Motivation and Cultural Distance in Second-Language Acquisition
Motivation and Cultural Distance in Second-Language Acquisition
in Second-Language Acquisition
Bjorg Svanes
University of Bergen
Previous studies have found that motivation, and in particular integrative motiva-
tion, is an essential factor in the acquisition of a second language. In the present
study of the acquisition of Norwegian by foreign students at the University of
Bergen, Norway, a questionnaire assessing motivation was presented to 167 foreign
students enrolled in classes of “Norwegian for foreign students”, a short time before
the examination in Norwegian, Level 2. European and American ctudents were
found to be more inregratively motivated than the Middle Eastern, African and
Asian students, who were found to be more instrumentally motivated than the
Western students. Moreover, a significant difference in means of grades was
recorded; Europeans having the best and Asian students the poorest grades. In the
total group a weak positive correlation between integrative motivation and language
proficiency, and a negative correlation between instrumental motivation and grades
were found. However, when motivation and grades were analyzed within each
group, no positive correlation between grades and integrative motivation was found.
When ‘cultural distance’ was entered into a multiple regression analysis, in addition
to the two motivation variables, the results indicated that the motivation variables
explained very little of the variance in language proficiency. The best predictor of
variance in groups of students with various language and cultural backgrounds was
‘cultural distance’.
INTRODUCTION
341
342 Language Learning Vol. 37, No. 3
METHOD
SUBJECTS
VARIABLES
Table 1
Distribution of Subjects
Europe Middle East
Austria 1 Iran 3
England 3 Lebanon 1
Finland 2 Palertine 4
France 2 Turkey 15
-
Germany 5 23
Itall 2 Africa
Jugoslavia Cameroun I
Poland Ghana 5
Portugal Morocco 4
Spain Tanzania 1
Switzerland 2
- Tunisia 2
-
25 13
N o r t h America Am
Canada 4 Bangladesh 12
USA 31
- llidia I2
35 Paki\tan 15
Sri Lanka 24
Vietnam 8
-
71
DATA TREATMENT
For the analysis of the data, the SPSS (Statistical Package for the
Social Sciences) is used, especially frequencies, factor analysis, reliabil-
ity, breakdown with analysis of variance, pearson correlation and new
regression analysis.
RESULTS
Table 2
Distribution of Subjects According to Sex
Men Women Total
Europe 16 9 25
North America 15 18 33
Middle East, Africa 32 4 36
Asia -57 14
- -71
Total I23 47 165
NB: T w o American5 habe no1 reported $ex.
Table 3
Mean Scores on 20 Motivation Variables
P- Values of Analysis of Variance
North M-East
Europe Amer. Africa Asia Prob-
n=25 n=35 n=36 n=71 ability
Reasons f o r Coming lo Norway:
* I . Seeing Norway, the Fcenery 3.28 3.54 2.33 2.46 0.0000
* 2. Getting to know Norwegians 3.08 3.63 2.68 2.51 O.oo00
* * 3. Getting a degree 2.71 2.32 4.42 4.24 0.0000
* 4. Finding out how people live 3 .on 3.46 2.28 2.25 0.0000
* * 5. Studying in Norway 2.83 2.94 4.24 4.06 0.0000
** 6 . Getting training in my field 2.40 2.26 3.27 3.06 0.0137
* 7. Having a chance to live in 2.20 3.94 2.00 2.03 0.0000
another country
* 8. Finding out how students live I .67 2.45 I .76 2.26 0.0056
9. Joining family member5 2.39 I .n2 1.23 1.38 0.0005
* 10. Having new experiences 3.52 4.06 2.94 2.95 0.0000
* 1 1 . Meeting different kinds of 2.71 3.57 2.56 2.74 0.0005
people
12. Fleeing from my country 1S O 1.06 1 .97 1.74 0.0051
the two other groups on the integrative motives; Seeing Norway, the
scenery, Getting to know Norwegians, Finding out how people live,
Having new experiences, Interest in Norwegian culture and Interest in
Norwegian language, whereas the Asian and Middle EasterdAfrican
students scored highest on the instrumental motives; Getting a degree,
Studying in Norway, In order to be able to study at the university, To get
a good j o b in home country and To get an education in order to serve my
350 Language Learning Vol. 37, No. 3
Table 4
Factor Loadings f o r 20 Motivation Variables
Factors
Motivation Variables 1
Reasons f o r Coming to Norway:
* 1 . Seeing Norway, the scenery 0.73 -0.140 -0.13
* 2. Getting to know Norwegians 0.80 -0.070 -0.06
** 3. Getting a degree -0.06 0.800 -0.21
* 4. Finding out horn people live 0.81 -0.200 0.04
** 5. Studying in Norway 0.07 0.740 -0.27
** 6. Getting training in my field 0.12 0.470 -0.04
* 7. Having a chance to live in 0.70 -0. I90 0.03
another country
* 8. Finding out how students li\c 0.58 0.160 0.04
9. Joining family members 0.03 -0.260 0.26
* 10. Having new experiences 0.67 0.020 -0. I4
* 11. Meeting different kinds of 0.70 0.090 -0.05
people
12. Fleeing from my country -0.28 0.060 0.45
gratively motivated than the other students, while the students from
Middle East/Africa and from Asia were significantly higher instrumen-
tally motivated than the Western students. There were significant differ-
ences in means of proficiency in Norwegian; European and American
students having the best grades, the students from Middle EastIAfrica
third best and Asians, the poorest.
Table 5 shows that in the groups with the best grades integrative moti-
vation is high and instrumental motivation low, and in the groups with
352 Language Learning Vol. 37, No. 3
Table 5
Meun Scores with Standard Deviations on Grades, Integrative Motivation
and Instrumental Motivation
P- Values of Analysis of Variance
Grades Integrative Motivation Instrumental Motivation
Standard Standard Standard
Mean Deviation Number Mean Deviation Number Mean Deviation Number
Europe 2.18 0.74 25 3.01 0.64 25 2.47 0.86 25
Grades: Best = I
Poorebi = 4
%lotibation: M a u n i u m = 5
Minimum = 1
Table 6
Intercorrelations bet ween Grades, Integrative Motivation,
Instrumental Motivation and ‘Cultural Distance’
Integrative Instrumental
Grades Motivation Motivation
Integrative -0. I6*
Motivation
Intrumental 0.21** -0.03
‘Cultural 0.48** -0.33* * 0.58* *
Distance’
*p < .05 **p < .01
(Group m e m b m h i p : Europe= I . North America = 2, Middle Eas[/Africa= 3, Aria = 4 )
(Grades: best = 1, poorest = 4)
(Mothation: may = 5 , min I )7
Table 7
Correlations bet ween Grades, integrative Moiivation and insirumental Motivation in
the Four Groups of Studenis
Grades Integr. Motivation
Europe Integrative -0.09
( N = 25) Motivation
Instrumental 0.11 -0.08
Motivation
Tuble 8
Cumulative Amount of Vuriance IR’) in Language Proficiency Expluined by Integrutive
Motivation. Incti~irnenralMotivution and ‘Cultural Distance’, Entered Hierarchically in
Three Different Regression Models
R? F R2A FA
Predictor Variables in
Three Different Models
I . Integrative Motivation 0.02 2.71
11. Integrative and 0.09 7.84** 0.08 12.76**
Instrumental Motivation
111. Integrative and 0.23 14.79** 0.13 26.11**
Instrumental Motivation
and ‘Cultural Distance’
0 T
Predictor?
( I ) Integrative Motivation 0.019 0.25
(2) Instrumental Motivation 0.017 0.19
(3) ‘Cultural Distance’ 0.471 5.11**
* * p < .O1
Table 9
Sex Differences in Grades, Integrative Motivation and Instrutnenral Motivation. Mean
Scores with Standard Deviations
Middle East/
Europe N . America Africa Asia
Standard Standard Standard Standard
Mean Deviation Mean Deviation Mean Deviation Mean Deviation F
lntegrative Motivation
Men 3.11 0.65 3.38 0.68 2.54 0.78 2.61 0.70 6.66**
Women 2.84 0.62 3.71 0.69 2.23 1.03 2.47 0.85 9.31**
F 0.971 2.05 0.512 0.409
Instrumental Motivation
Men 2.45 0.90 2.29 0.81 3.82 0.70 4.03 0.64 36.68**
Women 2.50 0.85 2.49 0.81 4.29 0.77 3.10 1.33 4.28*
F 0.022 0.533 1.55 13.24**
Grades
Men 2.00 0.54 2.14 0.61 2.96 0.64 3.25 0.80 19.02**
Women 2.49 0.96 2.39 0.68 2.43 0.38 2.83 0.85 0.91
F 2.71 1.22 2.58 3.01
* p < .05
* * p < .O1
(Grades: best = 1, poorest = 4)
(Morivation: m a x = 5 , m i n = I )
The ages of the students range from 19 to 45. Two-fifths of the stu-
dents are over 26 years of age. In the European group and in the Asian
group about half of the students are over 26. There is a significant
correlation between age and proficiency (r = 0.28**), so that higher age
is related to higher grades-poorer grades. Studies of the age factor in
second-language acquisition (SLA) have mostly dealt with differences
between child and adult second-language acquisition (Krashen 1979,
Snow and Hoefnagel-Hoehle 1978). However, the Heidelberg Research
Project (1975) of the acquisition of German by adult immigrants, shows
that increasing age correlates with lesser command of the second lan-
guage. Our result supports this finding.
DISCUSSION
another Indian language than for studying the language of the host
country for their professional education.
The results indicating that students with the best grades also score
highest on the integrative motivation variable and lowest on the instru-
mental motivation variable, may at first sight appear to be in agreement
with Gardner’s findings. The correlations between grades and motives,
however, were not strong. The rather strong correlation between grades
and ‘cultural distance’ could under no circumstances be explained by the
differences in motivation found to exist between the groups. Summing
up these findings, it could be concluded that whereas ‘cultural distance’
seems to be decisive for language learning in the present study, motiva-
tional factors seem to be of much less importance as a predictor of
language proficiency. The correlation analysis of grades and the motiva-
tional variables within each group support this finding. There is no posi-
tive correlation between integrative motivation and grades in any of the
groups. On the contrary, there is a negative relationship between grades
and integrative motivation in the American group, a result that seems to
be in agreement with Oller et al.’s (1977) findings. All the motives
included in the integrative motivation variable in the present study are
perhaps not favorable for hard language study. Thus, integrative motiva-
tion, as assessed in the present study, cannot explain the variance in
second-language proficiency either within the groups or between the
groups.
The results of the present study do not support Gardner’s findings
concerning the importance of the integrative motivation. The disagree-
ment may be due to a difference in methods used to assess motivation;
Gardner using a more comprehensive test battery than the present study.
However, differences of groups being studied is another important rea-
son for the discrepancy. Because Gardner’s subjects (Gardner and Lam-
bert 1972, Gardner and Smythe 1981) were high school students, there
was probably great variation in motivation. In the present study, on the
other hand, there was great variation in the linguistic and cultural back-
ground of the students and little variation in integrative or instrumental
motivation. All the students were motivated, integratively or instrumen-
tally. This may point to the conclusion that although motivation is an
important factor in the acquisition of a second language, type of motiva-
tion is of less importance in groups of adult university students, who are
all well motivated. In this context ‘cultural distance’ has been found to be
the best predictor of second-language proficiency.
358 Language Learning Vol. 37, No. 3
REFERENCES
Gadamer. H . G . 1960. Wahrheit und Methode (Truth and Method). Tubingen, Federal
Republic of Gerniany: J . C. ti. Mohr.
Gardner R. C . 1979. Social psychological aspects of second language acquisition. In Lun-
guage and Sociul Psychology, eds. H . Giles and R . St. Clair, Oxford, United Kingdom:
Basil Blackwell.
Gardner, R . C. 1980. O n the validiry of affectite variables in 5ecorid language acquisition:
conceptual, conte\tual and \rat istical con5iderations. Lunguuge Learning 30:255-270.
Gardner, R . C . 1983. Learning another language: a true social psychological experiment.
Journul qf Langiruge und Social Ps.vcho1og.v 2:219-239.
Gardner, R . C. and W. E. Lambert. 1972. Attitudes and Motivation in Semnd Lunguuge
Leurnrng. Rowley, Massachusetts: Newbury House.
Gardner. R . C. and P. C. Smythe. 1981. O n the development of the attitude/motivation
test battery. Cunudiun Modern Lunguage Review 371510-525.
Gilcs, H. and I . 1.Byrne. 1982. An intergroup approach io wcond language acquisition.
Mulrilinguul und Mirltrculrural Developnrent 3: 17-40.
Heidelberg Research Project. 1975. In Second Lunguuge Acqirivirion: The Siure ofthe A r t ,
eds. H . Nicholas and J . 11. Meisel, Tubingen, Federal Republic of Germany: Language
Development 5 ( 1983).
Krashen, S. 1979. Age, rate and eventual attainment in \econd language acquisition.
TESOL Quurter1.v 13:573-582.
Laine, E. 1984. Variations in FL Learning Motivation: Some Theoretical Consideration\.
Paper presented at AILA, Brussels, Belgium.
Latnbert. W. E . 1974. Culture and language and factor\ in learning and education. I n
Culturul Factor5 in Learning und Education, eds. F. E. Aboud and R. D. Meade.
Bellingharn, Washington: Fifth Western Washington Sympo5ium on Learning.
Oller, J . W.,1. Baca and F. Vigil. 1977. Attitudes and attained proficiency in ESL: a
\ociolinguistic study of Mexican Americans in the southweit. TESOL Quurterlv
11:175-183.
Schumann, .I. H . 1976. Social distance a \ ii factor in yecond language acquisition. Lull-
,yiru,qe Learnin,? 26: 135- 143.
Schumann, J . H. 1978. The accultural model tor second language acquisition. In Second
Lunguuge Acquisition und Foreign Lunguuge Teaching, ed. R . C . Gringas, Washington,
D.C.: Center for Applied Linguistics.
Snow, C . E. and M. Hoefnagel-Hoehle. 1978. The critical period for language acquisition:
evidence from zecond language learning. Child Development 49: I 1 14-1 129.
Spol\ky. B. 1969. Attitudinal a5pects of second language learning. Langiruge Learning
19:270-285.
SPSS: Slalistrcal Package f o r t h e Sociul Science5. 1975. Neu York, NeN York: McGra\r-
Hill.
SPSS: Update 7-9. 1981. New York, Neu Yorh: McCiraw-Hill.