Theories in Second Language Acquisition-196-242
Theories in Second Language Acquisition-196-242
com
Input, Interaction, and Output in SLA 181
development of the interaction approach from its inception over the past two and a
half decades. In fact, Jordan (2005) suggested that the Interaction Hypothesis shows
signs of progression toward a theory, using it as an example of how “an originally
well-formulated hypothesis is upgraded in the light of criticism and developments
in the field” (p. 220). At the point when this book was published in its first edition,
various aspects of the Interaction Hypothesis had been tested and links between
interaction and learning clearly demonstrated, thereby suggesting that it was time
for a change in the term hypothesis. Its inclusion in a volume on theories of SLA,
references to it as the “model that dominates current SLA research” (Ramírez,
2005, p. 293) and “the dominant interactionist paradigm” (Byrnes, 2005, p. 296)
supported this view, together with the appearance of book-length critiques of it
(Block, 2003), which collectively showed that researchers were moving toward
thinking about the Interaction Hypothesis in terms of a model of SLA. Using the
framework of this book, for example, it is a model in the sense that it describes
the processes involved when learners encounter input, are involved in interaction,
receive feedback and produce output. However, it is moving toward the status of
theory in the sense that it also attempts to explain why interaction and learning
can be linked, using cognitive concepts derived from psychology, such as noticing,
working memory, and attention. In this chapter, then, as in much of the current
literature, including recent handbook and encyclopedia articles (García-Mayo &
Alclón-Soler, 2013; Gor & Long, 2009; Mackey, 2012; Mackey, Abbuhl, & Gass,
2012; Mackey & Goo, 2012), we refer to it as the interaction approach.
Since the early 1980s and since Long’s update in 1996, the interaction approach
has witnessed a growth in empirical research and is now at a point where meta-
analyses and research syntheses can be carried out (Keck, Iberri-Shea, Tracy-
Ventura, & Wa-Mbaleka, 2006; Li, 2010; Lyster & Saito, 2010; Mackey & Goo,
2007; Norris & Ortega, 2000; Plonsky & Gass, 2011; Russell & Spada, 2006). It
is now commonly accepted within the SLA literature that there is a robust con-
nection between interaction and learning. In the current chapter we provide an
update in which we present a description of the constructs of the interaction
approach as well as a discussion of the theoretical underpinnings that account for
the link between interaction and learning.
The interaction approach attempts to account for learning through the learner’s
exposure to language, production of language and feedback on that production.
As Gass (2003) notes, interaction research “takes as its starting point the assump-
tion that language learning is stimulated by communicative pressure and examines
the relationship between communication and acquisition and the mechanisms
(e.g., noticing, attention) that mediate between them” (p. 224). In the following
sections, we turn to an examination of the major components of this approach.
Input
Input is the sine qua non of acquisition. Quite simply it refers to the language that
a learner is exposed to in a communicative context (i.e., from reading or listening,
free ebooks ==> www.ebook777.com
182 Gass and Mackey
or, in the case of sign language from visual language). In all approaches to second
language acquisition, input is an essential component for learning in that it pro-
vides the crucial evidence from which learners can form linguistic hypotheses.
Because input serves as the basis for hypotheses about the language being
learned, researchers within the interaction approach have sought over the years to
characterize the input that is addressed to learners, and like UG researchers (see
Chapter 3), interaction researchers also see input as providing positive evidence,
that is, information about what is possible within a language. Early interaction
researchers have shown that the language addressed to learners differs in interesting
ways from the language addressed to native speakers and fluent second language
speakers (for overviews, see Gass & Selinker, 2001; Hatch, 1983; Wagner-Gough &
Hatch, 1975). This language that is addressed to learners has been referred to as
modified input or, in the earlier literature, as foreigner talk.
One proposal concerning the function of modified input is that modifying
input makes the language more comprehensible. If learners cannot understand the
language that is being addressed to them, then that language is not useful to them
as they construct their second language grammars. An example of how individu-
als modify their speech and the resultant comprehensibility is given below (from
Kleifgen, 1985). In this example, a teacher of kindergarteners, including native and
nonnative speakers of English at varying levels of proficiency, is providing instruc-
tions to the class and to individuals.
www.ebook777.com
free ebooks ==> www.ebook777.com
Input, Interaction, and Output in SLA 183
which, in turn, ease the burden for the learner. It is important to note that sim-
plifications are not the only form of adjustments, which can also include elabora-
tions, thereby providing the learner with a greater amount of semantic detail. An
example of elaboration is seen in (2) (from Gass & Varonis, 1985). In this example,
when the NNS indicates a possible lack of understanding (Pardon me?), the NS
replies by elaborating on her original comment about nitrites, adding an example
and restating that she doesn’t eat them.
(2) Elaboration
NNS: There has been a lot of talk lately about additives and preservatives in
food. In what ways has this changed your eating habits?
NS: I try to stay away from nitrites.
NNS: Pardon me?
NS: Uh, from nitrites in uh like lunch meats and that sort of thing. I don’t
eat those.
Input, along with negative evidence obtained through interaction (to which
we turn next), is believed to be crucial for acquisition to occur, not only in the
interaction approach but in other approaches as well (e.g., input processing) (see
Chapter 7).
Interaction
Interaction, simply put, refers to the conversations that learners participate in.
Interactions are important because it is in this context that learners receive infor-
mation about the correctness and, more important, about the incorrectness of
their utterances. Within the interaction approach, negative evidence, as in the UG
literature (see Chapter 3), refers to the information that learners receive concern-
ing the incorrectness of their own utterances. For our purposes, learners receive
negative evidence through interactional feedback that occurs following prob-
lematic utterances, and provides learners with information about the linguistic
and communicative success or failure of their production. Gass (1997) presents
the model in Figure 10.1 to characterize the role negative evidence plays in the
interaction-learning process.
Interpreting this, negative evidence, which can come inter alia through overt
correction or negotiation, is one way of alerting a learner to the possibility of an
error in his or her speech. Assuming that the error is noticed, the learner then has
to determine what the problem was and how to modify existing linguistic knowl-
edge. The learner then comes up with a hypothesis as to what the correct form
should be (e.g., he wented home versus he went home). Obtaining further input (e.g.,
listening, reading) is a way of determining that in English one says he went home,
but never says he wented home. Thus, listening for further input is a way to con-
firm or disconfirm a hypothesis that he or she may have come up with regarding
free ebooks ==> www.ebook777.com
184 Gass and Mackey
Negative Evidence
Notice Error
Search Input
(confirmatory/disconfirmatory)
the nature of the target language. The learner may also use output to test these
hypotheses, which we address next.
Output
Known in the literature as the Output Hypothesis (Swain, 1985, 1993, 1995, 1998,
2005), Swain’s observations about the importance of output emerged from her
research that took place in the context of immersion programs in Canada. Swain
observed that children who had spent years in immersion programs still had a level
of competence in the L2 that fell significantly short of native-like abilities. She
hypothesized that what was lacking was sufficient opportunities for language use.
She claims that language production forces learners to move from comprehension
(semantic use of language) to syntactic use of language. As Swain (1995) states,
www.ebook777.com
free ebooks ==> www.ebook777.com
Input, Interaction, and Output in SLA 185
form of a clarification request (‘what?’), the NNS in (3) appears to realize that his
utterance was not understood. Pushed to reformulate his initial utterance in order
to facilitate NS understanding, he modifies his linguistic output by reformulating
the utterance in a more target-like way.
negotiation for meaning, and especially negotiation work that triggers inter-
actional adjustments by the NS or more competent interlocutor, facilitates
acquisition because it connects input, internal learner capacities, particularly
selective attention, and output in productive ways. (pp. 451–452)
Furthermore,
Feedback
There are two broad types of feedback: explicit and implicit. Explicit feedback
includes corrections and metalinguistic explanations. Of concern to us here are
implicit forms of feedback, which include negotiation strategies such as
www.ebook777.com
free ebooks ==> www.ebook777.com
Input, Interaction, and Output in SLA 187
Negotiation for meaning has traditionally been viewed and coded in terms of the
“three Cs”: confirmation checks, clarification requests, and comprehension checks,
each of which we defined earlier. A confirmation check was seen in example (5).
Examples (6) and (7) exemplify clarification requests. In example (6) the clarification
request and rephrasings result in input that the learner finally seems to understand.
(7) Clarification Request (from Gass, Mackey, & Ross-Feldman, 2005, 2011)
LEARNER 1: ¿Qué es importante a ella?
What is important to her?
LEARNER 2: ¿Cómo?
What?
LEARNER 1: ¿Qué es importante a la amiga? ¿Es solamente el costo?
What is important to the friend? Is it just the cost?
www.ebook777.com
free ebooks ==> www.ebook777.com
Input, Interaction, and Output in SLA 189
Mackey, 2006). It also must be kept in mind that even when learners do understand
the corrective nature of recasts, they may have trouble understanding and address-
ing the source of the problem (as discussed by several researchers, including Car-
roll, 2001). However, it is possible that neither a response nor a recognition of the
corrective intent of the recast is crucial for learning (Mackey & Philp, 1998) and
a substantial body of research, using increasingly innovative methods, has linked
recasts with L2 learning of different forms, in different languages, for a range of
learners in both classroom and laboratory contexts (for a review, see Mackey &
Gass, 2006).
Language-Related Episodes
Another construct, language-related episodes (LREs), is also studied within the
context of interaction. Briefly defined, LREs refer to instances where learners
consciously reflect on their own language use, or, more specifically “instances in
which learners may (a) question the meaning of a linguistic item; (b) question the
correctness of the spelling/pronunciation of a word; (c) question the correctness
of a grammatical form; or (d) implicitly or explicitly correct their own or another's
usage of a word, form or structure” (Leeser, 2004, p. 56; see also Swain & Lapkin,
1998; Williams, 1999). LREs, as Williams (1999) notes, encompass a wide range of
discourse moves, such as requests for assistance, negotiation sequences, and explicit
and implicit feedback, and are generally taken as signs that learners have noticed a
gap between their interlanguages (or their partners’ interlanguages) and the system
of the target language. Example (10) illustrates a language-related episode where
students discuss the gender of the word for ‘map’.
Attention
While input such as that provided in recasts may be regarded as a catalyst for
learning, and LREs as evidence that learning processes are being engaged, attention
www.ebook777.com
free ebooks ==> www.ebook777.com
Input, Interaction, and Output in SLA 191
is believed to be one of the mechanisms that mediates between input and learning
(or intake, as the input-learning process is sometimes called). It is widely agreed
that second language learners are exposed to more input than they can process,
and that some mechanism is needed to help learners “sort through” the massive
amounts of input they receive. As Gass, Svetics, and Lemelin (2003) explain, “lan-
guage processing is like other kinds of processing: Humans are constantly exposed
to and often overwhelmed by various sorts of external stimuli and are able to,
through attentional devices, ‘tune in’ some stimuli and ‘tune out’ others” (p. 498).
Attention, broadly conceptualized, may be regarded as the mechanism that allows
learners to “tune in” to a portion of the input they receive.
Although generally held to be crucial for SLA, attention has nevertheless been
the focus of much recent debate in the field. Schmidt (1990, 2001), for example,
argues that learning cannot take place without awareness because the learner must
be consciously aware of linguistic input in order for it to become internalized;
thus, awareness and learning cannot be dissociated. Similarly, Robinson (1995,
2001, 2002) claims that attention to input is a consequence of encoding in work-
ing memory, and only input encoded in working memory may be subsequently
transferred to long-term memory. Thus, in Robinson’s model, as in Schmidt’s,
attention is crucial for learning, and in both models, no learning can take place
without attention and some level of awareness. An alternative and distinct per-
spective, emerging from work in cognitive psychology (Posner, 1988, 1992; Pos-
ner & Peterson, 1990), is presented by Tomlin and Villa (1994), who advocate for
a disassociation between learning and awareness. As can be seen from this brief
overview, not all researchers use the same terminology when discussing attention,
and in fact, there have been proposals that have divided attention into differ-
ent components. What is important for the current chapter is that interaction
researchers assume that the cognitive constructs of attention, awareness, and the
related construct of noticing are part of the interaction-L2 learning process.
Working memory (WM) has also been implicated as a potential explanation
for how interaction-driven L2 learning takes place, as well as language learning in
general. For example, in a study of Japanese L1 English language learners, Mackey,
Philp, Egi, Fujii, and Tasumi’s (2002) research showed that WM was associated
with the noticing of recasts, Trofimovich, Ammar, and Gatbonton (2007) sug-
gested that WM (along with attention control and analytical ability) was associ-
ated with their Francophone learners’ production of English morphosyntax. Such
research suggests that WM may play an important role in the processing and use
of recasts by L2 learners. Another factor that may relate to a learner’s ability to
benefit from interaction is their ability to suppress information, referred to as
inhibitory control. Gass, Behney, and Uzum (2013) found evidence that those
individuals who were better able to suppress interfering information were also
better able to learn from interaction.
There have been in total nearly a hundred empirical studies of the various
different aspects of interaction since the mid-1990s. As outlined in Mackey et al.
free ebooks ==> www.ebook777.com
192 Gass and Mackey
Tasks
Various ways of categorizing task types have been discussed (for discussions of
task categorization, see Ellis, 2003; Mackey & Gass, 2007; Pica, Kanagy, & Falodun,
1993). For example, a common distinction is to classify tasks as one-way and two-
way. In a one-way task, the information flows from one person to the other, as
www.ebook777.com
free ebooks ==> www.ebook777.com
Input, Interaction, and Output in SLA 193
when a learner describes a picture to her partner. In other words, the information
that is being conveyed is held by one person. In a two-way task, there is an infor-
mation exchange whereby both parties (or however many participants there are
in a task) hold information that is vital to the resolution of the task. For example,
in a story completion task, each learner may hold a portion of the information
and must convey it to the other learner(s) before the task can be successfully com-
pleted. Each type of task may produce different kinds of interaction, with different
opportunities for feedback and output.
Interaction researchers are usually interested in eliciting specific grammatical
structures to test whether particular kinds of interactive feedback on non-target-
like forms are associated with learning. Learning is sometimes examined through
immediate changes in the learners’ output on the particular structures about
which they have received interactional feedback, although short- and longer-term
change on posttests is generally considered to be the gold standard.
Obviously, tasks need to be carefully pilot-tested to ensure they produce the
language intended. It is also possible, and becoming more common in interaction
research, to try to examine learners’ thought processes as they carry out a task or
to interview learners on previous thought processes. For example, if a researcher
employed a dictogloss task (a type of consensus task where learners work together
to reconstruct a text that has been read to them; Swain & Lapkin, 2002), that
researcher could examine the text that learners produce (the output). Or, instead
of examining the output in isolation, the researcher could also ask the learners to
think aloud as they carry out the task (this is known as an introspective protocol
or “think aloud”). Alternatively, the researcher could ask the learners to make
retrospective comments as soon as they are finished with a task. This is often done
by providing the learners with a video replay to jog their memories (a procedure
known as stimulated recall) (Gass & Mackey, 2000).
we need to ask what learning has occurred. Is she simply repeating what the NS
had said without true understanding, or did some type of learning take place? Or
was some process engaged that might eventually lead to, or facilitate, later learning.
Example (11), taken from Hawkins (1985), illustrates a similar concern:
The question that must be addressed is what does help and for help mean. Is it
a recognition that implies comprehension? Or, can we assume that this learner
has indicated comprehension and that this is indeed an initial part of the learning
process? In fact, an interview with the participants showed that no comprehension
had taken place and hence no learning. The response was only a means for keep-
ing the social discourse from falling apart.
These examples help foreground the concern that whatever the data source,
the important point is not to rely solely on the transcript of the interaction but to
investigate the link between interaction and learning by whatever means possible.
For this reason, research designs which employ pretests and posttests (and ideally,
delayed posttests and possibly tailor-made posttests as well) and/or designs that
include introspective or retrospective protocols are of value. As research designs
progress, clearer answers to the questions about interaction and learning can be
obtained.
Common Misunderstandings
Here we will consider two common areas of misunderstanding about input, inter-
action, and SLA. These relate to the nature of the interaction approach and the
relationship of the interaction approach to teaching methods.
The first misunderstanding concerns the scope of the interaction approach.
Although occasionally criticized for not addressing all aspects of the learning pro-
cess (such as how input is processed, or the sociocultural context of the learning),
the interaction approach, like all SLA approaches and theories, takes as its primary
focus particular aspects of the second language learning process. Some theories focus
on innateness, others on the sociolinguistic context, and still others purely on the
www.ebook777.com
free ebooks ==> www.ebook777.com
Input, Interaction, and Output in SLA 195
www.ebook777.com
free ebooks ==> www.ebook777.com
Input, Interaction, and Output in SLA 197
perceptions about lexical and phonological feedback, and were generally inaccu-
rate in their perceptions about morphosyntactic feedback. Morphosyntactic feed-
back was often perceived as being about semantics for the ESL learners and about
lexis for the IFL learners. Proponents of the interaction approach have suggested
that interaction can result in feedback that focuses learners’ attention on aspects
of their language that deviate from the target language. If learners’ reports about
their perceptions can be equated with attention, then the findings in this study are
consistent with the claims of the Interaction Hypothesis, at least with regard to the
lexicon and phonology.
www.ebook777.com
free ebooks ==> www.ebook777.com
Input, Interaction, and Output in SLA 199
Conclusion
In this chapter, the perspective offered by input and interaction has been pre-
sented. The central tenet of the approach is that interaction facilitates the pro-
cess of acquiring a second language, as it provides learners with opportunities to
receive modified input, to receive feedback, both explicitly and implicitly, which
in turn may draw learners’ attention to problematic aspects of their interlanguage
and push them to produce modified output.
Discussion Questions
1. The authors describe this approach as a model and not a theory. Do you
agree? Why?
2. Is the Input-Interaction-Output approach compatible with, for example, the
UG approach (Chapter 3) and frequency-based approach (Chapter 5)?
3. Describe the role of negative evidence within Gass and Mackey’s approach.
Does this differ from other approaches you have read about in this volume?
4. The concept of “negotiation of meaning” has gained wide acceptance in
language teaching circles in North America. Why do you think this is so? In
what contexts or situations do you think negotiation of meaning might not
be as enthusiastically embraced?
5. One possible critique of the Input-Interaction-Output approach is that it
ignores the broader social context of language learning variables that may
come to play in peoples’ interactions, for example, power relationships, social
status, or gender. Do you think this is a valid criticism? To what extent would
a theory of SLA need to consider such social factors?
6. Read the exemplary study presented in this chapter and prepare a discussion
for class in which you describe how you would conduct a replication study.
Be sure to explain any changes you would make and what motivates such
changes.
Long, M. H. (1996). The role of the linguistic environment in second language acquisi-
tion. In W. Ritchie & T. K. Bhatia (Eds.), Handbook of language acquisition: Vol. 2. Second
language acquisition (pp. 413–468). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
One of the most often cited articles in the field, Long’s article discusses the theoretical
underpinnings of the interaction approach, including positive evidence, comprehensible
input, input and cognitive processing, and negotiating for meaning.
Mackey, A. (2007). Interaction and second language development: Perspectives from SLA
research. In R. DeKeyser (Ed.), Practice in second language learning: Perspectives from lin-
guistics and psychology. (pp. 85–110). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
In this chapter, research on interaction in second language acquisition pointing to the
importance of a range of interactional processes in the second language learning process
is discussed. These processes include negotiation for meaning, the provision of feedback,
and the production of modified output. Highlighted in this chapter is the importance of
cognitive (learner-internal) factors such as attention, noticing, and memory for language.
Mackey, A. (Ed.). (2007). Conversational interaction in second language acquisition. Oxford,
England: Oxford University Press.
This book provides an edited collection of empirical studies on a variety of issues
concerning the relationship between conversational interaction and second language
learning. In particular, it highlights the benefits of interactional feedback, explores the
relationship between learners’ perceptions and learning, and investigates individual dif-
ferences and social and cognitive factors.
Mackey, A. (2012). Input, interaction, and corrective feedback in L2 learning. Oxford, England:
Oxford University Press.
This book provides a comprehensive and up-to-date survey of 20 years of research on
interaction-driven second language learning, with a particular interest in the recent
growth in research into the role of cognitive and social factors in evaluating how inter-
action works.
Mackey, A., & Abbuhl, R. (2005). Input and interaction. In C. Sanz (Ed.), Internal and exter-
nal factors in adult second language acquisition (pp. 207–233). Washington, DC: Georgetown
University Press.
This chapter provides a detailed overview of the interaction approach, discussing both
empirical work that has investigated the relationship between interaction and L2 devel-
opment and implications for L2 pedagogy.
Mackey, A., Abbuhl, R., & Gass, S. (2012). Interactionist approach. In S. Gass & A. Mackey
(Eds.), The Routledge handbook of second language acquisition (pp. 7–23). New York, NY:
Routledge.
This chapter provides an overview of the historical background of the interactionist
approach and discusses core issues surrounding it. It examines some ways of collect-
ing data, explores practical applications of the approach, and gives directions for future
research.
References
Ayoun, D. (2001). The role of negative and positive feedback in the second language acqui-
sition of the passé composé and imparfait. The Modern Language Journal, 85, 226–243.
Ammar, A. (2008). Prompts and recasts: Differential effects on second language morpho-
syntax. Language Teaching Research, 12, 183–210.
Ammar, A., & Spada, N. (2006). One size fits all? Recasts, prompts and L2 learning. Studies
in Second Language Acquisition, 28, 543–574.
www.ebook777.com
free ebooks ==> www.ebook777.com
Input, Interaction, and Output in SLA 201
Basturkmen, H., Loewen, S., & Ellis, R. (2002). Metalanguage in focus on form in the com-
municative classroom. Language Awareness, 11, 1–13.
Bigelow, M., delMas, R., Hansen, K., & Tarone, E. (2006). Literacy and processing of oral
recasts in SLA. TESOL Quarterly, 40, 665–689.
Block, D. (2003). The social turn in second language acquisition. Edinburgh, Scotland: Edin-
burgh University Press.
Braidi, S. (2002). Reexamining the role of recasts in native-speaker/nonnative-speaker
interactions. Language Learning, 52, 1–42.
Bygate, M., Skehan, P., & Swain, M. (Eds.). (2001). Researching pedagogic tasks: Second language
learning, teaching, and testing. Harlow, England: Pearson.
Byrnes, H. (2005). Review of task-based language learning and teaching. The Modern Lan-
guage Journal, 89, 297–298.
Carpenter, H., Jeon, S., MacGregor, D., & Mackey, A. (2006). Learners’ interpretations of
recasts. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 28, 209–236.
Carroll, S. (1999). Putting “input” in its proper place. Second Language Research, 15, 337–388.
Carroll, S. E. (2001). Input and evidence: The raw material of second language acquisition. Amster-
dam, Netherlands: John Benjamins.
de la Fuente, M. J. (2002). Negotiation and oral acquisition of L2 vocabulary: The roles of
input and output in the receptive and productive acquisition of words. Studies in Second
Language Acquisition, 24, 81–112.
Donato, R. (1994). Collective scaffolding in second language learning. In J. Lantolf & G. Appel
(Eds.), Vygotskian approaches to second language research (pp. 33–56). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
Dornyei, Z. (2009). Individual differences: Interplay of learner characteristics and learning
environment. Language Learning, 59(Suppl. 1), 230–248.
Doughty, C., & Varela, E. (1998). Communicative focus on form. In C. Doughty & J.
Williams (Eds.), Focus on form in classroom second language acquisition (pp. 114–138). Cam-
bridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Egi, T. (2007). Interpreting recasts as linguistic evidence: The roles of linguistic target,
length and degree of change. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 29, 511–537.
Ellis, R. (2003). Task-based language learning and teaching. Oxford, England: Oxford Univer-
sity Press.
Ellis, R. (2007). The differential effects of corrective feedback on two grammatical struc-
tures. In A. Mackey (Ed.), Conversational interaction in second language acquisition (pp.
339–360). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
Ellis, R., Loewen, S., & Erlam, R. (2006). Implicit and explicit corrective feedback and the
acquisition of L2 grammar. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 28, 339–368.
Ellis, R., & Sheen,Y. (2006). Reexamining the role of recasts in second language acquisition.
Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 28, 575–600.
García-Mayo, M., & Alcón-Soler, E. (2013). Negotiated input and output interaction. In J.
Herschensohn & M. Young-Scholten (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of second language
acquisition (pp. 209–229). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Gass, S. M. (1997). Input, interaction and the second language learner. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum.
Gass, S. M. (2003). Input and interaction. In C. Doughty & M. H. Long (Eds.), Handbook
of second language acquisition (pp. 224–255). Oxford, England: Blackwell.
Gass, S. M., & Alvarez-Torres, M. (2005). Attention when? An investigation of the ordering
effect of input and interaction. Studies in Second Language Studies, 27, 1–31.
Gass, S. M., Behney, J. N, & Uzum, B. (2013). Inhibitory control, working memory and
L2 interaction gains. In K. Drozdzial-Szelest & M. Pawlak (Eds.), Psycholinguistic and
free ebooks ==> www.ebook777.com
202 Gass and Mackey
sociolinguistic perspectives on second language learning and teaching (pp. 91–114). New York,
NY: Springer.
Gass, S. M., & Mackey, A. (2000). Stimulated recall methodology in second language research.
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Gass, S. M., Mackey, A., & Ross-Feldman, L. (2005). Task-based interactions in classroom
and laboratory settings. Language Learning, 55, 575–611.
Gass, S. M., Mackey, A, & Ross-Feldman, L. (2011) Task-based interactions in classroom and
laboratory settings. Language Learning, 61, 189–220.
Gass, S. M., & Selinker, L. (2001). Second language acquisition: An introductory course. Mahwah,
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Gass, S. M., Svetics, I., & Lemelin, S. (2003). Differential effects of attention. Language Learn-
ing, 53, 497–545.
Gass, S. M., & Varonis, E. (1985). Variation in native speaker speech modification to non-
native speakers. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 7, 37–57.
Goo, J. (2012). Corrective feedback and working memory capacity in interaction-driven
L2 learning. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 34, 445–474.
Goo, J., & Mackey, A. (2013). The case against the case against recasts. Studies in Second
Language Acquisition, 35, 127–165.
Gor, K., & Long, M. (2009). Input and second language processing. In W. Ritchie & T.
Bhatia (Eds.), The new handbook of second language acquisition (pp. 445–472). New York,
NY: Academic Press.
Han, Z. (2002). A study of the impact of recasts on tense consistency in L2 output. TESOL
Quarterly, 36, 543–572.
Hatch, E. (1983). Psycholinguistics: A second language perspective. Rowley, MA: Newbury.
Hawkins, B. (1985). Is an “appropriate response” always so appropriate? In S. M. Gass & C.
Madden (Eds.), Input in second language acquisition (pp. 162–180). Rowley, MA: Newbury.
Ishida, M. (2004). Effects of recasts on the acquisition of the aspectual form -te i- (ru) by
learners of Japanese as a foreign language. Language Learning, 54, 311–394.
Iwashita, N. (2003). Negative feedback and positive evidence in task-based interaction.
Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 25, 1–36.
Jeon, K. S. (2007). Interaction-driven L2 learning: Characterizing linguistic development.
In A. Mackey (Ed.), Conversational interaction in second language acquisition: A collection of
empirical studies (pp. 379–403). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
Jordan, G. (2005). Theory construction in second language acquisition. Amsterdam, Netherlands:
John Benjamins.
Kim, J., & Han, Z. (2007). Recasts in communicative EFL classes: Do teacher intent and
learner interpretation overlap? In A. Mackey (Ed.), Conversational interaction in second
language acquisition (pp. 269–297). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
Keck, C., Iberri-Shea, G., Tracy-Ventura, N., & Wa-Mbaleka, S. (2006). In J. M. Norris
& L. Ortega (Eds.), Synthesizing research on language learning and teaching (pp. 91–131).
Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins.
Kleifgen, J. (1985). Skilled variation in a kindergarten teacher’s use of foreigner talk. In S.
M. Gass & C. Madden (Eds.), Input in second language acquisition (pp. 59–68). Rowley,
MA: Newbury.
Krashen, S. (1982). Principles and practices in second language acquisition. Oxford, England:
Pergamon.
Krashen, S. (1985). The Input Hypothesis: Issues and complications. London, England: Longman.
Leeman, J. (2003). Recasts and second language development: Beyond negative evidence.
Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 25, 37–63.
www.ebook777.com
free ebooks ==> www.ebook777.com
Input, Interaction, and Output in SLA 203
Leeser, M. J. (2004). Learner proficiency and focus on form during collaborative dialogue.
Language Teaching Research, 8, 55–81.
Li, S. (2010). The effectiveness of corrective feedback in SLA: A meta-analysis. Language
Learning, 60, 309–365.
Loewen, S., & Philp, J. (2006). Recasts in the adult English L2 classroom: Characteristics,
explicitness and effectiveness. The Modern Language Journal, 90, 536–556.
Long, M. H. (1996). The role of the linguistic environment in second language acquisition.
In W. Ritchie & T. Bhatia (Eds.), Handbook of language acquisition: Vol. 2. Second language
acquisition (pp. 413–468). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Long, M. H., & Robinson, P. (1998). Focus on form: Theory, research, and practice. In C.
Doughty & J. Williams (Eds.), Focus on form in classroom second language acquisition (pp.
15–41). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
Lyster, R. (1998a). Recasts, repetition, and ambiguity in L2 classroom discourse. Studies in
Second Language Acquisition, 20, 51–81.
Lyster, R. (1998b). Negotiation of form, recasts, and explicit correction in relation to error
types and learner repair in immersion classrooms. Language Learning, 48, 183–218.
Lyster, R. (2004). Differential effects of prompts and recasts inform-focused instruction.
Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 26, 399–432.
Lyster, R., & Izquierdo, J. (2009). Prompts versus recasts in dyadic interaction. Language
Learning, 59(2), 453–498.
Lyster, R., & Saito, Y. (2010). Oral feedback in classroom SLA: A meta-analysis. Studies in
Second Language Acquisition, 32, 265–302.
Mackey, A. (1999). Input, interaction and second language development: An empiri-
cal study of question formation in ESL. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 21,
557–587.
Mackey, A. (2000). Feedback, noticing and second language development: An empirical study of L2
classroom interaction. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the British Association for
Applied Linguistics, Cambridge, England.
Mackey, A. (2006). Feedback, noticing and instructed second language learning. Applied
Linguistics, 27, 405–430.
Mackey, A. (2012). Input, interaction and corrective feedback in L2 learning. Oxford, England:
Oxford University Press.
Mackey, A., Abbuhl, R., & Gass, S. (2012). Interactionist approach. In S. Gass & A. Mackey
(Eds.), The Routledge handbook of second language acquisition (pp. 7–23). New York, NY:
Routledge.
Mackey, A., & Gass, S. (2005). Second language research: Methodology and design. Mahwah, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum.
Mackey, A., & Gass, S. (2006). Introduction: Methodological innovation in interaction
research. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 28, 169–178.
Mackey, A., & Gass, S. (2007). Data elicitation for second and foreign language research. Mahwah,
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Mackey, A., Gass, S., & McDonough, K. (2000). How do learners perceive interactional
feedback? Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 22, 471–497.
Mackey, A., & Goo, J. (2007). Interaction research in SLA: A meta-analysis and research
synthesis. In A. Mackey (Ed.), Conversational interaction in second language acquisition: A
collection of empirical studies (pp. 407–449). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
Mackey, A., & Goo, J. (2012). Interaction approach in second language acquisition. In
C. Chapelle (Ed.), The Encyclopedia of applied linguistics (pp. 2748–2758). Malden, MA:
Wiley-Blackwell.
free ebooks ==> www.ebook777.com
204 Gass and Mackey
Mackey, A., & Oliver, R. (2002). Interactional feedback and children’s L2 development.
System, 30, 459–477.
Mackey, A., & Philp, J. (1998). Conversational interaction and second language develop-
ment: Recasts, responses, and red herrings? Modern Language Journal, 82, 338–356.
Mackey, A., Philp, J., Egi, T., Fujii, A., & Tatsumi, T. (2002) Individual differences in work-
ing memory, noticing of interactional feedback and L2 development. In P. Robinson &
P. Skehan (Eds.), Individual differences in L2 learning (pp. 181–208). Amsterdam, Nether-
lands: John Benjamins.
Mackey, A., & Sachs, R. (2012). Older learners in SLA research: A first look at working
memory, feedback, and L2 development. Language Learning,62, 704–740.
Mackey, A., & Silver, R. E. (2005). Interactional tasks and English L2 learning by immigrant
children in Singapore. System, 32, 239–260.
McDonough, K. (2005). Identifying the impact of negative feedback and learners’ responses
on ESL question development. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 27, 79–103.
McDonough, K. (2007). Interactional feedback and the emergence of simple past activity verbs
in L2 English. In A. Mackey (Ed.), Conversational interaction in second language acquisition: A
collection of empirical studies (pp. 323–338). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
McDonough, K., & Mackey, A. (2006). Responses to recasts: Repetitions, primed produc-
tion and linguistic development. Language Learning, 56, 693–720.
McLaughlin, B. (1987). Theories of second-language learning. London, England: Arnold.
Morris, F. A. (2002). Negotiation moves and recasts in relation to error types and learner
repair in the foreign language classroom. Foreign Language Annals, 35, 395–404.
Muranoi, H. (2000). Focus on form through interaction enhancement: Integrating for-
mal instruction into a communicative task in EFL classrooms. Language Learning, 50,
617–673.
Nassaji, H. (2009). Effects of recasts and elicitations in dyadic interaction and the role of
feedback. Language Learning, 59, 411–452.
Nicholas, H., Lightbown, P., & Spada, N. (2001). Recasts as feedback to language learners.
Language Learning, 51, 719–758.
Norris, J. M., & Ortega, L. (2000). Effectiveness of L2 instruction: A research synthesis and
quantitative meta-analysis. Language Learning, 50, 417–528.
Oliver, R., & Mackey, A. (2003). Interactional context and feedback in child ESL class-
rooms. The Modern Language Journal, 87, 519–533.
Philp, J. (2003). Constraints on “noticing the gap”: Nonnative speakers’ noticing of recasts
in NS-NNS interaction. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 25, 99–126.
Philp, J., & Mackey, A. (2010). Interaction research: What can socially informed approaches
offer to cognitivists (and vice versa)? In R. Batstone (Ed.), Sociocognitive perspectives on lan-
guage use and language learning (pp. 210–227). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
Pica, T. (1994). Research on negotiation: What does it reveal about second-language learn-
ing conditions, processes, and outcomes? Language Learning, 44, 493–527.
Pica, T. (1996). Second language learning through interaction: Multiple perspectives. Uni-
versity of Pennsylvania Working Papers in Educational Linguistics, 12, 1–22.
Pica, T. (1998). Second language learning through interaction: Multiple perspectives. In
V. Regan (Ed.), Contemporary approaches to second language acquisition (pp. 1–31). Dublin,
Ireland: University of Dublin Press.
Pica, T., Kanagy, R., & Falodun, J. (1993). Choosing and using communication tasks for
second language instruction and research. In G. Crookes & S. M. Gass (Eds.), Tasks and
language learning: Integrating theory and practice (pp. 9–34). Clevedon, England: Multilin-
gual Matters.
www.ebook777.com
free ebooks ==> www.ebook777.com
Input, Interaction, and Output in SLA 205
Plonsky, L., & Gass, S. (2011). Quantitative research methods, study quality, and outcomes:
The case of interaction research. Language Learning, 61, 325–366.
Posner, M. I. (1988). Structures and functions of selective attention. In T. Boll & B. Bryant
(Eds.), Clinical neuropsychology and brain function: Research, measurement, and practice (pp.
173–201). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Posner, M. I. (1992). Attention as a cognitive and neural system. Current Directions in Psy-
chological Science, 1, 11–14.
Posner, M. I., & Peterson, S. (1990). The attention system of the human brain. Annual
Review of Neuroscience, 13, 25–42.
Ramírez, A. G. (2005). Review of the social turn in second language acquisition. The Mod-
ern Language Journal, 89, 292–293.
Révész, A. (2012). Working memory and the observed effectiveness of recasts in different
L2 outcome measures. Language Learning, 62, 93–132.
Révész, A., & Han, Z. (2006). Task content familiarity, task type and efficacy of recasts.
Language Awareness, 15, 160–178.
Robinson, P. (1995). Review article: Attention, memory and the “noticing” hypothesis.
Language Learning, 45, 283–331.
Robinson, P. (Ed.). (2001). Cognition and second language instruction. Cambridge, England:
Cambridge University Press.
Robinson, P. (Ed.). (2002). Individual differences and instructed language learning. Amsterdam,
Netherlands: John Benjamins.
Russell, J., & Spada, N. (2006). The effectiveness of corrective feedback for the acquisition
of L2 grammar: A meta-analysis of the research. In J. M. Norris & L. Ortega (Eds.),
Synthesizing research on language learning and teaching (pp. 133–164). Philadelphia, PA:
John Benjamins.
Sachs, R., & Suh, B.-R. (2007). Textually enhanced recasts, learner awareness, and L2 out-
comes in synchronous computer-mediated interaction. In A. Mackey (Ed.), Conversa-
tional interaction in second language acquisition: A collection of empirical studies (pp. 197–227).
Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
Sagarra, N. (2007). From CALL to face-to-face interaction: The effect of computer-deliv-
ered recasts and working memory on L2 development. In A. Mackey (Ed.), Conversa-
tional interaction in second language acquisition: A collection of empirical studies (pp. 229–248).
Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
Saxton, M. (1997). The contrast theory of negative input. Journal of Child Language, 24,
139–161.
Schmidt, R. (1990). The role of consciousness in second language learning. Applied Lin-
guistics, 11, 129–158.
Schmidt, R. (2001). Attention. In P. Robinson (Ed.), Cognition and second language instruction
(pp. 3–32). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Schmidt, R., & Frota, S. (1986). Developing basic conversational ability in a second lan-
guage: A case study of an adult learner of Portuguese. In R. Day (Ed.), Talking to learn:
Conversation in second language acquisition (pp. 237–326). Rowley, MA: Newbury.
Sheen, Y. (2007). The effect of focus written corrective feedback and language aptitude on
ESL learners’ acquisition of articles. TESOL Quarterly, 41, 255–283.
Sheen, Y. (2008). Recasts, language anxiety, modified output, and L2 learning. Language
Learning, 58, 835–874.
Smith, B. (2012). Eye tracking as a measure of noticing: A study of explicit recasts in
SCMC. Language Learning & Technology, 16, 53–81.
Storch, N. (2002). Patterns of interaction in ESL pair work. Language Learning, 52, 119–158.
free ebooks ==> www.ebook777.com
206 Gass and Mackey
www.ebook777.com
free ebooks ==> www.ebook777.com
11
SOCIOCULTURAL THEORY
AND SECOND LANGUAGE
DEVELOPMENT
James P. Lantolf, Steven L. Thorne, and
Matthew E. Poehner
The intent of this chapter is to familiarize readers with the principles and
constructs of an approach to learning and mental development known as Socio-
cultural Theory (SCT).1 SCT has its origins in the writings of the Russian psy-
chologist L. S. Vygotsky and his colleagues. SCT argues that human mental
functioning is fundamentally a mediated process that is organized by cul-
tural artifacts, activities, and concepts (Ratner, 2002).2 Within this frame-
work, humans are understood to utilize existing, and to create new, cultural
artifacts that allow them to regulate, or more fully monitor and control, their
behavior. Practically speaking, developmental processes take place through par-
ticipation in cultural, linguistic, and historically formed settings such as family
life, peer group interaction, and institutional contexts like schooling, organized
social activities, and workplaces (to name only a few). SCT argues that while
human neurobiology is a necessary condition for higher mental processes,
the most important forms of human cognitive activity develop through inter-
action within social and material environments, including conditions found in
instructional settings (Engeström, 1987). Importantly, and as an outgrowth of
SCT’s roots in Marxism and continental social theory (see discussion imme-
diately following), SCT and its sibling approaches, such as cultural-historical
activity theory, emphasize not only research and understanding of human
developmental processes, but also praxis-based research, which entails interven-
ing and creating conditions for development (see Lantolf & Poehner, 2014).
Indeed, contemporary SCT-informed projects increasingly weight intervention
studies and active engagement, as will be discussed in the exemplary study and
other sections of this chapter.
The intellectual roots of sociocultural theories of human development extend
back to 18th and 19th century German philosophy (particularly Hegel and
free ebooks ==> www.ebook777.com
208 Lantolf, Thorne, and Poehner
Spinoza), the sociological and economic writings of Marx and Engels (specifically
Theses on Feuerbach and The German Ideology), and most directly to the research
of Vygotsky and his colleagues Luria and Leont’ev (see Valsiner & van der Veer,
2000). Despite the fact that Vygotsky suffered an untimely death in 1934 at only
38 years of age, he had a tremendously productive career that was profoundly
influenced by the fact that he came of age during the Russian Revolution. In his
work, Vygotsky attempted to formulate “a psychology grounded in Marxism”
(Wertsch, 1995, p. 7) that emphasized locating individual development within
material, social, and historical conditions. Wertsch (1985, p. 199) has suggested
that Vygotsky’s developmental research was inspired by three essential principles
of Marxist theory: the idea (a) that human consciousness is fundamentally social,
rather than merely biological, in origin, (b) that human activity is mediated by
material artifacts (e.g., computers, the layout of built environments) and psy-
chological and symbolic tools/signs (e.g., language, literacy, numeracy, concepts),
and (c) that units of analysis for understanding human activity and development
should be holistic in nature.
This chapter describes the major theoretical principles and constructs associ-
ated with SCT and focuses specifically on second language acquisition (SLA) as a
psychological process that should be accounted for through the same principles
and concepts that account for all other higher mental processes. Particular atten-
tion is given to development in instructed settings, where activities and environ-
ments may be intentionally organized according to theoretical principles in order
to optimally guide developmental processes. In the first section, we elaborate on
mediation—the central construct of the theory. We then discuss and relate to
SLA other aspects of SCT, namely, private speech, internalization, regulation
(closely connected to mediation and internalization), and the Zone of Proxi-
mal Development. We also consider SCT-informed L2 pedagogy, in particular
the growing bodies of work in dynamic assessment and Systemic-Theoret-
ical Instruction.
Mediation
Vygotsky developed a unified theory of human mental functioning that initiated a
new way of thinking about development. He acknowledged that the human mind
comprised lower mental processes, but the distinctive dimension of human
consciousness was its capacity for voluntary control over biology through the use
of higher level symbolic artifacts (i.e., language, literacy, numeracy, categorization,
rationality, logic, etc.). These artifacts, all of which derive from the historical accu-
mulation of human cultural activity and development (Tomasello, 1999), serve as
a buffer between the person and the environment and act to mediate the relation-
ship between the individual and the social-material world.
www.ebook777.com
free ebooks ==> www.ebook777.com
Sociocultural Theory and SLA 209
www.ebook777.com
free ebooks ==> www.ebook777.com
Sociocultural Theory and SLA 211
Internalization
Vygotsky (1981) stated that the challenge to psychology was to “show how the
individual response emerges from the forms of collective life [and] in contrast to
Piaget, we hypothesize that development does not proceed toward socialization,
but toward the conversion of social relations into mental functions” (p. 165). The
process through which cultural artifacts, including language, take on a psycho-
logical function is known as internalization (Kozulin, 1990). Drawing from
earlier theorists such as Janet (see Valsiner & van der Veer, 2000), Vygotsky (1981)
described the process of internalization as follows:
As this quotation makes clear, higher order cognitive functions, which include
planning, categorization, and interpretive strategies, are initially social and subse-
quently are internalized and made available as cognitive resources. This process of
creative appropriation occurs through exposure to, and use of, semiotic systems
such as languages, textual (and now digital) literacies, numeracy and mathematics,
and other historically accumulated cultural practices. In this sense, internalization
describes the developmental process whereby humans gain the capacity to perform
free ebooks ==> www.ebook777.com
212 Lantolf, Thorne, and Poehner
www.ebook777.com
free ebooks ==> www.ebook777.com
Sociocultural Theory and SLA 213
Development within the ZPD is not just about performance per se; it is also
about where the locus of control for that performance resides—in someone else
or in learners themselves. As learners assume greater responsibility for appropri-
ate performances of the L2, they can be said to have developed, even when they
exhibit little in the way of improvement in their overt performance. This means
that evidence of development can be observed at two distinct levels: overt inde-
pendent performance and at the level where performance is mediated by someone
else. This second type of evidence will go undetected unless we keep in mind
that development in the ZPD is understood as the difference between what an
individual can do independently and what he or she is able to do with media-
tion, including changes in mediation over time. Finally, because SCT construes
language as a cultural tool used to carry out concrete goal directed activities, tasks
such as traditional language tests designed to elicit displays of a learner’s linguis-
tic knowledge offer only limited evidence of development. In sum, evidence of
development in a new language is taken to be changes in control over the new
language as a means of regulating the behavior of the self and of others in carrying
out goal-directed activity.
www.ebook777.com
free ebooks ==> www.ebook777.com
Sociocultural Theory and SLA 215
the fact. In terms of the ZPD, development can be predicted in advance for any
given learner on the basis of his or her responsiveness to mediation. This is what
it means to say that what an individual is capable of with mediation at one point
in time, he or she will be able to do without mediation at a future point in time.
Moreover, as we mentioned in our discussion of the ZPD, development is not
merely a function of shifts in linguistic performance, as in the case of Krashen’s
model, but is also determined by the type of, and changes in, mediation negotiated
between expert and novice. This principle is illustrated in the study described in
the following section.
or a teaching function depending upon the particular context and purpose behind
the activity but both assessment and teaching are necessarily implicated in the
activity. It is this view of DA that has been brought into the L2 field (see Lantolf &
Poehner, 2004) and that we detail in the following case study.
Poehner (2009) reports the implementation of DA principles by Tracy (pseud-
onym), a primary school L2 Spanish classroom teacher. As the sole Spanish teacher
in the school, Tracy was granted considerable latitude to develop her own cur-
riculum at each grade level, and this included the creation of instructional materi-
als, sequencing of lessons, and assessments of learning. She first encountered DA
through a professional development initiative and became interested in explor-
ing how it might help her better understand her learners’ abilities and attune
her instruction accordingly. Although she was interested in pursuing a DA-based
pedagogy, the specific constraints of her instructional context compelled her to
opt for a scripted rather than open-ended approach to mediation. In particular,
Tracy had only 15 minutes per day with each of two classes, each with approxi-
mately 20 students. Tracy carefully prepared an inventory of mediating prompts
that she could administer when learners experienced difficulties with the day’s
learning activities.
Adhering to the general principle that mediation should be sufficiently explicit
to be of value to learners but not so explicit that it deprives learners of the struggle
necessary for development, Tracy prepared six to eight prompts arranged from
most implicit (pause to allow learners to detect and correct errors) to most explicit
(provide the correct response with an explanation). Additional prompts included
repeating a learner’s response with a questioning intonation; repeating the part of a
response that contained an error; employing metalinguistic terms to direct learner
attention to the nature of the error; and offering a choice between two forms.
To maintain a record of learner performance, Tracy created a grid where she
recorded the number of prompts offered to every student during specific interac-
tions over the course of a week as well as a space for notations on the nature of
learner problems. The grid used during instruction allowed Tracy to efficiently
generate a record to which she could refer to track the level of mediation specific
individuals required and how this changed over time as well as how much sup-
port the class in general needed. This approach to DA was highly systematic and
addressed a shortcoming frequently associated with formative assessment prac-
tices. As Poehner and Lantolf (2005) explain, research into formative assessment
reports that insights gained into learner abilities are often unreliable as individual
teachers are not consistent in how they assess learners, offer feedback, and docu-
ment this process and its outcomes.
Poehner (2009) also argues that Tracy’s approach creates the potential to
understand ZPD activity at both the individual and group level. By designing
activities that were sufficiently challenging that none of the students was able to
complete them independently, the instructor was able to establish a context in
which all learners could benefit from the mediation she prepared. Moreover, she
www.ebook777.com
free ebooks ==> www.ebook777.com
Sociocultural Theory and SLA 217
www.ebook777.com
free ebooks ==> www.ebook777.com
Sociocultural Theory and SLA 219
intervention for all learners and did not engage learners in the type of negotiated
mediation demanded by the concept of ZPD. A current project (Zhang, 2014;
also Lantolf & Poehner, 2014) has uncovered evidence that instruction organized
in accordance with principles of SCT and that is sensitive to learners’ ZPD can, in
fact, impact the sequence that classroom learners follow when acquiring features
of a second language.
Observations 5: Second language learning is variable in its outcome; Observation 6:
Second language learning is variable across linguistic subsystems. As we have shown in
our discussions of the ZPD and DA, variability in the development of any given
learner as well as across learners is a characteristic of L2 acquisition. In addition,
the evidence shows that learners variably acquire different subsystems of a new
language depending on the type of mediation they receive and the specific goals
for which they use the language (see Lantolf & Aljaafreh, 1995; for a discussion
of L2 variability that takes into account both SCT and a dynamic systems theory
perspective, see de Bot, Lowie, Thorne, & Verspoor, 2013).
Observation 8: There are limits on the effect of a learner’s first language on SLA.
From an SCT perspective it is important to distinguish form from meaning when
addressing this observation. While L1 formal features may have a limited effect on
L2 learning, it is clear with regard to observations on variability that L1 meanings
continue to have a pervasive effect in L2 learning (see Negueruela, Lantolf, Jor-
dan, & Gelabert, 2004). In addition, as was discussed in regard to L2 private speech,
L2 users have a difficult time using the new language to mediate their cognitive
activity, not withstanding high levels of communicative proficiency.
Observation 10: There are limits on the effects of output (learner production) on lan-
guage acquisition. In this case it is important to distinguish between use of the L1
to mediate the learning of the L2 and the effects of L1 on L2 production. Because
our first language is used not only for communicative interaction but also to
regulate our cognitive processes, it stands to reason that learners must necessarily
rely on this language in order to mediate their learning of the L2. However, there
is also evidence showing that social speech produced in the L1 and the L2 each
impact L2 learning. In a continuing series of studies, Swain and her colleagues
have documented how classroom learners of second languages, including immer-
sion learners, push linguistic development forward by talking, either in the L1 or
L2, about features of the new language (Swain & Lapkin, 2002; Swain, Lapkin,
Knouzi, Suzuki, & Brooks, 2009).
settings and which is not usually available to conscious inspection per se. The
latter system, on the other hand, supports lexical knowledge and other kinds of
explicit information that people generally learn through intentional and conscious
instruction (see Chapter 8).
According to Paradis (2009) as we mature into our teenage years and beyond,
learning through the procedural system declines, while learning through the
declarative system increases. Leaving aside, because of space constraints, many of
the complexities and subtleties of the processes entailed in the respective models
proposed by Paradis and Ullman, we wish to highlight two components that are
directly relevant for the SCT position. The first is that there are no neurological
pathways connecting the procedural and declarative systems, which means that
declarative knowledge cannot convert into procedural knowledge, no matter how
much practice one engages in (NB: neither researcher rules out the possibility
of developing the procedural system in immersion settings but it requires exten-
sive and intensive experiences—experiences that are not likely to occur in the
educational setting). The second is that the declarative system, with appropriate
practice, can be accessed smoothly and rapidly, although perhaps not as rapidly as
the procedural system. What this means is that explicit and systematic instruction
can result in functionally useful knowledge of a second language that learners can
access for spontaneous spoken and written communicative purposes.
SCT does acknowledge a distinction between implicit and explicit knowl-
edge, including knowledge of language. Implicit knowledge, which Vygotsky
(1987) discusses under the rubric of spontaneous concepts, is largely nonconscious
and appropriated from participation in the everyday activities of a community.
Explicit knowledge, which Vygotsky (1987) discusses under the rubric of scientific
concepts, is primarily learned through intentional and systematic instruction gen-
erally associated with formal education. The bulk of SCT research for the past
decade has largely focused on the intentional (or as Vygotsky put it, artificial)
development of second-language ability through systematic explicit instruction.
The framework through which this type of instruction is carried is called Systemic
Theoretical Instruction (STI). Again, concern with development through explicit
instruction does not deny the possibility, as argued by Ullman and Paradis, that
spontaneous development of the procedural system is possible in immersion set-
tings where implicit knowledge may be accessed.
STI, as its name suggests, is concerned with rendering such theoretical knowledge
accessible to learners in a principled, systematic manner. While differences exist
across specific approaches to STI, they share a commitment to identifying the central
concept within an academic discipline and maintaining this as the focus of instruc-
tion. A crucial tenet of STI is that the concept not be “broken-down” or “simpli-
fied” as this risks distorting it. Rather, the concept must be presented in a manner that
is age-appropriate for learners but that maintains its integrity. Additional concepts
are introduced as study continues, and the interrelations among concepts, including
their connection to the central organizing concept, are made explicit.
www.ebook777.com
free ebooks ==> www.ebook777.com
Sociocultural Theory and SLA 221
STI is also marked by the use of schemata, images, and models to introduce
concepts to learners. In a material form, the concepts are more easily referenced
and manipulated by learners than are verbal explanations alone. STI also prompts
learners to verbalize their understanding of a concept and how they use it as
they carry out practical activities guided by the concept. As learners internalize
the meaning of the concept through appropriate forms of practice, they are less
dependent on external symbolic tools to orient their actions.
L2 STI seeks to promote learners’ conceptual understandings of language, and
this means that teaching as well as the knowledge that learners acquire is explicit
in nature. To be sure, this implies that learners who have developed an L2 through
schooling are psychologically functioning with the language in qualitatively dif-
ferent ways from how they function in their L1, which they acquired primarily in
everyday contexts and perhaps even how they acquire L2s in immersion settings
(see Paradis, 2009). More specifically, this means that while L1 knowledge remains
largely implicit for most individuals, this is not the case for knowledge of an L2
developed in school. What matters, however, is not simply that L2 knowledge is
explicit but, following Karpov (2003), that the quality of knowledge must be sys-
tematic and theoretical to provide an appropriate basis for action that generalizes
across various domains. We refer interested readers to Lantolf and Poehner (2008,
2014) for discussion of specific L2 STI studies.
Conclusion
In this chapter we have outlined the primary constructs of SCT, namely, mediation
and regulation, internalization, and the ZPD. Additionally, we have considered
Dynamic Assessment and Systemic-Theoretical Instruction and how they inform
the study of SLA and the structuring of educational interventions. Mediation is
the principal construct that unites all varieties of SCT and is rooted in the obser-
vation that humans do not act directly on the world—rather their cognitive and
material activities are mediated by symbolic artifacts (such as languages, literacy,
numeracy, concepts, and forms of logic and rationality) as well as by material
artifacts and technologies. The claim is that higher order mental functions, includ-
ing voluntary memory, logical thought, learning, and attention, are organized and
amplified through participation in culturally organized activity. This emphasis
within the theory embraces a wide range of research including linguistic relativity,
distributed cognition, and cognitive linguistics. We also addressed the concept of
internalization, the processes through which interpersonal and person–environ-
ment interaction form and transform one’s internal mental functions, and the
role of imitation in learning and development. Finally, we discussed the ZPD, the
difference between the level of development already obtained and the cognitive
functions comprising the proximal next stage of development that may be visible
through participation in collaborative activity. We emphasized that the ZPD is not
only a model of developmental processes, but also a conceptual and pedagogical
free ebooks ==> www.ebook777.com
222 Lantolf, Thorne, and Poehner
tool that educators can use to better understand aspects of students’ emerging
capacities that are in early stages of formation.
Because of its emphasis on praxis, SCT does not rigidly separate understand-
ing (research) from transformation (concrete action). While SCT is used descrip-
tively and analytically as a research framework, it is also an applied methodology
that can be used to improve educational processes and environments (see Lantolf
& Poehner, 2008, 2011, in press; Thorne, 2004, 2005). SCT encourages engaged
critical inquiry wherein investigation into psychological abilities leads to the
development of material and symbolic tools necessary to enact positive inter-
ventions. In other words, the value of the theory resides not just in the analyti-
cal lens it provides for the understanding of psychological development, but in
its capacity to directly impact that development. Though certainly not unique
among theoretical perspectives, SCT approaches take seriously the issue of apply-
ing research to practice by understanding communicative processes as inherently
cognitive processes, and cognitive processes as indivisible from humanistic issues
of self-efficacy, agency, and the effects of participation in culturally organized
activity.
Discussion Questions
1. Both Sociocultural Theory (Lantolf, Thorne, and Poehner) and the frame-
work presented by Gass and Mackey are “interactional” in nature. How are
they different?
2. What is mediation? What is its purpose, and what tools do learners use?
3. The authors of this chapter see Sociocultural Theory primarily as way to
understand classroom language learning. How could Sociocultural Theory
account for language learning outside of the classroom?
4. Consider the Dynamic Assessment principle of providing mediation that is
initially implicit and only becomes more explicit as necessary. How does this
relate to discussions of feedback as Gass and Mackey describe in Chapter 10?
5. Systemic-theoretical instruction moves away from the presentation and prac-
tice of grammar rules and argues instead for the presentation of linguistic
concepts ways to construct meaning. In what ways is this different from the
functionalist approach Bardovi-Harlig presents in Chapter 4?
6. Read the exemplary study presented in this chapter and prepare a discussion for
class in which you describe how you would conduct a replication study. Be sure
to explain any changes you would make and what motivates such changes.
Notes
1. Scholars working in Vygotsky-inspired frameworks also use the term Cultural-Histor-
ical Activity Theory (or CHAT). However, most research conducted on L2 learning
within the Vygotskian tradition has used the term sociocultural, and for this reason, we
www.ebook777.com
free ebooks ==> www.ebook777.com
Sociocultural Theory and SLA 223
use this term throughout the chapter. It is also important to note that the term sociocul-
tural has been used by researchers who do not work directly within the theoretical per-
spective we are addressing in this chapter. These researchers use the term sociocultural
to refer to the general social and cultural circumstances in which individuals conduct
the business of living.
2. In this chapter, we restrict our discussion to symbolic artifacts, in particular, language.
For a more complete discussion of mediation, see Lantolf and Thorne (2006).
3. Elsewhere Vygotsky (1981) remarks that “social relations or relations among people
genetically underlie all higher functions and their relationships” (p. 163).
4. This should not be interpreted to mean that the amount of external mediation a learner
requires will diminish in a predictable and systematic manner, as has been assumed in
some cases (e.g., Erlam, Ellis, & Batstone, 2013). Rather, it is the quality of mediation
rather than a quantifiable amount that will evidence change as learners move toward
greater self-regulation. Moreover, this shift is not predicted to be the same for every
individual, with some making more rapid gains than others and some showing gradual
rather than dramatic improvement.
van Lier, L. (2004). The ecology and semiotics of language learning: A sociocultural perspective.
Boston, MA: Kluwer Academic.
Van Lier insightfully combines Vygotskian theory with detailed discussions of semiotics
and ecological approaches to language and L2 development.
References
Aljaafreh, A., & Lantolf, J. P. (1994) Negative feedback as regulation and second language
learning in the Zone of Proximal Development. The Modern Language Journal, 78,
465–483.
Arievitch, I., & van der Veer, R. (2004). The role of nonautomatic process in activity regula-
tion: From Lipps to Galperin. History of Psychology, 7, 154–182.
Chaiklin, S. (2003). The Zone of Proximal Development in Vygotsky’s analysis of learning
and instruction. In A. Kozulin, B. Gindis, V. Ageyev, & S. Miller (Eds.), Vygotsky’s educa-
tional theory in cultural context (pp. 39–64). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University
Press.
de Bot, K., Lowie, W., Thorne, S. L., & Verspoor, M. (2013). Dynamic systems theory as a
theory of second language development. In M. García-Mayo, M. Gutierrez-Mangado,
& M. Martínez Adrián (Eds.), Contemporary approaches to second Language acquisition (pp.
199–220). Amsterdam, Netherlands: John Benjamins.
Diaz, R., & Berk, L. (Eds.). (1992). Private speech: From social interaction to self regulation. Hill-
sdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
DiCamilla, F. J., & Anton, M. (2004). Private speech: A study of language for thought in
collaborative interaction. International Journal of Applied Linguistics, 14, 36–69.
Donato, R. (1994). Collective scaffolding in second language learning. In J. P. Lantolf &
G. Appel (Eds.), Vygotskian approaches to second language research (pp. 33–56). Norwood,
NJ: Ablex.
Duff, P. (2007). Second language socialization as sociocultural theory: Insights and issues.
Language Teaching, 40, 309–319.
Dunn, W., & Lantolf, J. (1998). Vygotsky’s Zone of Proximal Development and Krashen’s i + 1:
Incommensurable constructs; incommensurable theories. Language Learning, 48, 411–442.
Engeström, Y. (1987). Learning by expanding: An activity theoretical approach to developmental
research. Helsinki, Finland: Orienta-Konsultit.
Erlam, R., Ellis, R., & Batstone, R. (2013). Oral corrective feedback on L2 writing: Two
approaches compared. System, 41, 257–268.
Feuerstein, R., Feuerstein, R. S., & Falik, L. H. (2010). Beyond smarter: Mediated learning and
the brain’s capacity for change. New York, NY: Teachers College Press.
Frawley, W. (1997). Vygotsky and cognitive science: Language and the unification of the social and
computational mind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Frawley, W., & Lantolf, J. (1985). Second language discourse: A Vygotskyan perspective.
Applied Linguistics, 6, 19–44.
Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.
Gillette, B. (1994). The role of learner goals in L2 success. In J. Lantolf & G. Appel (Eds.),
Vygotskian approaches to second language research (pp. 195–214). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
Haywood, H. C., & Lidz, C. S. (2007). Dynamic assessment in practice: Clinical and educational
applications. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
Karpov, Y. V. (2003). Vygotsky’s doctrine of scientific concepts: Its role for contemporary
education. In A. Kozulin, B. Gindis, V. S. Ageyev, & S. M. Miller (Eds.), Vygotsky’s
www.ebook777.com
free ebooks ==> www.ebook777.com
Sociocultural Theory and SLA 225
educational theory in cultural context (pp. 65–82). Cambridge, England: Cambridge Uni-
versity Press.
Kozulin, A. (1990). Vygotsky’s psychology: A biography of ideas. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
University Press.
Krashen, S. (1982). Principles and practices in second language acquisition. Oxford, England:
Pergamon.
Lantolf, J. P., & Aljaafreh, A. (1995). Second language learning in the Zone of Proximal
Development: A revolutionary experience. International Journal of Educational Research,
23, 619–632.
Lantolf, J. P., & Poehner, M. E. (2004). Dynamic assessment: Bringing the past into the
future. Journal of Applied Linguistics, 1, 49–74.
Lantolf, J. P., & Poehner, M. E. (Eds.). (2008). Sociocultural theory and the teaching of second
languages. London, England: Equinox Press.
Lantolf, J. P., & Poehner, M. E. (2011). Dynamic assessment in the classroom: Vygotskian
praxis for L2 development. Language Teaching Research, 15, 11–33.
Lantolf, J. P., & Poehner, M. E. (2014). Sociocultural theory and the pedagogical imperative in L2
education:Vygotskian praxis and the research/practice divide. London, England: Routledge.
Lantolf, J., & Thorne, S. L. (2006). Sociocultural theory and the genesis of second language devel-
opment. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
McCafferty, S. G. (1992). The use of private speech by adult second language learners: A
cross-cultural study. The Modern Language Journal, 76, 179–189.
Negueruela, E., Lantolf, J. P., Jordan, S., & Gelabert, J. (2004). The “private function” of
gesture in second language speaking activity: A study of motion verbs and gesturing in
English and Spanish. International Journal of Applied Linguistics, 14, 113–147.
Ohta, A. (2001). Second language acquisition processes in the classroom: Learning Japanese. Mah-
wah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Paradis, M. (2009). Declarative and procedural determinants of second languages. Amsterdam,
Netherlands: John Benjamins.
Poehner, M. E. (2009). Group dynamic assessment: Mediation for the L2 classroom.
TESOL Quarterly, 43, 471–491.
Poehner, M. E., & Lantolf, J. P. (2005). Dynamic assessment in the language classroom.
Language Teaching Research, 9, 1–33.
Prawat, R. S. (1999). Social constructivism and the process-content distinction as viewed by
Vygotsky and the pragmatists. Mind, Culture, and Activity, 6, 255–273.
Ratner, C. (2002). Cultural psychology: Theory and method. New York, NY: Kluwer/Plenum.
Smith, H. (2007). The social and the private worlds of speech: Speech for inter- and intra-
mental activity. The Modern Language Journal, 91, 341–356.
Steinbach-Koehler, F., & Thorne, S. L. (2011). The social life of self-directed talk: A sequen-
tial phenomenon? In J. Hall, J. Hellermann, S. Pekarek Doehler, & D. Olsher (Eds.), L2
interactional competence and development (pp. 66–92). Bristol, England: Multilingual Matters.
Stetsenko, A. (1999). Social interaction, cultural tools and the Zone of Proximal Develop-
ment: In search of a synthesis. In S. Chaiklin, M. Hedegaard, & U. J. Jensen (Eds.),
Activity theory and social practice: Cultural historical approaches (pp. 235–253). Aarhus, Den-
mark: Aarhus University Press.
Swain, M. (2000). The output hypothesis and beyond: Mediating acquisition through col-
laborative dialogue. In J. Lantolf (Ed.), Sociocultural approaches to second language research
(pp. 97–115). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
Swain, M., & Lapkin, S. (2002). Talking it through: Two French immersion learners’
response to reformulation. International Journal of Educational Research, 37, 285–304.
free ebooks ==> www.ebook777.com
226 Lantolf, Thorne, and Poehner
Swain, M., Lapkin, S., Knouzi, I., Suzuki, W., & Brooks, L. (2009). Languaging: University
students learn the grammatical concept of voice in French. Modern Language Journal,
93, 5–29.
Thorne, S. L. (2000). Second language acquisition theory and the truth(s) about relativ-
ity. In J. Lantolf (Ed.), Sociocultural approaches to second language research (pp. 219–243).
Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
Thorne, S. L. (2003). Artifacts and cultures-of-use in intercultural communication. Lan-
guage Learning and Technology, 7, 38–67.
Thorne, S. L. (2004). Cultural historical activity theory and the object of innovation. In O.
S. John, K. van Esch, & E. Schalkwijk (Eds.), New insights into foreign language learning
and teaching (pp. 51–70). Frankfurt, Germany: Peter Lang.
Thorne, S. L. (2005). Epistemology, politics, and ethics in sociocultural theory. The Modern
Language Journal, 89, 393–409.
Thorne, S. L. (2009). “Community,” Semiotic Flows, and Mediated Contribution to Activ-
ity. Language Teaching, 42, 81–94.
Thorne, S. L., & Tasker, T. (2011). Sociocultural and cultural-historical theories of language
development. In J. Simpson (Ed.), Routledge handbook of applied linguistics (pp. 487–500).
New York, NY: Routledge.
Tomasello, M. (1999). The cultural origins of human cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Uni-
versity Press.
Valsiner, J., & van der Veer, R. (2000). The social mind. Construction of the idea. Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press.
Vygotsky, L. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes (M. Cole,
V. John-Steiner, S. Scribner, & E. Souberman, Eds.). Cambridge, MA: Harvard Uni-
versity Press.
Vygotsky, L. (1981). The genesis of higher mental functions. In J. Wertsch (Ed.), The concept
of activity in Soviet psychology (pp. 144–188). Armonk, NY: M. E. Sharpe.
Vygotsky, L. (1987). The collected works of L. S. Vygotsky: Vol. 1. Problems of general psychology
(R. Reiber & A. Carton, Eds.). New York, NY: Plenum Press.
Vygotsky, L. S. (1994). The problem of the environment. In J. van der Veer & J. Valsiner
(Eds.), The Vygotsky reader (pp. 338–354). Oxford, England: Blackwell.
Vygotsky, L. S. (1997). The collected works of L. S. Vygotsky: Vol. 3. Problems of the theory and
history of psychology (R. W. Rieber & J. Wollock, Eds.). New York, NY: Plenum.
Wertsch, J. (1985). Vygotsky and the social formation of mind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
University Press.
Wertsch, J. (1995). The need for action in sociocultural research. In J. Wertsch, P. Del Rio,
& A. Alvarez (Eds.), Sociocultural studies of mind (pp. 56–74). New York, NY: Cambridge
University Press.
Wood, D., Bruner, J., & Ross, G. (1976). The role of tutoring in problem-solving. Journal
of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 17, 89–100.
Zhang, X. (2014). The teachability hypothesis and concept-based instruction: Topicalization in
Chinese as a second language (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). The Pennsylvania State
University, University Park, PA.
www.ebook777.com
free ebooks ==> www.ebook777.com
12
COMPLEXITY THEORY
Diane Larsen-Freeman