Language Guide
Language Guide
1 Course
Linguistics
Chapter 1 | What is Linguistics?
Before the 19th century, language in the Western world was of interest mainly to
philosophers, the Greek philosophers Plato and Aristotle made major
contributions to the study of language.
كوذ هذّ اُلالعلخ،٢ْ اُلالعلخ ثشٌَ أعبعٜ ر٢ اُؼبُْ اُـشث٢ ًبٗذ اُِـخ ك،هجَ اُوشٕ اُزبعغ ػشش
. دساعخ اُِـخ٢شح ك٤ٔبد ًجٛ ٓغبٞأسعطٝ ٕٕٞ أكالؽٞ٤ٗٗبٞ٤ُا
Plato : the first person to distinguish between nouns and verbs.
.ٍاالكؼبٝ ٖ األعٔبء٤ض ث٤ٓ ١ٍ اُزٝ اُشخض األ:ٛتالذ
Nineteenth century: historical linguistics (In Europe)
)ستاٚ عٍُ اٌٍغح اٌراسٌخً (فً ا:اٌمشْ اٌراعع عشش
6871 is the year which regard as the birthdate of linguistics.
.بد٤ٗالد اُِغب٤ٓ خ٣ؼزجش ربس٣ ١ اُؼبّ اُزٞٛ 6871
Sir William Jones,
An Englishman points out that Sanskrit (the old Indian language), Greek, Latin,
Celtic and Germanic all had striking structural similarities.
ّ ض٤ٌِٗ سعَ اٞٛ
خ٤اُغِزٝ خ٤٘٤اُالرٝ خ٤و٣ االؿش،)ٔخ٣خ اُوذ٣٘ذُٜخ (اُِـخ ا٤ز٣ إ اُِـخ اُغ٘غٌش٠ُ أشبس ا١
.١ٞ٤٘ت اُج٤ًب ثبُزشٜب رزشبثًِٜ خ٤ٗاألُٔبٝ
According to these similarities he refers that these languages must spring from
one common source, he concluded.
.اؽذ ٓشزشىٝ اُِـبد ٓ٘جضوخ ٖٓ ٓظذسٙزٛ ٌٕٞغت إٔ ر٣ ٚٗ أ٠ُ أشبس ا،ٙزٛ ٚ اُزشبثٚعٝكوًب ألٝ
His discovery makes all the following linguists write about “comparative
grammar”. The work was with the reconstruction of Proto-Indo-European
languages and making hypotheses about them.
اػبدح ث٘بء اُِـبد٠ِاػذ أُوبسٗخ" ًبٕ اُؼَٔ ػٕٞ ػٖ "اُوٌٞزج٣ ٖٙ ٖٓ ثؼذ٣ٖ اُِز٤٣ٞ عؼَ اُِـٚاًزشبك
.بٜ٤ِبد ػ٤ػغ اُلشػٝٝ خ٤سثٝخ اال٣٘ذُٜا
Chomsky has talked about Creativity which is the ability of human beings
to produce and comprehend an indefinite number of novel utterances.
.ذح٣ش ٓؾذد ٖٓ اُؼجبساد اُغذ٤ْ ػذد ؿٜكٝ اٗزبط٠ِ هذسح اُجشش ػٞٛٝ ، ػٖ اإلثذاع٢ٌٓغٞرؾذس رش
Chomsky used the word grammar to mean not only the „rules‟ which a
person has inside their head which specify the sequences of their language,
but also a linguist‟s attempt to express these rules, which he labelled a
generative grammar.
٢ب اُجشش كٜٗخض٣ ٢اػذ" اُزٞ "اُو٠ِعب ً ُِذالُخ ػٝ اعزخذآب ً ٓضدgrammar ًِٔخ٢ٌٓغٞاعزخذّ رش
ش٤ٕ ُِزؼجّٞ٣ُٞخ اُِـٝ ٓؾب٠ُِِذالُخ ػٝ ،خٜ ٖٓ ع،ْٜب ُـبرٜ٤ِ رشزَٔ ػ٢رؾذد اُززبثؼبد اُزٝ ،ْٜادٓـز
.ٟخ أخشٜ" ٖٓ ع١ذ٤ُٞ اُزٞ ثـ "اُ٘ؾٙ رئُق ٓب اعٔب٢اػذ اُزٞػٖ اُو
In Charles Darwin‟s vision of the origins of language, early humans had already
developed musical ability prior to language and were using it “to charm each
other.” But it is just a speculation and simply we don‟t know how language
originated and there's no evidence to prove its origins.
خ هجَ اُِـخ٤و٤عُٞٔا ثبُلؼَ اُوذسح اٝسٞائَ هذ ؽٝ ًبٕ اُجشش األ،ٍ اُِـخٖٞ ألط٣ٝخ رشبسُض داس٣ سإ٢ك
ق ٗشؤد اُِـخ٤ً ثجغبؽخ ال ٗؼشفٝ ٘بدٌٜب ٓغشد رٌُٜ٘ ."ْ اُجؼغٜب "ُغؾش ثؼؼٜٗٞٓغزخذ٣ اًٞٗبٝ
.بَُٜٞ إلصجبد أط٤ُ٘بى دٛ ظ٤ُٝ
These are some theories about the origins of language :
:ي اٌٍغحٛي أصٛ٘زٖ تعض إٌظشٌاخ د
1- The divine source : ًّ ٌٙاٌّصذس اال
This theory according to Bible states that God created Adam and "whatsoever
Adam called every living creature that was the name thereof", most religions
believe that the human language comes by a divine source.
اؿِت،بٜٔخ ًبٕ اع٤ًَ ٓب دػب ادّ راد ٗلظ ؽٝ ّكوب ً ٌُِزبة أُوذط إ هللا خِن آدٝٝ خ٣ اُ٘ظشٙزٛ ر٘ض
.٢ّ ُٜخ أرذ ٖٓ ٓظذس ا٣بٕ رئٖٓ ثؤٕ اُِـخ اُجشش٣األد
The experiments: اٌرجاسب
a) The Greek writer Herodotus reported the story of an Egyptian pharaoh
named (Psamtik) who tried the experiment with two newborn babies. After
two years of isolation with goats and a mute shepherd, the children were
reported to have spontaneously uttered, something that was identified as the
Phrygian word bekos, meaning “bread.” That seems very unlikely. As
several commentators have pointed out, they must have heard what the
goats were saying. (First remove the -kos ending, which was added in the
Greek version of the story, then pronounce beas you would the English
word bed without -d at the end. Can you hear a goat?).
ٖ٤ِي) هبّ ثؤعشاء اُزغشثخ ٓغ ؽل٤ (ثغبٓزٚٔ اع١ٕ ٓظشٞرظ هظخ كشػٝدٝش٤ٛ ٢ٗٗبٞ٤ُسد اٌُبرت اٝأ
ْ ٓب رٞٛٝ ،ًب٤ا رِوبئٞغ إٔ األؽلبٍ هذ رِلظ٤ أش،ٌْ اث٢ساػٝ ٖ ٖٓ اُؼضُخ ٓغ أُبػض٤ٓ ثؼذ ػب.الدحُٞ ا٢ض٣ؽذ
ا هذٌٞٗٞ٣ ٕغت أ٣ ،ٖ٤ذ ٖٓ أُؼِو٣ ًٔب أشبس اُؼذ." "اُخجض٢٘ رؼ٢اُزٝ ،خ٤غ٣ ًِٔخ كشٚٗ أ٠ِ ػٙذ٣رؾذ
It has also been suggested that the original sounds of language may have
come from natural cries of emotion such as pain, anger and joy.
اُـؼتٝ ُْخ ٓضَ األ٤ؼ٤خ ؽج٤ٕ ٗبرغخ ػٖ طشخبد ػبؽلٌٞخ ُِـخ هذ ر٤ِاد األطٞؼًب إٔ األط٣َ أ٤ه
.اُلشػٝ
The “pooh-pooh” theory is rejected. (Justify) ٘زٖ إٌظشٌح سفضد تشس
the expressive noises people make in emotional reactions contain sounds that are
not otherwise used in speech production and consequently would seem to be
rather unlikely candidates as source sounds for language.
ُْ )وخ ُلظخ٣ؽشٝ دٞػؼ ًَ طٞ٣( اٌُزبة٢ ك25 طلؾخTABLE 3.1 ٍٝلؼَ هشاءح عذ٣ : ِالدظح
.ٙق أػال٣ٕ ُِزؼبسٞٔ أُؼ٢ كٚ ٓشبثًٚٗٞ ٌٙارًش
Glottal stop: a sound produced when the air passing through the glottis is
stopped completely then released.
.ظذس٣ ْٔش ػجش أُضٓبس رٔب ًٓب ص٣ ١اء اُزُٜٞهق اٞز٣ ٘زظ ػ٘ذٓب٣ دٞ ط:لف ِضِاسيٛذ
flap: a sound produced with the tongue tip briefly touching the alveolar ridge.
.خ٤ضح اُؾبكخ اُغ٘خ٤عٝ ِٔظ ُلزشح٣ٝ ٕ٘زظ ٓغ ؽشف اُِغب٣ دٞ ط:ًخفم
vowel: a sound produced through the vocal folds without constriction of the
airflow in the mouth.
.ْ اُل٢اء كُٜٕٞ اٗوجبع رذكن اٝخ د٤رٞ٘زظ ٖٓ خالٍ اُؾجبٍ اُظ٣ دٞ ط:خ عٍحٛص
diphthong: a sound formed by combining two vowel sounds (e.g. boy).
.)boy َ ؽشف ػِخ (ٓض٢رٖٞ ط٤ٕ ٖٓ اُغٔغ ثٌٞز٣ دٞ ط:َإدْغا
uvula: the small appendage at the end of the velum.
.بحُِٜخ ا٣بٜٗ ٢شح ك٤ الؽوخ اُظـ٢ٛ :اج اٌذٍكٌٙ
uvular: a sound produced with the back of the tongue near the uvula.
.بح اُؾِنُٜ ٖٓ ٖٓ اُِغبٕ ثبُوشة٢٘زظ ٓغ اُغضء اُخِل٣ دٞ ط:ي
ّ ٌٛٙ
Phonology Phonetics
1- deals with „phoneme‟. زؼبَٓ ٓغ٣ 1- deals with „phones‟. زؼبَٓ ٓغ٣
2- Specific. ٓخظض 2- general. ّػب
3- concerned with the abstract, 3- Concrete.
.خ٣ذ٣زْ ثبُزغشٜ٣ .طِٞٔٓ
4- Semantic function of sounds. 4- Not concerned with meaning.
.ادٞخ ٖٓ األط٤ُلخ اُذال٤ظُٞا .٠٘زْ ثبُٔؼٜ٣ ال
aspiration: a puff of air that sometimes accompanies the pronunciation of a stop.
.١د االٗلغبسٞبٕ ٗطن اُظ٤ ثؼغ األؽ٢ رشاكن ك٢اء اُزُٜٞ ٗلخخ ا:إٌطك تًّء إٌفظ
Distinctive feature: is any feature that distinguishes one phoneme from the
other.
.خش٥اؽذًا ػٖ اٝ رًبٞض ط٤ٔضح ر٤ٓ ١ أ٢ٛ :اٌغّح اٌٍّّضج
minimal pair (set): two (or more) words that are identical in form except for a
contrast in one phoneme in the same position in each word (e.g. bad, mad).
٢اؽذ كٝ دٞ ط٢ٖ ك٣ اُشٌَ ثبعزض٘بء اُزجب٢ أًضش) ٓزطبثوزبٕ كٝ ًِٔزبٕ (أ:)عحّٛاج اٌّصغشج (ِجٚاألص
.)bad, mad ،ٍَ أُضب٤ عج٠ِ ًَ ًِٔخ (ػ٢ػغ كُٞٔٗلظ ا
/kit/, /ked/ /bit/, /bet/ = it can distinguished with one sound.
.٠٘ش أُؼ٤٤رـٝ ٖ٤ٖ ًِٔز٤ض ث٤٤ٌٖٔٔ اُز٣ اؽذٝ دٖٞٓ خالٍ ط
The basic structure of the kind of syllable found in English words like
green (CCVC), eggs (VCC), and (VCC), ham (CVC), I (V), do (CV), not
(CVC), like (CVC), them (CVC), Sam (CVC), I (V), am (VC).
consonant cluster: two or more consonants in sequence.
.َ اُزغِغ٢ف اُغبً٘خ كٝ أًضش ٖٓ اُؾشٝ اص٘بٕ أ:اٌىرٍح اٌغاوٕح
Coarticulation effect: the process of making one sound virtually at the same
time as the next sound.
.٢ُد اُزبٞهذ اُظُٞ ٗلظ ا٢جب ً ك٣اؽذ روشٝ دٞ عؼَ طٚ٤ِٔ ػ:ًٍاٌرأثٍش اٌّفص
a) Assimilation: the process whereby a feature of one sound becomes part of
another during speech production.
b) Nasalization: pronunciation of a sound with air flowing through the nose,
typically before a nasal consonant.
c) Elision: the process of leaving out a sound segment in the pronunciation of
a word.
d) Normal speech
.ّد آخش أص٘بء اٗزبط اٌُالٞد عض ًءا ٖٓ طٞضح اُظ٤ٓ بٜ رظجؼ ث٢خ اُز٤ِٔ اُؼ:) االعرٍعابA
.٢ ػبدح هجَ اُؾشف اُغبًٖ األٗل،اء ٖٓ خالٍ األٗقُٜٞد ٓغ رذكن اٞ ٗطن ط:) اٌغٕحB
. ٗطن أٌُِخ٢ ك٢رٞخ رشى ٓوطغ ط٤ِٔ ػ:) اٌذزفC
) اٌىالَ اٌعاديD