Research On Negotiation: What Does Reveal About Second-Language Learning Conditions, Processes, and Outcomes?
Research On Negotiation: What Does Reveal About Second-Language Learning Conditions, Processes, and Outcomes?
493-527
Review Article
Research on Negotiation: What Does It
Reveal About Second-Language Learning
Conditions, Processes, and Outcomes?
Teresa Pica
University of Pennsylvania
This article was written while the author was Ethel G. Carruth Associate
Professor of Education. A version was presented at the First Annual Pacific
Second Language Research Forum (PacSLRF), 14 July 1992, University of
Sydney, Australia. I acknowledge the many contributionsof my colleagues at
the University of Pennsyjvania, some of whom are now a t other universities,
who have helped me with collection and analysis of the data cited in this
paper.
Requests for reprints may be sent to the author a t Graduate School of
Education, University of Pennsylvania, 3700 Walnut Street, Philadelphia,
Pennsylvania 19104-6216. E-mail: billb8nwfs.gse.ubnn.edu
493
494 Language Learning Vol. 44, No. 3
Learner-Oriented Conditions
k
plays in bringing about compreh nsion, an important factor in
language learning. Indirect evide ce has come from studies such
as those by Chaudron (198333)and Long (1985a). Here, English L2
learners could better comprehend and recall ledurette content
when the words and sentences in the original versions of the
lecturettes were modified in ways that negotiation has been shown
t o generate. These modifications included repetitions-at the
word, phrase, and sentence level, the use of paraphrase, and the
insertion of conjunctions and enumerators t o mark relationships
of time and space.
Evidence of a .more direct relationship between negotiation
and comprehension has come from Pica et al. (1987a), who inves-
tigated the listening comprehension of 12 low-intermediate,
preacademic learners as they followed 30 different directions to a
picture assembly task under two input conditions. In one condi-
i
tion, the researchers first modified the direction uttera ces in
ways similar t o those used in the lecturette research described
above. Three femde NSs, who,were graduate students and
speakers of standard American English, then presented them
orally t o individual learners. Among the modifications in these
directions were reductions in syntactic complexity (from 1.20
clauses per sentence in the original directions to 1.02 in the
premodified) and repetitions and rephrasings of direction content
(from means of 2 0 content word repetitions and 16.47 total words
per each original direction t o means of 7.20 content word repeti-
tions and 33.47 total words for each premodilied direction).
In the other condition,the direction input was not premodified,
504 Language Learning Vol. 44, No. 3
but instead left in its original form, then presented by the same,
respective NSs during interaction with a matched group of learn-
ers. A s often a s possible, the NSs encouraged the learners to ask
for clarification of the directions (but not physical assistance with
carrying them out). An illustrative excerpt of what took place in
this second, negotiated input condition is shown in Example 4, and
will be discussed later in this section. Here, as noted in previous
excerpts, signals of negotiation a r e italicized. Responses t o these
signals a r e provided in boldface.
4. NS: [Initial, unmodified N S directions] Moving to the
top right corner, place the two mushrooms with
the three yellow dots in that grass patch down
toward the road. [Directionsduring negotiation]
Should I repeat it?
NNS: mmm
NS: OK. Moving to the top right corner, place
the two mushrooms with the three yellow
dots in that grass patch down toward the
road.
NNS: U m urhat means grass patch ?
NS: A piece of grass
NNS: Piece of grass, piece of grass
NS: Should I repeat it again? OK. Moving to the
top right corner, place the two mushrooms
with the three yellow dots in that grass
patch down toward the road.
NNS: I can’t understand
NS: OK. The top right corner of the picture.
NNS: mmm
NS: Place the two mushrooms, the two mush-
rooms with the three yellow dots in the
grass patch down towards the road
NNS: mm
NS: What’s the problem? Do you have a ques-
tion? I can help.
NNS: urn, on t h e - o n the right? left?
NS: On the top riglit corner of the picture
NNS: Top . . . top right right top corner
NS: in the grass patch down towards the road,
place the two mushrooms
Pica 505
NNS: t w o mushrooms
NS: O K ? . . . All right.
(Pica, Young, & Doughty, 1987b)
As reported and discussed by Pica et al. (1987a), both the
premodified and the negotiated input conditions enhanced the
learners' comprehension, which for this study was operationalized
as the number of directions carried out accurately. However, the
negotiated input was significantly better in aiding direction com-
prehension. Thus, the mean comprehension score ofthe 6 learners
in the premodified input condition was 69% and that for the 6
learners in the interactionally modified condition was 88%(t=3.78,
p c . 0 5 ) . Further, a related, classroom-based study involving the
same assembly task found that, among two comparable classrooms
of learners, participants who witnessed their classmates negotiate
over the initially unmodified directions were able to comprehend
them as well as did participants who engaged in the actual negotia-
tion (Pica, 1991). Thus, when it comes to comprehension,negotiation
appears to be a powerful commodity; even learners' being allowed
only to observe negotiation can improve their comprehension.
Analyses of the language used during negotiation in these
studies uncovered the same kinds of manipulations of L2 l e i s and
structure that were made t o the premodified'directions. However,
these arose spontaneously in negotiation and there were signifi-
cantly more ofthem. Thus, content word repetition increased from
its initial mean of 0.20 per direction to a mean of 13.17 as a r e s d t
of negotiation (compared t o the increase t o 7.20 per premodified
directions). Further, the mean number oftotal words per direction
increased from 16.47 t o 51.64 (compared to an increase to 33.47 in
the premodified).
These negotiated modifications in direction length and re-
dundancy are typfied in Example 4 above. Here, content words
such as mushrooms, dots, andgrass were frequently repeated and
rephrased into longer stretches of direction input. These modifi-
cations were also part of a general phenomenon whereby the
direction input was broken down or segmentedinto more processible
units, which was particularly apparent when the learners asked
506 Language Learning Vol. 44, No. 3
Beyond Comprehension
tasks, but also on the feedback that the learners weregivenand the
modified output they produced on these tasdks. Illustrations ofthe
role played by negotiation with respect t o these three areas can be
found throughout the excerpts of negotiation below. These have
been taken from the 2,361negotiation utterances generated by the
learners and NSs who participated in the studies. Of these
negotiation utterances, 558 and 675 were NS signal and response
utterances, respectively, and 578 and 550 were respectively learner
signal and response utterances. The excerpts have appeared
elsewhere; as noted following each.
The participants were 32 low-intermediate, preacademic L2
English learners, all speakers of Japanese L1, and 32 NSs of
standard American English, including students, skilled workers,
and professionals. They worked as same-sex and cross-sex NS-
learner dyads on four oral communication tasks. One of the tasks
required one member of dyad t o draw, and then describe, a picture
for the other t o replicate. A'similar task required them to reverse
these roles for the same assignment. Another task engaged dyad?
of learners and NSs in replicating unseen, researcher-supplied
picture sequences, for which each held equally pertinent informa-
tion. A final task involved the dyads in exchanging opinions about
the possible contributions of these tasks t o their language learn-
ing.
For each task, the data reveal numerous opportunities for
leaners t o attend t o L2 form in their negotiations with the NSs.
For example, when the learners signaled difficulty in understand-
ing the NSs, the NSs often repeated and reformulated their
original utterance for the leahers. Similarly, when the NSs
signaled that they could not unaerstand the learners, the latter
oRen gave these signals back as L2 reformulations of their own
interlanguage utterances. Opportunities for the learners to at-
tend t o their own interlanguage form were also abundant: for
example, when portions of their interlanguage utterances were
repeated back t o them in the NSs' signals. Such opportunities also
arose when the learners signaled the NSs and responded to them,
particularly with signals that provided interlanguage versions of
510 Language Learning VOl. 44, No. 3
Notes
'I thank an anonymous reviewer for pointing this out to me.
21n this article, the terms learner and NNS reflect the ways in which the
researchers cited have characterized their participants. However, the use of
the term learner by the present author reflects her current perspective on the
participants in her studies, as L2 learners who are also NNSs of English L2.
522 Language Learning Vol. 44, No. 3
References
I
Cross-discipli ary perspectives on language study (pp. 198-137). Lexing-
ton, MA:D.C. Heath & Co.
Pica, T. (1993).C mmunication with second language learners: What does i t
reveal about t e social and linguistic processes of second language acqui-
sition? In J. Alatis (Ed.), Language, communication, and social meaning
(pp. 434-464). Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.
Plca, T., Doughty, C., & Young, R. (1986). Making input comprehensible: Do
interactional modifications help? I T L Review ofApplied Linguistics, 72,l-
25.
Pica, T.,Holliday, L., Lewis, N, Berducci, D., & Newman, J.(1991).Language
learning through interaction: What role does gender play? Studies in
Second Language Acquisition, 13, 343-376.
Pica, T., Holliday, L., Lewis, N., & Morgenthaler, L. (1989). Comprehensible
output as an outcome of linguistic demands on the learner. Studies in
Second Language Acquisition, 11, 63-90.
Pica, T.,Kanagy, R., & Falodun, J. (1993). Choosing and using communica-
tion tasks for second language research and instruction. In S. M. Gass &.
G. Crookes (Eds.), Task-based learning in a second language (pp. 9-34).
Clevedon, Avon: Multilingual Matters.
Pica, T., Lewis, N., & Holliday, L. (1990, March). NS-NNS negotiation: An
equal opportunity for speech modification? Paper presented to 24th annual
TESOL Convention, San F’rancisco, CA.
526 Language Learning Vol. 44, No.,3
Pica, T., Lincoln-Porter, F., Paninos, D., & Linnell, J. (in preparation). Can
second language learners be a resource for their L2 learning? Only their
researcher knows for sure.
Pica, T., Young, R., & Doughty, C. (1987a). The impact of interaction on
comprehensiori? T E m L Quarterly, 21, 737-758.
Pica, T., Young, R., & Doughty, C. (1987b). [Transcription]. Unpublished raw
data.
Pienemann, M. (1989). Is language teachable?: Psycholinguistic experiments
and hypotheses. Applied Linguistics, 10, 52-79.
Porter, P. (1986). How learners talk to each other: Input and interaction in
task-centered discussions. In R. R. Day (Ed.), Talking to learn :Conversa-
tion in second language acquisition (pp. 200-224). Rowley, W Newbury
House.
Richards, J., & Rodgers, T. (1986). Approaches and methods in language
teaching. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Rutherford, W., & Sharwood Smith, M. (1985). Consciousness-raising and
universal grammar. In W. Rutherford & M. Sharwood Smith (Eds.),
Grammar and second language learning (pp. 107-116). Rowley, MA:
Newbury House.
Sato, C. (1986). Conversation and interlanguage development: Rethinking
the connection. In R. R. Day (Ed.), Talking to learn: Conversation in second
language acquisition (pp. 2 3 4 8 ) . Rowley, MA: Newbury House.
Scarcella, R. C,, & Higa, C. (1981).Input, negotiation, and age differences in
second language acquisition. Language Learning, 3 1 , 4 0 9 4 3 7 .
Schachter, J. (1983).Nutritional needs of language learners. In M. Clarke &
J. Handscombe (Eds.), On TESOL '82: Pacific perspectives on language
learning and teaching (pp. 175-89). Washington, DC: TESOL.
Schachter, J. (1984). A universal input condition. In W. Rutherford (Ed.),
Universals and second language acquisition (pp. 167-183). Amsterdam:
John Benjamins.
Schachter, J. (1986). Three approaches to the study of input. Language
Learning, 36,211-225.
Schachter, J . (1991). Corrective feedback in historical perspective. Second
Langucrge Research, 7,89-102.
Schmidt, R. (1990). 'The role of consciousness in second language acquisition.
Applied Linguistics, 1 I, 129-158.
Schmidt, R., & Frota, S. (1986). Developing basic conversational ability in a
second language: A case study of an adult learner of Portuguese. In R. R.
Day (Ed.), Talking to learn: Conversation in second language acquisition
(pp. 239-326). Rowley, M A Newbury House.
Sharwood Smith, M. (1987). Comprehension vs. acquisition: Two ways of
processing input. Applied Linguistics, 8, 237-255.
Pica 527