0% found this document useful (0 votes)
50 views20 pages

NIH Public Access: The Relationship Between Social Support and Subjective Well-Being Across Age

Uploaded by

anni
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
0% found this document useful (0 votes)
50 views20 pages

NIH Public Access: The Relationship Between Social Support and Subjective Well-Being Across Age

Uploaded by

anni
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
You are on page 1/ 20

NIH Public Access

Author Manuscript
Soc Indic Res. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2015 June 01.
Published in final edited form as:
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

Soc Indic Res. 2014 June 1; 117(2): 561–576. doi:10.1007/s11205-013-0361-4.

The Relationship Between Social Support and Subjective Well-


Being Across Age
Karen L. Siedlecki,
Department of Psychology, Fordham University, 113 West 60th Street, New York, NY 10023,
USA

Timothy A. Salthouse,
Department of Psychology, University of Virginia, Charlottesville, VA, USA

Shigehiro Oishi, and


Department of Psychology, University of Virginia, Charlottesville, VA, USA

Sheena Jeswani
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

Department of Psychology, Fordham University, 113 West 60th Street, New York, NY 10023,
USA
Karen L. Siedlecki: klsiedlecki@fordham.edu

Abstract
The relationships among types of social support and different facets of subjective well-being (i.e.,
life satisfaction, positive affect, and negative affect) were examined in a sample of 1,111
individuals between the ages of 18 and 95. Using structural equation modeling we found that life
satisfaction was predicted by enacted and perceived support, positive affect was predicted by
family embeddedness and provided support, and negative affect was predicted by perceived
support. When personality variables were included in a subsequent model, the influence of the
social support variables were generally reduced. Invariance analyses conducted across age groups
indicated that there were no substantial differences in predictors of the different types of
subjective well-being across age.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

Keywords
Aging; Life satisfaction; Social support; Subjective well-being

1 Introduction
1.1 Subjective Well-being
Subjective well-being (SWB), thought to comprise a cognitive-judgmental dimension
reflecting life satisfaction and an emotional evaluation characterized by positive and
negative affect, has been linked to important outcomes. For example, research has
demonstrated that happy individuals tend to have larger social rewards, better work

© Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht 2013


Correspondence to: Karen L. Siedlecki, klsiedlecki@fordham.edu.
Siedlecki et al. Page 2

outcomes, greater coping abilities, better immune systems, to be more cooperative, pro-
social, and charitable and to live longer than individuals who are not happy (see
Lyubomirsky et al. 2005 for a review). Because of the positive outcomes associated with
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

SWB it is important to understand the factors that contribute to well-being.

One of the most consistent predictors of SWB is the quality of social relationships (e.g.,
Diener and Seligman 2002). People who have satisfying relationships report feeling happy
more frequently and sadness less frequently, and report being more satisfied with their lives
than those who do not have satisfying relationships. It is however unclear why having
satisfying relationships is so beneficial. One possibility is that individuals who have
satisfying relationships can obtain support when they need it, whereas those who do not
have satisfying relationships cannot easily obtain support when they need it. Another
possibility is that the thought (expectation) of being able to rely on someone when they need
it is comforting, and contributes to a sense of well-being. Either way, social support is likely
to be a key in understanding the link between the quality of social relationships and SWB.
Because social support is a complex concept, as discussed below, the nature of the
association between social support and SWB is not yet clear. The two main goals of the
present investigation are (1) to clarify the link between social support and facets of SWB
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

and (2) to examine whether there are age differences in the strength of the association
between social support and SWB, as the type of support needed by older adults might be
different from younger adults.

1.2 Social Support


Barrera et al. (1981) provided an early definition of social support, specifying that it refers to
the “various forms of aid and assistance supplied by family members, friends, neighbors,
and others” (p. 435), which broadly encompasses a multitude of social interactions.
Although research has linked social support to measures of subjective well-being (Newsom
and Schulz 1996; Pinquart and Sörensen 2000; Thomas 2010), some researchers have found
negative or no consequences of social support on SWB (Lakey et al. 2010; Lee et al. 1995;
Lepore et al. 2008). One possibility for the variation in findings is how researchers
conceptualize and operationalize social support and subjective well-being- both of which are
often used as umbrella terms for complex constructs. There are several facets of social
support that have varied relations with SWB. In this paper we will examine the three
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

components of SWB (i.e., life satisfaction, positive affect, and negative affect) and the
following components of social support, as specified by Barrera (1986)—social integration
or embeddedness, enacted support, and perceived support. We will also focus on provided
support, which recent research has linked to SWB (e.g., Brown et al. 2003).

1.2.1 Social Embeddedness—Social embeddedness refers to the frequency of contact


with those in one’s social network (Barrera 1986; Granovetter 1985). In a meta-analysis,
Finch et al. (1999) found that the number of support providers (a measure of social
embeddedness) had a small negative association (r = −0.11) with psychological distress
(most frequently assessed with measures of depression). However, measures of social
embeddedness do not necessarily assess the quality of the relationship, which may be crucial
in determining emotional consequences. And, in fact, in a meta-analysis of subjective well-

Soc Indic Res. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2015 June 01.
Siedlecki et al. Page 3

being correlates, Pinquart and Sörensen (2000) found that life satisfaction, self-esteem, and
happiness showed a stronger relationship with ratings of social contact quality than with
social contact quantity (i.e., social embeddedness).
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

1.2.2 Enacted Support—Enacted support refers to actual received support, whether it is


received emotional support, received tangible support, or received informational support
(Barrera 1986). Enacted support and well-being share a complex relationship, with some
research showing a small positive relationship between enacted support and psychological
well-being (Finch et al. 1999; Wethington and Kessler 1986) and other research linking
enacted support to increased negative affect (Barrera 1986; Lakey et al. 2010; Seidman et al.
2006). Seidman et al. (2006) suggest that the relationship between enacted support and
distress may be mixed due to the confounding effect of distress on emotional well-being.
Obtaining tangible or emotional enacted support would likely occur as a result of
experiencing some form of distress, and the negative effects of this distress may be linked to
lowered well-being. Further, receiving support may activate feelings of low self-worth from
not having the capacity to take care of oneself (Lepore et al. 2008) and being in debt to the
support provider (Marroquín 2011). For example, Lee et al. (1995) found that among older
adults, aid received from children was positively related to depression. The authors suggest
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

that this relationship between received aid and parental depression can be linked to
“perception of a loss of independence in old age” (p. 823).

Collectively, research on enacted support indicates that enacted support is specifically


associated with an increase in negative affect or mood (as opposed to a decrease in positive
affect or life satisfaction) (e.g., Bolger et al. 2000; Liang et al. 2001; Shrout et al. 2006).

1.2.3 Provided Support—Provided support, which refers to the emotional, tangible, or


informational help that one is able to supply to others, has also been linked to health and
well-being (Brown et al. 2003; Lu 1997). Thomas (2010) specifically examined whether it is
more beneficial to give or receive support in a sample of older adults. She found that total
provided support was the strongest predictor of well-being in a model that included several
predictors, whereas total received (i.e., enacted) support was positively associated with well-
being, but only when examined in a model without provided support. Thomas (2010)
concludes that “it is often better for the well-being of older adults to give than to receive” (p.
351). Similarly, others have found that those who have a higher tendency to provide social
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

support report lower levels of depression (Piferi and Lawler 2006), and in a daily diary study
providing support to a partner was associated with both reduced negative mood and
improved positive mood (Gleason et al. 2003).

There are several potential explanations as to why providing support may influence SWB.
Providing support to others may give one a sense of meaning and purpose (Taylor and
Turner 2001), or increase self-evaluations (Williamson and Clark 1989). Alternatively, one
may feel pride or pleasure from the opportunity of influencing someone’s life, regardless of
how small the act (Sommer and Bourgeois 2010), and research on altruism suggests that it
increases positive affect (Post 2005). An exception to these findings is the experience of
caregiving, which is related to decreased well-being (e.g., Pinquart and Sörensen 2006).

Soc Indic Res. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2015 June 01.
Siedlecki et al. Page 4

1.2.4 Perceived Support—Perceived support refers to satisfaction with support


exchanges and anticipated support. It differs from enacted support in that perceived support
refers to the expectation that support will be provided rather than referring to specific
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

instances in which one has received support. Generally, perceptions of availability of social
support have been linked to better outcomes during times of stress (e.g., Sarason et al. 1997;
Wethington and Kessler 1986). In a meta-analysis Finch et al. (1999) reported that the
weighted r for the relationship among ratings of perceived availability of support and
psychological distress ranged from −0.29 to −0.35. These weighted correlations were
substantially larger than the correlations reported among measures of enacted support and
psychological distress (rs ranging from −0.12 to −0.17). In addition, Finch et al. (1999)
reported that perceived support satisfaction was a unique predictor of depression when
examined in the context of a structural equation model with several simultaneous predictors
(including the Big Five personality traits and types of coping). In regards to the cognitive-
judgmental component of SWB, Newsom and Schulz (1996) found that greater perceived
social support was correlated with higher life satisfaction and linked to fewer depressive
symptoms in older adults.

1.3 Personality
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

Personality traits are important factors to consider when examining predictors of SWB. In
particular, neuroticism and extraversion have consistently been shown to predict the
affective components of SWB (e.g., Diener and Lucas 1999; Schimmack et al. 2002). Steel
et al. (2008) argue that the constructs of SWB and personality constructs are very similar
and, “in particular, Neuroticism and Extraversion are nearly identical to two elements of
SWB, negative and positive affect, respectively. Neurotic individuals tend to be anxious,
easily upset, and moody or depressed, whereas extraverts tend to be sociable, optimistic,
outgoing, energetic, expressive, active, assertive, and exciting” (p. 139). Furthermore,
extraverted individuals are more likely to experience positive life events and individuals
high in neuroticism are more likely to experience negative life events (e.g., Magnus et al.
1993). In a recent meta-analyses Steel et al. (2008) reported the mean correlation between
life satisfaction (one component of SWB) and each of the Big Five traits as measured by the
NEO inventory (Costa and McCrae 1992) are as follows: openness to experience (0.03),
conscientiousness (0.25), extraversion (0.49), agreeableness (0.30), and neuroticism (−0.46).
Steel et al. (2008) report that personality can account for as much as 39 % of the variance in
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

SWB.

1.4 Age Differences


Carstensen’s socioemotional selectivity theory (1987, 1992) proposes that social goals
change with age. As people get older, their desire to interact with acquaintances and their
satisfaction levels from these interactions decline. Thus, they selectively prune their social
networks, focusing their time and energy on emotionally intimate social contacts, such as
close friends and family. For example, Bowling (2011) found that compared to those under
the age of 65, those over the age of 65 report having significantly fewer people to turn to for
comfort and support in a time of crisis and significantly fewer people available for practical
support (see supplemental material Tables 3 and 5 in Bowling 2011). However, it is unclear
whether the smaller numbers of individuals in the social network are due to choice or to

Soc Indic Res. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2015 June 01.
Siedlecki et al. Page 5

circumstances. Regardless, these differences in social contacts and social goals suggest there
may be age differences in how people report their social support experiences and subjective
well-being.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

It is clear that social relationships are an important aspect of well-being in older age. For
instance, Bowling et al. (2003) asked a series of open-ended questions to 999 individuals
over the age of 65 regarding quality of life (e.g., what makes your life good?). Eighty-one
percent of the sample mentioned that social relationships contributed to their quality of life;
this was the most frequently cited answer. How different types of social relationships (and
social support) are related to well-being in older age is less clear. Thomas (2010) suggests
that receiving support from others can threaten an older adult’s identity and sense of
competence, thereby creating a negative relationship between enacted support and well-
being. It may be the case, then, that enacted support is associated with increased well-being
in younger age groups, but not among older adults.

1.5 The Current Study


As summarized above, the nature of the association between social support and SWB may
differ, depending on the aspects of social support and SWB. The first goal of this study was
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

to examine the relationship among different dimensions of social support to different


dimensions of SWB. Based on the research summarized above, we expect that both provided
and perceived support will be positively related to SWB. Based on previous research, we do
not expect that there will be a strong relationship between social embeddedness and SWB,
especially since social embeddedness does not speak to the quality of relationships. Because
of the complicated nature of enacted support (i.e., the need for enacted support suggests
distress) it is difficult to predict the relationship between enacted support and SWB.
However, we expect that, based on the findings by Lee et al. (1995), the relationship
between enacted support and well-being may differ across age such that enacted support is
associated with increased well-being in young adults, but not among older adults. This leads
to our second goal, which was to examine whether there are age differences in the strength
of the association between social support and SWB, as the type of support needed by older
adults might be different from younger adults (particularly in the relationship between
enacted support and SWB). Because self-reports of social support might be colored by
respondents’ personality, in particular extraversion and neuroticism, we also examined
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

whether the relations among social support and SWB are altered when personality variables
are included in the model.

2 Method
2.1 Participants
Participants consisted of 1,166 adults who completed the Social Network questionnaire
(Shaw et al. 2007). The Mini-Mental State Exam (MMSE; Folstein et al. 1975) was used to
screen for dementia and individuals with a MMSE score of <24 (n = 55; 4.7 %) were
excluded from the analyses, resulting in 1,111 individuals between the ages of 18 and 95.
Participants were recruited from the Charlottesville, VA community through newspaper
advertisements, flyers, and referrals from prior participants.

Soc Indic Res. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2015 June 01.
Siedlecki et al. Page 6

One percent of the sample (n = 11) identified as American Indian/Alaskan native, 1.2 % (n =
13) identified as Asian, 9 % (n = 100) identified as Black, 85.4 % (n = 948) identified as
White, and 3.1 % (n = 34) identified as “more than one race”. Four participants (0.4 %) did
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

not answer the question. Additional participant characteristics are reported in Table 1,
divided by age group.

2.2 Measures
Participants completed several questionnaires, in addition to the Social Network
questionnaire, including the Satisfaction with Life Scale (SWLS; Diener et al. 1985), and
measures of depression, anxiety, positive affect, negative affect, and personality. The
research design was correlational in nature and all participants completed the same
measures.

2.2.1 Social Network Questionnaire—The Social Network Questionnaire (Shaw et al.


2007) comprises 27 items and was designed to assess the following dimensions: (1) social
embeddedness as provided by family and social embeddedness as provided by friends, (2)
enacted support which includes emotional, tangible, and informational support received
from others, (3) provided support, as assessed with emotional, tangible, and informational
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

support provided by respondents to others and (4) perceived support. In the current study,
social embeddedness as provided by family and as provided by friends was split into two
separate variables because the correlation among the variables representing the sum of each
dimension was fairly low in magnitude (r = 0.16, p < .01) providing evidence of
discriminant validity between the two constructs.

Composite variables were created for each of the social network dimensions, corresponding
to the dimensions described above. Both the family embeddedness and friend embeddedness
variables were the sum of three items from the scale that measured the frequency of contact
with family and friends. The enacted support variable was the sum of three categories of
enacted support from the scale (emotional support, tangible support, and informational
support). The provided support was the sum of the three categories of provided support
(provided emotional support, provided tangible support, and provided informational
support). The perceived support variable was the sum of two categories (satisfaction with
support exchanges, anticipated support). See Shaw et al. (2007) Table 1 for scale items.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

2.2.2 Subjective Well-being—The tripartite structure of SWB (life satisfaction, positive


affect, and negative affect) was examined recently in a review by Busseri and Sadava
(2011), who describe five conceptual models that have been used in the literature to examine
SWB. In the current paper, SWB is consistent with Model 1 described by Busseri and
Sadava (2011) in which life satisfaction, positive affect and negative affect are three
different, yet correlated, aspects of SWB. As such, subjective well-being was represented by
three latent variables—life satisfaction, positive affect, and negative affect.

Depression was measured with the Center for Epidemiological Studies-Depression scale
(CES-D; Radloff 1977). In the current sample Cronbach’s alpha was 0.88. The trait anxiety
subscale of the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI; Spielberger et al. 1983) was used to
assess trait anxiety (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.92). Positive and negative affect was assessed

Soc Indic Res. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2015 June 01.
Siedlecki et al. Page 7

with the Positive and Negative Affect Scale (PANAS; Watson et al. 1988). In the current
sample Cronbach’s alpha was 0.91 and 0.88 for the positive items and negative items,
respectively. For each measure, higher values indicate greater degree of each trait assessed
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

(i.e., depressive symptoms, trait anxiety, positive affect, and negative affect).

The negative affect construct comprised three observed variables representing the sum of the
CES-D items (positive items were reverse scored), the sum of the STAI- Trait scale items
(positive items were reverse scored), and the sum of the negative items from the PANAS.
The positive affect construct comprised the PANAS positive items; to reduce the number of
indicators, three parcels were created from the 10 positive items. The life satisfaction
construct comprised the five items from the SWLS (Diener et al. 1985; see Fig. 1) and
higher values indicate higher levels of life satisfaction.

2.2.3 Personality—The “Big Five” dimensions of personality were assessed with the 50-
item version of the IPIP inventory (Goldberg 1999). Ten items each were used to measure
openness to experience, conscientiousness, extraversion, agreeableness, and emotional
stability (the opposite end of the scale from neuroticism). The 50-item IPIP scale does not
assess lower-order facets. For a complete listing of the items go to http://ipip.ori.org/
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

New_IPIP-50-item-scale.htm. Composite scores were created by summing the items within


each trait respectively. For example, a composite extraversion score was created by
summing the 10 extraversion items (after reverse scoring the appropriate items). For each
composite score higher values indicate higher levels of each personality characteristic.

2.3 Analyses
Structural equation models and confirmatory factor analyses were conducted with Amos
19.0 (Arbuckle 2010). Maximum likelihood estimation was used to deal with missing data.
Two separate models were examined. In Model 1 the five social network domains,
represented by composite variables, were simultaneous predictors of three latent constructs
reflecting subjective well-being (life satisfaction, positive affect, negative affect; see Fig. 1).
The three SWB constructs were examined simultaneously to be consistent with a common
conceptualization of SWB (Busseri and Sadava 2011). The social network domains were
represented by composite variables, rather than latent constructs, in order to reduce the
number of parameters to be estimated, thereby ensuring adequate power when the sample
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

was divided into three age groups. In Model 2 the five personality composite variables were
entered into the model as predictors of SWB.

2.3.1 Invariance Analyses—Invariance analyses were conducted in order to examine


age difference in the relations among the variables that comprise the SWB constructs, and to
examine differences in the magnitude of the standardized coefficients from the predictors to
the SWB constructs. Several levels of invariance analyses were evaluated. Configural
invariance, in which the factor structure is constrained to be the same across the groups,
serves as a baseline model. Good model fit provides evidence of configural invariance.
Metric invariance is examined by constraining the unstandardized coefficients from the
latent constructs to the observed variables to be the same across the groups. Scalar
invariance is examined by constraining the intercepts on the latent variable indicators to be

Soc Indic Res. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2015 June 01.
Siedlecki et al. Page 8

equal across the three age groups. An aspect of structural invariance was also examined by
constraining coefficients from the predictors to the latent constructs to be the same across
the groups. Invariance is tested by comparing the fit of each model to the previous model
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

(i.e., the fit of the metric invariance model is compared to the fit of the configural invariance
model, the fit of the scalar invariance model is compared to the fit of the metric invariance
model, etc.). For all model comparisons, a change in CFI greater than −0.01 is an indication
of a substantial decrease in fit (Cheung and Rensvold 2002). Change in χ2 per change in df
can also be examined but because of its dependence on sample size it is often supplemented
with inspection of change in CFI, and inspection of overall fit. If a model fits substantially
worse than the previous model then evidence for invariance is lacking.

3 Results
Means, standard deviations, and age correlations for each of the variables are presented in
Table 1. Age was significantly positively correlated with 4 of the 5 SWLS items,
significantly positively correlated with positive affect, and significantly negatively
correlated with each of the negative affect variables. Age was also significantly negatively
correlated with four of the five social network composite scores such that increased age was
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

associated with less friend and family embeddedness, less enacted support, and less
provided support.

3.1 Predictors of Subjective Well-being


3.1.1 Model 1—The model depicted in Fig. 1 fit well across the total sample (χ2 = 439.99,
df = 81, χ2/df = 5.43, CFI = 0.957, RMSEA = 0.063). The standardized loadings from each
latent construct to its respective observed variables were all fairly large (ranging from 0.56
to 0.92) and significantly greater than zero (p < .01), thereby providing evidence of
convergent validity. The life satisfaction construct significantly correlated −0.65 and 0.32
with the negative affect and positive affect constructs, respectively. The positive and
negative affect constructs were negatively correlated (r = −0.48, p < .01). Standardized
regression coefficients from the social network variables to the latent subjective well-being
constructs are presented in Table 2. Across the total sample, perceived support was a
significant predictor of life satisfaction and negative affect. Enacted support was also a
significant positive predictor of life satisfaction. Family embeddedness and provided support
were significant predictors of positive affect.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

3.1.2 Model 2—The five personality composite variables were subsequently included in
the model as predictors of SWB. Only those coefficients that were significantly different
from zero were retained in the final model. Agreeableness and openness were not
significantly associated with any facet of SWB so they were excluded from the model. The
relationship between extraversion and life satisfaction was not significant, thus that path was
also excluded from the model. The ensuing model fit well: χ2 = 488.00, df = 106, χ2/df =
4.60, CFI = 0.96, RMSEA = 0.057. With the inclusion of the personality variables as
predictors, provided support was no longer a significant predictor of positive affect. To
further assess this relationship partial correlations between provided support and positive
affect were examined controlling for each of the relevant personality variables (i.e.,

Soc Indic Res. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2015 June 01.
Siedlecki et al. Page 9

emotional stability, extraversion, and conscientiousness). Provided support and positive


affect correlated 0.14 (p < .01) and this correlation was changed to 0.10 (p < .01), 0.15 (p < .
01), and 0.12 (p < .01) when controlling for extraversion, emotional stability, and
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

conscientiousness respectively. When all three personality variables were controlled for in
one model, the correlation between provided support and positive affect was 0.09 (p < .01).

In addition, the magnitude of the relationships between perceived support and life
satisfaction and perceived support and negative affect were substantially reduced (0.18*
reduced to 0.10*, and −0.25* reduced to −0.11*). However, the overlapping 99 %
confidence intervals suggest that the differences in magnitude are not significant.

Further, emotional stability and conscientiousness were positively associated with life
satisfaction and positive affect and negatively associated with negative affect. Extraversion
was positively associated with positive affect and life satisfaction.

3.2 Age Differences


To examine whether there were differences across age, the sample was divided into three
age groups: young adults (ages 20–45, n = 230), middle-aged adults (ages 46–65, n = 527),
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

and older adults (ages 66–95, n = 354) and invariance analyses were conducted on the model
with 8 predictors (family embeddedness, friend embeddedness, enacted support, provided
support, perceived support, emotional stability, extraversion, and conscientiousness). The
results presented in Table 3 indicate that despite significant decreases in fit as measured by
the Chi square, the change in CFI was not substantial for any model comparison.
Furthermore, the final invariance model fit well in absolute terms (χ2 = 931.89, df = 402,
χ2/df = 2.32; CFI = 0.94; RMSEA = 0.034). Thus, using guidelines suggested by Cheung
and Rensvold (2002), the results indicate that the model demonstrated configural, metric,
scalar and structural invariance across the three age groups. Specifically, the factor structure,
the magnitude of the factor loadings, the observed variables intercepts, and the magnitudes
of the coefficients from the predictors to the latent dependent variables were not
substantially different across the age groups.

4 Discussion
Both social support and SWB are multi-dimensional constructs and in this study we sought
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

to conduct a comprehensive investigation of the relationship among them. A novel aspect of


this paper was the evaluation of how the different facets of social support relate to all facets
of SWB simultaneously. By examining the social support predictors in one model we are
able to evaluate the unique contribution of each predictor, independent of the variance it
shares with the other predictors. Consistent with previous research, we found that the
different forms of SWB were differentially related to the facets of social support.
Specifically, life satisfaction was predicted by enacted and perceived support, positive affect
was predicted by family embeddedness and provided support, and negative affect was
predicted by perceived support.

Whereas past research has found that enacted support is often associated with increases in
negative affect, we found that enacted support was associated with increased life

Soc Indic Res. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2015 June 01.
Siedlecki et al. Page 10

satisfaction. One potential explanation of this finding can be linked to recent work that
found that material conditions (income, having a television, having a computer, access to
internet, etc.) are more strongly associated with life satisfaction than positive or negative
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

affect, whereas social psychological prosperity (measured by perceived social support,


feeling respected, learning something new, doing what you do best, choosing how your time
is spent) was more strongly associated with positive affect than life satisfaction (Diener et al.
2010). Receiving support (i.e., enacted support) suggests that living conditions were
improved when needed. To the extent that life satisfaction is based in part on living
conditions, it makes sense that enacted support would be positively associated specifically
with life satisfaction.

Perceived support was a predictor of both increased life satisfaction and decreased negative
affect, which is consistent with previous research (Newsom and Schulz 1996; Sarason et al.
1997). Perceived support may increase one’s coping competence by providing comfort that
supporters are available to help if needed (Wethington and Kessler 1986). Perceived support
is also not confounded with incidents of distress in the same way that enacted support may
be.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

In the current study, social embeddedness was measured by the frequency of contact with
family and friends. Results showed that family embeddedness predicted positive affect. Our
results are also consistent with results reported by Pinquart and Sörensen’s (2000) meta-
analysis of SWB in older age, which found that social network size (a measure of
embeddedness) is associated with happiness (a measure of positive affect). Although
Pinquart and Sörensen (2000) found that social network quality was a better predictor of
SWB than social network size, social embeddedness has also been viewed as a
psychological resource.

The relationship between neuroticism and extraversion and the affective components of
SWB is well-documented (e.g., Diener and Lucas 1999; Finch et al. 1999; Schimmack et al.
2002; Steel et al. 2008). This relationship can be explained through both direct and indirect
mechanisms. For example, research on the neurotransmitter serotonin suggests that there is
evidence of a neurobiological link between neuroticism and depressive and affective
disorders (e.g., Frokjaer et al. 2008). Personality can also indirectly impact SWB by
influencing life events. Notably, sociability (a facet of extraversion) has been linked to
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

increased positive affect (Emmons and Diener 1986); sociable individuals spend more time
in social situations, which has been linked to happiness (Emmons et al. 1985). Life
satisfaction has also been linked to personality traits, as demonstrated by the results of the
meta-analyses reported by Steel et al. (2008). Although the strongest associations involve
neuroticism and extraversion, conscientiousness and agreeableness also have moderate
associations with SWB. We found that when entered simultaneously in a model that also
included social support variables as predictors, emotional stability (conceptually at the
opposite end of the scale to neuroticism), extraversion, and conscientiousness had significant
associations with each facet of SWB.

Interestingly, once the personality variables were included in the model, the relationship
between positive affect and provided support became non-significant. Since provided

Soc Indic Res. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2015 June 01.
Siedlecki et al. Page 11

support has been consistently linked to increased well-being (e.g., Brown et al. 2003; Lu
1997; Thomas 2010), our findings suggest that it is important to include assessments of
personality when evaluating the relationships between social support and SWB. The
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

relationship between positive affect and provided support was reduced the most when
controlling for the personality dimension of extraversion. This suggests that personality, and
extraversion in particular, is related to provided support. This may be because individuals
who are more extraverted generally have more friends (e.g., Demir and Weitekamp 2007;
Hills and Argyle 2001; Pollet et al. 2011) and thus additional opportunities to provide
support. In addition, the magnitude of the relationships between perceived support and
negative affect and between perceived support and life satisfaction were reduced when the
personality variables were included in the model (although the differences were not
significant). This can be partially explained by the fact that perceived support has been
viewed as stable, individual differences characteristic which may reflect relationship well-
being (e.g., Goodwin et al. 2004; Sarason et al. 1986). For example, Sandler and Barrera
(1984) found that enacted support did not correlate with measures of anxiety, depression,
somatization, or psychological disorder, but a support satisfaction scale (i.e., perceived
support) related to all of the measures.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

We found no significant differences across three age groups (i.e., young, middle-aged, and
older adults) in terms of the relations among the variables, magnitudes of loadings, or the
magnitude of the standardized coefficients from the predictors to SWB. These findings
therefore suggest that social support and personality have similar influences on SWB across
age. Thus despite potential differences in social goals with age, the influence of the different
facets of social support do not appear to differ significantly across age.

4.1 Limitations and Future Directions


Despite the fairly large sample that spans the adult lifespan, there are some limitations to the
current study. Our sample comprised individuals who were relatively healthy and performed
normally on a screening test for dementia. As such, their need for help in everyday life may
be limited, and our findings may not generalize to populations who are not healthy and
relatively independent. In addition, our sample was composed of mostly White participants
and thus future studies should test the generalizabilty of our findings to non-White, non-
Western samples. Because the study was cross-sectional in nature, we are unable to make
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

casual inferences—longitudinal studies would help to explicate the directionality of the


relationships.

5 Conclusion
A major aim of the current study was to understand the nature of the association between
social support and SWB, in an attempt to understand why quality of social relationships is so
beneficial. We conducted a comprehensive examination of the relationship among two
multi-dimensional constructs—social support and SWB. Our results suggest that the
different facets of SWB can be predicted by different aspects of social support. We found
that perceived support was a significant predictor of life satisfaction and negative affect,
enacted support was a significant predictor of life satisfaction, and family embeddedness had
unique relations with positive affect.

Soc Indic Res. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2015 June 01.
Siedlecki et al. Page 12

It makes sense that both enacted support and perceived support are predictors of life
satisfaction. Life satisfaction is an overall evaluation of life and thus perceived social
support (namely an expectation of being able to rely on someone when needed) is important.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

Furthermore, life satisfaction is based in part on living conditions so enacted support may be
an important component of improving living conditions when needed.

The relationship between perceived support and negative affect may be interpreted as
consistent with previous research that has shown that perceived support is correlated with
anxiety and depression (Sandler and Barrera 1984), and to some degree may be considered
an individual differences characteristic. Thus is it not surprising that the relationship
between perceived support and negative affect was reduced when the personality variables
(including emotional stability) were included in the model.

In conclusion, our findings suggest that different facets of social support differentially
predict the facets of SWB. It remains unclear, however, why a particular facet of social
support is linked to a particular facet of SWB. For example, the relationship between
enacted support and living conditions can be examined to evaluate whether the relationship
between enacted support and life satisfaction can be explained by their link to living
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

conditions. Future studies should seek to further elucidate underlying mechanisms between
social support and SWB.

References
Arbuckle, JL. Amos (Version 19.0) [Computer Program]. Chicago: SPSS; 2010.
Barrera M. Distinctions between social support concepts, measures, and models. American Journal of
Community Psychology. 1986; 14(4):413–445.10.1007/BF00922627
Barrera M, Sandler IN, Ramsay TB. Preliminary development of a scale of social support: Studies on
college students. American Journal of Community Psychology. 1981; 9(4):435–447.10.1007/
BF00918174
Bolger N, Zuckerman A, Kessler RC. Invisible support and adjustment to stress. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology. 2000; 79:953–961. [PubMed: 11138764]
Bowling A. Do older and younger people differ in their reported well-being? A national survey of
adults in Britain. Family Practice. 2011; 28(2):145–155.10.1093/fampra/cmq082 [PubMed:
21084566]
Bowling A, Gabriel Z, Dykes J, Dowding LM, Evans O, Fleissing A, et al. Let’s ask them: A national
survey of definitions of quality of life and its enhancement among people ages 65 and over.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

International Journal of Aging and Human Development. 2003; 56:269–306. [PubMed: 14738211]
Brown SL, Nesse RM, Vinokur AD, Smith DM. Providing social support may be more beneficial than
receiving it: Results from a prospective study of mortality. Psychological Science. 2003; 14(4):320–
327.10.1111/1467-9280.14461 [PubMed: 12807404]
Busseri MA, Sadava SW. A review of the tripartite structure of subjective well-being, implications for
conceptualization, operationalization, and synthesis. Personality and Social Psychology Review.
2011; 15:290–314. [PubMed: 21131431]
Carstensen, LL. Age-related changes in social activity. In: Carstensen, LL.; Edelstein, BA., editors.
Handbook of clinical gerontology. Elmsford: Pergamon Press; 1987. p. 222-237.
Carstensen LL. Social and emotional patterns in adulthood: Support for socioemotional selectivity
theory. Psychology and Aging. 1992; 7:331–338.10.1037/0882-7974.7.3.331 [PubMed: 1388852]
Cheung GW, Rensvold RB. Evaluating goodness-of-fit indexes for testing measurement invariance.
Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal. 2002; 9:233–255.

Soc Indic Res. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2015 June 01.
Siedlecki et al. Page 13

Costa, PT., Jr; McCrae, RR. Revised NEO personality inventory (NEO-PI-R) and NEO five-factor
inventory (NEO-FFI) professional manual. Odessa: Psychological Assessment Resources; 1992.
Demir M, Weitekamp LA. I am so happy cause today I found my friend: Friendship and personality as
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

predictors of happiness. Journal of Happiness Studies. 2007; 8(2):181–211.10.1007/


s10902-006-9012-7
Diener, E.; Lucas, RE. Personality and subjective wellbeing. In: Kahneman, D.; Diener, E.; Schwarz,
N., editors. Well-being: The foundations of hedonic psychology. New York: Russell Sage; 1999.
p. 213-229.
Diener E, Seligman MEP. Very happy people. Psychological Science. 2002; 13:81–84. [PubMed:
11894851]
Diener E, Emmons RA, Larsen RJ, Griffin S. The satisfaction with life scale. Journal of Personality
Assessment. 1985; 49:71–75. [PubMed: 16367493]
Diener E, Ng W, Harter J, Arora R. Wealth and happiness across the world: Material prosperity
predicts life evaluation, whereas psychosocial prosperity predicts positive feeling. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology. 2010; 99:52–61.10.1037/a0018066 [PubMed: 20565185]
Emmons RA, Diener E. Influence of impulsivity and sociability on subjective well-being. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology. 1986; 50:1211–1215.
Emmons RA, Diener E, Larsen RJ. Choice of situations and congruence models of interactionism.
Personality and Individual Differences. 1985; 6:693–702.
Finch JF, Okun MA, Pool GJ, Ruehlman LS. A comparison of the influence of conflictual and
supportive social interactions on psychological distress. Journal of Personality. 1999; 67:581–621.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

[PubMed: 10444852]
Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. Mini-mental state: A practical method for grading the
cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research. 1975; 12:189–198.
[PubMed: 1202204]
Frokjaer VG, Mortensen EL, Nielsen FÅ, Haugbol S, Pinborg LH, Adams KH, et al. Frontolimbic
serotonin 2A receptor binding in healthy subjects is associated with personality risk factors for
affective disorder. Biological Psychiatry. 2008; 63:569–576.10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.07.009
[PubMed: 17884017]
Gleason MJ, Iida M, Bolger N, Shrout PE. Daily supportive equity in close relationships. Personality
and Social Psychology Bulletin. 2003; 29:1036–1045.10.1177/0146167203253473 [PubMed:
15189621]
Goldberg, LR. A broad-bandwidth, public domain, personality inventory measuring the lower-level
facets of several five-factor models. In: Mervielde, I.; Deary, I.; De Fruyt, F.; Ostendorf, F.,
editors. Personality psychology in Europe. Vol. 7. The Netherlands: Tilburg University Press;
1999. p. 7-28.
Goodwin R, Costa P, Adonu J. Social support and its consequences: ‘Positive’ and ‘deficiency’ values
and their implications for support and self-esteem. British Journal of Social Psychology. 2004;
43:465–474. [PubMed: 15479542]
Granovetter M. Economic action and social structure: The problem of embeddedness. American
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

Journal of Sociology. 1985; 91(3):481–510.10.1086/228311


Hills P, Argyle M. Happiness, introversion-extraversion and happy introverts. Personality and
Individual Differences. 2001; 30:595–608.
Lakey B, Orehek E, Hain KL, VanVleet M. Enacted support’s links to negative affect and perceived
support are more consistent with theory when social influences are isolated from trait influences.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 2010; 36(1):132–142.10.1177/0146167209349375
[PubMed: 19875827]
Lee GR, Netzer JK, Coward RT. Depression among older parents: The role of inter-generational
exchange. Journal of Marriage and the Family. 1995; 57(3):823–833.10.2307/353935
Lepore SJ, Glaser DB, Roberts KJ. On the positive relation between received social support and
negative affect: A test of the triage and self-esteem threat models in women with breast cancer.
Psycho-Oncology. 2008; 17(12):1210–1215.10.1002/pon.1347 [PubMed: 18613318]
Liang J, Krause NM, Bennett JM. Social exchange and well-being: Is giving better than receiving?
Psychology and Aging. 2001; 16:511–523. [PubMed: 11554527]

Soc Indic Res. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2015 June 01.
Siedlecki et al. Page 14

Lu L. Social support, reciprocity, and well-being. The Journal of Social Psychology. 1997; 137:618–
628.10.1080/00224549709595483 [PubMed: 9362145]
Lyubomirsky S, Sheldon KM, Schkade D. Pursuing happiness: The architecture of sustainable change.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

Review of General Psychology. 2005; 9:111–131.


Magnus K, Diener E, Fujita F, Pavot W. Extraversion and neuroticism as predictors of objective life
events: A longitudinal analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1993; 65(5):1046–
1053.10.1037/0022-3514.65.5.1046 [PubMed: 8246112]
Marroquín B. Interpersonal emotion regulation as a mechanism of social support in depression.
Clinical Psychology Review. 2011; 31:1276–1290.10.1016/j.cpr.2011.09.005 [PubMed:
21983267]
Newsom JT, Schulz R. Social support as a mediator in the relation between functional status and
quality of life in older adults. Psychology and Aging. 1996; 11(1):34–
44.10.1037/0882-7974.11.1.34 [PubMed: 8726368]
Piferi RL, Lawler KA. Social support and ambulatory blood pressure: An examination of both
receiving and giving. International Journal of Psychophysiology. 2006; 62:328–336. [PubMed:
16905215]
Pinquart M, Sörensen S. Influences of socioeconomic status, social network, and competence on
subjective well-being in later life: A meta-analysis. Psychology and Aging. 2000; 15(2):187–
224.10.1037/0882-7974.15.2.187 [PubMed: 10879576]
Pinquart M, Sörensen S. Gender differences in caregiver stressors, social resources, and health: An
updated meta-analysis. The Journals of Gerontology: Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

Sciences. 2006; 61B:33–45.10.1093/geronb/61.1.P33


Pollet TV, Roberts SB, Dunbar RM. Extraverts have larger social network layers: But do not feel
emotionally closer to individuals at any layer. Journal of Individual Differences. 2011; 32(3):161–
169.10.1027/1614-0001/a000048
Post SG. Altruism, happiness, and health: It’s good to be good. International Journal of Behavioral
Medicine. 2005; 12:66–77.10.1207/s15327558ijbm1202_4 [PubMed: 15901215]
Radloff LS. The CES-D Scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population.
Applied Psychological Measurement. 1977; 1:385–401.
Sandler IN, Barrera M Jr. Toward a multi-method approach to assessing the effects of social support.
American Journal of Community Psychology. 1984; 12:37–52. [PubMed: 6711491]
Sarason, BR.; Sarason, IG.; Gurung, RAR. Close personal relationships and health outcomes: A key to
the role of social support. In: Duck, S., editor. Handbook of personal relationships. 2. New York:
Wiley; 1997. p. 547-573.
Sarason IG, Sarason BR, Shearin EN. Social support as an individual differences variable: Its stability,
origins, and relational aspects. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1986; 50:845–855.
Schimmack U, Diener E, Oishi S. Life-satisfaction is a momentary judgment and a stable personality
characteristic: The use of chronically accessible and stable sources. Journal of Personality. 2002;
70:345–384.10.1111/1467-6494.05008 [PubMed: 12049164]
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

Seidman G, Shrout PE, Bolger N. Why is enacted social support associated with increased distress?
Using simulation to test two possible sources of spuriousness. Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin. 2006; 32(1):52–65.10.1177/0146167205279582 [PubMed: 16317188]
Shaw BA, Krause N, Liang J, Bennett J. Tracking changes in social relations throughout late life.
Journal of Gerontology: Social Sciences. 2007; 62B:S90–S99.
Shrout PE, Herman C, Bolger N. The costs and benefits of practical and emotional support on
adjustment: A daily diary study of couples experiencing acute stress. Personal Relationships. 2006;
13:115–134.
Sommer KL, Bourgeois MJ. Linking the perceived ability to influence others to subjective well-being:
A need-based approach. Social Influence. 2010; 5(3):220–244.10.1080/15534510903513860
Spielberger, CD.; Gorsuch, RL.; Lushene, R.; Vagg, PR.; Jacobs, GA. Manual for the state-trait
anxiety inventory form Y. Palo Alto: Consulting Psychologists Press; 1983.
Steel P, Schmidt J, Shultz J. Refining the relationship between personality and subjective well-being.
Psychological Bulletin. 2008; 134(1):138–161.10.1037/0033-2909.134.1.138 [PubMed:
18193998]

Soc Indic Res. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2015 June 01.
Siedlecki et al. Page 15

Taylor J, Turner R. A longitudinal study of the role and significance of mattering to others for
depressive symptoms. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 2001; 42(3):310–
325.10.2307/3090217 [PubMed: 11668776]
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

Thomas PA. Is it better to give or to receive? Social support and the well-being of older adults. The
Journals of Gerontology: Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences. 2010; 65B:351–
357.10.1093/geronb/gbp113
Watson D, Clark LA, Tellegen A. Development and validation of brief measures of positive and
negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1988; 54:1063–
1070. [PubMed: 3397865]
Wethington E, Kessler RC. Perceived support, received support, and adjustment to stressful life events.
Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 1986; 27:78–89. [PubMed: 3711634]
Williamson GM, Clark MS. Providing help and desired relationship type as determinants of changes in
moods and self-evaluations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1989; 56:722–734.
[PubMed: 2724066]
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

Soc Indic Res. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2015 June 01.
Siedlecki et al. Page 16
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

Fig. 1.
Rectangles represent observed variables and circles represent latent constructs. The latent
variables labeled “e” represent the error and unique variance associated with the observed
variables. The two-headed arrows represent correlations among the variables. The paths
from the observed variables to the latent constructs represent the relationship between the
predictors and the latent constructs. The paths from the latent constructs to the observed
variables represent the loading of each variable onto its respective construct
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

Soc Indic Res. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2015 June 01.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript

Table 1

Means, SD, and age correlations for each variable

Total Young (ages 20–45) Middle-aged (ages 46–65) Older (ages 66–95) Age r
N 1,111 230 527 354
Siedlecki et al.

Age 57.10 (15.26) 34.17 (7.26) 56.03 (5.35) 73.61 (6.18)


Gender (% female) 67.1 67.8 71.7 59.6
Years of education 16.10 (2.77) 16.05 (2.65) 15.86 (2.71) 16. 50 (2.89) 0.09*
MMSE 28.72 (1.36) 28.86 (1.31) 28.79 (1.33) 28.53 (1.40) −0.11*
SWLS
Ideal 4.76 (1.60) 4.53 (1.68) 4.67 (1.64) 5.02 (1.44) 0.12*
Conditions 5.02 (0.150) 4.87 (1.64) 4.95 (1.53) 5.22 (1.37) 0.09*
Satisfied 5.17 (1.45) 4.97 (1.57) 5.11 (1.47) 5.38 (1.32) 0.11*
Important 5.31 (1.46) 4.97 (1.60) 5.27 (1.51) 5.60 (1.20) 0.16*
Change 4.12 (1.76) 4.05 (1.83) 4.07 (1.72) 4.25 (1.77) 0.06
Positive affect
PANAS positive sum 31.75 (7.77) 29.69 (8.31) 31.90 (7.61) 32.85 (7.40) 0.13*
Negative affect
PANAS negative sum 12.31 (3.97) 13.26 (4.44) 12.19 (4.04) 11.88 (3.42) −0.15*
CESD total 16.43 (6.88) 18.00 (6.90) 16.54 (7.14) 15.24 (6.23) −0.15*
STAI total 33.97 (9.07) 36.60 (9.79) 34.11 (9.14) 32.06 (8.00) −0.18*
Social network

Soc Indic Res. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2015 June 01.
Family embeddedness 6.69 (2.03) 6.94 (2.03) 6.66 (2.06) 6.58 (1.99) −0.09*
Friend embeddedness 7.25 (2.16) 7.64 (2.24) 7.12 (2.22) 7.20 (2.00) −0.09*
Enacted support 25.55 (5.89) 26.90 (5.69) 26.01 (5.52) 23.99 (6.23) −0.19*
Provided support 27.11 (5.99) 28.85 (5.51) 27.56 (5.74) 25.31 (6.20) −0.23*
Perceived support 14.31 (2.16) 14.42 (2.13) 14.39 (2.11) 14.11 (2.25) −0.06
Personality traits
Emotional stability 35.43 (7.32) 33.67 (7.40) 35.38 (7.17) 36.65 (7.27) 0.14*
Extraversion 32.21 (7.49) 32.93 (7.95) 31.89 (7.53) 32.23 (7.10) −0.02
Page 17
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript

Total Young (ages 20–45) Middle-aged (ages 46–65) Older (ages 66–95) Age r
Conscientiousness 38.05 (6.10) 36.35 (6.71) 38.46 (5.73) 38.53 (6.02) 0.11*
Agreeableness 41.35 (5.54) 40.68 (6.34) 41.50 (5.48) 41.35 (5.54) 0.06
Openness 36.49 (6.46) 37.47 (6.48) 36.25 (6.47) 36.20 (6.39) −0.07
Siedlecki et al.

*
p <.01

Soc Indic Res. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2015 June 01.
Page 18
Siedlecki et al. Page 19

Table 2

Standardized regression coefficients (99 % confidence intervals) for each model


NIH-PA Author Manuscript

Model 1 Model 2
N 1,111 1,111
Life satisfaction
Family embeddedness → Life sat 0.03 (−0.04 to 0.10) 0.04 (−0.03 to 0.10)
Friends embeddedness → Life sat 0.04 (−0.03 to 0.11) 0.04 (−0.03 to 0.10)
Enacted support → Life sat 0.13* (0.03 to 0.23) 0.18* (0.09 to 0.26)
Provided support → Life sat −0.02 (−0.11 to 0.06) −0.05 (−0.13 to 0.03)
Perceived support → Life sat 0.18* (0.10 to 0.25) 0.10* (0.03 to 0.18)
Emotional stability → Life sat 0.33* (0.26 to 0.40)
Conscientiousness → Life sat 0.16* (0.09 to 0.23)
Positive affect
Family embeddedness → Pos affect 0.09* (0.02 to 0.16) 0.09* (0.02 to 0.16)
Friends embeddedness → Pos affect 0.07 (0.00 to 0.15) 0.02 (−0.04 to 0.09)
Enacted support → Pos affect −0.05 (−0.14 to 0.04) −0.01 (−0.09 to 0.07)
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

Provided support → Pos affect 0.13* (0.04 to 0.23) 0.07 (−0.01 to 0.16)

Perceived support → Pos affect 0.05 (−0.03 to 0.12) −0.02 (−0.09 to 0.05)
Emotional stability → Pos affect 0.24* (0.17 to 0.31)
Extraversion → Pos affect 0.22* (0.15 to 0.29)
Conscientiousness → Pos affect 0.22* (0.16 to 0.29)
Negative affect
Family embeddedness → Neg affect −0.02 (−0.10 to 0.05) −0.03 (−0.08 to 0.02)
Friends embeddedness → Neg affect −0.02 (−0.09 to 0.05) 0.02 (−0.03 to 0.07)
Enacted support → Neg affect 0.03 (−0.07 to 0.12) −0.06 (−0.13 to 0.01)
Provided support → Neg affect 0.00 (−0.07 to 0.07) 0.06 (0.00 to 0.12)
Perceived support → Neg affect −0.25* (−0.33 to −0.17) −0.11* (−0.17 to −0.06)
Emotional stability → Neg affect −0.66* (−0.74 to −0.57)
Extraversion → Neg affect −0.13* (−0.17 to −0.08)
Conscientiousness → Neg affect −0.20* (−0.25 to −0.15)
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

Model 1 included family embeddedness, friends embeddedness, enacted support, provided support and perceived support as simultaneous
predictors of the three facets of SWB (life satisfaction, positive affect, and negative affect. Model 2 included three additional predictors (emotional
stability, extraversion, and conscientiousness)
*
p <.01

Soc Indic Res. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2015 June 01.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript

Table 3

Invariance analyses across age groups for Model 2

Model X2 df X2/df CFI RMSEA Δχ2 Δ df p < .01 Δ CFI

Configural invariance 742.59 318 2.34 0.955 0.035


Siedlecki et al.

Metric invariance 780.76 334 2.34 0.952 0.035 38.18 16 Yes −0.003
Scalar invariance 867.33 356 2.44 0.946 0.036 86.57 22 Yes −0.006
Structural invariance 931.89 402 2.32 0.944 0.034 64.55 46 No −0.002

In the configural invariance model only the structure was constrained to be the same across the age groups. In the metric invariance model coefficients from the latent constructs to observed variables were
constrained to be equal across the three age groups. In the scalar invariance model the intercepts on the latent indicators were constrained to be equal across the three age groups. In the structural invariance
model the regression coefficients from the predictors to the latent subjective well-being construct were constrained to equal across the three age groups

Soc Indic Res. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2015 June 01.
Page 20

You might also like

pFad - Phonifier reborn

Pfad - The Proxy pFad of © 2024 Garber Painting. All rights reserved.

Note: This service is not intended for secure transactions such as banking, social media, email, or purchasing. Use at your own risk. We assume no liability whatsoever for broken pages.


Alternative Proxies:

Alternative Proxy

pFad Proxy

pFad v3 Proxy

pFad v4 Proxy