The Cognitive Gap Between Arithmetic and Algebra
The Cognitive Gap Between Arithmetic and Algebra
REFERENCES
Linked references are available on JSTOR for this article:
https://www.jstor.org/stable/3482666?seq=1&cid=pdf-reference#references_tab_contents
You may need to log in to JSTOR to access the linked references.
JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide
range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and
facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.
Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at
https://about.jstor.org/terms
Springer is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to Educational
Studies in Mathematics
This content downloaded from 62.204.213.86 on Fri, 20 Mar 2020 21:11:13 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
tNICOLAS HERSCOVICS AND LIORA LINCHEVSKI
ABSTRACT. Serious attempts are being made to improve the students' preparation for algebra.
However, without a clear-cut demarcation between arithmetic and algebra, most of these undertakings
merely provide either an earlier introduction of the topic or simply spread it out over a longer period
of instruction. The present study investigates the upper limits of the students' informal processes in
the solution of first degree equations in one unknown prior to any instruction. The results indicate the
existence of a cognitive gap between arithmetic and algebra, a cognitive gap that can be characterized
as the students' inability to operate spontaneously with or on the unknown. Furthermore, the study
reveals other difficulties of a pre-algebraic nature such as a tendency to detach a numeral from the
preceding minus sign in the grouping of numerical terms and problems in the acceptance of the equal
symbol to denote a decomposition into a difference as in 23 = 37 - n which leads some students to
read such equations from right to left.
INTRODUCTION
This content downloaded from 62.204.213.86 on Fri, 20 Mar 2020 21:11:13 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
60 NICOLAS HERSCOVICS AND LIORA LINCHEVSKI
First we find those students who master the traditional curriculum ... and
proceed to a course called pre-algebra in grade 7. Next we find students
not quite as successful, and they spend the middle grades reviewing some
arithmetic in more complex exercises while they wait to enroll in algebra in
the ninth grade. Finally, we observe the unsuccessful students, who stay in
school and are relegated to a complete review of arithmetic. Typically, these
students will never enroll in a course called algebra.
.. The data in the recent international assessment (Travers, 1985) indicates
that about 10 percent of the students in the United States enroll in algebra by
grade 8 and that about 65 percent are in a regular track in algebra by grade 9.
The concern for guaranteeing success in algebra for all students is then directed
towards the lower one-fourth of the student population, who presently do not
even think about algebra.
(Lodholz, 1990,24-25)
We are thus concerned with two main groups of students: those who ente
Algebra 1 and fail or encounter major difficulties, and those who do not even
enroll in an initial course. The first group does reasonably well in arithme
but experiences problems with algebra. For them, the traditional curriculum
arithmetic may not be sufficient and a bridge between arithmetic and algebra
to be constructed. The second group is rejected on the basis of poor performa
in arithmetic. The assumption here is that elementary arithmetic is a necessar
and sufficient prerequisite. Nevertheless, questions arise as to whether or not th
should be exposed to some other mathematical experience, such as pre-algebr
instead of just repeating their unsuccessful one in arithmetic.
In order to achieve better results and reach a greater population, several e
ucators have raised the possibility of spending some of the time allocated to
mathematics in the seventh grade to introduce pre-algebra, that is, the transit
phase between arithmetic and algebra. One such example was the ALGEBRID
project initiated by Educational Testing Service and the College Entrance Exam
ination Board (1990) whose objective was to bridge the gap between arithmeti
This content downloaded from 62.204.213.86 on Fri, 20 Mar 2020 21:11:13 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
COGNlTIVE GAP 61
This content downloaded from 62.204.213.86 on Fri, 20 Mar 2020 21:11:13 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
62 NICOLAS HERSCOVICS AND LIORA LINCHEVSKI
While the ideas presented by Collis are very interesting, his age levels must
be taken with some caution for the algebraic expressions used in his assessments
were formal and somewhat artificial. Nevertheless, the idea of a pronumeral
evolving into a generalized number is quite enlightening. However, it is not
sufficient to endow it with "the same properties as any number", for this can be
interpreted quite passively, as for example "let n be an even number". In fact,
the pronumeral must also be endowed with the operational properties of number;
the unknown must be perceived as a generalized number that can be subjected
to all the operations performed with and on numbers. Perhaps the expression
"operational generalized number" describes this necessary evolution.
That this additional property is far from trivial has been evidenced in some
prior research. For instance, clinical evidence of this problem appears in Davis'
interview of an exceptionally bright 12-year-old student (Henry), enrolled in an
experimental class following an enriched algebra program (Davis, 1975). After
being taught how to solve equations such as 3x + 2- 5x + 6 = 9x - 3x + 23, the
class was introduced to rational forms such as 3/x = 6/(3x + 1). Upon being told
to multiply both sides by x, Henry seemed to agree that by multiplying 3/x by x
he would be left with 3. However, when told to multiply the right-hand side by x,
he replied: "How can we multiply by x when we don't know whatx is?" Although
this kind of evidence is anecdotal, it is worth mentioning for it so clearly indicates
the cognitive problem at hand. Although Henry may view the literal symbol as a
generalized number, he cannot operate with it.
More substantial evidence can be gathered from a large scale assessment study
carried out in Great Britain involving 3000 secondary school students in their
second, third or fourth year aged 13.3, 14.3, and 15.3 respectively (Kuchemann,
1981, 1978). Only results obtained from the 14-year-olds in their second year of
algebra have been published. When asked to add 4 onto 3n, 36% gave a correct
This content downloaded from 62.204.213.86 on Fri, 20 Mar 2020 21:11:13 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
COGNITIVE GAP 63
answer 3n + 4, but 31% answered 7n while 16% gave 7 as the answer. When
asked to multiply n + 5 by 4, 17% gave correct answers 4n + 20 or 4(n + 5),
while 19% answered 4n + 5 or 4 x n + 5, 31% gave n + 20 as the answer, and 15%
simply wrote 20. The last two results indicate that at least 46% of the students
will not perform the required operation on the literal symbol.
Of course, some of the difficulties faced by the British students are specific to
algebraic expressions. One cognitive problem identified with this mathematical
form is what Davis (1975, p. 18) called the "name-process" dilemma by which an
expression such as 6x is both an indication of a process ("What you get when you
multiply 6 by x") and a "name for the answer". Sfard and Linchevski (1993) have
suggested that the term "process-product dilemma" better describes this cognitive
problem. A somewhat different slant is provided by Collis' theory of the student's
Acceptance of the Lack of Closure (ALC) which describes the level of closure at
which the pupil is able to work with operations (Collis, 1974). He observed that
at the age of seven, children require that two elements connected by an operation
(e.g. 3 + 2) be actually replaced by a third element; from the age of 10 onwards,
they do not find it necessary to make the actual replacement and can also use two
operations (e.g. 6 + 4 + 5); twelve year-olds can refrain from actual closure and are
capable of working with formulas such as Volume = L x B x H; between the ages
of 13 and 15, although students are not yet able to handle variables, they have no
difficulty with symbolization as long as the concept symbolized is underpinned by
a particular concrete generalization. Collis' ALC theory is particularly relevant
to the teaching of algebraic expressions since the operations performed on the
pronumerals cannot be closed as in arithmetic. This problem was evidenced in
a teaching experiment aimed at constructing meaning for algebraic expressions
(Chalouh and Herscovics, 1984, Herscovics and Chalouh, 1984). Even after
instruction, some students could not accept 8 x a as the area of an indicated
rectangle unless it was inserted in the formula "Area of rectangle= 8 x a".
Thus, if even after an introduction to algebra, students experience difficulties
in performing operations with or on a letter representing an unknown or a gener-
alized number, one can hardly expect them to do so spontaneously without any
instruction. Although the letter in an equation or an algebraic expression may
have a numerical referent in the pupil's mind, this does not necessarily render
it operational. Meaning for these operations with literal symbols still has to be
constructed. In fact, it is to supply such meaning that Filloy and Rojano felt the
need to provide some specific instruction for the solution of equations with the
unknown occurring on both sides of the equal sign.
The inability to operate spontaneously with or on the unknown indicates the
existence of a cognitive gap that can be considered a demarcation between arith-
metic and algebra. Given the students' difficulty with the Acceptance of the Lack
of Closure of algebraic expressions, algebraic equations seem to be more appro-
priate for any further investigation of this cognitive gap. It can be conjectured
that such a cognitive gap would establish an upper boundary to the scope of the
student's informal equation solving procedures. These informal procedures are
limited to inverse operations or numerical substitutions and approximations for
This content downloaded from 62.204.213.86 on Fri, 20 Mar 2020 21:11:13 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
64 NICOLAS HERSCOVICS AND LIORA INCHEVSKI
METHODOLOGY
This content downloaded from 62.204.213.86 on Fri, 20 Mar 2020 21:11:13 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
COGNITIVE GAP 65
interview in order to take notes and also to participate later in the analysis of
the student's responses. In fact, the observer participated actively by helping the
interviewer keep track of the semi-standardized questionnaire and by intervening
whenever he or she felt that the interviewer was misunderstanding the subject.
The function of the preliminary assessment was to gather some relevant informa-
tion about each student regarding his or her knowledge of the arithmetic concepts
and skills needed for the solution of equations. The first question was "Do you
know the word equation"? "Can you show me an example? ".
Nearly all our subjects responded with examples involving a numerical oper-
ation written vertically. When asked to write horizontally, most of them wrote
operations such as 5 + 7 = . In order to allow the students to become familiar
with the provided calculator, they were next asked to evaluate four arithmetic
operations involving large numbers.
The second set of questions verified their acceptance of the different uses of
the equality symbol. This problem had been investigated by Behr, Erlwanger,
and Nichols (1976) who had shown that most primary school pupils viewed the
equal sign operationally in the sense of indicating the need to perform the required
operation and writing the result as in 3 + 5 =? However, when faced with a
decomposition such 9 = 4 + 5, many children would refuse to accept it, claim that
it was written backwards and re-write it as 4 + 5 = 9. They experienced further
difficulties when the equal sign was used to represent an arithmetic equivalence
such as 3 + 7 = 6 + 4. Many would simply break it up and write two equations:
3+7 = 10 and 6+4 = 10. Several researchers have pointed out that such a limited
meaning of the equal sign persisted even among some students in secondary school
(Cortes, Vergnaud and Kavafian, 1990; Herscovics and Kieran, 1980; Kieran,
198 1a).
We asked our students if 34 = 19 + 15 was written correctly or incorrectly.
Two of our weaker students did not accept the use of the equal symbol for a decom-
position into a sum. In addition, during the latter part of the assessment dealing
with the solution of equations, we found that when the equations represented a
decomposition, 7 students ended up reading them from right to left. This was of
no consequence when only a sum was involved as in 35 = n + 16. However,
in the case of a subtraction such as 364 = 796 - n, three students changed the
intended problem by reading it from right to left "n -minus 796 equals 364".
To verify our students' acceptance of the equal sign as a symbol of arithmetic
equivalence, we asked them if 15 + 7 was equal to 10 + 12 and then how they
would show that they were equal. Most of them wrote down two equations
indicating that each sum added up to 22. Yet, when asked if we could express it
as 15 + 7 = 10 + 12, all of them agreed.
The third set of questions assessed the students' familiarity with the order of
operations. Prior research (Kieran, 1979) had shown that when solving a string of
arithmetic operations, students tended to perform them sequentially, one at a time,
This content downloaded from 62.204.213.86 on Fri, 20 Mar 2020 21:11:13 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
66 NICOLAS HERSCOVICS AND LIORA LINCHEVSKI
Hence not surprisingly, all our students knew that 3n represented 3 x n and
when asked what they would get if they replaced n by 2 or 5, all of them answered
6 and 15, respectively. This is in sharp contrast with the results obtained with
another group of students who had previously been exposed to concatenation in
an introduction to algebra, but in a post test reverted back to an arithmetical frame
of reference and therefore thought that the substitutions would yield 32 and 35
This content downloaded from 62.204.213.86 on Fri, 20 Mar 2020 21:11:13 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
COGNITIVE GAP 67
Selection of Equations
Knowing the difficulties that students experience with rational numbers, we de-
cided to restrict our equations to natural numbers. Thus, the obstacles encountered
by our subjects would be of an algebraic nature, not due to a weakness in arithmetic.
We wanted to submit to our students equations involving all four operations, as
well as variations in the position of the unknown. It was conceivable that when
faced with equations in which the unknown was the subtrahend or the divisor,
the students might solve them by operating with or on the unknown. The size of
numbers was also taken into account in order to determine if we would witness a
shift in solution strategies. This was a reasonable assumption since in many cases,
students will use primitive processes on equations they find very easy and use
their more sophisticated procedures when they feel they are warranted. Whenever
the same type of equation was repeated, the one with the smaller numbers was
presented first in order not to discourage the subject.
Another variable that was taken into account was the direction of the equation.
Prior to our preliminary assessment, we had no idea how well the students accepted
the equality symbol for the representation of either an arithmetical decomposition
into an operation or an arithmetical equivalence. We also varied the number of
operations to verify one of Kieran's early results (Kieran, 1984) showing that
some students were having difficulties in solving an equation involving several
arithmetic operations (e.g. 4 + n - 2 + 5 = 11 + 3 - 5). We included in our list
This content downloaded from 62.204.213.86 on Fri, 20 Mar 2020 21:11:13 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
68 NICOLAS HERSCOVICS AND LIORA LINCHEVSKI
TABLE I
8 equations in which the order of operations used by the students was specifically
examined.
The last and most critical area of our investigation was the testing of equations
in which the same unknown occurred twice. We included 10 equations in which
the unknown appeared on the same side of the equality symbol in order to assess if
students would group the terms involving the unknown and thereby exhibit some
ability to operate with or on the unknown. We also included 3 equations in which
the unknown appeared on both sides of the equal sign since 5 or sometimes all 6
of Kieran's pupils had not been able to solve such equations (Kieran, 1984).
RESULTS
Since all equations were solved by nearly all our students, we will focus o
attention on the solution procedures that proved to be the most frequently u
as well as on the specific difficulties encountered by the students with some
the equations. Our very high success rate might be due to optimal conditions
mentioned earlier. Table I describes the coding used to identify the differen
solution procedures observed during the interviews.
Coding
This content downloaded from 62.204.213.86 on Fri, 20 Mar 2020 21:11:13 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
COGNITIVE GAP 69
TABLE II
TABLE III
Equations involving multiplication and division
693
1269
This content downloaded from 62.204.213.86 on Fri, 20 Mar 2020 21:11:13 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
70 NICOLAS HERSCOVICS AND LIORA LINCHEVSKI
TABLE IV
Tables II and III show the procedures used to solve equations involving one
operation. The results indicate that essentially two types of procedures are used.
For equations in which only addition or multiplication are present, students
will overwhelmingly revert to solving these by using the inverse operation, but the
numbers in the equation must be large enough to go beyond known number facts.
These results are very similar to those obtained by Kieran (1981) but differ from
those of Gallardo and Rojano (1987) who found "some confusion between the
various operations, interpreting addition as subtraction, subtraction as division"
which might explain some of their students' problems with inverse operations.
For equations involving only subtraction or only division, one must distinguish
between cases where the unknown is a minuend or a dividend from those cases
where it is a subtrahend or a divisor. In the former cases, over 77% of the students
use the respective inverse operations. In the latter cases, students respectively
subtract the given difference from the given minuend or divide the given dividend
by the given quotient. At no time did we see any evidence of students directly
performing operations on or with the unknown. Thus we can conclude that students
solve these equations by working around the unknown at a purely numerical level.
Some difficulties are worth mentioning. Five students read some of the fol-
lowing equations from right to left (eq. 9, 10, 11, 13 and 22). Two students had
some problem in accepting the equal sign as a symbol for decomposition and felt
the need to re-write equation 9 as 37 - n = 23, and did so again with equations
16, 21 and 22.
Grouping of Numbers
This content downloaded from 62.204.213.86 on Fri, 20 Mar 2020 21:11:13 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
COGNITIVE GAP 71
TABLE V
This content downloaded from 62.204.213.86 on Fri, 20 Mar 2020 21:11:13 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
72 NICOLAS HERSCOVICS AND LIORA LINCHEVSKI
TABLE VI
Since none of the students had ever seen equations of this type before, the inter-
viewer had to explain: When we have the letter (or box) twice in the equation,
it means that you have to replace each letter with (or put in each box) the same
number
Equations 38 and 39 must be considered as trivial cases that were meant to
familiarize the students with a double occurrence of the unknown and thus cannot
be seen as representative. In comparing Table VI with the previous tables we
note a major change in the solution procedures used in solving equations 40 to 47.
Whereas some form of substitution (SS or SFT) had been used in the solution of
previous equations, the most frequent occurrence was in the 20% range (23% for
equation 35 and 27% for equation 37). However, in the solution of equations 40
to 47, the frequency of substitution procedures ranges from 87% (equation 46) to
100% (equation 45). These results confirm our conjecture regarding the students'
inability to operate with or on the unknown and thus having to rely on numerical
solution processes.
It is in this set of equations that we found isolated cases of spontaneous
algebraic methods, by which we mean operating with or on the unknown. One
student was able to solve equations 40 and 41 by grouping the two terms in the
unknown, a second student did so for equations 40 and 43, and a third student
solved equations 46 and 47 by first performing an inverse operation and then
grouping the unknown. It is noteworthy that none of these students extended the
grouping procedure to equations 44 and 45 where one of the terms in the unknown
did not indicate a coefficient. The sporadic incidence of this algebraic behavior
prevents us from viewing these students as having achieved any stable algebraic
knowledge. Instead, the overwhelming evidence points to the fact that grouping
of the unknown is not a procedure that is acquired spontaneously.
This content downloaded from 62.204.213.86 on Fri, 20 Mar 2020 21:11:13 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
COGNlllVE GAP 73
TABLE VII
In the last three equations the unknown occurred on each side of the equal sign. Our
results differ from those of Kieran (1984) whose 6 subjects came up with incorrect
solutions for the equation 4x + 9 = 7x. This same equation (our equation 49)
was submitted to our 22 students and 19 of them found a correct answer either by
systematic substitution or by succeeding in their first attempt at substitution. The
other two equations in this set were solved by 20 pupils. These equations were
more difficult to solve by substitution. Whereas in previous cases the systematic
substitution yielded a sequence of approximations to a given number, this was
no longer the case when the unknown appeared on both sides. The process of
systematic substitution yielded a comparison of the two functions on either side
of the equal sign and at some point the difference between the two was inverted
and the respective numerical values became increasingly divergent.
As mentioned earlier in the analysis of the procedures used to solve equation 29,
we found among some students a tendency to ignore the minus sign preceding
the number 2 in 4 + n-2 + 5 = 11 + 3-5. In fact, 16 of the 22 students
did at one point add 2 and 5. However, 4 of them corrected their initial mista
during the verification of their solution. The detachment of the minus sign wa
restricted to instances where a number was subtracted from an unknown, for
found evidence of this in the preliminary assessment on the questions dealing
global perception of a string of operations. For the string 17+59-59+18-18 =?
we found that 6 students were ignoring the minus sign in front of 59 and adding
59 to 18. In the second string 237 + 89-89 + 67-92 + 92 =? we found that 5
students were ignoring the minus sign in front of 89 and instead were adding 89
to 67; but even more students (10) ignored the minus sign in front of 92 and were
adding 92 to 92 and then subtracting the sum 184.
The detachment of a number from the preceding minus sign may also explain
the very different answers we obtained on the order of operations. It will be
recalled that 17 of our 22 students had worked out the string 5 + 6 x 10=? by first
performing the addition but in the case of 17 - 3 x 5 =? only three subjects had
performed the subtraction first. Although the two strings have asimilar structure,
This content downloaded from 62.204.213.86 on Fri, 20 Mar 2020 21:11:13 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
74 NICOLAS HERSCOVICS AND LIORA LINCHEVSKI
CONCLUSION
This content downloaded from 62.204.213.86 on Fri, 20 Mar 2020 21:11:13 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
COGNlTIVE GAP 75
This content downloaded from 62.204.213.86 on Fri, 20 Mar 2020 21:11:13 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
76 NICOLAS HERSCOVICS AND LIORA LINCHEVSKI
that we worked with a fairly gifted group and that these problems may be more
common among average and weaker pupils. On the other hand, the detachment of
a number from the preceding minus sign had a high incidence and this indicates
that evaluating strings of operations is not a trivial problem. These difficulties
indicate that some of the problems in early algebra find their origin in the students'
arithmetic background and warrant further investigation.
It may be difficult for a teacher to appreciate the problems experienced by
weaker or average students at an arithmetic level. But perhaps these are the
difficulties that become major cognitive obstacles in their learning of algebra.
Clearly, more research is needed to determine their prevalence and to find peda-
gogical interventions that might help the learner overcome them. By expanding
the cognitive processes needed to bridge the gap between arithmetic and algebra
many more students are likely to be recuperated. Perhaps even those students who
are presently shunted away from any algebraic experience might benefit from an
appropriate exposure to pre-algebra.
NOTES
1 This research was funded by the Quebec Ministry of Education (Fonds FCAR-92-ER-1032).
2 The authors wish to thank Patricia Lytle for her many helpful suggestions in the preparation of
paper.
REFERENCES
Behr, M., Erlwanger, S., and Nichols, E.: 1976, How Children View Equality Sentences, Project for
the Mathematical Development of Children, Technical Report No. 3, Florida State University,
Tallahassee.
Booth, L.: 1989, 'Grade 8 students' understanding of structural properties in mathematics', in
G. Vergnaud, J. Rogaiski, and M. Artigue(eds.), Proceedings ofPME-XIII, Paris, France, 141-148.
Boyer, C. B.: 1991, A History of Mathematics, 2nd edition, revised by U. C. Merzbach, John Wiley
and Sons, New York.
Carpenter, T. P., Corbin, M. K., Kepner, H. S., Montgomery Lindquist, M., and Reys, R. E.: 1981,
Results from the Second Mathematics Assessment of the National Assessment of Educational
Progress, NCTM, Reston, Virgina.
Chalouh, L. and Herscovics, N.: 1984, 'From letter representing a hidden quantity to letter representing
an unknown quantity', in J. M. Moser (ed.), Proceedings of PME-NA-VI, Madison, Wisconsin,
71-76.
Chalouh, L. and Herscovics, N.: 1983, 'The problem of concatenation in early algebra', in J. C. Berg-
eron and N. Herscovics (eds.), Proceedings of PME-NA-V, Montreal, Canada, 153-160.
Collis, K. F.: 1975, The Development of Formal Reasoning, Report of a Social Science Research
Council sponsored project (HR 2434/1) carried out at the University of Nottingham, University
of Newcastle, NSW, Australia.
Collis, K. F.: 1974, Cognitive Development and Mathematics Learning, paper prepared for the
Psychology of Mathematics Education Workshop, published at the Shell Mathematics Unit Centre
for Science Education, Chelsea College, University of London, U.K.
Cortes, A., Vergnaud, G., and Kavafian, N.: 1990, 'From arithmetic to algebra: negotiating a jump in
the learning process', G. Booker and T. De Mendicutti (eds.), Proceedings of PME XIV: Mexico
2, 27-34.
Davis, R. B.: 1975, 'Cognitive processes involved in solving simple algebraic equations', Journal of
Children's Mathematical Behavior 1(3), 7-35.
This content downloaded from 62.204.213.86 on Fri, 20 Mar 2020 21:11:13 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
COGNrTIVE GAP 77
Ebos, F., Robinson, B., and Tuck, B.: 1984, Math is/2, second edition, Nelson Canada, Scarborough,
Ontario.
Educational Testing Service and the College Entrance Examination Board: 1990: Algebridge: Concept
Based Instructional Assessment, Janson Publications, Providence, Rhode Island.
Edwards, Jr., E. L. (ed.): 1990, Algebra for Everyone, NCTM, Reston, Virginia
Filloy, E.: 1987, 'Modelling and the teaching of algebra', in J. C. Bergeron, N. Herscovics, and
C. Kieran (eds.), Proceedings of PME-XI, Montreal, Canada, Vol. 1, 295-300.
Filloy, E. and Rojano, T.: 1989, 'Solving equations: the transition from arithmetic to algebra', For the
Learning of Mathematics 9(2), 19-25.
Filloy, E. and Rojano, T.: 1985a, 'Obstructions to the acquisition of elemental algebraic concepts and
teaching strategies', in L. Streefland (ed.), Proceedings of PME-IX, OW & OC, State University
of Utrecht, The Netherlands, 154-158.
Filloy, E. and Rojano, T.: 1985b, 'Operating the unknown and models of teaching', in S. Damarmn and
M. Shelton (eds.), Proceedings of PME-NA VII, Columbus, Ohio, 75-79.
Filloy, E. and Rojano, T.: 1984, 'From an arithmetical thought to an algebraic thought', in J. Moser
(ed.), Proceedings of PME-NA VI, Madison, Wisconsin, 51-56.
Gallardo, A. and Rojano, T.: 1987, 'Common difficulties in the learning of algebra among children
displaying low and medium pre-algebraic profiency levels', in J. C. Bergeron, N. Herscovics, and
C. Kieran (eds.), Proceedings of PME-XI, Montreal, Canada, Vol. 1, 301-307.
Herscovics, N.: 1989, 'Cognitive obstacles encountered in the learning of algebra', in S. Wagner and
C. Kieran (eds.), Research Issues in the Learning and Teaching of Algebra, Reston, Virginia:
NCTM, and Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum, 60-68.
Herscovics, N.: 1980, 'Constructing meaning for linear equations: a problem of representation',
Recherches en Didactiques des Mathematiques, vol. 1, no. 3, Grenoble, France, 351-385.
Herscovics, N.: 1979, 'A learning model for some algebraic concepts', in W. Geeslin and K. Fuson
(eds.), Explorations in the Modelling of Learning Mathematics, ERIC-SMEAC, Columbus, Ohio,
98-116.
Herscovics, N. and Chalouh, L.: 1985, 'Conflicting frames of reference in the learning of algebra',
in S. Damarin and M. Shelton (eds.), Proceedings of PME-NA VII, Ohio University, Columbus,
Ohio, 123-131.
Herscovics, N. and Chalouh, L.: 1984, 'Using literal symbols to represent hidden quantities', in
J. M. Moser (ed.), Proceedings of PME-NA-VI, Madison, Wisconsin, 64-70.
Herscovics, N. and Kieran, C.: 1980, 'Constructing meaning for the concept of equation', The
Mathematics Teacher 73(8), 572-580.
Kieran, C.: 1989, 'The early leaming of algebra: a structural perspective', in S. Wagner and C. Kieran
(eds.), Research Issues in the Learning and Teaching of Algebra, Reston, Virginia: NCTM, and
Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum, 33-56.
Kieran, C.: 1984, 'Cognitive mechanisms underlying the equation-solving errors of algebra novices',
Southwell et al. (eds.), Proceedings of PME-VIII, Sydney, Australia, 70-77.
Kieran, C.: 1981a, 'Concepts associated with the equality symbol', Educational Studies in Mathemat-
ics 12, 317-326.
Kieran, C.: 1981b, 'Pre-algebraic thinking among 12- and 13-year olds', Equipe de Recherche
Pedagogique (eds.), Proceedings of PME-V, Grenoble, France, 158-164.
Kieran, C.: 1979, 'Children's operational thinking within the context of bracketing and the order of
operations', in D. Tall (ed.), Proceedings of the Third International Conference for the Psychol-
ogy of Mathematics Education, Coventry, England: Mathematics Education Research Centre,
Warwick University, 128-133.
Kuchemann, D.: 1981, 'Algebra', in K. Hart (ed.), Children's Understanding of Mathematics: 11-16,
London: John Murray, 102-119.
Kuchemann, D.: 1978, 'Children's understanding of numerical variables', Mathematics in School
7(4), 23-26.
Lodholz, R.: 1990, 'The transition from arithmetic to algebra', in E. L. Edwards Jr. (ed.), Algebrafor
Everyone, NCTM: Reston, Virginia, 24-33.
MacGregor, M. and Stacey, K.: 1993, 'Cognitive models underlying students' formulation of simple
linear equations', Journalfor Research in Mathematics Education 24(3), 217-232.
Sfard, A. and Linchevski, L.: 1994, 'The gains and pitfalls of reification: the case of algebra', to
This content downloaded from 62.204.213.86 on Fri, 20 Mar 2020 21:11:13 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
78 NICOLAS HERSCOVICS AND LIORA LINCHEVSKI
School of Education,
Hebrew University,
Jerusalem, Israel
This content downloaded from 62.204.213.86 on Fri, 20 Mar 2020 21:11:13 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms