high support
needs’, Good Autism Practice, vol. 15, no. 1, pp. 81-90.
Sainsbury, C. (2000) Martian in the Playground: Understanding the Schoolchild with
Asperger’s Syndrome, Bristol, Lucky Duck Books.
Stevens, J. and Shields, J. (2013) ‘Does attending an NAS EarlyBird or EarlyBird Plus
programme make a difference?’, Good Autism Practice, vol. 14, no. 2, pp. 82-89.
Tekola, B., Baheretibeb, Y., Roth, I., Tilahun, D., Fekadu, A., Hanlon, C., Hoekstra, R. A.,
(2016) ‘Challenges and opportunities to improve autism services in low-income countries:
lessons from a situational analysis in Ethiopia’, Global Mental Health, vol. 3 [Online].
Available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/journals/global-mental-health/article/chal-
lenges-and-opportunities-to-improve-autism-services-in-lowincome-countries-lessons-
from-a-situational-analysis-in-ethiopia/2CB31D9AAC4151DE9FA71E471AD4B9CE (Ac-
cessed 20 April 2018).

177 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183
Week 8: Reflecting back, looking forward
References

Baron, M. and Roth, I. (2017) ‘Michael Baron in conversation with Ilona Roth’, presently
unpublished audio recording.
Donvan, J. and Zucker, C. (2010) ‘Autism’s first child’, The Atlantic, October [Online].
Available at http://www.theatlantic.com/magazine/archive/2010/10/autism-8217-s-first-
child/8227/ (Accessed 7 September 2017).
Howlin, P. and Moss, P. (2012) ‘Adults with autism spectrum disorders’, Canadian Journal
of Psychiatry, vol. 57, no. 5, pp. 275–83.
Iemmi, V., Knapp, M. and Ragan, I. (2017) ‘Executive summary’, an extract from The
Autism Dividend: Reaping the Rewards of Better Investment, London, National Autism
Project. Available at http://nationalautismproject.org.uk/wp-content/uploads/2017/01/aut-
ism-dividend-executive-summary.pdf (Accessed 10 September 2017). Full report avail-
able at http://nationalautismproject.org.uk/wp-content/uploads/2017/01/autism-dividend-
report.pdf (Accessed 7 November 2017).
Kanner, L. (1943) ‘Autistic disturbances of affective contact’, Nervous Child, vol. 2,pp.
217–50.
King, C. and Murphy, G.H. (2014) ‘A systematic review of people with autism spectrum
disorder and the criminal justice system’, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
vol. 44, no. 11, pp. 2717–33.
Lawson, W. and Roth, I. (2011) ‘Wendy Lawson in conversation with Ilona Roth’, audio
recording from Open University module SK124 Understanding the autism spectrum. Full
transcript of conversation available here.
Magiati, I., Xiang, W.T., Howlin, P. (2014) ‘Cognitive, language, social and behavioural
outcomes in adults with autism spectrum disorders: a systematic review of longitudinal
follow-up studies in adulthood’, Cognitive Psychology Review, vol. 34, pp. 73–86.
Meiring, M., Seabi, J., Amod, Z., Vorster, A., Kern, A. (2016) ‘Transition for adolescents
with autism spectrum disorder: South African parent and professional perspectives’,
Frontiers in Psychiatry, vol. 7, p. 93.
Moore, C. (2016) ‘School Report 2016’ [Online] Available at http://www.autism.org.uk/
schoolreport2016 (Accessed 11 April 2018)
National Autistic Society (2016) ‘Government must tackle the autism employment gap (27
October 2016)’ [Online]. Available at http://www.autism.org.uk/get-involved/media-centre/
news/2016-10-27-employment-gap.aspx (Accessed 7 September 2017).
Sacks, O. (1995) An Anthropologist on Mars, London, Picador Books.
Slater-Walker, C. and Slater-Walker, G. (2002) An Asperger Marriage, London andNew
York, Jessica Kingsley Publishers.
Suh, J., Orinstein, A., Barton, M., Chen, C.-M., Eigsti, I.-M., Ramirez-Esparza, N., Fein, D.
(2016) ‘Ratings of broader autism phenotype and personality traits in optimal outcomes
from autism spectrum disorder’, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, vol. 46,
no. 11, pp. 3505–18.
van Heijst, B.F.C. and Geurts, H.M. (2015) ‘Quality of life in autism across the life span: A
meta-analysis’, Autism, vol. 19, no. 2, pp. 158–67.
The Autism Directory (2017) Autism friendly [Online]. Available at www.theautismdir-
ectory.com/listing/autism-friendly (Accessed 8 November 2017).
Autistica (2016) Your research priorities [Online]. Available at autistica.org.uk/our-
research/our-research/your-research-priorities (Accessed 8 November 2017).
Baron, M. and Roth, I. (2017) ‘Michael Baron in conversation with Ilona Roth’, presently
unpublished audio recording.

178 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183
Week 8: Reflecting back, looking forward
References

Boustead, G. (2015) ‘Debating the merits of “autism” as a diagnostic category’, Spectrum,


11 March [Online]. Available at spectrumnews.org/opinion/cross-talk/cross-talk-debating-
the-merits-of-autism-as-a-diagnostic-category (Accessed 8 November 2017).
Chris Packham: Asperger’s and Me (2017) BBC 2, 17 October.
Deweerdt, S. (2014) ‘Genetics first: a fresh take on autism’s diversity’, Spectrum, 24
November [Online]. Available at spectrumnews.org/features/deep-dive/genetics-first-a-
fresh-take-on-autisms-diversity (Accessed 8 November 2017).
Elsabbagh, M., Divan, G., Koh, Y. J., Kim, Y. S., Kauchali, S., Marcín, C., Montiel-Nava,
C., Patel, V., Paula, C. S., Wang, C., Yasamy, M. T. and Fombonne, E. (2012) ‘Global
prevalence of autism and other pervasive developmental disorders’, Autism Research,
vol. 5, no. 3, pp. 160–79 [Online]. Available at www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/
PMC3763210 (Accessed 8 November 2017).
Gould, J. and Ashton-Smith, J. (2011) ‘Missed diagnosis or misdiagnosis? Girls and
women on the autism spectrum’, Good Autism Practice, vol. 12, no. 1, pp. 34–41(8).
Grandin, T. (1996) Thinking In Pictures, New York, Vintage Books.
Hattenstone, S. and Hattenstone, M. (2016) ‘The A Word: a father and his autistic
daughter give their verdict’, The Guardian, 22 March [Online]. Available at www.
theguardian.com/tv-and-radio/2016/mar/09/the-a-word-autism-funnier-than-this-tv-drama
(Accessed 18 April 2018).
Iemmi, V., Knapp, M. and Ragan, I. (2017) The Autism Dividend: Reaping the Rewards of
Better Investment, London, National Autism Project [Online]. Available at nationalau-
tismproject.org.uk/wp-content/uploads/2017/01/autism-dividend-report.pdf (Accessed 7
November 2017).
Kishore, M. T. and Basu, A. (2011) ‘Early concerns of mothers of children later diagnosed
with autism: implications for early identification’, Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders,
vol. 5, no. 1, pp. 157–63.
Lawson, W. and Roth, I. (2011) ‘Wendy Lawson in conversation with Ilona Roth’, audio
recording from Open University module SK124 Understanding the autism spectrum. Full
transcript of conversation available here.
Matson, J. L., Worley, J. A., Fodstad, J. C., Chung, K.-M., Suh, D., Jhin, H. K., Ben-
Itzchak, E., Zachor, D. A. and Furniss, F. (2011) ‘A multinational study examining the cross
cultural differences in reported symptoms of autism spectrum disorders: Israel, South
Korea, the United Kingdom, and the United States of America’, Research in Autism
Spectrum Disorders, vol. 5, no. 4, pp. 1598–1604.
Nordahl-Hansen, A., Tøndevold, M. and Fletcher-Watson, S. (2018) ‘Mental health on
screen: a DSM-5 dissection of portrayals of autism spectrum disorders in film and TV’,
Psychiatry Research, vol. 262, pp. 351–3 [Online]. Available at www.sciencedirect.com/
science/article/pii/S0165178117304225?via%3Dihub (Accessed 3 May 2018).
Pellicano, E. and Stears, M. (2011) ‘Bridging autism, science and society: moving towards
an ethically informed approach to autism research’, Autism Research, vol. 4, no. 4,
pp. 271–82.
Stewart, S. (2016) Broken Promises, The National Autistic Society Northern Ireland/
Autism NI [Online]. Available at www.autism.org.uk/get-involved/media-centre/news/
2016-06-01-broken-promises.aspx (Accessed 8 November 2017).
Tekola, B., Baheretibeb, Y., Roth, I., Tilahun, D., Fekadu, A., Hanlon, C. and Hoekstra, R.
A. (2016) ‘Challenges and opportunities to improve autism services in low-income
countries: lessons from a situational analysis in Ethiopia’, Global Mental Health, vol. 3, no.

179 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183
Week 8: Reflecting back, looking forward
Acknowledgements

e21 [Online]. Available at www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5454792/pdf/


S2054425116000170a.pdf (Accessed 7 November 2017).
Tilahun, D., Hanlon, C., Fekadu, A., Tekola, B., Baheretibeb, Y., and Hoekstra, R. A.
(2016) 'Stigma, explanatory models and unmet needs of caregivers of children with
developmental disorders in a low-income African country: a cross-sectional facility-based
survey’, BMC Health Services Research, vol. 16 [Online]. Available at https://
bmchealthservres.biomedcentral.com/articles/10.1186/s12913-016-1383-9 (Accessed 3
May 2018).
Tilahun, D., Fekadu, A., Tekola, B., Araya, M., Roth, I., Davey, B., Hanlon, C., Hoekstra,
R. A. (2017) 'Ethiopian community health workers’ beliefs and attitudes towards children
with autism: impact of a brief training intervention', Autism, 25 September [Online].
Available at http://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1177/1362361317730298 (Accessed 3
May 2018).
Wallace, S., Fein, D., Rosanoff, M., Dawson, G., Hossain, S., Brennan, L., Como, A.,
Shih, A. (2012) ‘A global public health strategy for autism spectrum disorders’, Autism
Research, vol. 5, no. 3, pp. 211–17.
Zeliadt, N, (2017) ‘Partnerships with people on the spectrum yield rich research insights’,
Spectrum, 22 December [Online]. Available at spectrumnews.org/news/partnerships-
people-spectrum-yield-rich-research-insights (Accessed 18 April 2018).

Acknowledgements
This free course was written by Dr Ilona Roth and Dr Nancy Rowell.
Except for third party materials and otherwise stated (see terms and conditions), this
content is made available under a
Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0 Licence.
The material acknowledged below and within the course is Proprietary and used under
licence (not subject to Creative Commons Licence). Grateful acknowledgement is made
to the following sources for permission to reproduce material in this free course:

Week 1

Images
Figure 1: Autism Spectrum
Figure 2: from: US National Library of Medicine
https://collections.nlm.nih.gov/catalog/nlm:nlmuid-101420118-img
Figure 3: Courtesy of the Asperger Estate
Figure 4: Courtesy of Sir Michael Rutter
Figure 5 http://lovaas.com/
Figure 6: Simon Baron-Cohen; https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-sa/3.0/deed.en
Figure 7: © Robert Taylor

Audio/Video

Activity 6: Audio extract from interview with Dr Wenn Lawson by Dr Ilona Roth. © The
Open University

180 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183
Week 8: Reflecting back, looking forward
Acknowledgements

Activity 6: Video: Alex talking to Dr Ilona Roth © The Open University

Week 2

Images

Figure 1: Lisa McKelvie/Photolibrary/Getty Images


Figure 2: © The Open University
Figure 3: from Barton, M. (2012) It's Raining Cats and Dogs Book p43 published by
Jessica Kingsley Publishers www.jkp.com
Figure 4: ANDERS KRUSBERG / PEABODY AWARDS
https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0/deed.en
Figure 5: © Stephen Wiltshire
Figure 6: © Jessica Park
Figure 7: courtesy: Jon Adams
Figure 8: The Open University

Audio/Video

1.0: Video: Arabella about her daughter Iris Grace: © The Open University
Activity 1 (1): courtesy Surrey Autism Board
http://www.surreypb.org.uk/surrey-autism-partnership-board.html
Activity 1 (2) Think Differently about Autism: Misunderstanding: courtesy National Autistic
Society
Activity 1 (3) Think Differently about Autism: Socially awkward: courtesy National Autistic
Society
Video: (3 clips) Arabella talking about Iris Grace © The Open University
Video: (2 clips) Alex talking to Dr Ilona Roth: © The Open University

Week 3

Images
Figure 1: courtesy of Western Psychological Services

Audio/Video
Activity 1: 3.1, 3.2, 3.3, Activity 4: Clips from Autism Puzzle BBC 2 8/4/2003 © BBC 2003
Activity 3: clips from Growing Children, ep 1 13.8.2012 BBC 4 (c) BBC 2012
3.3: Mark and Son Zack: courtesy of National Autistic Society https://www.autism.org.uk/
3.3, Activity 5: Arabella talking to Dr Ilona Roth © The Open University

Week 4

Images
Figure 1: adapted from Frith, U. (1989) Autism: Explaining the Enigma, Oxford: Blackwell.
Figure 2: © Axel Scheffler
Figure 5: © Association for Child Psychology and Psychiatry; taken from the Reading the
Mind in the Eyes: Test Revised Version

181 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183
Week 8: Reflecting back, looking forward
Acknowledgements

Figure 6: © Janne Moren (Flickr) https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/2.0/


Figure 7: courtesy Robert T. Schultz
Figure 8: © Jason Wolff
Figure 9: © Science Photo Library / Alamy Stock Photo
Figure 10: courtesy of Professor Ferguson-Smith

Text

Activity 4: extract from Empathy Quotient (EQ) and Systemising Quotient (SQ) for Adults
(Autism Research Centre 2018)

Audio/Video
Activity 2: © The Open University
1.4 audio: Dr Wenn Lawson ©The Open University
1.4 video: © The Open University
2.2 video (clip 1): courtesy Surrey Autism Board
http://www.surreypb.org.uk/surrey-autism-partnership-board.html
2.2 video (clip 2): courtesy: National Autistic Society https://www.autism.org.uk/

Week 5

Images
Figure 1 Based on source material provided by Gillian Roberts, Principal, Robert Ogden
School, South Yorkshire, UK; Photos: PhotoEuphoria/iStockphoto; Vikram Raghuvanan-
shi/iStockphoto

Audio/Video
1.1 Video: Alex: © The Open University
1.2 Video: Arabella (mother of Iris Grace) © The Open University
5.2: Video: Joe: © The Open University (2005)
6.1 Video: Queensmill © The Open University
7.4 Video Arabella (mother of Iris Grace) © The Open University
9. Video: Arabella (mother of Iris Grace) © The Open University

Week 6

Images

Figure 1: courtesy: Zoe Wood


Figure 2: courtesy: from Earlybird Support Programme: National Autistic Society
https://www.autism.org.uk/

Audio/Video
2.2 two clips © OU
3.1 Video: Alex © The Open University
4.4 Video: Arabella (mother of Iris Grace) © The Open University

182 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183
Week 8: Reflecting back, looking forward
Acknowledgements

Week 7

Audio/Video

2.2: Video Alex: © The Open University


5.2: Video: Alex: © The Open University
9. Video: Alex: © The Open University

Week 8

Images
Figure 1: courtesy Ilona Roth
Figure 2: courtesy Ilona Roth

Audio/Video

2.2: Video: Arabella (mother of Iris Grace) © The Open University


Every effort has been made to contact copyright owners. If any have been inadvertently
overlooked, the publishers will be pleased to make the necessary arrangements at the
first opportunity.
Don’t miss out
If reading this text has inspired you to learn more, you may be interested in joining the
millions of people who discover our free learning resources and qualifications by visiting
The Open University – www.open.edu/openlearn/free-courses.

183 Tuesday 5 November 2019


of 183

You might also like

pFad - Phonifier reborn

Pfad - The Proxy pFad of © 2024 Garber Painting. All rights reserved.

Note: This service is not intended for secure transactions such as banking, social media, email, or purchasing. Use at your own risk. We assume no liability whatsoever for broken pages.


Alternative Proxies:

Alternative Proxy

pFad Proxy

pFad v3 Proxy

pFad v4 Proxy