Science Projects For Primary School
Science Projects For Primary School
and
1900 Frost Road, Suite 101
Bristol, PA 19007, USA
All rights reserved. Except for the quotation of short passages for the
purpose of criticism and review, no part of this publication may be
reproduced, stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted, in any form
or by any means, electronic, mechanical, photocopying, recording or
otherwise, without the prior written permission of the publisher or a
licence from the Copyright Licensing Agency Limited. Details of such
licences (for reprographic reproduction) may be obtained from the
Copyright Licensing Agency Ltd of 90 Tottenham Court Road,
London, W1P 9HE.
List of figures vi
Series editor's preface ix
Acknowledgements xii
Parti I
I Introduction — creativity in teaching 3
2 Science education — aims and research 11
Part 2 Case studies 33
3 Parks, woods and wastelands 35
4 Rabbit's House 65
5 Ourselves 81
6 The Channel tunnel 105
7 The river Thames 123
8 Seeing and light 146
Part 3 169
9 From action to reflection 171
Bibliography 183
Index 187
List of figures
Introduction — creativity
in teaching
the class to learn. The high priority she places on children's ability
to communicate is apparent. The complexity and diversity of ideas
and resources which are brought together are evidence of her abil-
ity to create an imaginative experience from many different parts.
The second case study, in Chapter 4, is one which people who
have been on courses at the Institute of Education are likely to
have heard, because I have used it on several occasions. 'Rabbit's
House' was an investigation undertaken by middle infants (Years
I and, 2, 5 to 7-year-olds) with Esmé Glauert; and it had taken
something like two weeks for the whole class to finish. This was
the first example where I asked a teacher to 'unpack' the planning
and the organization of a teaching/learning sequence, and found
that it was a far from straightforward task.
The management of time is something that features promin-
ently in the analysis of Rabbit's House. Time is a resource that has
to be divided up as part of the planning process and is one of the
aspects of teaching which beginning teachers find very difficult.
How do teachers plan for one afternoon, for a series of days, for
this week and next? How do they help children hold their learn-
ing over long timespans?
One of the most creative aspects of this example is the ability
to see the potential for a serious scientific investigation in a sim-
ple story and how to build in small but significant 'loose ends'
such that children have to sort out how to do something them-
selves. As in many of the case studies, the teacher focuses on
developing the children's ability to handle, record and interpret
data; the account includes her strategies for enabling these young
children to understand the significance of features of tables for
recording data.
Chapter 5 contains a case study on the topic of Ourselves taught
by Siobhan Quinlan, in a school in west London, just off Ladbroke
Grove. It was selected initially because the classroom was like an
Aladdin's cave, overflowing with interesting displays of the chil-
dren's work and resource material. The class was a Year 2 class
(6 to 7-year-olds) learning about skeletons and other parts of
the inside of the human body. One of the key elements here was
the sudden realization on the part of the children that they could
do simple science investigations on themselves. Like the teacher
in Rabbit's House, one of this teacher's objectives was that the
children should develop their skill at recording and interpreting
simple data. The personal ID cards, containing data which the
Introduction — creativity in teaching 7
The teacher in the final case study was Nicola Metcalf, teach-
ing in a school in the London Borough of Harrow. The class is a
Year 1 class (5 to 6-year-olds). She follows in detail four pairs of
children as they grapple with designing investigations to find
out what things people can do with and without their eyes open.
She shows how it is necessary for a teacher to have sufficient
insight into the thinking of these relatively young children to be
able to identify what for them will be a significant investigation
and discovery. Her documentation of snippets of conversation
has been an invaluable resource. This particular case study was
prepared initially from the teacher's own report of her research
in her classroom. I have quoted extensively from it; so in this chap-
ter more than the others the direct voice of a teacher is heard.
I believe the case studies provide good examples of the way
in which teachers reorganize their knowledge of a subject in the
process of teaching. The activities they create, the questions they
ask, the analogies they use, the interesting stories that they tell,
are all part of this reorganization. They form what Shulman (1986)
called 'pedagogic content knowledge', learnt from experience, from
other teachers, often fashioned in the process of teaching in re-
sponse to a particular child. There is no simple way to learn them
but the preconditions are teachers' own interest in, and enthusiasm
and knowledge of the subject, coupled with a clear focus on the
children's learning of science and a willingness to try anything to
help them understand and be skilful.
The final part of the book, Chapter 9, returns to the ideas of
Chapter 2 to review the six case studies and to see how they, with
all the details of busy classrooms, give exemplars to illustrate the
general ideas about 'knowing some science' and the processes of
'coming to know some science'.
While I was preparing this book, I was asked how I envisaged it
being used and how it would be useful to both beginning teachers
and practising teachers. I will venture to offer a few suggestions
only.
An important function is the sharing of experience that books of
this sort make possible. Teaching, like so many professions, can be
isolating. After initial teacher education courses, many teachers do
not have the opportunity to watch other teachers teach, although
the process of appraisal is making some inroads into this iso-
lation. To share practice, however, is not to advocate that it
should be copied unquestioningly. The case studies do nevertheless
Introduction — creativity in teaching 9
I cannot be sure of the extent to which these case studies and the
messages within them will translate to contexts in other countries.
I believe that the more general principles concerned with learn-
ing and the ideas for simple explorations and investigations will
be applicable in other places. The size of classes in the schools in
this book rarely exceeds 30 children, so some of the organization
10 Creativity in primary science
may not transfer to schools where teachers are coping with much
larger numbers.
An explanation also needs to be given about the apparent auto-
nomy of teachers to select what topic they want. The National
Curriculum provides outline programmes of study but allows
considerable scope for the order in which topics are taught. Many
schools have developed carefully planned sequences of topics to
be taught throughout the school, so that the children progress-
ively cover the requirements of the National Curriculum (DFE
1995). I have referred in several chapters to the teachers having
to teach a certain topic because it was on the school's 'planning
grid'. These whole-school plans generally specify the concepts in
science to be taught, but not the contexts in which they are taught;
consequently there is flexibility over what the teacher chooses.
Teachers and schools also vary in the extent to which a topic
carries the whole of the curriculum. In Chapters 3, 5 and 6, the
teachers were teaching an 'integrated topic' which covered the
whole of the curriculum. In the other case studies the work had
a more restricted focus.
As a whole, the book should provide windows on to teaching and
learning in science whatever the organization within the school.
2
Science education — aims
and research
things there are to know, but will know quite a few and will be
aware that there are many more that could be looked up in books,
or learnt from experience or from other people.
Understanding explanation
They will also know that there are explanations of how these
things happen: gravitational attraction between the ball and the
earth; increased movement of molecules in heated air; alignment
of the crystals in iron; irreversible chemical changes in the egg;
ability of the body of a living thing to convert the food it eats into
things it needs, either to fuel its body processes or to continue to
build its own body; that rain and wind can slowly erode hard
materials and that what was once rock may become part of the
soil. They will know some of these explanations and know how
they fit the phenomena. There will be many explanations that they
Science education — aims and research 13
do not know but they will be aware that these also could possibly
be looked up in books.
Asking questions
People are more inquisitive about the things they know about
than about the things they do not. So people who know some
science are also likely to ask questions about how things happen;
what causes them to happen; what the processes involved are;
how people have come to give such explanations; what evidence
supports the explanations.
Understanding experiments
Experiments are taken as one of the hallmarks of science. They
involve certain procedures, but in each case, experimenters have
to decide how to proceed in order to extract the evidence that is
needed to answer a question. People who know some science have
some idea about how to design an experiment, how to collect data
and possible ways of making sense of that data.
Interpreting data
People who know some science are likely not to be daunted by
looking at sets of numerical data relating to the natural and mater-
ial world; information on food packets; weather data; temperature
and blood pressure charts at the end of a patient's bed in hospital.
This does not mean that they can interpret any set of scien-
tific data. Understanding how data was collected and understand-
ing the theory behind an experiment is essential for being able to
interpret that data. Scientists involved in looking at the evidence
from the CERN generator and interpreting it as 'nine atoms of
14 Creativity in primary science
about fertility treatment; who should decide when and where cars
may be driven?.
Informal learning
Much informal learning of science develops from just being in a
society where scientific knowledge exists, where technical equip-
ment and processes are available. Considerable knowledge is built
up outside formal education: through hobbies, through medical
encounters, through reading instructions on technical goods; by
knowing that certain things are possible (travelling round the
earth in a satellite, using mobile phones to all corners of the earth,
knowing that hips can be replaced, having water and electricity.
automatically in homes); by watching television programmes; by
reading books and magazines; by interacting with the material
world through construction activities; from science fiction.
By the time that children come to school they already have five
years of experience of finding out about the material and natural
world; they have encountered a lot of technical gadgets, watched
television, looked in books, asked questions and had a range of
responses, and come up with their own conjectures about some
Science education — aims and research 17
Rather than treat each of the ten facets of 'knowing science' sep-
arately I have grouped them into three main clusters: the first is
concerned with knowing and understanding facts, explanations
and concepts, and asking questions; the second with understand-
ing experiments, interpreting data and developing technical know-
how; the third with science and everyday life, the nature of science
and the cultural significance of science. To date, research has only
addressed the first two clusters to any significant extent.
a line going from the centre, outwards along a radius and sky is
all around them.
Amongst the tools for diagnosing where children are on this
journey, Nussbaum asked them to draw pictures of, or talk about,
the 'round earth'. He found that responses included reference to
curved roads, round mountains, a round island with sea all round
it, and a round earth in the sky with the observer standing on
a flat earth. He also set them problems where they had to draw
what will happen to a ball dropped at different places on the
earth's surface. Many responded by drawing the ball going down
the paper, not going towards the centre of the earth. Where
correct responses were given, he probed understanding further
(see Figure 2.1), to reveal that the egocentric view of the world
often still persisted.
Nussbaum identified steps in a progression from the egocen-
tric (the perceived world) to geocentric view of the world (the con-
ceived world), but he made no claim that everyone will follow
that route, nor how long it will take to make the journey. He does
claim that a teacher can make a difference in whether the journey
is embarked on and whether some of the more challenging parts
are tackled.
What also emerges from this whole area of research is that the
ideas are not confined to children. Ideas found in children can be
similar to those found in adults, including people who have con-
siderable formal science education such as science graduates.
A few more examples from other topics will illustrate the point.
Studies of children's responses to whether a cow, whale, spider,
worm or person is an animal or not, revealed that it is not until
the age of 17 years that nearly all children are prepared to recog-
nize that it is reasonable to classify a person as an animal (Osborne
and Freyberg 1985). This is not surprising because there are fre-
quently notices up which say 'No animals allowed in here', where
people are not excluded.
Questions about where water goes to when puddles disappear,
or blackboards reveal that the idea that water, once it has
evaporated, is somehow still around although invisible, is not
easily accepted. Questions about light reveal that many people
believe it travels further at night than during the day. This again
is not unreasonable because we might see a light such as a fire-
work from a distance of several hundred metres at night but not
during the day (Osborne and Freyberg 1985: 10).
Science education — aims and research 21
(d) (e)
Details of the torch and mirror activity are given in Figure 2.2. It
starts with instructions for the activity, which is in fact a problem,
followed by a request for a drawing to show how the problem
was solved. Next comes a request for a drawing to explain how
the light travels (note that 'travelling' is not something which can
be observed but something which has to be imagined). Question
4 asks specifically about the direction of travel and question 5 is
pressing for a further explanation of the whole process (i.e. how
Activity
1 One child holds the torch which is switched on behind the second
child's head. The second child is seated and given a plane mirror. He!
she is asked to use the mirror to see the light from the torch.
2 Do a drawing to show how you used the mirror to see the light from
the torch behind you.
3 Show on the drawing how you think the light travels.
4 Is any light coming towards you?
5 How would you explain what is happening?
Figure 2.2 Torch and mirror activity and the related elicitation
questions (Osborne et al. 1990, Appendices)
24 Creativity in primary science
much of the story that the light is leaving the torch, travelling from
the torch to the mirror, bouncing off the mirror and travelling
into the child's eye, thus enabling him/her to see the torch, can
the child give?).
The progression which is evident in this series of instruc-
tions and questions is typical of many SPACE interviews. They
target specifically scientific ideas (in this case, light as an entity
separate from the source; light travels from source outwards; light
bounces off a mirror; seeing is associated with light entering the
eye), but they also provide the children with activities at the start
with which they can easily engage. The findings from the pre-test
were then reviewed in order to try to devise 'intervention strat-
egies', i.e. what and how to teach next, in order to develop the
children's thinking further. In many cases the intervention strat-
egy was an investigation based on questions children raised in the
exploratory phase.
In this topic they explored children's understanding of sources
of light and particularly whether they could explain secondary
sources; they studied the way children represented light and also
their explanations of vision. Only a minority could give a scientific
explanation of secondary sources, although there was an improve-
ment towards Year 6. Most children represented light by short rays
only, and even after the teaching did not extend the rays any dis-
tance from the source. In the matter of vision it was rare for chil-
dren to indicate the light coming from the source to the object
being looked at and then being reflected into the eye. Teaching
did make some difference as to whether children indicated links
of some sort between the eye and the object and the light source
and the object.
Topics in the SPACE project now cover light, earth in space,
earth, soils and rocks, electricity, growth, materials, sound, pro-
cesses of life, genetics and evolution, evaporation and condensa-
tion. These have provided a picture of the sort of understandings
achieved. I have included examples of findings from two of the
other topics, growth and evaporation and condensation.
In the research on growth, the project found that very young
children could name sun, water and soil as necessary for growth
but rarely named all three and viewed the soil as having a sup-
port function only. When a seed grew into a plant, they saw the
plant unfolding from the seed, not of it building itself up from
material it had taken in. Water was associated with growth and
Science education — aims and research 25
does that happen? Why does that happen?) and also on ways of
26 Creativity in primary science
Harlen 1990: 84, 85), because the clusters show that there is no
need to be observing all the processes at the same time. ('Start up
cluster' comprises observing, hypothesizing, planning, raising
questions, measuring; the 'planning and doing cluster' involves
planning, doing, critical reflection, measuring, manipulating vari-
ables, observing and recording; the 'interpreting cluster' involves
recording, interpreting, communicating — critical reflection, hypo-
thesizing can lead to new investigations. See Figure 2.3). In addi-
tion one of the spin-offs of the research for other teachers is
the collection of examples of what a hypothesis, prediction, etc.
looks like from a 9-year-old's point of view or, indeed, from any
>PLANNING
I
MEASURING RAISING QUESTIONS
PLANNING
I
CRITICAL
'J/ REFLECTION
DOING
RECORDING
(new investigation)
4'
HYPOTHESIZING
CRITICAL
I REFLECTION
COMMUNICATING
The link between research and teaching is elusive and yet all the
teachers involved in these and other research projects found that
their practice in the classroom was significantly altered as a result.
Below is an extract from the SPACE project:
Teachers had commented in an earlier meeting that the role
expected of them was one that they were not used to. The
work required that teachers should be less judgmental than
normal and gave children an opportunity to discuss and
express their ideas without criticism from the teacher. In
addition, the emphasis placed by the project on conceptual
development was one that was unfamiliar to the teachers.
Osborne et a!. 1990: 31
The involvement in research of the STAR project was reported by
all the teachers to be of value in understanding what they were
aiming for in their classes and being able to identify significant
events and products. The detailed analysis of the process skills,
Science education — aims and research 31
Summary
a conversation with her two years later when I started writing this
chapter provided further pieces. Third, I learnt at that time that her
current Years 3 and 4 class were studying the same topic and that
she was using many of the ideas she had developed previously.
I therefore visited her class and stayed for over an hour photo-
graphing events, displays and the wildlife area in the grounds.
Fourth, having omitted to photograph 'Professor Sense', a key player
in the drama, I returned to record him and inevitably to find
more pieces. Fifth, the first draft of the chapter was given to the
teacher who corrected it and added more linking pieces.
Metaphors have their limits. It is impossible to collect, let alone
present in the space of a small book, all the linking pieces in the
jigsaw of teaching. In this class were 28 children aged 8 or 9
years old, each with their own histories and personalities. Con-
versations and relationships between them, and between them
and their teacher, are part of the jigsaw which, save for a few
instances, have not been and cannot be captured. There is the
personal professional history of this particular teacher, the ethos
and the history of the school, the families from which the chil-
dren come; all are pieces which are relevant but in the main unre-
ported. The reader must imagine or at least surmise about them.
During the initial brainstorming the teacher had asked the whole
class to talk about what they thought would be in the topic, and
which parks, woods and wasteland they had seen or visited and
what they had noticed there. Not surprisingly she found that they
knew the wasteland to be a place where people dumped rubbish
and where children were told not to play. The children often vis-
ited the local park to play on the swings and climbing frames, but
had noticed little else there. None of the class had visited a wood
or a forest and some muddled 'wood' with 'would'.
She asked the class to think of questions they might try and
answer when they visited these places. There were not many;
some about what sort of rubbish people dumped on the waste-
land and a few about the playing equipment such as: 'How high
is the climbing frame?' 'How many steps are there on the slide?'
Most of the latter led to mathematics investigations rather than
science.
The first 'sensory visit' to the park was planned and imminent,
with the requisite number of other adults booked to go with the
class. Productive visits, however, need more than administrative
organization: they need the children to be mentally prepared for
the exploration ahead. This triggered the telling of a story and
this is how it went:
Professor Sense
One Monday morning, the headteacher told all the children
during assembly that they would be having a special visitor
coming to their classroom that day.
Later that day the door of our classroom opened and there
Parks, woods and wastelands 39
stood the most unusual man that the children had ever seen.
He smiled, walked in, sat down and invited all the children
and the teacher to come and sit beside him. At first, the chil-
dren stared at him. What they noticed most of all were his
eyes which were very large and round. They seemed to be
moving round and round slowly all the time. It was almost
as if they were on stalks. Next, they noticed his ears. They
seemed to move towards the slightest sounds around the
room. The children felt vibrations which seemed to come
from these. As he spoke to the children, his nose twitched
like a rabbit's. Sometimes he crumpled it up as if he didn't
like what he could smell. Every now and then, his tongue
appeared from between his lips and he seemed to be tast-
ing the very air that they were all breathing. The children
noticed that his hands were big and firm but they looked
very gentle.
The children were fascinated by this unusual person. As
they listened and watched they felt their eyes tingling, their
ears vibrating, their noses twitching and their tongues begin-
ning to water. They looked at their hands to see if anything
was happening to those and they noticed the lines that they
had never seen before.
'Who are you?' asked one little boy. 'I am Professor Sense,'
said the unusual visitor, 'would you like to learn with me?'
The story, like many classroom stories which teachers use for
explanation, would not have been written down if it were not
for the fact that she was researching her own teaching at the
time. It was a story which fascinated and intrigued; the children
responded enthusiastically to the request that they should draw
Professor Sense (Figure 3.1).
Sitting on the grass in the park the teacher reminded the chil-
dren of Professor Sense and asked them to use all their senses
while they were touching the grass. They sustained this in almost
complete silence with little difficulty for ten minutes; the tape
recorder was then turned on and each child invited to share some-
thing sensed. The things that the children described were mostly
about the grass — the scratchiness on the back of their legs, that
there were several little bits joined together at the bottom of the
blades of grass, descriptions of 'waving greenness'. Then the ques-
tions began to emerge:
0v
S
asked if they could take grasses back 'to see if our grass is in a
book'. (A small selection of grasses were taken back with the appro-
priate explanation as to why this was permissible when plants
should not normally be picked.) This child subsequently spent a
long time on several occasions pouring over a book matching up
the grasses and wanting to read the relevant information, asking
other children to read certain paragraphs for her.
Sorting grasses into various groups, discussing the different
characteristics which distinguished one from another, was one
task undertaken back at school. This requires attention to small
detail as differences between one grass and another are not easy
to spot, even though the flower heads on the grasses at that time
of the year would have helped.
I pause in the narrative about the teaching to focus on the class-
room. Primary classrooms generally look attractive with children's
work on the wall, but displays can contribute more to learning by
being interactive. My recent visit to the classroom revealed sev-
eral interactive displays intrinsic to the teaching. By then another
Professor Sense had been introduced and was one of six interact-
ive displays.
how much they had progressed by reviewing how much more they
now knew.
Professor Question was a small box pinned to the wall by the
computer, where every now and again a child went and wrote a
question, printed it out and 'fed' it to the professor. The display
included the words 'How? What? When? Why? Who?' as prompts
for starting questions. Only three weeks into the topic I found, in
the class I visited, that Professor Question had already been 'fed'
those listed below:
How does a snail make its shell?
Is a ladybird a bird?
How many legs has an insect?
What is a caterpillar?
Do earthworms feel dry or wet?
What does an earthworm eat?
What do ladybirds eat?
When do ladybirds get their spots?
What are aphids?
Is a spider an insect?
How many sorts of butterfly can you get?
How many legs has a spider?
Is an ant an insect?
Can a fox eat a bird?
Does an ant lay eggs?
and by the end of the topic she had been fed far more.
Who was Professor Question? She, like Professor Sense, was the
figment of the imagination of the teacher, who created her after
the success of the story of Professor Sense. This is her story which
depicts both the interaction between observation and questioning
and how questioning provokes questioning:
Professor Question
All of a sudden, one day, the classroom door burst open. A
very unusual person stood there almost as if on one leg and
asked, 'Hello. Have you seen my friend? We seem to have
lost each other. Now, where do you think he can be? Can
you help? I am lost, you see. Can you tell me which way I
should go next?'
One girl nearby said, 'Who are you looking for?' A boy said,
'Where do you want to go to?' The teacher moved across
44 Creativity in primary science
the classroom and asked, 'May I help you? Have you been
to the office?'
'You may help me if you can, but can you?' replied the
unusual person.
'I don't know. Who are you looking for? What do you
want?' asked the teacher.
The visitor looked very confused.
The teacher tried again, 'Have you been to the office?'
'Where's the office? Who will I find there? How do I get
there?' replied the visitor.
By this time, the teacher looked confused too and the
children thought it was a huge joke. The teacher put on a
disapproving look. One child, trying to be helpful, asked
'Who are you? Maybe, if we know that we can help you.
Who are you?'
At this point, the visitor's face lit up with excitement. 'I
can answer that one,' the visitor exclaimed, 'I am Professor
Question. Do you know me? Does that help you to help me?'
Another child called out, 'Professor Question! That's unu-
sual. Is that why you keep on asking questions? Who is your
friend?'
'Oh dear! You're all catching it, aren't you?'
'Catching what? Is it like measles?' asked another child.
Just at that moment, the classroom door opened again and
in walked the children's old friend, Professor Sense. 'Oh, here
you are, Question. I wondered where you'd got to. Getting on
OK? Pleased you've met my friends. Are they looking after
you all right?'
'Please, can someone tell me what is going on?' asked the
anxious teacher.
'This is my friend, Professor Question. Usually, we go about
together most of the time, you know. I tell her things. She
asks questions, don't you dear?'
'Yes, I certainly ask questions. But I don't know about you
telling me things. You notice things most of the time, don't you?'
'Yes, that's right. I notice them and I tell you about them
and you start asking questions about them and then I have
to notice more and tell you and then you ask...'
'Yes, yes,' interrupted the teacher, 'I think we've got all
that now. Now, where can we go from here?'
At that point the children burst out laughing.
Parks, woods and 45
length
width
area
weight.
had been played with the whole class, the children benefited im-
mensely from it. It became the source of much discussion, many
questions and several games. The speech therapist used it on sev-
eral occasions in order to encourage children to describe animals
using characteristics which enabled another child to work out
which animal was being described. The children invented many
games themselves. The use of scientific terminology improved;
children used words like 'insect', 'arachnid', 'mollusc', 'inverte-
brate' accurately, without hesitation, and were able to compare
groups by their characteristics.
The tape which the children had started on their first sensory
walk to the park was often replayed and added to during the
topic. It is not easy to do this in a busy classroom where other
noises can interfere with the tape. The teacher organized recording
by individuals during the 20-minute silent reading period which
they had every day after lunch. One child was allowed to go into
the teacher's cupboard and record, during this period, without
disturbing the class and without extraneous noises being picked
up by the microphone.
Parks, woods and wastelands 51
the soil next and the grass least. The teacher prompted a discus-
sion of why that might be and the children willingly offered sen-
sible and plausible explanations. 'Perhaps children in the area
affect it. We think that perhaps worms don't like children, but we
do not know what they do like. Can we try it in the park on our
next visit?'
As already mentioned, the teacher is particularly skilled in
having time to talk with children about the work in hand. Very
few of her conversations with children are about organization.
She therefore plays an essential role of 'lifting thinking' by ask-
ing probing questions. She encouraged children to discuss what
they were doing and thinking at every stage and asked questions
which helped them clarify their ideas. The investigation became
a series of events, each one followed by discussion. For the next
few weeks a pattern continued of planning, discussing; invest-
igating, discussing; recording and evaluating, discussing.
Because the groups had to undertake three investigations the
teacher noticed that during this time:
• children developed a method of writing their plan systematic-
ally;
• they grew in independence of thought and action;
• their hypotheses became more detailed and reasoned, e.g. 'I
think that ants prefer to live under logs because other animals
that eat them can't find them as easily as on a tree';
• they recognized more easily when they were not answering
their original questions;
• they became more confident about methods of recording;
• they were more able to evaluate their methods of working.
They seemed to grow to trust that they would not be told off if
they said that the work did not go well and became confident to
search for reasons and to share these reasons.
Consolidation
The visit to Epping Forest was the culmination of the project. The
autonomy of the children by this time was such that they decided
beforehand for themselves which of their questions they were going
to follow up in the woodland and what investigations they were
going to do. They prepared the necessary data sheets. It rained that
day, but nevertheless the visit went ahead. Data sheets had to be
put away on the coach, as a result, and they 'went on an adven-
ture'. They were fascinated by fungi, especially on fallen trees —
and asked lots of questions. They enjoyed the atmosphere; they
often stood and listened and watched quietly. Many animals —
fox, squirrel, innumerable frogs — were seen and they did several
investigations but they had to remember data. They acknowledged
the inaccuracy of this procedure later in their written accounts.
Several offered explanations for their findings, e.g. 'I think I
found more worms because there was good soil for them, plenty
of places to hide from their enemies, and not many people walked
there so they did not get squashed much.'
Formative assessment
Assessment checklists
To help keep frack of the children's development, in both their obser-
vation and investigation skills, the teacher devised the checklists
shown in Figures 3.4 and 3.5. The observation checklist focuses
on the level of detail which is observed, the use of the senses, the
skill with a hand lens, the ability to group similar observations
in a classification exercise, the ability to describe observations in
words and drawings, and the ability to recognize patterns. The
scale from 'beginning' to 'competent' is useful because it implies
skills and understanding that grow over time.
The investigation checklist requires little comment — it has all the
stages of an investigation (see Chapter 2) and allows the teacher
to record both oral and written contributions from the children.
Considering questions
Any classroom product (writing, speech, drawings, models, actions)
provides useful information if the teacher is able to be analytical.
54 Creativity in primary science
OBSERVATION CHECKLIST
Observes detail
Observes differences
Observes similarities
The questions, for instance, that children wrote soon after Pro-
fessor Question was introduced provided useful insight into their
development at that point. Group A could ask questions but not
write them down. Their questions tended to be fairly general:
'What animals do you find in the park?' 'What can you see in a
forest?' 'Why is there a forest?', which indicated that they needed
more experience in simple observations in order to build up a
better knowledge of these places. Group B asked similar questions,
mostly concerned with the difference between parks, woods and
wastelands, but they could write. Group C were already asking
questions of a more specific nature about life in the places, sev-
eral of which had the potential to lead to investigations: 'What
do trees do for us?' 'Why do ants like trees?' 'What is soil?'.
Concept maps
The concept maps shown in Figure 3.6 were drawn in July near the
end of the topic, and again were a product that gave feedback to
Parks, woods and wastelands 55
INVESTIGATION CHECKLIST
HYPOTHESIZING
Mentions 1 relevant feature in explanation
PLANNING
Aware of fair test
MEASURE
Makes comparison
INTERPRETATION
Offers related to data
RECORDING
Clear, consistent method
Accurate oral
Accurate written
Use of chart/graph
EVALUATE
Can offer improvement
Notices flaws
the teacher about what the children understood and what they
could put down in writing and drawing that expressed their
understanding.
Development of knowledge
because they could both fly, worms and slugs were paired together,
because of the similar shapes of their bodies. Cats and dogs were
put together because they were pets. The more they researched the
animals the more they were able to identify other features which
were similar. Snails and slugs were seen to be related. Cats and
dogs were put into a larger group of mammals as animals that
give birth to live young as opposed to laying an egg. They learnt
that insects are a large group of animals, all of which have six
legs. They also learnt that for invertebrates it was useful to record
information about legs, wings, antennae, body parts, segments,
size (length, width, area and weight).
Diversity of living things: Grass was obviously the first eye-opener
here; there was more than one type of grass, and then when the
children looked carefully, a grassy area was made up of many
plants other than grass. The diversity of minibeasts found was
impressive (several sorts of spiders, caterpillars, ants, woodlice,
slugs, snails. ).
. .
Food chain: This is a difficult idea. They often get so carried away
drawing a food chain that imagination takes over from reality.
(People eating dogs and dogs eating eagles...!) However, once
when the class had snails on the ground and there were birds
circling overhead, one child said 'I wonder if they are coming for
the snails and want to eat them?' — so some notion of food chain
related to first-hand evidence was registering.
There are many pieces to this jigsaw. They fit together and rep-
resent a highly developed and well thought-out teaching plan.
There was, however, something almost unremarkable on enter-
ing the classroom. The class were working busily and relatively
quietly when I visited. The children had all watched a video tape
first thing in the morning about animals in various habitats and
about animals building homes at specific times. Several were busy
answering questions about the tape from the information books.
One group was drawing a trapdoor spider from a book and dis-
cussing what the book said about it. Many were on the maths prob-
lem associated with leaves, and every now and again a child
would go and collect a leaf from the school grounds. Once in a
while a child would get up and write a question for Professor
Question, print it off and feed it in. Some children completed
paintings of trees or leaves and left them to dry. At the end of the
morning the children tidied the books away, organized their desks
and left for play. Two tearful arguments had also been sorted out.
The teacher moved systematically from group to group working
alongside the children and moving their thinking forward at
every opportunity.
When I returned again two or three weeks later, the parks
poster had been replaced by the work on minibeast acrostics and
several of the other classroom displays had been changed — show-
ing an ever-changing educational environment for the children
in this class.
Parks, woods and wastelands 59
5th and final stage: planning 5th and final stage: assessment
Assess the children's ability to plan an investigation that they want to carry • it rained so data sheets had to be put away on the coach and children
out in Epping Forest 'went on an adventure'
• fascinated by fungi, especially on fallen trees — and asked lots of questions
During the day
enjoyed the atmosphere and often stood and listened and watched quietly
assess the children's observational skills
• saw lots of animals — fox, squirrel, innumerable frogs
• assess their ability to carry out their investigations.
• did some investigations but had to remember data — acknowledged this
After the visit inaccuracy in their written accounts.
• assess their ability to display their findings
'I I
think found more worms because there was good soil for them, plenty of
• assess their interpretations
places to hide from their enemies, and not many people walked there so
• assess their ability to communicate and to evaluate their work.
they did not get squashed much.'
4
Rabbit's House
YEARS 1 AND 2 (5 AND 6-YEAR-OLDS)
A
c<1
In
A
P eb5U!
'I —
LoO Kt O..I'Q
U I
wrc±e
0.1!
L
4
oc' We took turns to make a sound in Rabbit's House.
o.N I used two shells and pencils. The pencils were
web! W2(22
hard and the shells were the easiest. We looked at
our chart. We wrote all of the things I have just
Là 4 written now. The ones with lots of X were hardest
and the ones with were goodest.
t $itl 4
4
We are doing Rabbit's House. I
t
Figure 4.3 Written account and drawing from A in Group 4
68 Creativity in primary science
in Figures 4.4 and 4.5. The account and summaries have been used
in my teaching, in both pre-service and inservice courses, and many
teachers have found they have enabled them to gain an insight
into the implementation of exploration and investigation with
young children; hence the decision to include this case study in
the book. As with other chapters the first draft was read by the
teacher and amendments made in the light of her comments.
The quality of the pieces of work and of the accompanying
table was what first attracted my attention, and provoked me to
find out about the teaching which had generated it. The work was
from relatively young children (5 and 6-year-olds). The accounts,
like the two shown were well set out, showing some evidence
of chronological writing, factual reporting, and interpretation of
data (Figures 4.2 and 4.3). The table (Figure 4.1) has the results
clearly presented with proper labels, titles and a key to explain
the significance of the ticks and crosses. The drawings show the
experimental set-up within the classroom.
Even from just the two accounts, drawings and the table it is
almost possible to piece together the details of the investigation.
The children were investigating how easy it was to identify the
sounds made by different objects. They had six pairs of objects
— two shells, two pens, two buttons, two plastic numbers, two
pencils and two crayons — and a large box which had been made
into 'Rabbit's House'. The two children whose work is shown
had worked in a group of four; one child was the subject who
had to sit outside the house and try to identify the source of the
sound. The other children selected one of the pairs of objects and
banged them together to make a sound inside the house. If the
child outside identified the source correctly a tick was recorded
on the table and if not, a cross was recorded.
The table of data (Figure 4.1) was the element of the children's
work which fascinated me most initially. It has all the features of
a good table in that it conveys most of the information about the
investigation. It has a title showing what the results refer to; the
key explains what the symbols mean; and it is set out so that ini-
tial interpretation is relatively easy. We can, for instance, see that
the noises from the shells and pens were identified more often than
not; and that the noises from pencils, buttons and crayons were
not easily recognized. The fact that it is drawn on squared paper
means that if the results were inserted as the children were doing
the experiment, they could keep track of what they had done, and
Rabbit's House 69
One child explained how her interpretation fits the data by the
use of the word 'because', stating that 'the pencils are hard to do
because the pencils had more crosses and the pens is easy because
they had more ticks'. The change in handwriting in Figure 4.2
indicates that L added her explanation, 'The ones with lots of x
were hardest and the one with were goodest', at a later stage,
possibly in response to the teacher asking 'How did you know?',
or to class discussion of what was found.
Several children mentioned 'taking turns'. Cooperation had been
a key learning intention on the part of the teacher (alongside data
recording and interpretation). The reason for repetition in science
is only slowly understood (see Chapter 2) and one child's comment,
'we got it right, so I did not go to have a go again', indicates lim-
ited understanding at this stage.
Examples of the children's work clearly showed their differ-
ent stages of development. The task, in the way it was organized
and supported, had allowed for differentiation in the outcome.
The drawing and writing does not, however, show how the chil-
dren organized themselves. A, in fact, played a key role in organ-
izing the group, which emphasizes the importance of different
methods of assessment to give a whole picture of a child. L, on
the other hand, whose work is shown in Figure 4.2, giggled most
of the way through the investigation because there happened
to be someone making a video of the classroom that day, and in
her case the written record revealed far more than the teacher's
informal observations!
Given these features I asked the teacher for detail of the sequence
of teaching, to explain how she had introduced the investigation,
why it was called 'Rabbit's House', and how she had enabled rel-
atively young children to produce and record data of considerable
quality.
The investigation had started with a Masai story about Rabbit's
House (Verna Aardema (1978) Who's in Rabbit's House?) in which
Rabbit goes away from his house and when he returns he finds
someone in his house: 'And a big bad voice from inside the house
roared, "I am the Long One. I eat trees and trample on elephants.
Go away! Or I will trample on you!" Rabbit is frightened and
does not know what to do; slowly one animal after another (Jackal,
Leopard, Elephant, Rhinoceros) comes to the house and each time
the same thing happens; they offer to use their strength in various
ways to get the Long One out but all would involve the demolition
Rabbit's House 71
DISCUSSION Generated ideas and strategies e.g. standing behind the screen,
hands in front of face, listening on other side of the door
EXPLORATION Tried out ideas informally — found that it was difficult to tell
Let's try to investigate Rabbit's House — two children offer to
make the house
IDENTIFY A Problem
FOCUSED What noises are easy to identify?
PROBLEM How shall we do the investigation?
When the house was made, the teacher again set aside time to
discuss the planning of the investigation with the whole class.
The first task was to focus the problem and to decide how to go
about finding an answer to it. The question selected, What sounds
are easy to identify?', is relatively straightforward and very close
to the question used in the first informal exploration the previ-
ous week. This was important because the investigation the chil-
dren eventually undertook was a refinement of a 'practice run'.
The children were able to suggest that they would have to make
different noises and that someone who was not watching would
try and guess what made the noise.
With older children it might be possible to leave them at this
point to design the investigation but younger children need con-
siderable support. Like other teachers quoted in this book, this
teacher aims to give children sufficient guidance to enable them
to get started, but to leave some parts slightly open ended, so that
the children have to address the problems as they come to them.
In this case she questioned them about methods of making
noises, equipment that could be used ('What could you use to
make the noises?' 'Should we have many things?'), organization
('How will you organize the activity?'), what to record ('What
shall we record?'), and how to record ('How shall we record?').
In her teaching, she was not telling the children what to do, but
helping to identify for them the things they needed to think
about so that they had to make the decisions themselves. More
importantly she expected children to listen to and evaluate each
other's suggestions.
After some time a skeleton plan emerged to which the whole
class had subscribed. The children were to use six pairs of class-
room objects to hit together and they would record their findings
on a chart. (Figure 4.6 shows this chart, as it was completed by
the first group). The objects would be hidden in the house and
one person would be outside listening, with another person choos-
ing which pair of objects to use for the sound. Crosses would be
used to record wrong identification and ticks, correct identifica-
tion. They would work in groups.
When most of the important decisions were made she used the
question 'Suppose we start, what are the difficulties we are likely
to meet?' She used this often to give an opportunity for last-minute
thoughts and questions about procedures, and to make the chil-
dren think ahead.
74 Creativity in primary science
Aq S
Our
We —
#o X — and
HarrAesf
Shefts k-
'
(R.
(
v Cr
io
The teacher asked the other children to look at the results and
decide what it told them. Questions about the meaning of the
ticks and crosses led naturally to the idea that it is useful to add
explanation by means of a key. The children also found that try-
ing to make sense of the numbers of ticks and crosses turned out
to be tedious because they were jumbled up. 'What could we do
about it?' The suggestion that squared paper would be better
emerged; with the ticks on one line — one per box — and the
crosses on the adjoining line. This process of identifying and
sharing a problem with the whole class and requiring the chil-
dren to sort it out is an important strategy. The improved method
of recording on squared paper was used by all subsequent groups.
This little episode gives an example of one of the loose ends which
had been left from the initial planning, and which was used very
productively as part of the teaching.
The leaving of this loose end was not an accident. The teacher
deliberately chose to do this, knowing that it would raise issues
about ways of recording, and about the ways that facilitate inter-
pretation of results. She attaches a high importance to allowing
children to make mistakes and learn from them. She explained
further that it was part of 'the process of learning why we use
particular forms of charts or tables, rather than just seeing tables
as a convention'.
Before looking at the subsequent set of events, it is worth noting
that by this point there had been three substantial elements of
whole-class teaching which had taken place. The first one focused
on trying out an early exploration; the second developed the explora-
tion into a more systematic investigation with detailed plans for
procedure; the third evaluated and improved the method of re-
cording. What can be seen in action and in context here is the
explicit teaching of the different stages of an investigation.
The third discussion also helped to remind subsequent groups
about what they had to do when it was their turn to collect data,
so that group 2 took over the equipment and did the investiga-
tion with little further help from the teacher. At the same time
group 1 transferred their data on to the new form of chart and
realized as they were doing it that a pattern was emerging that
they could see quite easily. The first and second charts of group
1 are shown in Figures 4.6 and 4.7 respectively. This reorganizing
of the chart is reported in the writing of one of the children in
group 1:
Rabbit's House 77
People couldn't see how many ticks and crosses there was,
so we had to do another one. We had to do them in a straight
line.
holding the shell was allowed to speak; others who did not hold
it had to listen. Her own actions and responses were also import-
ant because of the role model which teachers provide for their
pupils. She had to listen carefully and to give careful considera-
tion to what each child said. Her use of questions to extend their
thinking, many examples of which are in the text, is part of the
key to her success — 'Can you tell us a bit more about that?' 'How
did you do this?' 'What shall we do about this problem?'
Good classroom organization is crucial. In this teacher's class-
room, the position of resources are carefully organized and main-
tained, so that children know where things are and little time is
wasted on giving out equipment and resources. Daily routines, car-
ried out by the children for maintaining and checking resources,
play an important role in helping children to take responsibility
and to feel a sense of ownership and pride in the environment.
Above all it releases the teacher for more important interactions
with the children.
5
Ourselves
YEAR 2 (6 AND 7-YEAR-OLDS)
This case study comes from a school in west London. The design
of the displays, making the classroom look like a well-organized
'Aladdin's cave' full of treasures, first caught my eye and prompted
my request to the teacher to include her work. The Pine class scient-
ist (Figure 5.1) epitomizes the activities in which the children
were engaged. (Classes in this school are named after trees, hence
the 'Pine'.) I found examples of observational and illustrative
practical work as well as an investigation; measurement leading
to data collection; data interpretation; use of information books
and videos to find information to support first-hand findings; sys-
tematic research to solve a real problem; and the involvement of
the local community in the topic.
The material for the chapter was collected by photographing
(towards the end of the topic) the classroom and the adjoining
sections of the corridor, by talking with the teacher, by studying
children's work, by examining the teacher's written records and
by the teacher reading and amending an early draft of the chapter.
The topic of Ourselves was taught from the beginning of January
82 Creativity in primary science
and acted as a summary of the work covered. Each new part of the
body learnt about was added in words on one side of the figure;
on the reverse side were drawings of the parts of the body, added
systematically as the topic progressed. By the end, cut-outs of heart,
ribs, lungs and windpipe had been attached at appropriate places;
the brain had been stuck on; bones, muscles and blood vessels
had been drawn in. The words written on the body were used in an
adaptation of the song 'I have a busy body, a very busy body, and
it goes everywhere with me. And on that body, I've got a. .
with verses such as 'I've got a body, a very busy body, and it
goes everywhere with me. And in that body, I've got a heart, and
it goes everywhere with me. With a bump thump here, bump,
thump there, bump, thump, bump, thump everywhere. ., goes .
which had been the focus of the mathematics work in the previous
term, and to teach them about using tables and presenting data
in an organized way. The topic easily provided opportunities for
this as can be seen in the work described later in the chapter.
The children were not particularly excited by the topic at first
as they thought they knew all about their bodies, but it was not
long before they realized there was a lot more to learn and they
became engrossed. There were also two model skeletons that had
been put up in the classroom, one of which showed not only
the bones but many of the organs in the body. To begin with the
children gave them at most a cursory glance, and in some cases
appeared to find them distasteful, but before long they were going
to them and having very serious discussions about what all the
parts were and how many bones they had.
Hair
One of the key events in getting the children more involved and
interested was the studies and investigation on hair. There was
a sudden awakening of interest when the children realized that
they could do investigations on themselves. While not the first
activity, it was done very early in the topic.
Early descriptive work where children sought words to describe
hair was followed by an investigation of hair strength. It was an
investigation which lent itself to the use of a table for recording
the results (Figure 5.2) and hence reinforced understanding of the
use of tables. It is, in fact, an investigation which can be used for
a variety of materials (hair, threads of wool, cotton, etc.) and vari-
ants of it can be found in other books. Given the relatively low
weights needed to be hung on the bottom of hair before it snaps,
the use of paper clips is sensible and is simple to resource.
Interpretation is less easy. Children have a tendency to isolate
a single factor as significant (long, curly), e.g. 'long hair is stronger
than short hair' but several children quickly realized that more
than one factor was coming into play.
Having got the class interested, what else was done? In out-
line, one system of the body after another was studied. Each new
system was started by a discussion or other exercise which ena-
bled the teacher to find out what the class knew already. She then
tended to give them activities they could engage in together in
small groups, but as children finished activities at different times,
Ourselves 85
W koe
kay? COtou/
T
T
S
the difference. The fourth was an exercise iii interpreting data which
was done systematically with each child and required individual
interviews.
dates carefully, and they had to make sure that the Friday feeders
put in the pellets which will last two days.
Of the four activities selected for this section, this problem solv-
ing looks least like an assessment tool. Good assessment tools are
usually good learning activities, so it is often not possible to distin-
guish them. The purpose to which they are put and the way they
are used tend to determine their classification into one or other.
This example shows that children of this age can rise to the chal-
lenge of such a problem; they can collect and evaluate informa-
tion on a problem that interests them; they can use a variety of
sources for their information (books, pamphlets, people); they
can (with help) ask appropriate questions to elicit the information
they want; they can evaluate available resources to help them
make choices (e.g. the eventual decision to take the much loved
idea of a parrot off the list); they can come up with an answer in
which the teacher has sufficient confidence to follow it through
to the purchase of the pet.
Before moving to the third assessment tool, it is worth pausing
to review the organization of this one. First there were four groups,
each of which took on the responsibility of researching one of the
animals and displaying the information for everyone. The teacher
had a support teacher once a week. Originally all groups were to
go to the pet shop but in the event the supply teacher was ill for
some of the time and only one group went. One child from each
of the pet groups was selected to take questions for the whole
group. In every case the teacher selected a child for whom Eng-
lish was not their home language in order to boost their confid-
ence. One of the important lessons to be learnt by the children
from such an activity is that reliable information cannot be col-
lected quickly; that often two or three sources need to be consulted
because no single source has all the information needed; that it
is possible to pick up conflicting information from two different
sources, necessitating further research. Even the voting cannot be
done at one instance in time. This problem was a shared problem:
the care of the pet was to be shared, and therefore everyone had
to be involved in the voting. The absence of one or two children
through illness had to be allowed for by having a voting box in
which children could put their votes. The opening of the box was
then done only after everyone had had an opportunity to cast
his/her vote.
The teacher had wondered if the children would vote for the
90 Creativity in primary science
pet they had researched, but this did not necessarily happen; an
indication that some really were evaluating the information sens-
ibly and making an informed decision.
A minor interesting point was the naming of the fish as Rose
and Lily; two names that the teacher would have expected or
chosen herself. She realized, however, that she had recently been
talking to the class about a Victorian painting in which there were
two little girls called Rose and Lily. Children sometimes make links
the teacher does not anticipate!
In reflecting overall on how the children tackled this problem,
she commented that 'their behaviour modelled adults'; they could
pick up the problem at odd points, find out a few more points,
record them and then go on with other activities. Their interest
was maintained over several weeks.
Learning activities
Narfle:
Span Arm
1
17 eq
e7
3ennq 20
Ma.LC01r4 20 S4..
Mne, 21
- 22
2.3
Je,
Tack. 2q.
pQcpLc oh
have
/'0
ID Card
Name
Address
D0B
am years old
My height cm
My reach cm
My handspan cm
My stride cm
My eye colour
My hair
Figure 5.7 Does the person with the longest arms have the biggest
handspan?
claims such as 'I've got a bigger stride than you'; these were re-
solved by: 'Let's go and check on the ID cards.' Sometimes they
involved collating qualitative instead of quantitative information,
such as that needed to answer the question 'Do all black people
have curly hair?'
It is important not to underestimate the practice that is needed
96 Creativity in primary science
Skeleton
Children, by the age of 7, know something about skeletons, be-
cause they appear in cartoons, stories (the Funny Bones series)
and non-fiction books. In one sense they know they have a skel-
eton, but somehow do not make the connection between the
pictures and cartoons and their own bodies. Consequently 'finding
our bones' by feeling for them is a discovery — a sort of review-
ing their knowledge of skeletons through a new perspective.
A simple fact is that bones cannot bend because they are hard;
so if the body bends at any point there must be a joint between
two bones. One activity therefore involved the children drawing
round their hands putting a cross where a bend occurred, trying
to work out how many bones they had in their hands and then
Ourselves 97
referring to the books to check out how many of the total they
had found. The sheer number of little bones that are present in
the wrist (necessary to allow the complicated movements that our
hands can make) is always a surprise to children, but of course
these cannot all be felt from the outside. The fact that several of
the drawings of the body at the end of the topic showed the many
finger and wrist bones indicated that children had registered and
remembered this (refer back, for instance, to the right-hand draw-
ing in Figure 5.4).
Hunting the bends in their bodies to find the joints helped the
children understand the significance of these for their movement.
They made models of their arms and legs (cardboard for the
bones, split pin paper clips for the joints), carefully counting the
bends to get the models as accurate as possible.
Spine, ribs and skull were also easily found by feeling, and the
length of the spine was added to the ID card. It is interesting to
note that the brain in the February drawing in Figure 5.4 is shown
outside the skull but inside the head, giving yet another example
of what could be used as the basis of a conversation between the
teacher and the child.
Hung in the classroom were three 'basic' skeletons made out
of strips of thick paper to form 3D models. One was normal size,
one half size and one twice normal size.
Heart
Many people cannot easily find their pulse, so the task set in
the sheet shown in Figure 5.8 was not that straightforward. The
children did, however, become aware of the beating of their hearts
in FE, anticipating it partly because the teacher had talked to them
about the effect of exercise on the heartbeat.
There was a lovely incident which occurred during PE. One child,
who was not a particularly fast learner, suddenly rushed over and
asked the teacher to feel his heart beating; it was going quickly
because he was exercising. He knew this and was delighted to
have noticed that what had been discussed in class applied to him.
For him, this was a discovery.
FE reinforced the work on the body. The teacher had also found
the information that 7-year-old children are able to run for ten
minutes without stopping and had read this to the class, who
were keen to try. She made the children build up to this slowly
98 Creativity in primary science
I / T
We
Lungs — breathing
Learning about where the lungs are, how big they are and what
they look like was done mainly through the teacher talking to the
class, showing them pictures and allowing them to look up informa-
tion in books. It was in this context that the teacher related an
incident which had stuck in her mind:
I had asked one child [of average abilityl to look in informa-
tion books and find out about lungs for the class 'busy body'.
About a fortnight later when I was starting to talk about lungs
to lead up to an investigation, the child put up his hand and
described the lungs, where they are in the body, function,
etc. This was significant because this child [not usually inter-
ested in science] had been inspired. He had read informa-
tion about lungs, discussed it with his parents, etc. and had
been able to remember it all.
So 'information told by the children' must be added to the list of
how this section was taught.
The investigation referred to by the teacher was titled 'What
happens when we breathe?' It moved from qualitative observa-
tions (What happens to the chest when we breathe?) to quantit-
ative observations, involving both measurement and calculation,
and finally to thinking about how that might relate to what is
going on inside. As with the whole body, drawings were again
used to find out what children knew about the parts of the body
which help us to breathe.
Growth
Young children first think of growth only in terms of 'getting
bigger', but they cannot readily answer 'how much bigger?'. Pre-
paring the display shown in Figure 5.9 (drawing round a child
and an adult) helped give the idea of the relative sizes.
The children brought in photographs they had of themselves
when younger and built up their own personal timelines, showing
100 Creativity in primary science
how they had changed. Growth is much more than 'getting big-
ger' — this was a point the teacher brought out in her discussions
with the children; she even related it back to the work which had
been done on plants the previous term — as a plant grows it also
does not just 'get bigger'. An appreciation of the increased skill
and changing functions of themselves even in a fairly short life-
time is well appreciated by the child whose work is featured in
Figure 5.10.
The mother of one of the children was expecting a baby and
was having regular photographs taken from an echoscan; she
was prepared to lend these to the teacher so that the children
could see the growth. Normally the teacher found that the chil-
dren had little interest in babies, but this time they were really
interested to see the baby when it was born.
Senses
This was not a major part of the work, more like revision from
a previous topic. The teacher used a story which was a spoof on
Little Red Riding Hood and had appeared in Guardian Education
(Baird 1994). She decided she could adapt it as a drama exercise
for her class. (The original story had Grandma telling Little Red
Riding Hood all about her big ears and what they could do etc.
until eventually she had revised all her GCSE Biology for her!)
So the children acted out a similar drama with the one playing
Grandma telling Red Riding Hood about the senses, not to GCSE
level but at their own level.
Brain
This study was mainly done by one independent small group.
The teacher, however, did spend a little time talking with the
class about the brain as the part of your body which sends
messages to make the rest of the body work. To enable the class
to appreciate the complexity of messages and incidentally to do
a valuable language exercise, she got the children to give her mess-
ages (to her arm) in order to make it pick up a pencil. She only
did exactly what they told her and they realized how difficult it
was to give precise instructions! Through playing the game several
times their ability to express their ideas improved no end.
cpvjd
•1 COL1(d
I
Supporting resources
The resources used, apart from the classroom displays, were a
video about the human body which covered bones, hair and
skin. It supported the children's learning and extended it to areas
they would not be dealing with in depth in the topic (animal
skin and camouflage).
Book resources included books on the human body which were
lent by a parent: the Ladybird books on pets, which have already
been mentioned, and a large picture book on the human body.
The equipment needed was relatively simple, mainly station-
ery items and measuring equipment.
Final note
I,
PrQn,?
Figure 5.11 Display for starting the next topic — more words
6
The Channel tunnel
YEAR 6 (10 AND 11-YEAR-OLDS)
Figure 6.1 Planning chart for one term's integrated topic on the Channel tunnel
The Channel tunnel 107
• Why and when have people wanted to cross the channel in the
past? How did they do it? Did they succeed?
• The site of the tunnel — why there?
• Do compasses work underground?
• How are tunnels made?
108 Creativity in primary science
Week 10 Organization of tunnel — the treaty — how it will be run, when it will be open
Mar
Week 11 Operation of tunnel — car and lorry shuttles and other vehicles
Mar
for tackling the last two weeks of the topic, when the management,
organization, legal and operational features of the Channel tunnel
project were studied in weeks 10 and 11.
The modelling of the TBM (tunnel boring machine) was also
wisely left until after the visit, where there had been a good oppor-
tunity to look at the machine and understand how it worked.
The Year 6 one-day trip to France was made in the summer
term. During this trip children surveyed people's opinions about
the use of the tunnel as opposed to the ferry and also visited the
exhibition at Sangatte on the French side, where most of the labels
were in French, and viewed the new landscape that had been
made out of the chalk spoil from the tunnel.
There were then two weeks when the class learnt about the
formation of continents, earth plates and the Ice Age, and hence
began to know that the map of the world has not always been as
it is now. The more local effects of the Ice Age, causing Britain to
be separated from the body of the mainland by a water channel
(although it is still connected to the plate underneath the water),
were then studied. The difference between separate plates, and
separate pieces of the same plate, is not easy to understand when
from the surface they all look like bits of land with water in be-
tween. But the difference is important when you are tunnelling;
tunnelling between places on the same plate is feasible; tunnel-
ling between plates is not!
The fact that there are different types of rock in the earth's crust
needs to be appreciated, because the route the tunnel goes may
well be determined by the particular rock formations at any point.
The study of the formation of rocks in the earth's crust which
was undertaken in week 3 set the groundwork for the study of
the specific rocks which were in the neighbourhood of possible tun-
nel routes. The teacher reported that this topic fascinated the class
and she could have done with an expert to help answer all the
questions raised, or at least to know where to find all the answers.
Between her and the class they put together a considerable collec-
tion of rocks, and recognized from preliminary observations and
simple scratch tests just how different they were.
The sheer breadth of knowledge that primary teachers have
to have is illustrated by this topic. The teacher recorded two of
the resources that were particularly useful to her for the first
two weeks' work; first was the book Discover Physical Geography
(Grimwade 1987), and second was a BBC schools programme
Environments which showed rock formation in coastal areas. This
latter is pertinent to the South-East of England as all the rocks
are sedimentary and were originally laid down under water.
Week 4 was, in the teacher's mind, one of the turning points
in the topic. The task she devised for the children was simply
to plan how to construct a tunnel from the school to the swim-
ming pool, which was in a sports complex adjoining the school
grounds. The journey was not far, but when the weather was rainy
or snowy, it was unpleasant. This task turned out to be one of
the most fruitful exercises of the topic because it made the chil-
dren understand, in a way which was very personal to them, the
issues involved in tunnel building. In an initial discussion the class
The Channel tunnel 113
What do we need?
A plan
Workers
Digging tools and machines
Building materials
Problems encountered
1 Will it fall in?
2 Are there any pipes or cables in the way?
3 How can we be sure we are digging in the right direction?
4 What shall we do with the spoil?
5 Where do we get the money from?
6 Who is going to dig it?
IS IT REALLY WORTH IT?
listed things they would have to think about in making their plans
and drew up a list of things that they would need (Figure 6.3). The
problems slowly began to emerge; a summary list put together
at the end of the drawing is shown in Figure 6.4. A teacher really
could not ask for much more; the questions are all relevant to
tunnel builders whether going half a mile or 20 miles.
One child's plans are included in Figure 6.5. The accompany-
ing explanation was one of the most detailed, where the child con-
sidered the construction and support of the tunnel, the necessary
slope at one end and trapdoor at the other, the number of carriages
needed and the means of moving the carriages through (pulled by
a rope). The rope is to be attached to a winding handle; there is,
however, no information about who will be turning the handle.
The response which I received, when I asked the teacher
how she thought of the idea of the tunnel from the school to the
pool, was typical of what other teachers say about their bright
ideas: 'I don't know, it suddenly came into my head.' There seem
to be preconditions that are often reported: a certain unease that
Making a tunnel from the school to the swimming pool
1 Make a shed like this out of plywood and cover
it with Artex plaster and use orange tiles on the roof.
Location: lawn by football playground. Make a ramp going
eight metres downwards, (for route see the picture marked A)
2 To make a start to the tunnel use a machine called a mole
and dig a tunnel six feet high and four feet wide. Support this by
wooden beams made of oak wood. Cover with metal sheets (see pictures here).
3 Pin down rails on the surface of the floor going across the floor and down through the tunnel and fix the winding
handle at the end of the shed just above the door. Get a very long rope and hook it into the winding handle and put
hooks all the way through the tunnel on the ceiling and thread the rope through each hook. Make sure the rope is a
bit too long so that there will be room on the winding handle and fix a winding handle on the other side of the tunnel
and fix the other end of the rope onto that and thread it through, so that there will be plenty of rope on the floor.
4 Make a joinable group of little carts about 15 cm high (see picture B) seating six children in each. Make seven of
these carts. Join all the carts together and tie the ends of a rope to one of the carts at each end. When you turn a
handle the carts should move through the tunnel. When you turn it the other way, they should come back again.
5 At the swimming pool cut one of the slabs out from underneath, come through and fix it in with two hinges. Fix a
hoop on the wall and put a small rope over it. Fix one end to the slab and put the other end through the slab (see
picture C). Pull the rope from the inside and the slab lifts up.
Figure 6.5 One child's plans for the tunnel from the school to the pool
The Channel tunnel 115
The water level was then checked again several hours after the
chalk had absorbed the water;
• finding out how rapidly powdered chalk settled in a mixture
of chalk and water.
These were all relevant because the means of getting the chalk
out was to grind it up, mix it with water and pump it out into
tanks where the slurry had to settle.
The historical study in week 6 provided the opportunity for the
children to appreciate that projects of this sort require carefully
worked out cooperation between the two countries (the 1877—81
tunnelling ceased because the French and English could not agree
about a completely different matter, i.e. who should govern Egypt).
They also require a lot of money (the 1975 boring had to stop
because at the time of the world oil crisis the British government
could not afford to carry on).
Week 6 was the week when snow happened to fall and the
teacher had the inspiration to take advantage of it, by asking the
class to collect it into a pile to try and make one cubic metre. In
fact the children never quite managed to collect enough, which
helped them to register just how much material there is in one
cubic metre and to begin to have some idea of the sheer volume of
spoil that was being dug out of the tunnel every day (1100 cubic
metres) and which had to be dealt with in one way or another.
Week 8 was the week of the visit to the exhibition. By this point
the children were armed with considerable information about,
and understanding of, the problems of tunnelling and hence were
likely to be able to make use of the technical explanations which
featured in the exhibition. They had done less on the effect of the
tunnel on travel times and routes, so at the start of the week the
teacher set an exercise of finding details of the current journey
from Manchester to Paris. This particular journey was selected so
that the children could compare it with the journey when the
tunnel was open, which featured in one of the displays at the
exhibition.
The journey from the school to Folkestone takes less than an
hour, so it was possible to spend three hours at the exhibition
and still fit the whole excursion into the normal hours of a school
day. The question sheets devised by the teacher covered the tech-
nical side (materials and construction of the tunnel linings, size
of the tunnel); differences in transport arrangements for cars,
The Channel tunnel 117
Dear Eurotunnel
Thank you for letting us visit the exhibition. I learnt a lot of
things to do with the tunnel especially the times of how to
go from Britain to France by the Eurotunnel. I also enjoyed
the game that was in the centre. I was a couple of minutes
out. I especially enjoyed the Marcus Mole quizzes above the
gift shop. Also I enjoyed the painted world where you had
to search for Marcus, in the world. I also enjoyed the film.
From
K
Summary
My narrative in this chapter has foliowed a chronological sequence.
I am, however, still aware of the jigsaw metaphor I used in Chap-
ter 3 to describe my finding out about other people's teaching:
fitting the bits together piece by piece until they all make sense
120 Creativity in primary science
and link together. In this chapter the metaphor fits also the chil-
dren's learning: they found a whole range of information relevant
to the topic and slowly pieced it together to make sense of the
news items periodically heard on radio or television. The children's
folders slowly accumulated a wealth of information and ideas:
the factual reports of the Channel tunnel construction; drawings
to explain the construction; their own plans for tunnelling from
the school to the swimming pool; letters; accounts of the geolo-
gical history of the area; ideas about how one might survey what
was under the sea and accounts of how it was actually done;
mathematical exercises on journey times; mapping exercises on
routes; designs and reports of the experimental work with chalk;
drawings to show the landscaping plans at the French and English
ends of the tunnel; descriptions of the environmental concerns
and solutions; and poems about being in a tunnel. The reader
will have to imagine the wealth of material which was in these
folders from the few brief extracts reproduced here and from the
descriptions of the activities in which the children were engaged.
Right at the end of the term the teacher asked the class to write
what they now knew about the topic and these accounts were also
in their folders.
There was also a wealth of poster resources accumulated which
had become, in the absence of books about the tunnel, the main
source of up-to-date information for the children's research.
For the teacher, perhaps the clearest feedback showing just how
much had been learnt came during the trip to France the follow-
ing term. This was very much a family trip with as many parents
as possible joining the group. When they visited the exhibition
the children could explain precisely what all the exhibits were
about, without having to read the labels (many of which were in
French). They were pleased with how much they knew and how
much they could explain to their parents (who were, of course,
impressed). They also visited the area of Sangatte, which was
originally a flat area, but which had been reshaped with the chalk
which had been dug out of the tunnel in water, and then used to
form hills. It made an interesting contrast to earlier studies they
had made about routes through Kent when they had learnt how
routes had had to follow the geography of an area. Now people
no longer needed to 'follow, a landscape' because they could alter
it quite dramatically. Again the children were able to explain the
whole process to their parents.
The Channel tunnel 121
The accounts of what the children had learnt, written at the end of
term, were at least three times the length of those written at the
start in January and some were much longer. They contained a
lot of detail, as in the following two accounts:
When the French started to build their tunnel they found out
that they would have to go through chalk and that chalk
absorbs water so at their end of the tunnel they put rubber
linings so that the water would not go into the tunnel. . the
.
soil they got out at the English end they poured down a cliff
and made a nature reserve with it. At the French end they
had chalk so they mixed it with water and then made hills.
The workmen use tunnel boring machines to build the tun-
nel and they follow a laser beam to make sure they're going
in the right direction. . . England and France used to be joined
under the sea, so now we will be joined again. . To start off
.
with the people have to dig a shaft. . The French have spe-
.
found in the oceans and are formed by bits of rock, and dead
plants or animals, I know such a lot that I'm going to need
another page!! I know about the history of the tunnel as well.
Albert Mathieu had an idea about a tunnel. His idea was a
horse-drawn transport throughout the tunnel. His idea also
included a tall chimney that raised above the sea so fresh
air could come in. Another person interested in tunnels was
Thome de Gamond. He said that he would have to make
geological surveys before you could build a tunnel and it
will take ages to write it so look through my topic folder for
more information. So, happy reading!
7
The river Thames
YEAR 4 (8 AND 9-YEAR-OLDS)
Introduction
The topic of the river Thames described in this chapter, like the
previous one on the Channel tunnel, came from the Aquatech
Project, in which attempts were made to explore the potential of
links between industry and schools in a modest way. The indus-
tries concerned had some connection with water technology, and
hence schools participating took a theme related to water as the
basis of these studies. In order to collect information, teachers were
asked to keep a log file of the teaching, in which they recorded
plans, successes, frustrations, resources used, and things learnt.
In addition a selection of the classroom products were displayed
in an exhibition also showing the outcome of the project. Both
the files and the exhibition panels afforded valuable information
about teaching; they became the initial source of data for the pre-
paration of Chapters 6 and 7 of this book. They were of course
supplemented by discussions with the teachers and by the teachers
commenting on the first drafts.
124 Creativity in primary science
Figure 7.2 shows her next planning chart. The layout and detail
of the original has been retained, but it has been typeset rather
than left in her handwriting, because of the need to reduce it for
publication. It is worth pausing here to identify features revealed
in this second plan.
Observational drawing and writing has been selected as the
starting activity, with the teacher being quite clear about the ideas
she would be developing. She planned a discussion of differences
and similarities (a point she emphasized in discussion with me)
between humans, fish and shellfish. Within that section, the first
of the key questions appears: 'What defines a fish?'
what lives in rivers? shape:
historical rafts
fishing E— fish
water creatures movement through development houseboat
________
water shape/function containers
Boats paddle boats
waterproofing
sailing boats
'TI
Dams
/
measuring speed of water how to stop water flowing
water currents changing sand, Lego, plastic locks
flooding Medway
water flow
drainage
'I,
reclaimed land
(Abbey Wood)
The teacher gave me three reasons for starting with the obser-
vational drawing. The first is that it starts from an object, rather
than something more abstract. The second is that all the class
can be working together and have a common experience, which
can then be used as a basis of discussion with the whole class.
(She knows that later, as the topic develops, children's activities
diverge.) The third is that close observational drawing provides
information about the object directly from the object with the
teacher giving little input. However, to take the learning further,
and to enable children to move from specific experiences to gen-
eralizations or unobservables, the teacher used a range of useful
questions: 'Mine has spots' 'Do they all have spots?' 'Mine has
clusters of round things' 'Could they be eggs?' (A useful follow-
up text to this conversation was Charles Reasoner's book Who's
Hatching?, 1995).
The teacher was clear at the planning stage about ideas that
she would bring out in the discussions of the drawings: 'breath-
ing', 'moving' and 'scales' were all marked on the plan. She was
also clear that she would make sure that some questions would
link observation to explanation, such as those about what the
parts of the fish enable it to do. This would lead to the study of
books and study of an actual fish (a goldfish) to find answers
(both of which activities are incorporated in the plan). The plan
also shows several activities which require the children to use the
information they are learning in a new way so that they show
that they understand it (see for instance on the top right-hand
corner of the plan, 'information work to back up observations',
'together [as a class] pick out the main words', 'to use in recon-
structing writing').
The links through to the later part of the topic were indicated by
the arrows. Questions about what fish live in the Thames, what
else lives in the Thames and where the Thames becomes salty
lead through to the study of the Thames. Note again a language
exercise which is built in, namely the writing of a letter to the
Thames Water Authority for information. The concepts of stream-
lining, driving mechanisms (moveable tails) and balance mechan-
isms (fins) are linked to the work on the movement of boats through
water, the two-way arrows indicating the expectation that the later
work will inform the earlier work and vice versa. The means of
learning has been written across the corner, i.e. 'construct and
find out'.
The river Thames 129
8 fLIDAY
I F'
EASTER HOLIDAY
End of
April!
early
May
I will leave the reader to track through the rest of the chart to
find further key questions, linking threads, and ideas for experi-
ences, along with new features which were not hinted at in the first
planning sheet (Figure 7.1) but which appear in the second one.
Figure 7.3 gives the time sequence of the teaching, indicating
the main blocks of time when the different sections of the topic
were tackled. The starting date for each indicates the point at which
a new resource and set of activities were put into the classroom
and introduced to the class as a whole. Week 9 required a major
reorganization of the classroom to accommodate the tank and the
river, and half-term provided a useful point for the transition.
Thinking carefully about the spatial organization of the class at
different points is crucial.
Having the time sequence drawn out in this way showed one
other important feature I might otherwise have missed, namely
the increase in the diversity of science activities occurring as the
topic progressed (obviously there were other non-science activities
going on). To start with there was only one science activity, then
this increased to two; after half-term there were four and finally
six, reducing after the Easter break to two as the topic came to a
natural end and new studies were started.
130 Creativity in primary science
Classroom products
After the children's first 'response to the fish', the teacher wrote:
'There are some fantastic drawings and paintings that can be
developed into batik. Written descriptions are less interesting
than conversation.. I need to develop observations through to
.
Th2
Pr-own haue
thQir is
cvrr1
cor 0-
bIoL&
eP
are (hi?.
curia1 uPc:
o.
Some things which seem obvious can take a long time and a lot
of experience for children to change in their thinking. It is also
important to help children think about relevant similarities (nei-
ther fish nor humans live on Mars but that is not very relevant).
When the teacher was talking about the fish 'breathing' by
taking in water, this presented even more of a puzzle than the
similarities discussion, because the process is so different from
breathing in humans, especially because if humans take in water
the one thing they cannot do is breathe.
The questions 'What defines a fish?' or 'If something is a fish,
what do you expect it to have, or do?' were at first challenging,
but slowly the children analysed and put into words the charac-
teristics of a fish. Their initial difficulties illustrate the problem
that everyone has of explaining a self-evident fact (children by
the age of 8 believe they know whether something is a fish or not,
without having to explain it, unless of course if someone asks them
if a shark is a fish!).
The improvement in the writing came not only from further
discussions, but by the teacher using a simple technique of chil-
dren reading their writing out to other children. 'There was huge
value in the class reading back their writing to the whole class';
she explained that they seemed to hear their own mistakes, recog-
nize that they had left things out and then go and improve it by
themselves. The activity did not always involve the whole class;
it could be done by small groups 'conferencing'.
Use of information books was not as simple as it sounds, because
she found that most of them were 'completely inappropriate for
the age group'; they were either patronizing and simplistic or too
technical. The Eye-Witness series had excellent photographs but
difficult text that required mediation by the teacher. She used this
to model how to cope with such a text, by identifying the diffi-
cult words and discussing their meaning, or looking them up in
a dictionary.
A letter was written to the Thames Water Authority at about
this point in the topic to find out about fish in the Thames. What
needed to go into the letter was discussed with the whole class,
but only two children put letters together and only one was sent
(for the obvious reason that the Authority would hardly appre-
ciate having letters from every child in the class!).
It is often later that a teacher gets evidence of what has been
learnt and assimilated. This teacher became aware that the children
136 Creativity in primary science
had understood about the action of the tail of the fish in pro-
pelling it forward and the significance of the streamlined shape
from an incident which occurred two or three months later in the
summer term. 'When the class were studying tadpoles, they quickly
focused their attention, without prompting, on how the tadpole
swam, how it moved its tail and on its streamlined shape. Other
features were not so quickly noticed.' It is also a useful reminder
of the importance of previous experience in aiding observation.
Map studies
The map studies started after half-term with children trying to
use maps to answer the following questions:
Use the different maps and atlases to find the river Thames.
Follow its course and see where it goes.
Can you see where it started?
Can you see where it ends?
Where does it go? What places does it pass?
What bridges and tunnels can you see? (Look around London)
As the ability to read maps and take information from them
increased, then the maps became more sophisticated. They were
a major prop in the role play area as they were consulted on the
various 'journeys' taken in the classroom yacht. Navigation maps
showing the sandbanks in the Thames estuary were particularly
popular, and children came to realize the importance of under-
standing the detail on these if you are trying to navigate a safe
passage up or down the Thames from or into open sea.
pen if. . ?'). The findings were drawn together in class discussion
.
Paddle boats
The making of the paddle boats was a highly disciplined activity
The paddle boats were made eventually from corroflute (rigid
enough to withstand the forces of the wound-up rubber band, suf-
ficiently non-resistant for young children to be able to cut it safely,
waterproof so that it can be put in water, of sufficiently low dens-
ity that it will float), although other materials were offered — one
child did try cardboard first.
To get this activity started, the teacher showed the children the
mechanism of the paddle and a simple shape. She discussed with
them what they could do differently. They came up with the pos-
sibility of having the paddle in a different place, using different
material and altering the shape of the boat.
The making of the boats occurred over a period of four weeks.
The making, as could be seen by the children's drawings and
written accounts, involved several stages: the drawing of the plans
(based on a design given by the teacher) on squared paper; trans-
ference of the plan to the corroflute, by means of pricking a pin
through the paper; cutting of corroflute with a Stanley knife to
make the main body of the boat, and then cutting out the paddle;
attachment of the paddle by means of a rubber band. Finally the
boats were tested and accounts written of the whole process. It
140 Creativity in primary science
20 85 75
30 99 99
to make a boat. I made it with an old spa water bothe. I thought that it
I -
made it with wood it might not float because some wood doesn't float well. I first
looked at a water book and saw a bottle boat. The one I made was nothing like the
boats in the book but it gave me ideas. I got the bottle and sawed a hole in the top for
the engine. It was moving around so I got a vice which is a holder you can put wood
in to hold it still. I had a problem cutting out a hole for the motor because the tools
were too big, so I got a small hand saw and cut out a hole of the correct size. The
wiring was a piece of cake! But it didn't work. So I changed around the wires and it
worked. Next I put the battery higher up on a piece of plastic and the propellor
worked but quite slowly. Then I asked Miss Klass if I could test the boat on the pond.
It went backwards! I thought it was the propellor which was the wrong way round but
it wasn't. So I changed the wires round.
Role play
The role play area became an important feature of the children's
learning. Role play corners are a common feature of infant class-
rooms but less common in the junior classes. It was established ini-
tially in several drama sessions where children role played being
in a small yacht in different conditions. They had to sail away
from the mainland and act out some of the adventures that they
had at sea in which they had deep water, rough water, islands,
shipwrecks and pirate attacks to cope with. They also wrote post-
cards to people from the ship to give accounts of their voyages
and adventures. Thereafter children were allowed to use the role
play area during the day as they wished, to develop their own
stories. The teacher found that the children became completely
involved; and as they learnt more and more about boats, rivers,
and water flow, so she found their new knowledge and vocabul-
ary was built into their play. Somehow the role play experience
allowed them to browse through their new vocabulary. The boat
slowly acquired life jackets, a fishing rod for catching food, maps
(especially the navigation maps), and snorkel and flippers for fish-
ing just below the surface. It had a mast, with sails which could
be hoisted, and a rudder.
The river Thames 143
Sail boats
The sail boats were a second 'construct and find out' activity,
similar to the paddle boats, with the teacher hoping that children
would be able to compare the performance of boats with differ-
ent sails. The design was drawn initially on squared paper and,
as before, was transferred to the actual construction. In terms of
construction and design, a lot of new skills were taught or old
ones consolidated, using different materials and tools (saws, drills,
knives, etc.). The children were increasingly aware of the import-
ance of careful measurement, and were more independent in
their organization.
There were some frustrations in the organization of the testing
of the boats. The battery-powered fans were completely unreliable.
In the end a hairdryer was used to create a wind, but children
found it difficult to compare results and get any satisfactory data.
In any case the teacher found the children keener to construct a
boat than to plan for differences and comparison.
children choosing with whom they worked. The teacher kept con-
trol of what the children did by each day indicating on a diagram
on the board what activity each child would start on, where they
would go next and then the point at which they could have some
choice over what they did. The diagram was in the form of a
double wheel. The inner wheel had on it the core activities; the
children had to move round it and complete everything before
they moved to the outer wheel. The outer wheel had extra activ-
ities and promoted considerable independence. Children's names
were put against different activities to indicate where they should
start for the day, and they knew that this determined a large part
of their daily activities.
Most of the decisions about who should be doing what were
determined by the ongoing assessments she was making and the
need to ensure that children tackled all areas of learning; some
decisions were also determined by the resource constraints, for
instance she could only have a small group working at the river
at any one time, or making and testing boats.
Despite this organization to allow children to work at their
own pace, and hence often to work on their own, she neverthe-
less brought the whole class together on several occasions during
the week for the review of learning, for tackling new work, for
administration and for story reading.
I have built the teacher's evaluations of each section into the early
part of the chapter, but reproduce here her reflections on the whole
topic in which she raises issues which have been identified by
other teachers in the other chapters.
It was a successful topic but raised several questions/issues
for me:
1 Often what the children learn/benefit most from is not
reflected in the volume of written work, or detail. Much of
the value of working with the river model was the phys-
ical tactile experience of experimentation with water in a
new way. Children spent hours trying different things and
learnt a lot but what they learnt is summed up in a few
sentences of shared writing.
The river Thames 145
Introduction
in the reception class in the previous school year. English was not
the first language for over 50 per cent of the class and the teacher
could not speak any of their home languages. Development of
language, and particularly of vocabulary, was therefore an import-
ant part of her day-to-day work, seeking to improve children's con-
fidence to express their ideas both orally and in writing in what
for many of them was their second language. She made a point
of listening carefully to make sure that as best she could she was
hearing the ideas some of the children were trying to express
behind their limited knowledge of English.
The presence of a language support teacher for a high pro-
portion of the week also allowed the teacher to work with small
groups in considerable depth, initiating (and recording) the con-
versations and interventions which were essential to support the
children's thinking. In her teaching generally she tried to main-
tain the appropriate balance between the need to direct and steer
children's thinking while providing children with opportunities
to test and sort out their own ideas. With this class, with so many
children still only 5 years old, she had a clear picture of the need
for structure:
The topic of Sight was prescribed in the school topic plans. It was
part of a whole-school topic on Light and Darkness in which dif-
ferent classes focused on different aspects. The teacher had some
autonomy over how she tackled the topic and what she attempted
to achieve. She planned initially to cover five main areas in the
following order:
rnYey
brown blue.
mrs Da,
C
co
I'
b i" a
To
brown eyes was easily noticed, and the children came up with a
hypothesis that eye colour might be linked to ethnicity. They sub-
sequently collected data from the other two Year 1 classes, and iden-
tified that on the whole their hypothesis was reasonable.
I
Seeing and light 153
category and then counting up all the strokes at the end. Inter-
pretation was considered by the whole class through discussion
with the teacher. The boy who made the original hypothesis was
delighted to find that his hypothesis was borne out by the addi-
tional data.
The teacher's role during the original drawing exercise required
her merely to watch and identify any problems and quietly inter-
vene: 'If a child had any difficulties I intervened by drawing their
attention back to the real thing, encouraging them to correct their
own mistakes.' The commonest difficulty in this particular exer-
cise is that children tend to draw their mental image of an eye,
without checking it against the real thing; hence the iris is often
drawn as a small circle with lots of white round it, when in fact
it is rare that you can see white above and below the iris. The
teacher had to do little prompting to start children comparing their
eyes. 'Though working individually the children soon became
aware of similarities and differences through chatting with friends
and comparing pictures.'
The model eye provided several language exercises. One in-
volved learning the names of the parts of the eye and talking a
little about what the parts did; a second involved spotting lan-
guage links in many of the words, e.g. 'eyelid', 'eyebrow' and 'eye-
lash' all contain the word 'eye'. A third was a conversation about
one set of eyelashes which happened to have been stuck on the
wrong way round; the teacher asked the children to account for
why this was wrong. One child's response was 'because you would
blink, and blink and blink', indicating quite clearly that he under-
stood that this would cause a lot of irritation to the eye. The teacher
wrote in reflecting on this episode:
tried out by all the children in the class, and those planned by a
small group of children who were particularly keen to extend their
investigations further. Those planned by the teacher required the
children to find out if, with their eyes closed, they could:
• cut out a circle properly;
• draw faces better than with their eyes open;
• follow a straight line of masking tape on the hall floor;
• tell shape and colour.
Figure 8.4 Results of 'Can we cut out a circle as well with our eyes
shut as with them open?'
by their name. They went away and completed the test with ease.
Because of the policy of not including individual children's names
in the book, the results cannot be included, but they met with more
success than in drawing the faces with their eyes shut; the writ-
ing was not as good, however, as with their eyes open.
The children who planned their own investigations were effect-
ively self-selected. They had shown great interest in the work and
taken considerable initiative. They were not, in conventional terms,
the brightest or most intelligent — there were others who were
more skilled at reading and writing — but they were incredibly
enthusiastic about the experimental work. L and M were of course
in this group. The teacher put them in pairs (B and L, S and N,
D and M, A and C) and asked them to devise more tests that they
could try out on themselves and other people.
B and L decided to find out if people could do a jigsaw with
their eyes closed. They started with a plastic number jigsaw, in
which numbers had to be slotted into appropriate slots on a board.
Seeing and light 159
L
- 2345
LI IL.3 f Tél
L ' ! ''SI
L 125b
Figure 8.6 Gauges used for measuring eye size (with results
marked)
Figure 8.7 Teaching sequence for work on relating the size of the
pupil of the eye to the available light
H: It shines.
B: When we see light it gets in our eyes.
P: . Because our eyes are open.
. .
Another group start off on the fact that you can go blind if light
gets into your eyes (the teacher had previously warned the chil-
dren not to look at the sun when they were trying the pupil
investigation) and she has to redirect their attention to the ori-
ginal question she asked. Later she presses the children for further
explanation when she says 'How did we catch it?':
R: Because you get blind.
B: When light comes in your eyes, you go to hospital.
Teacher: But when you just want to see something...
B: The light comes out of the eye.
A: The sun is out and the lights are on. When you turn
the light on it comes in our eyes, when you turn it
off, it doesn't.
M: Electricity comes into the light and it gets on. We
catch some light.
Teacher: How do we catch it?
M: By our eye.
The teacher posed the question 'Do our pupils look the same all
the time?' to one group of children prior to their watching the
pupils of her eyes during the demonstration. One child shows
quite clearly that he had noticed the phenomenon before, or at
least had read about it in the book which was available in the
classroom:
N: Yes.
A: Sometimes big, sometimes, small. Near the window they
go smaller near the sun.
The question Why do you think they go smaller and bigger?'
provoked interesting answers, including a sense of wonderment
in the use of the word 'magic'. I suppose on reflection it is like
magic: it happens all by itself, you do not appear to do anything
and you cannot feel it happening! From another group it elicited
the comment that the child was making a connection with a pre-
vious experience of watching her baby sister's eyes:
V: It's magic.
B: Because the light comes in, it goes smaller. . it's magic.
.
166 Creativity in primary science
A: The little bit of thing under your eye — when it's dark,
dark gets into your eyes and gets it bigger. Light gets right
into your eyes and it gets small.
N: When my baby sister opens her eyes, the pupils go smaller.
M: If we were in the cupboard and it was dark we couldn't
read.
K: More light gets in our eyes when it's bigger.
B: It scares someone in the dark.
properly.' When she was asked why you can't see them in the
morning, she explained 'because it's too bright'. It seemed to the
teacher that C was relating back to a previous conversation about
the fact that fireworks can be seen better at night than during the
day, because 'it's too bright in the morning'.
A conversation with D about his drawing shows the difficulty
of children distinguishing a source of light from something that
is bright:
Teacher: What have you drawn coming from the room light?
That's the light. . it sort of. . . comes off in lines.
.
stops.
Teacher: Does a cloud give us light?
D: Yes, they are very bright.
'This was unexpected and is probably because D is linking 'light'
and 'bright' — not entirely mistakenly so. My only idea was to
call his attention to clouds on a dark gloomy or rainy day when
the sun was hidden and compare that to the bright sunny day
when the clouds appeared white.'
Conversations with the children towards the end revealed a
growing awareness of the importance of eyes for seeing and that
somehow light was connected with seeing. I shall end the chapter
with the teacher's own summary from her original account.
This last chapter is devoted to stepping back from the busy class-
rooms to review the events that occurred within them, in light of
the discussions in Chapters 1 and 2.
Understanding explanation
There are many instances where the teachers asked children to make
links between pieces of information, and in doing so were hunting
for explanations, while at the same time introducing new concepts.
The concept of the 'function' of parts of a system appeared in sev-
eral of the studies: the function of the fins and tail of the fish for
movement; the function of the paddle in making the boat move;
the function of joints in enabling the body to have moving parts.
Hunting for explanations and links was triggered by ques-
tions asked by the teacher such as: What does this do? How does
it work? What is it for? How is it made? Why is it like that? The
chapters are full of questions or discussions that require links to
be made, explanations to be sought. Why do you think people
found it easier to distinguish the sound of the shells than any
From action to reflection 173
other sound? Why did the French have to have rubber tunnel
linings? Why did they build the tunnel where they did? What
are the factors that make the container sink? What are the factors
that give a boat a large carrying capacity? Why do you think
there are so many earthworms by the car park? Why did we see
so many frogs in Epping Forest on our trip out? What are the
ways in which rabies can be spread? Why can't we see when
we turn the light out? Why can't we do a jigsaw when we shut
our eyes? How does a compass work? How do you find your
way underground?
Work was planned so that opportunities for making explana-
tions were provided. In the paddle boat, the teacher deliberately
gave precise instructions, with minimal choice over design, so that
everyone would have a successful boat and hence have a chance
of explaining how the boat worked. It would be easy for a teacher
to make the construction of the boat take up so much time that
there was no time for explaining.
Asking questions
The children in the case studies asked quite a lot of scientific ques-
tions. The Year 2 class in Chapter 5 learnt how to ask questions
From action to reflection 175
that children will automatically ask questions about all the things
teachers want them to learn. The very deliberate strategies used
by the teachers in the case studies to enhance the children's abil-
ity to ask well-focused questions and to know how to go about
answering them should at least give pause for thought, if not
challenge the notion entirely.
It would be a mistake to leave this section without acknowledg-
ing the role teachers' own knowledge plays in their willingness
to entertain questioning. Research shows that teachers become
more and more didactic, adopting a 'transmission' mode of teach-
ing, the less they know about a subject. Does the willingness of
the teachers in the six case studies to encourage and foster the
asking of questions represent in fact a considerable knowledge of
science?
Understanding experiments
Interpreting data
Research from APU, from the studies in Durham and from the
PACKS project shows that without help many learners pay little
attention to evidence or perhaps need far more help than is often
recognized in being able to make sense of it.
The case studies show several strategies teachers used to help
children value data and to go back to the evidence they had in
front of them. There is the Year 1 group in Chapter 8 having to
face up to the evidence that they could not cut out circles with
their eyes shut, even though they believed strongly beforehand
that they could. Part of this teacher's success came from the fact
that the evidence could easily be recorded so that children had
something tangible to go back to and talk about. She had sim-
ilarly found two simple ways to display the eye colour of all the
children, so that it was possible to see the proportion of different
colour eyes in the class.
The teacher of the Year 4 class in Chapter 7, in the river Thames
topic, insisted that the children kept going back to the object or
phenomenon under observation and drawing and writing about
what they saw, not what they knew. Her insistence on this valuing
of evidence improved the quality of observation both in the cases
of the fish and prawn and the later work on the paddle boat and
water flow in the channel.
The teacher of the Years 1 and 2 class who helped the children
examine the data from Rabbit's House used a well-tried tech-
nique for looking at data, i.e. she asked the children merely to
describe it first. Often interpretation starts too early, before peo-
ple have had a good look at what is there. Just as the teacher
in Chapter 6 asked the children to look at the structure and the
movement of fish, before she linked structure and movement, so
having a good look at the data should precede interpretation and
explanation. The questions used to help children describe the data
in Rabbit's House were: 'Which one has more ticks than crosses?'
178 Creativity in primary science
'Which has more crosses than ticks?' 'Where are they the same?'
'What else do you notice?' 'Which ones did children try the most?'
She often followed her questions with another, 'How do you
know?', requiring more detailed description of the evidence. She
used comparison, 'Did everyone find that?', and made the chil-
dren search the other recording sheets to see if there were similar
patterns there.
Only when the data had been described did she seek explana-
tions: 'Why do you think children tried that one the most?' 'Which
was the most difficult to identify?' 'How do you know?' 'Why
do you think that was so?' 'Did everyone find that?' 'Which was
the easiest?' The conclusions drawn were tentative: 'Christopher
thought...'
The children in the Year 2 class trying to answer the ques-
tions from their ID cards had been taught a simple technique for
exploring data: they put the information in numerical order, not
by rearranging it physically on the table, but by marking the
order with 1, 2, 3, 4, etc. against the measurements.
Valid conclusions can of course only be drawn from evid-
ence if the evidence itself is valid. Poor design of investigations
generates useless evidence; if children are to value the evidence,
they must value the design. The evidence can also be spoiled if
the necessary technical know-how and practical skills are not there
to execute the investigation well.
Creativity
See references to Driver et al. (1985, 1994) and Osborne and Freyberg
(1985) above for extensive bibliographies not only to the CLIS Project
at Leeds but to related research worldwide. Most of the CLIS titles refer
to the secondary age range, but the techniques have been applied to the
primary age range and influenced particularly the way in which the
SPACE project was carried out.
Contents
Pre-school science experiences — The importance of exploration in the develop-
ment of early years science knowledge and skills — Seeking creativity in science
activities — Developing positive attitudes in science — Developing the teacher's
role — References — Index.
• What is differentiation?
• How can it be put into practice in primary science lessons?
• Is it really possible to provide differentiated learning activities for a
class of thirty or more primary children?
Taking a refreshingly pragmatic approach, Anne Qualter addresses these
challenging issues in this book. Drawing on her own experience as a
classroom teacher combined with research undertaken in a wide range
of primary classrooms, the author shows that genuinely differentiated
primary science is achievable. Using several classroom examples, she
argues that differentiation is not simply about categorizing children as
more or less able but involves the teacher in complex decisions which
take account of the child's understanding and capabilities, their cultural
background, gender, linguistic capabilities, interests and a variety of
other factors.
The book will be invaluable reading for both trainee and practising
primary school teachers.
Contents
The place of differentiation in primary science — What do we mean by ability
in science? — Finding starting points — Factors influencing children's science
— Developing models for differentiated learning — The process of differentiation
— Bibliography — Index.
and promoting appropriate behaviour — Time for teaching and learning — De-
ploying adult help effectively in the classroom: delegation and responsibility —
Evaluating classroom organization and management — Conclusion: the primary
classroom, a place and a time — References — Index.