Various Authors-Materials and Structure of Music
Various Authors-Materials and Structure of Music
* li
i
^
Wf
m f,
9
WiUiamC
illTom Thcmison
Mrs. M, Dale Nixon
38 Princeton Circle
Longmont; Colorado 80501
II
vrf %
K?
MATERIALS
AND
STRUCTURE
I
OF MUSIC
I'l
t^
'>.di'^mf
MATERIALS
AND
STRUCTURE
OF MUSIC
William Christ
Richard DeLone
Vernon Kliewer
Lewis Rowell
William Thomson
Indiana University
10 9 8 7 6 5 4
.•'^."
Prehace
There can be only one valid reason for writing still another
book tiiat deals with the materials of music: the desire to
contribute insights and techniques of learning that are fresh
and that are superior to those already presented for our
understanding of music. The two volumes of Materials and
Structure of Music are unique in several ways: they repre-
sent the synthesis of the broad experience of five people who
know music as composers, performers, listeners, and teachers;
they are an accounting of the time and pitch bases of the
music of Western Civilization, not a rechewing of the dubious
"rules" of a limited body of music. Perhaps most significant is
the fact that they stand as an attempt to strip away the sur-
face peculiarities of the music of any one composer or "style"
and delve into more basic matters.
In these two volumes we have attempted to accomplish two
main tasks: (1) To set out in effective pedagogical order a
framework of understanding for the materials and structure
of music; and (2) to transmit this understanding by means of
the actual music produced in the Western world.
To achieve the first goal, Volume I begins with a study of
melody. The complexities of the traditional four-part "har-
mony" work have been deferred until step-by-step knowledge
of basic pitch organization in two- and three-voice combina-
tions has been introduced. In this way the learning sequence
is realistically geared to student comprehension, and a true
v;;
VIII PREFACE
examples from before 1700 and after 1900 than any other
basic theory text that is available at this time. There is an
emphasis, of course, on the music of the so-called "period of
common practice" which is included with the full realization
of the significance of this repertoire both to the performing
and listening musician.
Materials and Structure of Music I and // are designed for
a basic music theory curriculum. It is assumed that the ma-
terials of the two volumes and accompanying workbooks will
be used as the basis for the entire course of study, with the
supplementation of melodies for music-reading practice and
at least some of the scores for works that are discussed in the
texts. Each chapter is followed by suggested Exercises designed
to meet the needs of his own class and his own academic
situation. The separate volumes are planned around the thirty
week academic year of a four semester course, the "freshman
and sophomore theory classes." Many of the concepts and
procedures presented here have been applied successfully in
music programs of such diverse institutions as a state univer-
sity of the Northwest, a southwestern teacher's college, a large
midwestern university, and an eastern private school.
Some of the chapters might well be dealt with in less than
a week of class time, while others conceivably should be
granted more time for discussion and assimilation. The ex-
perienced teacher should encounter no difficulty in establish-
ing a workable time sequence that will fit the texts and
workbooks to the needs of his own classes.
It is intended that Book I be the basis of a beginning theory
though familiar with the first volume, feels the need of review-
ing some of the important terms and concepts that are treated
there.
Furthermore, there is approach during the
a shift of basic
final six chapters which are devoted to Impressionism and
contemporary materials and procedures, a shift necessitated
by the fact that the music involved represents a diversity of
musical types for which no universal pattern of common prac-
tice has been developed. These final chapters are devoted to a
TONALITY 19
MELODIC CADENCES 50
7 BASIC MELODY 87
XI
XII CONTENTS
»\
Basic Properties
7
orii
Notation
of Tone
organized sound. Any pattern of musical sounds has four properties pitch, which :
may be described as high or low; intensity, described as loud or soft; timbre, or tone
quality; and rhythm, or tone duration. In this chapter we shall review some of the
important characteristics of two of these properties, pitch and rhythm.
Characteristics of Rhythm
Ex. 1-1.
^ ^
w S
r
w
^^
S w
^ ^w
^
w w w S w
S w w w w
s
S w
*
w S
Ww
The particular grouping of beats within a composition is called meter. Metric
accent is the impetus or inflection created by the accented pulsations of strong
beats. Unaccented beats are called weak beats. In Ex. 1-la the ordering of beats
creates the pattern strong=weak {S=w), and the pattern in Ex. 1-lb is S=w = w.
The irregularity of Ex. 1-lc results from alternate groupings o£ S = w and S—w =
w.
Our perception of meter is not limited to the recognition of accented and un-
accented beats. In fact, in many compositions our comprehension of meter results
more from the grouping of melodic pitches or chords than from the mere ordering
of strong and weak beats. To understand better this aspect of rhythm and meter,
let us reconsider the term beat.
A beat is understood to last until a next beat occurs. But if we imagine beats as
instantaneous pulsations or ticks, view one beat as continuing
then it is difficult to
until the next beat occurs. However, if we regard the beat as only the beginning of
a regularly recurring time span, then we can more accurately describe the total
time span from one beat until the beginning of the next as a basic duration. Further-
more, the various durations used in a composition can be related to the basic
duration. Meter^ then, is more accurately described as an ordering of basi c durations ,
into groups of two, three or four units per measure, or whatever pattern the com-
poser may wish to establish. Basic duration refers to the time span from the articula-
tion of one beat to the next, and this unit is usually represented in notation by one
of the note-values shown in Ex. 1-2.
Sixteenth note = J)
Thirty - second note = ^ Sixty -fourth note = ^
S
We have noted several processes that are involved in creating meter. First a pulse
J J IJ J IJ J J IJ. IJ J IJ J IJ J J u.
^
Ex. l-3a. Beethoven: Symphony No. 3, I.
(>)
(» {» (>) (>) (>)
^ (>)
t
^
f
(Theactualmetersignaturefor the melody in Ex. 1-3 is I). When a conflict occurs
between the accents of melody or chords and the metric accents, syncopation results.
By notating the melody from Ex. 1-3 in ^ meter, a conflict is created between the
implicit triple meter of the melody and the metric accents {S=w) of the notated
meter. This is shown in Ex. l-3b.
S S (w) S
(
w
Syncopation)
S w
^
S
J
w
J
(Sync.)
I
( S
J
w )
S tw) S w (S) %v S w S w (
iB: i ^ J J 1^
in
( Sync.)
^ (
T
Sync.)
4 BASIC PROPERTIES AND NOTATION OF TONE
The rate at which basic durations occur in given meters is usually indicated by
an appropriate designation such as Allegro (fast) or Adagio (slow) or other descrip-
tive words. Since these terms, which are generally chosen from Italian, French,
German or English, are not precise, composers also often assign a metronome mark-
ing. The metronome is a clock-like instrument which divides the minute into a
precise number of beats ranging from thirty to two-hundred. By indicating a metro-
nome speed, (M.M.= 120, or M.M. = 80)^ the composer can set the desired tempo
accurately for performers.
The number of basic durations per measure is shown as the upper part of the meter
The lower part, a number such as 2, 4, or 8, represents the basic duration.'^
signature.
For example, the number 2 represents a half note; 4, a quarter note; and 8 in-
dicates that eighth notes equal the basic duration. Meter signatures whose upper
numbers are 1, 2, 3, or 4 are called simple meters. The prevailing note-values
in simple meters generally represent divisions or multiples of the basic duration
by two, four, or eight. The conductor's beat patterns for duple, triple and quad-
ruple simple meters are shown in the diagram of simple meters at the top of
page 5.
[3
Notation of Rhythm
stem, (for all durations except whole notes) which is formed by a straight line
,
that joins the note-head at the left or right, depending upon the location of the
J J = J J which are used to join series of similar durations that would nor-
Beamed notes are associated primarily with instrumental notation, while the
use of flagged notes is indigenous to vocal music, where the flag is used for notating
tones that occur with single syllables of text, as shown in Ex. 1-4.
m ^^ Mur -
^
mur- ing bree/. -
i
es
^
r
Soft
P
flow -
p
ing by ,
3
^^^^ ^Fl^fc^ "T'pL^
^^m ^ ±1
i i
' If the note head is located above the third line of thestafT, its stem usually
points down, or hangs
below the note head. The opposite is true when the note head is written below the third line.
M>
,
J' I
I > h r p I
Sweet are your kiss es Balm y your sigh.
33^5 5^ ^^=^
i ^r=f
m m
Flags are always used for isolated notes or where single durations of an eighth
note or less are preceded or followed by quarter, half, or whole notes (or their
equivalent rests). See Ex. 1-5.
Ex. 1-5.
^ c;>^ ^^ ^j^ l
pyp^py yj)|j J^yyyj) »
^ '' d ^a
m"
Beams help the reader to group note patterns according to basic durations.
In the group of patterns shown
first in Ex. 1-6, the location of beats has been
obscured by illogical beaming.
Ex. 1-6.
s w
In Ex. 1-7 the same patterns are beamed in groups that reveal the meter's beat
structure.
Ex. 1-7.
i
(J)
A general guide for notation is to avoid beaming more than six notes together,
unless more are needed to consume the equivalent of one basic duration and ;
further, beams begin on the beat unless the notes they join are preceded by a rest
sign.
Ex. 1-8.
s^ MIZ
r[_rr i '
u xLium^r
^
When possible, avoid mixing flagged and beamed notes, except in notating vocal
music.
Ex. 1-9.
Poor Better
"^r 'Pc;cp i
r ppbt h 'PULs \
r pu^
Absence of tone is indicated by rests, and each of the basic durations
Rests.
discussed earlier has an equivalent rest symbol. Rest equivalents for each basic
duration are shown as Ex. 1-10.
Note value
- Rest Name
-^ —H- Breve (or double whole -note)
1 Half note
1 Quarter note
Eighth note
1 Sixteenth note
Ex. i-n.
(a) 9
4
- J I J. II
better written I ^ J | J.
{r rr r rrr r) {r rr r r rr r)
its normal duration. This means that o J could also be written as J. Augmenta-
tion dots are not used if they would prolong a note into the next measure v 4 J a- ).
In such a situation, and where a continuation of sound between two notes of the
same pitch is sought, ties are used. The tie is a curved line which connects two
with the slur, which is used to indicate a smooth connection of two notes oi different
pitch: AHfc ^
Ex. 1-12 illustrates several notational signs and principles of clear notating.
Ex. 1-12. Beethoven: Sonata for Violin and Piano, Op- 23, II.
I
BASIC PROPEHmS AND NOTATION OF TONE
I ^ J n N ? :
m P I
y J^ y j)
^f
U
m^^ ^ i=a: S ^^ w
^ ^ ^^^Ht
1
if.
? ^^^^
B i^
i*
jg^^^
^?F
CTt?5C.
i
JJ^
^ iM
5
{/:
^^T"
^ ^^
yt #_^:-
-^^
5=fc=5 i
^? 1 ^
?r = ^^f
pSii
cresc.
^ffi
* £5
W
1 ^
*^
decresc.
^ £
J^i^l
^ -^ —
i|t^ ''
^
>>f! ^
5^ ^ f^^-fe^
«:
i>i5
f: ^
fe
decresc.
S ^Ht
*pt
J
c
VjJ^ilJ i
3
I
-hiHr^
^i*
#
-^^^^
Compound Meter
We have noted that the common divisions and multiples of the basic duration
in simple meters are duple. Since in a simple meter the division of the basic dura-
tion by three (the triplet) is not a usual one, it is generally shown by writing a 3
'
3 —
above the beam, or by a. J) J) J) bracketed 3 above the stems if a beam is not used.
In contrast to simple meters, triple divisions of the basic duration are common to
meters called Compound Meters.
Compound meters, those calling for a triple division of the basic duration, have
a dotted note as the basic duration, as shown here:
J. J~7
J)- « «
J. J J J
Compound meters, like simple meters, are called duple, triple, or quadruple,
depending on the number of basic pulses per measure. The family of compound
primary) division of the basic duration, rather than the number of main pulses and
basic duration, as in simple meter signatures. For example, a common compound
BASIC PROPERTIES AND NOTATION OF TONE II
meter signature is |. This signature would seem to denote a meter of six basic
durations per measure, each of whose value equaled the eighth note. While such
an interpretation can be made, it does not present an accurate picture of general
performance practice and rhythmic interpretation. This is true because in most
instances, except in very slow tempi, performers acknowledge two, not six, main
note as a division of the main pulse (basic duration). Read the melody that follows
and note the analysis of its rhythm shown below.
Ex. 1-13.
Note the essential grouping of each measure into two main pulses (duple). The
eighth notes divide each basic duration by three and are in fact a division of the
basic beat rather than a primary level of accent. Only those eighths that fall as the
first or fourth (eighths) of a measure correspond with the main accentual bases of
the measure. It can be seen that the signature |, which accounts for the total num-
ber of eighths that occur in a measure of | meter, actually refers to a grouping that
results from dividing each basic duration by three, thereby producing a total of
six eighths (or their equivalent). Six-eight meter could be more effectively shown
as 2, and has been by some composers.
f n
If we apply the same reasoning to other compound meters we note that | is
a triple compound meter, easily grasped as 3, and g is a quadruple meter under-
IH!
stood as 4. Any dotted note can serve as the basic duration for a compound meter.
r
The most common compound meter signatures are grouped below:
6 6 2^
8 if) Uf) 'iif) 16 'il)
16 Vp-/ 'HI-)
16 \p
h— basic duration
9 12
if) 4 }) 4 f)
:
Although the primary division of the main pulse in compound time is usually
triple, the sub-divisions of the basic duration, that is, divisions of the primary divi-
sion, are generally duple. Triple sub-divisions of the basic duration are called ir-
regular, as are triple divisions of the beat in simple meter. The first four measures of
the melody in Ex. 1-14 contain regular (duple) sub-divisions of the basic duration,
while the second four measures illustrate both regular and irregular sub-divisions.
An analysis of the rhythmic content of the example shows
regular sub-division — J J J J J J J J J J j5
irregular sub-division = JT2 JT^ fH J72 JT2 J^
Ex. 1-14.
Moderate
^m. i
X
J /»! 1
3 3
J ^ ** J * J i -
$ ^
Notation of Pitch
By itself the musical staff is meaningless. However, by placing a clef sign at its
left edge, a specific letter-named pitch is designated for each line and space. The
treble clef sign is actually an elaborate script G that designates the pitch g^ on the
second line of the staff.
$ B D
~9
D
^
C
* For example, the tone that results from a vibration rate (frequency) of 440 vibrations per second
(v.p.s.) is known as a^
BASIC PROPERTIES AND NOTATION OF TONE 13
The bass clef specifies the note/ for the fourth line of the staff.
FGABCFEDCBAGFE
The question may arise as to the need more than one clef. If our musical
for
system were limited to only seven or so pitches, there would be no such need. How-
ever, the capabilities of hearing are such that a broad range of pitches has been
used in most Western music. Consequently, a number of clefs are used, each em-
ployed to represent pitches in particular areas of the pitch spectrum, or gamut.
The gamut of pitches most used in music is shown in Ex. 1-17, which illustrates
the "Great" staff.
Ex. 1-17.
4.. n jLU
r> f^
-2g p
The mid-point of this staff, which joins or links the treble and bass clefs, is a
broken line that represents middle c, or c^. Added line segments above or below
either staff are called ledger lines, and they are used to notate pitches which lie
c clefs." Using these c clefs, it is possible to assign middle c to any line of the staff.
Four c clefs are shown in Ex. 1-18.^
Ex. 1-18.
i
t
Musicians have developed a useful terminology for pinpointing the various
pitches of the gamut. This amounts to grouping the pitches of the pitch spectrum
into seven-note segments, called "octave segments." Each segment begins with a
C and includes all possible pitches between C and the B seven notes above. With
the addition of the two tones below the lowest C [CC) on the piano, AAA and BBB
(the partial segment called "Sub-contra"), the different seven-tone segments are
named Contra (CC-BB), Great (C-B), small {c-b), prime or 1 (c' or c^ b' or b^), —
double prime or 2 (c" or c^ —b" or b^), triple prime or 3 (c'" or c^— b'" or b^),
quadruple prime or 4 (c"" or —b"" or c'* b*). The highest note on the piano begins
the five prime segment, c'"", or c^. This nomenclature is illustrated in notation as
Ex. 1-19.
c4- b4 c5
;1. bl
;2. b2
TT"
-»'
^ 5
— I
— — I —
* o
middle c
P 331
J
c-b
^ ^ ^ C-B Small
J Great
AAA -BBB ^CC-BB
Sub-contra Contra
Sharps ( ff ) or flats ( |> ), double sharps (x ), or double flats ( b b ) are placed im-
mediately before the note head and indicate slight pitch modifications. Sharps in-
^ The two most often found are those that place c^ on the third and fourth lines of the staff.
c clefs
These are the alto and tenor clefs, and they are used for the notation of parts for alto and tenor
instruments (or voices) such as the viola, cello, and trombone.
BASIC PROPERTIES AND NOTATION OF TONE 15
dicate a raising of the natural note by one half-step, while flats indicate that the
natural note is to be lowered a half-step. Similarly, double sharps indicate a
raising of the note by two half-steps, and double flats are used to lower the note
by two half-steps.
Natural signs ( J] ) are used to cancel previous sharps or flats. Proper use of all
Ex. 1-20.
When accidentals are used, they are understood to continue throughout the
measure in which they appear without being rewritten. Therefore, all of the/'s
that recur in Ex. 1-21 should be performed as/' sharps.
Ex. 1-21.
')^v'r»rrr
Furthermore, accidentals govern only those notes that occur on the pitch
level at which they are introduced; they do not affect pitches in different octaves.
The result of the passage in Ex. 1 -22(a) is clarified in version b.
Ex. 1-22
(a) (b)
^m ^ i=F
) »
^^^^a\
Intervals
Thus a fourth denotes an interval that spans four different letter names of the
staff; for example, c' up tof spans four letter names, c' d' e and/', and a down to
which means successively sounded tones, or harmonic, which means sounded to-
gether.
Who/e and Half Sfeps. The half step is the smallest difference of pitch
commonly employed in Western music. The half step interval is easily observed by
locating those white keys on the piano that have no intervening black keys. The
relation between these pairs of keys, e-f and b-c is called a half step^, a minor second
(m2), or semitone. The relation between pairs of white keys separated by black keys
comprises a whole step, or major second (M2).
Thus although the numerical name of an interval describes the number of dif-
ferent lines and spaces spanned, and this is easily relatable to the positions of two
pitches on the staff, it does not account for the exact size of the interval. This can
be checked by comparing the sound and total number of whole and half-steps in
The term perfect is applied only to unisons (primes), fourths, fifths and octaves,
while the words major and minor are applied only to seconds, thirds, sixths and
sevenths. Augmented and diminished are applied to certain modifications of any type
of interval. As a synopsis of the preceding discussion, the following chart may
prove useful for reference.
The half step from e'f is more accurately called a diatonic half-step, which means that different
*
note names are employed in its notation. These same pitches in other instances may be notated as
e' and e' sharp. This notation differentiation calls for using another name, chromatic half step, for a
relationship that is identical in sound to that of e' and/'. By placing sharps or flats in front of notes
on the staflP, their pitch is raised or lowered by one chromatic half step. The various available pitches
of our musical gamut can be notated different ways, that is, spelled enharmonically, and ournotational
practices are usually determined by specific musical contexts, as we shall note in Chapters 3-8.
; ;
Ex. 1-23.
=^ 3x:
m 33: ^cn
^ II 8 3
-O-
xn 33:
331 -e- jOT
=ai
or
2nds 3rds 4ths 5th£
Note that seconds are notated on adjacent lines and spaces; thirds span two
lines or two spaces; fourths span two lines and a space or two spaces and a line;
fifths span three lines or three spaces; sixths span three spaces and a line or three
lines and a space; sevenths span four lines or four spaces; and octaves span four
lines and a space or four spaces and a line.
Having determined the generic classification of an interval (as shown in Ex. 1-
23), a more precise description, such a.s perfect, major, minor, augmented, or diminished,
can be made on the basis of the number of whole and half steps involved. This is
illustrated in Ex. 1-24.
Ex. 1-24.
Perfect
IXC 331
-o e
Unison or P4 = 2 Vo P5 = 3 H P8=5 whole -steps
Perfect Prime plus two half steps
Tritones (TT)
tmr
Xt: 3X:
+ 4 =3 whole steps o5 = 2 whole -steps
plus 2 half steps
Exercises
Additional materials are contained in Materials and Structure of Music I, Workbook, Chapter 1
2. Identify the names of notes written in both clefs, indicating the specific octave (contra,
small, 1, 2, etc.) of each note.
3. Locate the different note and rest symbols in a composition.
4. Name the equivalents of different notes in smaller denominations of note values. For
example, how many sixteenths equal a dotted half note?
5. Correctly rebeam patterns of incorrectly beamed notes.
6. Transcribe a vocal melody of many individually flagged notes into correct instrumental
notation.
7. Identify the notes that occur in each part on successive strong beats in Ex. 1-1.
9. Compose eight measures of rhythm in 2 nieter, trying to avoid the repetition of any
measure's pattern.
10. Make a neat copy of Ex. 1-1.
11. Play any note on the piano (within your voice range) and sing fourths above and below
the note played. Apply the same procedure to the practice of other intervals.
12. Write the following intervals above and below g^:
p5, M3, m7, +6, m3, M2, p8, "5, M6, "7, M7, +8, mlO, +4.
04,
Ex. 2-1. Taulipang melody (after Hornbostel). From Curt Sachs, The Rise of Music in the
Ancient World, New York W. W. Norton & Company, Inc.
:
i_/0
IJ J JrJ J^J^ jo:
ff** *
JIl
"^^""X
Ex. 2-2. Vitoto Indian melody (after Bose). From Curt Sachs, The Rise of Music in the
Ancient World, New York: W. W. Norton & Company, Inc.
r r r r-^ u u ^
i:
r^
It is clear that in each of these melodies one pitch emphasized so extensively
is
19
20 TONALITY
last dot to the right, all pitches in these simple melodies converge toward one point
of focus; in music, this point of focus is called the tonic, or center of tonality.
Ex. 2-3.
In the melody of Ex. 2-1 the pitch a is stressed, for it is both the beginning and
ending pitch, as well as the pitch of longest duration. The numerous g-sharps
clearly perform a more decorative role as pitch filigrees related to their more basic
neighbors, the a's.
The melody of Ex. 2-2 is organized with c as its tonic, or focal pitch. C is heard
first and last, and it is heard more often than its two associates, b and a. In other
words, dwelt upon as a kind of ho me ba se^ a frequent point of departure and
c is
reveal identical principles of organization. Note the repetitions and emphases (in
time and pitch) that are partial causes of the respective tonics of the melodies in
Ex. 2-4.
i
i=i
The melodic excerpt of Ex. 2-5 begins and ends with the same pitch, /'. But
unlike the simpler melodies of Ex. 2-1, 2-2, and 2-3, this melody does not confirm
its first and last pitch by frequent repetitions, nor does the tonic have excessively
greater durations than other pitches. And yet, the melody does not require a full
i ^3
JH
^''i'^^ ^
i
r 1
^
This leads us to the conclusion that something about tones in a melody, in addi-
tion to accents or durations or position as first or last, can create this element of
pitch-focus in melody. This further source of tonality lies in the interval relations
between the tones of a melody. In their movement in time, the various pitches
create a tonal frame work, a kind of "flooiL2Xld_ceiling," within which the melodic
parts will all sound as related elements of the tonal design. Sorne^itches are basic
to this framework like the beams of a house, while others are more important as
the decorative overlay.
In the Mozart melody of Ex. 2-5 the longer and metrically stressed notes seem
more important to the overall contour of the whole melody. The first pitch, /', is
the beginning of the melodic pattern; it performs the vital function of "leading the
listener" into the melodic organization. Here the composer's problem is similar
to that of the painter who wishes to compose an effective picture both must im- :
mediately attract the attention of an audience and, at the same time, ensure that
this first attraction is the beginning of comprehension.
To achieve this immediate goal the painter usually organizes his canvas into
forms that "point" to the focal areas of interest. Regardless of which area of the
painting we look at first, we are always led back (if it is a good painting) into the
picture-plane by the formal arrangements of lines and forms and colors. A clear
illustration of this attention-focusing can be seen in the reproduction shown in
Ex. 2-6 and the analysis of linear forces that reveals this painting's main linear
scheme of organization.
22 lOUAlHY
Ex. 2-6.
:>i'
—
ONALITY 23
The composer ushers the listener into a suitable framework of pitch reference
he "auditory picture-plane" — by beginning his melodies with patterns that
stablish a tonality.Melodies that begin with a downbeat pattern most frequently
)egin with the tonic, while melodies that begin with an up-beat figure frequently
)egin with some other pitch, but move to the tonic without delay, often on the first
aetric accent.
Regardless of the relation of the first pitch to the melody's tonality, the
irst few pitches usually make clear all or a part of the pitch framework that will
m
(a)
f f
EI
P
I
#J.#
c)
The idea of a pitch framework within which the tones of a melody are organized
las still another facet. Notice that the metric stress we associate with thef^ of meas-
ire 5 Mozart melody (Ex. 2-8) invites us to accept this pitch as an important
of the
)oint of arrival within the whole melody. It acts as the basic high point of melodic
notion. Though actually higher, the g^ of measure 3 is rhythmically less forceful
md thus sounds like a mere upper neighbor to the more important/^ that arrives
n measure 5.
The pitchy forms the bottom of the pitch framework of this melody, and so the
Mozart melody is essentially a pitch line that forms a path from/' up tof^, then
returns to its original point of departure. Its framework of activity is
^
t
For this reason alone, / assumes
an important function within this succession of
tones. This particular framework, combined as it is with f as the first and last
pitches of the melody, creates a tonality that unmistakably bears f as its tonic.
We shall refer to this relationship, formed by the tonic and the highest and lowest
structural pitches of a melody, as the tonality frame .
Pitch Roots
Within a melody certain special groupings of pitches can confirm or deny a par-
ticular pitch as tonic. Just as the words "I am going home" take on a quite dif-
ferent meaning when rearranged to the order "Am I going home?" so the tonality
of a melody is afifected by the way in which pitches follow one another and by the
rhythms they form. Although both melody (a) and (b) below contain the same
pitch materials, they represent different "tonal meanings" because they are or-
ganized in ways which make C the tonic for the first melody, F the tonic for the
second.
Ex. 2-9a.
Pitch material
I ^e^ o- o ^>
names of tones at every octave of the musical staff, so that the thirteenth note of
any series always has the same name as the first. For instance, if at the piano we
begin with any key and play every successive white and black key, the thirteenth
key will always have the same note name as the beginning key.
Other intervals share this quality of pitch identity in lesser degrees than the
octave; the octave is the only interval to bear notes of the same name. A clear
representation of the decreasing mutual identity of pitches is found in the ove rtone
or harmonic series._ This natural order of pitches functions as a part of our hearing
process, and it operates as a part of the tone production of almost all musical in-
struments. When we hear a tone, we actually recognize only its most prominent
characteristic, for the "tone" is in reality a complex sound pattern composed of
several different elements. In a sense, we hear only a generalized tone, for the less
^1>¥^
I
S TJ 1
Fewer or more parts (or partials, as these accompanying sounds are called) are
present in the make-up of a tone, depending upon what instrument produced it.
A violin normally produces a tone that contains more partials above its funda-
mental pitch than a saxophone or a flute. But the pattern is duplicated in a greater
or lesser fashion by any musical instrument,^ and except for unusual cases, we re-
' With training one can learn to recognize some of these other parts of a tone that lie higher in
pitch than the note by which we name it.
* Only pure tones contain one simple pitch element, and they are quite rare in music.
—
26 TONALITY
cognize only the lowest (or fundamental) as the pitch of the tone. These additional
members of the harmonic series affect the qualitative aspects of instrumental and
vocal tone, controlling the tone color or timbre by their presence or absence and rela-
tive strength.
This pattern of pitch relations represented in the harmonic series is imposed
upon our every experience of musical tone, and its particular formation (the way
the various parts are ordered in relation to one another) has many interesting
world of sound. The harmonic series of a single tone is itself a kind
parallels in the
of "pitch framework," for the fundamental member of the series serves as a nucleus,
or tonic, for all of its accompanying parts.
In listening, we impose the structure of the harmonic series as one kind of guide
to tonal organization. That is, we mentally gauge the jumble of incoming sounds
and, in so doing, we have recourse to the patterns that are intrinsic to tones them-
selves, the relations of the harmonic series.
We have already noted the unique quality of the octave, the way its separate
parts fit together in a relationship that suggests a mutual identity; for this reason
the t^v•o parts even bear identical note names. The second interval that occurs "in
the harmonic series after the octave, the perfect fifth, shares this mutual identity of
its parts to a lesser degree. It is not difficult to hear this interval as two separate
pitches, even when played simultaneously. But next to the octave, the fifth is the
simplest of all pitch relationships. It is not so simple as to sound like a single tone,
but it is simple enough to create a strong effect of tonal focus, this quality causing
its lower tone to act as the fundamental pitch of the interval.
(a) (c)
(Ty
^Root
E
Root
?
Root
The first five partials of the harmonic series create five different intervals, in-
fl M.^
"^
^^
p.5.r J
m [^
-
^M.G
8'ca
Since the fundamental pitch of the harmonic series is the focal point for the
whole pattern, the root pitch for any one of these intervals is the fundamental pitch of its
TONALITY 27
series. This means that if we hear one of these intervals apart from any extraneous
context, we tend to regard the fundamental of the series to which the interval belongs as
the interval's root. Whether the melodic (successive) or harmonic (simul-
interval is
taneous), each of these intervals implies a root that can operate in melody. When
combined with rhythms, these interval relations create a pitch framework of
melodic organization.
The intervals in Ex. 2-13 are arranged in pairs according to their inversion pro-
perties. Note that turning each interval upside down produces its inversion com-
plement: the inversion of the perfect fifth is the perfect fourth; the inversion of
the major third is the minor sixth; and the inversion of the major sixth is the minor
third. This similarity is also revealed in the common root that each interval of the
pairs shares with its inversion.
This root property of musical tones can best be illustrated by separating each of
the five elementary intervals and showing their separate root effects.
L-e-
to-
^
^f-
t-o-
C8 i or [8 i
We should remember that this root effect operates with more force for the octave,
the perfect fifth, and the perfect fourth than for any other intervals. Implied within
the harmonic series itself is a dwindling relationship to a fundamental pitch; the
higher in the series an interval appears for the first time, the weaker is its root
effect. The above the fifth partial become increasingly
roots of intervals that lie
negligible. For this reason, melodies that span a range of less than a perfect fifth
or perfect fourth rely most heavily upon rhythmic details (such as repetition, dura-
tion, and accent) to produce an effect of pitch focus.
As can be seen in Ex. 2-13, even the major sixth and minor third are subject to
two possible root analyses, depending upon their position within a group of tones.
As isolated intervals, the first root possibility is more probable, but in a musical
context, melodic emphasis on the upper tone of the major sixth or on the lower
tone of the minor third can impart to those tones a significance that outweighs
their effect alone.
Returning to theMozart excerpt of Ex. 2-5 (page 21), we can now see other
reasons for the importance ofyas the tonic.
^ ^^ ^
—
28 TONALITY
From its first skip, this melody traces a pattern that reinforces / as tonic; each
new tone of the entire first two measures confirms this relationship to / as root
tone.
P. 5
M.3
M.3
These inner relationships within the pitch framework of/' c^ pre-establish the
basis for this melody's pitch organization. After the first two bars any digressions
(such as the b-Jlats and e's of measures 3 and 4) are heard as subservient in their
relations to f as tonic. And then the return of/ in the seventh measure is like the
closing of a full circle, for it comes as the return and reconfirmation of the funda-
mental pitch from which the melody originated.
Other melodies reveal these same characteristics of pitch organization to lesser
or greater degrees. The relative simplicity or complexity of any melody is deter-
mined to a great extent by the clarity with which the various pitches have been
organized in relation to a tonic. Some simple songs of widespread use hymns, —
—
communal songs, children's play chants and the like are even more tenacious
than the Mozart example in their adherence to a tonic. (Sing through the first six
measures of America for such a sample.) More complicated melodies are compara-
tively vague as pitch organizations around a tonic, avoiding rhythm and pitch
patterns that could confirm and intensify the focal role of any single pitch.
Sing or play through the melodies of Ex. 2-16 as samples of this less definite
kind of pitch organization in melody.
(tonic?)
i^iJ-
^F
^
»r iTCrP ^P
pitch frame
m ] l J1^jJ1 J^SClLi^
l
g
..if,i
TONALITY 29
^^ p gf or g/ i
only with the entrance o^ a-fiat in the last pattern that b-flat is made a very negligi-
ble possibility. Once we have heard melody through measures 3 and 4 it is
this
much easier for us to hear the beginning notes within a framework that has e-flat
as its tonic.
Many other melodies, like the Mozart melody of Ex. 2- 16c, do not by them-
selves create an altogether clear tonality. Such patterns are normally associated
with a chordal accompaniment that fulfills, with the melody, the total pitch
orientation. Such melodies usually make clear a focal pitch at some point within
their courses, but often only at the end or at some location well within their interior
structure. So even without the chordal accompaniment, once we have become
familiar with these tunes we do not have to hear their total span to realize their
pitch orientation.
When rhythmic and intervallic emphasis of a particular pitch are combined in
melody, a sense of organization results where aimless, random successions of tones
might otherwise prevail. The effect can be engaging, for musical tone by itself can
be an object of beauty, but the whole might add up to nothing that reveals a scheme
of organization : the compositional technique in such a case does not communicate
form. Tonality provides this organizational basis for all of the pitches that comprise
man's range of hearing. When we sing, play, or listen to music, tonality is one
of the most important form-giving elements, helping us to orient our tonal
"thoughts" into comprehensible images. We shall learn about other agents of
musical organization as our study progresses, but of all those pertinent to the
musical experience, tonality is most intrinsic to musical tone.
)
30 TONALITY
We can isolate four simple questions that are important to any decision about
tonal organization of a melody. The study of any particular melody should begin
with the answers to each of these questions.
1 What is the last pitch, and what is the first pitch of rhythmic importance (be-
cause of metric position or greater duration or both)?
2) Is any single pitch made prominent because of its frequent occurrence, or its
several repetitions, or because of its and stresses?
relatively greater durations
3) What intervals occur during the first patterns of pitch motion, and what is
Many melodies incorporate at least three schemes for establishing a tonic, others
no more than two, and still others commandeer every available force to establish
a single tone as the focus of pitch organization. It is the basic problem of the per-
former and the listener to recognize the various clues of pitch organization and
then allow the conclusions to serve as guides to musical understanding. Three
melodies of simple pitch structure are shown below, each accompanied by its
tonality frame.
p^ip?
c;T7[; ^r i
cj
ii
i
^#
rr cp^' ^
Tonali^y frame
J J^ i
J ns A
Tonic
Tonality frame
;z=^
i
*r Tonic
:: : :
TONALITY 31
—m
m -zl
m m
Pr i
-P
r r ^r If r r
s:
i i ^
Tonality frame
P ^
^ flf I?
'
Tonic
Exercises
See Chapter 2 of Materials and Structure of Music I, Workbook for more detailed work.
1 . Using only the given prescription, write a short melody that fulfills each of the following
A) Tonality, F Meter, :
I
: Pitches ^^
B) Tonality,
C) Tonality,
A
D) Tonality, D: Meter,
: Meter,
flat: Meter,
C :
|: Pitches:
Pitches
2; Pitches
^
m
§ h '
Mode,
3
ani
Scale
zn
f i
zn s
? 3t
32
—
Pitch scale
-e-
The result of this abstraction is what we call the major scale of F. DnU^ KhO fJ
Any series of pitches that duplicates the same pattern of successive intervals
1,1, ^/a, 1, 1, 1, ^/2 — is a major scale. (The number 1 represents a whole step, ^/a
represents a half step.) The note combinations of Ex. 3-2 cojntain five pat,terns ^^
which are major, five which are not. ^tiCjil ,qIxJQ^ X'CuXiiLf ^^'/ ^^^
Ex. 3-2.
JUcdL<^ <c
-'^'
k^ l>o - ly ^E f O
t> I'O
(D^-
o ff"
in: =e=fct
©
o itr
i oj^^ Q
11
©
'>
O M °>" °- p
-»—1U3_
® o -e-
'' O
•®
tt
W^ ^3 n—
^n e-
1^
®
3 .. O lM> O '- ^ ^
We can transpose^; each of the scales shown in Ex. 3-2 so that the pattern of
intervals remains the same but the initial note is always c'. In this way the simi-
larities and the differences in the various arrangements become more apparent.
Ex. 3-3.
A ® ®
n O 331
331 -O-
331
O " -O-
$ O "
any series of notes can be transposed if the series is kept intact but changed to a higher or lower pitch
register.
34 KEY, MODE, AND SCALE
®
331 331
o ini 331
n o =o=n=
^o^
® ®
n O 331 i» O 1, i>o "
"or -e- o
Each of the scales in Ex. 3-3 is a " C scale," although half do not qualify as
"major scales."
. Q qD Our music reveals that certain arrangements of pitches have been
literature of
.1
i^^^ most favored by composers during the past several hundred years, and for this
reason we shall pay particular attention to these. We might note in passing that
our musical staff, with its alternating lines and spaces, and the black and white
key pattern of the piano both have a simple relationship with the C major scale
pattern.
Ex. 3-4.
331 O «> °
331 mnn
cl dl el fi gl ai bl c2
Ifbegin a scale on any note other than C (written on the staff or played at
we
the keyboard), and if we use only unaltered pitches, different kinds of scales are
produced. As in major, there will still be a pair of minor seconds at some two points
within the series, but they will no longer fall between the 3-4 and 7-8 notes where
they fell in the major pattern. It is the shifting of location of these two smaller
intervals within the series of scale tones that determines the scale form.
KEY, MODE. AND SCALE 35
Ex. 3-5.
^xfXtl fKcDMA£)J^
Major ( Ionian) Dorian
^^ ^^^
^ Mixolydian
Locrian
^
'-vur.j
m • •
-^^^tU^^^ V^C7)f/. Aeolian( minor) /)(rlurO
-' jj^
n
i
i
Each of these different arrangements of seven notes is called a scale or mode^ '^
Ifwe again use C as a common tonic, we can see more readily how changes of
mode are brought about with the relocation of the two half steps in relation to
the tonic note.
Ex. 3-6.
Phrygian Lydian
o-
-e- "
o ^^t° " I I
j ^ .U .. oJ:-.-^° "
Locrian
i> o t'o
.. l>o ^n ^o g
2 The word modi is derived from the sa me wo r d root a s mood. Each of the traditional
mode names
is of Greek origin Ionian, Dorian, Phrygian, Lydian, Mixolydian, Aeolian, and Locrian. In most instances
:
Since scales "1" and "6" of Ex. 3-6 are better known today as major and minor,
we shall henceforth drop their impressive Greek names in favor of the more
familiar.
Itjs significant that six of the seven modes contain a perfect fifth interval between
their tonics and their fifths. As we can recall from our study of the tonality frame-
work in Chapter 2, the diminished fifth is not an interval that is conducive to a
strong root effect. For this reason, the Locrian mode traditionally has been regarded
more as a "theoretical mode" (i.e., not used in music), and it is rarely found as
the pitch basis for a composition.
The melodies of Ex. 3-7 are labeled according to the mode used. Sing or play
each,making certain to produce all pitches accurately in order to recreate the true
modal character.
fM i JT r i
T^r I
pflj 1^^
^
t f
^
:
^ * w
^ -0 —w t
^
^ J) 1 (!;iJ
J 1
^ * d m * '
$ • w
t m ^ i
Mode: Dorian
m£ f i I
f
KEY, MODE, AND SCALE 37
m W
^^^^
L/ i
r
i'
Lf f i
the person using them. For instance, the melody in Ex. 3-8 is essentially Dorian, but
notice that the seventh scale note is raised a semitone in measure six.
(h (ii)
P ^^ p i ^P ^^
u i,^ nA
^ i- i.
la i
^ ?
By raising this note one semitone, the melody line leads with even greater
j
impetus into the tonic pitch, D. This strong melodic relation— the melodic half- /
step below tonic pitch —
gives this seventh scale degree the name "leadingjtone^"|/
Two modes contain this relation between their seventh and eighth notes without
note alterations: Major and Lydian. To contain the leading tone, the remaining
modes would require the raising (by sharp or natural accidentals as the case may '
require) of the seventh scale member. Without this change their seventh degrees
are called "subtqnics," to distinguish them from the semitone step of the leading
tone — tonic relation.
When the seventh degree of the Dorian mode is raised by one half-step, a pat-
tern very similar to the major mode results. It is different from major only in that
its lower half-step falls in a different location.
Ex. 3-9.
1
Y
^ ., o o o t^'^ Major
i
:^^fe Dorian with raised 7
j o ^^ :xsz
^
This scale is better known by still another name, melodic minor. It shares the
minor second placement between pitches 2 and 3 of the Aeolian mode, but it is
different in that its other minor second occurs between 7-8 rather than 5-6.
This whole scale, then, consists of the interval succession of 1 | 1 1 i. —————— 1 1
A third kind of minor scale results if the seventh note of the Aeolian (natural
minor) pattern is raised a semitone to create a leading tone. This new pattern
differs from the other modes discussed in that it contains three minor-second inter-
vals rather than the usual two. Such an arrangement leaves an unusual interval,
the augmented second, between 6-7,
Ex. 3-10.
^^- XE
4 o A- ° ^
Aeolian
m "cr
/i7q
+2
The fourth scale of Ex. 3-10 is traditionally called harmonic minor. Along with the
melodic and natural patterns, it occurs frequently in the music most familiar to us.
It important to remember that each of the modes derives its unique character
is
from (1) the intervals formed by the separate pitches with their tonic, and (2) the
intervalsbetween any pitch and its immediate predecessor and successor. For this
reason we can
profit from a growing acquaintance with the different intervals that
form the scale types discussed thus far: major (Ionian), natural minor (Aeolian),
melodic minor, harmonic minor, Dorian, Phrygian, Lydian, and Mixolydian.
Ex. 3-lla.
M2 M3 P4 P5 M6 M7 M2 m3 P4 P5 m6 m7
^ One tradition regards melodic minor as actually consisting of two different scale patterns, one
the ascending form (as shown here in Ex. 3-9), the other what we shall call natural minor.
)
M'2. m3 P4 P5 M6 M7 M2 m3 P4 P5 m6 M7
Dorian Phrygian m2
m2 A m2
-^»3-
311 fro-
TT ^^ m o " zz:
* o^o V °
*
?JT,,^l_^^
-
m2 mS P4 P5 mG m7
z^
M2 m3 P4 P5 M6 m6
Lydian m2 Mixolydian
-&-
o o h; ir^
M2 M3 +4 P5 M6 M7 M2 M3 P4 P5 M6 m7
In addition to the tonic, which is the central tone of any scale of pitches, the tones
of all scales are named in a way that classifies their particular function within the
total scale system. This gives us an additional name for reference. Ex. 3-1 lb shows
a C major scale and the various symbols which are commonly used to represent
each degree.
Ex. 3-llb.
C Major scale jj IV VI
Supertonic Subdominant Submediant ( Tonic
-o- 331
I I
Leading tone
in V vn
This set of names is applied to every scale or mode, no matter what its structure,
except when the seventh degree is not a semitone below tonic in this case the pitch
;
The name for each scale degree is derived from its functional relationship to
tonic. The set of definitions that follows explains the distinction of each name as
it relates to the scale set.
i
Tonic: Tone of focus for the scale
Dominant: The tone a fifth above the tonic
$ Tonic Dominant
.
Mediant: The tone between (the "medium" or "halfway" tone) the tonic and its
dominant
Dominant
Tonic Mediant
$ T^ "^o"'^
Subdominant
Submediant: The tone between (The "medium" or "halfway" tone) the tonic and
its subdominant
Tonic Submediant
E±S
$ F^ Subdominant
Key Signatures
The notation of music developed concurrently with the gradual adoption of our
major-minor scale systems.* One adaptation of music writing to these scales has
been the development of a shorthand method for previewing the pitches encoun-
tered in a composition. Such is the essential function of the key signature that appears
at the beginning of many compositions. These collections of sharps or flats (or
their absence) indicate the alterations of the "natural" note system that must be
made to achieve the desired pitches. Without sharps or flats the musical staff" re-
* Most of our current music notation practices were established during the seventeenth and
presents a set of notes that renders a major scale possible only from C, a natural
minor scale only from A.
Ex. 3-12.
. C Major
^ O
o O ini
331
m A Minor
TT «
( natural
ZJJZ
ICC
Since the lines and the spaces of the staff have "built-in" half steps and whole
steps, untransposed modes result from unaltered successions of pitches. A beginning
on any note other than C or A will produce one of the modes, Dorian, Phrygian,
Lydian, Mixolydian, or Locrian. (See Ex. 3-5.)
A composer writing a composition for soprano voice based on the natural minor
scale could, by pitch alterations, shift the intervals of the unaltered C scale so that
minor results. The required alterations are those which produce minor seconds
where they occur in the pattern of natural minor, between 2-3 and 5-6.
Ex. 3-13.
~0" 331
o l^o " ^
As a convenience to the composer and the performer, the three flats {B-Jiat,
E-Jlat, and A-Jlat) required to make this shift of pattern can be indicated as a
"signature" at the beginning of the line after the clef sign. This relieves the com-
poser of inserting necessary alterations every time these notes occur within the
melody.
Ex. 3-14.
In the same way, the key signature of three flats can be used to produce the major
mode from a scale that begins on the note E-flat.
42 KEY, MODE, AND SCALE
Ex. 3-15.
E Phrygian Et Major
o 9 o *
1 o irc o «>
The order in which flats or sharps are placed in the key signature follows a set
pattern; it is based on the sequence of pitch changes required to produce a reloca-
tion of the scale.
Using the unaltered mode based on F, one alteration is needed to create the
major scale the lowering of B to B-flat. Since this is the first flat demanded in
:
the notational system, it is always the first flat to the right of the clef sign of any
signature containing flats. Ex. 3-16 shows the successive alterations required to
produce major scales from the natural notes of the musical staff.
Ex. 3-16.
~rr xn
o 9
ZJJC -o-
* TT e- ^ =o=a=
o 9
IS
331 E^ o o - im
jCC -e-
It o ~XT-
v\r ^1 ?
P T> O- 331
O ^> "
t> 9
Notice that after introduced, E-flat follows; A-flat next; then D-flat and
B-flat is
BCDEFGABCDEFGAB
12 3 4 5
etc.
With the addition of sharps for the creation of major scales, the series follows
a sequence by fives. The first sharp alteration required is F-sharp; the second C-
sharp; the third G-sharp; and so on.
12
FGAB CDEF GABC DEFG
3 4
etc.
Ex. 3-17.
i» o " xn
33: n O 3X1
O o
o
^ LI O 33= =e=33=
i -e- 33=
-o- tl o =n=
31=
yu o
p
»«
~n~ 33=
331
33= T^ <> O
-
This series of pitch akerations has historical significance as the circle of fifths,
which is a circular scheme that pictorially represents the system of keys for our
major-minor scale system.
>—
/.^^\>
circle q/-
/''r AV, rD
*
-3b 3HA
61
H
Db 6b Cb
Two scales which have the same tonic but which do not contain identical pitches
on every scale degree are called parallel scales. For instance, the keys of B-Jlat
minor and B-fiat major are parallel keys. On the other hand, two scales (or keys)
which contain identical pitches but do not have the same tonic are called relative
scales. Thus G minor and B-fiat major are relatives, and D major is the relative major
of B minor.
Ex. 3-19.
-''"'^^
i ^ ^l,.^-
Bb Natural minor
M 3
i2L
m G Minor
Bb Major
'>
D Major
.11."
B Minor
»'
44 KEY, MODE. AND SCALE
Since double sharps (X )and double flats (b l>) are not generally used in key
signatures, the system of keys normally is extended no further than seven sharps
(for C-sharp major or A-sharp minor) and seven flats (for C-Jiat major or A-fiat
minor). But the progression by fifths can be continued until the original pitch,
spelled enharmonically^ as B-sharp, is reached.
The key signatures of our music are usually derived from the diatonic scale struc-
tures. As a music based on the pitch resources of some other scale does not
result,
readily fit our traditional signatures. A composition that makes free use of the twelve
tones of the chromatic scale gains little of practical use from a key signature. If
used, the composer and the performer are kept busy staying abreast of alterations
and, ultimately, their frequent cancellation.
^i ^
Scale
(|
Bb Db
^° ' ^°
^ r y l9-=-
^ Enharmonic tones are those which bear different note names but which have the same pitch. For
example, F-JIat and E, A-fat and G-skarp, B-sharp and C are all enharmonic pairs.
—
Our foregoing discussion of keys, modes, and scales must be amplified somewhat
to account for the pitch organization of many melodies. For the sake of variety,
more than one pitch framework is often used within a composition.
The word modulation refers to this shifting from one pitch focus, or tonic, to
another. Thus we can say that a modulation occurs at section "Y" of Ex. 3-22,
xi ^=zi—
"^•icrcircrcJ i
TO^
^ f
Tonic
V r V II
Tonic
J 11 ^^
Sections "X" and "Y" both utilize simple scale materials within their respective
boundaries, each incorporating only four different notes. It is the new focus on G,
following the focus on C,. that constitutes the modulation or change of key.
Change of mode alone does not achieve change of tonic. On
the same result as
the contrary, we can best describe the pitch organization of Ex. 3-23 by noting
that a common tonic, F, persists throughout. The change of mode from minor to
major at section "Y" represents mutation rather than modulation. In this case,
mutation refers to scale change as distinct from tonic change. In this example the
contrast is affected by a change to the parallel major scale.
^^
^^nnm^^ j
^ £ "r ^m
^^ ^
\[j
jt nn^ \S-}i
z-
s P ^P
Pitch frames
^
Modulation is closely allied with the sectional divisions the phrases of musical —
I'brm —
which we shall discuss later in Chapters 4 and 5. For the present, however,
we can observe that the melody of Ex. 3-22 illustrates this formal division by shift-
ing tonics halfway through the melody. In a similar way, Ex. 3-23 is sectionalized
—
by the change of mode the mutation that occurs after measure eight. —
It is this psychological shift from the old tonic to the new that makes modulation
an element of variety, for only the relation of the original tonality to another
establishes a condition of change. Either one without the other would represent
merely one particular tonality, and thus provide no contrast.
In establishing a new pitch focus, the same techniques of repetition, duration,
metric placement, and strong interval embellishment (fifth or third above, leading
tone relation) are used. The melodies which follow incorporate these various meth-
ods of establishing a new tonic. It is interesting to see that several techniques can
be combined, as in Ex. 3-25, to create an immediate and unmistakable shift of
pitch focus.
^— I I
-&
Bb major
m£m m.
pr cJ i r r
i J'J J
X2I m m m
^
I
t
pr r If P^ f^
i
—F major
n ^ u ^
H — Bt major
¥ r r r c
^ wnr gr u
Ex. 3-25. Chopin : Madchens Wunsch.
A major
^ •
H J J
^ u S g
^
^ iEt
J
rzs
it
^s>s
minor
I J- e
J'^ I ^-
, Ji
£
\± A major
i^^
^ ^j^ _l
KEY, MODE, AND SCALE 47
how brief, as key changes. In this sense we are dealing with an organizational mat-
ter that is subject to relative judgments. Pitch focus is itself a product of our psy-
chological response to tone patterns, so it is reasonable to assume that one person's
reaction might be slightly different from another's. For instance, one person might
regard the "Y" section of Ex. 3-26 as an interesting diversion within the tonality of
D; another might hear the passage as a definite modulation to the new tonic of A.
r r p
i
^J- J i
j^J p J i
J j'] ^ ^
tonic is^c onfirmed by at least two c onsecutive sections of the melody.^ Thus the melody
of Ex. 3-27 does not contain a change of key; the emphasis on E-flat in the bracket-
ed section represents emphasis on the dominant within the tonality of yl-flat. On
the other hand, the melody of Ex. 3-28 modulates, for the emphasis continues long
enough for the new pitch frame to be established clearly in the listener's mind.
fi'^^cS- ^ ^'
i
^cjcj- i
&r ^
^' } Pvpv
\ > |
I^r pv^P
^m
r^CfrTTJ J rJ3
• We shall make our definition more preci.se in Chapter 4 by regarding ihe phrase, with its attendant
cadence, as the unit of measurement for modulation.
J — #
W
±=R
H jn j J' J ^ s
j"
10
^r^^r/exf i i
^ nsj r i r ^r^^r,!; !
0^ —
20
p
15
i
gsf M'U ^HHf PLJ r i r Br rJr ^^
25
^P I- I S) ^-
i
j ^LLT i r Peg
^
30
I 5
w 5
t*f^ m
The melody in Ex. 3-29 contains a beginning section based within the tonality
frame of e' — e^. The leading tone —tonic ((/-sharp e) pattern in measure eight is
the final substantiation of this tonic before a change occurs, in the section marked
J
—— :
"X." This second section revolves around the pitch frame of ^^ g', with emphasis
on the fifth of d^. From measure fifteen through seventeen d and /-sharp are em-
phasized by repetition and by the leading tone created by the sharped c. Measures
eighteen and twenty relate these two pitches (rf and^-sharp) to a new pitch frame
based on a tonic of 5. Thus section "Y" brings a new tonic to the fore, followed by
a return (Section "Z") to the beginning tonality frame of e' e^. We can digest this
tonal movement into a simple diagram.
Tonics E B E_
Exercises
See Chapter 4 of the Materials and Structure of Music I, Workbook for more detailed work.
3. Find examples of melodies which begin and end clearly in the same key but which contain
separates one melodic unit from another. Like the written commas and colons
and periods of speech, the cadence is a heard signal that helps us organize our world
of tones into comprehensible forms.
The cadences of music arenamed according to the roles they play in tonal or-
ganization. In regard to melody alone we shall discuss only two basic types: the
terminal cadence, denoting a partial or total cessation of melodic activity; and the
progressive cadence, marking a break in tonal flow but with the suggestion of continua-
tion. Compared with the punctuation marks of language, the terminal cadence
resembles the period, while the progressive type is more like the break in a sentence
marked by a comma.
50
MELODIC CADENCES 51
The Beethoven melody in Ex. 4-1 contains each of these cadential types, pro-
gressive at the midway point in measure four, terminal at the end.
m /^'
-| '
^m r r If r r \
r
Progressive
cadence
s r r I
f r ^ i
r ^ 1
Terminal
cadence
^5'-'
J$>
Rhythm and pitc h com bine^t o produc e the c ad ence effe ct. Although each of
the two segments of the melody in Ex. 4-1 closes with the same rhythmic unit
( J- J J )' the two cadences create different expectations because of the positions
of their cadence pitches within the melody's tonality. The e of measure four is
foreign to the d— a frame established in measures 1-3. Since it does not create
an effect of repose, it sets the stage for continued activity. By contrast, the d of
measure eight, as tonic of the melody, renders permanent conclusion a logical
consequence.
In addition to the increased note durations in measures four and eight, another
aspect of rhythm confirms the arrested motion at these two cadence points. Notice
that the pitch contour of this melody consists of a two-measure shape that rises
gently in measure two, falling to a low point at the beginning of measure three.
^
Ex. 4-2.
m
The same shape is repeated in measures three and four, creating a balance of
melodic contour, a musical statement modeled from the repetition of a two-meas-
ure unit. The continuation of this pattern into measure four, combined with the
contrasting longer durations (dotted quarter and half), impHes that the close of a
definite musical section has been reached.
Ex. 4-3.
^m r r I
f r r I
f ^
—
52 MELODIC CADENCES
While these rhythmic groupings create the cadential caesura — the pause effect
it is the cadence pitch that determines the kind of punctuation.
It is interesting and informative
experiment with other pitches as cadential
to
notes in the middle of the Beethoven melody, to observe the punctuative effects
produced by different relations. Since the second four measures duplicate the first
four (excepting the cadence pattern itself), separation of the two by a terminal
cadence produces a monotonous, disjunct effect, as illustrated in melody a of
Ex. 4-4. Play or sing each of these versions to observe the contrasting cadential
effects.
\v- ^.c-^
•>
<^..^
(e)
T-^ etc.
rr i
f rr i
fpr
With the exception o[d a.x\df-sharp, the alternate cadence pitches perform about
the same role as e of the original. They imply, in varying degrees, the continuation
of melodic flow. However, all would not be successful choices for this melody be-
cause some, such as /-sharp and g, destroy the two-measure shape established as
a pattern of contour in measures one and two.
Cadences frequently lie at equally spaced locations within a melody, thus giving
a simple continuity to the flow of tones. But all melodies do not follow the same
four-measure pattern clearly stamped into the fabric of Ex. 4-1, and some melodies
are notably free of the regularity imposed by phrases of equal lengths.
Melodies "a," "b," and "c" of Ex. 4-5 have balanced pattern lengths; but
melodies "d," "e," and "f" deviate from this regularity of formation. The last two
examples are interesting because their cadences establish unbalanced melodic
units as the basis for melodic form.
M
^ 1(3) r
-0 —w »— =gfe=^ "2^ * r
i
(3)
^ n{3) 1(3)
(a(l> J.
J J
J I
J. J J i
^? w
i g W
U
Ex. 4-5b. Schubert: Piano Trio, Op. 100 (Scherzo).
^^ ^(4)
^
r
^^^ i '
^
P r r r If
(4)
^
Ex. 4-5c. Bartok :Mikrokosmos, "Staccato and Legato," Vol. V. Copyright 1940 by Hawkes
& Son (London) Ltd. Reprinted by permission of Boosey & Hawkes Inc.
n(3) 1(3)
$ ^ '
Vj J •!
t 'nj ^ g
Ij 'jTil fL''
^g I
^ 1(3)
(3)
I
r
?^^ m 3
1(3)
(3)
t rrrnrrrr i
rrMrrrrirrrrirrHr^
1(2) 1(3) r 1(3)
rrrrirrrirrrr rrrrirrHrrrr i i
rrrrK^
54 MELODIC CADENCES
^^
1(5)
f i[^ ^ i r
- »
(5)
ti
$ r ic-Zcj ^
rj^ i
nmrJ P
1(4)
M' F m w
^^
f ucj- If ^"ir/ i r
l
U^^if^ (5)
The patterns that span the distance from one cadence to the next are called
phrases. This unit of melodic form will be discussed in greater detail in Chapter 7,
but no discussion of the melodic cadence could be clear without some attention to
its nature. We might turn to language again for comparison to note that the
melodic phrase is similar to the "clause" or "phrase" of the sentence. And like its
language counterpart, the musical phrase can usually be reduced further into
smaller units called motives. For instance, the third phrase of the melody of Ex. 4-5d
Ex. 4-6.
repetition
motive a motive b
For reason a break occurs between the patterns of measures three and four
this
As we noticed earlier (page 52), it is the particular pitch at the cadence that
determines the cadential effect by implying or denying continued motion. A
strong progressive cadence forcefully suggests continuation, while a strong terminal
cadence unquestionably marks finality. The most appro-
tonic as cadence pitch
priately fulfills this latter function, while its scale relatives a major or minor third
or perfect fifth above can also, under most circumstances, imply closure to a limited
degree.
The third cadence of the Brahms-Haydn melody in Ex. 4-5 is an imperfec Uerminal _
caden ce. Since the ultimate pitch d is preceded by the tonic B-flat, and since B-Jlat
is the root of this melodic relation B-flat — d, the cadence pattern does not un-
equivocally demand continuation. And yet, it also does not suggest that melodic
motion has concluded; such a cadence lies in the never-never-land between the
weak p rogressive cadence a.nd^tjtrong terminal cadence.
The cadences of the melody in Ex. 4-7 are similar. Here there are two based on
a, the perfect fifth above tonic d. In measure four the a acts as a point of brief rest.
It fits into the frame of the D-tonality (since the tonic, D, is also the root of the re-
lation d' — a), so it does not demand resolution to the same extent as other pitches,
such as e, c-sharp, or g. Compare the relative intensification of this cadence when
these other pitches are substituted for a. Play or sing each version several times
to realize the precise cadential effects.
Ex. 4-7.
^ Original
p
O I (
^^
g ^ it.
^m
Revised with cjf
m \
Pr
i -e-
This combination of two phrases separated by a progressive cadence and ending with a terminal
^
Revised with g
m ^
?z==®^
:^ ^
Revised with
P=:^
e
£^ ^
In a later chapter we ways accompanying chords and melodic
shall discuss the
activity in other parts can emphasize or weaken the cadential effect of a particular
pitch. This is one of the ways chords can add variety and alter the function of
pitches within a melody. Without chordal underpinnings a melody must depend on
rhythm and pitch relations within its tonality frame to create the available nuances
of cadence, which act as form-giving elements.
Transient-Terminal Cadences
If melodies always hovered tightly within the orbit of a single tonality frame and
scale, the foregoing discussion of cadences would suffice for all music. But since
many melodies fluctuate in their obedience to a single pitch focus,^ some cadences
momentarily create the effect of closure, and yet they do not meet the other speci-
fications demanded by our definition of the terminal cadence.
The middle portion of the melody in Ex. 4-8 pauses momentarily on d, a tone
made prominent by its leading tone, c-sharp. If the initially established tonality
were d, then this would be a simple terminal cadence. But since G is the tonic of
the tonality frame (g' —
d^), the pause on d does not represent a perfect terminal
cadence.
is ^
m ^ ^m
^^
We shall call such a cadence transient-terminal. Only if the motion to the cadential
d is interpreted as a modulation from the tonality of G to the tonality of D can it
^ See the earlier section of Chapter 3 that deals with modulation, pp. 45-49,
MELODIC CADENCES 57
pitch degrees that frequently function in this way are the subdominanl, the mediant,
and the submediant.
Each of the melodies in Ex. 4-9 contains at least one transient-terminal cadence.
Of particular importance to melodic structure is the way each of these "temporary
tonics" is established as a pitch of emphasis, either by association with its leading
tone, with pitches a fifth or third above, or by its successive repetitions.
^ ^ al 3JI
n
p-
r
mf
wm
^
rrr f i
^7 MC;^^ ^i^p
i![
^m
'^^'^r &cr i
r
s £j^a:' i
r
j:?^^
^-^
^=^
') r r r h ium mmm Submediant
S M f
'
r p \
i!
m ^
Ex. 4-9c. John Dunstable: Soncta Maria.
1i ^ *—^r
J-
^N- JJJ1
Dominant
S
58 MELODIC CADENCES
tariejt
m^ m m
^m Mediant
^ p m
A more detailed discussion and precise classification of cadence patterns must
wait until harmonic features can be added to the rhythm and pitch elements that
create the cadential effect.^ The present limitation of basic types to the progressive,
the terminal (both perfect and imperfect), and the transient-terminal suffices for a
comprehension of the usual patterns found in melody.
ture contains melodies whose cadences operate at both of these extremes, as well
as at levels in between.
It is not difficult to follow the melodic structure of some fourteenth century
music, for instance, because many of the outlining cadences use a specific scale
formula, 7-6-1 as their pitch content.
m U 7 6—1
£
J)lJ ^H i5
7 6 — 1
^^ 7 6
^
1
a)r °
»r
Pitch:
m
Ex. 4-1 Ic. Lassus : Cantiones, No. 1,
*5ii
$
The use of these established "signals" of cadence goes somewhat beyond the mere
"rhythmic pause" of the cadential effect as we discussed it earlier; their repeated
use in music established "road signs" of musical form. The
them as the powerful
appearance of a conventionalized pitch-rhythm pattern reinforces the mere dura-
tional and tonal ingredients in differentiating the parts of melodic form.
60 MELODIC CADENCES
Bach, Handel, Haydn, Mozart, and Beethoven shared as common property the
perfect terminal cadence of 7-1, the leading tone resolving to tonic, with a weak-
to-strong metric location. It power of this simple pitch-rhythm pattern to
is the
confirm tonality relations that makes it a significant cadential formula in the entire
history of music. Aside from its frequent appearances in music written by com-
posers of the classic and Baroque eras, this cadence pattern can be found in the bulk
of popular and community music whose most conspicuous features are simplicity
of tonality and phrase organization.
One special adaptation of the 7-1 figure became a cadential trademark in the
music of Haydn, Mozart, and Beethoven, whereby the usual weak-strong rhythmic
placement of the two parts of the 7-1 pitch pattern were regrouped into a strong-
weak relation of syncopation. The leading tone of such a cadence is suspended
from a preceding weak position, thus delaying resolution to the tonic pitch.*
^m strong weak
7
JH
strong weak
In our own day, some composers have used certain cadential patterns frequently
enough to justify their identification with the composer's personal style. Since
harmony plays a crucial role in determining the character of most recent music,
it is increasingly difficult to identify cadential patterns as melodic formulae alone.
However, composers whose music emphasizes melody as a dominant ingredient
use melodic cadences of some uniformity.
Ex. 4-1 3a. Hindemith : Theme and Four Variations. (C) 1947 by B. Schott's Soehne, Mainz.
Reprinted by permission.
^± ^ weak
'
This delay is further enhanced, as we shall see later in Chapter 17, by the underlying chords.
:
MELODIC CADENCES 61
Ex. 4-13b. Hindemith : When Lilacs Last in the Dooryord Bloomed. (C) 1948 by Schott &
Co., Ltd., London. Reprinted by permission.
It seems clear, then, that in addition to its role as a determinant of form, the
cadence can be elevated to a position of significance and individuality, thereby
recognizing its contribution to the "personality" of a particular work, the works
of a single composer, or even the compositions from an entire era of music-making.
In this latter role the cadence is a prime element that suggests, perhaps in a more
direct way than other musical elements, the peculiar flavor that makes one melody
utterly and uniquely different from another.
Exercises
See C:hapter 4 of the Materials and Structure of Music I, Workbook for more detailed work.
1 . Write several melodies, each of which fits one of the following schemes
Compose other melodies with similar characteristics.
3
(a) Key, D minor Meter
transient-terminal terminal
cadence on A cadence
(d)
Progressive terminal
cadence on F cadence
(G)
2. Copy several complete chorale tunes from a collection such as the 371 Bach Chorales.
Using only the melody as a basis for determination, name the cadence types.
3. Write one- or two-measure fragments which illustrate each kind of cadence type. Use
a variety of keys, major, minor, and modal.
Example: -/L-t y - *
i
ttj "w transient-terminal cadence in D-major
4. Analyze the melodies of any chosen composition for scales. Having determined the tonic
pitch for a passage, arrange the pitch materials in an ascending scale that begins with the
lowest note and extends to the highest. From this determine the basic scale.
Forma/ Characteristics
of Melody:
5
The Motive
and Phrase
Since music takes place in time, we hea r musical patter ns in successioa. At times
a pattern is repeated immediately; at other times a new unit is followed by a pat-
tern stated earlier; and at times a pattern is changed slightly or a great deal, there-
9 10 11 12 13 14
5-1 illustrates, the phrases need not be of equal length; however, each of the phrases
is set off by a clear cadence. Not until we count the measures do we become aware
of unequal phrase lengths.
Both phrases of Ex. formed from successive statements of smaller pat-
5-1 are
terns, each of which is two measures long. Thus, just as this melody is made up of
two sections, so each of its phrases is made up of smaller units called motives. These
motives are simple rhythmic patterns such as those seen in measures 1 and 2
and the settling on tonic, brings the first phrase to a terminal cadence. The
phrase contains three two-measure units, two of which are rhythmically alike.
first
The third unit contrasts somewhat with what has gone before, completing the
rhythmic design of the phrase.
The motive at the beginning of the second phrase estab-
restatement of the initial
lishes a strong bond between the two phrases. In this case the phrase does not end
with the statement of the contrasting pattern, but continues for two more meas-
ures.Because of the particular rhythmic and tonal points of emphasis that have
been established, we might expect the melody to halt on the first beat of measure
12 on the pitch g. However, the dotted rhythmic figure is preceded by a new pat-
tern as well as a pitch motion up to d; consequently, more music is demanded to
ing force of this tune. In many melodies a short pattern may form the basis for the
unfolding of the initial musical phrase and subsequent phrases as well. In some
melodies this is not as easily observed as in the tune America, and in still others no
short unit or section is repeated (see Ex. 5-2). However, some characteristic pat-
tern usually appears as a formal binder.
*e:r:= tef
s
'y-M'i^ f|fr.s' |
fv rr:!]i^
i
Ex. 5-3 does not have a noteworthy rhythm, and yet its pitch outUne forms a strik-
ing pattern of great melodic potentiality.
-o- ux:
331
IE
Cs /7\
pi
In Ex. 5-5, repetition of the motive ( F^ I
JV ) forms the rhythm of the
whole phrase until the change at the cadence. The pitch pattern of step-down step-
up is restated at different pitch levels until the approach to the cadence. This rep-
etition at different levels gives prominence to the rhythmic figure and makes the
motive the organizing feature of the phrase.
Still another factor gives shape to the Bach phrase. Notice the filling-in of the
—
octave {g g'), and the fifth {g — d'). The sketch in Ex. 5-6 illustrates that while
the phrase unfolds rhythmically, dominated by a melodic pedal on g', and
it is
—
balanced by the descent from d' g. The introduction of c'-sharp and the change
to ascending motion momentarily shifts the emphasis from G to D, creating a
transient-terminal cadence.
Ex. 5-6.
FORMAL CHARACTERISTICS OF MELODY: MOTIVE AND PHRASE 65
In many melodies the motive's pitch pattern is varied when it is repeated- The
emphasis created by the repetition of a particular rhythmic pattern is typical of
compositions based on dance forms. Such an emphasis is created by the rhythmic
the close (measures 6-7; measures 14-15). As we can see, this new pattern
( J JV J I
^n J J\l) assumes greater importance in the second phrase by
being stated twice. Notice also that as this motive is made more prominent, the
initial motive receives less emphasis by appearing three instead of five times.
^
Ex. 5-7. Pachelbel : Sarabande.
m
*t
1
m m
ffr
11
pr irjr
12 13
J^
14 ,
15 1^
^^ 16
i«t
r'cJitnrpr i
^fr m i
When a motive is repeated hnth pi trh and rhythmic changes may occur. Ex. 5-8
shows some changes a motive might undergo when it is repeated. Notice that the
also that the restatement is shorter, three beats instead of four. However, the re-
petition has six articulations: the fourth, c —g is filled-in in the restatement.
Ex. 5-8. Piston: Symphony No. 4, I. (C) 1953 by Associated Music Publishers, Inc., New
York. Reprinted by permission.
Piacevole 43d:CUt^5
'
i St
, ,
^^--
^ m
Sometimes a motive and a phrase are the same. When such a merger occurs,
as in Ex. 5-9, a motive has the significance usually attached to a phrase.
Allegro
66 FORMAL CHARACTERISTICS OF MELODY: MOTIVE AND PHRASE
tion; the repetition of the motive begins on the second instead of the first beat.
Ex. 5-10. Prokofiev: Violin Concerto No. 2, I. By permission of the International Music
Company, New York.
Allegro moderato
r ri'Tr'^r i
r^
Phrase Structure
Earlier in this chapter and in the preceding chapter we discussed some pro-
perties of the musical phrase, and the vital role which the motive frequently plays
in its organization. At a larger level of structure, the phrase is also a distinctiye
agent of form. Thejnotive is relatively short and is usually a sub-unit of the phrase^
An examination of music shows that there are numerous possibilities for construct-
ing phrases.
A common type of phrase structure is seen in Ex. 5-11. Each of the four phrases
When the two elements of greater duration followed by rest are combined, a
rhythmic cadence is unquestionably established. Therefore, at the beginning of
measure 5 we recognize, in retrospect, a complete rhythmic structure, a phrase.
The succession of shorter rhythmic figures shapes the entire phrase. Notice that in
this melody the shortest durations occur in the first measure of each phrase, and
that the second longest duration occurs in the second measure.
^^
Ex. 5-11. German folk melody. •Ui'^Jf
mm
___ 2
'
^ *-j-»
udJ^'^^^^
*• * s
J i^
JH-'
^^12-^^V
i j j'^Jj
.
j3
8in
^^
^)Y'
E=5
^4 j5
10
^^jg
FORMAL CHARACTERISTICS OF MELODY: MOTIVE AND PHRASE 67
If the measures of the first phrase are considered separately, we can see that each
has a different rhythmic arrangement. The design of each creates balance within
similar to measure 3, and measure 2 has longer durations in common with mea-
sure 4. Even if this phrase is performed without the assigned pitches, we recognize
it as a complete rhythmic structure; nothing else is needed to complete it as a
an upbeat precedes each durational accent. The rhythmic motion from a lesser to
a greater accent also occurs between measures, as from measure 1 to the durational
accent of measure 2. Considered in this light, the phrase is grouped rhythmically
by groupings of two's.
Phrase grouping of this type occurs so frequently that it is considered to be the
norm. By this criterion, the phrases illustrated in Ex. 5-12 would have to be con-
sidered "abnormal," although in their context they are perfectly normal.
Ex. 5-12a. Alec Wilder: Concerto for Oboe, String Orchestra and Percussion, I. (C) 1957
by Associated Music Publishers, Inc., New York. Reprinted by permission.
ii t;
Oboe
J = ab. 92)^'^'^^
Tnf
S ^
^'t^" '^ ^^ afiOM^y^ r^iUAi-
z
s;
^
331
^
ft r r
cJt I
r ii
^ ^
ii u r
/
Ex. 5-12b. Bartok : Mikrokosmos, V, "Boating." Copyright 1940 by Hawkes & Son (London)
Ltd. Reprinted by permission of Boosey & Hawkes Inc.
Allegretto J=116
^^^
68 FORMAL CHARACTERISTICS Of MELODY: MOTIVE AND PHRASE
Larghetto JL66
mm
1 r
^m J l
^ rrl J^J i
Pitch factors also influence phrase structure. We often speak of a melodic "line,"
which is an analogy to the graphic arts. For example, if we perform only the rhythm
of Ex. 5-11 without a change of pitch, we can represent its pitch contour with a
straight line, . A complete rhythmic structure still remains; how-
ever, most of us would agree that such a phrase is comparatively dull, that it is
not really a melody. When changes of pitch are added to our performance a dif-
ferent kind of "line" results. This line generally corresponds to a wavy line, with
the high points, low points, final note, etc., delineating a melody. The pattern set
up by a succession of different pitches influences the general shape of a phrase. The
old expression "What goes up must come down" also applies, broadly speaking,
to melody.
In the first phrase of Ex. 5-1 1 the highest pitch occurs very near the beginning.
As soon as the highest point is reached a descending pitch motion takes over. The
contour of this phrase can be represented by a wavy line which has one pre-
dominant arch, _*-*-~* A more precise representation of the phrase contour
.
The rhythm of the second phrase in Ex. 5-11 is identical with that of the first
phrase. However, a change in pitch structure is made so that the cadence tone now
becomes d. The change is accomplished by interrupting the descending motion
with a minor third skip, then descending to tonic. This, together with the rhythm,
delineates the phrase.
Using the same rhythm for two or more successive phrases is common. This
type of phrase relationship is caWed parallel phrase construction. Phrases related in this
manner melody by restatement of similar material. In the melody of Ex. 5-
unify a
1 3 only the rhythm of the first phrase is retained in the second every pitch is dif- ;
ferent, i ^ . •
IL (_
- -
« — :-_ V ._
„ ^u
w -0_^Jw
. ti
u>till>rY\
.Up CiiJkl
FORMAL CHARACTERISTICS OF MELODY: MOTIVE AND PHRASE 69
^ -fJr^^^rJ^ijJr'-'ncrcjcj- ^
i^ji'i^jinu ^lI'iili
^j^
J"""
In Ex. 5-14 both phrases are four measures long. Since most of the second phrase
differs in rhythm from the first, this phrase creates a contrast. Such a relationship
is called contrasting phrase construction. The two phrases together form asymmetrical^
structure because both are of the sam,e length. it>/U^^^ ^
MjrtJL
Ex. 5-14. Beethoven: Piano Concerto No. 3, Op. 37, C
Allegro
ji't 8 I
J. r
i^BE ^
In Ex. 5-15 the phrases vary in length. This means, simply, that some phrases
are longer than others. Balance is achieved by combining phrases of unequal pro-
i (J
Ex. 5-15. Hovhaness Prayer of Saint Gregory, for Trumpet and String Orchestra. Copy-
:
^ g^j i i ^m ^
70 FORMAL CHARACTERISTICS OF MELODY-. MOTIVE AND PHRASE
Formal units containing phrases of equal duration (as Ex. 5-14) are common,
for they create anunderstood musical form. In Ex. 5-16 both phrases are
easily
four measures long. Moreover, each phrase is a complete rhythmic unit. However,
the phrases are dependent upon one another tonally; a terminal cadence does not
appear until the end of the second phrase.
^ ^4^ i^#
rf i
f f i
tfrnrp
of the phrase. The two-phrase formal unit, then, is understood when the ter-
first
^te ^ r^*^
/
^
M'
'f
T '^r j)yj^
a close on the first beat, completing the rhythmic design of the melody.
Even though each of the phrases in Ex. 5-18 is a rhythmic unit, a larger formal
unit is not heard until all four phrases have been stated. Not only are the first and
third, and the second and fourth, phrases rhythmically similar, they are also alike
in pitch contour. Notice the subtle pitch changes in the last phrase which provide
variety and aelay the arrival of tonic. The second cadence (measure 8) binds the
FORMAL CHARACTERISTICS OF MELODY: MOTIVE AND PHRASE 71
first phrase pair to theby closing on supertonic. Each of the phrase pairs forms
last
a period. In this example, however, the two periods are dependent upon one
another both tonally and rhythmically. Such joining of phrase pairs is often called
a double beriod.
Allegro assai
^ u w
10 11
^ 12 13
^
14
r
15
m
16
m
We have seen that certain typical cadences mark off the phrase as a formal
unit, that a motive frequently dominates the unfolding of a phrase, and that each
phrase is a particular kind of pitch and rhythm structure. These distinctions apply
to phrases in general. However, any phrase of music is heard as a unique structure,
even if many of its elements are similar to those in other phrases we know. As we
music we are probably aware first of the "highs" and "lows" of the pitch
listen to
motion, the contour of the phrases and their relative balance rather than their exact
lengths.
Phrase contour produced by the rhythmic placement of the high and low
is
pitches. In the first phrase of Ex. 5-19 the pitch apex is in measure 4, the highest
point of the entire melody. After this highest point the predominant pitch motion
is down to tonic. The phrase forms one broad arch that includes several smaller
waves. Notice that the pitch apex is the dominant scale degree, which is important
in creating tonality.
^ P^ pi
P
^ a
^ rir
pg:
ir rrcf ^r
^
r r i r r
i i ^
c;-^
S^ ^ V
^ ^m
72 FORMAL CHARACTERISTICS OF MELODY: MOTIVE AND PHRASE
The contour of the second phrase forms an inverted arch. The second phrase
also contrasts with the first phrase because of its length and its range. A phrase-
arch of this type is less common than that produced by the first phrase ; in part,
this arises from the resulting ascending motion cadence pitch. This rising
to the
inflection raises a "question," similar to the way the pitch of a spoken word rises
when we ask a question, producing an expectation of something to follow. Notice
also that a progressive cadence is used. Thus, ascending contour, combined with
progressive cadence, results in anticipation of continuation.
In Ex. 5-20 the pitch apex appears at the beginning of each phrase. However,
both phrases differ in contour and range. Considering the number of waves in the
line, it is "complex," each phrase consisting of several secondary arches.
Ex. 5-20. Tansman: "Berceuse," Pour les Enfanfs 4th Set. (C) 1934 — by Editions Max
Eschig, Paris. Renewed 1962. Reprinted by permission.
The five-tone melody in Ex. 5-21 forms another type of contour. The wave pat-
tern is characterized by descending motion until the rise to b (measure 5) which is
the highest point in the phrase. The appearance of the highest basic pitch (b)
coincides with a change of tonal emphasis. The opening of the phrase outlines a
D tonality (the perfect fifth, a — d) However, the tonality of the excerpt is E. The
.
tonal center of E is created by using b, the dominant degree, as the apex of the
phrase and e as the final pitch.
The arch formed by the phrase in Ex. 5-22 is a common one. Notice that the
highest pitch is reached around the middle of the phrase and that this pitch is the
dominant scale degree. Notice also that the leaps centered around c in measures
2 and 3 slightly disguise the tonality of a.
^'^J 1^ ^ r r i r r
^
r r c/c/i-^ i
—
The possibilities for different types of phrase contour are numerous. Ex. 5-23
illustrates other possibilities.
^
Ex. 5-23b. J.S. Bach
m
: St. John Passion, "Rohf wohl,
[J- If
n n
-^
ihr
i
heiligen Gebeine."
^m
A 4^r\
U^U rr l J LT I
^ ^^ '
"• 0* 4
m
Ex. 5-23c. Bloch :
^g fern
^ 'ii'i 'ii"iiiJi]uiayiii^i"JjjlL"jiy 5
Ex. 5-23d. Ravel : String Quartet, I. By permission of the International Music Company,
^
New York.
r>
i m
I^
ZZl
#
^
^ Ji.j i.r3 /3
$ i i
jjj Jiij]::^ ^
i pjp-^ jjjJOT i i
p aj'J jjJ
Safe rji:j'irr i rf i
r[:; i r:£; i riiJ i
m ^: \ n.mrrn m
. . :
^
: I.
^^ la 3?t^ m p^^^
.n n i I, :i^^^ ^W
Generally, the apex of a line will be a pitch that is basic to the tonality frame
the dominant, the tonic, or the mediant scale degrees. Other parts of the scale also
appear as highest points, but they frequently have clearly decorative relationships
to pitches that are basic to the tonality.
Pitch climax is also associated with the contour of phrases. As a phrase unfolds,
the pattern of motion to the highest pitch directs our attention to that pitch. In a
manner of speaking, the "energ)'" of the directional pattern is concentrated in the
pitch apex or in its opposite, the low point. Since we know that a "build-up" of
energy is usually followed by release, we expect the same to happen in a phrase
of music. The pitch apex and the low point, then, are important factors that out-
line the contour of a phrase or section of music.
It is impossible to exhaust all the possibilities for constructing phrases, for every
composition presents new solutions. However, certain principles are present in
all compositions.
Exercises
For more detailed assignments see Materials and Structure of Music I, Workbook, Chapter 5.
1 Perform each of the examples in this chapter. Listen for the larger formal sections. Isolate
the motive, or motives, and describe the rhythmic structure and pitch structure of each
motive found.
2. Use several of the motives contained in examples cited in this chapter as the principal
unifying factor in two-phrase melodies.
3. Devise three or four original motives. Then use these motives to organize original three-
phrase melodies.
4. Listen to and analyze the phrase structure of songs by Schubert, Schumann, and Brahms.
Describe phrase lengths, pitch structure, and rhythmic structure of the melodies selected.
5. Find examples of melody in the literature for your voice or instrument containing paral-
lel phrase construction, contrasting phrase construction, and period construction.
FORMAL CHARACTERISTICS OF MELODY: MOTIVE AND PHRASE 75
6. Listen to the first movement of Mozart's Symphony No. 40 in g minor, K. 550. Compare
the prominence given to the initial motive with the use of the motive in the first move-
ment of Beethoven's Symphony No. 5.
7. Find examples of melodies in a collection such as the Harvard Anthology of Music in which
motive repetition, phrase repetition, etc., play a small organizational role. Describe how
unity is achieved.
The
Extended
6
Melody
^^mfci
tVLj ^^
More frequently the repetition of a pattern will contain some change, usually
at the cadence. In Ex. 6-2 two changes appear: the progressive cadence (measure
4) is replaced by a terminal cadence in measure 8, and the pitch contour of measure
7 is slightly altered from its prototype in measure 3.
76
THE EXTENDED MELODY 77
Ex. 6-3. Prokofiev: Rigaudon, Op. 12, No. 3. Reprinted with permission of Robert Forberg
(Sole ogents : C. F. Peters Corporation, New York).
Changing the pitch level is one of the many ways musical materials can be
varied. Notice that the intervals are not precisely the same in both phrases. This
will always be the case when a sequence follows the notes of only one scale
(diatonic sequence).
Both phrases in Ex. 6-4 use the same motive. The first phrase unfolds sequen-
tially.(The use of sequence within a phrase usually involves the repetition of a
motive) The end of the sequence is signalled by the change of the perfect fourth
.
Allegro
wtj * m *
^ CjiJ r i^i^U-^^ ^ *
In Ex. 6-5 the motive that begins in the last portion of measure 2 is treated
sequentially, spinning out an extended contrasting phrase.
m '
; ^^r>J
''''"' i
-^-
Jj
t^uur^- ^^J '
ijiju.
^
yp
'^
J J J J IJ- [^ mi
J
Ex. 6-6 illustrates a sequential section that is not diatonic. Note that the melodic
pattern remains identical because the same intervals are used.
Ex. 6-6. Elliott Carter : Piano Sonata, Used with the permission of the copyright owner,
Mercury Music Corporation.
j 'i ^ Jffl-^ ? 3 ##
^ mm
Repetition need not be as obvious as in the previous examples. Some composi-
tions repeat only the rhythmmelody of Ex. 6-7 none
or motive of a phrase. In the
of the pitch patterns are exactly the same, but the phrases that have the same
rhythmic shape contribute to a balanced design.
iif Fr pirprjij i
JJy jjJ- i hj J'J JiJ JJ j^
f J
pr p i
^^^
^ J'j
p i J J'j
r Mr
i ^ J-'
\ i i
f pr
i'j j-mJ pr p
i
^^
"
Allegretto
» » I J 1 1 i j
* # r^ihi'ii
^
^ ^ ^m
1 r 1 >:
m ? te:
:zt
it
—
Another type of extension delays the appearance of the cadence pitch. Because
of the stress pattern and the pitch motion, we expect the cadence to occur at meas-
ure 8 in Ex. 6-9. But our anticipation is deceived, for the motion to tonic is tem-
porarily halted by afermata, followed by the repetition of measure 7 in a stretched
out version in measures 8 and 9 and measures 10 and 11. This delay intensifies
our expectations and emphasizes the finality of this section.
J J l^'
' — 1*
j('f [j r
J I
J J J y-j
M ^ ^
P m
>):.r i rcrr irfrr rrr i
pTTI i f^
recognized phrase lengths. Thus a phrase built from motive repetitions or a phrase
in which a motive fragment is repeated is not necessarily an extension by inter-
polation.
As can be seen in Ex. 6-1 1, interpolation by sequence is another possibility. The
first phrase establishes the phrase length we expect to hear continued, but the
sequential repetition of this initial pattern extends the second phrase considerably.
three measures long. In this case the compression results from the "mosaic" block
statement of the motive of phrase one in the second phrase. This type of com-
pression is called truncation. Since truncated phrases result from shortening phrase
duration, the effect of compression will be understood only if the phrase lengths
have been previously established, and if the truncated phrases are easily relatable
to an earlier phrase.
j^ii
Andante
ginning of the next phrase, as in Ex. 6-13. Such phrase interlocking is called _elision._
When phrases elide continuity is produced by negating a cadential "breathing
point." Elision also creates a shorter total time span for the interlocked phrases
than when a separation occurs between phrase endings and beginnings.
Ex. 6-13. Bach: Sonata No. 3 for Flute and Harpsichord, II.
^ h^ x
P
r-
r
irijir
iLC/ i
r
^
p rt
^m 1 r
^
THE EXTENDED MELODY 81
1 r
* ^ *
c/'^Pto \ui^ i
r ^ {nj \ i
C-££l Pa
» ^ *
'rn P r^rr l l
c/^ g i r B' l
[rrr ^^
The third and fourth phrases are repetitions of the first two. The two part se-
quence of the fifth phrase contrasts with the preceding phrases, and the sixth phrase
adds still further variety to the whole melody.
For convenience, a letter system of diagraming is used to designate the various
parts of a melody. If we call the first phrase of the preceding melody a, the form
representation of the total melody would be ab ab cd cd' There are four sections, .
each resulting from the combination of two phrases. Since each of these sections
forms a larger unit, it is helpful to "reduce" the diagram to its lowest common
denominator, A A B B'
a b a b c d c d'
fourth produces a change of pitch contour in measure 7. Note also that some of
the pitches in measure 4 are eliminated in measure eight to create a clearer
cadential motion. As a whole, phrase four is a variation of phrase two.
Andante
^ ^m S
1 r
fc
^ ^
zz
1 r
0—0 M 0-
^^
We have already observed that phrases in parallel construction have the same
rhythm, but the pitch structure frequently is changed. In Ex. 6-16 the second
phrase begins like the first, but the pitch structure is a variant of the first phrase.
Notice that each of the phrases is four measures long, containing a three-meas-
ure and a one-measure extension. Both measures 4 and 8 are varied by simple
elaboration and are thus variants of measures 3 and 6, respectively.
Some forms of varying a melody normally occur when melodic units are re-
peated in the later portions of a melody. In Ex. 6-17 both the pitch and rhythm
patterns of the phrases are slightly ornamented, producing a more active variation
of the initial statement.
yfc^F^ #—(^
m. 1-4
p u \
^
a^ ^J !
^
7^^
f^K —^ Lj/|* P m ^
""Tr
^ M f M f '— f
rrrrrr rr
f » f * ^
lt r ••••••
i-»—i^id
§ \' ^
h
m 13-16
Frequently only parts of a phrase are varied in a later appearance. In Ex. 6-18
the approach to the final cadence of the excerpt is the same rhythmically in both
versions. However, the varied version contains figurations not hinted at in the
original.
Ex. 6-18. Satie: Fifth Nocturne. (C) 1920 by Editions Max Eschig, Paris. Renewed 1948.
Reprinted by permission.
^m ^
E
&. * #
"m ^^^^^^^^^
r F r P ^ s
r r p^ ^ ^m
In variation movements melodic embellishment sometimes becomes the prin-
cipal procedure of melodic organization. Ex. 6-19 shows typical changes a phrase
might undergo in a variation movement.
THE EXTENDED MELODY 83
n^ it rj^n ^^ ^
WmT^
,y ii^f'f i
Pm
The pattern that begins in measure 11 of Ex. 6-21 is a diminished (shortened)
version of the pattern that begins in measure 5. If the tempo remains the same,
augmentation lengthens a phrase; conversely, diminution shortens a phrase.
Ex. 6-22a. Bartok: Mikrokosmos, Vol. IV. Copyright 1940 by Hawkes & Son (London)
Ltd. Reprinted by permission of Boosey & Hawkes inc.
112 3 4 •
^f p m , ^f r> m ^
i jj u jj J or cjij I
J J J J j)^
I I
j^ I
Inversion
t-
gi ^J N J|J
W
Inversion
Since change of contour creates only pitch contrast, the inversion of a pattern is
easily recognized. Notice that only the first portion of Ex. 6-22a is inverted. Note
also that measure five is a partial augmentation of measure 2. (This excerpt is
noteworthy for yet another reason; it contains only eight different notes.)
Variety of phrase structure frequently is produced by mutation. In Ex. 6-23a, the
fourth phrase repeats the third phrase in the parallel minor key; the third phrase
in Ex. 6-23b also is in the parallel minor. In the latter, mutation coincides with the
beginning of a new section, while in Ex. 6-23a phrase repetition is involved.
m i
P h \
> r ^ p
Mr pn> MT
^^
j i r If r i
r
r
r If
^ m 0- M m.
THE EXTENDED MELODY 85
Changes of tonality help to intensify our awareness of form. In Ex. 6-24 the
first and last sections are in G, while the middle section is in D. This change of
tonal focus, plus other aspects of phrase contrast, delineates the form. In fact, it is
probably more accurate to say that the tonality change coincides with other con-
trast-producing elements, such as the change in contour and pitch motion. As is
often the case, the return to G provides unity, even though earlier melodic material
is not restated.
m r pr r ^^^
^ ^
r f i
fr ^ m
Change of tonality frequently is introduced at the end of a section, announcing
that a new formal unit will follow. The return to the original tonality and the
restatement of the opening material coincide in the following excerpt, vividly
delineating the form.
86 THE EXTENDED MELODY
j^^ i
f J j)
i /^^^ i
r ^ i
Our awareness of musical form is aided when a change of tonality coincides with
the statement of a new formal unit. Generally, changes of tonality are more essen-
tial to an extended composition than to a short melody. In either case, a new tonal
center provides variety with its new focal point.
Exercises
For more detailed assignments see Materials and Structure of Music I, Workbook, Chapter 6.
1. Extend Ex. 6-1 for four more measures. Use one of the procedures discussed in the
chapter.
2. Find examples in music for your instrument or voice that contain sequence, variation,
and change of tonality.
3. Write a melody that has as its basis the rhythmic structure of Ex. 6-7.
4. Listen to and analyze the phrase combinations of various works from Bartok's Mikro-
kosmos, Volume IV.
5. Use one of the motives invented assignments as the basic unifying factor of
for previous
a three-phrase melody. Create variety through the use of the procedure of inversion.
/ 6. Write an experimental melody four phrases long that contains no repetition of pitch or
rhythm patterns.
7. Listen to the first movement of Beethoven's Piano Sonata, Op. 2, No. 3. List and
describe the ways in which Beethoven varies the motive and the phrase lengths.
Basic
Melody
7
In the last five chapters our attention has been drawn to various
aspects of melody — tonality, scale, cadence, phrase, and —
motive that constitute
the organizational procedures and materials. It is now possible to study the full
melody and reduce its total pitch structure to a skeletal outline. Through this
"elaborations" of the frames that create the uniqueness and the charm of particular
melodies.
The Mozart and Beethoven melodies in Ex. 7-1 not only share the same tonality
frame types; they also resemble one another in the way this common pitch nucleus
has been elaborated.
87
88 BASIC MELODY
j.rirrx^Dli.i rrir^t^i^
^ 5
S?ES i
^ 3 i
^
3 i
^^
Ex. 7-1 b.
?
Mozart: Symphony No. 40,
fS
fe
K. 550, IV.
i^ ^
Tonality frame
^ SH
?
'IWiC
The tonality frame is, a basic level of pitch organization, serving as the
therij
tonal outline within which melodic activity takes place.
Moving on to the second higher level of organization, the pitches that determine
total melodic shape constitute basic melody. By shape is meant the general sweep of
melody, its important high and low points, its beginning and ending, an d the im-
portant junctional pitches in between.
2^
y m m^
i
•-'
()
;z:
Ex. 7-2b. Mozart : Basic melody of Symphony No. 40, K. 550, IV.
3 4 5 6|,^r^ 7 8
The third (and most complicated) level of melodic organization is the full-blown
pattern, complete with the rhythmic life that creates the wonderful illusion of
1
BASIC MELODY 89
From the standpoint of basic melody, the most important tones are the firstand
t he last. As end-points of what is heard, these tones are the structural time bound-
aries of melody.
The last pitch is particularly fundamental to melodic design because, as the last
sound heard, it offers an ultimate point to which all preceding pitches can be re-
ferred. Thus the final pitch in most melodies is tonic. When it is not, the pitch is.
usually a nieml^er of the tonality frame, the dominant or the mediant.
The first pitch is the beginning of melodic pattern Chapter 2, it ; as we saw in
helps to lead the listener's attention to the tonality frame within which the whole
melody will operate. The first pitch of melodies beginning on a metric accent is
generally the tonic, with the dominant and the mediant as lesser possibilities. What-
ever the first pitch of the downbeat beginning, it is usually linked immediately
with another member of the tonality frame, thereby leaving no doubt as to pitch
orientation.
Tonic beginnings
d:
W £
JM J J,i
J J J I
J Ji j J)
1
W
Ex. 7-3b. Mozart: Piano Sonata, K. 545, I.
^M
C:
^
Ex. 7-3c. Schubert : Symphony No. 8 in b minor, I.
aa ^ b:
^ m 5
90 BASIC MELODY
Ex. 7-36. Hindemith: Third Piano Sonata, I. (C) 1936 by B. Schotfs Soehne, Mainz. Re-
newed 1963.
Mediant beginnings
S L/ ^^'
P
*4
m
A:
p ic:i;r ^ \ <^>
^'r p
i
r r/r
p
^^
Db:
^F
*
^ #
Dominant beginnings
if ^ 1
^ ^
1-
. IJ . 1>
f
Ex. 7-3h.
fiiir-
Schubert: Symphony No. 8
\ri^ \
{
in B Minor,
i fii^j
Melodies with weak metric beginnings the anacrusis or upbeat pattern do not —
I.
^ —
often start with tonic. Rather, their first sound is usually the dominant, the mediant,
or, in rare instances, the leading tone. This first pitch is then followed on the first
strong beat by the tonic or another member of the tonality frame. The excerpts of
Ex. 7-4 show various ways in which the anacrusis melody initiates its tonal pattern.
J)
^
IJHRML^
BASIC MELODY 91
(Ih {',
!) j) h
^ a iiism^^ ^m
Ex. 7-4c. Shostakovitch : Symphony No. 7, I. (C) Copyright by Leeds Music Corporation,
m ^
New York, N.Y. Used by permission. All rights reserved.
m ^ m rO^'^ I'
l
r^ II
**
fr^[lJ^ l c^3
In still other melodies the first tone's relation to the tonality made clear
is not
immediately. In association with other parts the relation might be made clear by
accompanying chords (as in Ex. 7-5). But in terms of pure melodic structure,
the delayed establishment of tonality can create a degree of tonal suspense for the
listener. This is particularly true in melodies like (c) of Ex. 7-5.
Ex. 7-5a. Copland: Concerto for Copyright 1949 by Boosey & Hawkes Inc.
Clarinet, I.
(Di i r
lS^ F m
^ S
S 4 i
¥
i
¥
i i
+ '+•
T
Ex. 7-5b.
*
w^
k^
Ab:
Chopin: Etude, Op. 25.
^
Ex. 7-5c. Bortok : Concerto for Orchestra, IV. Copyright 1946 by Hawkes & Son (London)
Ltd. Reprinted by permission of Boosey & Hawkes Inc.
92 BAS/C MElOOy
The cadence pitches at phrase endings are also basic Hnks in the tonal chain.
It is not an exaggeration to think of the cadence as a point of respite at which the
listenercan take stock of what has preceded, instantly forming an impression of
the important tonal events that have led to this point in the melody. The role
performed by cadence pitches is basic, then, to the total organization of the melody.
At this point we can make a rudimentary analysis of melody by abstracting the
first and last pitches and all cadence patterns to reveal a great deal more than the
tonality frame.
^^
J J
*^
I
r
Reduction
to termi-
s^ is;
nal points
^
Ex. 7-6b. Barber Schoo/ for Scandal, Overture. Reprinted
: by permission of copyright
owner, G. Schirmer, Inc.
ZZI f=m
W m
'^-
4. 1 - I
^ i
__ L-" i - -L____j3
(b*)
To make our search for basic pitches more penetrating we must study units
smaller than the phrase. In addition to the terminal pitches, others are basic
because of: (1) their positions as parts of the overall melodic contour, (2) their
relatively great duration, and (3) their favored metric position.
Melodic Contour
If we regard a melody as a line that weaves through points on the musical staff,
we see that the resultant wave possesses height and depth. Like the first and last
pitches, the highs and lows of melodic motion are impressive parts of melodic shape.
BASIC MELODY 93
In_melodies where a succession of peaks and valleys is formed, the resultant out-
'
linine; pi tches ar e basic. ~^ "
The Samuel Barber melody of Ex. 7-7 becomes clearer as a basic structure when
these tops and bottoms of emphasis are incorporated into the basic melody reduc-
tion that was first attempted in Ex. 7-6.
( Partial reduction)
The g of measure 6 is not a low point of the immediate pattern, for the e that
follows is still lower. The g is structurally important, however, because it is part
of a step-p rogression formed between the low pitch of measure 5 (a) and the final/.
As one level in this brief descending stair-step, its basic role in the melodic shape
is established.
Thi s does not mean that every step relation in a melody automatically forms a
sierra gression. On the contrary, step-progression refers to delayed ascending or
descending motion by steps that outline the contour of the line over the whole or a large
segment of the phrase.
Almost any melody contains a step relation of some kind between some two or
more pitches, but this alone does not warrant recognition as a determinant of basic
structure.To achieve the status of step-progression such a pattern must create a
recognizable uniformity within the pitch design, an obvious linking of highs and
lows that controls the melodic progress from one melodic segment to another.
Ex. 7-8 through Ex. 7-12 clarify the function of a step-progression within a melodic
form.
The circled notes of Ex. 7-8 establish a clear line of ascent by step for a sizable
portion of the pattern. The successive steps in other parts are mere decorative
motions, activities which lead from one basic pitch to another. Bach did not
allow this rather strict and obvious ladder of steps to dominate his line alto-
gether, for the e of measure 3 overshoots the conclusion of the ascending pattern,
thus avoiding monotony and stressing the pitch d as the beginning of the following
part of the melodic shape.
These tracings of pitch by delayed steps form the backbone for many melodies,
either in their entirety or in isolated parts of their total structure.
As such, they
lead the listener to expect to arrive at a highest or lowest pitch (depending upon
whether the line ascends or descends), and in most instances this pitch of "arrival"
will be a part of the tonality framework or a pitch that rhythmically "leans" on a
member of this group.
In some melodies the step-progression forms a periodic ascent or descent clearly
allied with metric accent, as in the Bach melody of Ex. 7-8. In others its contour
is more subtly imbedded in the pitch motion, as in (b) of Ex. 7-9.
s ^'
v^ rrfP V
i i
vpn J3^r P
¥
S^tep
"pro-
gression
331
lH
i ZUZ
-or
-O-
P fe
m ^z
m r^ ==¥
Still other melodies display partial ascent or descent by delayed step relations
without forming a clear step-progression as such. The patterns marked with brackets
BASIC MELODY 95
in Ex. 7-10 are fragmentary; none adds up to a structural pattern that controls
the ascent or descent of the whole line.
zzi
fe
r i
^rn w r i
^r W
The highest pitches of the smaller groupings in measures 2 and 4 do not lead in
a regular way to the apex of the line, the a of measure 5. Even if^ is heard as the most
important high pitch (because of its repetition in measure 6), it too is not reached
by delayed steps.
than the descending, but the same organization can be found as a falling pattern
in enough melodies to justify recognition. In many cases the low pitch of a line
will return after intervening patterns, creating a mild form of pedal or "drone."
This kind of repetition serves as a structural ground over which the melodic motion
freely unwinds, as in Ex. 7-1 lb.
^m m
Step
m
-
proijression
96 BASIC MELODY
The pitch basis of the melody in Ex. 7-1 Ic is clearly a double step-progression,
the upper pattern moving in contrary motion to the lower. The combination of
two such distinct step-lines can create the illusion of two separate parts if the lines
have a distinct separation of range. The Bach melody in Ex. 7-12 is typical of such
"one-line counterpoint." Its step-progressions descend.
Step
^^^^^^^m
progression
Implied ,s^^^jr-aj,=a
"-:^
parts
''pf^cj 1
^
Like two wires suspended in space, the delayed steps formed by these two lines
frame the pitch activity sandwiched in between, which in this excerpt is relatively
negligible.
The pitches that constitute a clear step-progression are significant parts of the
melodic shape and are, therefore, parts of the basic melody. When no step-progres-
sion is evident only the peaks of melodic motion can be regarded as basic pitches,
and then only when their metric location and duration favor them over their
neighbors.
If other elements are equal, a tone that sounds longer than those around it will
attract more attention. Even when metric accent coincides with one pitch, another
close by will be regarded as more important if its duration is considerably greater
or if it acts as the cadence point for the phrase.
SP3=5
f"''^JJj J-i- ! j: i
Basic
melody
BASIC MELODY 97
Basic
i a:
'): . i
ni
?
-^u- r r
QI
^^
P"
i
^'
melody
The measure of the Brahms melody of Ex. 7- 14b shows how repetition
third
within the measure can confirm a pitch's basic role. Obviously, repetition without
intervening pitches is a simple extension of duration, for no other pitch competes
But repetition within the immediate pattern (of approximately
for attention.
one measure), even after intervening tones, also emphasizes the returning pitch.
The Beethoven melody in Ex. 7-15 shows arabesques of eighth notes moving
around the repetition of the pitches that fall on metrical accents in measures 1 and
2. Because of their subsequent return these pitches are confirmed as basic.
Basic
melody
S ^a
P m PP
'ei
W- S3
Bassic
I \
J/ —*-
mel(ody I W) =
.ff r ^[Jrfj \
f T rrfr ^
Ex. 7-16b. Corelli : Concerto Grosso.
? s i s I
-pz.
^
• .
-^ . ^^= ^^i=^
basic structure. The separation of basic from decorative is made for purposes of un-
derstanding rather than for the establishment of a standard of musical values.
The pattern (a) in Ex. 7-17 by itself would interest no listener for long. But when
used as the basis for the pattern shown at (b), it is transformed into a dynamic
melodic statement that seizes a listener's attention and begs of continuation.
BASIC MELODY 99
^ Basic melody
=^^
t)^
P^
Melody
? ^ * m
^
Exercises
For more detailed assignments see Materials and Structure of Music I, Workbook, Chapter 7.
(a)
(b)
(c)
Make up other contour patterns to be used as guides for melodies, and analyze the con-
tours of melodies found in literature.
6. Find several melodies which clearly incorporate a step-progression (or step-progressions)
as a main feature of organization.
7. Have a friend play a melody for you. Measure off the number of bars contained in the
melody and plot the basic pitches of the whole pattern. Don't try to get every pitch at
first; begin with the first and the last pitches, proceed to the highest and lowest, and then
fill in other pitches as they become known.
Melodic
Elaboration
8
and b-Jlat are the basic pitches. Because of their metric positions and lesser dura-
tions, the notes marked * decorate the basic e-JIat. In this example both g and b-
Jlat share e-fiat as root; thus, they create the effect of extending the duration of
the basic e-flat.
^ ;z=
f
^m
Basic melody
Decorative pitches such as those in Ex. 8-1 are basic associates;. th.ty elaborate
by skipping and from a basic pitch. The basic associates often occur in weak
to
metric positions, and they generally are shorter in duration than the basic pitch
with which they are associated.
The simplest form of melodic elaboration is created by repeating a note. In Ex,
700
MELODIC ELABORATION 101
8-2 the repetition of g-JIat emphasizes that note; however, its relative duration is
Ex. 8-2. Bartok : Mikrokosmos, No. 131. Copyright 1940 by Hawkes & Son (London) Ltd.
Reprinted by permission of Boosey & Hawkes Inc.
ganization, but they represent only two types of decorative patterns. Other, less
^ n rln\^ 2 > .3
m [r;
'if; \ \
i i^
s^
fe
TT tot
:
O m -o-
Basic pilches
: £1
^
k nx tat
3X tnt
J xr ^ 331
U^
Basic pitches, basic associates, and their decorative pitches
In measures 3-6 the first note of each measure is a basic pitch because of its metric
position. As can be seen, the predominant pitch activity is a sequence of thirds.
The upper note of each third is a basic associate. In the descending motion to the
last note of each pattern the third is filled in. This filler tone smooths out the
disjunct motion of the sequential pattern and at the same time embellishes each
types of tonal activity: (1) the decorative pitch exceeds the range of two basic pitches
or a basic pitch and a basic associate (as in Ex. 8-4a and Ex. 8-4b) ; or (2) the de-
102 MELODIC ELABORATION
corative pitch remains within the range of two basic pitches or a basic pitch and
a basic associate (as in Ex. 8-4c). In Ex. 8-4, and in all subsequent examples of this
chapter, the basic pitches are represented by o , the basic associates by • , and
Ex. 8-4.
J^^
^^ xn J. 33= 331 J-
Decorative pitches have names that describe clearly the role they play. For
example, since most melodies use a preponderance of steps, decorative pitches
called ^sing tones appear frequently. This is an apt description, because passing
tones connect two different basic pitches (or a basic pitch and its associate), as in
Ex. 8-5.
Ex. 8-5.
/Ml W
^m ^
-o-
^^
Passing tones are often shorter in duration than the pitches they connect, and they
appear in both accented and unaccented positions. Since accented tones tend to
impress us as more important than unaccented, thejunaccented passing tone is
more common.
Ex. 8-6a and Ex. 8-6b show both types. The d-natural in measure 4 of Ex. 8-6c
is a chromatic passing tone. Note that the diatonic passing tones (notes belonging
to one diatonic scale) link two notes a third apart; chromatic passing tones generally
link two notes a major second apart.
Ex.
£t^ ^
8-6a. Beethoven -.
String Quartet, Op. 74, IV
j),n KyJ^JJ^ |
^mX s
S ^ i 331 ^ ia=
^^ ^
31 i
MELODIC ELABORATION 103
P^ ^
Ex. 8-6b.
m ^m i
ma 33:
F^
^ te TI"
^
U^^'iigii'
^m ^
Ex. 8-6c. I.
^^
'^^^
^ pCJI^j-^p p
t^ t"> " ( # )" ^^ J l>o trrr
1>V xc ^
Ex. 8-7. Bach : "Gigue" from Liffle Notebook of Anno Magdalena Bach.
JJ^JJJ^
-(r&h f> It
^
Another decorative pitch that appears frequently is the neighbor tone} Like the
passing tone, this type of embellishment can be found in both accented and unac-
cented positions. Step motion is again involved, the neighbor moving away (either
up or down) from a basic pitch and returning to it. Neighbor tones also may be
diatonic or chromatic. Since neighbor tones embellish a single tone, delayed repeti-
tion is involved. This emphasizes the repeated tone, creating a more interesting
pattern than simple rearticulation.
In Ex. 8-8 the neighbor tones are indicated by J . The neighbors {f-sharp in
If' 'i
UIU ^ i *5
I
it -e- nc m: J.. J ..
I
i
Ex. 8-8b. Beethoven : Piano Sonata, Op. 90,
-&-
331
s xn
Chromatic passing tones and neighbor tones do not change the tonal center.
Because of their brevity, the emphasis they produce is insufficient for the creation
of a new focal point. If their duration were increased, a new tonal focus might
result. Therefore, even if a note has the appearance of a new leading tone, duration
The frequent use of passing tones and neighbor tones does not make them more
significant than other kinds of decorative pitches. As a matter of fact, their repeated
occurrence only indicates that the conditions for their particular decorative role
are often present. Thus, in those melodies containing structural pitches that are
a third apart, passing tones smooth the line by filling in melodic gaps, thereby
bringing the structural pitches into closer relationship. Similarly, if repeated tones
play an important part in the structure of a melody (as in Ex. 8-2), the effect of such
repetition is intensified by the incorporation of neighbor tones.
Both the passing tone and the neighbor tone are approached and followed by
step. Although motion by step is a factor in most decorative patterns, certain types
do not use this step-step configuration. Sometimes a decorative tone is given greater
emphasis because it is either approached or left by skip. This added emphasis
directs attention to the decorative tone; consequently, decorative patterns con-
taining skips other than between a basic pitch and a basic associate appear in-
MELODIC ELABORATION 105
frequently. Two different decorative pitches can be classified in this way: the
leaning tone^ and the escape tone^.
Leaning tones are approached by skip and then move to a structural pitch by
step. Since skips tend to create emphasis, the leaning tone directs attention to the
note of arrival by delaying it momentarily. Furthermore, the motion by step to
the embellished tone creates the impression of "leaning," which explains the use
of this particular descriptive term.
Ex. 8-9.
* za
Tnr
i 33:
=QI
f
X-eaning tones appear in both accented and unaccented metric positions. They
are usually shorter in duration than the notes they embellish, and the motion to
the subsequent basic pitch is frequently in the opposite direction from the skip
that introduces the leaning tone. In all cases it follows the pattern of skip (either
up or down)^tep.
In Ex. 8-10 the leaning tone in measure 1 is easily recognized because its relative
duration is clearly indicated by the notation. This literal representation of a leaning
tone is common.
^I:u_
^
zaz
^ 1-&- JOO
If this type of notation is not used, the duration of the leaning tone is designated
the same way as the other notes. In Ex. 8-1 1 the leaning tones are not set apart by
notation, but are notated as part of the total fabric of the phrase. In this example
the basic pitches create an ascending step progression.
:
(iV-'^
•>
( . P ^ «« '
*
* * m m m m J:J
.JJ..'-J -SZ_ZSZI2I
|
^4^
s -^•^
xt.> *F o =fc|P3=
Sometimes the leaning tone and the pitch it embeUishes have the same duration.
\Vhen this occurs it can be difficult to difTerentiate between decorative and struc-
tural notes. However, in Ex. 8-12 the leaning tone is also the leading tone of the
key, and it embellishes the tonic note.
XH
-?*-
rrsz
XE
iM
^
331
'iij^'j
xc
331
r r-
In Ex. 8-13 the chromatic leaning tones emphasize the second and third scale
steps, while the diatonic leaning tones emphasize the tonic and the subdominant.
Notice that the duration of the structural pitches varies, and the half-step relations
create a strong motion to the structural tones.
jO^
i :^Mz ^i=^
Ex. 8-14. Carter: Woodwind Quintet, I. (C) 1952 by Associated Music Publishers, Inc.,
The melodic opposite of the leaning tone, the escape tone, moves to a basic pitch
or a basic associate by skip, while the motion preceding it is by step. Although it
^/^
Ex. 8-15.
rin h \ } s 32
i=^ i 31
i 3X 33:
Like the leaning tone, the motion away from an escape tone usually involves
a change of direction, as shown below. In both examples the decorative note "es-
capes" away from one important tone before moving to the next.
r
i^<"ll^\,\ r r i
L^^^utnr p
xn
i i^
Ex. 8-1 6b. Bach: St. John Passion, Chorus No. 23.
^^ ^^ ^ ^
m ^ ^S O I
'm
108 MELODIC ELABORATION
Sometimes, however, the motion continues in the same direction, as in Ex. 8-17.
We can still recognize this as a variation of the more common pattern which
changes direction.
Whichever direction it moves, the escape tone is usually of shorter duration than
the tones surrounding it. If not, it normally appears in a less prominent metric
position.
The decorative patterns discussed in the preceding sections involve only one
embellishing tone, with the exception of a group of passing tones used to fill in
an interval larger than a third, or a third with chromatic activity. The figure called
a neighbor group always involves two decorative tones that embellish a single basic
pitch. Both decorative notes are neighbors to the basic tone, one located a step
above, the other a step below; a skip occurs between the two neighbor tones, the
resulting pitch pattern forming a step-skip-step sequence. Often one of these
decorative tones is unaccented, the other accented.
The neighbor groups in Ex. 8-18 embellish the cadence pitch. Since the repeated
cadence tone is in evidence for a relatively long period of time, the neighbor groups
in this excerpt create greater tonal activity and variety.
r» !
i
r
''
i
rrr
. .r i r rr
m r?lf f rrif r
|
331
XE 33:
$ zee
f ^. ^^EEi^
rrruf rrrLj r^r' iri i
^ ^*
#
331 ^ ~cr
^ U3I -Q-
MELODIC ELABORATION 109
In Ex. 8-19 the neighbor groups extend the duration of the basic pitches. Notice
how closely the decorative activity of this figure resembles the neighbor tone
figure.
m
fct
«4 -tTTT
^ ri
i
i. 331
^ -O-
's"
i ^ -O-
331
t
Another type of decorative preannounces the appearance
pitch, the anticipation,
of a basic pitch. It is by nature an unaccented pattern, almost always of lesser
duration than the pitches surrounding it. The pitch motion is generally step-re-
articulation, as in Ex. 8-20.
fi.i,MLrr££gC/^ 331
_Q_
SE
=^^t -e-
i 3X1
$
t=
B S ^
a -^rr
xn
The suspension is established by a basic pitch that passes to another basic pitch
or to a decorative pitch by step, usually down. In this passing process the first
770 MELODIC ELABORATION
Ex. 8-22.
^ i
^ ^
The suspension figure is characterized by the following: (1) a basic pitch is pro-
longed by means of a tie or rearticulation; (2) this basic pitch extends past a metric
or rhythmic accent; and (3) this suspended basic pitch moves to an unaccented
basic pitch or decorative pitch by step.
In Ex. 8-23 a suspension appears on the first beat of measure 3. Here the sus-
pension is created by the tied basic /. The result is an elaboration of the phrase
rhythm. In addition, the entire process emphasizes the basic pitch more than if
the suspension had not occurred.
Ex. 8-23. Hindemith: Mathis der Maler, I. (C) 1934 by B. Schott's Soehne, Mainz. Re-
newed 1961. Reprinted by permission.
_Ol
^m i i
XL
In Ex. 8-24 the duration of the basic g is extended by its rearticulation on the
first beat of measure 4. This delays the anticipated appearance of a different pitch
m 5
33:
•e- — "cr
MELODIC ELABORATION lU
Sometimes the motion of the suspended basic pitch to the second note of the
pattern is "interrupted" by the interpolation of a decorative pitch, as in Ex. 8-25.
Here the two tones of the suspension figure are separated by the interpolated lean-
ing tones.
$ ^ e ri r i
j
r i
CjT cjf
I r 1
^1
Pitch Rhythmic
Type Pitch Characteristics
Motion Characteristics
c
^ n
772 MELODIC ELABORATION
Exercises
For more detailed assignments see Materials and Structure of Music I, Workbook, Chapter 8.
1. Determine the basic pitches of familiar melodies. Then use these basic melodies as the
pitch framework for melodies that you write.
2. Use the phrase patterns from any example as the rhythmic basis for melodies that you
write. Before writing, sketch in a pitch framework.
3. Create "new" melodies by elaborating the basic melodies of any of the examples in this
chapter.
4. Create an original basic melody. Then elaborate this framework, using the same basic
melody for melodies in simple and compound meters.
5. Describe all of the decorative pitches not identified in the analyses given in this chapter.
6. Analyze melodies selected from the following: Bartok, Mikrokosmos, Volumes I and II;
Bach, English Suites; Mozart, Piano Sonatas; Beethoven, Piano Sonatas; Stravinsky, Sonata
for Two Pianos; etc. Reduce each of the melodies selected to its basic pitches; then identify
the role of each of the decorative pitches.
;
Two-Voice
Combinations
9
Our study thus far has dealt with melody. Although we usually
associate melody with a prominent voice or instrument (as it is quite natural
single
to do), it is how melody exists in one way or another in each
essential that we see
voice or part of a composition, and how features of melodic organization are pres-
ent in varying degrees in different styles, forms and textures of music.
Texture
Rhythmic Association
The possibilities of rhythmic association for two combined voices are enormous,
but several basic observations can be made. A
comparative study of the examples
in this section will reveal several different kinds of rhythmic combinations, each
of which represents a common treatment of two relatively independent parts.
?J3
* ^
^ ^ ^^ s ^ ^
^^ o 1 1 r i,C< c ^
P
m ^^ -i^ —5
fe^ ^^ i i3
A (
i
Ex. 9-1 contains two voices of contrasting rhythms organized in % meter. The
upper part clearly predominates because of its greater activity and pitch variety.
Both parts are unified through the use of recurrent patterns. This is a rudimentary
kind of rhythmic association.
A relationship of 2 to 1
(
Ex. 9-3. Marini : Sonata for Violin and Organ (two outer parts).
Vln
rP#=^ ^S ^ ^
^ 33=
-iS^
XE
— —
^m^ f
P ^ i P
In contrast with the excerpt of Ex. 9-2, the lower voice stands out as a more Hnear
associate of the top part, while providing a solid foundation for the activity and
tonal motion in the upper line. The rhythmic contrast between voices is perhaps
the most engaging feature of the example.
More equality of movement occurs in Ex. 9-4, and the distinction between upper
voice and accompaniment is less obvious than in Ex. 9-1 and Ex. 9-2.
/Ts^-l
'^^—1» P — ^ —r -f
'hK I
[ J [J I [^ Li
Both phrases are begun by the upper part alone, and its rhythmic diversity estab-
lishes it as the more interesting. In the second phrase, however, the voices are
treated imitatively, and the lower part matches the upper in activity and design
for two measures. The close of the second phrase restores the original relationship
of leader and subordinate associate by contrast of rhythm and design.
Two equally active parts compete for attention in Ex. 9-5. Such competition is
infrequent in two-voice textures, since the capabilities of our own hearing almost
rule out the clear perception oftwo equally active voices. Although we can shift
attention from one voice to another to understand essentially what is going on,
the problems of doing so when both parts are equally active are many, particularly
if such activity is maintained for very long.
776
teidAj>-vu-
TWO-VOICE COMBINATIONS
m ^ ^ ^^^^
si 3Z
^ i
^ ^ *-n^^
m ^^ ^m 0-m-0
m
«- *
p I
V- J j r^
^
J
fj^^^^ r ^ s
In contrast to Ex. 9-5 by Ockeghem, a clear rhythmic distinction between two
#-^
interesting independent voices can be easily perceived in Ex. 9-6.Both parts re-
veal individual contours and remain rhythmically independent. Although the lower
(piano) voice clearly provides a strong tonal framework [a minor) for the upper,
its rhythmic individuality makes it equally important.
Ex. 9-6. Piston: Sonata for Violin and Piano, III. (C) 1940 by Associated Music Publishers,
Inc., New York. Reprinted by permission.
Viol
lolin
^ ^ -0 0- N—— I
r^
t
^
• I
F
M ^ m w^
'lano
'
P^h
~
Col sva
m m m
_f _
^s ^ ^
i
^ m ^ ^ ^ ^i 5
^
ffs
7WO-VO;CE COMBINATIONS 117
more readily the activity of both voices. The_recogniti on and perc eption ., of
equally impo rtant, simultaneou sly heard melodies is made most clear when the
voices have contrasting durations. When identical rhythms occur simultaneously,
independence is minimized, and other factors, particularly pitch association, direc-
.uv;
The cadences of this excerpt are clearly marked with eighth notes in both voices
and are always placed so that they coincide with metric strength. Perhaps the
most interesting feature of rhythm in the example is the way Bach leads the voices
to a rhythmic climax at measure 5. In measure 4, the lower voice, as if pulled
along by the activity in the upper part, joins in the movement established by the
178 TWO-VO;C£ COMBINATIONS
top part to produce a logical and convincing rhythmic peak. Longer durations are
introduced in the lower voice in measure 6, approaching the cadence at measure
7, and natural relaxation of activity.
thus affecting a coherent
The opening of Bartok's First String Quartet, shown in Ex. 9-8, contains a
rhythmic association somewhat comparable to the Bach excerpt discussed pre-
viously. It affords an excellent study of two-voice association in contemporary
music and illustrates several of the basic principles noted in this chapter.
Ex. 9-8. Bartok : String Quartet No. 1 (opening). Reprinted by permission of Boosey &
Hawkes Inc. Sole agents for " Koitura " (Hungarian Trading Company) in the
U. S. A.
Lento, ^"=50
1st =tssjf
Violin
2nd
Violin is ^ p
c.
# n n m s
Vioh
m
Cello ^
Earlier we noted that most melodies are unified in terms of pitch structure by
a simple tonality frame. This frame is formed by prominent pitches which are
tonic or are strong relatives of tonic. The addition o[ a. second voice generally further
clarifies the tonahty of any single lipe, and the two voices frequently create vertical
intervalswhich emphasize the same strong intervals, particularly octaves, fifths,
and thirds which-are-oorjaally lound in the tonality frame of a single melody^as
in Ex. 9-9.
irr- ff
•--'
[['
-^^.^.H^:,S
tr- tr.
r-F" ^"-^
y r
S^
1
f=f=|
-^—. —i— ^ r ^
^
Ex. 9-9b. Reduction of Ex. 9-9a to tonality frame (See same in Chapter 2.).
Ck
—• ^ ^
1
O
fpN^ It
r»
-•
11
It
CI
l-» 1 1
O-J
•
-©-
XT
1 1 1
720 TWO-VOICE COMBINATIONS
Both voices clearly move through the tonality frame of C major. The upper
voice spans the 1 1 th (perfect 4th + an octave) from dominant to tonic, while the
lower part outlines virtually the same tonality pattern. Furthermore, the two
and major thirds involving members of the same
voices often form octaves, fifths
C major frame. Both phrases begin, and the second phrase closes, on members of
the tonality frame.
Not all melodies are as clearly and simply organized around an easily percepti-
ble tonality frame as those shown in Ex. 9-9. Ho%\ever, a second voice frequently
clarifies or confirms the tonality of a less clear upper (or lower) part by creating
relations to tonic that are not explicit in the activity of the single voice. In Ex. 9-10,
the tonality of the upper voice, sounded alone, seems to hang precariously between
e minor and G major; however, with the addition of the lower part, and its e to
e' tonality frame, the upper part is clearly heard in e minor.
A comparison of the example with its reduction shows that the rhythmic ratio
of the prevailing upper part to the lower voice is 2 : 1 (two eighths — quarter) rhythm.
It involves the elaboration of essentially stepwise basic pitches. The intervals formed
by coinciding basic pitches are mostly thirds. The rhythmic independence of the
parts contrasts with the essentially similar rise and fall of the simultaneous step-
progressions occurring in the two parts. The dual step progression found in the lower
part in measures 5 and 6 reflects the predominance of leaps in that voice, in con-
trast to the prevailing step motion in the upper. The high and low basic pitches of
both parts are almost identical with those of the tonality frame; although this is
typical of the simplicity of structure and tonal content of this excerpt, it is by no
means the rule.
The lower part of a two-voice texture frequently acts as a tonal support or
foundation for a more active top voice, and often creates considerably less melodic
interest than the upper line.
In Ex. 9-12, the bass and violin parts of a sonata for violin and organ are shown,
followed by a two-voice reduction. Despite the contrast of activity between the
two parts, both are organized on a simple tonal plan, and the movement to basic
Violin ^^ y-U^
m P fT it-
^ r ^^ p-rrp-
?
^
Organ I
^ 331
P ^^ xn P
m^m^
s^ i ?f=p
122 TWO-VOICE COMBINATIONS
331
TT"
XC
xn
m 331
laz
33:
331 -©- ty
331
jdl
TT"
^^ TT-
^^ -&-
xc
XE
Ex. 9-13 contrasts with Ex. 9-12 in that both voices adhere to similar basic
melodies.
^^ i ^ ^^ ^
^^
I ^i^ ^ ^ /
^ ^ i
^\. I ,
V
'
I* I*
^
— 1 M —
^
Although the upper part somewhat more elaborate than the lower, the two
is
voices, treated imitatively, compete for attention on a more equal footing. Both
parts have similar contours and an equal number of step-progressions. Notice that
the lower step-progressions of the two parts occur successively, about two beats
apart. The syncopated treatment of the upper line, in the first phrase, establishes
its rhythmic identity and individuality, but a comparison of both basic melodies
will show how much the two voices mirror one another. Unity of two-voice design
results when both parts are fashioned from essentially alike basic melodies. Two-
voice frames such as this are common to many two-part contrapuntal textures.
The texture of Ex. 9-14 involves considerable rhythmic independence of the
two parts (with the exception of the latter half of measure 8). The rhythmic in-
dependence, however, is balanced by an agreement of contour between the voices,
seen in the prevailing descent of both parts. Although cadences are avoided in
favor of continuous activity throughout the excerpt, rhythmically strong assertions
of members of the tonality frame occur in measures 3, 4, 9 and 10. Sing the ex-
ample, then study the reduction in Ex. 9- 14b.
—rf—
J-^ [f^
J — Uj
r-&^ 1
If
— p#..
^-^
1
-^^^ ^ —L 0^ p\ =V
'
—— —^
r^ -^ --'m P n
U r
J
\-*^
J
—^-^
1
jij]
S^ ^ f r <s»-
^m ^ nn
—
^• O ()# '
— —-^ 5
^ ^,.
1 9
1
i>
1
'
*^
^1
S^
1
•).
/
•„ • i~
1 1
~l1
1
iV A
1 1 1
1 1
' 1
1
m
1
*)• i 1
• • • m ^ *
/ » m •^ * « iJ
The tonal variety of the two parts is reflected (in the reduction) by an absence
of coinciding basic pitches which are members of the tonaHty frame. The disjunct
motion of the first five measures in the bass is indicative of the leaps formed by
basic pitches in the same measures, but note that the leaps are comprised mostly
of members of the tonality frame, a and e, and are quite stable. Step-progressions
are present (but somewhat obscured) in the latter half of the example in both
voices, and these contrast with the more disjunct beginning. However, the con-
tours of the step- progressions show an essentially diflferent direction in the two
parts, since the upper line comprises a basic ascent from / to a, while the bass
descends a fifth from 6" to .4.
These factors, plus others (as a study of the different forms of elaboration of basic
pitches in both melodies will show), result in a two-part texture whose voices are
independent, while unified by tonality {A), adherence to common pitch materials
(aeolian mode), and similar kinds of durations.
The two-voice framework, like basic melody in one line, provides a basis for ap-
praising the structure of each voice and its relationship to the other in textures in
which both parts, to a greater or lesser degree, participate in the creation of a
cohesive and intelligible musical product. As we have seen, the framework is useful
for pinpointing those features of the parts which are structural, rather than dec-
orative, and it also enables us to view those elements of the texture which hold it
together.
Pitch Material
A glance at any of the preceding musical examples will confirm the fact that
most two-voice textures employ the same key or mode in both voices. We have
also seen that the addition of a second part often clarifies the tonic in a melody
whose key or tonic ambiguous. Comparing Ex. 9-15 and Ex. 9-16, we find that
is
the tonic {A-Jlat) is strongly asserted by both voices in Ex. 9-15, but only by the
lower part in Ex. 9-16. The upper voice of the latter example is apparently in E-
flat (considered as a separate line), while the lower clearly establishes A-Jiat (the
actual tonic of the combined parts).
i ^^'
^n ., f;rrrr rT jJj ^
Although both parts any discussion of tonic, key or mode,
must be considered in
the loweiLYQic e, because it is the to nal foundation for most two-voice works, often
exerts mor e tonal force than the upper. This is well illustrated in Ex. 9-16, which is
heard in A-JIat primarily because of the pull towards A-JIat created by the lower
voice.
Two voices which possess the same tonicmay use conflicting variants of the same
note. This is particularly true in the minor mode with its different scale patterns.
In Ex. 9-17, D-flat in the upper part conflicts with the lower part's D-natural.
D-natural in the lower voice is explained by the ascent of the line and the avoidance
of the augmented second which would were not altered. result if D-Jlat
Considerable variety of pitch resources has been used in two melodically in-
dependent parts in Ex. 9-18. The tonality is D, but it incorporates all twelve tones
of a chromatic scale, rather than a seven tone mode.
Ex. 9-18. Hindemith : Ludus Tonoiis, Fugue in E. (C) 1943 by Schott & Co., Ltd., London.
Reprinted by permission.
Rhythm pattern
m 6-
^^
'
Tonic
V s
i p t ^ ^
jerr ^ ^^^
^
^i
^): y
-
Hf
sequence
1
;^ =^
p ? ^ \^p \,
726 TWO-VOICE COMBINATIONS
Repeated rhythm patterns and melodic sequences create unity, and melodic in-
terest is sustained by avoiding the simultaneous use of identical durations in both
parts. Both lines reach a climactic point about a third of the way through the
excerpt, and then begin a general stepwise descent to the cadence.
Some composers of the twentieth century have created further independence of
lines by employing two tonalities or keys simultaneously, as in Ex. 9-19.
Allegro
Ex. 9-20 contains a section of a canon by Josquin des Pres, a fifteenth century
composer. The most significant feature of this example, from the standpoint of
pitch organization, is that the two canonic voices consistently cadence on different
tonics. The result is that, although the two parts are highly unified through the use
of identical melodic material, they are delineated tonally through their adherence
to different tonics, D and G.
P ft ^ r f f
^m
^ P-
m^
i
'y-L rrr
^ #-
m^
^m
m
TWO-VOICE COMBINATIONS 127
P^
P ^ ^
^^Ff m m m f Id
-& ^
The tonal separation of voices in Ex. 9-20 illustrates the common practice of
bringing in each contrapuntal voice in a different key, or by centering the pitch
motion of successive entrieson different notes of one key, such as tonic and domi-
nant. We shall see the importance of such a technique in our subsequent study of
fugue and other contrapuntal forms.
We have been concerned thus far with the horizontal aspects of pitches in
melody. But if we are to combine melodies, there must exist a basis for the selection
of the harmonic intervals that they will create. A glance at any two-voice composi-
show a variety of vertical combinations, and further study will reveal that
tion will
composers employ systematic techniques for organizing harmonic intervals and
successions of interval roots.
In Chapter 2 interval roots were discussed, and their relationship to the harmonic
series was established. It is entirely logical that those intervals which are ^riost
st able, octaves, fi fth s and most often found as
third s^ are the beginning and caden-
tial^ harmonic intervals in two-voice textures. These intervals are essentially stable,
or consonant, and because of the role that they have played as basic combinations
in music are described as cadential or basic consonances. It is only in recent times that
composers have accepted the possibility of beginning or closing on less stable inter-
vals.
The perfect fourth and major and minor sixths seldom occur as cadential inter-
vals in two voices. Because of their consonant but non-cadential role in musical
practice, these intervals are regarded as decorative consonances, and their use in two-
voice textures is subject to many considerations of contrapuntal technique. The
perfect fourth in particular is generally afforded a special kind of preparation and
resolution.
We should not conclude from composers' general neglect of certain intervals
for cadence that these combinations have no significant part to play in the organiza-
tion of two-voice music. However, because of the tension-resolution tendency and
— — 1
Interval Classification.
The two-voice composition shown as Ex. 9-21 affords a simple point of departure
for a study of the interplay between consonance and dissonance.
—•
C
3 - •>
.
"^ J T
^
|-# r
Tenor ||>):ytFrr 1 0ff] h
1
1
Bass 'y.2
^^4 P n
c r. ^^Lprr 7 f
1* m
m^ V- ''
m m
$
\ TSL
mm ^
15
m^
^ p i m ^
^jt,
fe^
•0 25 ,
[|.>: r r 1
1
# f 1 Iff
r ff 1
3 ^=
y 7 /
M .
a'
« P
3
^ = —
t^4=J
.^e-T-"
•
M
""*
i»
— — — — ^
-?'
?:3C
¥r
:?: '
:
f^
[|0: f—l -|S»
5
h Eff^ ffffrtfrf-\
^ r
-p >— ^^ V * ?
« ^^
—
—
f
f—
V r 1
-f9
^ h
1* Ti
i
h
ll. 4U la. ^ _^ 1
M^—f^
• P-
r#¥|
p.
^ ^^ T^'^44-p-
f
— -* —9 -p — « V ^
-4^ /:
Our main consideration here is the interplay between consonance and disso-
nance, both of which figure significantly in the organization of this two-part tex-
ture. Let us examine first the vertical combinations at cadences. These occur in
measures 4, 9, 16, 22, 27, 32, 36, 39 and 43. An analysis of the cadential intervals
used is shown below.
Cadential Intervals in Ex. 9-21.
Measure Number Cadential Interval Between Voices
4 Perfect Octave
9 l{^r<>T Minor Third
16 Perfect Octave
22 Minor Third
27 Unison
32 PerfectOctave
36 —
Minor Third Unison
39 Perfect Octave
43 Perfect Octave
The beginning intervals of each phrase are:
Measure Number Beginning Interval Between Voices
6 Perfect Fifth
13 Perfect Fifth
^t^'T-h'^yJi
17 Major Third
24 Perfect Fifth
29 Perfect Octave
33 Major Sixth
37 Major Third !
/ ni, q v^'TK\r(\
We may conclude from the above analysis that phrase beginnings and closes are
generally made with cadential consonances, although decorative consonances are
occasionally found at phrase beginnings or phrase endings. We shall see later how
730 TWO-VOICE COMBINATIONS
dissonances too may be used in a similar way. Now reexamine the Bach e minor
Invention, Ex. 9-7, and note that Bach appHes a similar use of the same intervals.
Throughout the first sixteen hundred years of Western music the organization
of melody depended most upon the vocal forms that dominated that broad
time span. In these early forms, e.g., Gregorian Chant, Secular Song, Motets,
Chansons, and Masses, our predecessors established and developed techniques of
melodic composition that exploited the capabilities of the human voice. Instru-
mental melody had its origin in vocal music of the fifteenth and sixteenth centuries.
During these years vocal works were often transcribed for small instrumental
groups, or vocal parts were sometimes doubled by instruments. Many of the me-
lodic patterns we have come to know through instrumental performance are of
vocal origin.
In our beginning two-voice studies we shall limit our writing to the human voice,
laterexpanding our study to include instrumental combinations. The necessity of
performing all musical exercises and illustrations cannot be overstressed.
Consistent with most vocal compositions, we shall employ the following as prac-
tical voice ranges:
Soprano
Alt
°
$
Tenor /' ti"
Bass ^=^
Each voice is granted a potential range of a twelfth. Needless to say, consider-
able care should be taken in approaching the extremities of any voice range. The
low areas of any voice is lost when overused, and average
effectiveness of the high or
singers are not "at home" with melodies that remain in extreme lows or highs for
long periods. The highest note {g') for the tenor in the Machaut composition,
Ex. 9-21, occurs only once during forty-four measures of melody, while the lowest
note of our given tenor range (c) is never used. Low tones in the bass, like high
notes in the soprano, are often thin and lack definition. Great care should be given
to their use.
The distances betweentwo parts will depend partially on the particular voices
used. For example, the widest gap that might occur between alto and soprano (in
two-voice writing) would be a perfect fifteenth, considering the given ranges of
—
both voices. Although possibly effective in isolated cases, such a wide space between
two adjacent, unsupported voices would be rare indeed. On the other hand, uni-
sons represent the smallest distance (or relationship) and are quite common caden-
tially. The usual limit for two adjacent voices is the octave with tenths
, and even
twelfths as rare possibilities.
Less common two-voice combinations such as tenor and soprano, or alto and
bass, may exploit wider spacings because of the natural separation of the individual
registers. In these cases the interval of a twelfth should be regarded as normal,
with two octaves as a usual limit of separation. Common two-voice spacings are
shown in Ex. 9-22, Ex. 9-23, and Ex. 9-24.
Soprano te
^
^ ^ i ^ zz:
?S txj
Alto i
cat
m ^-* 22
Tenor ^^ — w
1 ^
U
Bass ')'
(i ('
$ m p
J 'W L
^ zz:
Ex. 9-24.
The principles for two-voice spacing that have been outlined here can be sum-
marized as follows: adjacent voices seldom exceed the octave, while non-adjacent
pairs (such as soprano and tenor, or alto and bass) may move as far apart as two
octaves. These principles will generally apply even when more than two parts are
.
present. For the time being these principles may be used as a reference for instru-
mental textures as well as vocal. A more detailed discussion of instrumental spacings
will occur in Chapter 15.
Exercises
For more detailed assignments see Materials and Structure of Music I, Workbook, Chapter 9.
1 Organize two-voice rhythmic phrases of eight measures, choosing rhythm patterns from
Ex. 9-2 or Ex. 9-3 as a basis for unifying repetition.
2. Make two-voice reductions of Ex. 9-2 and Ex. 9-7.
3. Locate all cadential consonances in Ex. 9-1 3a.
4. Locate all decorative consonances in Ex. 9-15 and Ex. 9-16.
5. Locate all dissonant intervals in Ex. 9-22, Ex. 9-23, and Ex. 9-24 and note the resolu-
tion of each.
6. Build cadential and decorative consonances above g and/!
c', e', b,
7. Build five dissonances above d', labeling each interval and spacing them for alto and
soprano.
8. Add a tenor part above the bass-line in Ex. 9-12 a, using only cadential or decorative
consonances. Use predominately stepwise motion, writing in the same rhythm (note
against note) as the bass. Plan cadences carefully, and avoid using successive fifths or oc-
tavesbetween parts. Try to use a wide variety of cadential or decorative consonances.
Analyze each harmonic interval used, and circle its root.
9. Make a two-voice reduction of the preceding exercise, indicating any important step-
progressions (as shown in this chapter).
Two -Voice
Combinotions;
70
Decorative
Pitch es
Decorative Pitches
the form of harmonic intervals. Thus our perception of basic and decorative pitches
in two-voice combinations is affected by both melodic and harmonic elements.
Usually the addition of a second part will confirm an appraisal of basic or dec-
orative activity in a single line. This is exemplified by the following pattern in
which decorative pitches in the melody (a),
j^r icii;
m
are heard as unstable in association with the supporting voice in (b)
analysis of (a)
i=.==i
^mm ^
P. t. p. t. p. t.
e. t.
m
733
;
In some examples, however, the greater weight of two voices will result in an
interpretation of basicand decorative patterns on the basis of stable intervals (con-
sonances). In other words, consonant relations between voices may well effect a
stability not apparent in the single melodic line. In the pattern which follows,
basic pitches (apparently) occur on the beat, as shown in the analysis following the
pattern: (c)
f 'i ^n U
analysis of (c) (jki> o J J ^ J J 1
"-^-" J
However, with the addition of a lower voice (d), the on-the-beat pitches become
unstable, creating momentary dissonances, and the after-beat pitches represent
brief points of relaxation or stability, and are therefore regarded as basic pitches
(d)
.fly^^|J|,^-pl^^
analysis of (d)
^^ r
^^ Jl Jo J
ffi
a
Ex. 10-1. Bach: Invention in E Minor.
.fiM^^^ CJ-
jpr ^s o-^r..fl'
: ^ g
'y-u^^i vQ'c^
^ 1=^^ ^ i^ m i
A passing tone a^ is also found in the top voice on the first beat of the first measure.
It is perceived as an unstable pitch, filling in the gap between two structural
TWO-VOICE COMBINATIONS; DECORATIVE PITCHES 135
^
Ex.
^m ^
A B
JCOEM
?
^ ^ ^ a
^ffi^M m ^
s
^m ^ ^ ^ %
Play Ex. 10-3, and notice all passing tones. Consider both voices.
m^^^ '- S J » J-
i
^ m r t O"
:
The e' after the first beat of measure 3, top voice, is obviously a passing tone. It is,
however, consonant with the lower voice a, since it creates a perfect fifth with it.
The e' connects two basic pitches,/' and on the third beat of the
d', as does the e'
same measure. The/' is a basic pitch both because of its metric position and be-
cause it forms a strong step progression with the preceding g' and the e' of the next
measure. The importance of relating decorative pitches to basic pitches, rather than
simply singHng out dissonances, is illustrated in Ex. 10-3.
(a)
^=i £ is a dissonant lower neighbor to F, while in
F
(b)
$T , G embellishes the to which it returns, as an upper neighbor
may
In
be in an accented position
e'.
b', but it
It is essential to understand, then, that passing tones and neighbors are both
embellishments that are present in melody and controlled by the activity of the
single line. In other words, these are figurative elements which comprise horizontal
aspects first, vertical relationships second. We are often aware of these decorative
pitch relationships even when hearing a single strand of melody.
The neighbor tone, which may occur chromatic or diatonic embellishment,
as a
is a most useful figure for developing activity while focusing attention on one pitch.
In two-part writing the neighbor, like the passing tone, is most eflfectively balanced
by relative inactivity in the opposite voice. Just as melodic activity in two-part
textures is generally divided between voices, so decorative activity is often dis-
tributed between parts, rather than centered in one. Some typical illustrations of
neighbor tones are shown in Ex. 10-4. The figure formed by successive occurrences
of both upper and lower neighbors is called a neighbor group. It is shown in the last
two measures of Ex. 1 0-4.
_
^^
(dl
^m 2
^
Both neighbor and passing tones occur in Ex. 10-5.
^^m
Contrapuntal Motion Between Parts and Approaches to Structural Intervals
and oblique motion. Any two-voice work will reveal a variety of motion types. Those
obtainable are illustrated in Ex. 10-6a, Ex. 10-6b, Ex. 10-6c and Ex. 10-6d.
(a)
^ : t ir)- -s>
A
f
738 TWO-VOICE COMBINATIONS ; DECORATIVE PITCHES
Parallel motion occurs when both voices move in the same direction while maintain-
ing the same intervallic distance.
i ^^tttf ^^ i«=^ w ^»
f 3
Similar motion resuks from movements in the same direction but which involve
changing interval combinations.
(C)
* « » m _
i^' S
• — • — V % ^«
m— •* m 1
*/•
^7^
1
/L
" 1
u
'
»
~ m 1
VU
f(r>
m m <
- m m
• 1
•J m m-
II
I r
(d)
E it
TT'
r CJT r
i
M^ i f-DT
r
^
TT
Study the counterpoint in Ex. 10-7, having first performed the music. Locate
the various types of motion that are employed.
Ex. 10-7. Johann Christoph Bach: Chorale Prelude: In dieh hab ieh gehoffef, Herr.
i& i
—^-^
i ^m ^
>
feS
^m ^
( mi s
,/?<V^
^ i ^ # 10
J: 3:
Cp :£:
#^^ r r f p g
rWO-VO/C£ COMBINATIONS i DECORATIVE PITCHES 139
Ex. 10-8. Zachau : Chorale Prelude: Vom Himmel hoch, da komm ich her.
cadence
^^
y-h>'.
vtrrr f#^
It is clear in Ex. 10-8 that the composer has used a wide variety of consonances
as basic intervals, and that unstable intervals, which further contribute to a variety
of sonority, are the result of decorative patterns. The choice of intervals used and,
to a large degree, the way in which they are treated, differ somewhat in individual
musical However, the proportion of
styles. thirds and sixths usually outweighs_
fifths, octaves, unisons, and fourths.
A comparison of Ex. 10-9 with Ex. 10-10 will illustrate the intervallic variety
possible in two-voice writing.
740 TWO-VOICE COMBINATIONS; DECORATIVE PITCHES
Ex. 10-10. Hindemith: Morienleben. (C) 1924 by B. Schott's Soehne, Mainz. Renewed 1951,
Reprinted by permission.
Consecutive
r^ ^
fifths in contrary motion
^ m m
1 £
J J
-S>-
Oblique
fifth
In Ex. 10-9 Bach has relied upon thirds and sixths, primarily, as on-the-beat
sonorities. Not until the cadence to .4 is a prominent octave heard. It is approached
by contrary motion. The excerpt by Hindemith, Ex. 10-10, reveals considerably
more intervallic variety, but it shows a kinship in the way fifths and octaves are
approached by contrary motion.
Rhythm also determines vertical relationships. Fifths and octaves that fall on
strong beats naturally attract more attention than those occurring on weak beats,
and so the approach to these cadential intervals should be viewed in relation to
metric position. Again, contrary and oblique motion are usually found in the ap-
proach to fifths, octaves, or unisons on strong beats. Similar motion, on the other
hand, is not uncommon at cadences to the octave. In such cases the upper voice
most often moves by step, while the lower voice skips up or down. As in Ex. 10-1 la,
and Ex. 10-1 lb cadential finality can be emphasized by directional agreement.
Final cadences to the perfect fifth (which are quite rare) are almost never approach-
ed by similar motion (direct fifth).
*
f*rt ^ ^t± ^
^^^"[ur*' I
jjj
— m 0^
^^
s p^
^i
Contrapuntal Treatments of Other Consonances.
^ ^^m ? m
m ^ ^ ^
wm m Jtv #itv#
^^t^m-
^
\n ^
^
742 TWO-VOICE COMBINATIONS; DECORATIVE PITCHES
voices moving in similar motion by skips to a fourth are extremely rare. As we shall
see, fourths, especially when accented, usually resolve to thirds.
When parallel fourthsdo occur, they generally involve durations of less than
the prevailing basic duration. An example of movement in parallel fourths occurs
in measure 1 of the excerpt in d minor by Bach (Ex. 10- lib.) Here the parallel
motion in consecutive fourths is the result of movement around the basic pitch A.
D", the upper member of the first fourth moves to a lower neighbor, c"-sharp,
while a passing tone g connects the lower a with f. The prominence of these
fourths is negligible, since they fall on a weak beat.
Thirds and sixths are used more freely in two-voice writing. Thirds, of course,
occur cadentially, while sixths are seldom found as cadential intervals, never in
final cadences in two voices. Like other intervals, thirds and sixths represent only
Viola [b'»«) jr ^ W^
Cello
m fE ^
^ i
m^
^ ' ^ 1^
1 ^ P
im w—w
i
J
¥
10-14b. Parallel Fifths (avoid).
^^
Ex.
^U^r r
a^fs
r
f d ^ t
Ex. 10-14c. Parallel Octaves (avoid).
fV^ . '.;
H-^^~-
'^'1'.^
^ r~r
-J \
'
i
ls ^'
^ n ^
r
-r
-i J
r £££
## O . I iJ 1
POJ:: 1
fe
Fr.ff.i... t
f^ i
m V
i
*
r
Basic Contrapuntal Treatments of Unstable Intervals
We are familiar with the unstable and ambiguous quality of the melodic tritone,
and the problems attendant to vocal performances of it. It is this same unstable
characteristic that, on the other hand, gives the tritone its special quality of ex-
pressiveness which many composers have exploited, as in Ex. 10-15.
k'' i
>j r -^'
U3
pt^ ^U W IP
^
. 1
zee
-0=0- 3x:
31 1^ 331
"cr ~o~
T.T. T.T.
TWO-VOICE COMBINATIONS; DECORATIVE PITCHES 145
The tritone is perhaps most prominent in major — minor music where it occurs
between the subdominant and the leading tone (4-7). This relationship occurs
both melodically and harmonically in two-voice counterpoint, and is quite effec-
tive in developing melodic or harmonic tension and in building strong "resolution
tendencies." We shall see in subsequent study that the tritone figures significantly
in two-voice cadences.
There are predictable treatments of melodic and harmonic tritones which are
basic to much music, and which can easily be related to our notational practices.
Some of their more common treatments are shown below.
1. Skips up or down an augnieh''^je«HrduVm are usually followed by stepwise motion
in thesame direction as the leap. This is true with a// augmented intervals.
2. Like skips of an augmented fourth, skip/l<^ ^n ascending or descending di-
minished fifth are normally succeeded by stepwise motion, but generally in the
opposite direction from the leap. This is true with all diminished intervals.
3. Tritone leaps occur more often as unaccented leaps, often in the course of repeti-
tive patterns whose main feature is rhythm. Stepwise resolutions are usually
found here too.
4. Tritone skips, like most leaps, are most often approached by step, generally in
the opposite direction from the leap.
5. Leaps that follow melodic tritones usually take the opposite direction and fre-
quently resolve to pitches that form step progressions with members of the
tritone.
6. Harmonic tritones formed by the movement of two contrapuntal voices usually
resolve by the same kinds of stepwise movement associated with melodic
tritones. Augmented fourths will "expand" by step, while diminished fifths will
contract.
Locate all melodic and harmonic tritones in Ex. 10- 16a and Ex. 10- 16b. Then
compare the appearance of each with the various principles described above.
P.T. (VT)
146 TWO-VOICE COMBINATIONS ; DECORATIVE PITCHES
Most of the tri tones in Ex. 10-16 are the result of decorative patterns. Both mem-
bers of a harmonic tritone are seldom basic pitches. Furthermore, the tritones
illustrated in Ex. 10- 16a and Ex. 10- 16b involve the 4-7 scale degree relationship,
as is most often the case in —
major minor music, while the tritones in Ex. 10- 16c
occur in a modulator^' passage which closes with a progressive cadence in g minor.
In later study we shall deal with the treatment of tritones as more structural pitches,
particularly as members of fuller harmonic textures.
The treatment of other unstable intervals, such as seconds, sevenths, and aug-
mented and diminished intervals, is similar to that of the tritone; they are generally
decorative, and they move by step. (Important exceptions to this will be dealt with
inChapter 12 in connection with other forms of decorative activity.)
Unaccented passing tones in the upper voice create a variety of dissonances in
Ex. 10-17. They clearly decorate basic pitches which form consonances.
'hi'.i r r ^ r r r r m
Considerable tonal movement and much decorative activity can be found in
Ex. 10-18. \'irtually all of the unstable pitches, producing a great variety of
melodic and harmonic intervals, are treated in stepwise motion. Passing and
neighbor tones prevail.
TWO-VOICE COMBINATIONS; DECORATIVE PITCHES 147
m^4U^ ^
^ ^^
In this chapter we have some of the basic ways of organizing pitch
outlined
motion involving both consonances and decorative pitches. At this point we have
established a framework for continued study of pitch relations in two-voice tex-
tures. In the next two chapters we shall see how these same patterns of movement,
and others, provide a basis for the structure of the phrase, for the creation of points
of movement and relaxation, and for the further elaboration of the two-voice
frame.
Exercises
For more detailed assignments see Materials and Structure of Music I, Workbook, Chapter 10.
1. Using the lower voice of 10-7, add a new upper part in prevailing quarter notes intro-
ducing passing and neighbor tones to create decorative eighths.
2. Write your own versions of the example for aural identification of basic consonances and
decorative pitches.
3. Analyze aurally the different types of motionin Ex. 10-17.
4. Play the lower part of Ex. 10-16a, Ex. 10-16b and Ex. 10-16c on the piano and sing the
upper part. Then reverse the process.
5. Use the pitches in the lower voice of Ex. 10-16b as basic pitches, one to each measure.
Then add an upper part that is entirely consonant, demonstrating the principles of con-
trapuntal treatment of consonances discussed in this chapter. Write nine measures in
simple triple meter.
6. Sight-sing Ex. 10-8. Analyze all decorative pitches in the excerpt.
7. Play the patterns in Ex. 10-14 for identification of parallel or consecutive intervals.-
Continuation
if
77
Two -Voice
Combinations
Cadences
One of the most significant areas of a musical work, regardless of its texture or
form, is is here that both tonal and
the final cadence, also called the close. It
rhythmic elements must be drawn to a convincing and satisfying conclusion.
We shall concern ourselves first with final cadences in two-voice music, although
it should be understood that a study of cadences which delineate phrases and
sections must also lie within the scope of any study of musical construction.
Despite the diflfering cadential characteristics of diverse musical styles, three
principles of cadence construction are basic to music in general: (1) a slackening
of motion, usually evidenced by long note values; (2) the confirmation of a tonal
center; and (3) consonant intervallic relations between the parts.
The illustrations of cadences in Ex. 11-1, Ex. 1 1-2 and Ex. 1 1-3, although prod-
ucts of musical styles separated by hundreds of years, clearly adhere to the princi-
ples described above.
^ ^ ^^ » m
i ^ ^^ ^ % -niz
148
CONTINUATION OF TWO-VOICE COMBINATIONS 149
1 ^ i
>^''^ J J
#
'>
'i r (SJ
fcfc=4
m ^
In each case (Ex. 11-1, Ex. 11-2, and Ex. 11-3), cadential to nes occ ur on strong
heats. C adences on weak beats or weak parts of the beat (sometimes called
that fall
loc ation, and through their obvious importance as the last tones of a melodic pat-
t ern. Furthermore, this rhythmic-metric prominence is often made more evident
by broadening or slowing down the melodic pace with longer note values intro-
duced in the approach to the actual cadence. Such is the case in each of the excerpts
of Ex. 11-1, Ex. 11-2 and Ex. 11-3. The importance of these and other rhythmic
aspects of the cadence and its approach cannot be overstressed.
Composers sometimes prolong activity in one voice after the cadential tone has
been clearly arrived at in both parts. Through such an extension, each voice main-
tains its individuality and independence into the cadence proper. In Ex. 1-4, the 1
lower voice asserts the tonic A, while the upper voice, having touched upon a',
continues down by a series of leaps to the octave a.
^ zuz
33:
Final cadences to intervals other than cadential consonances, though possible and
sometimes found in contemporary music, are tonally ambiguous and must be re-
garded as exceptional.
The particular choice of cadential interval is determined by the prevailing tonal-
ity, the kind of melodic activity that reaches its fulfillment in the cadence, and the
composer's choice of sonority. The octave is common as a closing sonority, and with
the unison it represents the strongest, in terms of stability. It is perhaps less in-
teresting as a sonority than the third or fifth; The perfect fifth, though a strong
tonal relationship, is not found as often in two-voice cadences as octaves, unisons
or thirds. Both major and minor thirds, the choice of which is determined primarily
by the mode of the work, occur frequently. Any cadential consonance that con-
firms the tonality of the piece or its closing section may be employed.
The cadential interval is often called the ultimate and the interval that
interval,
immediately precedes it is called the approach interval. Cadence patterns are formed
by the movement from an approach to an ultimate (or final) interval. Some of
the most common two-voice cadence patterns are shown in Ex. 11-6.
CONTINUATION OF TWO-VOICE COMBINATIONS 151
xir
* ~cr
zcn
^ ^^
^«-
~cr
zaz
l^ -ee-
^o-
V^ W -ee-
* e=
-e-
=w %==^
-e- 331
^^^ To^
o n
^^ ^
-&- in
n
(b) Contrary motion, one voice moving by step.
=g=
XT
-e-
tu>
^ HX
xn ^^
XT ^»^ ^ ^ '
te
i b^ ^
xr
=
^_
^ ^
^^
'
-»
-
" "^^
-o-
XT
-e-
-e-
I -e- 3X 33:
ij .g. » ^^
1 ^8 ^ b » t^^ s -e-
s -e-
Direct fifths
331
I^ ^ -»
- ^-^
S 331
S JOZ
—
33:
-©-
^ 331 -e-
-o-
^r - ^^
:
Various forms of scales are implicit in the different patterns. These patterns are
often labelled according to the melodic movement of the voices in their relation
For instance, the first pattern in Ex.
to tonic (the root of the cadential interval).
11 -6awould be indicated as 2-1 and the first pattern under Ex. 11 -6b would be
written as 5-1 The numbers 7-8 indicate step-movement from the leading tone
(or subtonic) to tonic, whereas 2-1 indicates a step descent from supertonic to
tonic. These patterns represent an intervallic framework that results from melodic
movement at the cadence. However, any of these basic pitches could be decorated
with a variety of embellishment involving passing tones, neighbors or other elabora-
tive pitches.
Two voices moving in similar motion to a perfect fifth produce the direct fiftho£_
covered fifth. This effect has been generally avoided in two-voice cadential practice.
Direct octaves, on the other hand, are quite common, if the upper voice moves by
ascending or descending step.
It is approach intervals of the basic patterns shown in Ex.
significant that the
11-6 are allcomprised of cadential and decorative consonances. Perfect fourths do
not occur frequently as approach intervals in two-voice writing, nor do tritones.
Modal Cadences Two Voices. We are familiar with some of the char-
in
acteristics associated with modal melodies, and the group of scales known as
modes
Ex. 11-7.
JbuxiiiA ?^-> ^
.^4<^ ^' ^^
m Dorian
ti o zcn
331
Phrygi
ice:
i» o 3x:
JcnjocuA
Lydian ac..u)ci i
Mixolydian
^
-^e- _Q_
m: O i. " " 3r o '^
n o
( Natural minor
Aeolian )
«» O HSZ
3i: -o-
*
Just as modal melodies exploit certain characteristic interval relationships, so two-
voice combinations adhere to a group of basic cadence patterns that incorporate
modal patterns. It is significant also that many of these patterns, some of which
are shown in Ex. 1 1-8, are by no means limited in use to modal melodies; they can
also be found in compositions oriented around major-minor scales and more
chromatic works. All of these patterns, associated with two-voice textures, involve
7 8
Ex. 11-8. »", Two-voice Modal Cadences.
J-
Phrygian 2-
Dorian and Aeolian 2^
_Q_
xn 3a:
331 o -e- nr
$ ~&- "D"
t»
TT -e-
HE
J.
l5_po
Lydian Mixolydian 2^
^
^ fl o t»
O
11
.DR v-v
tK>
l» ^
^ tl
n j'^j'^
'^^
11
II
>^ v.^
IJ L-S- 1
1
P
S?
^ r'
i
'Lc£; |
r pr r i
^rtrrir'^
=^ £
^
y I g
3r*^ — 7-8
2 - 1
'Y 2 fr r r M 1
r ^ f9
=] fl
"^^
a f
— P ^ A ^ ii
iLfr r
1
1
2 1
7 8
^
^'
J J^^=^ ^ST^ i
^^ DCH
7 - -
-e-
8
2 - - 1
Dorian and Aeolian mode compositions, as shown in Ex. 11-8, usually employ
cadences that approach tonic from the whole step below and the whole step above,
while Phrygian cadences approach tonic from the whole step below and the half
step above. Cadences in the Lydian mode, as in major and minor, involve a lead-
ing-tone to tonic ascent while the other voice (usually the lower) moves by a de-
scending whole step from 2-1. Mixolydian cadences, like Dorian and Aeolian, con-
tain no half step, and tonic is approached from the whole step below and above
154 CONTINUATION OF TWO-VOICE COMBINATIONS
i$ ^^ ^ m J
m^
s
P- i9-
m gf^ I ^
SI
" -#
ijjjJ.' ^ -&^
^
)^ r r r f r iii^ # zsn
Roots
Roots ^m V I
Dominant Tonic
:
Pi wt
S ^ i
Roots
V I
Dominant Tonic
In contrast with the ascending leap of a fourth (or descending fifth) that is
found most often in the lower part, the upper voice usually moves by step from 7-8
(leading tone to tonic), or from 2-1 (supertonic to tonic).
fc:
znz
* 3E ini
-
"O"
1
-1
As shown in Ex. 1 1-lOa and Ex. 1 1 -10b, the melodic movement of both voices can
be indicated by numbers which represent the horizontal movement of the voices.
Several basic V-I patterns are shown in Ex. 11-11.
-p
/r i> It
i&i ^-\ C) 7t> *' *»>
ysu ~
o ^^ It
/-» *-» 1
hnt *'
J
-e- e- -e- -e-
r\-
•/•
^
'-' t> o
^-k ^-k i~\
'/ n LI
Ex. 11-12.
m i
'j-^i r r r 5?z:
2. The inversion of the above, with 7-8 movement in the lower voice and a leap
up a fourth or down a fifth, although possible, is an unusual exception in final
two-voice cadences. 2-1 movement in the lower part is virtually never found
when the upper part moves by a leap.
Ex. 11-13.
^^ i f d • ^
^ P
3. Contrary motion prevails, although direct octaves may be found when the
upper voice moves by step.
Ex. 11-14.
s Tnr^^ m
uT-
4. 7-8 movement most often involves the leading tone to tonic, but 7-8 motion by
a whole step (subtonic to tonic) may be found. (Leading tones in minor keys
must be indicated by accidentals.)
^^^ ^
Ex. 11-15.
:nj
j^'' \l'ii i
^
i
I J J
^ ^ -O-izz
'fjfsm:
'CONTINUATION OF TWO-VOICE COMBINATIONS 157
5. V-I cadences create root relations up a perfect fourth (or down a perfect fifth).
The roots of these two intervals always correspond with dominant and tonic at
final cadences.
Ex. 11-16.
V I
Ex. 11-17.
^ LJlI ^=J
Ex. 11-18.
I codetta
pitche s, such a s the leading tone or subdominantj usual!) will form part of tlie
^^^ ^m ^ m
Ex. 11-19. Handel: Fughetta.
^m
^ ^
IV
The cadential major third is formed by the leading tone A-sharp and F-sharp.
The root of this interval, F-sharp, is the dominant of ^ minor; "more to follow" is
strongly implied by the absence of tonic, and by the presence of the leading tone.
Progressiv e c adences to intervals^ whose roots- are._th£.jiominant-ar£_ traditjo na ly 1
I
I f
fe
s zaz
-e- s 33=
331
~n:
f r
3 4 e 5
r
^ 33:
f -o-
^ Deceptive
T T
zaz -e- ^
T
3jr.
Cadences which confirm or imply changes of tonal center are an important means
of creating tonal variety, and help to delineate melodic form. These cadences, occur-
7-8
ring at phrase or sectional closes, are often of the V-I group, although <-,'. cadences
and any cadences which are terminal in effect may be found. As in final cadences,
the root of the cadential interval is generally in the lower voice.
The opening eleven measures of Bach's G Minor Invention contain a terminal ca-
dence to G, a transient-terminal cadence and a modulation to d minor. All
to B-Jlat,
of the cadences are basic V-I patterns, elaborated by various forms of decorative
activity. Each cadence marks the close of a phrase and is signaled by the introduc-
tion of longer durations in one of the voices. Melodic activity continues the other
voice, however, thus diminishing the rhythmic effect of cadence. The cadential
pitches inevitably occur on strong beats.
760 CONTINUATION OF TWO-VOICE COMBINATIONS
i 2^ ^^jP^m Szm
^y^ ^ ^ S
is transient-terminal, since B-flat has been established as tonic in the music preceding
the cadence. F momentarily emphasized, by the leading tone e'-natural, which
is
with c" forms a V-I pattern. We anticipate a return to the Bflat tonic. This is
denied, however, and F is developed in the subsequent phrase as a continuing
point of focus. The cadence in bar 5 confirms Fas a new tonality. As in the example
by Bach quoted as Ex. 11-22, the change of key represents a modulation to the
dominant key. The interplay of tonic and dominant keys is the most common tonality
CONTINUATION OF TWO-VOICE COMBINATIONS 161
Tonal Function Within the Phrase; Root Relations in the Two-Voice Frame.
The melodic and harmonic action of the cadence has been described in terms
of the root relations of cadential patterns such as V-I, V-VI, I-V, and so forth.
Any cadential pattern can be described with Roman numerals representing the
roots of basic intervals. This procedure is equally applicable to an entire phrase or
composition. In other words, having discovered the basic harmonic intervals and
their roots, one can represent their relationship to tonic by using a Roman numeral
for each scale degree, regardless of the type of key or mode dealt with.
The passage of Ex. 11-24 consists o{ basic harmonic movement I-VI-II-V-I, as
Ex. 11-24.
When a change of tonality occurs, the same procedure can be adapted to the dif-
ferent degrees of the new key, as shown in Ex. 1 1-25. This analytical set of symbols
762 CONTINUATION OF TWO-VOICE COMBINATIONS
m
(Articulated Pedal)
^m M^ *i_*
^
^ £
m mm
*»•=
^5
F:V IV V
^ ^^ ^^ ^^
I
^^ ^^ ~
fe =t= ?
* ^
nV V
^
IV I B^:I IV II
LllJ
:^
> r •
^F./l ^ ^ :zi ^ 1*—y^
^
^^
C£[fr
^
p
^ ^^ ^
^ ^ -e-
' Basic rhythmic associations of two-voice textures were discussed in the beginning of Chapter 9.
A rev iew of this information is appropriate at this time.
CONTINUATION OF TWO-VOICE COMBINATIONS 163
Adagio
^m n^ j ^
i ar
l -'-Mi! i^lr J ^ m
Two voices of more equal rhythmic interest are shown in Ex. 11-27. Again,
however, both voices outline similar contours, and the low and high points of both
occur at about the same points in the phrase, although not simultaneously. The
accompanying reduction to basic pitches shows the similarity of contour in a skeletal
form.
Ex. 11-27.
P
^ s
if^
^m
3=*
m ^ t
s«
p ~&
T21
^
s -JX-
XE
JQI
IC
^ ztn
dependent melodic curves. Here the voices take turns as predominant parts, but
164 CONTINUATION OF TWO-VOICE COMBINATIONS
even so, they outline basic pitches in essentially contrary motion, and the high and
low points of succeeding motives occur one after the other, rather than simulta-
neously.
Contrapuntal interest can be achieved, as in Ex. 1 1-28, not only through an equal
distribution of activity, but also through variety of melodic contours.
Two equal voices compete imitatively in Ex. 1 1-29. Although they use identical
melodic materials, successively, the two voices are delineated effectively by two
simple processes: Each successive basic interval is approached through contrary
motion, which is continued right up to the last interval. The highest note of the
upper with the lowest note in the bottom part. Furthermore, each basic
line occurs
pitch, as shown in the reduction, is approached in contrary motion. An organiza-
tional pattern such as this can easily become as dull as any other procedure, if
overdone. The effectiveness of the example, however, illustrates the degree to
which contrasting contours can contribute to melodic interest and independence,
even when other factors are virtually equal.
)
j'''^
-^LLLr P 7 P f^ m ^ ^^ m _ m
S ^ ^ P
0^0
(Art]cu]ated_ Pedal
:^
^m
m^ feS ^^ ^
The motive between the two parts, creating continuous eighth-note
fluctuates
activity to the cadence. Both voices are punctuated by rests, and the relative in-
activity in one voice makes it easy for the listener to shift attention to the other
part. This kind of rhythmic exchange, based on repetitions of one basic motive
treated in two parts, is a fundamental principle of two-part writing.
766 CONTINUATION OF TWO-VOICE COMBINATIONS
A different kind of repetition organizes Ex. 11-31. The upper line unfolds en-
tirely through successive patterns of three eighths. The less active lower part, which
is essentially accompanimental, is differentiated from the upper line by quarter
notes. The prevailing contrary motion further contrasts the two voices.
4''<^^^UsaJ ^^ w~7^:
^
"^'iii i PUj' "Usr J)
Although both voices are unified through repeated eighth-note activity, they are
at the same time clearly contrasted rhythmically and move independently of each
other.
A example of motive organization is shown in Ex. 11-32, which contains
final
two independent voices, each unfolding through sequential repetition of a motive.
The brackets, indicate that it is entirely possible to perceive the beginning of each
repetition at several points. This is true because the successive melodic units are
alike, seem to overlap, and are not separated by rests or contrasting durations.
Sequence.
A similar sequential organization occurs in exact form three times and then
modified a fourth time in Ex. 1 1-34.
Ex. 11-34. HIndemith : Ludus Tonalis, Fugue in E. (C) 1943 by Schott & Co., Ltd., London.
Reprinted by permission.
-/
^ ^ \'j
i,
^ m^ ^
§ i I P m^ P
uJ
i*^ l
l,J^^t^ m
Roots
"^
As one might expect, the step relations of the voice movement create a stepwise
succession of harmonic roots.
Successive phrases are frequently built out of a pattern that is treated in the
first phrase as an exact sequence and then "borrowed," so to speak, as a point of
departure for a contrasting or "answering" phrase. The listener is led into the
second phrase with material which is derivative from the first but not maintained
to its termination. Such a technique of smooth phrase delineation is seen in Ex. 1 1-
35.
'*^®
CONTINUATION OF TWO-VOICE COMBINATIONS
A A Vlnl
B^ i ^^
g
S ^M P P P V
ff'
55^1 131
Roots g
P ^
Roots
The second phrase, which begins at measure 5, is not actually part of the sequence.
—
Both the sequential root pattern (up a fourth down a fifth) and the step progres-
sion formed by basic pitches in both voices is broken. However, the second phrase
clearly begins with a restatement of the first half of the two-measure motive that
has occurred sequentially in the upper part.
Turn to Ex. 11-22. Study both parts separately, paying particular attention to
the amount of sequencing that occurs in them. Note where Bach has treated both
voices sequentially at the time, and also notice where he has not. A careful
same
study of this work shows that sequential repetition is one of the most continuously
applied techniques in the unfolding and development of this Invention. Together
with imitation, which will be discussed later in this book, sequence is indigenous to
contrapuntal writing, and represents a common technique for unifying a two-phrase
organization.
Exercises
For more detailed assignments see Materials and Structure of Music I, Workbook, Chapter II.
1. Illustrate ten basic cadences in two voices to the tonics F, 5-flat and D. Play the lower
voice of each pattern and sing the upper, then reverse the process. Use some of the same
examples for class dictation and recognition.
2. Use the same patterns as basic pitches and write short two-voice elaborations of them,
using passing and neighbor tones to create rhythmic play. Sing or play these patterns and
use them for dictation. Vary the meters that you choose for elaborating the patterns, and
limit them to two or three measures' duration.
CONTINUATION OF TWO-VOICE COMBINATIONS 169
3. Analyze the sequences in Ex. 11-26 and Ex. 1 1-27. How do they differ?
1-
4. Write a four-measure two-voice phrase, based on motivic treatment of measure 1, Ex. 1
14.
5. Continue Ex. 11-13 for five more measures, using two measures as the basis for
the first
7—8
a sequence that occurs two more times, followed by a ^ cadence to G. Use the ex-
,
Conclusion
.f
72
Two-Voice
Combinations
Suspensions
sustained through a subsequent strong beat, and resolved by step on the following
weak beat to another basic pitch. Syncopation often results from suspension
patterns, and the three elements which comprise the suspension figure
W W
are shown in the following: i^ ^
Preparation Susp- resolution
( basic pitch) (decor) (basic pitch)
The addition of a second voice generally involves the treatment of the suspended
tone as an unstable pitch, as shown in the figure that follows
s, w
$
The suspension pattern in two-voice textures, then, creates a tiny cycle of melodic-
harmonic movement comprised of stability-instability-stability, and the addition
170
.
^
^&
^
W
*i
^ ^ iM
^ •)-(.
1. g' was first heard as a consonant pitch, having been prepared as a consonance
(upper member of a minor third), on the last beat of measure 1
2. g' was sustained into the next strong beat so that it created a dissonance (minor
seventh) with the lower voice.
3. g' resolved by descending step tof 'sharp — a consonance (major sixth) —on the
subsequent weak beat.
The suspension, g', is actually heard as the middle member of a figure of three
parts: preparation, suspension and resolution.
Preparation, then, refers to the point at which the suspended pitch {g') is in-
troduced as part of a consonant vertical relationship. Syncopation, a common
feature of the suspension figure, results when the preparation occurs on the weak I!»
/\ a. Syncopation
Pt ^&
Prep. Sus. Res. S R
g
m
i i ^^ 19
Syncopation
m ^ S
-&
R
i&^
s (21
Syncopation
^
b.
^ m ^ R
S ^^ i P
^ ^ ^
^ ?
S R S R
In Ex. 12-2a, the preparations are at least as long as the prevailing beat (a
quarter note or more), and they are of no less duration than the suspension itself
These represent the most common preparations. Less typical preparations occur in
Ex. 12-2b, where the preparations are less than one beat's duration and, in most
instances, of less duration than the suspended tone.
Rearticulated suspensions are shown in the first and last examples of Ex. 12-2b.
Although the suspended pitch is generally sustained, a rearticulation after the prep-
aration is common.
Different Forms of Suspensions. In Ex. 12-2a and Ex. 12-2b, the suspended
tone formed a minor seventh with the lower voice. Though it is common to refer
CONCLUSION OF TWO-VOICE COMBINATIONS 173
to the suspension as a dissonance, all suspensions in two voices do not form dis-
sonant intervals. However, they do create relative instability, and the mterval
of resolution will always be a cadential or decorative consonance. Furthermore,
the resolution is usually to a basic pitch.
Suspensions are described by referring to the intervallic distance between the
two voices at the point of instability (or dissonance), followed by the interval of
resolution. For instance, in Ex. 12-3, a seventh is created by suspending/, which
in turn forms a sixth when it resolves by step to e.
LH r^
Ex. 12-3. Suspensions.
J J ^
*
,.
i
r r r T ?
The resulting pattern is called a "7-6 suspension." The interval of preparation is
not usually included in the analysis, although it is taken for granted to be con-
sonant. The 7-6, then, describes one basic form of suspension. There are four other
types: 4-3, 6-5, 9-8 and 2-3. All of these patterns, except the 2-3, have one impor-
tant common feature : the suspension pitch is in the upper voice. Although occasional
exceptions can be found, the 7-6, 4-3 6-5 and 9-8 are suspensions in the top voice,
,
Mi
a.
J- r ^ i
^p
R
^>Mi r ^'J
^ m :zz
^^
a. b.
4-3^ P
4-3
^^ ^i^^ tfd
^^ P ?^
tit
-#
,
^ ^ tf^
^ 4-3
tts^
4-3
WhexL used cadentially, the 4-3 suspension most often resolves by a descending-—
minor second to the leading tone (7) of a V-I cadence pattern.
Analyze the 4-3 suspensions in the excerpts shown in Ex. 12-6.
^ ^ ^ ^^ ^
<->
r J
%f-#^
l^^ P m ^ i^ e
P r r P " <=
5^^P=5 ^ te
^
^^f?^ ^ g ^
•-3-
r ^pr
t-3-i
^
L3J
p
is mild, and the interval of resolution often sounds more strongly related to the
prevailing tonality, than to an anticipated point of arrival.
CONCLUSION OF TWO-VOICE COMBINATIONS 175
Ex.
^m ^
12-7. 6-5 Suspensions.
f
The 6-5 suspensions o ccur frequently in approaches to V-I cadences, as seen in
Ex, 12-8. Note that the movement constituting the resolution of the sixth continues
a descending melodic line, and this contributes to our expectation of descending
resolution (6-5).
^^ # f lef
^^ m-m-ft
6-5
JOO
1 -e-
-e-
6-5
"TV
6-5
-&-
6-5 W TT
^
1 ^=^ £ S
^m ^ ^ m
7-6 7-6 4-3 6-5
i ^
9-8 Suspensions. 9-8 suspensions are rare in two- voice writing, especially
at cadences. They are more common to threeand four voice music. Although
they occasionally occur within a phrase, as shown below, they represent unusual
exceptions in two-voice practice.
^
Ex. 12-9. 9-8 Suspensions.
^=pt
m
u> i!
r ^ r
7- 6
^ 9-8 7-6
m ^^
—
An extremely common
suspension in two-voice music has the suspension figure
in the lower part. This suspension, the 2-3 (or 9-10), is used both cadentially and
within interior areas of the phrase like the others. It is not often found in final
cadences, however, but more often in sectional or phrase endings.
b.
4^ ^ J »
^^ i
i rJ 0_ P -^ §
^ ^ 2-3
Ornamental Resolutions.
X t^'^
M
Whenone or more pitches are interpolated between the suspension and its note
of resolution, the resolution is called ornamental. In most instances decorations of
the suspension involve step movement. However, movement to basic associates
may an embellishment of the suspended tone. The suspensions in
also occur as
Ex. 12-1 la, measures 3 and 4, have been rewritten in Ex. 12-1 lb, ornamented.
The d^ in measure 3 is reached by step and creates an escape tone, while the a^ in
measure 4 is a basic associate.
^m rzi
—
^ P ^=^
^^ i
m * (^
i
7-6
i
7-6 i
. ^jtf I
m^ ^
#
^ wm ^^
® f^^p
i
1
I54=?i: m
7-6
n 7-6 s
CONCLUSION OF TWO-VOICE COMBINATIONS 177
Several ornamental resolutions of a 7-6 suspension are shown in Ex. 12-1 Ic.
r-U'X Pr zaz
^ 7-6
32:
J < y*
7 -
-o- ^« 1
m £ nx
fe^^^ m
sixteenth notes {b^, which anticipates the note of resolution, and a^, which is a neigh-
bor to b'^), but the resolution occurs with the arrival of the quarter note b^ on the
fourth beat of the measure, and so corresponds with movement to a basic pitch.
Ex. 12-12. Praetorius: Chorale Prelude, Ein fesfe Burg ist unser Goft.
i
^
r r r m m^ pp &
m
^ s
^ m
^m f f
-
f
6
^
778 CONCLUSION OF TWO-VOICE COMBINATIONS
12-13 the g^ of measure four forms the resolution of the suspended a^ heard on the
first beat of measure three. Five notes of an ascending pattern intervene between
the suspension and its anticipated resolution. The step progression formed by the
two basic pitches, a and g, is another factor that causes g to be heard as a natural
point of arrival of a.
*l W J V
i»^^
=^ m
^m m ?itF«rf
A similar delayed resolution occurs in Ex. 12-14. Locate it and analyze the
suspension.
One of the most interesting techniques associated with the suspension is the so-
called "change of bass."
CONCLUSION OF TWO-VOICE COM6INATIONS 179
In this type of suspension the lower voice, which normally remains stationary
as the suspension in the upper part is resolved, moves by an ascending perfect
fourth to another pitch. The
formed at the point of resolution of the
interval
suspended seventh is a third, and the resulting pattern is described as 7-3. Note
that the anticipated stepwise resolution of the suspension to a consonance still
takes place. The 7-3 figure is common as an approach to the V-I cadence. It
is also common within the phrase.
T he change of ba_s s (or lower voice), or change of soprano (or upper voice) jm ay
hejnun^ in any suspe nsion figure, although it is most commonly found as 7-3. In
general, the bass may move to any pitch that is consonant with the resolution of
the suspension. Ho wever, it usually m oves in contrary motion with, the top voice^
The illustrations of Ex. 12-16 illustrate change of bass patterns found in con-
junction with all of the suspensions that we have studied.
V-.- f
^7-6 3i:
^^ 7-3
331
j" ^ r P^ 331
^^ -Q"
^? JClL
p ^
4-6
§ ' ^ r ^
nx
^m ZUl
^
2-3 2-5
780 CONCLUSION OF TWO-VOICE COMBINATIONS
p ^ /?
'^
^ n r
ini
^^
iE
/•-^
s p6-3
6-5
P s e
i ft=i^ ^^ ^^ ini
^)-lI',
g-tr-^
<' r a ^
9-8 9-6
ij " ^ f r r
nx:
^^ lai
^E^^
^ 7-6
33=
i ^^
7-5
33:
m^ U
•r-i <t
m
#
^ m i
m
m
i
9-10
(2 - 3)
^
^ ^
? ^
-p-i
4-5
:
^rijim
^^ 7-8
The various forms of the suspension clearly constitute an important facet of two-
voice counterpoint. Like other contrapuntal and organizational techniques, the
suspension figure is found in varying degrees in most musical styles, representing
another type of patterned movement that establishes an interdependent relation-
ship between basic pitches in two musical lines.
A summary of the foregoing study of the suspension should recall the following
principles
2. The three steps that comprise the figure are: Preparation, Suspension and
Resolution, abbreviated P.-S.-R.
3. Suspensions are usually, but not always of the same duration as the prevailing
basic duration.
4. The preparation is usually equal (or greater) in duration to the suspended tone.
The duration of the resolution may be equal to or less than the suspended note.
5. Suspensions are described according to the vertical intervals formed at the
suspension and its resoluton; they include the 7-6, 4-3, 9-8 (rare in two parts)
and 6-5^ and 2-3.
* Further discussion of the suspension figure and resolutions will be found in Chapter 15.
— —
motion with the upper voice, and forms a consonance with the tone of resolu-
tion.
Despite the diversity of style reflected in these works, the principles of the sus-
pension that we have discussed are incorporated in Ex. 12-18, Ex. 12-19, and
Ex. 12-20.
Ex. 12-18.
\fiMr 1
r*i — 9^ —— P*h
«-J- J-
m
f
— -P ^
-n
^
M
—
3 ^ m p
J\ ^Tf^K,
')-| /^ }
^ J
IT,
g
L-e)
*"»
vt
Ex. 12-19.
?t I
P ^ ^=^ *-H J* f
?F
m
'hm f r
^^ ^
^i3 "m
^' ^^
W ^^ r Pr p^
l-L
f
^
^^ ^
\
pp rn^J^
u
Ex. 12-20. Obrecht : Missa Sine Nomine, Agnus Dei.
j> i J- ,
=
# ^^ f^^^
m -*-^
m iP^ ^^^
CONCLUSION OF TWO-VOICE COMBINATIONS 183
m^ *-*-#
mm ^ -t»^
p ^m
yr^ Vc^
jn=
}(^
^
V4
i » icn
Imitation
Imitatio n results from the stat ement of a melodic pattern by one voice, and the
a nswer of the same pattern (e xact or modified) by another voice. The pattern
imitated (usually a motive) is often heard first as a solo statement, thus underlining
its an imitated subject. Compositions of two or mor e voices which employ
role as
strict mitation are called canons. Other types of compositions which employ imita-
i
'^
tive sections interspersed with non-imitative (free) sections include fugues, inven-
tions, chorale preludes, ricercares, motets, canzonas, and others. We are con-
cerned here with the principle of imitation, rather than the study of large musical
forms in which imitation is found.
Shostakovitch has employed imitation at the octave in Ex. 12-21. The time
lapse between statements is quite close, one beat.
Ex. 12-21. Shostakovitch: Symphony No. 5, Op. 47, I. (C) Copyright MCMXLV by Leeds
Music Corporation, New York, N. Y. Used by Permission. All rights reserved.
^^
'^'<'
1
1^ m.
Although the imitating voice may enter at any pitch-interval above or below
the initial statement of a pattern, im itation at the unison (beginning the imitative)
answer on the same pitch as the first statement), octave, fifth or fourth, xepresents,
the norm. Furthermore, the imitation of a melodic unit may be strict, the answer
duplicating the first statement in rhythm and interval, or it may be modified
rhythmically or intervallicly. Three kinds of imitation are shown in Ex. 12-22,
Ex. 12-23, and Ex. 12-24.
- The term canon refers to "law," in this case denoting the "laws" of one voice dictating activity
in the other.
784 CONCLUSION OF TWO-VOICE COMBINATIONS
^ ^E^ zn
i.
^ a- P
^
^i^ p mI
pff^f p^
^
Co! era
i^ V
loco
"^^i^ ^
m^^
Ex.
^
12-24. Shostakovitch:
I ^^-4^
rgfS^
^^
Symphony No. 1,
=^
m
Op. 10,
i
II.
s
as«
(C)
^
Copyright
^te:
;i^
MCMXLVI by Leeds
Music Corporation, New York, N. Y. Used by permission. All rights reserved.
I Solo
Oboe
i
Clarinet in A ^^^i
Violin I
*
Violin n
t
Viola
i
Violoncello g -^
J' ^ J' <
,S>
^
ll
^M
ContraBass
1^ fMi
CONCLUSION OF TWO-VOICt COMBINATIONS IBS
^^ ^=^
n Solo
^ /
fc± i r jfli ^ ^r^ I
-
p tJg J) '
i
s ^ ^# 7 J) 7 .J) ^ h^ '^ J)
^^
3 ^m
Imitation need not be limited to the opening of a composition, but may occur
at any point in the unfolding of a work. In fact it is often effective in later portions
of a piece, where contrast and changes of texture intensify and heighten develop-
ment. In other instances, a piece that begins imitatively may continue without
subsequent imitative entries.
The excerpt from a two-voice composition by Lasso, Ex. 12-25, affords an ex-
cellent opportunity to see how some of the structural elements that we have been
studying are integrated into larger sections of pieces.
o |> - t»
^=s fg
#
im ^ 2t=^ P P m
w ^P^ p ^ ^
^
-S-
P ^ 'j^^'
^ ^m p p ^^i fj p ^
:
^ ^ *=iF»
» P^ i m
I ^3E
? P ? i ^ 3x:
The texture of this passage, written in the key of G Dorian, is contrapuntal, and
free imitation maintained throughout. The music of both voices is developed,
is
with rhythmic variations, out of the two motives heard first in the lower voice
1 12
;i2
^ . o
The second motive is a modified
parts. Despite the similar melodic content of both parts, contrapuntal independence
is maintained by staggering the entrances and avoiding the simultaneous use of
identical durations. Not until the final cadence of the excerpt do the two voices
converge rhythmically.
Even though no strong cadence occurs until measure 14, the music "breathes,"
because the voices rest at Thus the piece takes on shape, and
staggered locations.
the parts are more independent than they would be if they moved continuously
from opening to close.
Sing through the piece, then analyze the structure, paying particular attention
to: melodic motion and treatment of skips, the occurrence of dissonance, vertical
combinations, and imitation.
Other Forms of Melodic Decoration : Anticipation, Leaning Tone and Escape Tone
4L
^ ^
g ^^
i^'r
me^Lh
— —
In Ex. 12-26, the tonic (e) occurs as a rhythmically weak anticipation of the
cadential basic pitch (e). The anticipation, of course, involves pitch repetition,
and is effective where a relaxation of movement is desired, as in the cadence.
When anticipations occur at the beginning or within the phrase, they generally
form segments of characteristic motives, often treated sequentially, rather than
mere pitch repetitions. A sequential treatment of anticipations can be seen in Ex.
12-27. Another example of a patterned use of anticipations is shown in Ex. 12-28.
^
Ex. 12-28. Binchois : Rondeau, De plus en plus.
^^ P S^ X X
W—JZMZ
¥
^& ^ ZSl
^^ 331
Tied anticipations create syncopations, not unlike those often found in suspen-
sions. In the example by Stravinsky, (Ex. 12-29) f-sharp in measures 2-3 is intro-
duced as a tied anticipation.
Ex. 12-29. Stravinsky: Octet for Winds, Hi. Copyright 1924 by Edition Russe de Musique ;
9-
-
-4)i-9 2_. -^^ —
'-\rrf- iiip
-^ ^ ^
ff.-.
Y—l II
,
1^^
jJT3
leaning Tones.
1 —
-0 *
The
J—« N
use of the leaning tone in two-voice writing
J J ^ W^ is limited
almost entirely to the upper part, as illustrated in Ex. 12-30.
788 CONCLUSION OF TV/OVOICE COMBINATIONS
* <^~)
m 6
T* m fcss
FS
^5 ^
^ £ S
The leaning tone has been regarded as a particularly expressive melodic ele-
ment, and its appearance is a predictable feature of many full-textured works of
the nineteenth century. Most leaning tones create harmonic interval patterns
similar to those of suspensions: 4-3, 7-6, 6-5, 9-8 and 7-8. When the pattern creates
augmented or diminished intervals, it usually resolves in a way consistent with the
resolutions of those intervals noted earlier.
m ^ fei^ ^ ^ ^
^g p ^
SS
s * d • i '
r p
© ^
®
f^^ P i^
^1^ s ^
CONCLUSION OF TWO-VOICE COMBINATIONS 769
fc
te m (^J ^
(
i ^
ig'^ J ^
^n\ <'
^) ^
p fe p ^ J)
-^
P
Pedal
Escape Tones. Escape-tones, approached by step and left by leap, are usual-
ly unaccented and occur most often within the phrase, rather than cadentially.
Like leaning tones, escape tones are generally found in the upper of two voices,
as seen in Ex. 33 and 34.
Escape Tones
j¥ -j
r C-fi ^
'):tiji '4
rJ r m
Ex. 12-34. Bach: Invention in E Minor.
fe =e=^ # J *
f^=U
fc^ ±
m^ f-r
^
^
Wehave seen in this and earlier chapters how decorative pitches connect and
embelUsh basic pitches of a melodic line. Pitches that form unstable intervals with
a second part may be a part of the basic melody, thereby assuming a more
structural role. One of the main considerations is metric placement and duration.
In Ex. 12-35a dissonance is formed by pitches which are related to a recurrent
basic pitch that is established by its repetition and metric placement.
'y-^'..
i } I :*=t T^ J ' J>
I
The tone e^-JIat occurs throughout the passage, except in measure 7, as the lower
member of an accompanimental pattern in the lower part. In the second measure
it upper line. This conflict continues
creates a persistent dissonance with d- in the
through measure 3, and is finally resolved in measure 4. There is nothing decorative
about the continued presence of the e-JIat throughout the example, so it must be
regarded as a pitch of considerable structural importance. Its role is that oi^ a. pedal
(or ground), that produces a strong tonal reference for the melodic activity above.
Two voices are implicit in the accompanimental pattern, as the reduction to basic
pitches shows (Ex. 12-35b).
A b^-natural occurs three times in the upper voice in measure 5 of Ex. 12-35a.
Again, although we anticipate a resolution of this pitch to c^, it is heard as a basic
pitch, and therefore a fundamental element in the design of the melody. Notice
that its duration is considerably longer than that of its note of resolution, c"^.
^ Whether they are sustained or rearticulated, pitches which create similar effects are called
pedal points Although more common
. to homophonic music, pedal points may be found in two-voice
compositions as well.
CONCLUSION OF TWO-VOICE COMBINATIONS 191
t±
m ^
e.t.
4^-)-
i
P.t.
^
^"^-V^I1
j %
4 ^
^ ^
t i
i
p;
r r
lA
?^
a -i*4-
^
(#)
P ^^
i
ip.t
15>^
(^)
-J)u.n.
^
i
<9-
f f
The four-measure excerpt from Bach's A minor Invention, Ex. 12-36, contains
a predominance of disjunct motion. This activity, associated with the melodic
oudining (horizontalization) of chords, suggests analysis which takes chordal im-
plications into account. These procedures will be discussed in later chapters deal-
ing with thicker textures.
Two shown as Ex. 12-36b and Ex. 12-36c.
reductions of this Bach invention, are
In reduction (I), the melodic lines are shown as the chords which they outline.
Reduction (II) eliminates decorative pitches and shows the addition of a third
voice in measures 1 and 2, that is suggested by the activity on two levels that forms
the lower voice.
itJ J U^ '^
\)' (;
^
^^ ^
rr-n ea--a'^
\l ^.rrrrr^^r^rrTrr .r n
TTM
1
H- ri 1
ri fri i f ri
-^ ^
— frr: ,
— )
J
1
U,.^^^ M^
B 1
^^
Ex. 12-36b. Reduction (I).
4^?
j j j 3
t •eM g= z;
') f^,)
p.t. '
^te
i=
^^ *w^
i
Ex. 12-36c. Reduction (II).
—
I n ^
r
is
$ ^ S'
J J ^^
m J ii
Even in this more complex and chord oriented counterpoint, resolutions of dis-
sonances associated with melodic embellishment are still applied. The D in the
lower voice, measure 1, acts as a passing tone between e and c. Its progress is inter-
rupted by leaps to and a reiteration of d. Then d'^, e'^, f^ and g^ are
b and g-sharp,
heard as a step progression in the upper voice, beginning with the third beat of
measure 1 and continuing to the third beat of measure 2. As members of a sequence,
,
Cross Relation
Gross relation (sometimes cdAXtd false relation) results when variants of the same
note occur on successive beats (or beat divisions) between different voices. In Ex.
12-37 a., /-natural in the lower part is followed by anf-sharp in the upper. A cross
relation results, although the melodic effect of each voice is perfectly satisfactory.
In the approach to the cadence of the same passage, b-natural and b-fiat are cross
related, producing the effect of a change of mode. Ex. 12-37b contains three cross
relations,and creates an inconsistency of mode, (modal conflict), by pitting A
major in the upper voice against a minor in the lower. Simultaneous cross relation
occurs when different note variants occur at the same time, rather than successively,
as in Ex. 12-37c.
^sssssti.
CONCLUSION OF TWO-VOICE COMBINATIONS 193
^m
, a -^"^
^m
h
yTOr
^^^m. ^M ^ ^
^^ 1
i
t
m p
m:
7
^
Ef^ ^
An interesting use of cross relation can be found in Ex. 12-35. The b-mtural, on
the first beat of measure 3, creates a cross relation with the b-fat just heard in
measure 2 in the lower voice. This colorful effect is created by the use of the lowered
supertonic in the bottom part. The lowered supertonic often occurs before ca-
dences, and somewhat jarring effect makes the establishment of tonic even more
its
effective. Ex. 12-38 and Ex. 12-39 also illustrate cross relations.
Ex. 12-39. Stravinsky: Symphony in Three Movements, I, (piano part). (C) 1946 by Schott
& Co., Ltd., London. Reprinted by permission.
m W m m
-3*^
-^
^m t
1
$ i
^ 3
¥ ^ w. ^ W ^ te
^
' :
(^
<'
^-^J^' J^r
Cf r jgr J
^^TTT-fT^
LLLT^J
2. Cross relations occur naturally from combinations of essentially diatonic move-
ment in one part with chromatic alterations in another. Chromatic alterations,
such as those in measure 1 of Ex. 12-38, and those in Ex. 12-41 are essentially
decorative, rather than basic pitches. Basic pitches seldom create cross relations.
^
^1'''
1
'
f
^£Q'»^^^
ijUi^J^
^^ ^^
^
^
-
j fTf
b^
t±
m- m m m
*'
i -
k
^P d ' S
C Minor
:
—^i —
\^ '^^'^^^
^ \^^^
i
te
m i
fct
C :
(Mutation)
Major
j
^*
Wj ^ ^:
^
f^i^^SHh
CONCLUSION OF TWO-VOICE COMBINATIONS 195
Cross relation has had a variety of treatments in the course of Western music.
It seldom arbitrarily used, and in most works it is either a logical outgrowth of
is
Exercises.
For more detailed assignments see Materials and Structure of Music I, Workbook, Chapter 12.
1. Use the examples in the section dealing with suspension for sight reading and aural
recognition of suspension figures.
2. Using the two motives from Ex. 12-25 which appear in the text (p. 194) follow the
example as a lower voice and add a new upper part which incorporates suspensions. Ex-
tend the second motive so that it ends with a cadence on G.
3. Analyze all forms of decorative activity in Ex. 12-13 and Ex. 12-14.
4. Make up framework of four measures in C time. Write two basic pitches per
a two-voice
measure and close with a V-I cadence in the key of E-flat major. Elaborate both voices
with all forms of decorative activity studied in this chapter. Label each decorative
pitch.
5. Add a new lower part for Ex. 12-14. Use an imitative entry for the added part.
6. Make up short two-voice examples (no more than two measures long) illustrating a dif-
ferent type of suspension with achange of bass in each. Label each step (P.S.R.).
7. Rewrite Ex. 12-19, keeping the upper line as it appears, but using a different accompani-
ment and transposing the example to E-flat.
8. Work out a keyboard setting of the upper voice in Ex. 12-12 that begins with an imitative
entry in measure 2. Accompany the upper voice so that suspensions occur in measures 5
and 6. Close with a cadential 4-3 suspension.
9. Make up short illustrations of all forms of decorative pitches that have been dealt with in
the book. Use basic cadences as a framework for the illustrations.
Three-Voice
Combinotions
13
Rhythmic Association
the active parts of Ex. 13-2 are so thoroughly enmeshed with the motion of the top
line that they, too, represent a form of homophonic texture.
^5 M^ ^1 s
f ?
^m^ 1S2
196
THREE-VOICE COMBINATIONS 197
P^ m^ s fr r
i
^ < ^^ tj) . J j)
^ ^^
^ r r tF
i
m ^ s i ip ^
The Mendelssohn excerpt of Ex. 13-2 contains brief suggestions of part in-
dividuality, the rhythmic separation of the middle voice in measures two and three,
and the independent contour of the bass at several points. But on the whole, all
three voices are relatively unified in pitch contour and rhythmic play : a minimum
kind of counterpoint present.
is
"
To justify the description contrapuntal." a three-voir.p r.om V»ma|jr>n piii<;t rr>n-
ph^ '^-
* dominating voice
Ex. 13-4. Hindemith, Ludus Tonalis, Fugue in E, No. 4. (C) 1943 by Schott & Co., Ltd.,
London. Reprinted by permission.
i
s
J .
^^
m "h^
u 1
^ i
f
#
* dominating voice
f
Even two examples of the one basic type, a latitude of linear interest is
in these
displayed by the separate "subordinate" parts. The upper parts of the Bach ex-
798 THREE-VOICE COMBINATIONS
cerpt form a simple foil for the greater melodic interest of the bass through the
first three measures (the roles of the middle and bottom voices are switched in
measure four). The upper parts of the Hindemith excerpt are less subordinate in
character, for they move in faster relative note values and cut more interesting lines
in contrary motion to the bass.
Type 2: Two voices of about equal interest; a third voice decidedly subordinate.
,j J i^i ;J
$ ^m
i
r ^r=r r
J
E^
i
r
w |)iJ. J)
^
1^
^frffi^
P^i #
Ex. 13-6. William Schuman, Symphony No. 3, Fugue. Reprinted by permission of Copy-
right owner, G. Schirmer, Inc.
^^^m s^^
• S i» \?
mi
*^
g¥
i
r=""*=f
^ Iffl ^
In the Franck excerpt of Ex. 13-5, the two outer parts dominate because of
greater rhythmic interest and imitation of patterns (the bass of measure two and
three imitate the soprano of measures one and two). The Schuman example is
similar, for the disposition of melodic interest again is in the outer voices. But here
the lower part does not contain imitative statements, nor is it quite as active as
the top part.
Type 3: Three voices of approximately equal interest, each sharing the listen-
n /l,^
^m^M^S^ ^-^
^ P^ r
i !
^ffi m ^^ a n^ j
r
^^
:
j'^'UJ ^
yr^ f rr
^
Wf^
i
rt —i
ss
f ^ f=r=^ rrrf i ^^
fT^ frr
"N
3 ^^ ^ i
Both the Handel and the Lasso textures use imitation. In Ex. 13-7 the bass line
imitates the top (beginning in the last half of measure two), and the top imitates
the bass (beginning in measure three). During this exchange the middle voice
winds through its own separate and quite individual motion. Ex. 13-8 contains
imitative statements among all three voices at various points.
Almost any contrapuntal texture of three parts can be fitted into one of these
three categories, but music does not normally remain set in a single disposition for
long. The composer is free to shift or vary textural relationships. Consequently, it
is often futile to describe an entire composition or large section by any one of these
types.
Within a three-voice comVjination one line can dominate the other two by
virtue of its more interesting rhythmic pattern. But faster motion alone does not
ensure melodic domination over slower moving lines, unless this faster motion is
couched in rhythmic patterns that are diversified enough to be interesting. The
comparatively rapid motion of the "continuous basses" in many compositions of
the Baroque era recedes into a subordinate "background" role because it also
happens to move in rigidly uniform patterns.
^^
'y-Hi) f P f ii
^^^
f^ r f
rJ
^ J.
r-
^^ r
p
r ^
^ —
Both of the upper voices in Ex. 1 3-9 are marked by rhythmic diversity. This
sets them off as conspicuous in comparison with the continuous eighths of the bass
hne. When rhythmic diversity is present in all three parts, the pattern of faster
articulation usually dominates the texture.^
An exception to these rhythmic "rules of thumb" can occur when a melody
that has been firmly established in the listener's mind is sounded in slower motion
than its accompanying parts. For instance, the tune that serves as the melodic
basis for forms in which variations occur simultaneously with the known melody
since it has the advantage of familiarity —
may be accorded more attention than
voices of considerably greater rhythmic interest.
The chorale prelude uses a pre-existent melody which, in many settings by com-
posers, employs this contrasting relationship of slow voice accompanied by busy
rhythms in remaining voices. In spite of its less interesting motion, its familiarity,
coupled with emphasis of registration or instrumention in performance, may still
focus our attention on it as the dominating voice.
Ex. 13-10. Schiedt: Chorale Prelude on Gefobef se/sf du, Jesu Christ, Verse 4.
^ rr ^^M ^ r ^r p
4
n f
fm
9^^
Chorale melody-
Rhythmic Unity
The rhythms of the separate voices normally are related so that they complement
one another, avoiding extremes of pattern contrasts that could thwart the associa-
tion of the three as compatable parts. For this reason, voices normally share the
same set of rhythmic divisions, though these divisions are not always articulated
simultaneously. In many three-voice textures the separate patterns, though dif-
ferent, when added together produce a periodic splitting of the meter's basic dura-
tion. In this way the combined rhythms of Ex. 13-11 divide the basic duration (
d )
into a continuous stream of quarters, while Ex. 13-12 contains divisions into six-
teenths. The result of these combinations is a continuity of flow for the whole tex-
ture.
^All other factors being equal, we are prone to regard the highest voice of a combination as the
—
most important as the "melody".
2 In organ chorale preludes the "melody" is frequently reinforced by octave couplings to strength-
en its prominence.
.
J I J^j h i J
Combined motion: f T f f f f f T f f f f f f f f f f T
^,2 ii ?^^^
J'"'''l\'g f
P ^^W f
-^^
nh a) r C
^ r :
i !^
Although all part combinations do not consistently divide the metric basis into
a set division, the separate voices often use the same (or closely related) patterns.
Ex. 13-13 shows a less constant division of the basic duration. The result is a some-
what less-driving rhythmic propulsion than the passages of Ex. 13-11 and Ex.
13-12.
Allegretto
^^ ^ PP?
{.'ip}
tr
n . :
^^---^ J :
M
f=^
^^^
%^
^^ m ^
Combined
motions: c/ r [_&• f p r r r c/ r
1
l;
^
r r r p
way of creating part separation. Combinations of three equally active voices usually
incorporate a broad diversity of motion, in this way accentuating further the in-
dividuahty of each part. The section shown in Ex. 13-14 contains parts whose
rhythms typify this kind of individuality.
f
'
. h
ici^
«?' i
J- Jt^
i
4
W^
^^
^ -o-
i
"s
Here each voice moves in its own individual rhythm, the occasional sixteenths
acting as a minimum rhythmic bond between the two upper parts, but with no
closer hint of kinship in evidence. Passages of this kind are infrequent in music of
three parts, for most textures rely on a closer rhythmic union between at least two
of the lines.
articulations ( J "
J J •
J J J J ) that blends the three different parts together into
a more severe separation of parts than two versions of a single metric division.
When the contrast is prolonged it is difficult for the listener to determine the funda-
mental metric division unless a third part reinforces one of the patterns.
In addition to contrasting or contradictory beat divisions, the displacement of
accents in one part, by syncopation, forcibly differentiates that line from its as-
sociates. The ties across bars in Ex. 13-16 contribute to the clear rhythmic defini-
tion of each of the parts, as well as to the harmonic interest.
i ^ 5=^=?^
m
^
J
*„ I
r- r^^
I J ^ ^ i^ 1
^ ^ p
i ^
r i
ni^f,.
r
'itf^[(j I
m ^
rmf # 1
i
/7\
# .3^
f^ =Qff
^^ ^ mr^ W^ 1 ij
i
J j/i
g
^hJflUj
w ?
On the other hand, textures of greater contrapuntal interest do not always dis-
play this singularity of cadential form; individual parts sometimes create their
own phrase lengths and cadences separately. This condition avoids rhythmic
stagnation by allowing continued motion in at least one voice while another pauses
at its own formal junction. Ex. 13-19 is bracketed to show the phrase structure of
each part, thereby revealing the variety of cadence locations that create a con-
stantly mobile texture. Only in the sixth measure do all of the voices combine in
a halt that creates a strong cadential effect. The imitation between the three voices
further enhances this high degree of motion.
Z —
Voices
/
a^^^
rT=^ rfff
:a ^ ^
f ^r- ffT
^f
rrrr
S £f^ i
ZZI
li^ f
^ f
rfff
^^
ff rW
TTf
f^
^m
r
>
m m m m m^ i
In less imitative combinations, momentum is sustained at some phrase endings
by continued articulation in only one part. This singular movement reduces the
conspicuous pause of the cadence, and it throws the active voice into stark relief
against the inactive.
Ex. 13-19. Mozart: Sonata for Violin and Piano, K 376, II.
Andante
^^
Violin
i -^
jjjjJi^J -^^ > >!^
-? ' J fi
-9
i
1 f mf WB
Piano
^m
'
j''
U^jU^U^^. w^
m
— a
^jlm J,nm
lm^ycf^
If_^_
Cadence
f*4-
±?±:
VBfi^^
THREE-VOICE COMBINATIONS 205
The introduction of a third voice to the musical fabric creates the possibility of
a new reference unit of harmonic structure, the chord. Up until this time we have
gauged pitch associations by their intervallic content without relating them to
chord structure.
We shall defer discussion of the chord until Chapter 14, for the present dealing
only with the melodic principles that are pertinent to combinations of three rela-
tively independent parts.
It is helpful to view a three-voice texture as a two-voice basis to which a third
complementary part has been added. This added part forms a two-voice counter-
point with each of the original voices, and these two new associations follow closely
the principles discussed for two-voice textures in Chapters 9, 10, 11, and 12. These
contrapuntal relations of three voices are represented in Ex. 1 3-2 by brackets where ;
in a two-voice combination there is only one set of vertical relations, there are three
sets in a three-voice texture.
Top voice
Middle voice
Bottom voice
Since the two outer voices of a three-part texture form the structural pitch limits
— the —
"top" and the "bottom" they constitute the vertical framework for the
combination. For this reason, the success of a three-part union depends strongly
upon the compatability of the outer voices.
The middle part in some combinations serves as a rhythmic and harmonic
"cushion," filling in where outer parts take momentary pause or where they,
alone, do not complete the desired sonority.
Pt
m . I J
f
.
S 5^ i m m r
g^^ I
206 THREE-VOICE COMBINATIONS
actual note values alone, thereby producing a slower motion in relation to the other
voices.
Ex. 13-23. Shostakovitch : Quartet, Op. 49, I. Used by permission of MCA Music, a division
of MCA Inc., New York, N.Y. All rights reserved.
^ jmji jmji nu g.
m^ I tfe
s
'^
m ^ w
^ 'P-
f^f^
Wf ^ ^> Ff
T-
'-^p^
n Jjgji juoj juajg
W^
r-
p^ P^ r-
»»?-
thus represents the ultimate in static pitch contour. The bass is itself relatively static,
for it consists of an ostinato pattern of the most rudimentary shape.
Individuality of pitch contour determines to a certain extent, then, the equality
or inequality of linear interest discussed earlier. Three voices that form quite dif-
ferent pitch outlines, their rises and falls occurring at separate locations, create the
greatest degree of contour separation. Most
do not incorporate parts so
textures
individually contoured, but a texture of three interesting parts will have lines
w^hose highs and lows occur separately as often as together.
Ex. 1 3-24 shows a three-part texture in which each line is rhythmically unique,
separated from its associates by virtue of individual contour.
THREE-VOICE COMBINATIONS 207
The two outer parts of a three-voice texture can be regarded as a framework that
follows the same organizing principles developed in earlier sections devoted to two-
voice textures. The potential cadence formulae remain identical, and the inter-
vallic relations between the two lines retain cadential consonances except when
decorative patterns form dissonances.
The middle voice forms a two-part union with each of the outer voices, as we
illustrated with Ex. 13-20. Its relations with each of these parts is also that of a
two-part texture, but a few extenuating circumstances create some notable dif-
ferences when the three parts sound together.
For instance, the perfect fourth, which does not appear frequently as a basic
interval in two-voice textures, appears commonly between the upper voices of
three-voice combinations. The tonal instability of this interval precludes its oc-
currence between the lower two parts as a cadential consonance formed ; when it is
between the two upper parts it is normally accompanied by a major or minor third
or a perfect fifth between the lower two parts.
Ex. 13-25.
® and and
»** ^^"^^f
* =P4=&
fe
lJm3I H«
V^ V^T^
3=E
A style of writing that makes extensive successive use of the interval combination
shown in Ex. 13-25 can be found in the works of composers from many different
periods of music history. Its effect is so striking when it occurs within a generally
contrapuntal texture that it has been assigned the special mime faux bourdon ("false
bass"). As we shall see in the following chapter, a more apt name might be sixth-
chord style, because of the sixth formed between the outer parts.
fauxbourdon
^1 t iw
T$ r
m zpz
^
208 THREE-VOICE COMBINATIONS
'y-Hmr pr
Reduction
r '^pr pp
jl^
n^r^r^^
parr py
r
p ? ^f=^
r
5
^,p} rP J J J hm .-,
^ P
gf
8-63-53-53-3
M^'l l
^
--6
^
r r »r F-
4^
P f
r
^^
Intervals
1.
2.
3.
6
3
--6
--5 --5
3 6
4
3
8
6
3
536
3
685
3 3
^
Ex. 13-28. Landini: Ballata.
»^ ^^ ^ fe i
3
jJ ^^ J^
P fM-^ d=bi^
P
J
P 5-5
?^
-33 -34
1 8 3 6 8
2 4 - 5 1
3 5 3 5
Ex. 13-29.
citi 33=
Unis
i^tKi
. (D
nr
(i)
It
(I)
H
f ti
* ^ Cadential Consonances
t s o-
tl
Decorative Consonances
^=s
All other interval combinations which include seconds or sevenths, or which
have fourths or tritones between the lowest and either of the other parts, usually
arise from a decorative pattern of some kind. We shall deal with some of these
matters in greater detail in Chapter 14, where we will discuss chord structure for
the first time.
^
»#-^ti)-p- ^ t ^^
270 THREE-VOICE COMBINATIONS
In Ex. 13-30, each of the three parts remains within a clearly delineated range
of activity, maintaining its unique patterns within its assigned region. An abstrac-
tion of the spacing characteristics in this passage shows more clearly the distribu-
tion of pitch activity within the three lines, all of which cover a total span of two
octaves and a fifth.
'
N
m
Textures of this type cannot remain for long in a single relationship of parts
unless similar motion prevails, the individual part contours assuming approximate-
ly the same shapes.
Type 2: Two parts coupled in close range, the third part separated by approx-
imately an octave (or more). (The coupling can obtain between the two upper
or the two lower voices.)
ju-irf^^jffi\jn^im \
m^^4
^
MiK^I
'
fl i
\;'^l I
These two excerpts show the coupling possibilities in three parts, in terms of
register. In the Schumann sample the upper parts are combined within close
THREE-VOICE COMBINATIONS 211
proximity, while the bottom part winds through a range approximately two octaves
below the middle voice. The Bach excerpt is a reversal of the Schumann, for here
the upper part is separated from the coupled pair of lower parts. In both instances,
the separate part is more conspicuous by virtue of its highly contrasted octave
position in the combination.
Features other than spacing can serve to emphasize a particular voice. Although
the two upper parts in Ex. 1 3-34 are coupled in range as distinct from the bottom
part, the longer durations of the upper line make it a distinct pattern from the
other two. Here rhythm, not spatial coupling, is the distinguishing factor.
Ex. 13-34. Telemann : Chorale prelude, Chrisfus, der uns selig macht.
^^
^^ ^ wt ^^^ ^^
D^ erf mr f mw
'h^W) [J i ^ ^ ** -e-
In addition to the specific musical effect desired by the composer, voice distribu-
tion in a texture depends upon the instrument or instruments that play them. A
distribution such as that in Ex. 13-35 is impractical for a piano because some of the
spacings would be clumsy for the right hand. Ex. 13-36 would be ineffectual for
the piano, but for different reasons; here the problem would be clear articulation
and separation of individual melodic strands. The constant voice crossings can be
clear when played by flute and two violins (or by any combination of single-line
instruments), but the articulation difficulties for the pianist would be ticklish.
Added to this purely physical problem would be the lack of tonal contrast between
the three separate voices, and the combination of objections make such a spacing
most improbable for piano.
trnttCT
1^p-m m»0»m»mm pp mp mpmm -m-^^mmppp
^^m ^
^ p'mm pdm^ i^P
^^
.
Flute
^^
s
^ P
i#
Violins
n ffhM4
\
¥
'fil
r^i ^ ^^^
f
P^
r=T
In general, gapped spacings that contain no couplings of any
heterogeneous, or
kind are associated with highly contrapuntal textures in which individuality of
parts is paramount. In more homophonic {homogeneous) combinations a closer
spatial alliance of parts becomes essential. The effect of harmony is most readily
achieved when parts are close enough to make them fuse into a block of sound.
A simple experiment will demonstrate the differences of spacings. If the com-
binations of Ex. 13-37 are played by separate instruments, the more unified, com-
pact effect of (a) contrasts discernably with the more disparate sound of combina-
tion (b).
Ex. 13-37.
a.
Flute or Clar. i ^
Clar. or Trpt.
4''i i
r-f
DI
rT xc
Clar. or Trb.
i
^^ L^
Flute or Clar.
P ^=T 1
^^
Clar. or Trpt.
Bassoon
or
Trombone
^
"n P
Aside from the differences of tone quality produced by the register changes for the
top and bottom parts in (b), the wider separation alone reduces the effect of tonal
fusion that is more apparent in the closer-knit alliance of (a)
7HRff-VO/C£ COMBINATIONS 213
Exercises
For more detailed assignments see Materials and Structure of Music I, Workbook, Chapter 1 3.
1. Plan a three-part rhythmic texture for percussion (clapped hands will suffice for perform-
ance if necessary) that consists of three complementary patterns.
Model
f r ^ci/ i
f r ^ai ^
r pr pt r p
I r I r I r i r r r irr n :mp
2. Look through piano, choral, orchestral, and chamber music to find passages of rhythmic
association which correspond to one of the types discussed early in this chapter.
3. Find two parts (or extract them from a composition) which will serve as an outer frame-
work for a three-part texture. Add a third part in complementary pitch and rhythm
patterns.
4. Write an eight to ten measure passage in which the top voice dominates, the two lower
voices are relatively subordinate and are coupled together in register. Write another
passage in which the bass dominates and the two upper voices are coupled.
5. Analyze a number of contrapuntal textures (Bach, Handel, Mozart) for basic pitches in
three separate voices. Construct a three-voice basic framework as an abstraction of the
texture. Then decorate the resultant framework to create a different composition.
Chord
Structure
14
In Chapter 2 we discussed the relative stability and instability, the relative con-
sonance and dissonance of intervals. As a general rule, a consonant,chord is stable
one that contains only consonant intervals, while a dissonant chord contains one
or more dissonant intervals.
Ex. 14-1 shows four different chords, the last three of which are considerably
more dissonant than the first. Note that each of these last three contains at least
one unstable interval.
-mS-
i aita:
Consonant
n i^?3=
3:^5= ^ *Ll M2
-M3-
dissonant
-m^
^^ «5-
-i-]p4
dissonant
m^
V
[¥¥f-
fi-
dissonant
The more complicated the total interval structure within a chord, the greater
the chord's dissonance. Thus we could make a scaling of harmonic sonority that
would extend from the most consonant sound combination, the octave, to the most
dissonant, in which all twelve pitches of the chromatic scale might sound together
in a biting combination.
214
CHORD STRUCTURE 275
m
pt
b
^
^m
Not only is chord (b) more dissonant than chord (a), its many different pitches
(and thus many different intervals as well) also impart to it a high degree of density
(in this particular spacing) that a few-toned chord would not have.
Between these two sonorities of Ex. 14-2 lies the gamut of chord types that are
found in most music. The bulk of our musical heritage has used a restricted har-
monic palette that lies closer to the I sonority of Ex. 14-2a than to Ex. 14-2b, but
composers since the beginning of our century have considerably broadened the
possibilities toward the dissonant side of the scale. One of the recognizable trade-
marks for any composer is the chord combinations to be found in his music and the
way these sonorities are linked together into harmonic successions. Many of our
subsequent discussions in this book will be concerned with the recognition and
classification of chord types.
Any chord that can be reduced to no more nor less than three different notes is
called a triad. That is, even a chord made up of five different tones might still be
reduceable to a triadic basis if two of the five pitches are octave replicas of two
others.
t^oitttLcAid^ MuAjJxMj^ - tmWiwtXui tl X J
1 $
triadic
chord chord triadic
reduction reduction
m w
~tT"
ZC^
triadic
*Q_
321
33=
chord reduction
^ W3
chord triadic
reduction
276 CHORD STRUCTURE
Ex. 14-4. Siamese Orchestral Piece: Kham Horn (Sweet Words), Hisforical Anthology of
Music, Volume 1; Copyright by Harvard University Press. Reprinted by per-
mission.
±5z L
2
i» 3
m3
M3 4^ n 1=W5=
] m3
major triad minor triad
; ;
when the first third above the root is minor, a minor triad results.
Two other of the simpler triad types are known as diminished and augmented.
These designations are derived from the interval that encompasses their outer
tones, the diminished and the augmented fifths respectively.
5 m
^f^ 3
'
6
§ ^'^ll^Wi
-M-8-
ITl3
diminished augmented
triad (°5) triad (+5)
Both of these triads are relatively unstable, for they lack the intervallic simplicity
that creates a clear root effect. Neither contains a perfect fifth or fourth, and the
diminished triad contains the relatively unstable tritone. Since both the aug-
mented and diminished triads involve equal-sized intervals (minor thirds in the
diminished triad, major thirds in the augmented), they are slightly ambiguous and
lack the clarity of tonal focus that is required of a cadential consonance.
Major and minor triads are more stable, and so they are more important to our
present study. Furthermore, they will form the basis of many other chords that will
be discussed Note that major and minor
later. scales yield more major and minor
triads than diminished and augmented.
')
Ex. 14-7. -k^MUT
Major
scale
W
M
^ II,
M M
I III
,m
H i i
o
i ^ 3
1
major
diminished
; 3 minor
m o M m m M ^^
Nat 3 major ; 3 minor ;
m inor t'|
j t^f I >» '
scale
^ | 1 diminished
m o + m M M o
Hai 2 major 2 minor
^1 »i ^i lit ^^' ; ;
m m + M M o °
Melodi
iij \i
'^ 2 major ;2 minor
1,,
minor seal i Hi ii»
diminished 1 augmented
I",!, t 2
Each of the chords of Ex. 14-7 is a "diatonic" chord for the particular scale it
is built within. Thus the triad g-b-d is diatonic to D major, but it is non-diatonic
278 CHORD STRUCTURE
for (^-natural minor; and the triad B-flat-D-F is a diatonic chord of F major, non-
diatonic for C major.
Although the major, minor (and less so the diminished and augmented) triads
are the harmonic staples of music of the past eleven hundred years, any three dif-
ferent pitches must be regarded as potential triad material. The chord succession
in Ex. 14-8 shows six three-note chords, or triads, only the last of which is similar
to one of the four types we have been discussing. The remaining five triads are
relatively dissonant combinations which are essentially products of the linear
motion of the three lines.
^
f
^ ^ ^^ i
/?\
m i^
T^r-
^ r f^
m
In addition to sonority type — major, minor, diminished, or augmented, of the
classified forms — another vital bit of information about a chord concerns its rela-
tion to other chords in its proximity. That is, chords are like people in that they
"behave" or reflect their own identities only in relation to other chords, and these
familial relationships within a tonality are a necessary aspect of chordal descrip-
tion.
The individual chord's relation to the tonic pitch is denoted by the Roman
numeral that represents the scale position of the chord's root. If we build a triad
on each degree of the major or minor scale, we can number the resultant chords
according to their relationships to tonic within that scale.
a ^^ ffi
A
f Major:
U t 1)
A Minor: ;
U ^
m
"
IV
^^ VI vn
ffi
(i)
'
The two essential facts to be known about a particular triad within a tonality are
and
its sonority its tonal function. Therefore, it is important that a distinction be made
here between and lower case Roman numerals, for these, in conjunction
capital
with the sign for diminished and the + sign for augmented, are concise ways
°
Chord Succession.
Within a tonality, the most important chord is the tonic (I or i, depending upon
major or minor sonority), for it is the only chord that can render total stability
within a progression. Just as almost all melodies end with their tonic pitch, so almost
all multi-part music ends with a tonic chord; it is the sonority that can best produce
/L I* m ^' •
m C •
m r. •
U-- i i'
9
'1: (.
C •
1
d — c •
V I
220 CHORD STRUCTURE
the music of our Western tradition. This basic two-chord association has been
used to accompany every conceivable kind of melody, from the modest child's song
to art works of considerable complexity.
^
Ex. 14-1 la. Childrens Song: Hove you Ever Seen a Lassie.
^ ma ^ ^
^m u u u u u
F F
P^m #
m _ m
'
)--h 1 f r
"^ —f p
r r # V
•
J-'
— —
— — inr^-rf
r- —0 f~irf
F
ttII
T
w r 1 r 1
a
I I I
Any digression from the tonic chord is, in a restricted sense, a weakening of
tonality. However, the root of the dominant chord does not contradict the role of
the tonic pitch as tonic, for the root of their melodic relationship lends emphasis to
the tonic pitch. This can be illustrated if we note in Ex. 14-12 that the interval
formed between the two roots is a fifth or fourth, and the root of both of these
intervals is the same pitch, the root of the tonic chord.
P5
P4
«m
It
ffi^
T
V^ root root
CHORD STRUCTURE 221
The change when one chord gives way to another is called harmonic
that occurs
succession or harmonic progression. The motion of chordal change imparts another
element of psychological movement to musical tones; the change away from the
tonic chord establishes a certain degree of expectation and tension; the eventual
return to tonic promises fulfillment and relaxation of movement.
If the harmonic structure of a composition remained solidly imbedded in only
the tonic chord, no effect of harmonic tension could result, and the important feel-
ing of completion that results from departure and eventual return to a tonic would
be missing. The total effect would be dull and static if significant rhythmic or me-
lodic or textural contrasts did not compensate for this harmonic sameness. Viewed
as melodies, bugle calls suffer from this severe harmonic limitation, for they are
the mere spinning-out of a single chord pattern that is based on the instrument's
harmonic series.
£_---
A major: _.---.
music that serve distinctive functions within a musical or social setting. One way
of emphasizing the implied motion of one musical section is to precede it by a sec-
tion that lacks harmonic changes. The excerpt of Ex. 14-14, from a Toccata by
Monteverdi, illustrates the avoidance of forward propulsion that can result from
such a solidly grounded texture.
Clarino
quinto
^ i
m im
•/
iTTi]
Aire e basso
vulgano habso ^S ]
—
L^ J J i ii.
Sa
P^^^r r
rr
xn J: 331
XT
J i
h.J^ J J
^
^ ru^
Chorale Setting, AUe Menschen miissen sterben.
fTr?
I7\
^^^ D.
r
I
r
^
vi
r
[
I
r
r
rv
T
;:j
r
j
r r
j
m II
f
I
I
m
II V I
Voice
fjDMEJ-P' na-rrs- i
f ^piM^- M^r
Piano
^ * iw -d- * *^* *~^^ m^m
§53l|^ * .&
Chords t t w
V
CHORD STRUCTURE 223
indispensable for denoting sonority types and root relations within a tonality. Ex.
14-16 shows a simple analysis of a musical phrase.
mmm
II I II V VI (iv) wi"
Chord Position.
The structural names for the various parts of triadic chords are derived from the
intervals they contain. Thus the three parts are called root,^ third, and fifth. These
terms are meaningful as designations when derived from the simple or fundamental
position of the triad, all notes in their closest relations, as illustrated in Ex. 14-17.
' With the exception of rootless chords which will be discussed on pages 226.
224 CHORD STRUCTURE
-p^
m ^ t
:^Eti:
^ ^m
fundamental fundamental fundamental
position position position
Even when the position of a chord changes, the individual parts are still named
according to their unexpanded relations in the fundamental triadic representation.
Each of the chords in Ex. 14-18 consists of the same root [E-flat), the same third
(G) and the same fifth {B-fiat), even though vastly different chordal textures are
present.
Ex. 14-18.
1
m ^
T
•>•
^
''F-i l
W.
'?-i
T
^
Even when more than three pitches make up the chord, some are duplications
if
of triad notes, then the basic structure is still xhc fundamental triad. Thus the chord
type remains the same although voicing and texture change.
Ex. 14-19.
21
^ ii=
w
^
y ^^
^
w
;
9-S-
Chord Inversion.
The simpler chord types (major and minor triads and limited derivatives from
these)can be turned upside down and internally reordered without appreciably
changing their basic sonority. This is corroborated by the fact that these topsy-
CHORD STRUCTURE 225
turvy arrangements do not alter root effects in any appreciable way. We can see
that any redistribution of the notes of a major triad not only yields decorative con-
sonances, but also the same harmonic root is retained.
^ a t
I
root
For the reasons illustrated above, any distribution of tones is known by the
name of its fundamental (simplest) reduced form. Thus both chords in Ex. 14-21
are F major triads, even though/is not at the bass position in either arrangement.
a b
1^^ ^
s s
* m
root first second
position inversion inversion
Another explanation for the three possible triad positions would recognize the
root as lowest member in a root position chord; the third as lowest member in a first
inversion; and the fifth as lowest member in second inversion.
tion of chord members. This is done by appending Arabic numerals to the Roman
designation of root tone; these numerals denote interval distances above the bass
note. A complete representation would be as follows:
?
1(1) IV| V 6
m)
It important to note that these numerical designations are derived from the
is inter-
vals above the bass as if the chord were all contained within a single octave.
$ C:
3Q
But :
V(|,
la lb 2a 2b 3a 3b
i a *
(°5)
3
^ This method of symbolization breaks down with some more complex chords which are not dis-
cussed until Book II, Chapter 5.
^ In subsequent discussions, we shall adopt the term prime for the fundamental pitch of chords
which do not possess simple roots.
CHORD STRUCTURE 227
We shall study other principles of chord structure later when our harmonic
material includes more than the simple triad. But for the present, the principle of
inversion is helpful in that it simplifies the problem of chord classification according
to structure.
To
determine the structural type of any chord, one must first locate the root of
the combined intervals. With major and minor chords this process can be simplified
by a search for perfect fifths or fourths or, when these more powerful intervals are
missing, thirds or sixths; the root pitch of either of these combinations will be the
root for the whole chord. Lacking any of the above evidence (as in diminished and
augmented triads when two equally plausible choices of thirds or sixths are availa-
ble), the principle of superposed thirds produces a workable answer. The process
is illustrated below.
i=: 1^3^*:
•\
root
* »T=-
P4
root #
P5 m3
xn
¥
^root
root
g^ .£>_ -&f-
S S ^i
?&
,^
S ^Me^ prime
When a three-pitch chord does not contain a Jifth or fourth, the next simplest
interval, the major or minor third (or sixth) determines the root. Many three-voice
textures contain these non-triadic structures because individual voice motion rules
out the simultaneous occurrence of three different chord members. Notice that
Ex. 14-25 ends with a two-note sonority (or diad) because the composer demanded
certain resolutions of the melodic tendencies already established.
^
Ex. 14-25. Lutheran Chorale: Wer weiss, wie nohe mir.
^ m
=P^^^ i
r rn^ rr ^ ^m rf f rrr T
^^f
Chord analysis
^ ^
r
^P
i
^^f^ 3
* l''' tjl
'*'
l tj l" J f'll^' ii j'i'
\ * i i^#
m*
Tonality: g i V I V \i ir V (V) V m iv i \i i V i
228 CHORD STRUCTURE
Melodic-Harmonic Synthesis
Few compositions of any scope reveal the simplicity of chord structure and voice
movement found in Ex. 14-25. In much music our cognizance of chords is so related
to and affected by melodic elements that melody and chords form an inseparable
synthesis of tonal pattern, each reflecting the other, each delineating and clari-
fying the other. In other words, chords and melodies do not operate in music as
mutually exclusive properties. Because examples such as the following
this is true,
which exemplifies a remarkable synthesis of harmonic and melodic elements,
cannot be explained in purely harmonic or melodic terms alone.
( mod. fast)
{ V7) prolonged
J J-
J
At first glance, and in consideration of the allegro tempo, one is taken by the
apparent complexity of this example. If we attempt to isolate chords by simply
noting the sonorities formed by the tones heard simultaneously on each beat, we
shall have acknowledge some chord types (as on the first beats of measures
to
2, 3 and 4) that have little bearing on the style and idiom of this composition
and its composer. Secondly, by trying to reconcile both the b-naturals and b-flals
sounded simultaneously in measures 1 3 and 4 to a single chord we produce an
,
H>'.'
CHORD STRUCTURE 229
stated on the first beat of measure 1 and reiterated in measure 5. But it is through
essentially melodic means that the opening chord is projected and prolonged in
such a way as to take on meaning in the structure of the piece and importance as
a simple point of departure for musical development.
The top voice unfolds the upper three notes of the chord {e, g and b-Jlat) while
spanning two octaves. Rhythmic interest is created by syncopating the first three
articulations of ^, g, and b-JIat. The tenor part (viola) is rhythmically coupled with
the top voice and melodically reiterates the three lower notes of the chord in pairs
{c, e, and g). The technique used in these two voices is, of course, arpeggiation of
quite different, more explicitly melodic. Both voices establish a give-and-take as-
sociation and both span a C — C octave, filled in with a series of pitches which, in
effect, pass from one C to another. The b-naturals are simply part of the passing
groups that embellish Cs, and can be regarded as brief leading tones, which, in,
view of their duration, are hardly heard as conflicting with the basic b-fiat's sound-
ing above. Furthermore, C is the root of the chord prolonged throughout the exam-
ple, and one can see that it is partly due to the continuing reemphasis of c in the
two voices under consideration that the harmonic function (V7) in F and the
structural weight of C is made most clear.
Chord in this example can be described as a vertical group of notes that frames
and channels the passage; it is through essentially melodic means, however, that
the chord is prolonged.
The excerpt shown in Ex. 14-27 appears to involve a change of harmony on each
successive eighth note.
(fast)
sage, its basic pitches the sonorities created by the moving voices, grouped around
;
the repeated b^ in the alto, contribute to the momentum and expectancy of the
dominant effect without replacing it in any way. The passage is a contrapuntal
—
elaboration of dominant tonic movement, prolonged by stepwise (passing) move-
ment and a repeated (upper pedal) dominant pitch. The "chords" which occur
as each voice carries out a stepwise elaboration of the dominant pattern should be
viewed as chords of melodic importance, passing chords, and are neither heard nor
generally viewed as a significant part of the basic harmonic structure. The mean-
inglessness of a detailed description of each of the "chords" that occur in the exam-
ple, as shown on the music, can be best understood by listening to the passage.
Contrapuntal elements are not always as significant or prominent as in the two
examples just cited. However, any analysis of chord, or music in general, must
acknowledge the coordinate relationship between melody and harmony and the
way in which they interact in making musical structure more comprehensible.
Exercises
See Chapter 14 o( Materials and Structure of Music I, Workbook for more detailed exercises.
1. Practice spelling major, minor, diminished, and augmented triads from a given note as
root or prime. Think through the component intervals of each chord before spelling with
note names. For example:
B-Jlat augmented triad:
Augmented triad = augmented fifth
major third
major third
' —F-sharp augmented
B-fiat fifth
—D major third
B-flat
{D —F sharp major third
Spelling: B-flat— D—F-sharp
2. Analyze a passage chosen by the instructor, denoting each chord by its proper symbol.
Sample
Schumann:
M
T r
G major:
£
V
^e4
VI
^I IV6 I| V
3J=
:
3. In class or informally with a friend, listen to major, minor, diminished, and augmented
triads played on the piano or organ and identify as to sonority.
a. Identify which member of the major or minor triad is in the top of the chord.
b. Identify which member of the major or minor triad is in the bass of the chord.
4. Using a three-voice texture, write a passage that is based on the following chord pro-
framework first, add the middle voice, then elaborate each
gression. Write the outer basic
basic melody to create an effective but simple contrapuntal texture.
Gmajor: I / !« /V/vi / Vg /I / V / I //
5. Plot a simple chord progression of about four measures length, one chord per measure.
Using an instrument (or voice), improvise a simple melody that corresponds to the selected
chords. Use a simple repeated rhythm for all pitch patterns.
Example
Progression VI V h II
Rhythm: J. J>J
''i
I
J. ^i
I vi
J.
J'Jij. j,jU
^. ^^
u^
VI
:
Conclusion
f
15
Three-Voice
Combinations
Spacing.
m uai m: i. jto °
The similarity between the natural interv^allic arrangement of the series, (widest
intervals at the bottom), and the most common arrangement of vocal parts, (greater
distances betvveen lower voices), is evidence of still another relationship between
musical practice and the inherent properties of musical sound.
wm
^5?
'. '4 'I
7"
az
J
j^
t ^ f i t
^ ^ ^ 4 #
i
1Z2
CONCLUSION OF THREE-VOICE COMBINATIONS 233
mm
^^
"V
^m ^^ W
rr
-.a
i=i ^-p
^
i
[£[f
"
r p
r "
i
r
-oe-
In the madrigal shown in Ex. 15-1 we find a typical distribution of parts. The
upper voices lie generally within an octave of each other, while the two lower
parts are occasionally as far apart as two octaves. The upper parts move as close
together as a unison, and while not illustrated in this example, the same relation-
ship also occurs between the lower voices, particularly when they are coupled.
While subject to individual vocal ranges and melodic contour, the outer voices
seldom ^re farthex .a part than two octaves and a fifth, as mentioned earlier in
Chapter 13. Two octaves is a much more common limit. An interesting exception
to this rule of thumb can be seen in measure 2. Here the soprano and alto move in
contrary motion to the interval of a minor tenth. This exception to the rule is the
logical result of a primarily contrapuntal association involving contrary motion.
Most three-voice works reveal considerable variety of spacing procedures. This
can be seen by comparing the first and second phrases (measures 1-2 and 5-7) of
the Marenzio example. Throughout the first phrase the voices are deployed so
that different intervals occur between voices on nearly every beat. The second
phrase begins with parallel movement in all voices, thus establishing a contrasting
textural relationship between two successive phrases. The rhythmic association
between parts, however, is basically the same in both phrases, since identical dura-
tions generally occur motion in triads, as in
in all parts simultaneously. Parallel
Ex. 15-2. Three-voice distribution of a C major triad (Root position and first inversion).
A
Soprano \0 M , • ^ • # ~m~
Alto
Tenor S
— —
B
_• *_
Soprano
Alto < f
Bass W^
Alto U— — —— — — —
4)
•J
. . : i i
"
:
:
:
;
1
• 1
'
1
———
'i
' ', ',
^
Tenor
Bass
^. \ \
— —
• '
\
—.—
-f *
!
I
'
:
i
D
Soprano '^^
* *
Tenor
Bass ^^
Exceptional spacings sometimes occur as the result of decorative pitches. These
unusual separations occur most often in weak metric positions, and they do not
attract the same attention or create the sense of imbalance that would result from
their use in accented positions or longer durations. Several examples of more ex-
ceptional spacings which result from melodic decorative activity are in Ex. 15-3.
3C\ S. A.
Soprano
Alto
Tenor
^m
The principles of spacing three voices should not be taken as absolute rules, but
rather as guides to the intervallic distribution of melodic movement between parts.
These principles are useful in achieving a clarity and balance of pitch relationships.
CONCLUSION OF THREE-VOICE COMBINATIONS 235
1. The upper voices usually lie an octave or less from each other.
2. The lower voices may move as far apart as required by melodic considerations.
3. As in the overtone series, wider intervals are more often found between lower
parts; smaller intervals separate the upper voices.
Crossing Voices.
positions of tv^^o parts. If the alto moves above the soprano or below the tenor, for
example, the voices involved in the high-low exchange are said to have crossed.
Crossing most often occurs between adjacent parts, and it is usually the result of
the fulfillment of a particular melodic pattern.
Voice crossing most often occurs within a phrase, although cadences involving
crossed parts, or phrase beginnings using this procedure can be found. A typical
example of crossing is seen in Ex. 15-4.
^^
Ex. 15-4. William Byrd Madrigal.
m
:
^ i ^
$ rr
^m ^
i^
1 ^EEi m f^ t
^ ^Em" fcit*
i ^
^^ ^ £^ ^ ^ ^ ^ r
r f
^
A predominant upper part is accompanied by repeated pedal c's in the left hand
and a middle voice is developed entirely out of motion from e^ to its upper neighbor
/' and back to e'. The harmonic activity, featuring a harmonic rhythm of one chord
per measure, consists of three chords: tonic, subdominant and a tritone, all or-
ganized above a tonic pedal. The contrapuntal resolution of the tritone occurs in
measures 5 and 8, while each voice maintains at least some degree of rhythmic in-
dependence. It is, in fact, through the incorporation of decorative activity that the
work derives its minimum contrapuntal motion. Stripped of these decorative ele-
ments, the piece would appear as in Ex. 15-5b.
a^ ^ i i ^
T f f T r P f f
i i i ^ # B t i
The point is that even the simplest homophonic texture may reveal elements of
organization that reflect contrapuntal relationships, however mild and unpre-
possessing.
Contrapuntal writing dominates the texture of Ex. 15-6a.
^
Ex. 15-6a. J. G. Walther : Chorale Prelude for Organ.
1 2 3 4
^^ i 5
^ ^ ^
^ ^ w
i-
f
^
CONCLUSION OF THREE-VOICE COMBINATIONS 237
Ex.
12
15-6b. Harmonic reduction of Ex.
3
15-6a.
4 5
i
m
s
« J. n
^ -0—^ \-
11 12
i i
;«•)
y i W t*rr
%
^ X
^
J
^ r
238 CONCLUSION OF THREE-VOICE COMBINATIONS
s
Adagio
/
^ p
rlJlrJ- ^ n j-^;i
'
I
fDomi nant /T:s;
r
'r-i'.- T' p
P m
Where doubling occurs in a three-voice chord, the resulting sonority is usually
a decorative or cadential consonance, with one member of the interval duplicated.
Several such combinations are shown in Ex. 15-8.
$
331 :xsz
•-e-
331
s ^ ini Tl"
:fe
^
w ~n~ -nr
S 331 IT" 331 "TT~ ZCSl 331 331 331 331 331
Although either note of a consonant interval may be^ doubled, it is most fre-
quently the root. Doubling of the interval root usually occurs in cadential con-
sonances whose roots are members of the tonality frame, particularly tonic and
dominant. By doubling these pitches the composer emphasizes and helps to clarify
the prevailing tonality.
More freedom has been exercised in doubling members of decorative conso-
nances, whose roots, as we have described, are less evident. Either member of a
decorative consonance may be doubled the choice is usually determined by me-
;
lodic considerations. However, where major or minor sixths contain members of.tlie_^
tonality frame, particularly tonic or dominant, the pitch corresponding to tonic or
dominant will more often be doubled.
Common doubling procedures are illustrated in Ex. 15-9. The piece is in ^ minor,
CONCLUSION OF THKEEVOICE COMBINATIONS 239
and both of the endings opening section of the sonata (which is repeated)
for the
are achieved through transient-terminal cadences to the dominant. In both in-
stances the cadential pitch a, the dominant, has been tripled.
^ ^rr^m m ^ is m ^
^^1
r
^
7yF
^
s p ^ m ^^ p ^ ^
7
_
y y ff I
m ^ ^^ ^ i.
,Mt^^^¥-
^
fi
i
'n r ^r p
r yyl^ ^ ^^
^gVj^
P
s ±± fc ±±: I
^m ^ ^^ 5
*
J
m
f-^-^
^^
j)yr}
^m ^ ±3z
f
^^ S^
^^
XJ^
1.
>^HHh
m- m
J
J-
»•
y y
It important to point out that leading tones, pitches that form tritones, dec-
is
orative pitches, and altered tones generally are not doubled in three-voice tex-
tures. In other words, pitches which suggest continued movement and create
instability are not reinforced. When exceptions to this basic consideration occur,
they are usually located in weak metric positions, thus attracting less attention.
Two exceptional doublings, involving the interval of the fourth above the bass,
are seen in Ex. 15-10. Here the doubling of a fourth results from simultaneous use
of decorative tones which coincide on weak portions of the beat.
240 CONCLUSION OF THREE-VOICE COMBINATIONS
X I
^
-/ -
J rni J J J
"
r r ^ T r
S'r=f
pS i
Decorative Patterns.
^
^S ^ # «
#
f «
JJZ
t
^^
m
Ex.
m
15-12. Buxtehude: Organ Prelude.
w ^ m•r f
^Pi
^^
fe g^ 'g^ i
?: ^^>^;^^
CONCLUSION OF THREE-VOICE COMBINATIONS 241
Ex. 15-13. Hindemith: Ludus Tonalis, Fugue, No. 8. (C) 1943 by Schoft & Co., Ltd., London.
^ ^ *^ ^ ^ r— cfcj • i
F
^
^
J
J''i:,, ^^ '
SJ n.^
r r Cj>
*
r
H J
r
')^ > V
P U'm i
^^
In some instances, such as Ex. 15-14, decorative patterns may represent a color-
ful aspect of the organization of voices whose function is mainly accompanimental.
f ^ i J J ^^^ m ^^^
^
^^ ^m
^
J- ^Tj. i Jii J-
BP ? i
^
s rn f7] „i J
UJtS'-^M' i * rdA PL
"FCS^
, I
^ f^ # T ^^ r
Double passing tones in thirds occur on the second half of beat three in measure
1. At the same point, the bass, moving in contrary motion, ascends through a pas-
sing figure to a. Consecutive dissonances are heard on both parts of the beat, but
each dissonance is resolved by step to a member of the prevailing harmony, a d
minor triad. A similar treatment of passing tones is heard in measure 2, organized
around an a minor triad.
Syncopated upper neighbors, moving in parallel tenths, form an entirely con-
sonant embellishment of the prevailing d minor sonority in Ex. 15-16. The im-
portance of members of the tonality frame, here d^ and a, as doubled tones in
three-voice writing, is significant.
s rT^jTj m ^ ^m
^ CJUUS-U^ ^ ^
r
I
r r
Note that g^ and b-JIat are upper neighbors to the basic pitches/^ and a^, despite
their triadic agreement with d^, because of their short duration. The root progres-
Alto
m ? ^ ^ m
L^
Tenor
Bass
N
a i^
f^
J
^
U'
T
-J J
P I ^ i
^
r^ ^^
i- ^n n
^
1
^
i
%
*
^p f=T P f
An interesting analytical problem is encountered in Ex. 15-18:
Ex.
^
''
15-18. Neighbor chord.
> rjr
Ex. 15-19.
k;,. J.— 1
J ijJj m J
ft^
^
r p[j>rc£f-
^^ # ^^ #!•
d^'ii'
244 CONCLUSION OF THREE-VOICE COMBINATIONS
The role of decorative pitches in Ex. 15-19 can be studied by comparing Ex. 15-19
with its reduction (Ex. 15-20).
f^¥?
±^
f^=T -« :zt
^ jh isn
T^ f'
^
r
i
tfo-.
When all three voices move in parallel motion, contrapuntal interest is denied
in favor of harmonic color, and we tend to hear as basic those sonorities that occur
in strong metric positions. See Ex. 15-21.
Two factors are generally present in sonorities whose roles are primarily decora-
tive: melodic motion by step^ and short —weak duration and metric placement.
These "chords" may be viewed as one views pitches of a single strand of melody
which, although not part of the basic melody, fill in and connect more structural
points in the line. Their role in the construction of a "finished product" is nonethe-
less essential.
Ex. 15-22.
The excerpt in Ex. 15-23 contains several suspensions. Perform the example,
paying particular attention to the way the music is impelled forward by the suspen-
sions, whose resolutions coincide with the intioduction of a new voice.
12
J. C.
3 4
')% '^
lOI
^
^_l J J
icc:
17 18 19 20 21 22 23
^^
,
r r n
fe
ZUE -0-
r
-o-
_J:
O
±j-K
xn
At measure 1 1 a 4-3 suspension occurs in the middle part. The top voice is con-
sonant with the bass. However, the instability created at this point (in measure 1
1
is not solely a product of the perfect fourth between the bass and middle voice.
On the contrary, it results from the 2-3 suspension created between the middle and
upper voices. Although it is common to identify this typical configuration of three
parts as a 4-3 suspension, it is equally important to be aware of the double nature,
so to speak, of this and other suspension figures. The voice above the bass that does
not participate in the suspension figure usually forms a consonance with the bass
(or lowest sounding part). This aspect of the suspension in three voices is clearly
illustrated in measures 9, 11, 12, 15, 19, 20 and 21 of Ex. 15-23.
We noted in our study of two-voice combinations that the lower voice sometimes
changes pitch while an upper voice suspension resolves. The "change of bass" is a
common feature of suspensions in three-voice textures too.
246 CONCLUSION Of THREE-VOICE COMBINATIONS
( ^M
1
i I 7p
nr
m^
^ 2M ^^U
^
^
i
=nz;
^ J
—
8
J J J
I
~rn]i-i J I
11
The reduction in Ex. 15-24b reveals the importance of suspensions in the excerpt.
Ex.
12^
15-24b. Reduction of Ex. 15-24a.
')7u
T!^^=t
a
^ ^^i r
rrr
^
^ fiU
rt=f P
^
^E^
f
P
7-3
11
j—-!^
^m
f f
^-v ^
rf P ^
j-fj J H-J
P
J
^ J
^^ #
§sfit
^ P
p^ ^#^
A major.
T
m r^^
^
^ fl
I I
rzs^ E E^r
ni
l£ ^^'^ m
248 CONCLUSION OF THREE-VOICE COMBINATIONS
The escape tone, the leaning tone, and the anticipation are less frequent forms
of melodic embellishment in three-voice compositions. The techniques associated
with their use do not differ significantly in three-voice writing from their treatment
in two voices. The same dissonant to consonant intervallic movement formed by
CONCLUSION OF THREE-VOICE COMBINATIONS 249
the suspension, i.e., 9-8, 7-6, 6-5, 4-3, is created by leaning tones, although they
occur more rarely than suspensions. Accented unprepared dissonances are un-
common until the nineteenth and twentieth centuries, although they can be located
in scattered instances before the nineteenth century. Escape tones, anticipations,
and leaning tones are illustrated in a variety of musical styles in Ex. 15-28. A further
similarity between two and three-voice procedures lies in the fact that examples
of these forms of decoration occur most often in the top voice.
^
Ex. 15-28a. Binchois : Rondeau, De plus en plui.
^
e.t. ant. e.t.
?
X
5
^m X
JM
X
5 5
^m i
J.
^^
i-
LM^p
Ex. 15-28b. Bach: Sinfonia in E-flaf.
»'•'-
?hS-ffl
Ln.t
X
m
in.'i\*r '^; ^
unaccented
leaning tone susp.
X 7-6 « X ace. p.t.
f'^'M-^J j *^
n. tones
m
*\
-e-
tonic pedal
250 CONCLUSION OF THREE-VOICE COMBINATIONS
d p.t.
X
S » ^
ga m
Ln.t
Ln.t •,
X
X
iE^ ^
^
i
n^ r r
f^ ^
^ ^ r »f
*^=F^
The tone of resolution is usually not sounded along with the leaning tone, there-
fore, the sequence in Ex. 15-29 will almost never be found.
Ex. 15-29.
#%^-i^ 'S
Exercises.
See Chapter 15 of Materials and Structure of Music I, Workbook, for more detailed exercises.
1. Write ten different spacings of the D major triad for each of three different combinations
of three voices, such as soprano, alto and tenor.
2. Use examples in this chapter for sight reading vocally and at the piano.
3. Analyze all decorative patterns in Ex. 15-6a.
4. Transpose the tenor (middle) part of Ex. 15-6a up a whole step; then add an upper and
lower part in the style of Ex. 15-6, using appropriate decorative patterns. Analyze your
work, accounting for chord roots and decorative pitches.
5. Use Ex. 15-28 for aural recognition of decorative pitches.
6. Add two voices above the bass line in Ex. 15-19. Include examples of every type of sus-
pension in the setting. Transpose the bass down a major second before beginning the
setting.
The following example homophonic there is one dominating melody and the
is ;
3 I 3
252 HOMOPHONIC TEXTURES; NON-CHORD TONES
Even when one voice is of decided prominence, the other lines certainly may be
"melodic," in the sense that they contain some elements of rhythmic or contoural
independence. In textures such as the following the overall effect is not exclusively
that of counterpoint or of pure homophony; the result is rather that of a succession
of block harmonies interwoven with some isolated rhythmic activity and lines of
simple melodic distinctiveness.
^^ m -Q- 331
nrr
m
m ^^ 8 fc
3X
* o
jOlL
jOO
^ ^r:
nn 331
S 33: 32:
33:
33:
Example 1 6-3 shows still another kind of homophonic texture, this one achieved
through only two which are rhythmically differentiated. The lower part
lines
outlines simple chords, while the melody unfolds with simple decorative patterns
(upper neighbors) around a basic pitch line that belongs to the chords of the lower
part.^
I P
m
i ^
i ^¥
^ The accompanimental figuration of this passage is typical of classic-period works, both as piano
and string patterns. It is called an Alberli bass, after the composer Domenico Alberti, who apparently
pioneered its use.
HOMOPHONIC TEXTURES; NON-CHORD TONES 253
motior
Textural Considerations
Soprano
s ^E^y TTrr
Alto
P^ f
Tenor
Bass
^-^
P^ ^^^ ^ ^^^ i
Observe that the stems of individual parts indicate which voice the particular note
belongs to. Even when parts cross, this expedient shows the proper orientation of
the lines.
^ ii ^ ^ i
1^
r r T
^ doubled
root
^
tripled
root
*
doubled
root
T-
^
doubled
fifth
doubled
root
4
doubled
third
Doubling the root of a major or minor triad emphasizes that chord's stability.
In the case of tonic, subdominant, and dominant triads, this doubling procedure
also reinforces an important scale degree, 1, 4, or 5. Since these tones remain con-
stant (do not vary with change of mode), they act as the fixed tonal elements of any
key. As a consequence, they are often doubled within a four-pitch chord rather than
some other tone.
As can be deduced from the chart above, certain duplications are generally
avoided. For example, we expect the leading tone of a key (when that key has been
established) to resolve by step upward. To double this tone in a chord would exag-
gerate this expectation. And in chords containing a tritone (the diatonic chords
ii° in minor, vii° in major and in melodic and harmonic minor) the member
that is not a part of the tritone, the third, is usually doubled.
\
HOMOPHONIC TEXTURES; NON-CHORD TONES 2J5
M i J
^
a f
i erd-
r
3rd 3rd
!
s fe
^ i •
"ii°6 D: ii»6
VII a: ii
The same voice distributions apply to four-voice combinations that were dis-
cussed in relation to three voices in Chapters 13 and 14. However, the addition of
another part makes possible a greater variety of voices, as well as a fuller texture.
As we noted wider intervals appear more frequently at the bottom of a
earlier,
texture than between the upper voices, except when lower voice couplings are
used. Intervals greater than an octave are found often between the bass and tenor;
they do not appear as frequently between adjacent upper voices, alto soprano, —
tenor — alto.
Two basic types of spacing traditionally have been applied to the dispositions of
256 HOMOPHON/C TEXTURES, NON-CHORD TONES
four voices: the term "close" applies to any arrangement in which the three upper
members of a chord are in their closest possible positions. In such arrangements the
soprano and tenor voices normally lie no more than an octave apart.
The term open is used to denote distributions in which the three upper parts are
not arranged in their closest possible relations. In such arrangements the soprano
and tenor notes will usually (though not always) lie more than an octave apart.
u
ffef^ H^ If:
rr^
'y-hk r. iP '^
"trrr
r
The designations of^ close and open are helpful, but they are such general descrip-
tions that many kinds of spacing are not adequately identifiable by them. We can
add the term homogeneous to apply to any distribution, close or open, that makes
use of approximately equal intervals between the adjacent parts of a texture.
vrj»i^>>:
HOMOPHONIC TEXTURES; NON-CHORD TONES 257
J3
A ^
10
59:
i
r 11
r
J i
¥ i 3=51
$
33:
f
r
tive merits of the six settings, for the value of any one type could be decided only
in terms of its fulfillment of a particular musical need. Examples (a) and (b) are
similar to choral textures used for this kind of communal song however,
; the re-
maining settings might be more appropriate for other uses. Each version should
be played or sung several times, followed by a detailed discussion of the various
factors that contribute to its general musical effect.
rrj T
^ f f
258 HOMOPHONIC TEXTURES; NON-CHORD TONES
'^m d s
^
/|i
T /i' ii' i' i' ii' $ f
^ If
i id.
i M i
r
(d)
^
Heterogeneous coupling; soprano; alto-tenor, bass
^ i^U
P^
i^ ^ i
r
^^ J A J.
pr ^ r
J:
r
J.
r »pr
;)J
V r r
1
^"
(e)
tm^
rrr
Homogeneous; emphasis on low
rrr ^^^
5
r
^^ ^ ^1
•\
-l£ ^ L
r r
'
r I? $ ^ #
i
r^ r=r^
^^
<llll (f ) Open, homogeneous: emphasis on breadth of sonority, fullness of timbre
f
^ ^J^U
rr
f=7f
i i J 1. J)A
HOMOPHONIC TEXTURES; NON-CHORD TONES 259
Composers control the spacing properties of their music with the same care
lavished on chord structure, melodic pattern, and rhythmic motion; the appro-
priate chord could be inappropriate if its spacing were not matched with the desired
effect. In this respect, spacing, individual voice range, and dynamics are insepara-
ble factors. Example 16-13 shows a passage in which all three of these factors are
combined in a complementary union that creates a sense of placid solemnity. Note
that all four voices lie within the lower reaches of their respective ranges, that the
spacing is generally homogeneous, and that the dynamic level is piano.
Soprano
P
pax
-0
ho -
—a
mi -
^
ni -
-^
bus, ho - mi ni
m
-
I m
bus
m bo
P
Alto i
^ ^5^ * «•
pax ho - mi ni - bus, ho - mi ni - bus bo -nae.
Tenor
P
^ ? i ^^
pa X ho - mi - ni - bus , ho - mi - n I
- bus b(5 -nae,
u
^
t^'tt \
>
B ass //•fttt
I'll X
•
It r^ *
<u .
r^ •
« i r. i 1 i II
The excerpt in Ex. 16-14 illustrates the way a single musical phrase might shift
quickly from one kind of distribution to another. Here the upper and the lower
pairs of voices are coupled ; after a beginning that is open, the couplings move to-
Reprinted by permission.
Soprano
Alto
iJ 'i'
i' 'i' i'
Tenor
Bass
\y- i
I
'f
r p i
toll them out like bells
260 HOMOPHONIC TEXTURES; NON-CHORD TONES
Harmonic Succession
Allegretto
In some works this consistency of harmonic change joins with other elements
to create a tightly-knit structure. The compositions from which the passages in
Ex. 16-16 are extracted are almost totally organized upon the rate of harmonic
change established in their initial measures.
Moderate # = 112
Chord
changes: .
i
¥
i' 'rn'M gffl^E
W.
j^^ Ifeffl
?J).-^ !>-i ?Jr-J ?>-[ ^ir-i ?iri
S^ r " r ^^=^^
X 5W
^^
VI
j
•''
M.'' i
iffi- la/ffl
^^S
SbEgSa
^ 2i).-i
Si
zr
ik-1 lyi
g
iiw
J
?>J
i L
?i)nJ
i
Ex. 16-16b. Mendelssohn: Op. 102, No. 3.
Presto
^ r r r j
'
i-j)
-^
s
Chord
changes:
L. I I L.
feii
I 1
1^i
^^
^
^^ ^^ %X%
P I I
I I L I 1 I
m I
iJMi -^^^
I I
Nonetheless, strict uniformity is not always the case. In most works (or at least
sections) a generally uniform pattern of chord change is retained, but deviations
from this norm occur, in some cases to fit particular melodic patterns, in other cases
to intensify climactic sections through an acceleration of harmonic rhythm.
The passage in Ex. 16-17 is typical of simple and orderly phrases of the classical
period; it displays a deviation from an established norm, but this deviation itself
becomes another norm. Observe that the rate of change of the second phrase is
i
rJ ms
-\ r
t ^
T'T rn
f
^^ g:
^ ^
m "S t R
^4 &
T r
> rnm w^ ^
i T r f
^ri
^m f W*" ^
When the rate of harmonic change is rapid (relative to the rhythms ot'participat-
ing voices), a feeling of greater momentum is imparted to the whole texture. On
the other hand, a relatively plodding rate of change effects relaxation.
Although linear matters are not paramount in homophonic textures, the joining
of one chord with another is normally achieved by means of predictable motion
from one chord tone to the next. The individual chord members are links in separate
unfolding chains of lines, even if those lines are sometimes of negligible melodic
interest. In this sense, linear considerations are pertinent.
Except for unique instances, all lines of a four-voice texture do not move in
parallel motion, and it is obviously impossible for all voices in such a texture to
move in contrary or oblique motion in relation to one another. In the usual four-
voice homophonic texture a general balance of all of these possible relationships is
^m#4^
k=^
^
i
J
3
i^^
f
i
r
1
^^
I*
S^ I
r
i
r
g
Parallel octaves
f
Parallel fifths Contrary octaves
2. Fifths and octaves usually are not approached by similar motion in the same
voices, particularly in the outer voices. This occurs occasionally at cadences,
but even here one voice almost always moves by step^to the cadential sonority.
m
^^rf
hb^
¥
1 A
m m
B
gg f
J
i
T
t
a
?^
T r
Similar 8x'es, both voices Similar 8ves, upper voice Bk: ii« I
st xc ^TT~ ^
r T
^
or o5 P5 and
^- :
^^^ m
>-o
jOZ
a mmor ifoO
vu 5
f f
vii" 18
f
264 HOMOPHONIC TEXTURES; NON-CHORD TONES
Ex.
'$
16-22.
*
Schumann: Freue
r
^
dich,
f
O meine See/e (Album for the Young, Op.
r If
j-
f
J
68).
m^& P SI
4
O'
Reduction
^ A ii
m i ^
f 'o
6. When linear considerations do not preclude it, every vertical point contains a
full complement of chord tones, root (or prime), third, and fifth. When this pos-
sibility is overruled, a simpler sonority might well result diad, unison, or octave
:
doubling, etc. (The composer's desire for a particular sonority also can overrule
this norm of the complete chord.)
7. W'hen two successive chords contain one note that is common to both (or in
some instance more than one), this note is retained in the same voice part unless
melodic considerations demand another mode of linkage.
f
HOMOPHONIC TEXTURES; NON-CHORD TONES 265
*
^ i: > J .1
1
me^
Bb: V
f Bb: IV Bb: lii
^
(A)
(B)
Melody
Added
^
7SZ
^
basic pitches for bass voice
^m m
ftfp^ ~=^- p¥=^ -^ m- ^•
^y^ ^'
M
-J ^- 2 m
A
1 ~i~l Y-
m -• • —
s ^m^ •
-• -• -• -•
^
m ±^ -^ ii h^ i i
s W
Implied
chords:
I V Nli" I (16) lie (ii) lii vi^ iii
Wivsys I ii v i
i^ =f=^
•Q
^ ^ i^ •
^
?
^ ^
266 HOMOPHONIC TEXTURES; NON-CHORD TONES
Non-chord Tones
m
HOMOPHONIC TEXTURES ; NON-CHORD TONES 267
VI
Neighbor tones fulfill a similar role in relation to a chord they are non-chord tones
:
approached by step from a chord member which return to the same chord mem-
ber.
*
m i^ i
¥ ^ E 3
r
D:
J.
I5
J.
r
V7
I
^r J.
IV
step from a chord member and is left by leap. As described first in Chapter 8, it
may be left by leap in either direction, but a change of direction occurs most fre-
quently. The leaning tone, on the other hand, is a non-chord tone approached by
leap from a chord tone, but left by step. Again the direction of resolution is vari-
able, but a change from the approach is most frequent.
te i
te
r ^ T ^
IV6
^ C : ill ii» 16
.
^
Ex. 16-28. Anticipation.
^
m r
A i
r
S
E minor :
i
V
1.
C
m^
55?^
major: V
13
The following excerpts should be studied in terms of their use of the non-chord
tones discussed thus far.
Ex. 16-29. Mozart: Marriage of Figaro, " Deh vieni, non tardar."
c
JMip I
f
f J p I J J^r^ l J J)^ y l
fi J)J^'f
|
^
^
Deh, vieni,
E
non tar
^
- dar
t
^i
I
-^
^ rr
^
s
Ex.
J.
I.
1'
HOMOPHONIC TEXTURES; NON-CHORD TONES 269
^ 35:
Dissonances often are formed between the suspended note and members of the
chord other than the bass. In measure three of the above example a 7-6 suspen-
sion occurs between the highest note in the piano part and a lower voice, in addi-
tion to the 9-8 suspension formed with the bass.
270 HOMOPHONIC TEXTURES, NON-CHORD TONES
Suspensions formed by the lowest voice are measured as before: from the sus-
pended note and its resolution to the upper member of the chord that forms the
characteristic dissonance. This type of suspension, illustrated in Ex. 16-33, is one
of the most frequent. A complete description of the intervals present within this
suspension would be 2\- For simplicity we can reduce this figuration to 2-3
alone.
^^ r r
J J i
r r
^^
t^^^E^
J. i)frn J
^^^^
,
I
3x:
Pedal Point
however, that its most frequent use is in the bass, for the "pedal" actuates the bass
notes of the organ.
An example of this technique is the Perotin excerpt shown below. The
early
lowest voice merely sustains a single pitch, above which the other two voices move
in a strictly measured rhythm.
^^ ^j:
^^ ^j iJ : ^J JJ ^-.
f I'l/
i
;i
^ ii-
m
HOMOPHONIC TEXTURES; NON-CHORD TONES 271
The tone that forms the pedal may or may not be a member of the successive
chords that occur above it. In the following it is the tonic; as such it is a member
of both the tonic and subdominant triads, but it is foreign to the dominant (as in
measure 4).
^
Ex. 16-35. Haydn: Quartet Op. 64, No. 2, IV.
m w ^m
m
g ^
J)
A i
sr r r
Pedal tones are not always sustained. Rearticulated pitches produce the same
harmonic effect in a slightly "busier" way, as in Ex. 16-36.
^^
Ex. 16-36. Haydn: Quartet Op. 50, No. 6, I
S 122
^ fe
7
#
m ^ ^
^m «: ^ 3JZ
The and dominant pitches serve the pedal function most frequently, par-
tonic
ticularly in music of the eighteenth and nineteenth centuries. Whatever pitch is
used, the pedal tone becomes a kind of static pillar of reference, the associated
chordal weavings acting as decorative patterns which prolong motion but do not
effect real changes in the harmonic structure of the passage.
The tonic pedal often appears near the beginning of a movement (as in the pre-
vious example by Haydn) or near the end. B^th are locations in which the com-
poser usually wishes to solidly establish the tonic. An examination of the Preludes
and Fugues of Bach's Well-tempered Clavier will disclose his fondness for the closing
tonic pedal. Used in this manner the pedal gives the impression that the harmonic
progress of the composition is closing, the moving voices are merely elaborating
previous material in a prolongation of the cadential pattern.
272 HOMOPHONIC TEXTURES; NON-CHORD TONES
On the other hand, the dominant pedal is used with telUng effect as a signal of
the return of the tonic key. For obvious reasons, this is often found in compositions
just prior to a closing section. The following passage contains a pedal on the dom-
inant.
^ I
sempre M.I
^ ff M
'
Gt.
^ it w^ £
^H J^
*ttJ* 2Z
More than one pitch may act as a pedal. Sometimes the tonic and dominant are
combined, as in the following example.
m
^^ ^^
^LLiir ^m ps
tS>-=-
it is clearly functioning as a member of the chord. One of the main tonal fascina-
tions of the pedal is the way in which quite foreign chords (in relation to the pedal
pitch) can be sounded with it and yet retain a sense of tonal stability. In this sense
chords which move over a pedal are decorative chords (passing chords, neighbor
chords, etc.), and as such they prolong the harmonic significance of the pedal
pitch itself.
Even more complicated uses of the same basic technique occur. Ex. 16-40 con-
tains a passage in which two violins arpeggiate a C major triad through three
measures, under which viola and cello move in parallel minor sevenths. Note again
that it is the unchanging triad arpeggiation, the pedal, that provides the stable
element; the moving sevenths form a decorative motion.
Ex. 16-40. Shosfakovifch : Quartet, Op. 49, By permission of the International Music
Company, New York.
Violins i.
S ^
^^ ^:
Viola ^
^¥^
Cello
w
Sinnultaneous Non-chord Tones
f 4 g»
T
i m
r
^ r
S p e
"Tj
m f
Other non-chord tones are frequently combined, particularly when they are of
of the same type —
double passing tones, double neighboring tones, etc. Example
16-42 contains a double suspension. This type of pattern is particularly common
when two lines are moving in parallel thirds or sixths.
27A HOMOPHONIC TEXTURES; NON-CHORD TONES
-7 ^^A?*.*^
^ m rrt
^ ^
J
^
1
^^ ^
A more complex combination occurs when an entire chord issuspended above
a foreign bass note. In Ex. 16-43 the first beat of the final measure contains a
diminished triad suspended over an arpeggiated tonic chord. The third tone (the
g'), unprepared in the previous chord, is added to the upper parts to form a more
complete sonority.
t^
^^ f
A non-chord figure containing a so-called free tone (which in terms of melodic
analysis would be merely a basic-associate) occurs when a pitch that is clearly
foreign to the prevailing chord is approached and resolved by skip. The cadence
shown example actually is similar to the bi-chordal effect illustrated
in the following
in Ex. 16-43; the lower line is merely an arpeggiated tonic triad, part of which
forms dissonant relations with the chord in the upper voices.
^
^W^ ^
In some musical passages it is not easy to make an absolute judgment about what
is "chordal" and what is "non-chordal." We can note such a passage from an
earlier example, shown again below for our present purposes.
— —
We might ask what is the chordal basis (or bases) for measure
f1
3. Is the progression
here VI —
with the c" of the top voice a leaning tone? Or is the whole measure
iv,
best described as founded in IV, the b flat' and a' functioning as successive passing
tones between the basic-associates c" and g'? The answer to this problem is best
sought through a basic-melody analysis, as well as attention to the total set of
melodic/harmonic patterns. The two lower parts, through skips, outline the IV
—
chord (C e-flat g). The boundary tones of the melody are easily accommodated
into this chordal unit, for they are the root and fifth (C G). As a consequence, the
entire measure can be described most accurately as a IV chord over which two
consecutive passing tones add melodic interest.
In some instances the total set of voices will not favor one interpretation of
harmonic structure over another. When this occurs, the simplest explanation should
be chosen if a choice must be made. The potentiality of a dual (or even triple) analysis
indicates the probability of an ambiguous passage, in terms of harmonic structure,
thus making a precise analytical distinction irrelevant anyway. The essential pur-
pose of analysis, after all, is to support and to intensify our understanding of musical
organization as an aural experience.
Exercises
For more detailed assignments see Materials and Structure of Music I, Workbook, Chapter 16.
In this chapter we
examine some of the relationships that are
shall
276
TONIC. DOMINANT. AND SUBDOMINANT CHORDS; DOMINANT SEVENTH CHORD 277
Ex.
i
Wr^
^ 17-1. Mozart: Trio
i
^
in E Major, K. 542,
* *s
I
I (adapted)
or
m i
i mw
S
-0-
V^ V7 IV
Compositions frequently begin with the tonic chord. This procedure, illustrated
in Ex. 17-2, establishes the tonic immediately. (At times a delay in the initial
appearance of the tonic may produce a desirable effect of expectation for the
listener.)
M
6, I.
1^ 33
^^
the bass changes. This can be seen in measures 1-2 of Ex. 17-3; here the bass out-
lines the tonic chord, with all chords except the second in root position. The Ig
chord is recognized because no change of root
heard between the first two is
chords, and the second chord contains a decorative consonance between the bass
and another part.
V f t f^-^
m
ijRf* -^-^ f=f ^ i
^ I
^ h-
16 V7
f ^
1 1 i
16 I\-
r
Because the tempo of the preceding example is Adagio, the motion I —^Ig is
easily perceived. Sometimes a rapid tempo makes it unlikely that the listener will
clearly perceive a change of position, as in Ex. 17-4. In this instance the motion to
E-flat in measure 2 adds to the illusion of motion created by the bass line.
^ u
w
^^-t^
rrrf
a
nK (i J
-6h
V
^^ S is i
The impression of prolonging the tonic chord i^ also present in Ex. 17-5. Here
the ig chord is associated with the beginning of step motion in the bass that leads
to the dominant note.
j^^
^
I
r
^
^
J
^1 ife=t
W'' Hi'' '.6
* IV
TONIC, DOMINANT. AND SUiDOMINANT CHORDS; DOMINANT SEVENTH CHORD 279
Although Ig (ig) often follows I (i), this does not indicate the use of Ig as a cadence
chord. As a fin al cadential chord the most stable form is the r oot position triad.
i r^^
h
1 fcn ?i^
" '" •
;
^L
X ^
-XI
-X IT 'M* T 1^4 -L
In major keys the dominant triad is, of course, major. In minor keys, however, '
Aatidhi /i]uiun
a wider range of possibilities exists. The diatonic dominant triad in minor is minor.
Although attractive and (to our ears) somewhat exotic when used in a cadence,
the minor dominant chord is not found as frequently in simple tonal music as a
major dominant. The latter requires a scale alteration the raised seventh degree. ,
Despite the required alteration, the leading tone should not be regarded as an
"altered" tone, since it is common property of the melodic and harmonic minor
scales. The effect of the minor dominant (v) is effectively demonstrated in Ex. 17-7.
Ex. 17-7. Vaughan Williams: Mass in G Minor, Kyrie. Reprinted by permission of the
copyright owner, G. Schirmer, Inc.
^^
i^
i
=? i 1
r f
i i
16 V
280 TONIC, DOMINANT. AND SUBDOMINANT CHORDS; DOMINANT SEVENTH CHORD
chiefly because of the tendencies present in the bass Hne in the form of the leading
tone. When the leading tone is exposed in one of the outer voices, it is usually
resolved up by a half step. If the leading tone appears in an inner part, however,
it is often resolved with greater freedom. This may be stated another way: harmonic
considerations often govern the movement of inner parts (doublings, spacings, etcTJ;
linear considerations most frequently govern the motion of the outer voices which_
constitute the basic framework of the texture. Since the leading tone is the bass
note of the Vg chord, the chord of resolution usually will have the tonic note in
the bass, a result of ascending half step motion.
Example Vg both preceded and followed by tonic. As is often
1 7-8 illustrates
the case, the first inversion of the dominant is associated with step motion in the
bass. This example also contains a shift of texture from the two-voice imitative
writing in the first two bars to the chordal texture of the last two measures. Despite
the thinness of the texture the chord root in measures and 2 is clearly e. 1
ms ^ ^^ i i
e=t fe^
rr
^^ g f-g-r
V
^i V6 i V
S^
^
^^ m
^ra
T^
tt
ititz
/*
y^i
^ p^^
ii
m
m
I V6 V I 16 I IV
^^ V|I
TON/C, DOMINANT. AND SUiDOMINANT CHORDS, DOMINANT SEVENTH CHORD 281
The subdominant triad, however, does not appear only as an approach chord
to the dominant: it also precedes the tonic directly at cadences or elsewhere in
a phrase.
A wider variability in the triad quality is one of the characteristics of the sub-
dominant. Ajt^hpugh tonic and dominant triads are relatively consistent as to their
quality in a major or minor context, the two forms of the subdominant (IV or iv)
appear music with considerable interchangeability. Conditions influencing this
in
usage undoubtedly include the melodic use of the raised sixth scale degree in minor
keys (necessitating a major triad when harmonized by the subdominant) and the
frequent nineteenth-century practice of lowering the sixth scale degree (by a
semitone) in major keys.
282 TONIC, DOMINANT. AND SUBDOMINANT CHORDS; DOMINANT SEVENTH CHORD
.h=^
m fi J' j^
" i# «i
^ ?
r
^
IV V7
S 211 :2i
E
s
-&- l^^^=d=4
i
')^<i I
J
f
5 ^ S
J J J i
331
^
IV (6) V7 V7 I
Allegro
fe^ fQ A nnip
-•
±E
r^ fc rrs
i^ f
J
i)
g^ i
C\
13^i
§ S P=»
^^ ?^^
I IV6V7IV6V6 I
A more complicated treatment of ivg is seen in Ex. 17-15, the complexity due in
part to the elaborate use of suspensions. The essential harmonic progression of this
excerpt, shown in Ex. 17-16, is merely a weak terminal cadence from ivg to i^.
This basic progression, however, is elaborated and prolonged for several measures
before it settles on the cadential i^ before the fermata.
TONIC. DOMINANT. AND SUBDOMINANT CHORDS; DOMINANT SEVENTH CHORD 283
M^\ <SPfl
r p-
>t-
j^f-r 'f i ff i
' 1^
^^ ^ o
n^ a! J r '^
?
ivo iv iv iv6 JV6
'S
^ 9^
^ 1*^
IV' i
'
6
4
If the bass pitch of the ivg does not move by step, it often moves to a root position
subdominant chord, as in Ex. 17-17.
i
V\, (f,
j) ^m ^ ^ ^^^ i #
^BS ^ # i XTQ- i
f
iv6
)
I.
IV
)
The resolution of IVg upwards toVg can be seen in Ex. 17-18. The ultimate
melodic goal of this bass line is the tonic, which in this case is somewhat delayed.
^
Sonata in A Major, Rondo.
^
a^f -0 0- 1
~ma-
IV' V^ 16
284 TONIC. DOMINANT. AND SUBDOMINANT CHORDS; DOMINANT SEVENTH CHORD
The harmonic structure of the last measure in Ex. 17-19 can be interpreted in
two different ways: it can be interpreted as a IVg, or the c" (the "root") can be
interpreted as a delayed accented passing tone (which makes e the root). Me-
lodically, the latter analysis seems more musical.
m r
i ^^h^ 1. I' r2 ^M=#
f
S f
4
r (IV6) VI
In Example 1 7-20 both ivg and IVg are juxtaposed in a texture with considerable
contrapuntal independence. (It should be remembered that on the organ pedal
stop would be employed which would double this line an octave below.)
Manual
^^
Pedal
'
^
Y^i* J . I
iV 6
s=^
P
jy6
^
V6
i^
£
Although most of the examples shown thus far in this chapter are excerpts from
approximately 1650-1900, we should not infer that the harmonic relations illus-
trated are an exclusive property of this period. Much contemporary music, as the
following example demonstrates, reveals similar harmonic organization.
t
Ex. 17-21. Copland: Sonata for Violin and Piano. (C) 1944 by Aaron Copland, copyright
owner, and Boosey & Hawkes Inc., Sole Licensees. Reprinted by permission.
$ P
jn
t -9 — ^^- M
s
1E3
^T-r 'SL
I ^i
IV V I IV V I
The following phrase, dating from the middle of the sixteenth century, is
clearly in d minor. The final cadence emphasizes D as the tonic, and the other
chords — all root position triads — belong to the diatonic materials of this key and,
in their total effect, affirm the tonality.
m ^
J
331
i: A
P¥
A J J
m
9tFpg i 19 1*
)V :i\
iy V "X ,1
sures the use of the minor dominant and the emphasis upon G and C suggest other
;
alternatives. It seems clear, though, that the cadence, viewed against all that pre-
cedes i-V rather than
^
it, is iv-I.
i -r
f fr ^fW-ff^ * tBT
55??
§ s 331 J^k^^sU. a
r^ f^
286 TONIC, DOMINANT. AND SUBDOMINANT CHORDS; DOMINANT SEVENTH CHORD
Together with major and minor triads, other more complex sonorities also have
the dominant seems apparent that if the triad may be described as
as root. It
consisting of the piling up of two thirds, the process may be carried one step further
to produce a four-tone sonority known as a "seventh chord". In this section we
shall concern ourselves with but one of these, the dominant seventh (V,), a "major-
minor" seventh chord type which we will abbreviate as Mm,.
The final measure of Ex. 1 7-24 contains a dominant seventh in block form. From
this arrangement we can readily see the reason for its name. It has as its basis the
— —
major triad on the dominant D F-sharp A to this is added the minor seventh C
;
above the root D. Hence, the term "major-minor" (Mm) seventh, the first factor
referring to the quality of the triad, the second to the type of seventh. When this
seventh chord is found on the dominant, we shall refer to it as V,.
•»•» #
i£ 4 i J j- ^ Ji
y<'^ 1
^^
«R »^ 1 e£
^E^ H
^ ^
The seventh of the Mm, in the previous example is introduced without melodic
preparation. Often, however, the seventh of the chord is melodically prepared.
In addition, the resolution of the seventh down by step is directly related to
the decorative (non-chord) character of the harmonic seventh.
Early occurrences of recognizable Mm, chords are found when the seventh
is introduced as a passing tone above a dominant root. The introduction of the
seventh with the dominant triad intensifies the motion to tonic because the
seventh forms a tritone with the third of the dominant chord (notated as a
diminished fifth in Ex. 17-25). It is the subsequent resolution of the tritone that
creates the effect of increased melodic activity.
^ f^
ifiv
f ^
i
^ p
TON/C, DOMINANT. AND SUBDOMINANT CHORDS; DOMINANT SEVENTH CHORD 287
In addition to passing tones, the seventh of the dominant seventh often results
from a suspension. Such an introduction of the seventh, by preparation in the same
voice, demonstrates the traditional treatment of the dissonant seventh: approached
by step motion or prepared in the same voice, and resolved by descending step.
^ Hoh
Ex. 17-26. Scheldt: Chorale Prelude, Allein Goft in der sei Ehr.
L
S
f r P w
^-
^^ ^^
i J ~rr
-&-
X .Ka
-1 J.^
In Ex. 17-27 the dominant seventh is introduced in such a way that the chord's
seventh must be regarded as a chord member rather than as a result of linear
activity. Its metric location, duration, and approach (by leap) confirm the fact
that the seventh is an integral member of the chord and not merely the result of
linear impulse.
'J i,'/i'ii"i'i' : ~a
^ A
m
J. J -.
t
jJ' ^^
i-~r |i
lodic treatment of members. Perhaps the most important result of this is the
its
tritone formed by its third and seventh, the leading tone and fourth scale degrees
respectively. When the tritone is notated as an augmented fourth, it usually re-
solves by expanding to a sixth; when the tritone is spelled as a diminished fifth, it
usually contracts to a third. This results in the upward
resolution of t he leading-
tone and t he downward resolution of the chord seventh, both by step.
288 rON/C, DOMINANT, AND SUBDOMINANT CHORDS; DOMINANT SEVENTH CHORD
o5 >• 3rd
6th
Bt: V7 Bk V7
resolve the leading tone downward to the fifth of the tonic chord in order to obtain
a complete triad on the resolution chord. This is seldom done, however, when the
leading tone is in an exposed melodic position.
o5
Harmonic Cadences.
Harmonic progression is but one of the means by which the composer can
create the feeling of conclusiveness that he desires at the cadence. We observed
earlier that a single melodic line can attain a convincing and satisfying cadence
through certain manipulations of pitch and duration, and these same factors
produce similar effects in two and three voices. We noted also that merely a cessa-
tion of motion can produce a cadential efTect.
In recognizing cadences in harmonic texture it is important to remember that
chordal succession alone cannot produce a cadence it must be coupled with other
;
2-1, 2-3, 5-8, etc.), and the bass line (including 7-8, 2-1, 5-8, etc.). The following
examples illustrate various approaches to the authentic cadence.
^
VI
^
Ex. 17-31. Mendelssohn: Symphony in A Minor (Scotch), I.
LJ t^ 1^.
f t^
P ,t: tf'
p I
Hfl^f ^^ is^ p V^
#—
V^ I
^ ip^p^ ^ m
Z ,
1 m m
^ p
It
^m
^
Ex. 17-33. Mozart: Requiem, Offertory, Domme ieiu Chrisfe.
^
i
-* * r
^ tit
"\
Do - mi - ne Je - su Chri
~J^
- ste
X
290 TONIC, DOMINANT. AND SUBDOMINANT CHORDS; DOMINANT SEVENTH CHORD
(^
1 Ex. 17-34. Buxtehude : Instrumental Sonata, Jesu, Meine Freude.
o
^ff^ yg=^i -iHi
f
^^ J.
f
J il
M.
fe n r^
Sein Herz.
^^ :xjl:
f7\
331
IV I
Occasionally the tonal center is not clear at the end of a composition as in Ex.
17-36. The final root movement suggests a plagal cadence, but there are indica-
tions that A is the tonic.
Ex. 17-36. Bach: Cantata No. 161, My inmost Heart doth Yearn.
i7\
',:-<''
TONIC. DOMINANT, AND SUBDOMINANT CHORDS; DOMINANT SEVENTH CHORD 291
Progressive cadences may also be classified more precisely into different types,
suggesting continued motion in a number of different ways. A frequent manifesta-
tion of this is which the final chord is the dominant. Since
the half cadence, in
composers have approached the dominant in any number of ways, there are count-
less varieties of half cadences. In Ex. 17-37 the cadence is little more than the
\%
^ mm^
^
» f tr
fejgp^
1
ij ^
p r
?
Other diatonic (or even chromatic) chords may precede the final dominant
chord, however, as in the following.
fcm
m^ $ f
f="
^^ ^ w ?
iV6
mV
The ivg commonly occurs in a particular species of half cadence^ that is known
as the "Phrygian cadence." Its name stems from the typical cadential melodic line
of 2 — 1 found in the Phrygian mode. The characteristic features of this cadence,
illustrated in Ex. 1 7-39, are first the chordal progression ivg (or iv) to V, and second
the contrary motion between the outer voices, expanding or contracting to the
octave of the dominant. This cadence is common in minor keys, less frequent in
major.
Phrygian cadences are often seen as interior cadences, less often as terminal ca-
dences. Occasionally, however, Baroque composers used Phrygian cadences at the
end of one of the inner movements of their sonatas, concertos, or suites, suggesting
a close harmonic connection with the following movement.
Exercises
For more detailed assignments see Materials and Structure of Music I, Workbook, Chapter 17.
1. Write an eight measure melody in G major which clearly outlines Tonic, Subdominant,
and Dominant chords. Use passing and neighbor tones to decorate basic pitches. Then
apply the same procedure to writing a melody in b minor.
2. Practice spelling I, IV, and V chords in a variety of major and minor keys.
3. Listen to a recording of Friihlingstraum, by Schubert, and identify the I, IV, and V^
chords used in the opening section of the song.
4. Write an eight measure piano piece in A major, using an essentially homophonic tex-
ture. Write in | meter, allegretto, creating two 4 measure phrases, both of which
illustrate the chord sequence I, IV, V, I. (Use V or V^ as desired).
5. Make several settings of a bass line consisting of 4-5-1 in different major and minor
keys. Write for 4 voices in chordal and contrapuntal textures.
6. Practice spelling Mm^ chords on different roots.
7. Write out V^ chords for four voices in at least five different spacings. Resolve each chord
to tonic.
8. Practice writing and identifying V-I and IV-I cadences in different textures.
of thi
78
Dominant Seventh;
Harmonic Rhythm
In this chapter we
examine the inversions of the V7 chord,
will
pointing out the characteristics of each and studying their approaches and resolu-
tions. Our concern will be almost entirely with melodic movement, since the use of
different inversions of this chord does not involve any changes in the typical root
progressions. The differences are those of bass motion, approach to and resolution
of the seventh of the chord, and the treatment of certain characteristic intervals in
the chord, especially the tritone.
As with triad inversions, we shall indicate these seventh-chord inversions by
means of figured bass symbols. Since we are dealing with a more complex sonority,
however, the figurations must be more complete. For the four positions of the dom-
inant seventh (and for all seventh chords in general), we will use the following
figured bass symbols.
Ex.
^ -e-
a
m xt;
331
-o-
331
ISH
V7 V6 V(6) . \J
'S 4
(I)
1.3 s
respectively.
293
294 INVERSIONS OF THE DOMINANT SEVENTH; HARMONIC RHYTHM
In the resolutions of these inversions, two melodic factors governing voice motion
are important. We discussed in earlier chapters the tendency of the members of
the tritone to resolve by step, and the intensification of this tendency when found
in an outer voice — soprano or bass. This same tendency is present in the dominant
seventh chord, in root position and inversions.
The seventh is a dissonance. It is dissonant not only with the root of the chord
but with the third as well, even though these relations are mild by contemporary
standards.
The seventh of the V7 is approached in a limited number of ways, and its resolu-
tion is virtually restricted (with a few exceptions noted later in this section) to a
downward step. In this descending resolution, the seventh possesses an active
tendency as strong as the leading tone.
The interval formed by these prominent "tendency tones" is significant in itself,
since it is the tritone —
the most unstable of intervals and the one that most urgently
suggests resolution. The harmonic context
typical resolution of this interval in a
may be observed in Ex. 18-3 in "x" and "y," the two voices moving in contrary
motion by step according to their tendencies. An exceptional resolution of the same
interval (in similar motion) occurs at "z."
P^ 3m
tr-
^
^ ^ ^^ ^
? "27 j|
J J^
^
v
Several patterns are possible in the approach to (or "preparation" of) the
seventh. In Ex. 18-2 one of these is illustrated — by step from below. In addition
to this, the seventh is often approached by step from above,
by leap (usually from
below), or it may appear as a suspension from the previous chord. These approaches
will be illustrated later in this section. Certain inversions are seen more frequently
with one or another particular approach; these will be noted as we proceed.
We will not dwell at length on the V7, since its use has been amply illustrated in
an earlier chapter. One characteristic, however, should be noted unlike its inver- :
sions, which are almost invariably found in complete form, the V, appears occa-
sionally with the fifth missing. In four- voice textures composers have doubled the
root in such cases. Authentic cadences in four voices that employ V7 fall into one
of the following categories If both the leading tone and seventh resolve according
:
±
a
"X"
^^
Incomplete "Y"
i
T
Incomplete
1
"Z" Both Complete
J I 4-
a ffi ^
f f
The Ve Chord
5
In its approach and resolution, the Ve is similar to V,,. Here, however, the resolu-
tion possibilities are more limited, since the leading tone (in the bass) and seventh
generally resolve by step according to their tendencies. Thus, the only frequent
chord of resolution is the tonic, as illustrated in Ex. 18-4. In this example we see
the characteristic tritone between the outer voices.
^ r—f ^
Ex. 18-4. Haydn: Quartet Op. 50, No. 6, I.
n» ^ .. J-
i -e-
T
m^ -&-
-e-
(» # i»
P
v|
^ f=T
Since this resolution is standard, most of the differences in the use of Ve appear
in the approach is often preceded by l\\, a progression suggested
to this chord. It
by the 6-7-8-scale line in the bass, as in Ex. 18-5. (This also allows the seventh
to be prepared in the same voice in the previous chord, thus introducing the dis-
sonance smoothly.)
( 7th prepared )
296 INVERSIONS OF THE DOMINANT SEVENTH; HARMONIC RHYTHM
P ^ra
f 3=« i
^
^^ ^
J
V8
J
I
J
Occasionally Ve does not move directly to the chord of resolution but suspends
one or more of its members as in Ex. 18-7. The chord change is still on the first
beat of the last measure, but the resolution of the individual voices is staggered
rather than simultaneous.
m r^^ f « t^ ^
i
rr^
^^ nrr ^^ ^ J
^
vS
==j
(i) i'
When the leading tone is approached by leap from above, as in Ex. 18-8, a par-
ticularly strong emphasis is placed upon its resolution. This type of approach is
Skip to l.t.
INVERSIONS OF THE DOMINANT SEVENTH; HARMONIC RHYTHM 297
An obvious exception to the upward resolution of the leading tone occurs when
Ve is followed by V7. In this case the bass note moves down to the dominant and
then resolves instead of resolving directly. This is not actually an irregular resolu-
tion, since there has been no change of harmony; the resolution is merely delayed.
^
Ex.
^m ^^^
18-9.
^m
Delayed resolution of Vb chord.
$ ^
i-
=i
j=^
^
V| (V?)
T
The V4 Chord
3
The \'^4 is perhaps the least frequent of the inversions of the dominant seventh.
We shall see, in later chapters, that the second inversion in general is the least
common position for both triads and seventh chords. One of the most typical uses
of V4 is as dt. passing chord between I and Ig (and vice versa).
I V|I6V4 I«
Ex.
'h
18-11.
^UJ
Schumann: Piano Concerto, Op.
F Ur pt.45
54,
i
^ d5 to p5
31 t"
In both Ex. 18-10 and Ex. 18-11 note that the common tone between V| and I
(the root of the V chord) is kept in the same inner voice; this feature is character-
istic of this progression, although
it is not always present.
The V^ Chord
In our discussion of V^, the third inversion of the dominant seventh, we will note
four types of preparation of the seventh. In resolving this chord the presence of the
seventh in the bass virtually dictates that the chord of resolution be Ig or another
sonority that allows for the step resolution of the seventh. In this way the bass line
carries out its strong melodic tendency. There are no other common treatments
of V| its resolution
; is extremely stereotyped. We may now turn our attention to
the manner in which the chord is approached.
Ex. 18-13 illustrates one of the most frequent seventh preparations: from below
by step. Inmelodic context the seventh appears to be treated like an upper
this
neighboring tone. For this reason, when the chord seventh is approached stepwise
from below and resolves to the same tone, it is said to be prepared by the "neigh-
boring tone figure." The melodic interest of the middle parts in this example is
particularly noteworthy.
INVERSIONS OF THE DOMINANT SEVENTH; HARMONIC RHYTHM 299
m ^^ ±=^
r tfr r r tfr r
^
s ^E^ ^^
.B^^ 4 aj=^
I« v| 16
W^ z:
^ ^ ? ^
1^^ mrm r=T^
V
mnv .^
rr"^
rnfm
r i
16
* 2
The fourth type of preparation is by step from above. Here the seventh acts as a
passing tone between V and Ig, as illustrated in Ex. 18-16. In this example a further
exception to the usual resolution procedure of the V7 occurs. The seventh is trans-
ferred to another voice (in this case the bass line) before resolving. Since no change
of chord occurs, the listener is not conscious of an unresolved dissonance.
s ^m
m^ ^
i-^-i u
f=
V7 ^ 2 18
^^ f Til t
32
ciP^ =^
'y-^\ (^ I i
I*
We —
have noted three ways other than downward by step in which the seventh —
of the V7 or one of its inversions resolves when it actually resolves upward as in
: ( 1 )
the resolution of V^ (see Ex. 11); (2) when the resolution is delayed by some type
of suspension pattern (see Ex. 18-7) and (3) when it is transferred to another voice
;
ornamental. The upper line of Ex. 18-18 leaps away from the seventh to a leaning
tone E-sharp on the first beat of the next measure before returning to the expected
resolution tone F-sharp. This procedure is similar to No. 3 in that it is merely
a decoration of the predictable resolution.
INVERSIONS OF THE DOMINANT SEVENTH; HARMONIC RHYTHM 301
V|
The diminished triad on the leading tone bears a marked resemblance to the
dominant seventh chord, since all of its members are also members ofV,. Walter
Piston and others have argued that it is actually a dominant chord, and that its
root is missing (this root would be the dominant). One cannot deny the similarities
between vii° and V,, but we must regard this triad as a separate chord in its own
right. As discussed in Chapter 14, we shall regard the lowest member of this chord
as its prime, for the sonority does not possess a root.
Like most diminished triads, vii° rar ely appears in any inversion other than
first. The bass thus is not a member of the prominent tritone that characterizes this
chord. A typical progression involving viig is shown in Ex. 18-19.
^^ J
y-¥^
M:tf i :
I —
-(•
m
T
(•-
i
f P -^
viie 18
302 INVERSIONS OF THE DOMINANT SEVENTH; HARMONIC RHYTHM
This chord is used frequently as a passing chord between I and Ig in Ex. 18-19.
The obvious between vii| and V^ account for their frequent inter-
similarities
changeability. Their sound is virtually identical, creating an aural problem in dis-
tinguishing the one from the other. In most contexts viig and V| are equally
appropriate, for they create the same aural expectations.
Ex. 18-20 and Ex. 18-21 illustrate customary doubling practices in the vii|.
These procedures, however, apply equally to any diminished triads, \ yhen either
r oot or third is in the soprano, composers have^preferred to double the thirdj^lhe
one member that is not a part of the tritone) . The fifth of the chord, ho wever, is
s m
^ 1
r
i I
r
r
vns
S^ S i
-J ;-;J
^^ •s
r r
XT
r
V* 16
In writing the vii|, particular care should be taken not to double the chord's
prime — the leading tone — mentioned many times in previous chapters.
for reasons
Despite the similarities between the vii° triad and the various inversions of
V,, composers have preferred to use the dominant seventh in all but one inversion.
Ex. 18-22 illustrates the various possibilities and the general preference. Surprisingly
INVERSIONS OF THE DOMINANT SEVENTH ; HARMONIC RHYTHM 303
enough, there are very few exceptions to these conclusions. It seems hkely that the
exposed tritone in both vii° and
was a decisive factor, as well as the greater
vii^
s s =8= 331
r or
f Preferred to Preferred to
-I i
m icn
-e-
lOI
Vii°6
n
v1 n VM" V| vir
16 V
304 INVERSIONS OF THE DOMINANT SEVENTH, HARMONIC RHYTHM
W |
,j
,H ^
^
Molto agitato
^i ^m ^^ ^
TTTr
i
rr
J-i
m^ ^
/7S
d
z:
F^ ri
enters into the problem in at least two ways: harmonic structures that would be
dismissed as "passing chords" at a rapid tempo must be considered as basic units
in the harmonic scheme at a slower tempo. If, for example, the harmonic changes
of Ex. 18-26 occurred in still slower tempo or in longer note values, their effect
would be considerably different, and their harmonic separation as individual
chords would be more apparent to the listener.
Tempo can also influence the rate of harmonic change in another way: extremely
slow tempi often seem to demand a faster harmonic rhythm than faster tempi in
order to sustain the motion of the music. Conversely, fast tempi often necessitate
306 INVERSIONS OF THE DOMINANT SEVENTH; HARMONIC RHYTHM
a slower harmonic rhythm so that the separate chord changes will remain distinct
(to say nothing of the convenience to the performer). Thus harmonic rhythm
creates an analytical problem for the listener and a practical one for the composer.
The use of pedal point is a further consideration when studying harmonic
rhvthm. Not onlv does its use render the inversion of a chord meaninarless, but it
can also "camouflage" the harmonic rhythm. Our ears depend upon the bass so
much in identifying a harmonic progression, that the basic harmonic framework
is obscured when a pedal is present. The "pedal fugue" of Brahms' A German
Requiem is a prime example. This composition employs a rich and varied harmonic
vocabulary —
however, over a tonic pedal sustained throughout the movement
all,
by the bass instruments. The beginning of this movement is quoted in Ex. 18-28.
Tenors
ulTjf prJpr
technique. Ex. 18-29 and Ex. 18-31 illustrate a cross-section of harmonic rhythm
from interior passages in the respective compositions. Ex. 18-30 and Ex. 18-32
reveal a faster rate of harmonic rhvthm at cadences in the same movements.
^^
^
INVERSIONS OF THE DOMINANT SEVENTH; HARMONIC RHYTHM 307
^m fc^ ^
T ^ rr
^^^=^
i ^y- ^-^ 1
aa pT^ p f
.VT
r
IV ,V7-
^^
Ex. 18-31. Franck : Symphony in D Minor, I (interior).
i
P^
• V 121
J L.
f J L
^m J U
^i^
iiJ
^ ^^
n.
taz
J J.J
^ .
h<i
)-ll|i (! ' ^ «^
J L
We
can make other observations about the use of harmonic rhythm as a deter-
minant of musical form. Its rate is noticeably faster in formal sections that are de-
velopmental rather than expository. Likewise, sections that display an uncertain
or unstable tonality often make use of a relatively rapid harmonic rhythm. A com-
poser frequently differentiates between his thematic materials in a composition
by setting them in differing rates of harmonic motion.
In contrast to these procedures, a composer often chooses a slow harmonic
rhythm for sections in which a theme is stated as a whole or in which a single key
is emphasized. Obviously it is harmonic changes
easier to reinforce a tonality if the
are relatively slow. Thus harmonic rhythm can be an agent of form in music and
can influence both thematic and tonal aspects.
:
Exercises
For more detailed assignments see Materials and Structure of Music I, Workbook, Chapter 20.
1. In textures of four parts, resolve various root pxjsition and inverted versions of dominant
sevenths to appropriate versions of the tonic chord.
2. Select from music several passages that contain examples of dominant seventh chords
in root position and inverted. Study these, noting chord of resolution and melodic-
motion of resolution in each voice. If the texture in question has less than four voices,
what chord member has been omitted? If more than four voices, what member has been
doubled?
3. Write an original passage of three phrases in which the two interior cadences occur
on inversions of the dominant seventh chord.
4. Examine the harmonic rhythm of Beethoven's Sonata for Piano, Op. 2 no. 2, Scherzo
and Trio. What (if any) differences exist in the harmonic rhythms of the Scherzo and
its Trio? Do the harmonic changes coincide with the metric accents throughout? Com-
pare measures 1-4 and 17-20 of the Trio. Does harmonic rhythm differ?
5. Practice playing \^, \"|, \\, in both major and minor keys. Play in four voices with any
member in the soprano, then resolve with appropriate voice motion to the tonic chord.
6. To the two-chord progressions of (5) above, add a preparaton,- chord which makes it
possible to precede the dominant chord with one of the following figures:
a. Passing tone
b. Suspension
c. Neighboring tone
d. Leaning tone
7. Using the following progression as a guide, improvise a melody of simple rhythmic prop-
erties that arpeggiates the given chords
^ 3 is iv/i6/ V^ /i
Make up similar progressions in major and minor keys for the same practice.
8. Continue the aural practice suggested in recent chapters, concentrating upon recogni-
tion of the dominant se\enth chord in root and inverted positions. Learn to identify these
both as isolated sonorities and within progressions.
9. Melodies for sight singing and melodic dictation should incorjKtrate the outlined Y.
chord.
10 and hearing of intervals, concentrating on
Drill in the spelling, writing, relations found
in the dominant seventh chord.
Second-Inversion
Triad I
19
The student of music literature will notice that triads appear less
often in second inversion than in either root position or first inversion. A probable
reason for this exists in the intervallic structure of the chord : major and minor
triads in root position and first inversion contain at least one cadential consonance
between the bass and upper parts (specifically, root position contains both a perfect
fifth and a third; first-inversion triads contain a third and one decorative con-
—
sonance a sixth). Only the second inversion contains no cadential consonances
above the bass; it is constructed entirely of decorative consonances (the sixth and
the perfect fourth). Hence it is a less stable sonority.
The perfect fourth, in particular, sets this inversion apart. The fourth has been
treated as an unstable interval throughout much of music history and normally
has not been used above the bass without some special treatment to cushion its
The Cadential 1^
309
310 SECOND-INVERSION TRIADS
tones: the E suspended from the previous chord and the G an accented passing
tone. To continue this line of reasoning we could logically conclude that this is
merely an embellished \^, that the "real" harmonic progression is from ivg — —\'
i.
Examples such as this have led some musicians to deny that this usage of the six-
However, not all examples of the cadential I| are as easily explained. Ex. 19-1
illustrates, though, the salient features found in virtually every occurrence of the
cadential I^. One of the most significant of these is the metrical placement of the
six-four — usually on a strong beat, resolving immediately to \' on the next beat.
The and the fourth generally resolve, as
notes that form the intervals of the sixth
they do here, down by step to the nearest tones of V. Another chracteristic is the
doubling of the bass note (the dominant) in another part. These same features are
present in Ex. 2.
^ rnsT^pi
•?—
P
rfy
3
J:
J i
r-
i
^m £
*
i
16 15
f=
V
^ ^
r
2
This example also demonstrates the metrical placement of the I^ in triple meters.
Since the resolution is directly to V (found customarily on the third beat of the
measure), I^ often occurs on beat 2. It can be found on the first beat as well.
Ex. 19-3 illustrates the cadential i^ in an unusually thick texture of six voices.
This example should be analyzed carefully to determine whether the same princi-
"
pies of doubling and melodic motion apply under conditions of greater textural
density. Aside from this, however, the characteristic features of our previous exam-
ples may be seen.
K »
» ^
t J J- i njz
^ r
i|
ft'' !>
r
rft=g
i=a
[^^^ #
12
r
ffi
Ex. 19-4 illustrates the If in a more homophonic texture. The high registers used
in thisexample (in the flute, strings, and tenor soloist) increase the effect of the
m^
n - a tu -
P
a
f m
glo
^^ri - a, glo - ri -
»
a
I g
tu -
l7\
iea=
Vr- =5^
z^
S
v
Soprano
Alto
Tenor
Bass
Orchestra<
n i
i^i ^^
n V n
The second-inversion triad is sometimes formed as a link between two more basic
chords (basic because of their duration, metric location, or other factors) or in
extended examples of similar motion. In cases such as this, as in Ex. 19-6, the
entire sonority may be viewed as an aggregate of non-chord tones or as a decorative
chord. It is frequently of very short duration. In plotting the harmonic rhythm,
however, it is helpful to recognize that a separate harmonic unit has been formed.
Passing six-four chords are found on the tonic and, less frequently, on the dominant.
^
:
i
^ fe
r r ^^ i
rr *—H^
f
#
IS iv« i|
SECOND-INVERSION TRIADS 313
Ex. 19-7 also illustrates a typical passing I5. In contrast to cadential six-four
chords, passing six-fours occur most frequently in "weak" (metrically unaccented)
locations.
^^
Ex. 19-7. Corelli : Concerto Grojso, Op. 6, No. 11, " Sarabande."
m
^
4^£U ^
5 6 6 6
^
6
4
Ex. 19-8 is less obvious because of the presence of many accented non-chord
tones, but the passing six-four is similar in all other respects to the earlier examples.
rij^ls
Mm^m
W
^ff?^-n^
LJ
a
r
n n ^ n
^s 6
^6
4
cj- c-'r
It 6
rj;
6 7 6
p
Examples of V^ used in passing are rare. The following, however, illustrate the
typical procedure: V^ appears
from Ig to I or vice versa. In most similar
in passing
circumstances composers prefer harmonizing the ascending second scale degree
with V^ or viig. The reader should experiment with these three sonorities to dis-
cover their relative tonal effects. Examples of V^ are not numerous enough to
justify detailed conclusions about their doubling, etc., but the general melodic
principles governing the use of six-four chords usually apply effectively.
m
III.
p r r r
^
9^ V6
4
3M SECOND-INVERSION TRIADS
m^^^^^
^^ ^ ^^^
^ s^
V| l«
Numerous examples of IV^ can be found in works from the Classical period. In
many of these the chord occurs virtually at the beginning of the movement, as in
Ex. 19-12. In this context it helps to focus attention on the tonic note and thus
tends to establish solidly the key of the movement. In an obvious sense, it functions
as a pedal figure on the tonic pitch.
1^ ^ ^^
Ex. 19-12. Haydn: Symphony No. 102, IV.
M1
g rr^
m iv»
U
, ;
A similar, yet more example appears in Ex. 19-13. The IV5 occurs
elaborate,
briefly in the first measure and again in a more prominent and extended role in
measure three.
m
Ex. 19-13. Cannabich : Sinfonia in B-flat Major, II.
fei w ^
^ r
m^k^
r^
fe:
CN
$Sr ^—n-l v ^ h
f
2:
(O
')-it n r 21 z:
+ <SPfl
u
^i
The harmonic progression interrupted by the cadenza is resumed at the end of
the cadenza. The soloist usually ends with a trill or some other prolongation of V
thjs resolves with the entrance of the orchestra on the I chord. There are, of course,
some striking exceptions to this procedure,^ but they are in the minority.
The appearance of six-four chords can be a useful aid in harmonic analysis,
especially in the sometimes difficult problem of determining the key. Since they
are relatively infrequent, a six-four chord of even a beat's duration usually appears
within a context of tonal stability and often points unmistakably to the dominant
of the key. In the absence of other evidence, it seems most logical to interpret an
isolated six-four chord as I^, unless the context clearly contradicts this analysis.
1 Sec Beethoveiis Piano Concerto No. 3, I, bar 481 for a brilliant and unusual exception.
376 S£cond-;nv£rs;on triads
Cadences ending with a six-four chord are not common in music of any period.
An unusual example of such a cadence occurs in Ex. 19-15 (the cadence is, of course,
an interior one) The cadence appears to be the result of the extended dominant
.
In Ex. 19-16 a six-four chord appears as the structural climax of a section. Al-
though the overall tonality of this excerpt is not as clear as eighteenth and nine-
teenth century standards would demand, the relationships between the individual
chords are, for the most part, the most traditional of all root relationships the —
perfect fifth, and the chords are mostly major and minor triads. The enharmonic
spelling of the six-four chord {g-sharp — a-flat; d-sharp — e-flat) appears to be for the
convenience of the eye in its organization of the melodic line.
Ex. 19-16. Bartok : Concerto for Orchestra. Copyright 1946 by Hawkes & Son (London)
Ltd. Reprinted by permission of Boosey & Hawkes Inc.
i
+
sm
8^a
f^
^p m i
p BZZ
i Q
izg:
i t**-"
^
m s ^m \, mfr I 't
i
¥i t
^ ^ toeJ
S =a
M^^W^
Vm
ES z:
In Ex. 19-17, \\ is again used as a cadential chord; its major quality comes as
a surprise after the minor implications of the B flats in the previous measure. Al-
"^^m
SECOND-INVERSION TRIADS 317
though the chords in this excerpt are more complex than the diatonic triads we
have been studying, the cadence on the G major six-four chord clearly confirms
G as the tonal center.
Ex. 19-17. Barber: Hermit Songs, "The Desire for Hermitage." Reprinted by permission
of the copyright owner, G. Schirmer, Inc.
^; = ^ ^
obvious example of this technique occurs in Ex. 19-18, in which the motion is
chromatic.
^m i A
%
Ex. 19-19. Bloch : Sche/omo. Reprinted by permission of the copyright owner, G. Schirmer,
Inc.
j -i
grj \
h wa ^^
mm^ .
^
Ex. 19-20. Obrecht : Miaa Graecorum.
gE^
Exercises
i'
I'J.'Mi'^ f
jnj
e
Music
j
I,
j -
^ j_^
-o-
1. In music of various composers, find several examples of the main kinds of second-inver-
sion chords discussed in this chapter {cadential, passing, embellishing). Also find examples
which do not fit the definition of these three types.
2. Write several textural v^ersions based on the cadential pattern shown below. Do these in
major and minor keys and in different meters. Write the two-voice outer framework
first for three- and four-voice examples.
At the piano, play the following progressions in major and minor keys
(a) I-Ii-Ie (b) I-I\-6-I (C) IV,-I6_V,-I
Improvise an arpeggiated melody based on a progression such as the following. Be sure
that the lowest note of each arpeggio pattern corresponds with the given inversion.
Choose keys that make this comfortable for your own voice.
Progression: I /IV« / Ig / \1 / Ig / I //
Sample:
Bp major: lah
SECOND-INVERSION TRIADS 319
d. Harmonic dictation for Roman numeral designation and one or both of the
outer voices
e. Melodic and rhythmic dictation with continuation of syncopation and increas-
ing complexity of rhythm
f. Intensive drill in rhythmic reading and sight-singing
'O (,
- (p,'+. . n .t 1^
Ternary
Form ;
20
The Supertonic
Chord
parts," two of which are the same or similar. Depending on the length of the total
composition, each part contains at least one or more phrases.
Frequently the return of material appearing earlier serves to "round off" a com-
position rather than give the impression of a distinct section of "return." This
practice can be obser\^ed in many folk melodies and popular songs. In the melody
of Ex. 20-1, the fourth phrase is a slightly altered version of the first. The contrast
of the second phrase is minimized (1) by the similarity of its rhythmic structure to
the first,by the use of a transposed version of the cadential pattern of the first
(2)
phrase, and (3) by the importance assigned to g-sharp and /-sharp in the first two
measures of both phrases. Since these similarities exist, the third phrase resembles
a bridge that links the first two with the last; it does not have an impact compara-
ble to the beginning portion of the melody.
320
TERNARY FORM; THE SUPERTONIC CHORD 321
^ i, h
|¥>.r]j. ^iiij:pr
,
'I I n ^ *•
i
<i
-)j
5
i
^
*:*
rjj-JiJjj.
j^
II
ii
t.
2.
j
^f r^^- I
j,^
As is often the case in melodies of this type, the third and fourth phrases actually
balance the first two. Thus, even though the principle of return is present, the
formal design that results two equal parts, a
is ba
divisible into :
|| ||.
A similar design is the basis for Ex. 20-2. However, the interrelationships we
observed in Ex. 20-1 are not as immediately evident because the element of con-
trast is more striking. For example, the phrase that begins on the second beat of
measure 8 brings into play a new rhythm, a wider range, and a change in overall
pitch direction, as well as a change in the established phrase lengths (four beats
instead of eight). However, the accented non-chord tones that appear on the first
beat of nearly every measure are retained, as are the basic accompanimental figure
and the upbeat beginnings of each phrase.
^
7,
^ .^
f f
n-nm
f
-#
^M •
Y f f f f=t
ife *=#
•
F \
f^P r>rr ^ m f
m
^ 0-0-0- 0-M
m
322 TERNARY FORM, THE SUPERTONIC CHORD
a ^Ek I
«_^
^ ^-0^
a ^
1V
s ?-r¥ 1||«
m
A
^^ |>
^
its
second part is the embryonic stage of ternary form. As the name ternary impliesT"
this formal plan involves three distinct sections, instead of the two discussed earlier.
The third section frequently is a full restatement of the opening section, or if not
a literal restatement, it is distinctly separated from the contrasting middle section.
Each of the three parts is more or less equal in length, and the contrasting material
is more than a mere bridge, linking statement with restatement. In contrast to the
similar two-part design with restatement, the middle section in ternary form fre-
quently impresses us, as a definite and separate musical unit, as strongly as does
the initial section. This is particularly true when we listen to compositions in which
several phrases are combined to produce each section.
Ex. 20-3 displays clearly this division into three parts. The change of rhythm
at measure 9 gives the impression of a change of tempo, even though this need
not be the case. Furthermore, the elaborately "decorated" melody of the second
section adds to the contrast, counterbalancing the emphasis given to the rhythmic
1. a.
m ^ IQ
^
frm Itm W
^W^^ r»
TERNARY FORM; THE SUPERTONIC CHORD 323
i\
ji ijgrr^fl"l-^> v^-^ k
^^^^^^ ^^ r
I
f ^
^ r eS T^ J i iafr ^ rJ-'V> p-^r? ^
^^ U t^ I*
,
|. ^antfrt jr i
jj^
r ss
— —;a^-fi; ]t=«
f'^j
7
iij "J iij
r=r^
S^ i^^ z:
i :P U •g iS g
^ (
^
^Pl ^ ff n I
^
Furthermore, the upper part in measures 9-16 uses a larger range than the
preceding, and is characterized by triadic outlining. In this instance the opening
accompanimental figure is used in each section (with some modifications), as in
measures 9 and 17.
The restatement (measures varied by changing the register (measures
1 7-24) is
17-20) and by doubling the melody in octaves (measures 21-24). This heightens
the effectiveness of the return by avoiding the monotony of a literal restatement.
Frequently, however, the third section of a ternary form is a literal repetition
of the first. If this is the case, the restatement need not be written out. On the con-
324 TERNARY FORM; THE SUPERTONIC CHORD
trary, it is often indicated by da capo {D. C.)^ at the end of the second section. Some-
times the indication Dal segno {D. S.)^ is found at the end of the second section, as
in Ex. 20-4. This means that the repetition begins at some indicated point (where
"the sign" appears) rather than at the beginning. The sign usually looks some-
Even though Ex. 20-4 is longer than Ex. 20-3, its formal design is the same basic
ABA. Similar relationships delineate the larger sections, but there are notable
differences.For example, two distinctive accompanimental patterns are used in
the Chopin. The first, measures 1-16, is distinguished by a durational accent on
the second beat in the inner parts, nullifying the upbeat pattern of the lowest part.
In the second section, measures 1 7-33, the rhythm is distinguished by a downbeat
pattern, as well as a change in the rhythm of the melody. And as a final confirma-
tion of sectioning, tonal contrast between the two sections is achieved by modula-
tion to a new tonal center. In Ex. 20-3 tonal contrast was produced by mutation.
m ^
^
^^ ^ r
u-
rr
^
r
^r
f
^h ^ ^ \
r^,i
rr f
M ^m
^^ m i-
> J^ji
i
Ij-j
r
jj ^^ ^^
r r
P Fine
r r
^
Skt i i ^ g^
^ si m ^
^s f
fei^r
t
f^
^S r
^ 4
st^^^^ ^P T^ =fe
ai^za:
^Itrr^ ^ ?g
^=« *"^ mis
^& ^ i
?i
? t tt
i»—(^
^ i
S^^ P m I P^ 5 fe
a:«
•VJ •• ^
3 1»
a^^ja
Pii^
^I
fct * J vo
i
2-
# I ai
m
f r
D.5. a/ Ftne
Another factor is present in Ex. 20-4 that tends to overshadow the other formal
elements as a determinant of form. This is the change of texture.
The overall texture of both the Schumann and the Chopin examples is homo-
phonic. We noted in Ex. 20-3 that the pitch range increased in the second section.
This increase alters the texture slightly by changing the space between the melody
and its accompaniment. In Ex. 20-4 the textural change in measure 18 is more
pronounced because the accompaniment changes character and is moved to a
different register. The number of parts is essentially the same, but spacing starkly
326 TERNARY FORM; THE SUPERTONIC CHORD
distinguishes the textural "top" from "bottom." The motion in parallel sixths and
thirds (measure 24 and following) represents still another change of texture.
Our discussion has emphasized the overall plan of ternary design, as well as
some of the demarcate or produce contrast between the sectional parts
factors that
of the total form. As a matter of fact, it is very difficult to describe a piece of music
without taking into consideration both the structural plan and the smaller factors
that fill out this plan. Generally speaking, many compositions have a ternary
structural design, just as many buildings are basically rectangles. However, we
know that architects have adapted the rectangular structural principle to many
different situations, and similarly, the ternary principle of statement, contrast,
and return has been used in many quite different compositions.
A cursory examination of Ex. 20-3 and Ex. 20-4 reveals in both cases a basic
ternary plan, but a closer look at the structure of each section shows that the
processes of unfolding are not the same in both pieces. Thematic material, tonality,
rhythm, and total length are obvious differences.
Another difference is the way each section closes and connects with the restate-
ment. The B section of Ex. 20-3 closes with a half cadence that is combined with
a definite rhythmic halt. The listener's demand for a return to the beginning prob-
ably results from the instability of the half cadence and the extreme rhythmic
contrast. In Ex. 20-4 the end of the contrasting section is not signaled as boldly
because the rhythmic activity in the upper parts continues through to the begin-
ning of the restatement. The two sections merge (elide), and it is not until the
return is in progress that we become aware of it. Even though other points of com-
parison and contrast have not been touched, the reader should examine and
compare other design factors of the two contrasting sections.
In summary, ternary design consists of three distinct sections: statementzrzcmz^
trast —restatement Each section may follow a particular formal pattern of its own^
.
and each section usually has a different tonality. The statement frequently closes
with a terminal cadence, as does the contrasting section. Sometimes the contrasting
section closes with a progressive cadence or merges with the restatement. That is,
finality is avoided at this point to enhance further the cons equent retu rn to ateriaL_ m
of an earlier statement.
In Chapter 14 we learned that triads can appear on any scale degree. We have
also seen in previous chapters that the triads on the first, fourth, and fifth scale
degrees delineate the mode, e.g., in major keys, I, IV, and V are major triads. In
this sense these three triads confirm the scale basis of a composition. The other
triads of the harmonic spectrum can be identified initially by their quality (i.e. as
—
major, minor, etc.). In this chapter we will discuss only one of these, the supertonic?
Our discussion will focus primarily on its appearance in major and minor, even
though its use is not restricted to these two scales.
The supertonic triad is minor in a major key, setting it apart from the major
triads I, IV, and V. In Ex. 20-5 the second scale degree appears as the bass note
in measure 5; it is also the root of the chord. Since G is the tonic of this excerpt,
the minor triad on a is supertonic. However, the relationship of the a minor triad
to the tonal center is not strongly confirmed until the G major triad is heard in
measure 8. Thus, in retrospect, it can be related to G and understood as ii.
The ii chord also contains the fourth and sixth scale degrees, so it can be used to
harmonize either the second, fourth, or sixth scale degree.
m
Vivace
pp
Orchestra
^\
I V
^^
n
^^
I
B f
Ei
P Ff ^
=E3E ^F=n
i*
i
:
^m V m m 9 —w-W • VfV
i=5 £
:•: wt
£
3
'
J-U
I
-^
i
^m & Mzza: t
I
h
<^\
^1
\\
r,
In Ex. 20-6, the lowest note, the prime, of the chord is doubled after the resolution
of the leaning tone g in the soprano voice. The diminished fifth {/-sharp c) moves —
to an octave (B —
b) in similar motion, making clear that /-sharp is in fifth relation
with the root b. The characteristic resolution pattern of this interval is not used.
This is often true in minor when the ii° functions in the same way as the ii chord
in major, in root relation of a fifth with the dominant chord.
Ex. 20-6. Carl Loewe : Der Pilgrim vor Si. Just. Op. 99, No. 3.
The second scale degree is frequently the root of a major triad. Because a tritone
is created between the third of this chord and tonic, a major triad on the super-
tonic has a different relation to tonic than its unaltered counterpart. Therefore, its
structural role is often magnified. It will be discussed in detail in Chapter 24.
^ $
^ i^ U
f rr
i
f
f
LM L^A
^ i
XV^f
T:
The difference in sonority quality distinguishes ii (or ii°) from I, IV, and V;
however, this does not explain the tonal relationships that can exist between them.
Because of the tones in common, ii (ii°) is related to both the subdominant and the
dominant. It has two tones in common with IV, one tone in common with V.
TERNARY FORM; THE SUPERTONIC CHORD 329
m
C:
'
rd^
Wr¥-
^J
^T5th ^ Sa ?^^ m
c :
|
P^ a 0-0-
m
• #V
mum
J^
^ 0-0-
s
Ml *
m P
si
»# it
P
t^
IV ii
^I| V
In Ex. 20-9, ii is preceded by IV. Thus ii appears between two different chords,
each of which has at least one tone in common with it. In this sense ii can link IV
to V because it has common elements with both.
Sometimes ii is rhythmically accented. In Ex. 20-10 it precedes V^, but the ac-
tivity of the other parts adds complexities not present in Ex. 20-9. Notice that the
lower two voices move in contrary motion to the upper two, and that the third of
the supertonic is doubled rather than the root.
m ^J n.nj
^^ r r r CJ^ '
ii
r
V^ I
330 TERNARY FORM; THE SUPERTONIC CHORD
An analogous rhythmic formation exists in Ex. 20-11. Both IV and ii have the
same duration, and the duration of V is equal to IV and ii combined. Consequently,
because of duration and because of being preceded by ii, V is emphasized as the
cadential chord.
Ex. 20-11. R. Strauss: Nichfs, Op. 10, No. 2. By permission of the International Music
Company, New York.
Vivace
As its root position form, the first inversion (iig) of the supertonic triad frequently
precedes V. In the three-part illustration (Ex. 20-12), the two lower parts move in
parallel thirds until the dominant is reached (measure 3) in the lowest part. How-
ever, instead of a subdominant root, the fourth scale degree (measure 2) forms the
third of the supertonic chord.
^
^ m m
^ ^ i
^ fct,
^^
^
P IM
P
II 6
g'
<
V7
.
In Ex. 20-13 the upper three parts move motion in measures 1 and 2.
in similar
When the fourth scale degree appears as bass in measure 2 the upper three voices
move in contrary motion, creating a iig chord.
^ ^^ m f
^
f
^ i
f — ^
f
k
^
iis 16
The succession iig —V represents a fusion of the patterns IV—V and —V be- ii
cause the bass motion up a second is characteristic of the root relation between IV
and V, and because the fifth root relation is also present. In this sense,iig and IV
are used interchangeably to harmonize the fourth and sixth scale degrees, and to
precede V. The bass motion up a second is an important melodic factor; the use of
iig instead of IV provides the harmonic element of the fifth relationship, while still
i^
\
J i
|
Lir i
i
J r \r frr r .
p^ ^=^
?=T
^=#
^N=^
^f^ rr^
i
^ itF
IV
^
11°
p
V
^^ ^ r Lf r *
^
Vln.
ciJ
i
r I
f
^=^ i
§ i j ^ s
rr ^
^^ ^r^
9
s i?F=
^
IV
p
V
The occurrence of iig (iig) is often characterized by the harmonic sixth between
the two outer voices, placing both the root (or prime) of the chord and the fourth
scale degree in prominent positions. Consequently, instead of the characteristic
harmonic diminished fifth of ii°, an augmented fourth is created between the prime
and some other part.
(rM SI IZ
^ :a^
^ JV« i°6 i
zsb
ini
;.
TERNARY FORM; THE SUPERTONIC CHORD 333
In this excerpt ii| moves to i; therefore, the augmented fourth resolves typically.
The bass motion in measures 2 and 3 outlines 4 — 1, supporting an upper voice
motion 2 — 3. The motion by step in the upper voice balances the motion by skip in
the lower voice.
A ii| chord in a major context is shown in Ex. 20-17. The middle part, because
of its chromatic motion, has considerably more independence than minor if the
supertonic had been used. Notice that the augmented fourths in the upper two
parts (*) move by similar motion to other augmented fourths. Here it is the de-
scending chromatic pattern of the middle voice rather than the iig that makes the
diminished supertonic inevitable.
^
^
Ex.
^
^m
20-17. Chopin: Mazurka, Op. 7, No.
^^
^
T r
^^
f rr
2.
f^^T^
ISZ
¥
SPi ^
r^f
The ii chord's relation to the dominant is one of the important structural roles
it performs. In other contexts the supertonic is frequently decorative in character.
That is, it is used for creating linear harmonic motion rather than as a structural
harmony in conjunction with V and I. This more decorative function is illustrated
vividly in Ex. 20-18. From measures 2-6 all of the chords that appear are in root
position, unfolding the phrase sequentially. The root movement is down by seconds;
consequently ii moves directly to tonic.
^m m ?
^m 5"
A similar root relation is illustrated in Ex. 20-20. Each of the chords is in root
position. Tonic does not become clearly defined until measure 3. Since ii is used
to harmonize the sixth scale degree,/, it moves directly to tonic (*) in measure 1.
—
In measure 2 the harmonic succession ii V is formed by the bass part. However,
ii remains in effect in the upper parts. Therefore, the supertonic and the dominant
chords merge.
Ex. 20-20. Debussy : Bruyerei. Permission for reprint granted by Durand et Cie., Paris,
copyright owners, and Elkan-Vogel Co., Inc., Philadelphia, agents.
^m m^n^
^^
^a ^7^-^: u
^7-^^—7-^: f: =: ^:
(iii) ii I
rhythmically prominent with a syncopation, and the b'-JIat in the upper and g in
the lower part are the pitch apexes of the phrase.
Ses xn m:
n ii^' ^^ nA
^^ J. J- i> J J
i
f
3t
s ^^ ^
mM
ll
r-
J^
p
UUm ^^ $ ^w^
^J=^ jj J
^
r I II r
Root movement down a second sometimes occurs cadentially, resulting in the
progression ii —
I. Since I is the harmonic goal of this pattern, it is a type of terminal
—
cadence. Furthermore, ii I most closely resembles the plagal cadence, because
the supertonic chord has two tones in common with IV. Therefore, it can be
described as a supertonic plagal cadence, to distinguish it from the usual IV I plagal —
form.
Ex. 20-22. Hindemith : Mafhis der Maler, I. (C) 1934 by B. Schott's Soehne, Mainz. Renewed
1961. Reprinted by permission.
t^ fi- —
-e-
i^
r^ ^^
S2
2^
(m)
The first inversion supertonic, as part of a terminal cadence, also creates the super-
tonic plagal cadence. The plagal nature of the iig I cadence is primarily a result—
336 TERNARY FORM; THE SUPERTONIC CHORD
of the prominence given to the fourth scale degree. This explains why ii — I is also
a supertonic plagal cadence. In both cases this cadential pattern results from the
relationship between the fourth and the first scale degrees. The secundal root rela-
tion modifies the effect of the more common IV I cadence. —
Ex. 20-23. Supertonic plagal cadence.
Er ^
g ^
N
lie
Exercises
For more detailed assignments see Materials and Structure of Music I, Workbook, Chapter 20.
1. Listen to and study through compositions such as the following. Isolate those musical
elements that play a prominent role in creating the ternary design of each work,
a. Schumann: Traumerei
b. Rachmaninoff, Prelude, Op. 3, No. 2
c. Debussy, La fille aux cheveiix de lin
2. Spell, write, play, and sing the chords that could appear on the second scale degree of
any diatonic scale.
3. Use the harmonic rhythm of Ex. 20-5 as the basis for a four-part vocal composition.
Write in a contrapuntal texture with or without a text.
and ii°, from I, IV, and V, making possible an immediate qualitative distinction.
Other distinctions will become apparent as we examine the appearances of iii
and vi in various contexts.
In major the submediant is a minor triad. The submediant chord contains the
sixth, first, and third notes of a scale, and is used to harmonize any of the three.
In Ex. 21-1 the sixth scale degree appears as bass in measures 1 and 3, but it is
IV« V« I
337
V
In example the repeated g (the third scale degree) in the upper part is har-
this
monized by both I and vi. The association of the two roots with g results in a
change of harmonic rhythm that would not be present if only the tonic chord had
been used for the first two beats.
In a minor key the submediant triad is major in quality. Like its counterpart in
major, it is frequently associated with melodies that contain successive repetitions
of the tonic note. As a consequence, VI is used frequently to create an active
harmonic rhythm. This is seen in Ex. 21-2, where VI alternates with i in measure
2, even though the voice part remains on the same pitch.
J^TJ J J J j J J'J^J- II
<j I
f pp I
j j
^ I
^^t } i li ^ 1 I jt
p
/^
^ t J* ^ 44
it.,L
W'
..
K''
[•
i=t
cjl: V M
rhythm begun in the first measure. Furthermore, this relationship is part of the
—
alternating major triad minor triad pattern that begins in measure 1.
The submediant chord shares tones in common with tonic, supertonic, and sub-
dominant, if we include only those triads previously discussed. These tonal relation-
ships are illustrated in Ex. 21-3. In Chapter 20 we did not show the relation of the
supertonic chord to the submediant; here we can see that such a relationship
exists. As a matter of fact, the root relation of vi —
ii, in major, is analogous to ii —
—
and V I. Notice that in minor the root VI and the prime of ii° are a diminished
fifth apart.
Ex. 21-3.
'>
Ti ir^-13 n^-u \ \%\\\i m\AjiVy-h% \
w
3rd
3rd 5th o5th
C: I vi Vi IV vi I VI VI iv vi ii°
SUBMEDIANT AND MEDIANT CHORDS; CHORD RELATIONSHIPS 339
iJ^ j^ iEi
rrrr
i^
n Wf ^
U
P^ ?
J 1
^^
11
^
ffff f
V7
Here vi is preceded by I; thus, each of the chords in measures 1-4 is related to the
next with at least one common tone. Furthermore, root relationships by fifth occupy
most of the phrase.
In Ex. 21-5 IV appears between vi and ii, producing root movement in thirds.
Consequently, each chord in measures 1 and 2 has two notes in common with its
immediate predecessor. Notice also that the common tone relationship is not pres-
ent in the supertonic plagal cadence, and that the phrase does not contain any
fifth relationships.
Ex. 21-5.
PU
Wagner:
i
Parsifal.
J'
^
w
s
-o-
^ Ak
a:
^
IV
-a
ii
1
^331
The submediant chord also appears in the role of a neighbor chord, and creates
root movement by a second, thereby omitting the possibility of common tones.
Ex. 21-6 shows vi as a neighbor to V, clearly indicated by its durational relation
to V. Notice that the resultant motion by step in the bass adds variety to the
phrase.
340 SUBMEDIANT AND MEDIANT CHORDS; CHORD RELATIONSHIPS
^
Ex. 21-6.
m
Bach: Ermvfre dich, wein sehwacher Geist.
1^-r
^ I
n r
.^^
S^
G: I
^
IV
r NVi V
f f
measure 2 of Ex. 23-7 could be harmonized with the I chord. By using vi, however,
the bass line in measure 2 conforms to measure 1, as well as adding a different
harmonic color.
m r
i ^^^
r r i
^i
$
5
f
s 1
r ?
VI IV
^f
^
^
1
/ •/
I
te^ nqrz p^ :q-
Z) TS-
^ a
tS
3j:
— fe
a:
Piano
^^5:
tP
/
3:
1^
-e-
^
^ -o-
I« IV V7 VI
More tonal activity leading to the expected tonic chord generally follows the
deceptive cadence. In Ex. 21-9 a cadential vi appears in measure 8. Here the
deception afforded by vi intensifies the eventual appearance of I, as well as en-
hancing the immediate repetition of previous material. Unlike the major-minor
relation formed in major, the deceptive cadence in minor involves two successive
major triads^
^ i
^ ^
~?"
^^ m ff=JF
^ P
'^
d:
^
342 SUBMEDIANT AND MEDIANT CHORDS; CHORD RELATIONSHIPS
^M. i m P f F
I n J ff
^m ^ p f*-
1^^ ? ^ p
% r
v
I
VI
£^
I«
^
Since the root relationship between V and vi is a second, they have no tones in
common. Therefore, V—vi, Hke the succession of all chords whose roots are a
second apart, generally involves considerable contrary motion. The third of the
vi chord (the tonic note) is generally doubled when it is preceded by V or V,;
the root obviously also can be doubled. In minor the root of VI is usually not
doubled when preceded by the dominant, because the leading tone would then
it is
resolve to a tone an augmented second lower. Ex. 21-10 gives the usual doublings
for both major and minor.
^ ^
f w i $ 1=f
tKl
f
m^
F: V
-e-
vi
Third doubled
W ^V vi
^
-&-
VI
i*:
^^
VI VI
I fe mi
«
te S t±:
m f: \l
^ V VI
-^ 9 y
—
VI VI
SUBMEDIANT AND MEDIANT CHORDS; CHORD RELATIONSHIPS 343
In Ex. 21-11 the vi chord ends the first phrase in harmony with the tonic note.
Here vi is preceded by V'^, which harmonizes the second degree. Since the leading
tone appears in the upper part, it is resolved to tonic. The fifth of the V, is part of
a descending line and also moves to the tonic note. Consequently the third of vi
is doubled.
f± i m^ i
n f7 '
1 i
r^
mM rn g
^
^ m ^^ n
r
rr=r
^
E\>: I \nvi
The mediant is a minor triad in major and a major triad in minor. Since it
contains the third, fifth, and seventh notes of a scale, it has two tones in common
with and V, and one tone in common with VI (vi). In minor keys the mediant
I (i)
'): irVt |
p:i „ r):, l
„ iPO i^ I !
^i^
C: I III m^'K I IE V or m or in+
3 :*
i
KJ
—
as shown in Ex. 21-14. The result, however, is quite different; e.g., the harmonic
In addition, the half step root relationship between iii and IV is eliminated.
^ f^-^i
^ J
.^h
P i :* ^£ ^
m I --. 27 ^:
of an upbeat pattern that deemphasizes its location in the measure. Since iii is
used to harmonize the seventh degree, and since the V chord contains the lowered
seventh degree, an alternating major-minor triad pattern is created.
Ex. 21-15. Revel: Trio, IV. Permission for reprint granted by Durcnd et Cie., Paris, copy-
right owners, and Elkan-Vogel Co., Inc. Philadelphia, agents.
Ill IV3 V
In Ex. 21-16 the harmonic rhythm of the second phrase creates a distinctive
contrast with the first. In addition, the appearance of both VI and III produces
a brief skirting of the relative major tonality. Notice that the root movement of
— —
VI III is similar to iv i. Since this similarity exists, we can say that the iv i. —
of measure 3 is a sequential continuation of vi iii; that is, a systematic harmonic
pattern is immediately repeated at a different pitch level.
SUBMEDIANT AND MEDIANT CHORDS, CHORD RELATIONSHIPS 345
i^r]
^ rt ^
f LJU
J-3J
CUT
^u g: I
J i
^^ ^aaj J
V«
J^
VI ffl Vi I
^m ^* J-^ #-=:
^m i
i^^;i J ^ p
The III chord appears between i and V in Ex. 21-17. The melody could have
been harmonized with either i or V. Interpolating III enhances the harmonic
structure, and maintains the harmonic rhythm established in the preceding two
movement outlines the tonic chord, thereby confirming the tonality; also note that
each of the chords is related to its successor with at least one common tone.
^m
s
^
^
tj m P
a e^ J vf'.
fe:
m i5 V
SRoots:
331
331
33=
331
33=
331
Outline of
33=
tonic triad
Sometimes the iii chord precedes tonic in a cadence. This places root movement
by thirds in the important terminal position. The two tones of the iii chord which
are shared with I (root and third of iii equal third and fifth of I) create a smooth
—
relationship that borders on chord repetition. The presence of the leading tone in
the iii chord provides the one factor that makes this relationship progressive.
Bl»: iii I
Ex. 21-19 and Ex. 21-20 illustrate terminal cadences involving iii — I. Both
examples are similar in cadential effect, but there is a notable distinction: in Ex,
21-19 the I chord is melodically outlined, whereas in Ex. 21-20 the iii chord is
melodically outlined. Thus, even though the cadential harmonic pattern is the
same in both excerpts, the terminal motion of each melody has a different char-
acter because of the chordal outlining.
fc f -^ ^
cresc.
~o
e
ff *'" " dim.
molto
^^ P I
i9-'
^
Et: (I)
m i
VI
1
III I
^ iAs- i
W'
i^-i
W ^
r^t
4^
^p 5^
'9-.
ifl-S^
9^. Sd.
B:
Ex.
i
t^
21-21. Augmented mediant.
i
i
i
^
m ^ ^=T m
e: m^ in+6 IV ni+ VI
Furthermore, it has a tonal kinship with i, VI, and V because of the tones they
share in common.
«ll— y Jl
f in+ I
II
lljL
m'
II
l ljl
ni+
¥VI
In Ex. 21-23 111*6 is approached by contrary motion between the two outer
parts. A logical chord at this point would have been Illg, but instead the tenor
moves by step from a to g- sharp in the first measure. This forms the augmented
sonority of the chord, at the same time directing attention to the a that follows
on the second beat. This leading tone action propels the texture of G-sharp — A
forward through iv to the dominant that eventually cadences on tonic.
I7\
SJ I
J J
r r r r T r
^ i ^ f
a: m+6 IV V ->
i
348 SUBMEDIANT AND MEDIANT CHORDS; CHORD RELATIONSHIPS
As in most unstable triads, the tones doubled are those that do not suggest resolu-
tion. Thus the third of an augmented triad (the note E in Ex. 21-23) is generally
doubled.
The augmented mediant frequently precedes V, producing the effect of an em-
bellishment of that chord. This possibility exists because of its close similarity with
V, and their common bass tone when III"^ is in the first inversion. Without the
passing activity in Ex. 21-24, Ill^g would move directly to tonic.
i
^^ ^
ri
f
^^:
S *
ni+6 v^ i
The first inversions of both the vi and the iii chord appear in harmonic situa-
tions that are similar to their root position counterparts. Since the root of the chord
does not appear in the lowest part, the resulting effect of harmonic change is con-
siderably weakened in some contexts. For example, if vig follows tonic, the root
of I remains in the lowest part. Even though there is a root change, the tonic note,
because of its harmonic motion.
tonal predominance, nullifies the effect of the
The result resembles a harmonic embellishment of the I chord rather than a change
of harmonic function. Stated another way, the tonic note resembles a pedal, and
any chordal change above it must be decisive to change the basic pattern of
harmonic rhythm.
=§=
S tf^=^
VI 6
—
The first inversions of both iii and vi are often used in passages that unfold the
musical materials sequentially. In Ex. 21-26, and IV.
iiig appears between vi
Its use here coincides with the established pattern that alternates root position and
first inversion chords. Therefore, even though the roots change in every measure,
the harmonic rhythm, because of alternating inversions and root positions, is made
sTinii J nn-n
/
T
-rri r
i
^ f
F: I VU
'y\ '-^
Analysis 4th 2nd 4th
of roots:
i
^ rt ^^
— m I •
fE^^^^Ur
'f
w^ ^ m/ J 7
iii'
^ f
IV
f 16
r^
_IL
2nd 4th 2nd
Ex. 21-27 has two important features. First is the sequence formed over tonic
and dominant pedals in measures 1 and 2. Superimposed on this strong root rela-
tion of I— V
a kind of "sub-progression" that accompanies
is in harmonic sequence
the melodic sequence of the top voice.
350 SUBMEDIANT AND MEDIANT CHORDS; CHORD RELATIONSHIPS
A second feature of Ex. 21-27 is its use of vi and iii as interior cadential chords
which, though terminal in effect, relieve the passage of the monotony that would
result if I had appeared more frequently in their stead.
At times, extended passages of first inversion chords including iiig and vig are
found together with a scalar motion in the lowest part. When this occurs the me-
lodic forces outweigh harmonic progression. This results partly from the root rela-
tions by seconds, the lack of common tone relationships, and the absence of strong
bass progression. Passages such as that of Ex. 21-28 bear a direct relationship to
the practice of fauxbourdon^ in the fourteenth and fifteenth centuries. In such pas-
sages harmonic relationships that are cadential in character are avoided; there-
fore, this kind of movement extends or lengthens a phrase by sequential patterns.
The use of the submediant and mediant triads completes our study of the
diatonic triads. As we have seen, iii (III) and vi (VI) frequently occupy a secondary
harmonic role; in other contexts they contribute a fair share to the structural
aspects of harmony.
Now that we have studied all of the diatonic triads, we should consider the
numerous tonal relationships existing between them. There are two significant
aspects about the relation between any two chords: the pitches (if any) shared
by both chords, and the kind of root relationship formed between the two. Each
triad of a single scale has at least one tone in common with another triad from
that scale. Any two triads having as many as two tones in common are closely
related. If they have one tone in common, they are less closely related. If there
are no tones in common between a set of two triads, they are not closely related.
Three principal types of root relationships may exist between any two triads.
They are fifth, third, and second. The root relationship of a fourth (like IV I) —
is an inversion of the fifth, just as root relationships of a sixth and a seventh are
inversions of the third and second, respectively. Triads whose roots form a fifth
relationship have one tone in common, such as V I and IV —
I. If the root rela- —
tion is a thirdj_as in ii — —
IV or I vi, there are two tones in common. But if the
root relation is a second, as in V- -vi, there are no tones in common.
-29 sTiows chord refationships to tonic when roots are related by fifths.
The vii° chord is not included because the leading tone does not function as a root,
and the I V is not included because it does not support the tonic as the root of its
relations hip. In minor the ii° chord would precede V. The ii° often is used in the
same manner as ii in major; therefore, in minor keys root succession by fifths
could begin with the iv chord, followed by the VII chord and continued as illustrat-
ed, III VI (ii°) vi.
C: ill VI ii V I
i
Mio~«
3
C: vi IV li (vii°) V I
The summary that follows shows the tonal relationships of each triad in both
major and minor keys. Root relationship by second is omitted because it is created
by the roots of any two adjacent triads, with the exception of vii° in major and ii°
ill minor.
Summary of Tonal Relationships Between the Diatonic Triads
/ Root Relationship
One Tone in Two Tones in
Common Common -sttrn^^ 3rd
Exercises
For more detailed assignments see Materials and Structure of Music I, Workbook, Chapter 21.
1. Sing and spell the mediant and submediant chords in all diatonic keys.
2. Use the melodic material from measures 1 and 2 of Ex. 21-9 as the motivic basis for a
16-measure piano composition in ternary form.
3. Reduce Ex. 21-11 to a two-voice framework. Then elaborate this basic frame to create
a "new" four-phrase work.
4. Reset the melody of Ex. 21-16 with a "faster" harmonic rhythm. Incorporate all of the
diatonic chords into your setting.
5. Write an experimental composition that uses a preponderance of chords other than
tonic, subdominant, or dominant, Make a formal plan, and sketch in the chordal forms
that will serve as the basis for a phrase or section.
6. Write a three-phrase vocal composition. Use the following plan: first phrase, only root
position chords; second phrase, parallel first inversion chords; third phrase, alternating
root position and first inversion chords. Close the example with a mediant to tonic ter-
minal cadence.
Through-composed
Form /
22
Mutated Chords,
Harmonic Sequence
However, there are compositions in which the repetition of larger formal units,
such as a period or a section, does not occur. Broadly speaking, this means that no
two parts of such a composition are identical in all respects. If this were adhered
to consistently, the composition would be a series of contrasts without any sense
of return or "rounding off" of the whole form.
The creation of a unified musical design depends upon many factors; large scale
repetition is only one of these. Compositions in which each section is essentially
different are sai d to be through-composed^, to distinguish them from repetitive struc-
tural schemes. Through-composed compositions may contain several contrasting
parts or sections; however, total length is ultimately limited by the listener's reten-
tion capacity.
Compositions based on the formal principle of non-repetition generally do not
contain exact restatements. In Ex. 22-1 none of the four phrases is precisely like
another {abed); even so, it is a balanced structure. Each of the phrases is only
externally different; within the separate phrases similarities exist that create unity.
Ex. 22-1.
Slow
inyUl'.P^ \ r^m ^ v^
PC rr i
-^^^-i i
' The German form of this term, durchkomponierl, appears frequently in books that discuss musical
form.
353
354 THROUGH-COMPOSED FORM
AH four phrases have a similar contour, various rhythmic patterns recur, and each
phrase begins with an upbeat. In addition, the tones focus decisively on A as tonic.
Thus, even though this melody does not contain repeated phrases, other factors
help to produce unity.
The through-composed formal design is used also for compositions that are longer
than the melody of Ex. 22-1. Simply stated, the duration of a composition is deter-
mined by the length of each through-composed composition
section. In a longer
each section will comprise several phrases. Within each section repetition of phrases
may occur, creating unifying elements within the section that differ from those
found in all subsequent sections.
In Ex. 22-2 the four sections are clearly delineated by changes in meter, tonality,
accompanimental patterns, texture, and rhythm. These major external differences
make the form (abed) easy to comprehend as through-composed.
Ex. 22-2. Schubert: Der Jungling auf dem HiJgel, Op. 8, No. 1.
*l* *
f ' ^' .g i
r p r p T' ^ ^ 1
^
^r p i
r^r-S
Ein Jijng ling auf dem Hu - gel mit seinem Kummer saB; wohl
"* ^ -^ ^ * -i-
fi^
r pr p
ward der Augen Spiegel
p
ihm
If
triib
P
und tranen
cj'p
-
i
rr
naB,
f' i
^/PC;'^
wohl ward der Augen
^ s
^
M t
4 ^ j J ^
f 7 p
rr r r # ; r
o
f^rlrr p
'
r p a^ ^
f^r
s
Spie - gel ihm triib unci tranen - naB
fe
t f ri—
MA ^ PP /7\
m f- wi #-= -0 (•
-.
9-"
s
It i-
i
MaBig
^^=^ ^~^
Sah fro
i
r PQ-CT
heL'am-mer spie
:
Z2I
len am
^^ ,>
grii-nen Fel
J^rr ;^
sen
fe^ ^^ p^#
- -
d* * d
Jq^
^ ?=^ i-—^J-—j) ^-'^i^J-''^;)
^^ ^^^ f^
j.—^J,J:-^J)
^ r >
^^ i
fH^- ^^m
i
hang.
^^1^^:
J—^--^
sah fro
#^#^
J.-^jJ.-^;)
- he Bachlein
mm ft^Pi
quil
J^V-'-.j
- len das bun
Jr-;j^
- te_Tal ent
^^ F^^
)
j)
W^^ P^^
i lang;
^m
die Schmet-terlin - ge so - gen am ro -
^m
ten Blii - ten -
S *-*^
#•»
i^i "• ^^T^
I
T — — — —
Jr-jJr^;,
r
J.— ;J—-^ Jr^J J.—
r
i > Jr— j>J.-^i>
r
;i;>;i
^
' i
"r pcj-xJ P l
Lf^^CJ
mund. wie Mor - gentrau-me flo - gen die Wol ken - in dem
fe » » m «— ^^# ±jt
r — r r r r
— « —
'jr ' r
1^'
^Liin^ r -pn'' ^m
Rund
^^ ^m
Jr-^J,Jr-^j>
und al
J^ij^i
- les war so mun
^^ mm
Jr—xj^r-^j,
- ter und al -
[p J]
\^ri
J^ij.^-i
les schwammi n
^^^^ ^^^ ^^ ^^
Gliick,
>
C\
^j^
nur
i r P C
Herz hin
*
j^Mcrpp^Y unter sah nicht
p
der
i
i
H
s
:
Freu
*
in sein de
mm
- -
^ £ ^ ^ :#^
$ ^^
^^
zft:
£
^ ;J
1 h
fTS
^^_J
^ UJ) i> J^ I
^'^^
Freu
^
^^
Blick, sah nicht der - de Blick.
r7\
^^ ^
1^
S ^^ 1
^ #
dimin.
IS
•» •«*
3i:
Langsam
' ^j'
#' l
j'.
•'• j'j!
0' m- —ir
rn •
i'p
y
i
^'PP'^'r
^^ 1
w
j'
Ach! dumpfesGrab-ge-lau-te im Dor-fenunerklang,schon
i r r r p 1-^ —r^^ pr r r p
^ i?p
« ^—* • — 4 — «'
f' j'.i'j!
ton-
Jif j'pi^'PP' ^'r
aus derWei-teein kla- gender Gesang; sah nun die Lich-ter scheinen.den
^p i
p-p^P' M P^P
^
te
"^'
^
pr r r p'fpr r r pTpt r r
m. -* 4- ~4 «- 3 <I
-# -# #
THROUGH-COMPOSED FORM 357
f p-
pp-p^r ^pic'p p-pr
schwarzen Lei-chenzug, fing bit- ter an zu wei-nen,weil
pM r
man,
^»p p
well man sein
p
^m^ 4
I I
-V
i ii
crcsc. ^f
i'' T r r? ^ Jun^jurM)
I^^TiJV^iy
-^j'liii^- * S' d d -
'
tee|. te
w
^^2-p
*!
I #^^ I i ^
sy i?p
iV «i^^^ ^"^ li i J
F *
P^
^ t=^ f)ij).
;>;)j)r j^F'
i^^^g' ^Y 'i^
To- tengraber kam, und gab der Er-de wie-der,was Gott aus sel-ber nahm.und
ii^ ^^ 5 i » •# r -0
% —y^ 0'
J'J'JT
f!0- S'
gab der Er-de wie-
I
J'P
(T*
der, was
J' P p-
M#tfl s t
i«
l|J iJ 4 f^^ -0 -0
^
3
Etwas geschwinder
^' '
' p
i
Da schwieg
r~p cjp des Jiinglings
i
r'
Kla
M
ge, und
ic;p
be
^
tend ward sein
p
^^
- -
^ i * g
*' g
PP
^
Hi* .
M
^-^
J i>J ^ ^
r
358 THROUGH-COMPOSED FORM
Blick,
^^
p If
sah
p cjMf
schon am schbnern Ta -
^
ge des
p
i
Crp ^
Wie der sehens
p 1
^
Gliick.
'
'
p
Und
i
S
i ^ *H^ E
J ^J ^
r r r r
y r Pc;f'
wie die Sterne
^
ka- men, der
p
i
'f
Mond
PCJNf
her auf- ge- schifft,
r
da
pif
las
p[j i
er in den
* ^
'
m Ji f J J' *rjj
^
1=1
'J ^ J JiJ J i
r r r r r
t
^ ^^
^ ^
y
i r ^ p 'LTp 2 r p ir" ^
I
t
Ster -
^ S
nen der Hoffnung ho- he
^
Schrift, der Hoff nung
ar J ^J ^ J ^ J ^ ^=^^=^ J ^J ^ ^jg^
r r r r
^ r7^
^^ ^
i ho he Schrift.
i i i I f 1 f^ 1
pr ^^Ff
«
r p
r
J J'j ^ j=
r
t^
^
*<t *
f p f p
v:;
The key basis of this song, e Cg G, is a crucial factor in the delineation of the
whole design. The use of both g minor and G major contributes to the unity of
the composition, through retention of the single tonic, and counterbalances the
contrast of tex*;ural change.
The potentials of this design principle are limited only by psychological prac-
THROUGH-COMPOSED FORM 359
^ ^
n
f
^ f
'^ i
V r > ^
w ^^ ^ E
{^^r^-^
i
^m^ -0-
y ,
J) y
^ ^^^
P
^^^e ^
•^ l-z^Z-J--'^^^
wr^*5t*v*it*
P ^^^ i
^ "27
i 'l-zJ
When these digressions are set in close proximity, as here, the result is a fusion of
different but complementary elements, obscuring the more distinctive qualities
of each. It is probably more accurate to designate compositions in which elements
of both major and minor appear consistently as major-minor.
Any chord containing notes whose tonal roles are flexible may appear as a muta-
tion. Both the mediant and submediant are particularly suited to mutation be-
cause their roles in the establishment of tonality are less direct. As mutated chords
they add to the harmonic spectrum by making available sonorities that are more
closely associated with other tonal contexts.
In Ex. 22-4 the elided phrases represent a prolongation of V. The deceptive
harmonic activity in measure 4 is intensified by the appearance of the B-flat major
chord, a mutation of the diatonic vi. It is created by the chromatic inflection of
the third and sixth scale degrees, and changes the root and fifth of the triad by
lowering them a half step.
Ex. 22-4. Haydn: Quartet, "The Lork," Op. 64, No. 5, III.
VI.
VI.
Via.
Vc.
THROUGH-COMPOSED FORM 36?
Like its diatonic counterpart, the 'VI prolongs harmonic activity when it is
used to create a deceptive cadence. The difference we perceive between ^VI and
vi in the deceptive cadence is the result of several factors: (1) a major triad is
heard instead of a minor triad, (2) the root motion is by minor second rather than
major second, and (3) the inflected third and sixth scale degrees bring into play
regional chromatic activity.
The non-cadential use of 'VI is the same as the diatonic vi. If "VI is directly
preceded by I, as in Ex. 22-5, both the diatonic and the mutated spellings of the
third scale degree are juxtaposed. Furthermore, the resultant root relation is a
major third. This combined with the chromatic inflection produces a third rela-
tion.
I W I W
Third_relation is present when chord roots are ^thnd^part. If the chords in-
volved have two tones in coinnioii, a diatonic third relation exists, e.g., vi IV; on
the other hand, if the chords inxoKccl oiiK ha\c one tone, or no tones in common,
a^ "ch romatic" third relation exists. In a ll chromatic third re ations one of the
l
lowered sixth scale step, both as root of ''VI and as the third of iv in the cadential
pattern.
The mutated mediant also is used in the same way as its diatonic equivalent.
To create the ''III, both the root and the fifth of the mediant triad are lowered
one half step. Like the ''VI, ''III adds another sonority to the harmonic palette.
When'' III precedes V or V, a cross relation may be created by the juxtaposition
of both the diatonic and the mutated seventh scale step, as in the second measure
of Ex. 24-6.
li^f^J ^m ^J B t— tL ±i^
^^^
At: I
*^
VI
f
¥^=^
IE
1
V^
The 'III chord is in third relation to both I and V, and has one tone in common
m r
^^
with each. The ''III also bears an interesting relation to ''VI because of their root
relation of fifth and their common tone.
The purely coloristic roles of ''VI and ''III are sometimes regarded with less
attention than their structural roles. In Ex. 22-7 the cadential activity is heightened
by introducing ''VP before proceeding to tonic.
Ex. 22-7. R. Strauss: Die Nachf, Op. 10, No. 3. By permission of the International Music
Company, New York.
zt
^
rtft?
33
zr ^
Bt: bVI
At first glance it appears that the cadence is both deceptive and authentic. How-
ever, after the harmonic motion is completed, ''VP is heard as a chromatic em-
broidery around I that is reminiscent of non-chord activity.
Earlier in the chapter we stated that consistent use of mutated chords produces
THROUGH-COMPOSED FORM 363
the cfTect of elements mixed from two different tonal sources. The process we have
seen thus far has always involved juxtaposition. In some rare instances the mixing
involves both juxtaposition and superposition, as in Ex. 22-8.
Renewed 1958. Copyright & Renewal assigned to Boosey & Hawkes Inc. Re-
vised Version Copyright 1948 by Boosey & Hawkes Inc. Reprinted by per-
mission.
Such stacking of diverse elements carries the practice of mixing to its logical further
development.
Ex. 22-9. Bartok : String Quartet No. 2, I. Copyright 1920 by Universal Edition. Renewed
1948. Copyright & Renewal assigned to Boosey & Hawkes Inc. for the U.S.A.
Reprinted by permission.
Violin I
P dolce
Violin n
e ^m P dolce
i
fi £ r^^
Viola
M pizz.
i
r r r #
Cello
m tr
se: 3
iU- T-
9—
T»
m.
2 2
1
Harmonic Sequence
O O ^J C3H m
<i *Jn ^-O^ o XE \\i\i\\\'^\\^ ^W ft n
^^W JCE
^&w
an. 8«««g'
g
u on""Qoo
--0- IE
TlvvTu i\'
zaz
V I
g
I Vi III I V III VII°V I
m
m^^-^ jCX EC
I6i\'v0°6iii vjeii
O O
3X IX -&-
v^i
I II I I 1 I
In Ex. 22- 10a the sequential pattern involves root movement up a perfect fourth,
and its repetition a third lower. The pattern is broken here when root movement
by a second appears between IV —V.
In Ex. 22- 10b a pattern involving roots related by thirds is the basis for a har-
monic sequence. Each unit of the sequence is connected by roots in a fourth rela-
tionship, and the interval of repetition is a third above.
THROUGH-COMPOSED FORM 365
First inversionand root position chords are alternated in Ex. 22- 10c. The result-
ing root pattern is by fourths and fifths. Notice that in a harmonic sequence the
leading tone "functions" like a root. Consequently, the vii% chord is assigned a
status determined by its prime relation to the other diatonic triads.
Each of these illustrations involves a pattern of only two chords. The number of
times a pattern is repeated is a matter determined by compositional necessity. One
pattern might become dull if repeated too often, while another bears repetition
because of its melodic and harmonic interest.
The phrase in Ex. 22-1 1 opens with a harmonic sequence. The pattern is based
on the chord root relationship of a fourth and a bass pattern of root movement up
a fifth. The melodic sequence is carried out between two different parts and is
continued through measure 3, but the harmonic sequence ends in measure 2.
Andante
$
t
V (^ i^
(J^) % % % <
^
^
P# ^^
^
i
s^^^^ ^
mm m m
'y-'i!
<''
W¥=^
J'l
Bt: VI
The harmonic pattern treated sequentially in Ex. 22-12 contains three chords.
The root pattern up a fifth, down a second the interval of sequence is a minor
is ;
third below. Since a minor dominant chord is used, the sequence contains only
minor chords, whereas the initial statement contains only major chords.
366 THROUGH-COMPOSED FORM
^ 5;
'^ ^
-<9^
^
^
3
12
'^^'i^
ji im
m ^ w £ ^p
At I
I IV vi (V)
In Ex. 22-13 both root position and first inversion chords are used. The sequen-
tial pattern begins with vii°, indicating again that the leading tone functions as
a root when it appears as part of a harmonic sequence.
Harmonic sequences often involve non-diatonic chords. When this occurs the
repetition can be the harmonic replica —equal sonorities as well as duplicated root
relations —of the original statement. In Ex. 22-14 the basic harmonic pattern is
Ex.
f^^
^^
22-14. Beethoven: Plana Sonata, Op. 81a,
^^ ^^^
III.
gf^ ^
^m *
Eb: I yi
^
Ex. 22-15. Puccini: // Tofaorro.
m MM ^
O e ter - ni. in - na
f^
mo - ra - ti , buo
^m
na sera
b
§S ^! f y
JL
1
m
F P
In other contexts a series of parallel chords creates variety w^ithin the phrase,
as in Ex. 22-16. In both Ex. 22-15 and Ex. 22-16 parallelism incorporates first
inversion chords exclusively.
Ex. 22-16. Brahms: Variations and Fugue on a Theme by Handel, Op. 24, Variation !.
f .^-^-^m M imif^itir ^
B\>: V I
Z^
V 16 ije lije IV6 v6 IV6- V6 I
Ex. 22-17. Debussy: Preludes, Book I. "Les sons et les parfums tournent dans I'air du
soir." Permission for reprint granted by Durand et Cie., Paris, copyright owners,
and Elkan-Vogel Co., Inc., Philadelphia, agents.
^P H^ ^^^=^=^=^-
mm r M H Hi i
f a i , |i| im n
Ex. 22-18. Bartok Mikrokosmos, Book V, No. 139. Copyright 1940 by Hawkes
: & Son
(London) Ltd. Reprinted by permission of Boosey & Hawkes Inc.
^ ^m
^
ty-^-
m w ^m #
p
m
f
^^ 5
'rrt
i m
More complex motion found in Ex. 22-19.
sonorities are involved in the parallel
In this passage the parallel chords move above an ostinatd^ figure. Both factors
parallelism and ostinato —
create a static harmonic rhythm.
FU.
Horns
^^
f^A > k^^
^^ %^
Bass Clar.
Vic. m^'r ^j j
- ^
- »
Exercises
For more detailed assignments see Materials and Structure of Music I, Workbook, Chapter 22.
1. Make a detailed analysis of Ex. 22-1. What other patterns than those mentioned in the
chapter are used to unify the composition?
2. Write a 16 measure through-composed composition for piano that uses a key scheme
such as the following: G-flat major, parallel minor, G-flat major.
3. Spell and sing mutated chords (i, iv, III, VI) in all major keys.
4. Listen to compositions such as Brahms' i?/!a/;iO(/)', Op. 1 19, No. 4. Discuss the use of mutated
chords.
5. Locate, sing through, and analyze several through-composed compositions.
6. Create various harmonic patterns that could be treated sequentially. Use one of the
examples in this chapter as your model.
Binary
Form
23
music, ensemble and solo instrumental music, and keyboard works of other periods
as well, including the current era. Furthermore, the principle of two-part organiza-
tion is often found as the basis for sections of compositions as well as entire pieces.
The themes of variation movements, for example, are commonly ordered in
binary form.
Example theme from a movement whose sub-
23-1 shows the statement of the
sequent variations retain the structure, harmony, and melodic outline of the
opening and its accompaniment. Note that the passage is arranged in two eight-
measure periods, A and B, each of which is repeated. It is common to regard the
fi* second section as a logical "answer to" or continuation of the first. A letter repre-
sentation of the form of the example would read AA BB. The letter B denotes con-
trast between the two sections, in this case involving rhythm, contour, harmonic
progression and texture.
370
BINARY FORM 371
.<rt/^UiA^
Ex. 23-2. Shostakovitch: Prelude No. 18, Op. 87. © Copyright MCMLV by MCA Music, a
division of MCA Inc., New York. Used by permission.
Moderate (J = 88)
/
^^^ ^^m ^ fl
PT C m
^^ ^
dim.
n.
-9
10
^nz:
i?±i
dim.
^ 15
^
- J.[> # *
^ i j I
'f~ T^
20
i i 1^^
^^? * jr r W^
cresc.
rr
^ f r
Adagio =
»
ritenuto (J 66)
355=^
^
ii
1p
^^ i
r*- i- » i,o
25
ra
s
j:^? sub i to accelerando
WZ,i-?-
^ ^. i
f
'^ !!'
^^
t\>Moderat o con primo (J = 88)
i^
^ ^^ />o<:o a
P
poco
c? W^ fcK^JPP
^
p^ %
9
21
^^ P
dim.
liJplil '
^^'' ^^^^J^
35
cre5c.
^ M i5?
-pj.
s :s:
BINARY FORM 373
/vvUrtu>^
')^
J' t
i Hi'
^g -1112 ^i^i^i
19-
g ^ id ^
^^
50
a
^
^S ll'
m Z2 ^ zz m S ^
zz
\fr
Many of our observations on the nature of this form will apply equally to other
compositions in two-part structure. It should be understood, however, that these
specific descriptions are based upon Baroque binary compositions such as Ex. 23-3.
Two general aspects are almost always present in this type of form: an obvious
parallel between the two sections in their use of thematics, and an absence of strong
d ivisiv e contrasts such as those found in the ternary form., A certain basic "same-
ness" of themes, texture, register, tonality, etc. often characterizes the binary form.
This quality is readily apparent to the listener. Both of these attributes are clearly
demonstrated in Ex. 23-3, which is a piano sonata by Domenico Scarlatti.
$
h 1 »
^^=^^
([iczc^Miz/^
^ 15
m m m
mi m^ n^
tr
i;^.'-"--
-^
rn^fin
. c.
20
±=^ ^ '\?ff^\^
jij ^ I
j^j
p
m^ jj
A ^ f*
m
F
Ai.
JJJ JJJ ,
B m m .
25
SP d ' ^ P
r,
%
BINARY FORM 375
30 t
y^ ^^^^ =
?
m^ ^
^
V ^ or- ^eMM?<^
^
k^jj^
f\^^\
^¥=i^
p' i;
n ^^ I
^ s^ i m ^m
Oa
^^
'^^'1
1
^ LiLr
^^
1r 40
\
p
(
^ ^^
D
. ...-fence /"^
^^
n
^^ ^^
45
iiU £ f^
AYOA^l^j
p # s ^^ ^
tt4r^t,ux1b
p P
S ^ ^ a ^ ^
P^
55
#-# — ^-(^
^ i J
tr #,tr
i
60
m m
^^ a #-^ !lp .£1
65
* i mw
it
^^ Fl I
376 BINARY FORM
^
-jl
''^
^^te ?
^^ ^^ . - •
(
^ ^
75 «^
^^ :^
^ ^ ^^ ^ fe^
80
P
^
^ t=m. ^-^-» q?^ ^>-m-^ 0'
f^^^ ^
V^"!
^^
!
^ ^J^J
85
l±±.
- ^^fr
f
^=±
a (beginning
J m V ^ m
^F
-- ll
^ ^
N ^
p
o: i, a P^
^ 1 jy
— * d — — U
1 1
J J. J
1
b (measure
SS
7)
*f P= = !^
W~^~W
i
c
i
(measure 29)
*=s
^
^
d (measure 35)
^
? ^^
^
The thematic outhnes of each section are very similar. Fragment (a) occurs in
much the same form at the beginning of each part (measures and 48). Similarly, 1
fragment (d) is the thematic basis for the last part of each section (measures 35
and 75). The principal difference between the two halves of the form is the order
of appearance of fragments (b) and (c) they are reversed in the second section.
;
Before leaving this example, however, we must consider the tonal design, a factor
equal in importance to the thematic outline. Examine this composition again,
analyzing for keys and the relationships between successive keys.
apparent that the key scheme of the two sections does not duplicate the same
It is
neat parallelism of the thematic design. Section A begins in the key of b minor,
changes to D major in measures 25 and 26, and remains in that key until the double
bar. The tonality at the beginning of section A', however, is not clearly defined (G
major and D
major are both possibilities), but it becomes stabilized in b minor,
arriving there in measure 55, and it remains there until the end of the sonata. The
tonal direction of the second section is reversed, in a sense, as the following diagram
indicates:
The apparent tonal indecision or instability at the beginning of the second sec-
tion is present in the majority of binary movements. In many cases it is not pos-
sible to rule out all but one key at this point. In the analysis of compositions
showing this characteristic tonal ambiguity it is sufficient to point out the general
key area.
The relationship between keys is somewhat different when the composition be-
gins in a major key. In that event the contrasting key is almost always the dominant^
(for example D major to A major). The minor key to relative major key progres-
sion of the Scarlatti is the rule, however, when the basic key of the composition is
minor.
To summarize the general characteristics of binary form, the thematic arrange-
ment of the two sections will be parallel, but the tonal direction will be reversed
in section two. This latter statement will apply almost without exception.
The above elements hardly exhaust the list of possible factors which contribute
to the perception of form in this or any other composition. Considerations such
as changes of texture, register, cadences, phrase structure, phrase extensions,
contrapuntal factors, sequences, melodic contour, harmonic implications, and many
others affect our mental image of the structure of a piece of music. These may not
all be of equal importance in any given composition, but all of them —-along with
countless other factors not mentioned — contribute in some degree to the total
obvious example occurs in measure 34 and again in measure 75, each bringing the
previous material to a terminal cadence. These mark the points of arrival for the
keys in which each of these sections concludes.
An element that is not readily apparent often serves as a unifying formal device.
Such is the case in measures 1-10 where the left hand part is an ostinato, repeating
the same two-measure fragment again and again. Similar figures appear in at least
two other locations in each section, particularly near the final cadence. Notice also
the use of augmentation in measures 7 and 8; both hands play the same melodic
fragment but in differing note values. Suggestions for further analysis of this com-
position will be given at the end of this chapter.
The main differences between the binary and ternary principles apply
following
virtually always to movements in these forms, Although all of these contrasts will
not apply to every composition, enough will be present to indicate clearly the
proper category.
2. Each of the two sections ends in a 2. Each of the three sections usually
different key than that in which ends in the same key in which it
it begins. begins.
Rounded Binary
A basic element of almost all musical forms is the return or restatement of opening
material near the close of a composition. This element, prominent in the ternary
design, is absent (except in terms of key return) from the binary form as described
above. In the Scarlatti Sonata, the opening theme does not return intact in the tonic
key. The only reprise of this opening pattern appears at the beginning of the second
section.
After the Baroque era some composers began to display in their binary composi-
tions traces of a partial or even complete restatement of the opening thematic ma-
terial near the end of the movement. Ex. 23-5 demonstrates how a movement that
basically is binary integrates this principle of a recapitulation.
380 BINARY FORM
Trio yr^-r
-^ f^v^^
go
Violin I
m^ mi ^m m ii 3: E
Violin n
P a ^
Viola
^ t?
» 1^
^
^B
Cellc ^ P
:v
S f
B ^ wm « p
70
ii^
i ^ ^ /'
^ ^ ^/ ^
m g ^ ^^ /
^ 3J
75 ^ns frr
i ^f ^
p
^
•
\
fS.
^ # ^
*j
W^ =^
o>
i?p
i^ =^^
i?P
1^^ f=FT i
# i i P PP
i
ft: r «. 3-c
. 80
s^ I '^ ^
^ ^
^
^ 85
1
g^ ^ ^ ^
i^
^J:
P
-T- •
^-t i ^
J- fl -14 J
BINARY FORM 387
$ *:
^P
90
^ i^^ ^ ^3^
1
i
fr\\^c>V
$ -9-^
i ^
^
')
-&-
J
w
3EE ^J 3E£
ii
3^ J3EE
3^=F
{ \
i 1 f
J-> 3^
95
^ P ^
-i 1
f
'
^
i.
^
^
^^ t?
f
P ^ ^P ^
D.C.
The first three of the four criteria suggested above apply to this composition.
Significant alterations have been made, however. The two sections are not approx-
imately the same length as in the Scarlatti Sonata; the second section here is more
than four times the length of the first. We also find a restatement of the opening
material in measures 93-100, where the mutation of the parallel major key returns
to minor. In contrast to measures 56-64, these^measures are not modulatory but
remain solidly in a minor. Another example of tonal instability occurs following
the double bar at measure 64. This additional factor contributes to the recognition
of the binary structure. The rounded binary form, of which this composition is a
typical example, must be viewed as a hybrid form. Although it reveals traces of
the ternary form, notably in the restatement it contains, it belongs clearly to the
binary category.
Ex. 23-5 is the middle part, or "trio," of the third movement of Haydn's "Em-
peror" Quartet, a movement that illustrates clearly the song form and trio princi-
ple.This formal arrangement was used frequently by composers of the Classical
period as one of the middle movements in large works such as the symphony.
382 BINARY FORM
String quartet, sonata, and concerto, and it was cast in the spirit of a dance or at
least a movement of a relatively light nature. The
and the scherzo and
menuet and trio
trio are the most frequent names given to these movements which are moments of
relaxation in the midst of the weightier and more serious movements that surround
them.
Actually this formal design incorporates two different small compositions (the
so-called "trio" is merely a second menuet or scherzo). The trio apparently derives
from the Baroque practice of lightening the texture in sections such as this, some-
times even to three voices; hence the name "trio."
Exercises
For more detailed assignments see Materials and Structure of Music I, Workbook, Chapter 23.
1. Listen to a recording of Ex. 23-3, noting each formal change and comparing the two
large sections. Analyze further for the following details:
a. Examples of repetition, sequence, imitation, phrase extension, motivic develop-
ment
b. cadence types
c. Points of contrapuntal interest
d. Points of contrapuntal sterility
2. Play Example 23-5 from the score at the piano. Analyze for the following points:
a. In what ways does example exhibit principles of effective counterpoint?
this
b. Analyze for phrase and period construction; analyze and label each cadence
jr^ by key, Roman numerals, and cadence type.
4. Following exercise 3 above, study a score of the movement, taking note once again of
the above points. Then make a harmonic analysis of selected passages.
5. Write a brief composition for piano that is cast in simple binary form. Make each section
no longer than sixteen measures, and retain a simple texture of two voices throughout
(prominent top line, accompanimental bass voice).
Secondary
Dominants
24
Andante
:) y J- J^
-
^ E»
p
^ 1
gffia
At:
T^
x
f
^ M i
i^ i:**'-d m
0-If )i^ i^^
K
(
^i ffi
f f
1 ±3;
VS V'/v
^
384 SECONDARY DOMINANTS
The d-natural is part of a melodic sequence begun in measure 6 that leads to the
focal point of dominant of A-flat major. The last chord in measure 7 is
e-flat, the
a Mm 7 chord on b-flat, the V7 of E-flat. Mozart here heightens the effect of the
cadence on the dominant by preceding the dominant with its own "dominant
seventh" chord. In other words, he employed a "secondary dominant"^ (more
precisely, a "secondary dominant seventh"), the symbol for which is V7/V.
It is interesting to conjecture why Mozart used this pattern for the second ca-
dence rather than for the first, or why he used different patterns at all. Probably
two different cadence patterns were used for the sake of variety, and the particular
order was established to sustain interest. Had the order of cadences been reversed,
the resultwould have been somewhat anticlimactic, for the point of maximum
harmonic tension would have been reached at measure 4 rather than at measure 8.
A further study of the second cadence in Ex. 24-) reveals other interesting facts.
Notice the third chord from the end. It is the dominant in second inversion, the
Vf. This chord and the two following constitute a progression (V^ — V^/V —V) that
resembles one of the most common cadential patterns, I^ —V^ — In both I. in-
stances the middle chord is the "dominant," which is preceded by its "tonic" in
second inversion, then followed by its "tonic" in root position. The cadence in this
— —
example may be designated as follows: V® V^/V V. The cadence could have
been accomplished through the use of V^ — V/V — V. The addition of the seventh
to the triad V/V which increase the expectation of
creates dissonant elements
resolution. Using this example as a point of departure, and from observations made
of musical practice, we may generalize about "secondary dominants" as follows:
1. Any diatonic major or minor triad may be embellished by its own dominant.
2. Such embellishment creates harmonic color and strengthens linear motion
(leading tone effect), thereby heightening resolution tendency.
3. Their presence sometimes indicated by the appearance of one or more chro-
is
4. The secondary dominant seventh, because of the added dissonance and the re-
solution tendency of the members of the tritone, stands in stronger relation to
its "tonic" than does the corresponding secondary dominant triad.
In this sense, the dominant of a key might be regarded as the "primary dominant.
SECONDARY DOMINANTS 385
^¥ ^
^m m^ ^ m
i u JZ
i
f
t^ m -*^ m M- mfe
0—0-
-.
;^
f f*
h i* -
J
II
>
m
•<•
H
VI 'Q^/i
vi
'
In
^
J3J,J
^^ JJJ I
?
In Ex. 24-3, composed some two centuries later, chromatic alterations are again
used for line inflection, resulting in a secondary dominant chord function.
m #um ^
Allegretto
Violin I
la*
Violin n
m ^ m i^
^ ^ ^
^ ^
P P
tr
'^
^
J3
Viola
^
^ f
t
f ^ ^m M ^
Cello
G:
/
^
^ pi ? VJ, V
^ ^ ^
386 SECONDARY DOMINANTS
It worth noting that second ary dominants are generally used to embellish
is
Ex. 24-4. Bach: Chorale, Cantata No. 140, Wachet Auf, Ruff uns Die Stimme.
3 i
/7\
331
i
T P^tM
r
Li i 1
9^S^
E^
1
m ^% V
g m P
I ^ ^ 1
A
di
ot
-o-
zaz
4 i
r
^
-«-
i i- J 1
--^i
n. 'rry i i
\ " ^Af ^
^ PP
I V4 . IV6 I VV, IV
I
ii7
f
s
It is two are
interesting to observe that of the three cadences in this excerpt,
on the dominant. In both instances these important structural points have been
emphasized by the use of secondary dominants. In so doing, Bach has stressed the
dominant tonal area, and in the third phrase he is faced with the necessity of re-
emphasizing the tonic in a convincing fashion. He by turning
accomplishes this
toward the subdominant through the use of its secondary dominant. In other
words, in the course of three phrases he stresses the upper dominant, the lower
dominant (subdominant) and finally at the end of the third phrase arrives at the
"mid-point," the tonic.
In seeking to create variety, composers are not always content to follow tradi-
tional patterns. We find secondary dominants used to obscure the tonic tern-
SECONDARY DOMINANTS 387
porarily, thus creating an added element of suspense. For instance, Ex. 24-5 begins
on a Mm^ chord with E as root (V^ of IV).
Andantino
3j
iV
^
VT
f.
m
T
Through hindsight we are able to hear the harmonic design and to establish
the tonal focus of the passage. This is not immediately apparent upon a first
m ^ ^
4 SSS 4
S
F: Vy
3 £: 3 3
W f ^
2
388 SECONDARY DOMINANTS
Allegretto
B'-
"Jl '/I I%1
It seems illogical to regard this as a series of fleeting modulations, particularly at
the indicated tempo. In this instance the simple root relations are still apparent,
but the so-called "functional" relationships are not so clear. As the study of har-
mony more complicated chromaticism, the analysis of such an
progresses through
example will become less difficult. For the moment, suffice it to observe the rapid
rate of harmonic change, the particular root relations, and the secondary dominants
appearing at the points marked, each of which resolves to its respective "tonic" or
"tonic substitute."
From Ex. 24-7 we may generalize that to^ establish s eq uences of secondarv
domin ants which each "tonic" successively becomes a "dominan t" of the n ext
in
chord will weaken tonal stability. This is especially true if the sequential pattern
is maintained so loi;ig that an overabundance of non-diatonic chords is introduced.
This very factor made the harmonic sequence a popular modulatory device, for
example, in the Classic period. A pattern of chord motion could be established and
maintained until the original tonality had been obscured. Then, upon arrival at
the desired new tonal region, a cadence pattern would establish the new tonality.
altered note (the sharped fourth degree of the scale which is the third of the chord),
will con tainJTie notes of the diatonic ii chord. As a matter of fact, it is helpful to
think of the V/V in both ways : as a major triad built on the second degree of the
scale, and as a variant of the supertonic chord (with its third raised). (See Ex. 24-
8.)
a) Major b) Minor
331 33=
-O- ini UI
e- o-
I _i
ii6
WV«/v
>V
true dominant. For instance, in Ex. 24-9a the V/V is preceded by a chord common
to the tonic (vig) and to the dominant keys (iig) in Ex. 24-9b, the V^/V follows ;
a) b)
Vmi tfi'
" n V 1
A. If
'f o o
r» Jf
r* f» **^
1
iffS
^K '^ Jj o
r» Vu
fyft
^ .r ;t X 1
IT ||o J
O o
^•< D **
o
r* H
M J%"
•
^5
•j.Jdtt
tt t^ o
r»
•!•
'' *^
*• O
i-i
11
1
A: Vi6 V^ V a :
16 V
E: iic V I e : iV6 V7 I
Although the approach to the secondary dominant indicated in Ex. 24-9 may
result in a smooth melodic/harmonic progression, such progression is not always
desirable or possible. Other cases may necessitate the creation of a chromatic
relationship between members of the secondary dominant and the preceding chord
one tone of the scale may appear in its natural form in one chord followed by its
chromatic alteration in the other.
m -e-
"0~ m 331
IV6 V4 V
Vv
390 SECONDARY DOMINANTS
IV6 V6/ V
In Ex. 24- 10a, note that the g is approached from the g-flat in the same voice.
The resuk is an unbroken melodic ascent to the fifth degree of the scale. In Ex.
24- 10b the g-flat appears in one voice and the g in another. This cross relation is
somewhat rougher in that both notes are prominently located, and their relation
is exaggerated by the fact that they do not appear in the same voice. Although the
cross relation is not always undesirable, it is usually avoided when a less dis-
jointed result can be obtained, as in Ex. 24- 10a.
In connection with melodic movement in secondary dominant progressions one
other observation should be made. It is often true that a chromat icallY_infl££j.f.d
note progresses in the direction of the inflection. That is, if a diatonic note is
raised by an accidental the melodic line tends to proceed upward the reverse is ;
true if the note is lowered by an accidental. In Ex. 24-11 this tendency is quite
apparent. Note that the /-sharp is followed by a ^; the/ by an e.
^
s
1 J
: Ge/obef
^U ^
seist du, Jesu Christ.
^^
i
r
4
^
This rule of "tendency" corresponds to the functional relations of tones within
the major-minor scale systems in that the leading tone (here represented by f-
sharp) progresses up and the fourth degree (here represented by /) progresses
down.
The principles of resolution of the V/V and V'/V are not different from those
of the V and V. The usual resolution is to their "tonic," that is, to a major chord
SECONDARY DOMINANTS 391
whose root stands in a fifth relation below. The raised fourth degree, the "secondary
leading tone" in the secondary dominant, generally progresses to the fifth degree.
In other words, it follows the tendency of the inflection. See Ex. 24- 12a. One ex-
ception occurs when the third of the chord (the inflected tone) proceeds to a note
a third below, that is, to the fifth of the chord of resolution. This usually occurs in
one of the inner voices (see Ex. 24- 12b) in order to provide a full triad for the
resolution chord.
b)
^m
a)
i "O"
m: jQI
nsz §^ K3L.
-*-Tr
JOZ
F: V7 f: V, V
'/v yy
The notes of the tritone in the V'/V usually resolve as do those of the V^. The
augmented fourth expands stepwise and the diminished fifth contracts stepwise.
(See Ex. 24- 1 3a). One exception to this appears where the third of the V'/V
proceeds to the fifth of V. (See Ex. 24-13b.) Notice that each of these resolutions
corresponds to the melodic progressions illustrated in Ex. 24- 12a and Ex. 24- 12b.
a) b)
::ii=
-» o nsz
t no
i "cr
m CT
^te^ CT
e: V7 V E: Vl
Vv '/v
Mozart: Symphony
^w
Ex. 24-14. in E-flat K. 543,
Allegretto
^^ h
^ ^ 0- i9-
Trm
^ m^
^^ ^^
— -d — l^-d- —
f — :^-
^3
i < I i
S ^ Vi6
n V7
'/v
^^
V
m m.
: ) i
Note the similarity of the cadence pattern (Vig V| V^/V V) to the cadence — — —
— —
"formula" previously discussed (iig 14 V^ I). This similarity becomes more
apparent when the two possible analyses of this kind of transient-terminal cadence
are compared.
P^ f
S fe
E^: Vi6 V6 V
V7 I)
As illustrated in Ex. 24-15 the V'/V is frequently "prepared" by the V^, and in
such instances, this chord is often preceded by vi or vig. In other contexts the
V^—V7/V—V pattern may be preceded by I, resulting in a harmonic progression
comparable —V^ —
to iv 1% I.
—— -
Allegro
i2.- »;
-^
i«ig 1
ffffi
-9-= >
gf^
U— hi
»—
3"
5-
31
p rH crf5c.
—— f -4 —- •
•
-•—
f
>.f-
' 1 m
-9
M.
-f- •-
.
i.-^y^'ij -J -0 1
'
-h
r p 1
- -J- f •
•
Bi*: I V| V7/^ V
[F: IV 16 V7 I ]
4
In the study of V and V we noted that the resolution of the chord might be
delayed momentarily by the interpolation of other harmonies, by suspensions and
by other means. This is also true of the resolution of the V/V, as will be illustrated
and discussed in subsequent chapters.
When used in the interior of a phrase, V/V or V'/V generally occur in connec-
tion with transient-terminal cadences. In such contexts they frequently resolve to an
inverted form ofV, thereby contributing to the forward movement of the harmony.
Note that in Ex. 24-17 the V/V is preceded by ii, its diatonic counterpart.
Andante maestoso
^ f
wmm ^ S ' It
zz
i ^p d ^m ' '^
T rT r 'r r
f
^/V^Jv ^S
The V/V and V/V occur somewhat less frequently in minor. Creating a major
triad ormajor-minor seventh chord by altering a ii chord involves two chromatic
changes, and in this respect the V^/V in minor is more remote from its key than
the V^/V of major. It is possible that composers frequently bypassed the V^/V in
minor for other sonorities more closely related, or they bypassed the tonic minor-
dominant key relationships for others such as tonic minor-relative major.
—
When we compare Ex. 24-18 and Ex, 24-19 with Ex. 24-17 we find that there
are no major differences in the use of V/V or V^/V in minor; they are preceded
and followed as in major. In Ex. 24-18, i precedes V/V, whereas ig precedes V^/V
in Ex. 24-19.
Moderate
m p
{^^i
dolce V P ^TT -SM-
^
^ P (*
^
VTt FfT
# P
^ff^ f iEE£
W^
s-
$
^TTT? i ??
nfTrfr
^ M*^
jr V7
r r
^5'v
Allegro maestoso
^ lii
jsr
^f
i^
i 1*
s^t t
J ^
f
^^ ^^ =^
v| i6 V7
^KV
It is not unusual to find V/V in minor used in conjunction with v, the minor
dominant. In Ex. 24-20 the V'/V is preceded by v and followed by V^ in the
approach to the cadence on tonic.
SECONDARY DOMINANTS 395
Andante
J
When working with V/V and V'/V in minor in a choral style, care should be
taken to avoid augmented intervals in the melodic lines containing the chromatic
inflections. In instrumental performance these melodic intervals are not as difficult
to produce accurately.
Inversions of V/V and V/V are used with considerable frequency, and generally
in complete form. They may be designated by the following symbols:
V/V; inversion,
.31 iiiv..iaiuii,
first VJV;
V 6/ V
, second inversions, V^/V.
\rTi\7. c_„*
V/V; first inversion, V|/V; second inversion, V^/V;
third inversion.
inversion, V|/V.
VilV.
I* i ^ ^ i ^
i s
r
T f f
^s i
fV
1
^
i i
fV4
^
^
A^-
r
/v
T
^
V6/
/v
V5/
i
vv
f
V6
ii
5/v
V4/
^
Pc:
3/v
^
:2te:
/
V
rV P ¥ ¥
f:
/v
V6
7v
V6
VV vv
/v
V6
/v 3/v
V4
Vv
396 SECONDARY DOMINANTS
As indicated in the previous chapter, the V/V and V/V often appear in con-
nection with the transient-terminal cadence. This is equally true of the inverted
forms. Inversions make possible a conjunct bass line that is more "melodic" in
character. To some degree, the use of inversions stresses the "temporary" nature
of the transient terminal cadences, since in terminal cadences the penultimate
chord is usually in root position, thus producing a more conclusive cadential effect.
In Ex. 24-22, IV precedes the —
cadence pattern V|/V V. The movement to V
is heightened by the chromatic ascent of the bass g-flat
—g —
a-flat. A smooth connec-
tion between IV and V|/V is possible because the third and fifth of the IV chord
{B-flat and D-flat here) are common to V^/V. In typical fashion, the cadence
chord, V, appears on the strong beat and is preceded by V|/V on the previous weak
beat. This cadence may be considered as a variant of the simpler pattern, IV V, —
here modified by the insertion of an intermediate V|/V. The introduction of the
chromatic leading tone (here g), and the presence of the dissonant elements in the
— —
Mm'' chord {g d-flat and e-flat d-flat) increase the sense of inevitable move-
ment to the cadence.
n 1 n
^m SP
iz^
^m is: 3
Ex. 24-23 contains another treatment of the V^/V. Here the chord appears in
second inversion preceded by Such an approach is quite smooth, for there are
vi.
notes in common between the two chords. As in Ex. 24-22, there is an accelera-
tion of harmonic rhythm in the approach to the cadence. Notice also the simple
step progression in the middle pattern, A-flat B-flat —
C D E-flat. — — —
Adagio cantabile
— ^ — — — —
^ ^^
Allegro
i f rm =
^
^^
if
^
mf 'f
ms W— at
?
1;^ J ^ J
w
2i'\, m rwtmW- r 1,; lu r -^1
_,L^ — y y
Z— ?-t 3-#
4? #? —#? ,
- ^^^
j-^, *- *-— -V#- t_?Z_ 1
J ^
. Kl-
J' ^
1
'y r
>
V4
Vv
Ex. 24-25 and Ex. 24-26 show inversions of the V'/V within phrases.
S
Moderate semplice
^ ^J = 96
^ f^
Tranquillo
398 SECONDARY DOMINANTS
fc^^^
^g
i
^8~
dim.
§
m jOZ
331
ini
ISt
un
m
^M
Allegretto
p
iS;
m m
^ £ # ,
i
p
^ ^m
?
p
?
Vi6 V§
s/v
V^ Vi ii6 I| V I
The V|/V in the fifth measure of this excerpt is clearly outlined, having been
preceded by a fleeting suggestion of vig and followed by V. This in turn is followed
by a succession that cadences on the tonic, B-Jiat.
The dominant is frequently the tonal goal of a phrase or larger melodic section.
As previously indicated, the tonal movement to this goal is often intensified by the
inclusion of secondary dominants, for example, V/V. This relationship, in turn, can
be strengthened by the addition of other secondary dominants which precede the
V/V, for example, the V^IV and the V^ii.
SECONDARY DOMINANTS 399
Andante
p ^m J^ ji^''^^
ll
1^ I
T3—\
&7:sj^
% i
')Wi\ \ ^^ i ^
V7/ IV V6/. V
^IV 5/v
The domi nant seventh of the subdom ina nt (VjIV; V^/iv) is a rhord whose
ro ot is th e tonic. Its appearance in minor requires a change of the tonic chord
from minor to major, thereby creating the leading tone of iv.
a) b)
tf 3i:
i
4t
W W e-
* ife ^ T^ ^
Ml 331
-e- ll^
_Q_
s _Q_
331 -H-
-o-
E: V^/
/rIV
V6.
^IV 47IV ^VlV
c : VV,
/iV 3/i
V4/
ViV
V4
2/iV
/
Arr
i^^D
tonic" relationship is not so evident between the major tonic triad and its sub-
dominant until a minor seventh is added to the former, thus creating an unstable
sonority on the tonic.
^
Ex. 24-30. Handel : Piece for Harpsichord.
m
fct
^=f^
r
-tSl-^
^ 5
r^
^ -zn
^
\^ J
l t J
^ fi
«
,i
^ 33
^S
^'/iIV
^i #
IV
^ ^# ^ ^
400 SECONDARY DOMINANTS
In normal resolution the dissonant elements (minor seventh and tritone) are
treated as were those of the V'/V. That is, the tritone as an augmented fourth
generally expands stepwise, just as a diminished fifth contracts stepwise. Excep-
tions are made in order that the chord of resolution be a complete triad (see Ex.
24-3 lb), or because of other considerations, such as a desirable voice doubling
(see Ex. 24-3 Ic).
^ -o- :xx:
-e- ice:
fc-&- EEJI
zee:
o
i F^o=
(irregular)
$ _ti_ >o-
L[
+4
|
"-» o
(irregular)
i
^^
«^^"^
o
-»o -o=
o
-e-
^
_Q_
an m ti ti
3s;
^=^
^TT
F: V7/ IV V7/. IV b: VO iV G: V4/ IV V4 IV6 a: V4/ iV6
'/iv '^/iv /iV •^/^ TiV
w^
^a
Presto
y
ma non troppo
i
t§^
m ig ^m
±±
V4
m ^±k^-±L^
IVO
ff 5
16 V
^
/^ 4
On the other hand the V'/IV also occurs as in Ex. 24-33, at the beginning of
a section before tonic has been established. Although tonic is not readily apparent
—
at the beginning of this exerpt, it becomes clearly established in the ensuing mea-
sures.
Andantino
^^ P
In Ex. 24-34, the subdominant is stressed by the V'/IV, and by the prominent
—
melodic use of the interval d g at the head of the motive appearing in measures
1 and 3. Any tonal ambivalence between d and g which may have arisen by
measure 4 is soon dispelled by the cadence pattern, iig I V^ — V—
I, in measure 7-8. —
Ex. 24-34. Bartok: Little Pieces for Children, Vol. I, No. 2. Copyright by Edwin F. Kolmus.
Reprinted by permission.
Molto sostenuto
^ dole
=iF te^ u
D :
%
V7/ ,
i
IV6 IV
^ ji
F' Vi iic
i ft^
V IV6
4
I
Because of the common pitch shared by I, V'/IV and IV (root, root and fifth,
respectively), these chords often appear in conjunction with a tonic pedal. This
402 SECONDARY DOMINANTS
procedure offsets the weakening of tonality that otherwise can result from the use
of a V^IV.
The V^/IV in Ex. 24-36 appears in conjunction with a shorter tonic pedal, and_
as in the previous examples gives way to the subdominant in second inversion.
Note the smooth introduction of the seventh of the V/V in the tenor, where it
^m ^
Ex. 24-36. Gounod : Faust, Act II, "Salut I Demeune."
Larghetto
^m^ *=i
f ^
*=*
I
p cresc. T^
^S ^M^ Ul iffi
\? ^ t>
pf^ i m
Yl IV«
4
/iv
Allegro (J = 80)
xt
^^
M.T.
W >i*^
mS -
-r4^n
^A-
f ^T^
te ^ra= # m j'^ }U}}
SECONDARY DOMINANTS 403
s i^=^
m ^ i=4
f
i i
^ i
9^<i{ r r
J
# ^
1 A
IV
J-
n/„
i
IV
Ex. 24-39 illustrat^es another cadential use, but in this instance the subdominant
is the penultimate' chord of a plagal cadence. The b-flat pedal underlines the first
two measures, above which a V^/IV appears. The V^/IV links the two forms of
the same sonority, IVg and IV, which occur at the phrase's high point.
w^w '^m^-^m
ms 5
WlTf
1 'JU 3W I h
5 \^
0\>t»\Z
I
1
i
^^ ^VtIV
p=^^
IVC (ivo) I
r=^
V8
s
Vk iveyt, IV
f
I
Viv
There are many other uses of this chord; obviously, any appearance of a sub-
dominant could be embellished by its dominant. The standard "blues progression"
of jazz cadences on a V^IV at the end of the first four-measure phrase.
404 SECONDARY DOMINANTS
Ex. 24-40. Arlen-Mercer : Blues in the Night. Copyright 1941 by Remick Music Corpora-
tion. Used by Permission.
^ Blues tempo
xr
1-3-
iy hni \ , h
r-5-i
mifm
X5~
r-3-i
s m p^
^ ^ pi ^ ^ii
£
¥
Bt:
The relation of V/ii is generally limited to the major mode, for the supertonic
chord in minor is diminished and does not function satisfactorily as a "temporary
tonic." Its^root is the sixth degree of the scale, and its presence is suggest ed by _the
appearance of the raised first d egree of the scale (leading tone of 2) that usually
resolves upward.
fefe lOI
zssz
ni =8= -(»)-
-e
-o
— fo)-
fo>-
I il Vi 3nr -e^ ^ jol:
n o
As indicated earlier, the presence of the raised first degree of the scale (leading
tone of 2) suggests the V/ii. In Ex. 24-42, the c sharp in the second measure is the
third of an arpeggiated A major triad which moves to D. Similarly, a secondary
leading tone is formed in the penultimate measure by the F-sharp.
3
^
^^
Allegro
zm
P P
XL.
f=-
^
mf
C:
fffHtf-
^gf#^
"/«
^ ^^ - M
ih-t
W V/
^
/v
•
•
V
-
^m
J#
a
SECONDARY DOMINANTS 405
The V/ii is almost entirely limited to the interior of phrases. It seldom appears
at cadence points, except rarely when there is a transient-terminal cadence on the
supertonic.
^^ w^ U^lU ^ g
IV
i w
i,t iJ ^.i
?
m f
y tf^
^ y
f ^ 1^
i
V7 V>ii
" V I v'/v V7
^p £?
^^ i
tr
Ma
f^^ ?
y 1 yjtl
± ^^
v§ 16
^^^¥^ ^
Andantino
#^-»
i i i
:2i
i
^ i i
nm
M-
¥
ii7 V9 V7 I
406 SECONDARY DOMINANTS
Although the V/ii is often preceded by I, other chords may be used for this
purpose. For instance, vi is used in the Haydn excerpt of Ex. 24-44, and IVg in
Ex. 24-45, in which the secondary dominants are treated sequentially.
(J^=116)
V4 , TVS V4 . iis
Viv Mi
An interesting use of the Vg/ii occurs in Ex. 24-46 where it appears as the open-
ing chord, again forming the initial part of a sequence (VJii — ii) that is completed
by the pattern Vg/I.
V8/. ii V6 I
CTIJ
It is not unusual to find the V'/ii used in sequential patterns such as, (V|/ii — ii)
Allegro
^
,jj'..trr,t7r
|
vrB
w
i #-i» —
f»
tt £^
• *
m *
V-iJ 'U p p 21 w
" Vi ivg vii°8
^3^ ^V^ 3
SECONDARY DOMINANTS 407
The V/ii is often used to prolong the action of the ii chord, as in the beginning
of the second phrase of Ex. 24-48. In this passage the V^/ii appears between suc-
cessive occurrences of the ii in the forms of and iig. This treatment parallels the
ii
first three measures of the first phrase, where I and Ig are linked by V^.
Allegro
Similarly, V/ii is used to predict the arrival of ii. However, in this instance a
progressive cadence occurs on V/ii. In such a context, the impression of actual key
change is sometimes considerably stronger than in the V^/V transient-terminal
cadence.
m rr i P
^
^
pp
i? ^ ^
Vii
f ' w * w
In many instances the temporary "tonic" of the V/ii is altered, so that it too
becomes a secondary dominant (V/V). In Ex. 24-50 V|/ii progresses to V'/V,
which in turn resolves deceptively.
408 SECONDARY DOMINANTS
^m
Ex. 24-50. Chopin: Nocturne, Op. 9, No. 2.
Andante
0.
fe mP P- .
ps* » # * — -"
^C
^fg r^^yt £f f^fhfe >fr
'^^"i, Ir cP
O'D^ ^ tP
i „ . „ -
#
C^ vy V
vi V4/V> V7
'S/i ^>v /v M\ /v
Ex. 24-51. Mozart: The Magic Flute, Act II, "March of the Priests."
Andante
sotto voce
t ^m * i
f
%
71
9- -m-
^V.
However, it is followed not by ii but by its own dominant, the V'/vi. This is then
followed by V|/ii which resolves to iig.
'^m
Andante
ppp
F
f
i J-.
TTTf
„ J] ^
r »c;"^ r
^
^
i
dolcissimo
H-
^z. 21
Vi7 VT V7
/v
— :
3;=
-o* M 3
i =3
qg
^^ 331
331
-e-
^^
^«-
w
311
-^^ — rxsz
T^
1
Vt W7
/V
V4
V4
2/-
iio Vii°i
' J 4
often u sed in phrase interiors in connection with some form, of the basic progression
i i — V'^
I. and in many instances it is approached throughj. (4) Xh.e usual les-
oj^ution is to supertonic, but occasionally to \' l\L (5) In rare instances it appears
as a cadence chord, or even as the first chord of a phrase:- (6) In other typical
tr^tments it may serve tO "prolong" the supertonic or appear in various sequentiaF"
patterns. (7) The V/ii° rarely occurs in the minor mode, a use that would require
mutation of the supe rtonic chord (thus y/ii).
Exercises
For more detailed assignments see Materials and Structure 0/ Music I, Workbook, Chapter 24.
1. Find examples of the use of secondary dominants in the music you perform.
2. Scan new music rapidly, looking for chromatic inflections as possible indications of
secondary dominants.
3. Indicate the major and minor keys in which a-c-sharp-c-f might appear as a .secondary
dominant seventh, and the appropriate identification of each.
4. Practice spelling V/V and V^/V in various major and minor keys.
5. Consider various pitches as root, third, fifth or seventh of V"/\' and spell the chord and
resolve it to V.
6. Use a V'/V in a transient cadence in a four-part choral texture.
7. Use a V'/V in the interior of a phrase for piano.
8. Compose two phrases of music using Ex. 24-1 as your model. Employ at least one V/V
(or (V^/V). Arrange for four woodwinds.
9. Analyze some of the following, indicating keys, chords and non-harmonic tones
Haydn: Piano Sonata in D major. III (1-8)
Mozart: Piano Sonata in D major. III, var. 12, (1-9)
,f
25
Secondary
Dominants
There are two basic forms of the submediant chord, vi in major and VI in minor,
with each having a related secondary dominant whose root is the third degree of
the scale. The V/vi (major mode) requires that the fifth of the major scale be
chromatically raised. The c-sharp m Ex. 25-1 is the third of a chord whose root
is a, the third degree of the /major scale. The chromatically inflected note, func-
tioning as a leading tone to d, is an important member of a major-minor seventh
chord that has a dominant relation to the submediant.
Poco adagio
^^ p'
?r
^
^m ^^
^^ i r^^n^-
I
nig
f
412 CONCLUSION OF SECONDARY DOMINANTS
There are no new problems of resolution for the V'/vi, because it resolves the
same way as each of the secondary dominants discussed earlier.
LMi S
*
^^
C: V V7 Vg/ Vi
yvi
Pf
Allegro
^
I
J-
4
WAk 4
^m^ J ,jTJ
^ f
n
C:
p^ p Ie
V6V
^^ Vi
i^
V/ V
^
i
p
iRl
/Vi /v
Ex. 25-3. Bartok : Little Pieces for Children, Vol. I, No. 9. Copyright by Edwin F. Kalmus.
Reprinted by permission.
m > \i n m
s
p
^ lis ir
D: V7 ^' V7 V7.
^%i yrv
CONCLUSION OF SECONDARY DOMINANTS 413
t ^ 4t=i:d
sR-
IV
Sehr Lebhaft
mr ^ 0-m-
i t i=y s ,i^=M
^=^ $
r nf
*^
nI i
t^ «-
^^^M i
^ ^m ^ ^^ l>y J., ij j
I i
i
*j it
m/^ dim.
f=f
EI
^— &
V4/ Vi V7
>V
In the minor mode no chromatic alterations are required for the triadic form
V/VI, but the seventh chord (V7/VI) necessitates the lowered second degree of the
scale.
* ^ 331 ff
a: in VI VI
"'M
In measure 6 of Ex. 25-6, the b-Jiat creates the seventh of a chord that embel-
lishes the submediant.
414 CONCLUSION OF SECONDARY DOMINANTS
Andant e es pressivo
ti
^ J fl i i
mf
f=TT f
'imn TfTTl
m P i6
^ ^ . ,.rri>
iife
T dim. r
if-
i
if- -^
i
VI
Sf^z^ fa^P
r
* =g
r
S
^Vv,
a^
Allegro
I m
fmfT
«-^ \
j^
^^
WTf
mf
4' 0-
¥ m p-if
n
> >
i
A A A '^
Ex. 25-8. Brahms: Liebeslieder Walzer, Op. 52, No. 8, (vocal parts).
^^ ^
Slow waltz
^=^ ^^ si
J-J ^ ^ f
JJ ii ill
')ah>ii I r r
p^ f^ p
V
Vii°6 ii Vi6
In the next quoted passage the submediant chord is prolonged through its repe-
tition and its attendant secondary dominant. The result is the creation of a mo-
mentary tonal "cell" on vi prior to the progressive cadence on V.
^
:
Violin I
mi m
^mU
Violin n
^ SI
Cello 'r-mi i i
W
P 3
^^
^ ^
P Vi
V/vi W V/„. Vi
^
§
-iS> (^ -e-
A cr
^^ 331 ^
Ptt -o-
—
sF¥^
Menuetto
i
/
^^ ^^ ^j^i
^m k
^y
4
V7/_. IV
'>iv
mmn m C3 i
j^
I r
* e
^ V6
^^
I
^« m-
^
V7 I
In another similar treatment a V|/vi proceeds to V'/ii the effect of the dominant ;
$
Andante
e^
i>P
is: 04^u
m ¥'l^ «;?
m^ Tt
Sm ZEE
331
m ^^ — ^*F
• •
i a P
V7/ IV? V4, V7/ V4^ 116 ii V7 Vi ill V7 I
/IV ^
Xi /" ^ ii
degree of the scale. Its presence in major is suggested by the appearanc e of the
raised fifth degree of the scale, in minor by the lowered second degree. This sec-
ondary dominant frequently appears at the head of or within a phrase, pi
CONCLUSION OF SECONDARY DOMINANTS 417
It is often involved in sequential passages in wjhuch it sometimes prec edes the V/ii.
As is true of the submediant chord, the mediant chord usually appears in one
of two forms, iii in major and III in minor. Each has a related secondary dominant
whose root is the seventh degree of the scale. To introduce a V^/iii (major mode)
requires the chromatic alteration of two diatonic tones; the second and fourth
scale degrees both must be raised a semitone. In minor, the root of the V'/III is
the subtonic of the scale.
a) b)
^ ^
a -e- 331
i
rjsi
* ~D"
w
P5
F:
;n=
L
I
-e-
V7
L.T.
— 1
(of-
3x:
III
^& Ty
V7/
ZSSl
ICC
n
>fi
III '^i
By raising the second scale degree (major mode) the leading tone of iii is created.
The raised fourth scale degree permits a perfect fifth between the root and fifth of
the chord (see Ex. 25-12).
Ex. 25-1 3 contains a passage in which V'/iii follows Vg. Notice that this particular
form of the progression uses the common tone between the two chords as a kind
of pedal bass.
^m **
^
^ *-M
^m
1
f
^m m V4
3/..
III
^^ E
m^
The inverted forms frequently occur in the course of a phrase and thus minimize
the strong root progression. In Ex. 25-15 the V^/iii is used in a short sequential
pattern, measures 3-4 being a melodic-harmonic sequence of 1-2.
Allegro molto
1 PS IS
m
zc,
Pt ~w- 9-
s p
^ ¥=*
^ =!»
^ P
m^
V "iAi V/ii v^i III
B
i-
# i ^E^
1r
^ f
^
')\ r
III
i >
7^
^X;
§. ^ V7'/y
n
fV
£E£
17. In each instance the period closes with a transient-terminal cadence on iii,
followed by an immediate return to tonic upon the repetition of the period. Com-
pare the treatment of the first and second phrases of both examples. Also note that
in both of these examples the V/iii appears in root position, thereby creating a
more decisive cadence on the iii chord.
Allegro
t
m
^ ~»—
If^^ #
i P
V^/.. Hi
III
Allegro moderate
fc
^ ^
1=J
'W~w
P?re
f=« :s k
f=i
£ S
•-•-•-^ 21
is:
^
^t# ^-4-»
% % % % i^iiiii
f pp
^ m~0-
r \1S ^^^t^ -
Vv,
V/.. V/.. V/
Vi /ii /ill /vi ^/1i ^'/v
^.y. IS
i
"XT
e/':
IE
pp
za.
J- i
-e-
^
In a different context the dominant of the mediant might occur in connection
with a transient cadence on (major) III, again exemplifying the mutation of its
"tonic." In Ex. 25-19 the III does not function as a secondary dominant, for it
5^S m
IV V|^,
® 16
As previously mentioned, the root of the V/III in the minor mode is the subtonic
scale degree, hence a conventional fifth relationship exists between the roots of
V^III and III.
CONCLUSION OF SECONDARY DOMINANTS 421
Andante
^ "^ —r^
ft^
^^ F
'ij> f-T p
^
'H\?<'' \
"
t
VS
^Vm
In Ex. 25-21 the V'/III is so prominent that its role in this transient-terminal
cadence on III becomes that of supporting dominant in a more structural sense.
The pattern of harmonic movement introduced in the first measure is interrupted
by a two-measure appearance of V^/III, followed by III, which persists for four
beats. It is the extension of the phrase its fifth measure —
that accommodates the —
harmonic assurance that C is not, after all, to be regarded as a new tonic. The
V" chord ably impresses this upon the listener during the final two beats of this
fifth measure.
Voice
m^m Chri - ste e lei -
^son
r
nri
-^ P^P
ste e
Piano
^
a: i6 Vg
^^Im
i
""'A m
zpi
^
lei - son Chri-ste
t t t t t ^ i iit i i M '
^ttt
'^^> J y U^ ^fe^ ^=ff=^
P
m W Vi V^ I
422 CONCLUSION OF SECONDARY DOMINANTS
The V'/III in Ex. 25-22 is used in the approach to the transient-terminal ca-
dence on III, measure 4. If the subsequent phrase continued in G, the entire first
phrase would represent the modulatory pattern that first establishes the new key.
m P=^
n -=
ty
if a :$
f*=F
m I
m1 ^
r
s e: V V42
iaI
i6 V6. IV V?/ HI V7
i
i iV ii6
:
i6
J
V7
\
K
fi
5X,, /TTr 4
^/iv
Because the \"'III in minor is identical to the V" of the relative major, it fre-
quently is used to exploit this "duality." In Ex. 25-23 the V^/III supports a tran-
sient-terminal cadence on III at the end of measure 4. The harmonic resources
of ^ minor have been expanded here by this vacillation between these two closely
related keys, each tonic is in turn supported by its own dominant. Since the passage
does begin and end in g minor, the fleeting focus on B-Jiat as temporary tonic is
best regarded as more a prolongation of the mediant than a true change of tonic.
i^3
^
9—t
Andante
^fcji
?
t ^ ^ f-^' • '
?
O '
•
^
m
4-^
S ^ J'r ^gr p r g.ij^^
s -0 —0-
-^z^
-0—0-
-»-^
-0—0-
-^-^
-it
m d
ni m iis V7
"'/w
CONCLUSION OF SECONDARY DOMINANTS 423
Moderate
Vtin in ii°B^iV7) V V4
3
i'J io
4
V7 i
A number of important points have been made about the dominant of the
mediant. It is a chord whose root is the seventh degree of the scale —leading tone
of the majo r mode, subtonir of the minor mode. Tn major, two rhrnmafi<;;. altera-
on the mediant in minoi", the progressive qadence (or so-called '"Phrygian Ca-
derue""; in major; in progressions in which the mediant chord itself is altered to
become a secondary dominant (\'/vij or to exploit the inherent duality betw een
;
This survey of secondary dominants would not be complete without brief men-
tion of the V/VII in minor. Since the leading-tone chord (vii°) is diminished and
therefore never functions as a temporary tonic, its secondary dominant is not used.
However, a major on the seventh degree of the natural minor
triad (VII) built
scale (subtonic) may be embellished by its dominant, the V^/VII. In Ex. 25-25
note that the V^/VII is used in a sequential pattern that begins in measure 3 and
terminates in measure 6. Observe also the jolting effect of the root relation (tritone)
in the progression V^/l>n —
V'/V, in measures 6-7, in spite of the very smooth
—
motion o^ g-flat g-natural in the melodic line.
424 CONCLUSION OF SECONDARY DOMINANTS
Cantabile
^m /''III /S.VI An /v
P ^ S * 1 & m
r f
i it
s f T^ I
f r
k
m3 f:
F: VJ
^
I« V«, iii»
i
vi
f
10 V V4 16 Yl V6/
^
Vi
^
16 V
In Ex. 25-26 the V|/V is resolved "deceptively," to chords whose roots are a step
above that of the secondary dominant. In (a) the chord of resolution is iii^, and in
CONCLUSION OF SECONDARY DOMINANTS 425
(b), V*/vi, the root of each being the third scale degree. This is but one type of
irregular resolution; others will be discussed later in this chapter.
Deceptive Resolution
deceptive resolution of the dominant chord are applicable. Recall that the fifth
of the secondary dominant is sometimes omitted, and that the third of the chord
of resolution frequently is doubled. In addition, care must be exercised if parallel
fifths and/or octaves are to be avoided. The dissonant elements are treated as they
are in regular resolution: the tritone, (between the chord's third and seventh)
usually expands an augmented fourth, contracts if a diminished fifth. Thus the
if
chromatic "leading tone" moves to its "tonic," even though this "tonic" is no longer
the root of the chord of resolution.
(a)
«»=^ J J J -e-
i^ -&-
t ^ T=^
-e-
I
m D:
^ I
y.
v;iv " V
-e-
g: i vy^
i^
m V4
i
^
(c)
J-
^ i
^
ZSSl
-o-
(d)
i
fei i
frf w-
^^
i
P 1
^8^
^ ^^V|^ Ft
_o_
-e-
Ex. 25-28. Mendelssohn: St. Paul, "O Great Is the Depth," Voice Parts.
^ Wm ^
Poco a poco piu animate
J ..
i^P i.
^ ^
F:
r rr
Vi/^ "'«
4
v;.^
#
p r
'« V7 Vi
f^
T « V
As is true of the \"/\', the \''/I\' also may resolve deceptively. In such cases the
chord of resolution is usually a form of the supertonic. In measures 3-4 of Ex.
25-29 the V'/IV (root d) progresses to ii (root e).
^ „ Andante I
¥^
—>» 31
i
i^
14 (
P
P P F5^
')^>ic - i Z££L
zaz
D: VV V7/ IV
IV /iv
subtonic of the major scale. In Ex. 25-30 the chromatically raised first degree of
the scale (E-natural) follows its "leading tone" tendency and resolves to the super-
tonic pitch. The effect of the deceptive progression to ''VII, and the attendant
disturbance of the tonality caused by the extended duration of''VTI is later nul-
lified by a clear return to E-flat. The effect is that of a brief '"region" based on a
tonic of D-flat.
Allegro
hi
^ ^^m i
dim.
Vi
^ P .
I
?VII
^ ^
CONCLUSION OF SECONDARY DOMINANTS 427
i
ft »F3t» mI i li H |P m &-^3 s dim
m^ j vJ^Jj^Jj^ ^^ #^
^^
p
A deceptive relationship is found between the V/vi and its irregular resolution
to V. As in Ex. 25-31. this progression frequently creates the effect of an approach
to the diatonic chord (IVg in this instance) by a chromatic leaning tone chord
built a semitone below (B major—* C major).
^^jp i
r i[Xr \
^& ^^ ^m ::zi
p:iS»
WW
£
m^
VB/.IV6
m *^ ^ mm j=±
^
Ex. 25-32.
——
1 17,
M » F
it
Bach
i^"^
: Passacaglia
r .
1 ^j=j.
^ff
in
P"^^
C Minor.
^ £^S*yF"
?
XJ
% P
im
^S ii.^ ^ fl
^ ^^
##
>''ii-v'j I f
r
V7/_
'/vi
;v
^
Ex. 25-33 is particularly interesting because it illustrates the use of simple chords
in complex relationships. Note particularly the deceptive resolutions of the sec-
—
ondary dominants, V^vi IV and V'/iii I in measures 3-4 and 7-8, respectively. —
428 CONCLUSION OF SECONDARY DOMINANTS
Ex. 25-33. Prokofiev: Classical Symphony, III. Copyright 1926 by Edition Russe de Musi-
que. Reprinted by permission of Boosey & Hawkes Inc., Assignees of the
Copyright.
Gavotte
/
I fet
mw t
m t
p'f i f jff?
/
U
f
^m ^ ^^ 9
V7V
/Vi
fP
j
'
V? vy
V",. V7/
Y^f /Vi
fe^
IV
^ m Fir
n $
i
m ijca
gjjp
^i
=-*
Vi
mf
m ^ i
iii
*
vy..
p f
^ ip^rlr\
^
a non-diatonic chord.
m a)
LA
^ i
r m
b)
J
"T^ r=^ T
i
A 4 Li i 4
i
G:
J
I
^^ V!
Yy rvi
V9
4
I
^^
I
^Kr ^>^vi
p
^'
V' I
If two complete seventh chords are used in such a succession, they are generally
inverted in a conventional resolution of the dissonance (see Ex. 25-34a) Other- .
wise, if in root position, one or the other is usually incomplete (see Ex. 25-34b).
Frequently, when two secondary dominant sevenths are inyolyed_in_jJ^decep-
CONCLUSION OF SECONDARY DOMINANTS 429
tive" pr ogression, usua l voice loading prorrdurrs arc ignored and the ypi£es.rT).Qye
from one chord to the other in parallel motion. Although the seventh of the first
clioid may rtOt resolve in a conventional sense, the note of resolution is usually
found in another voice of the chord of resolution.
The V|/V in Ex. 25-35 resolves deceptively to V|/vi (measure 8) in the first
voices move in parallel from one seventh chord to the other, each "line" moving
up a major second to the corresponding note of the next chord.
Allegro agitato
Voice
^^ i2=^
wm i
Piano mm mn a:43:
fff
11111 mill rffff
C:
i ^^ P 2==t ^ i y I y i
yw—V y 2==:t
(j
-
pp j^pirr -''
^ PnOP p i
O i^ ^
^S/v ^k
A similar progression involving a V|/iv appears in the second phrase of Ex. 25-
36. Here the chord of resolution is V|/V whose root is a step above that of the
previous chord. The accompanying texture moves in parallel motion, in opposi-
tion to the descending pattern of the melodic line.
Ex. 25-36. Debussy : Preludes, Book I,No. 8. Permission for reprint granted by Durond
et Cie., Paris, copyright owners, and Elkon-Vogel Co., Inc., Philadelphia agents.
Viv Vv
430 CONCLUSION OF SECONDARY DOMINANTS
The number and variety of irregular resolutions is great, and there is consider-
able variation regarding frequency of use. The subsequent discussion will be limited
to the most common types with the assumption that more thorough knowledge of
this practice will be acquired through continued observation of the works of various
composers.
As a point of departure, consider the various irregular resolutions of the Mm"
chord (root c) appearing in Ex. 25-37. Keep in mind that any one of the progres-
sions might represent the resolution of a secondary dominant seventh chord in one
tonality context or another. For example, (a) might represent V|/VI in f V—
minor, or \'"/iii —V| in D-flat major, etc.
M
6i TT
(b) (c)
m
(d) (e) (f)
:#S
(g) (h)
m
(i)
ft fr TT
11 11
W^ rr ^ T ^ w f^ Tf ^
The irregular resolutions indicated as (a), (b) and (c) exemplify chromatic
third relations. Of these three, (b) and (c) permit the simplest connection because
there are two common tones between the first chord and its chord of resolution.
Note the conventional resolution of the seventh in (a) and (b) in which the roots
move down a third, while in (c) (in which the root moves us a third) the seventh
is retained in the same voice. In each of the three, the other member of the tritone
(the "leading tone") is not resolved in the usual manner, either being held over as
in (b) or resolved downward by step as in (a) and (c).
It is apparent that such resolutions create various chromatic relationships, and
as a general rule, cross relations between voices are avoided. In progression (d)
(root movement down a fourth) the seventh B-flat ascends as does the "leading
tone" E. The resolution by root movement down a second (examples (e) and (f ))
permits a conventional treatment of the leading tone ; however, the seventh is
important structural units because they are the final chords of transient-terminal
cadences. The subsequent chord of each is one whose root is a third below that
of the secondary dominant.
^m Allegro moderato
$
s m j^^ i
f #
G:
J
r^rr
JJ
^— J-.
zaz
i
zsn
^/1i
J-
Ud
^^ w^
IV
i JJ
i w oo
J J J- J J A J
3 ^^ =ni i
f #^
_CL.
In the Beethoven excerpt the cadence chord is V/vi which progresses to I upon
the repetition of the phrase, and subsequently to V'/vi. The cadence, ivg —V/vi is
i
£
Allegro con variazioni
m
T rr
^
f m m ^m ^s r- FT rr
1^ f ^ W 8^4?
^
^ ^^
eI-: I
^ ri j=^ ^=^
i^ ^ ^
a
j>p
^J
n''i,l r
£j ^ P
^
V4/
rvi
Vi IV lie V
/Vi /vi
V7/
/ji
W /V
V7 I
432 CONCLUSION OF SECONDARY DOMINANTS
This use of V/vi as a transient-terminal cadence chord emphasizes the close rela-
tion between a major tonic key and its relative minor.
i
& i
Adagio
iA
f*ff
S
^ iH^-m-d
r
^ i^
rrr
i
^i
^ ^/V/..
'|^%i
I
*=^ i ^ ^M
V4 Vi6
7Vi
When one secondary dominant resolves to its own secondary dominant, the
seventh of the latter supplies the former with its "leading tone" resolution. As a
general rule, the chord of resolution is followed immediately by a return to the
first secondary dominant, in a basic scheme such as V/V — V^/ii — V^/V. This
represents the embellishment of an already chromatic secondary dominant.
Andante
te
_Q_
^i
±
p £
vn
^A J=J=J=J ^
» ^FW?
-e-
~n~ sc
:2=
^^
Bk n. V7/.
'>ii
V/
Vv V7 W /iv
IV iV
Although other possibilities of voice leading exist in the resolution of one sec-
ondary dominant seventh to another (root movement by step down, for example),
we frequently find all voices moving in parallel motion. Obviously such practice
precludes the regular resolution of the dissonant elements. In Ex. 25-42 this treat-
ment is made obvious by the use of the block chords and by the repetition of the
irregular pattern (V^/ii —V^).
— —
Allegro
^Mmr
^ sl^rn::t
Iff f
=3
te
^
4r
#
;2
1/:
^ m ^ r'it LU V7/ V7
ife
li
V7
>^ii
9 I
r
a
lA J-^ J
• 1
—tn —
-M
^ — —— m
9
f
1
-A
e
^— 11 Q u
organizational feature is the contrary motion between the outer lines in the long
melodic descent in measures 1-5. Through the use of secondary dominants the
harmonic potential is increased, particularly through the irregular resolutions.
Lento
h
*t
m I
^ i^ »• #•
M
w ^^
o _
t=f:
rr
p^« p' iV6 VMi
434 CONCLUSION Of SECONDARY DOMINANTS
4t
^P
rof
-rw
FfFtp
k
^P
/)ZMi>
r
^
f
9^^
^ ^^
Vi6 Vy 16 IV
^
^ ^ii
f
"'
m il
Sequential Use
^m
Piii presto
3 ^
i ^ i
r I
E
I
''^
m ^^
^^ e:
f i
,V7
1=^
^ ^^ J Jl
^
V>'iii "'
'
P
,^v'\
r -^^
,^v
J L
Passages such as these may or may not be modulatory in effect, but they do
unsettle the tonality to a degree, thus allowing the option of returning to the
original key or proceeding to a new one. The powerful unity of the sequential
CONCLUSION OF SECONDARY DOMINANTS 435
in preparation for the cadence on the dominant of A. From measure 8 the key is
Allegro
^MVMf r r
'
'^
¥-
Pff^
^
fp?
^ ^^
^m
IV
V7 W' '
% Vi
I
^
5pf^ W ^ J=U a
m#^ »?fi
i^ ^m
t^ ^±
^
— ^ — z=z: ^*^
mm f aoz
W^
\f^~~~. f f^
^^m in
^ ^^ u^
^Ljpj^
a:
T
—
f£fi^'^ <
ia^
r
V" ' f F^ r
i
%
VI
r-
VI4
i
i
r
^ pd^
r r
f
^'/m "'AVI
Andante
i
^ ^
^^^ f7\
^i
f
'y, \ , .
U, y ii i i, £^^I 1 -^r T r
f
'^ ^
16 vy V7
^I'tn ^^w ^^im ^>|vi
True, there are some familiar root relationships as indicated by the symbols
(V/^V— V^) and (V^— III-VV— VI ; also, there are third relations (V|/II— V7III)
and (V'/vi — ig), as well as a tritone relation (V/VII Vj — III). As logical as such
analysis may appear on paper, the question arises as to the information imparted
by such an assortment of symbols, harmonic
as they pertain to the unfolding of the
materials. The rigid application of the secondary dominant classification to any
and all M-m seventh chords can sometimes lead to naught, and the resultant symbolic
representation may contribute little or even impede the understanding of the
musical relationships involved. Generally speaking, the application of this concept
should be limited to those relationships in which the chord of resolution is a rec-
ognized member of the diatonic family, or one clearly related.
The major-minor seventh chord, once used frequently as a secondary dominant,
has fallen into disuse. Contemporary composers usually avoid it entirely, or treat
tonal resolution is too compelling to enable it to fit into the more chromatic textures
Exercises
For more detailed assignments see Materials and Structure of Music I, Workbook, Chapter 25.
h. V^/iii, E e. V7/III, d
c. Vg/bVII, c f. V^/vi, D-Jlat
2. Devise a phrase featuring a sequence employing the following secondary dominants:
V of supertonic, of mediant, of subdominant, of dominant, and of submediant. Use
root position and/or the various inverted forms.
3. Consider various pitches as root, third, fifth or seventh of dominant of submediant
chords. Spell and chord from the bass up, indicate the key and function and the chord
of resolution. Do the same for the dominant of mediant.
4. Using the following harmonic progressions as models, create two different settings of
each, one for piano, the other SATB. Use different harmonic rhythms, textures and
keys for the two settings.
a. I iig V V|/vi vi V*/m iiig V|/IV ii V
b. i \'|/VI VI ivg V5/VII VII Illg V^/III III i6 V^ i
5. Compose a phrase to follow that of Ex. 25-19, maintaining the same general texture.
Employ at least one secondary dominant (other than V/III) and cadence on tonic.
Arrange the whole for solo clarinet, violin, viola and cello.
6. Seek out examples in which V/vi and V/iii appear. Pay particular attention to the ap-
proach and resolution and their significance in regard to context.
7. Analyze a number of the following, indicating keys, chords and non-chord tones.
Haydn: Symphony No. 85 (La Reine), II. (1-8)
Beethoven: Symphony Op. 21, I. (1-21)
jVo. /,
11. Construct short progressions using different forms of the dominants of dominant, super-
tonic, mediant and submediant, and resolve them deceptively and irregularly (up a
third, down a third, tritone, etc.).
12. Write an instrumental and choral setting of each of the following progressions. Use dif-
ferent textures and harmonic rhythms for each.
3.. I
X vigV'/V iii vig V|/ii V6 V7/V V
Vi.e
Changes
26
Modulation
To modulate, a composer must establish two different tonal centers — the initial
tonic and a new one. This relationship may be created in a number of ways, but
it exists at the point of actual tonal change, a point usually established by a single
pitch or chord that ismutually identifiable in both keys. A modulation takes place
when there is a change from one tonality to another, the second of which is estab-
lished cadentially and confirmed by a subsequent phrase beginning in the new
key.
Tonality change may be accomplished in a smooth, almost imperceptable
way, concealing the actual shift of tonic, or it may result from an abrupt shift.
In this book, the former will be described as a modulation, while the latter will be
described as a tonal shift.
A modulation may or may not be indicated by a new key signature^, but it may
be recognized visually as the result of a new set of accidentals which in a real
sense "signal" the change of key. A modulation from c minor to A-flat major
occurs in Ex. 26-1.
^ Modulations within short sections are seldom indicated by a change of key signature.
439
#
Voice
^^
^Aj . =^ ; S^
^
Piano
%'A^J^J^P
^E W
vw—-Tw— -:
?=^
— ^
i?P
C^CiJCJ^CXJ
* V amV* ^
1^\^[S^\J^
xz=
^ pr r
I
f
^ p
.F]nn 9# m m pi
»f' -"TW
The principal key of the song is c minor, and the change of key that occurs is a
modulation to the submediant of c, A-flat. Schubert might have changed the key
signature at measure 5; that he did not emphasizes the subsidiary relationship of
A-flat, the new key, to c minor. The note d-flat occurs as an accidental, and b-flais
replace the b' naturals which were the leading tones in the original key of c.
Melodic Modulation
The same melodic factors that establish an initial tonic usually function in the
establishment of a new one. For example, melodic motion from leading tone to
tonic (7-8), tonic triad outlining, dominant to tonic skips, repetitions of tonic and
the treatment of tonic as the beginning and/or closing pitch of a phrase or larger
section typify the estabUshment of a point of focus in melody. These factors (or
some of them) are usually evident in both initial key affirmations and subsequent
modulations.
TONALITY CHANGES 441
Several of the considerations just noted are found in Ex. 26-2. major is firmly D
established in the opening two phrases of the minuet, and the notes of the D
major triad frame the melodic activity of measures 1-4. In the subsequent phrase,
however, d becomes an unstable pitch, because of its tritone relationship with g
sharp. The tonality is unstable through measures 5-8, although we anticipate the
confirmation of /I in a cadence. A is confirmed in measure 10 by the melodic
motion 7-8 {g-skarp to a). A is convincingly reaffirmed as a new tonic at the be-
ginning of the next phrase in measure 1 1. Thus a modulation from D to A major
(the dominant key of D) has been effected.
oEf/irrr i ffrf-fiP'rcnr i
^ W PS 3^
tf^LL/
It is important to note that a serves in a dual capacity in Ex. 26-2. The repeat
sign in measure 10 indicates a return to the beginning and, therefore, to the key
of/). The importance of A as a new key is lessened, since it is quickly erased with
the return to D. In the light of these broader structural considerations, we need
not regard A product of a modulation, but rather as the point of arrival of
as the
a transient-terminal cadence on the dominant of D, which momentarily diverts
attention from tonic.
However, with the repetition of the section, a is cast in an entirely different
light, for theopening of the next section is clearly in A, and establishes it as a new
point of focus. The note a has been heard in a dual capacity: first, as a strongly
emphasized dominant in D; and second, as the tonic of a new key that supercedes
the previous one.
Ex. 26-3 contains a clear modulation from G to dominant key of D. The
its
i ^s ^ Alles
¥ rn n m
ist an Gottes Segen
m s ^^^
rrrr m
o
^m
r
UU
^ ^p r Wm
iiA iiij
^ r
v:;
442 TONALITY CHANGES
such a process are those most clearly and strongly perceived, changes of key often
occur entirely within individual phrases. Three changes of key, each contained
within a separate phrase, may be found in Ex. 26-4. In contrast with the more
subtly effected modulations shown earlier in Ex. 26-2 and Ex. 26-3, these more
abrupt key changes, contained entirely within one phrase, are called transitory
modulations.^
^
Slow
g min.-
«=*S
g mm.
^ I
^
ipz
P W -e-
W^
^ 2
p
-o-
d min.
£
r^ i
ii^rrr Id I
-g min.
o
The modulations shown in Ex. 26-4 are considerably more abrupt than those
discussed earlier. In each case the change of tonality emphasized by the long
is
(g, b-flat and d) comprise the notes of the tonic triad of^ minor.
An abrupt shift of tonic from B-Jlat to D occurs in conjunction with the repeti-
tion of a characteristic melodic motive in Ex. 26-5. No melodic cadence to either
tonal center (and key) is purely a product of the repetition of a figure based on
major triads. Each tonic is heard as the root of an arpeggiated chord. Shifts of key
such as this are found most often in developmental sections of compositions where
sudden shifts of tonality and passages of tonal uncertainty are common.
D:
' For a fuller description see Book II, and the discussion of Tonal Regions.
—
It should be clear from the preceding discussion and illustrations that changes
of key, either smoothly introduced or abruptly efrected, may be established by
single strands of melody. However, as we discovered in our earlier study of melody,
the tonality of a single line may be ambiguous, and more than one pitch may be
interpreted as tonic. In the opening of Ex. 26-5, g may easily be perceived as tonic,
whereas c is apparently confirmed as tonic in the third and fourth phrases.
^ |
o
l o
M ti
M n
-e-=
^ iBp
The ambiguity of the melody of Ex. 26-6 can be attributed partially to the
absence of a structural half-step relation in the first three phrases, an ambiguity
that is dispelled in the fourth phrase by the b — c (leading tone — tonic) cadential
figure.
The tonal ambiguity of a single melodic line usually disappears when it is
Ex. 26-7. Josquin des Pres : Ave Maria Motet (all voices).
ZSJZ
Tenor HBh itt:
:XC n n
l-^ " "
8
&-&»
Bass s «• «- -e-
HBh M »
qct
444 TONALITY CHANGES
^ T%
-e- =PC 33=
5r =Q=
m He(-
=|DC
mi
=n=
TV
33=
:W= =n=
det
HBh
»
=&
I w zz HB(- -o-
:W= =ee: -e- =0=
P
M o 33=
^ -e-
_Q_!
TT" Ha-
?2=^=CC
-e- 33=
HSh
2Z
-e —a- =£3=
n
Hei-
HEK-
Ut:
*
5
-o- _Q_
=n= Hei- =a= o M
:W:
A study of the first phrase shows that the beginning pitch (g^) in the soprano
forms the fifth of a C major triad, and it recurs in the same role at the close of the
same phrase.
After hearing the whole texture we no longer sense any ambiguity; the possi-
bihty of interpreting g as tonic is erased by the establishment of c as the root of
both the beginning and ending chords. Further emphasis on c results from the
melodic activity of both the alto and tenor voices the leading tone-to-tonic half :
step occurs cadentially in the alto, and c^ is clearly heard as the focal point of the
tenor line. The chords that are heard on successive strong beats create the progres-
sion I — IV — V — I. It is interesting that in this composition, which is quite chordal
in texture, a canonic relationship at the fifth exists between the soprano and tenor
parts.
There is nothing that suggests modulation in this excerpt. We have simply
established the fact that while a single melody may clearly define a tonic, tonality
will become definitely determined only after considering both the melodic and
harmonic elements.
The melody shown in Ex. 26-7 unfolds entirely within A. Aside from the progres-
sive cadence on the dominant that occurs in the final measure, there is very Httle
activity that could induce one to hear any other pitch than a as a point of tonal
focus. Leaps from e to a (measures 11-12 and 16-17) continue to affirm a as tonic.
Sing the line.
TONALITY CHANGES 445
ft iiyp I
P'PP ^^'p PPP^ l I i
V'p IP'PP I p^'p W^
f i'
;
.
^ , i jj j ppp p'^pij'i^^^
i
ture of tonality is gained. A modulation has taken place between measures 8 and
9, and the melodic patterns that seemed oriented to A are cast in an altogether
diflerent framework. The pitch a^ which seemed stable, is now heard as an un-
stablesubdominant, forming a tritone with the new leading tone of d-sharp. A
change of key, not perceptible in the melody alone, has been effected through
harmonic means. The new leading tone in £ major [d-sharp) is absent in the melodic
line. It is present, however, in the accompaniment as the third of V in E, replacing
the initial g-sharp as leading-tone.
Voice
ft iipp Ip- PP I p^'p W '
I
J'^P I
P'PP
Piano
f%^
W'
^^
1^
^m ^
i
m
tt m
r»-= —1*-(*
ft" p ^p I
PPP Mp p p ^m ^ «==*
ftf MJ i 0- i t
^ ^ ^m m nr-1
T^Tj) n^^
^
'p /3 J^ r
ftll* I
p p p I
p
I
J,
j)
^ I
J^ :
||
fti
^
i
'
f «^^P ^
1 ^ nz-^
- • g
i
^m m m
446 TONALITY CHANGES
Relationships of Keys
In subsequent chapters we shall deal in detail with many of the processes and
techniques associated with modulations. For the present we shall concentrate on
developing a more generalized picture of some of the most common key relation-
ships that have been used to create tonal variety in Western music.
The most frequently encountered key relationship consists of a change to a near
related key. Near whose signatures diffeii^y_jiQ_jnore than
related keys a re those
o ne sharpe r onejiat. For example, the modulation in Ex. 26-9 occurred between
near related keys, since the two involved {A major and E major) are one sharp
removed. That is, the signature of A major contains three sharps, while the key of E
contains four sharps.
Modulations from an initial key to the key of the dominant constitute the most
common type of near related key change. This seems logical and consistent when
we consider the strong relationship between tonic and dominant tones.
Near relationship exists between major tonic and major dominant keys, and
minor tonic and minor dominant keys. It does not exist between a minor tonic
and a major dominant key, because of the lack of common pitches shared by the
two keys. The foregoing discussion can be clarified by a study of Ex. 26-10.
Distant relationship
c minor (b''b) G major (#)
Several illustrations of near related modulations follow. Play through Ex. 26-1
and study the changes of key.
Ex. 26-11. Frauenholz : Der Herr gedenki an uns, for Voices, Strings and Organ.
jS
Violins
m s.
p=^=if ^
f $
E
r=r
i j
i
nfJP
Organ
P^ b m^o or
«
^
s
Voices
s.
T.
U
^Ji
tt
omxlL E
^^^
p^^
J JlTl i JlTTP i
J TO ^1-f^f^H^tf
p^r?^
r r
J J J J J J
J j J i ^
.'^''1
1
r r r
« — »
4v i — rr
-t Ul^l 1 i 1
> -..^'r «|
-,^ --^ —*^--i —— •
^^3
—— Lj
. ^'
p ^ i
^ b 4 » —
» »
— •< »
1
1
i
]t
—4
p
P
—— »
P
j^''
fj^f gf
t f
\ M 1-
p^ ni a f=¥
^^ .
t f
10
F
P^ s •
m.
ft »— ^^p^
^^y-;f"
yjp ^
^ •4- 4
C Major.
Ex. 26-14. Beethoven: Mass in
es M s
rr rr f
i4 i
s ^jg^
rfff
ZEE
r^ ^^
i ffit
? ^
^ 30
1
Jj JJJJ J
331
si
IE
rr rrfT
^ Jf
35
u.
^ J
f
i
s -e-
rr^
TONALITY CHANGES 449
t>Tn
I J .
.1 .1 -o-
40
331 i i^ .U
i zsn
1=T
g ?^
Ex.
J- J 11
^ 1 J
.^b /y/«.u 1^
P ??
^ ^
^
K jn.A,^^
7 ^ e ^JOT ic:
i
y
J
rvrfrr
W^ fe^ V .
»
!
ilf^^i=f £
^^
J)
i
J>
J. >i' J. ^
f
Violin I
fe^
JaJ ^i'^*
Violin n
Viola
f^^
^ ^ 5
-#-^»-
Cello
^
—— — :
^m
^
i^'
f
^ cr»r
^ » 1
^^r^L£^^
tt j #
p
* # J »
le
» #=^=g
^
K S f
—^ f ^53
m.
^^
iitf
££r/ [!r E^J^ C; i^ ^
i ^ N ^
.ttJt
L/r ^p ^^ ^ ^ w
')-;'
[ID ^ ? 1^ ^^
This group of excerpts, which constitutes a variety of textures and styles, contains
modulations of the following characteristics
Example no. Keys Established Modulatory Scheme
11 D major A major Modulation to the Dominant
12 5-flat major F major Modulation to the Dominant
13 ^-flat major —/minor Modulation to the submediant
14 C major minor
a Modulation to the submediant
15 •2, G-flat major d minor Modulation to the mediant
16 g minor c minor Modulation to the subdominant
scales contain one or no common tones and whose tonic chords are a major or
minor third apart. For example, C major and E major form a third relationship.
Their tonic chords possess one common tone, e, and a change from C to E (or E
to C) is often described as a third related key change. Actually, the change to a third
related key is usually felt as a sudden shift, rather than as a gradual change.
In Ex. 26-17 a third relation is introduced between D major and F major.
Beethoven has employed the pitch a as a common link between the tonic chords
of the two keys.
^
^ 33 3
.9
t
mE§. ^
# —
\
(»
$ ^ 3 3
^^s
5
fe^iS #^^^
3
Major and minor keys a half step apart constitute the key relation in Ex. 26-18
in which a new key is introduced on the leading tone of the original key.
Piano
Hv« MFmPmPmF.\> 0^¥^
^V(^ pfpr^rp py^
^J
1
m ^^M^
rrCT^gTlJ
^^
±t
^» i
452 TONALITY CHANGES
^
Ex. 26-18. continued.
*
t*
^ M
:^
;> i
vrFfiiFrFrFrFriii^^j^ ii' i^^ f^=^=^
«.
i
«^
^FF ^
^^^^=; -^
is:
^
1^
All of the modulations discussed in the previous sections of this chapter have at
least one significant feature in common ; in each case the new key was established
and then affirmed by a cadence. Cadences which confirm principal tonics are, as
we know, called terminal cadences, and most modulations are confirmed by terminal
cadences.
In contrast to the terminal cadence, both the progressive and transient-terminal
^^^^^
r r r7 Tr
t d t i
^
/
r^$=i i^
TONALITY CHANGES 453
Both terminal and transient-terminal cadences occur in Ex. 26-21. The excerpt
begins in the principal key of/ minor with a sustained statement on tonic, followed
by a transient-terminal cadence on the dominant. The subsequent phrase estab-
lishes a modulation to c minor, and the original key of/ minor is reestablished
fiJ HJ
®
-
^
ww
U-^
— r^
"3 5"
-^ ^
4i
-9—9-
^
^f
^i'
y
1 —g —
t±=n
p^z
7
^ ^^ ra=^ i ICG
^-^
g — 33
^
^^
^ r rrr
UJU331
i ^
-e^
~T5~'
«
^7
—«
Z)
^ • -d
15
12
©
^e i ft»#
nr rfW r^
m ffi
-e^
- - Jzz^
5
f)
- I
rp ^^^
We "modulation" denotes a change of key. Since the
recall that the expression
system of "keys" is most prevalent in music written between the seventeenth and
the beginning of the twentieth centuries (music with which we are most familiar),
it is natural that we begin the study of tonality changes with this music. However,
s ii f f fff
^
i
s Z3Z
~~"-~~~-|
{fiF=F^^rf^ -^ 1 1
— 5 1
=^^1
Jt^ a •
r — —
p-= —f^
r r
The first phrase and the measures of the second establish the mode
initial three
d. The mode of the close of the composition is d dorian, with a c-sharp introduced
to create a leading tone in the 7 — 8 cadence pattern. The change of tonic is not felt
had had more preparation. However, as the last pitch heard, d can hardly be dis-
counted as a point of focus, and a definite change of tonic from^ to d is perceived.
We are reminded, by glancing at Ex. 26-22 and Ex. 26-23, that alterations are
common to compositions employing modal bases, just as they are to pieces written
in major or minor keys. The designation of a specific mode is made on the basis of
the most consistently recurring pitch material. Unlike most works in major or
minor keys, many modal compositions begin and close on different tonics. In such
examples, the closing tonic is usually considered to be the principal tonality of
the work. The prevailing pitch material of Ex. 26-23 forms a natural minor (Aeo-
lian) mode on a.
r^a^
t rr ^^ ^
WTT r^rr r ffT? rr
i
C ai ^^a ^^ ^m ^^ ^^
SM
T
^^ P
c
P ^
lAdJ
Roots ^ ^ rnrr r r ii T
W i ^^ i ^ ^^^f ''
r nr
r- rrrrff r
^ ^^ -o-
J.
M^ J=^ iU
^
(Roots)
^ C
J
3^
J J
y n
i # ^ r
I
f ^r r
456 TONALITY CHANGES
10
^=^ i
tr FfT
^ff rrr r
^ -# ^ i^i
s
m «— p
^•-^Jj ii
J -e-
-
J ^
^ ^F^
^J i J'
rrr f
^
rr '
^(Roots)
P HI ^^
(C: V/v)
^
15
« ^
rr st^ ^^
rrr If
f# ^f
-J J
g^
^^ ^^ r ^^^ ^ ; i
r
(V I)
m(Roots)
r
^ I
r J f^
In Ex. 26-23 tonal variety is created by a series of shifts from a to C, and the
tonal plan of the example can be understood by singing the second bass part, mea-
sures 1-10. An analysis of the other parts will reveal that the notes a and c serve
as terminal points for the various successive imitative entries which continuously
overlap. At no point until the close of the excerpt can all five voices be found in
cadential agreement, and the subtle shifts of pitch emphasis are not affirmed by
strong cadences. The cadences are weak because they occur only in rhythmically
staggered locations, rather than simultaneously in all the voices. The subtlety of
tonal shifts in the excerpt must be related to the texture of the composition, which
is quite contrapuntal, since strong tonality-defining cadences are avoided in favor
a strongly defined tonality, but as a study of the root relations (accompanying the
r^jvii
—
example) will show, any description of its tonal organization would be incomplete
without some mention of c as a subsidiary point of focus. It is heard in a transitory
relationship to a, a point of momentary polarity, but its duration as a tonic is such
that should be viewed as part of a continuously unfolding tonal pattern com-
it
prised of a c —
a, which creates unity and variety within the principal tonality of
The principal tonic (a) of the Palestrina Motet ]ust discussed is the same as that
of the passage in Ex. 26-24. Despite the similarity of tonic and prevailing minor
mode, the two examples present a striking contrast of tonal material and organiza-
tion. But since both examples illustrate shifting points of tonal polarity, they are
more alike in basic tonal design than they would appear to be.
^^
:
Lento e languido
s
i^
^^^ fi^^ ±=^
p ^^^
10
j^;jj
fp=^
*r ^
^ I ^ i
ra:
j^m iJ^^~^
15
458 TONALITY CHANCES
^
Ex. 26-24. continued.
r=if^^^*^3 m
riT r^rrs'
^
r p=^J
i-
J'
jj-^j s
bi^5' i£^-jj
^
'^^^Tf
l
r— pr-
^ ±^
tp F^
3^
IP^
tion." But since wejiave_de fined modulation as a clearly established change from
one diatonic key to another, confirmed by a terminal cadence^ "continuous modu-
lation" would seem to be a contiadiction of what we hear in such an example. If
on the other hand we admit that all twelve available pitches may comprise the
tonal material of a composition with one principal tonic, and that any of the twelve
pitches may occur as a briefly established or implied tonic, then a more suitable
of short passages which only allude to particular keys. Sections or entire works
containing implied modulations are by no means limited to any specific period of
music (although they abound in works of the late nineteenth and early twentieth
centuries) as can be seen from a comparison of Ex. 26-24 and Ex. 26-25. Implied
modulations may be shown in analysis by indicating the implied key or tonic in
brackets, e.g., [C] or [a]. The impUed keys of the Wagner excerpt have been
shown in Ex. 26-24. Study the implied keys in Ex. 26-25.
TONALITY CHANGES 459
P * ^
W
^^ i^ $
^ i
rr
^
s ICC
^^ §=
* TT
r^
M ^^^
^ F=T r
i ^
r
^-j
e^d^ Py^^
ii J.
-e-
fro-
f
In the concluding chapters of volume two we shall deal extensively with some of
the materials of more recent music. As we shall see, changes of tonality are as
significant in the structure of many contemporary works as in music of past periods.
Much twentieth century music has a strong affinity for tonal relationships that are
typical of much earlier music. In Ex. 26-26, tonic and dominant relations con-
stitute the principal means of delineating the tonality of the opening phrases.
Ex. 26-26. Hindemith : Ludis Tonalis, Interludium in E-flat (The March). (C) 1943 by Schott
& Co., Ltd., London. Reprinted by permission.
March (J=66)
. :
Exercises
Additional materials are contained Materials and Structure of Music I, Workbook, Chapter 26.
1 Study Ex. 26-26 using as a guide the list of considerations that follows
a. Describe the "key" of the example.
b. In what way do tonic and dominant relationships organize the passage?
c. What are the prevailing root relations?
d. What types of contrapuntal relationships occur?
e. What are the most characteristic rhythm patterns used?
f. What types of decorative activity are found?
g. Where do changes of tonality occur?
h. In what way do changes of dynamics contribute to the organization of the
excerpt?
i. Where do harmonic or melodic tritones occur and how are they treated?
j. Contrast the harmonic language of the excerpt with that of Ex. 26-25.
2. Name all the near related keys to A major, c minor, D-flat major and b minor.
3. Invent an eight measure piano composition which fluctuates tonally between G major
and e minor. Begin in G and close in e.
4. Sight sing Ex. 26-23.
5. Transpose the soprano line of Ex. 26-3 to B-flat major and make a four-voice setting of the
melody which contains a near related modulation.
^
Procedures
<f
27
Tonality
Change
key.
Ex. 27-1 illustrates concisely the pivot chord modulation technique, in this case
involving two distantly related keys, which is not uncommon in nineteenth cen-
tury music.
*^
Voice
Piano
^ / ZT
5b£
^— ^
f
& s
W P
-f—^
^^^
r^ = m
o —
a fE^r
it
f=g-85r
* *^
It*:* I
ft
10
—
• p"^f*i
^ ^^':! ';;;:> S*=i^
:
^i=^
/I
iV
I
in a
in E
'_j u ^ r [_r
i
B\' measure 9 of this excerpt the change of key has taken place minor has been ; .-1
role of the pivot chord. Each modulation generally contains a signal or simple
"clue" that the old key is no longer in effect. This is usually an accidental foreign
to the old key but diatonic to the new key (often a new leading tone). Occasionally,
though, the change of key occurs without additional accidentals, in which case other
factors must be considered. Once this "clue" has been spotted, the pivot chord is
easy to locate; it is usually the previous chord.
In Ex. 27-2 no accidental signals the advent of the new key. Instead, the Mm"
chord in measure 6 (not a diatonic member of A minor but, as it turns out, clearly
V^ in D major) first indicates that b minor is no longer the prevailing key. The
pivot chord is thus the previous chord — i in b minor becomes vi in D major.
.
^^ ^ ^
Menuetto
Violin I
m Allegretto
'/
=fe^
/ f
^ p. i
Violin H
m i^ —6 «- m &
M w^
y
Viola rF=f=^
f
^— m Be
Cello
^m f
^ 0-
m
m mm ^ ^
— — m
/
i
fe tt
J ^ ^ ^ ^/ ^P ^^ ^ ^
m:
i^ ^ £
^ ^-#
f
I
P
W ^ ^
^Fm fs
f ^m f it / ^ ^
fD Major:
ff mig [11
iii
,i|i§
IV
^ §
Vi
H
Vii"
i
A
14
Major: IV
U ilti§ ^^
Vi Vii*
ff
I
f=%
III
»1W
—
We
can rule out several of these chord pairs ii (a minor triad) will not serve as :
the chord equivalent of V (although V/V will) IV and vii° are likewise incompati- ;
ble, as are vii° and iii. Four pairs remain I and IV, iii and vi, V and I, vi and ii
:
all equally possibly. It is obvious that the primary^ criteriqnjs jdentity of chord
quality.
Different pairs of keys display widely differing pivot chord potentials. In certain
key relationships, as illustrated in Ex. 27-4, the possibilities are more limited than
in Ex. 27-3.
Viola
m mm ^w
Cello W¥^ ^ W #^ ^
Ex. 27-6 contains a clear pivot chord modulation between two keys that are not
closely related. The process is more complex, though. The pivot chord occurs
not
immediately before the first departure from C major IV in C becomes V in B-jiat —
major in measure 17.
Allegro vivace 5
m #
m
±±€:^^±
iM ^^
p.'
J.
-J.
Oa7m • • •
m^
i 3t
*=:*
m
^^
f=TT
8va
a^ m$^
*
f ^m iff'
^t
^
sf
p
«L^*V a=a:
m fC: IV
Ib'': V
» •
—#
^ ^20
W
8va
-
»— r
\*-
» p
^=ii ^
^m
*
I
it
^ I
--#
7l9H
25
? 4
# •
7=rT=if=^^ ffiffi
Violin j^''U1||,)"^
If^ ^ i 331 zai
^
Cello
Piano
s
gffi^
^^ ^ 33C
^
^^
^pi \ ^i o
^
466 PROCEDURES OF TONAl/rV CHANGE
This principle is equally apparent in Ex. 27-8. The pivot tone here is the reiterated
G in the viola part; its role changes from that of tonic in g minor (in measure 8) to
that of mediant in E-flat major. Here the change is better described as a tonal
shift than as modulation because of its abruptness.
Ex. 27-8. Shostakovitch : Quartet, Op. 49, I. (C) Used by permission of MCA Music, a divi-
sion of MCA Inc., New York, N.Y. All rights reserved.
^ft ^ m
; jj j:7?^ ,
r
^r
m i nm
r f-
^
m :$
zc
g
i ^?^^
^ m P3EEE
}^.
^ ^ 1
^
^^^^
r f^
gliss.
ppf
^J
^^f yb in y
of course, there are no diatonic elements in common between the two keys. There-
fore the connection is through the stepwise melodic action of one of the lines.
Ex. 27-9 contains a modulation by chromatic inflection in the fourth and fifth
measures.
rj:^ fl^.
Ex. 27-10. Bartok :Concerto for Violin No. 2, I. Copyright 1946 by Hawkes & Son
(London) Ltd. Reprinted by permission of Boosey & Hawkes Inc.
Violin
Orchestra
468 PROCEDURES OF TONALITY CHANGE
poco rallent.
125
h©- ^
a tempo
PP
i^
p espr.
^
^W p-e-
I
II f I i -^^^if
^ ^
3i:
f5/)r.
^
331
(F and C) are connected by the chord in measure 126. Although this D-flat minor
triad is foreign to both keys, its use as a connecting link is obvious and relates it
to the technique described above.
Modulations by chromatic inflection frequently involve^ the dominant _as_at
l east one of theT chjQttifi. Composers often have modulated to the supertonic or sub-
mediant keys by this procedure. For example, the raised tonic pitch of a key be-
comes the leading tone of ii; the raised dominant becomes the leading tone of vi.
Ex. 27-1 1 contains a further example of this type of modulation, in this case
involving a chord that is not diatonic to either old or new key. The upward
chromatic inflection in the bass at measure 6 leads our attention from i in/minor
to \'|/\' in c minor.
'i^
-9-,
//(^
^
-
f
EE
* P
fe
m
-a-
PROCEDURES OF TONALITY CHANGE 469
t
&
fefe
'J' lI' i
I'
i-
I"
1^
^Jm t-
^ ^^ i
3-
w
i
fr
i
Descending chromatic inflection establishes the modulation in Ex. 27-12, again
in the bass line, in measures 2 and 3. It is interesting to note that the two tonics
in this excerpt lie a tritone apart, A — E-jlat; they are distant relations.
Ex. 27-12. Hindemith: Sonata for Organ, I. (C) 1937 by B. Schott's Soehne, Mainz. Re-
printed by permission.
Enharmonic Modulation
Composers occasionally have found it necessary to relate the two keys of a mod-
ulation through enharmonic spelling. This does not necessarily imply a remote
modulation, however, since the enharmonic spelling is often used merely for the
performer's convenience. The keys F-sharp major and D-flat major seem distantly
related at first glance, but they are in fact no more distant than C major and G
major. It is as natural, when in F-sharp major, to modulate to the dominant as
when in C major, and most people would find D-flal major easier to read than
C-sharp major, because of the notational complications of the latter.
The modulation in Ex. 27-13 is not between keys as closely related as f-sharp
and D-Jiat, but it does demonstrate that convenience of spelling can take precedence
over a more notationally consistent indication of key relationships. Measures 5 and
6 of this excerpt are actually notated in F-flat major, although the accompaniment
appears in the enharmonic key of E major. The pivot chord is also unusually
interesting, for it is not diatonic. In the last half of measure 4, V in A-flat major
becomes V/iii in F-flat major and resolves deceptively to the tonic of this new
key. However, a more musical interpretation of this modulatory relationship
would recognize the common pitch bond between the two keys; A-flat (tonic) of
the initial key becomes mediant of the new key.
470 PROCEDURES OF TONALITY CHANGE
Ex. 27-13. Schumann: Frauen/zeben und Leben, Helft mir, ihr Schwestern.
Voice j''i>'-j
}n S' \
} m- ^'
i
r p^'e^'^
Piano
:--;^^ii. ^ \
\
;.^^} \
rit.
a tempo
Chordal texture is absent from the enharmonic modulation in Ex. 27-14 from
E-flat minor to b minor; the change of key is brought about solely by the change
in function of the pivot tone: G-flat F-sharp (3 in e-JIat minor
j 5 in b minor). —
Schubert has cleverly expedited the modulation by the running chromatic scale
in the piano, effectively blurring the recollection of the previous key.
PROCEDURES OF TONALITY CHANGE 471
P g^
^n •»»»»^^»^»^»»i»^^##i»»*'« #>^0#l»»»i#ll#N»^l*>^»»»^»*.
ttT-^
Violin
I
\^
^ r I'T r
''^
r
^
cresc.
I
#i»»»#i^»i»»»'»^»»^-#»^»'»^i^.»*-*i^^»»^i^^*»»»«^**»
Cello £
cresc.
^
Piano
^ Gt major
^^ ^^•^^^ ^^^ ^^-^s^ ^l<>^^ » »<^»*>#N^^^^ > ^ ^^•^^^^^ ^^^S» ^^.
(T
Wi3?pJ^J^^'''
i»^ »^<»^^<#'^ ^
i fcb
^7
il
^»»0#>^-»*^»'#N#'#'^^'»^ ^ »»^>^>»^l^.^^'^<^S»^i^i^^>»<»^.^ »<!» ^^^^^^»'#\ ^^^^^•^^^^'^^^^^^•^^^^^•^^^^•^'^^^^^^'^^'^^•^^-^•^^^•^i^ » ^^^»^>^^ »»»>
^ 771"
p5
eSeiT^l* *!£*
vv^k^fei^^ilMl
j^^'i^
^;^ ^ici^ ^
» *»^ ^ »i»^^ > ^»^^ ^^^^^ ^^^ * ^^K^ ^»i^» ^^••^^'^^ ^ ^^.^^•^^
?p.>»»lf f f
i^
-•—•-
^ m m m
B minor
472 PROCEDURES OF TONALITY CHANGE
Disguising a change of tonic through a subtle relating of two keys has not always
been the composer's goal. A sudden shift of key can produce a stunning effect, one
that many composers have obviously considered desirable at times. We will not
dwell on the manifold effects of this procedure except to note that this lays bare
the relationship between the two keys, emphasizing their contrast, particularly
when they are not near related.
First, however, we should observe that modulation between closely related keys
Wm^ 1^^i mm
^m^ ^^m i
mf 'Hh
^
p
J-
P
'i.
^
M
P ^ P P ^ J
Violin I
lhi;fff}H te
? ^- if cresc.
^^
Violin n
>j'''''''J
^J'' ?; ^^ ^ £/• cresc.
?-e-
Viola ^s^^ ^^ 6 *
Q 331
sf cresc.
Cello
m ffi ^E
p
33:
ICE
5;^ cresc.
U
{/•
/
ia P ^^
^
^
S
»/
»-
^/
J
if
Y
i/
^
if
5^
#
V
if
^ M
f
/
ttrrf-iT^
t
a
10
^f
^fg m-M-m-
^
f=^=^
sf ^f -/
^
/
^
f^
?=i^
:a
dtrjtcif ^^ rJ 90 =Z2I
^^
9^^ ^^ ^
F^^
—
Ex. 27-17 and Ex. 27-18, however, contain shifts of tonal center that are not
only abrupt but, at times, of distant relations. In the Beethoven, for example, the
rapid tempo contributes to the effect of merely sliding the tonic down a minor
second —from /-sharp minor to F major. The succession of keys in the Brahms
excerpt is no less unusual.
p^
^ El
P
i
p
i ^» m
^ fc# ^ ^e 4
g
tel fe# i
Ex. 27-18. Brahms: Symphony No. 4,
Allegro giocoso
Although its harmonic style is less accessible than in the previous examples,
Ex. 27-19 contains a similar abrupt shift measure 3 to
of tonal center from C in
E-flat in the next measure. The two sections are related thematically through the
sequence of perfect fourths in the bass, the first note of which forms a step-progres-
—
sion of^ -f e-flat. But there is an unmistakable change of tonal center after the
double bar.
PROCEDURES OF TONALITY CHANGE 475
Ex. 27-19. Stravinsky: Symphony of Psalms, III. Copyright 1941 by Russischer Musik-
verlog Renewed 1958. Copyright & Renewal assigned to Boosey & Hawkes
;
Inc. Revised Version Copyright 1948 by Boosey & Hawkes Inc. Reprinted by
permission.
Soprano
Alto
^ - -^ i i
/ m
=te * ^
a
Tenor
iS:
s t
f
^
i' (' ^-
'i ^P
8 /
Bass
P^wr
> . >
I
f. jfgg ll
g
4 ^i 2 s'
=|:
^ ±=21
Orchestra"
^^ /
—n
cresc.
> ^
-a
—^.—5:
J2:
ffi
^ p subito
^ ^
'V\. J J J I
-I
^ i ^ i
_P subito
g
±
^^^^ ZZL
r r I
r
g p subito
^ p
p
subito
JQ_
i
g
f
^ ^
^ 3:
476 PROCEDURES OF TONALITY CHANGE
Ex. 27-20 contains a succession of tonal centers too brief to classify as full-fledged
modulations, since none is established by a cadence and subsequently confirmed.
In their succession, however, the sequence of tonal areas on C, E, G, B, and again
C is organized by the chromatically ascending bass line. Each transitory tonic
serves as a momentary point of focus in an unstable, constantly shifting tonal
context.
m =«
\ft 1|^ ^ ^|t^j^-l>^^
f ^^ f
^
^ lA
i
f f
4p ^ i^
i t
it
Ex. 27-21 represents the ultimate in abrupt tonal shifts. The keys in this excerpt,
E-flat, D-flat, and C majors, succeed one another through the successive descents
of the major triads that act as their tonics.
PROCEDURES Of TONALITY CHANGE 477
Flute
Oboes
Clarinetti
in Bk
Bassoons
Horns
in Ek
Trumpets
in Kli
Violin I
Violin n
Viola
Cello
Contrabass
27-22, the sequence can be a stabilizing factor, the repeated pattern sustaining
the ear through a series of distantly related, transitory keys.
Sehr langsahm
A7^ PROCEDURES OF TONAL/T/ CHANGE
Mutation
piu lento
Although not in itself modulatory, mutation often serves as an easy vehicle for
modulating to more remote keys. The following diagram demonstrates that, when
the tonic triad is altered through mutation, a variety of new near related keys are
made readily available.
E (original key)
e (mutation)
In Ex. 27-24 Schubert twice uses this technique to accomplish relatively remote
modulations: A modulating to F major through a minor, and later F modulating
to D-Jlatmajor through/minor. In each case the second form of the chord under-
going the mutation is the pivot chord, i in the old key becoming iii in the new.
Voice
pk) I
rH; P
J^
J u- Tr p ^-^M
^
ft
Piano
^ i y y y
-M-t
Uj f
mijL
p ^
^
^m ^m-'''fn
CP
-^f-^-
c>
tt
;
-f
1
r^—-p y 1
4t
i yijf-
ai
^
m
l^ ' f ^
'
P y vT- '^ ly-
J
m ^
r^r'i ^
*
T^¥-P
~
^
^
P^ —
j¥ i,j)7ri yj r> J' ll ^M^I pr p r p
Exercises
For more detailed assignments see Materials and Structure of Music I, Workbook, Chapter 27.
1. Explore the diatonic pivot chord possibilities of the following pairs of keys:
a. E-Jlat major and its supertonic
b. A major and its mediant minor
c. F minor and itssubdominant minor
d. F sharp and its submediant major
2. What are some possible pivot tones between the following pairs of keys? Write a melody
containing such a modulation.
a. C sharp minor and C major
b. A-flat major and D major
c. E major and A-flat major
3. Using Roman to a near related key. Im-
numerals plot a modulation from a major key
provise at the keyboard, or vocally, a harmonic pattern. Such a
melody that fits this
melody could be a simple arpeggiated version of the chords, or a more complex line.
4. Choose a simple melody and effect a modulation 1 to 2 measures long that leads to a
^restatement of the same melody a major third higher (or lower).
f 5. jUsing the following harmonic scheme, construct a number of 3- or 4-part textures and
\ 'arrange the best for some instrumental combination. „ L~m-.^J
,,'
,
'
6. Find other examples illustrating the various types of tonality changes illustrated in this
chapter.
Ind ex
• Note: Italic type indicates pages on vvhich the definition of a subject appears.
481
482 INDEX
M
Imitation: 164-165, 168, 183-186, 198-199, 280
Imperfect terminal cadence: 55 Major-minor seventh (Mmy): see Chord
Intensity: 1 Mediant: 40, 57, 89, 90
Interval: Meter: 2, 3
approach, 150 compound, 10
cadential, 150/ simple, 4
classification of, 128-129 signature. 4. 10
hannonic. 16, 119, 208-209 Metric accent: see Accent, metric
liarmonic succession of, N3 Metric rhythm: 3
melodic, 16 Modal cadence: see Cadence, modal
scale and, 23 Mode: 35, 125
stable, 134.214 change of, 45
table of, 17 Modulation: 45-47, 85, 160, 161. 435. 439-459
ultimate, 150 chromatic inflection, 466-469
unstable, 144-147, 181, 190, 214 enharmonic, 469-471
vertical, 119, 208-209 implied, 458-459
Inversion: pivot chord, 461-465
chord, 224-227 pivot tone, 465-466
first, 225-227 transitory, 48, 442
dominant, 279-281 Monophonic: 113
dominant seventh, 293-301 Motive: 54, 63-66, 165-166, 186, 187, 376
interval, 27 Mutated chords: 359-364
leading tone, 301-303 Mutation: 45-47, 84, 194, 323, 359-364, 381,
mediant, 347-351 478-480
melodic, 84
secondary dominant, 395-396
subdominant, 281-285 N
submediant, 347-351
supertonic, 330-336 Neighbor chord: 243, 273, 296, 324, 339
tonic, 277-279 Neighbor group: 108-109, 111, 136137
second, 225-227 Neighbor tone: 103-104, 111, 136-137, 177.
dominant, 312-314 241-244, 267, 273-275
dominant seventh, 293, 297-298 Non-chord tone: 266-267, 273-275, 286/, 321,
parallel, 317-318 383, (see also Decorative pitch)
secondary dominant, 395-396 simultaneous, 273-275
subdominant, 314-315 Note head: 5
tonic, 309-314, 315 Note stem: 5
triads, 309-318
third, 293, 298-301
dominant seventh, 298-301
secondary dominant, 395-397
Oblique motion: 137-138, 140, 151. 333
Octave segment: 14
Osrinato: 206, 368, 379
Overtone series: see Harmonic series
Key: 32
distant related, 465, 469, 474
near related, 446-450
parallel. 43
relation, 445-452 Parallel motion: 137-138, 142, 151, 233, 262-
relative, 43 263, 326, 368
signature, 40-44 Parallel phrase construction, 68
1
484 INDEX
^.i^
yijW^^SSg^::-:
-3«SBSffiKT:J7!!rS3SS»??B' rs^S^BR
I