100% found this document useful (1 vote)
503 views34 pages

The Science of Reading - An Introduction

Texes STR test and guide for students needing to pass the STR

Uploaded by

Felicia Laster
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
100% found this document useful (1 vote)
503 views34 pages

The Science of Reading - An Introduction

Texes STR test and guide for students needing to pass the STR

Uploaded by

Felicia Laster
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
You are on page 1/ 34

The Science of Reading:

An Introduction to

Research-Backed

Instruction

Breaking the Code Erica L. Meltzer


www.breakingthecode.com
Table of Contents

Introduction 2

II. Written English Is a Code 4

III. Reading is a Learned Skill 5

III. What Does It Mean to "Read"? 6

IV. Why Is the Three-Cueing System 10


(MSV) Ineffective?

V. Phonemes 12

VI. Graphemes and Morphemes 16

VII. Sight Words 22

VIII. Speed and Fluency 24

IX. The Reading-Spelling Connection 26

X. Dyslexia 30

Sources 32

1
Introduction
If you’ve spent some time reading about the state of reading instruction in American
classrooms, you could be forgiven for thinking that nothing poses as much of a threat to
children developing a love of reading as phonics. Endless, rote drills, boring flashcards, and dry
exercises detached from “authentic texts” all conspire to turn children against the written word
—or so the narrative goes. In reality, however, not only is this a hugely distorted picture of what
phonics involves, but the situation is exactly the opposite: many children fail to develop a
love of reading because they do not really learn to read.

Before going any further, I’d like to insist on the importance of distinguishing between short-
term and long-term thinking. Yes, it is reasonably straightforward to design activities around
simple picture books that keep young children engaged and excited. And yes, there is no
denying that some popular strategies for teaching reading can and often do produce something
that looks like proficiency. Many children successfully memorize numerous words by sight, and
some of them become highly intuitive and skilled guessers as well. In the early grades, children
with poor phonetic understanding may actually read above grade level. Given that, it might
seem pointless to make a fuss over how children are reading the words—why should it matter
as long as they can read them?

The answer is that enthusiasm and workaround strategies such as memorizing and
skipping and guessing can only take the place of fundamental skills for so long. The
emphasis of much early-reading instruction is on getting students to read on grade level,
without much consideration of what happens afterward. A child might memorize enough words
by sight to eke into the realm of proficiency in first or second or even third grade, but what
happens after that, when suddenly there is a flood of new words to contend with? A child can
read “just right” books for only so many years before it becomes clear that they are not going to
naturally catch themselves up, and any remediation beyond the early grades is likely to be
complicated by the psychological roadblocks of shame and low self-esteem. Once children
have developed an image of themselves as “bad readers,” it can be very difficult to shake.

To be clear, around 40% of children will pick up much of the reading "code" with no or minimal
instruction, and those who do not may be taught by a parent or tutor and become skilled and
avid readers. However, for the larger group of students who require some degree of systematic,
explicit teaching, and who are unable to access it outside of school, the results are at best
years of frustrating plodding through impenetrable mounds of text, and at worst nothing short of
tragic. (California is notorious for having used fourth-grade reading scores to estimate the size
of its future prison population.) Because the consequences for disadvantaged students are so
dire, discussions about the long-term fallout of poor reading instruction tend to focus on them; it
is often assumed that students from more affluent families are unaffected. In the years I spent
tutoring mostly very well-off teenagers, however, I saw very clearly that that was not the case.

2
Not only were many of my (SAT®) students unable to sound out unfamiliar words, but it did not
even seem to occur them that words could be sounded out. They guessed wildly, they misread
words, they confused similar-looking words, they added letters, they skipped letters, they
skipped words, they skipped entire lines, often without seeming to realize it. In some cases they
even struggled to read in a normal left-to-right sequence for a sustained period. These were
high school juniors, but they were often reading (and writing) at a middle-school level. Yet
somehow they had skated through until eleventh grade—at supposedly very good schools—
without anyone intervening, or even noticing. I was flabbergasted.

It took me nearly a decade to fully understand the system that had produced these kinds of
reading problems, and the more research I did, the more I became aware of the extraordinary
split between the scientific and educational communities, and of how much critical information
about how the reading process occurs—information that should play a key role in shaping
pedagogy—is far from common knowledge. Indeed, the information in this guide was compiled
over many months and from a diverse array of articles, research papers, published interviews,
and personal conversations. I have done my best to distill the key points from these very
disparate sources into as concise and coherent a form as possible.

That said, the bottom line is this: beginners and experts approach any skill in
fundamentally different ways, and reading is no exception. Any reading program that
fails to acknowledge that reality is bound to fall short.

Skilled readers can devote all their energy to thinking about meaning because for them,
decoding is an effortless process in which sound-letter combinations are retrieved from memory
and strung together automatically. To reach that stage, however, most children must pass
through an initial, often extended period in which learning to match sounds and letters is the
main focus. Asking them to skip that step—or learn in a way that downplays its importance—
deprives them of the opportunity to acquire a solid foundation. And that is a shame because
most children are thrilled to discover that reading is a code and that they can break it.

It is also important to remember that children’s understanding of spoken language generally far
exceeds their decoding ability, and that books for young children rely on very simple words.
Consequently, beginning readers do not need to be taught detailed comprehension strategies
—if they can decode a text fluently, they can probably understand it. As letter-sound
associations become fixed in children’s memories, they will recognize words more quickly,
become more confident as readers, and have more bandwidth available to process meaning,
allowing them to explore more challenging texts. In the meantime, adults can address the other
side of the reading equation by developing children's vocabulary and general knowledge by
reading more challenging texts aloud and discussing a wide range of topics. Eventually, the two
pieces will merge, allowing students to access the full power of the printed word.

–Erica Meltzer

3
I. Written English Is a Code

Letters and combinations of letters represent sounds. “Decoding” literally


means “undoing the code.”

Letters and letter-combinations in all parts of a word contribute to its sound

Unlike Spanish or Italian or German, English is not perfectly phonetic—certain


letters or groups of letters are pronounced differently in different contexts.

However, English is also not a pictographic language like Chinese, in which


characters stand for entire words and must be memorized individually.

While English has far more exceptions than other European languages, the
vast majority of its words can still be read more or less phonetically.

More than 85% of English words are either completely regular (50%) or
very slightly irregular (35%, usually one vowel sound).

Another 10% or so are moderately irregular but follow alternate patterns


(again mostly involving vowels).

Less than 5% are completely irregular and must be learned by sight.

English is irregular because it is a hybrid language: part Anglo-Saxon (Germanic)


and part Latin (often via French), with a sprinkling of Greek. Latin-derived terms,
which tend toward phonetic spellings, tend to be more formal and technical,
whereas the Anglo-Saxon-derived terms that predominate in everyday speech tend
to be less phonetic. As a result, English spelling is often considered less
predictable than it actually is.

4
II. Reading Is a Learned Skill

Unlike spoken language, writing was invented only about 5,000 years ago.

Although children cannot learn to read until all the relevant regions of the brain are
sufficiently developed, there is no developmental stage when they automatically
acquire the ability to understand print.

While a small percentage of children will learn to read on their own, the vast
majority (around 95%) require some degree of explicit instruction.

~5% read effortlessly.

~35% need minimal instruction.

~40-50% need explicit, systematic instruction.

~10-15% require extended systematic instruction, with significant repetition.

Learning to read phonetically can help virtually all children become better readers,
writers and spellers in the long term by given them the tools to manipulate the
building blocks of written language.

It reinforces the connections between speech, letters, and sounds.

It builds speed and automaticity.

It ensures that "natural" readers do not miss key skills.

5
III. What Does It Mean to "Read"?

Discussions about reading are often filled with misunderstandings because the
term is used to refer to two separate actions with very different meanings.

1) Decoding

The act of matching sounds to letters (or groups of letters) in order to


sound out words.

2) Comprehension

This ability is much more complex, requiring the integration of syntax


(word order), vocabulary, and background knowledge.

Decoding ability is necessary but not sufficient. It is impossible to understand


the meaning of a text of unless you can figure out what the words say; however,
knowing what the words say does not guarantee that you will understand their
meaning.

Most adults, for example, could decode an advanced physics textbook fluently,
but would understand next to nothing. This does not mean that learning to decode
is optional, however, or that it is just one skill among many. Rather, it is the basis
for understanding written language. At the same time, other factors—some quite
complex—are also involved.

Learning to decode phonetically will not necessarily turn a child into a


stellar reader, but not knowing how to decode this way will make it
unnecessarily difficult for them to read well beyond a certain level.

6
The Simple View

Reading = Aural Comprehension x Decoding Ability

In other words, a person's ability to read cannot exceed their aural


comprehension (which includes vocabulary, syntax, and background knowledge)
or their understanding of sound-letter correspondences.

Described in a 1986 paper by Philip Gough and William Tunmer; among


researchers, accepted general theory of how reading works.

Reading difficulties can be caused by a weakness in one area or, more often,
both ("garden-variety" reading disability).

Because reading is the product rather than the sum of two components, it
cannot occur if either is missing entirely. (Anything multiplied by zero is zero.)
A student who cannot decode cannot read, regardless of their other skills.

Until late middle school, children can generally understand spoken language
more sophisticated than what they can decode. The goal of reading instruction is
to bring their print comprehension up to the level of their aural comprehension.

The 5 Components of Reading


1) Phonemic Awareness

2) Phonics

3) Fluency

4) Vocabulary

5) Comprehension

7
Orthographic Mapping

Expert readers process words automatically and effortlessly as a result of a


process called orthographic mapping. (Orthography means "spelling.")

Essentially, words and sounds are wired together in their memories;


their brains have mapped specific sequences of letters as corresponding to
specific sequences of sounds, and stored the information for instant recognition
and retrieval. As a result, the vast majority of words are effectively "sight words,"
freeing them to focus on meaning.

Orthographic mapping is fundamentally different from memorizing whole


words visually. That method involves sight only and requires each new word be
learned from scratch rather than matched to existing sound-letter relationships.

To reiterate: students who read by memorizing words by their appearance may


seem to be on track for several years but develop problems later, when their
working memories become overwhelmed.

Orthographic mapping is NOT the result of:

Memorizing an entire text and reciting it while looking at the page(s)

Guessing based on pictures

Guessing words based on the first/last letters, or on "little" words


contained in a longer word

Guessing or making words up based on “what would make sense”

Guessing based on other context clues

8
What Is Synthetic Phonics?

Like the word reading itself, the term synthetic is cursed with two meanings that
add to much of the confusion in discussions about phonics.

On one hand, it is most commonly used as a synonym for fake. Interpreted this
way, the name "synthetic phonics" is often—and incorrectly—assumed to refer to
the fact that phonics programs may require children to read nonsense words as
well as real words. This fact is often used as grounds to criticize phonics
programs as being divorced from "authentic" reading, or for failing to encourage
children to focus on "making meaning."

In reality, however, synthetic comes from the word synthesis: it refers to


the fact that such phonics programs are based on teaching children the
building blocks of written language and then helping them put the blocks
together, or synthesize them, into words.

Synthetic phonics facilitates orthographic mapping by


explicitly and systematically teaching relationships between
writing and speech. When children's brains process sound-
spelling relationships efficiently, they can turn new words into
sight words after only a few exposures.

To be perfectly clear: The goal of synthetic phonics is to get children


reading real books as quickly as possible (when they have learned around
6-8 sound-letter combinations), not to provide endless isolated drills. It
does, however, rely on decodable readers that are assigned based on the
sound-letter combinations that children have already learned, allowing them to
practice applying skills in a non-overwhelming way. Early success with these
books builds confidence and reinforces the idea that new words can be figured
out logically. At the same time, adults can read more challenging texts aloud.

9
IV. Why Is the Three-Cueing System (MSV) Ineffective?

As a truce in the reading wars of the 1980s and 90s, many schools implemented
a program known as "Balanced Literacy," in which phonetic instruction would be
balanced with more general literacy-building activities such as read-alouds. In
reality, however, Balanced Literacy is often used as a cover for reading taught via
Whole-Language or the three-cueing system (or MSV), in which children are
taught to read by using orthographic (spelling), syntactic (word-order), and
meaning-based clues.

Children taught this way are typically required to memorize many common words
by sight, then taught to use first letters (and sometimes last) letters and sounds,
along with a variety of contextual information, including pictures, to identify
unfamiliar words. Teachers may keep a running record of students' reading errors
("miscues") for analysis but typically address such issues from the standpoint of
meaning rather than phonetic understanding.

The essential problem with the three-cueing approach is that it encourages


compensation strategies (guessing, memorizing, skipping) that weak readers use,
with the result that children often become weak readers. Establishing the
sound-letter correspondences that underpin skilled reading requires close
and continued attention to how words are written, whereas three-cueing
methods encourage children to ignore specific sequences of letters and
guess. The two approaches are incompatible.

Even if phonics is presented as an option, some children—particularly ones with


weaker skills—will realize that using pictures or other clues requires far less effort
than sounding words out and fall further behind.

To be clear: skilled readers DO use context clues all the time, but as an aid
to understanding meaning. Children who decode well may also use them
occasionally to help identify irregularly pronounced words, but this is different
from relying on them because of an inability to decode phonetically.

10
Additional Problems with Three-Cueing Methods

Reliance on pictures leads to difficulties with text-only books.

Repeated checking to see whether guesses make sense impedes


speed and fluency.

Multiple words may make sense in context.

Context clues may be ambiguous or absent.

Context clues themselves may be misunderstood.

Difficulty monitoring comprehension (children may not realize when


they are misreading words).

Difficulty reading proper names (including places) and subject-


specific terms, which often cannot be determined from context clues;
this can become a very serious problem in later grades.

Lack of knowledge transfer: a word identified only from context in


one situation may not be recognized in a different situation.

Long-term inability to focus on words in normal left-right sequence


(eyes racing around the page).

Increase in speed accompanied by decrease in accuracy.

11
V. Phonemes

Phonemes are the individual sounds that make up a word.


There are 26 letters in the alphabet but 44 common phonemes (chart on p. 14).

A phoneme can be made up either of one letter, or of multiple letters.

The word pin contains three phonemes, each represented by a


single letter: p/i/n.

The word sigh contains two phonemes (s/igh), even though it has
four letters.

The word chin contains three phonemes (ch/i/n), even though it


has four letters. The same is true for sing (s/i/ng).

The word brick contains four phonemes (b/r/i/k), even though it


has five letters.

The number of phonemes can also differ from the number of syllables.

The word I contains one syllable and one phoneme.

The word cat contains one syllable and three phonemes (c/a/t).

The word pens contains one syllable and four phonemes (p/e/n/s).

The word over contains two syllables and three phonemes (o/v/er).

12
Phonemic awareness is the ability to isolate the phonemes in a word.

Phonemic awareness not a "natural" skill—it is important only insofar as it


prepares children to read and write—but is absolutely crucial. It is impossible to
connect sounds to letters unless the sounds can be heard correctly in the first
place. As a result, phonemic awareness essentially predicts skill in reading.

Phonemic awareness is often underdeveloped in children who have not had their
language skills developed through activities such as reciting the alphabet, playing
with rhymes and sounds, etc.

Difficulty identifying and discriminating between similar vowel sounds (e.g., "eh"
and "ih") is often the primary issue. The developing brain has a "window" between
approximately six and 18 months to learn to hear and differentiate these sounds;
if a child receives insufficient exposure to them during this period, for example
because their primary caregiver speaks with an accent, they may permanently
struggle to identify them. This is not a hearing problem but rather a matter of
how the brain processes aural information.

The Unnecessary Schwa

A rampant problem in phonics instruction involves the insertion of a schwa (a


neutral “uh” sound) after a consonant. For example, cat is frequently broken down
as cuh/a/tuh. This is incredibly confusing to some children, who do not intuitively
grasp that the extra vowel sound must be omitted when the sounds are blended
together.

To avoid this issue, consonants should be pronounced unvoiced—that is, without


engaging the vocal cords. For example, an unvoiced "s" is the first sound in the
word snake: it consists of the only the consonant sound and a stream of air.

Note: for ease of blending, you may find it helpful to pronounce "w" as woo and
"y" as yee; these sounds cannot really be said unvoiced.

13
The 44 Main English Phonemes
b - boy c/k - cat d - dog f - fit

g - go h- hat j - job l - late

m - my n - not ng- ring p - pet

qu - quit r - red s - sun t - tip

v - van w - wet x - fix y - yes

z - zebra ch - chip sh* - shop th** - thin

th - that a - apple air - fair ar - far

aw - paw ay - pay eer - cheer ee - feet

eh - ten er - cover ew - few ie - tie


(long) (short)
ih - win oa - boat oo - food oo - took

or - for ou - loud oy - boy uh - cup

*An alternate phoneme, zh, is used in words ending in -sion, e.g., confusion, illusion, revision.

**A voiced version of this phoneme is used in a small number of words, e.g., though and that.

14
Building Phonemic Awareness: Manipulating Sounds

Blending and segmenting are aural/oral exercises designed to help children


understand how words are made up of smaller components: phonemes and
syllables. (A syllable can consist of a vowel alone, or of a vowel + consonant(s),
but it cannot consist of only consonants.)

In blending, children begin with sounds and put them together to create words:
typically, the instructor speaks the syllables or phonemes in a word slowly and
then has the child (or children) "blend" them quickly together (e.g., ch-air: chair).

in segmenting, they begin with whole words and break them into their component
sounds. In one common exercise, the instructor says a word and then has the
child repeat it, clapping for each syllable (e.g., pencil: pen-cil) and then saying
the number of syllables. Alternately, the instructor may give the child a word to
"stretch" by saying each phoneme slowly (e.g., team: t/ea/m).

Because blending builds more directly on spoken language, whereas segmenting


is essentially preparation for writing, blending exercises should normally be
introduced first.

A more advanced exercise involves replacing (adding and subtracting)


sounds. For example, the instructor says the word bat and has the child repeat it.
The instructor then asks the child to take off the "b" and say the sound that
remains (at), after which the child is asked to add an "m" sound to the beginning
of the word (mat). Next, the instructor asks the child to remove the "m" and
replace it with an "s" (or any other consonant), and so on. Although this may
seem like a simple exercise to most adults, it actually requires a fairly
sophisticated level of aural processing.

You can also present children with pairs of words and ask them whether they
rhyme. The ability to hear endings as "chunks" helps students recognize and spell
words with similar final sounds more quickly and easily.

15
VI. Graphemes and Morphemes

A grapheme is a written symbol that corresponds to a phoneme.

Graphemes can correspond to single letters, or they can correspond to specific


sequences of letters.

The grapheme "m" corresponds to the "mmmmm" phoneme.

The grapheme "kn" corresponds to the "nnnnnnn" phoneme.

The grapheme "igh" corresponds to the long "i" phoneme.

The grapheme "eigh" corresponds to the "ay" phoneme.

A blend is a combination of two consonants in which each letter makes a


separate sound.

A digraph is a combination of two letters (consonants or vowels) that make


one sound. Note that "ch", "sh", "th" and "wh" are usually taught first.

Blends: bl, br, dr, fl, fr, gl, gr, ng, pl, pr, sl, sm, sn, sp, st, sw, tr

Digraphs: ai, ay, ea, ie, ou, oa, oy, uy, ch, kn, ng, ph, ps, qu, sh, th, wh, wr

A few graphemes represent phonemes with three letters (trigraph) or more.

-air, -ear, -ure, -eigh, -igh, -ough

16
Sounds to Letters, Not Letters to Sounds

One of the ways in which phonics may be made unnecessarily confusing is if


letters are associated with sounds, as opposed to sounds being associated with
letters.

For example, a teacher may point out that the letter "i" is long (i.e., it says its
name) when a word ends in "e", e.g., fine, like, tile. If the sound is introduced and
studied only in context of this pattern for an extended period of time, children are
much more likely to become confused when the -igh pattern, in which "i" also
makes a long sound (fight, light, right), is finally introduced.

If alternate patterns involving other sounds are also introduced sporadically and
long after the original pattern has been taught—or worse, not explicitly introduced
at all— the potential for general confusion is enormously multiplied.

A far more effective method is to begin with the sound itself and, in the
same lesson (or in a series of consecutive lessons), cover all the spelling
patterns associated with it. While it may seem counterintuitive, this approach is
ultimately less confusing because children do not have to repeatedly work to
override internalized patterns.

A list of common alternate patterns is provided on the following page.

17
Common Multi-Spelling Phonemes
AW - al (always, mall); au (applaud, fraud); aw (drawl, saw); o (cob, jot); oa (broad)

AY - a + final e (late, page); ai (afraid, rain); ay (bay, stay); ea (break, great); eigh (eight, weight)

C/K - c (cat, cup); ck (back, deck); k (dark, milk); ch (choir, ache); qu (unique, mosquito)

EE - e (me, we); e + final e (eve, meme); ee (meet, see); ea (clean, dream); ei (conceive, receive);
ie (believe); ey (honey, money); y (funny, pretty)

ER - er (mother, under); ir (fir, sir); ur (burn, fur)

F - f or ff (fan, fit, stuff); gh (enough, rough), ph (graph, phone)

IE - i (child, mind); i + final e (fine, like); ie (lie, tie); y (dry, my); igh (fight, sigh); ye (eye, rye)

J - j (jam, jet); g + e, i, y (gem, giraffe, technology); dg + e (edge, judge)

N - n (name, no); kn (knee, know); gn (gnome, design)

OA - o (no, so); o + final e (bone, rope); oa (boat, coat); oe (doe, foe); ou (soul); ow (low, row)

OO (long) - o (do, to); oo (food, moon); u + final e (rule, tune); ue (clue, due); ew (flew, new)

OO (short) - oo (cook, foot); ou (could, would); u (put)

OU - ou (cloud, loud); ow (now, town)

OY - oi (coin, toil); oy (boy, toy)

R - r (ran, rip); wr (write, wrong)

UH - o (from, stomach); ou + gh (enough, rough); u (cup, stuck)

S - s or ss (say, miss, sip); ps (psychic, psychology); sc (science, muscle); st (listen, whistle);


c + e, i, y (race, pencil, fancy)

YOO - u (unique, unit); u + final e (cute, distribute); ue (argue, cue), ew (few, spew)

18
The Importance of Nonsense Words

One common criticism of phonics programs is that they often require students to
read nonsense syllables in addition to real words. In contrast, three-cueing and
Whole-Language-based approaches are based on the assumption that beginning
readers benefit most strongly from focusing on "authentic" texts.

As discussed, this view fails to distinguish between how novices and experts
read. The brains of skilled readers have already encoded numerous sound-letter
combinations for automatic retrieval. Although these individuals can sound out
familiar words, they do not need to go through the process of laboriously doing
so each time they read them. And when they encounter new words, they can
quickly sound them out by "chunking" them into familiar syllables and stringing
them together.

In some cases, the syllables that make up a word may themselves be real words
(e.g. car-pet), but in many cases they are not (e.g., shi-ver-ing).

A child who cannot read nonsense syllables in isolation cannot then string
those syllables together to sound out thousands of words. In fact, they will
have no reliable means to break down unfamiliar vocabulary into
manageable chunks and will essentially be forced to guess.

Novice readers do not yet possess these automatic sound-syllable associations.


In order to develop a "store" of them and be able to read new words quickly, they
must be able to decode isolated syllables, whether or not they exist
independently as words.

To be clear, however: reading nonsense syllables should not be the aim of


reading instruction but rather a side effect of learning to decode
phonetically.

19
Morphemes are the smallest meaningful units of speech. A morpheme can be:

A prefix (ex-, pro-, un-)

A suffix (-ed, -ing, -s)

A root or main part of a word (cat, graph, zoo)

A word itself can be a morpheme, or a word can be made up of multiple


morphemes. Note that the number of morphemes in a word can be different from
the number of syllables.

The one-syllable word dog also has one morpheme: dog.

The one-syllable word dogs has two morphemes: dog and -s.

Morphemes are very important for beginning readers because the most common
ones recur with extremely high frequency in virtually all texts and can be learned
as "chunks" to be recognized automatically instead of re-sounded-out each time
they are encountered. (See the list on the following page.)

Note that children must be able to hear morphemes correctly in order to


connect them to print. A child who, for example, is accustomed to hearing the
ending -ed dropped in speech must have their attention explicitly called to that
pattern and may need to be taught basic concepts about verbs and tense.
Listening, spelling, reading, and grammar are all intertwined.

20
Common Prefixes and Suffixes

Prefixes Suffixes
ante - before Nouns Adjectives

anti - against age - courage able - durable

er(s) - container(s)
dis - not al - comical

ment - apartment
co - with ary - ordinary
ism - criticism
im - not er - longer
ity - activity
est - longest
micro - small
or(s) - actor(s)
ful - beautiful
mid - middle s - books
ible - terrible
multi - many sion - confusion
ic - fantistic
non - not tion - reaction

(i)ous - anxious
ude - solitude
pre - before
Verbs ish - bluish
post - after
ate -create ive - active
pro - in favor of
fy - beautify y - pretty
re- again
ed - walked

sub - below
ing - reading

trans - through ize - realize

un - not s - speaks

21
VII. Sight Words

Technically speaking, a "sight word" is a word that a reader recognizes and


processes instantaneously, without conscious effort. In most discussions of
reading, however, sight words are short, high-frequency words that, if memorized,
allow children to read a significant percentage of most simple texts.

As discussed, English contains many common words with irregular


pronunciations (e.g., the, was, are) that cannot be sounded out phonetically.

A common practice, however, is to treat all short, simple, high-frequency words


as sight words, even if they are phonetic (e.g., name, in, and get). While this
appraoch may sometimes be justified, for example if students are reading a book
that contains a few words whose letter-sound correspondences they have not yet
learned, broadly failing to distinguish between “little” words that are and are not
phonetic deprives students of an important opportunity to acquire transferable
knowledge and to "map" common sound-syllable relationships.

To take a very simple example, students who memorize at as a sight word may
not automatically make the connection to cat, sat, fat, rat, and mat, and later to
more complex words containing that pattern.

As a general rule, the focus should be on emphasizing patterns that can be


easily applied to new material. If a word has “transfer” potential—that is,
rhymes—it should not be learned exclusively by sight as a whole.

In addition, note that many common words are only partially irregular; most
frequently, they contain only an irregular vowel sound. The regular, phonetic
aspects of these words can and should still be emphasized. For example, the
sole irregularity in the word from involves the "o" vowel; the consonants make
their normal sounds.

22
Irregular Words* Phonetic Words

As All - mall, fall, tall


Are An - can, ran, ban
Be At - sat, cat, rat, mat
Because Cry - dry, fry, try, by
Been Dark - park, mark
Come For - nor, or
Does Fun - bun, run, sun
From In - bin, pin, tin
Give It - bit, fit, kit, sit
Have Late - date, late, rate
Live Like - hike, like, pike
Of Name - lame, tame
One Not - got, hot, not
Pretty Old - bold, cold, mold
Son Red - bed, fed, led
This Think - drink, sink, think
Two Too - boo, coo, moo
What Well - fell, sell, tell
You Wet - bet, met, pet

To reiterate, virtually all of these words have phonetic components that should be
emphasized when they are taught.
23
VIII. Speed and Fluency

To become proficient readers, children must be able to recognize words quickly


as well as accurately. Otherwise, by the time they arrive at the end of a sentence
or paragraph, they have already forgotten the beginning. Reading too slowly to
understand a text is not proficient reading, even if the individual words are
identified accurately. Providing a slow reader with extra time is thus unlikely to
solve the comprehension problem. To a certain point, speed is skill.

Approximate reading speeds indicating proficiency:

K-3: 140 words per minute


Middle School: 170-240 words per minute
Adult: 200-300 words per minute

Fluency refers to a reader's ability to connect written to spoken language—that


is, to read at the speed of speech, with conversational intonation (prosody) and
attention to punctuation. For a reader to achieve fluency, all the sub-skills
(phonemic awareness, syllable- and word-level recognition) must be
mastered to the point of automaticity.

Skilled readers read quickly by default and speed up or slow down as


necessary. Speed is the result of proficiency, not the cause.

Consequently, insisting that children read quickly for the sake of doing so, without
addressing the other factors, will not improve their prosody, comprehension, or
accuracy. In fact, it will likely cause them to decline.

To be fully effective, a reading program must address speed


and fluency as well as letter-sound relationships.

24
Building Fluency

Prosody can essentially be viewed as a window into comprehension and ease of


processing. A child who reads with appropriate intonation must be understanding
what they are reading for the simple reason that it is impossible to interpret a text
vocally otherwise.

If you listen to beginning or struggling readers' rendition of a text, however, you


will generally notice a distinct lack of prosody. Instead, readers may speak in a
monotone, ignore punctuation cues, and group words in illogical phrases. For
example, the sentence The black dog jumped over the fence might be read as
The black... dog jumped over... the... fence.

Although this type of reading can largely be attributed to the mental exertion of
decoding, some children—particularly ones with speech or general language
difficulties—may simply not intuitively make the connection between spoken and
written language. In such cases, teachers must explicitly reinforce the idea that
the symbols on the page can be interpreted in a way consistent with normal
speech. If a child cannot read aloud with natural intonation, they almost certainly
cannot hear it in their mind when they read silently, hindering comprehension.

Fluency can be modeled in a variety of ways: expressive read-alouds with


attention called to the relationship between phrases, punctuation, and tone of
voice; reading a short passage to a student and then having them read it aloud
back; or, for more advanced children, having them read easier texts aloud to
younger children. To be very clear: fluency exercises should never be a
substitute for phonetic decoding, but rather a complement to it.

The importance of being able to read a text silently while hearing it in


one's mind should not be underestimated. As texts get more
sophisticated, this ability becomes crucial for recognizing situations
involving humor/sarcasm and other non-literal meanings.

25
IX. The Reading-Spelling Connection

Although reading is often treated as a skill distinct from spelling, in reality


they are two sides of the same coin. In fact, understanding how students
(mis)spell can provide important insights into how students process letters.

Phonetic Misspelling

Although a progressive-sounding method like inventive spelling might seem


more likely to be associated with the Whole-Language movement, it can actually
be an important technique for children learning to decode phonetically.

Because written English does have so many spelling irregularities, it is crucial that
beginning readers be given the opportunity to practice literally applying the
patterns they have learned and solidifying the relationship between letters and
sounds. Obviously, teachers do eventually need to begin insisting on correct
spelling, and persistent difficulties should be evaluated as necessary; however, in
the very early grades, phonetic-based spelling errors (e.g., writing ackshun
instead of action or riseve instead of receive) may actually be a positive
sign, indicating that a child is learning to match sounds to spellings in a
logical way.

Phonemic Misspelling

Some children may also misspell words because they cannot accurately
identify certain sounds. This problem frequently involves vowels, but
consonants can be problematic as well.

For example, it is reasonable to infer that a student who writes flessibol instead of
flexible or way instead of why in an attempt to spell phonetically may have
difficulty identifying the "x" and long "i" sounds. If the student's spelling is
corrected without their attention being drawn to the sounds contained in those
words, the underlying problem is likely to persist.

26
Memorization-Based Misspelling

On the other hand, the errors of children who have learned to read through a
combination of memorizing and guessing are likely to be far more random
and illogical. They may, for instance, write wrong as wonrg because they have
memorized by rote rather than by learning that "wr" makes an "r" sound, and are
unable to accurately remember the order of the letters. Or they may write frezze
instead of freeze because they remember that one of the letters in the middle of
the word is supposed to be written twice but do not connect the "ee" sound to the
doubled vowel. Although the error may seem minor, it actually suggests a
fundamental lack of understanding.

Likewise, students who have not learned to pay attention to sound-letter


correspondences may spell the same word multiple ways within the same
sentence or paragraph; a correct spelling may simply represent a lucky guess.

When children display this kind of haphazard attention to written language,


inventive spelling is of little use and may be harmful because it reinforces the idea
that words are not spelled the way they are for any particular reason.

In practice, of course, a student's writing can display multiple types of errors, and
you must be able to distinguish between them in order to determine the
underlying points of confusion as well as what sort of intervention is required.

On the next page, we're going to look at an excerpt from an essay written by a
fourth grader. (It's based on the question, "In your opinion, what is the most
important invention?")

27
(All inventions are useful in life the lightbulb, car, plane, and computer. But to me soler power is the best.
One resson is that some cars are powerd by soler power. Solor power cars can make us stop useing fosol
fules so much. NASA is makeing solor powered cars. Another resson is that hoase are useing solor power.
Solor powered hoases can stop us from a lot of power. Peope are adding soler panlans to their homes.)
https://tea.texas.gov/sites/default/files/2019_STAAR_G4_Writing_Scoring_Guide.pdf words per

Although this piece contains some straightforward phonetic misspellings (solor,


powerd, fosol), it also displays a host of other issues suggesting a need for
remediation on multiple levels.

The spelling of reason as resson, for example, suggests that the writer either has
difficulty identifying the long "e" sound or is spelling in part by memorization (a
phonetic misspelling would be something like reeson or reasun). The misspelling
of houses as hoase could also point to a phonemic awareness issue, or it could
indicate that the student learned to spell house by memory and cannot remember
which vowel follows the "o". Moreover, the missing "s" in that word as well as the
dropped "e" in powerd indicate the student is struggling with suffixes as well.
Finally, the misspelling of panels as panlans indicates a significant deficit in
understanding sound-syllable correspondences.

28
Reversing Letters

It is common for young children to reverse similar-looking letters, e.g., "b" and "d".
While this type of error is often taken as a sign of a potential reading disorder,
many children without serious issues do this through second grade.

It is important to keep in mind that letters are ultimately arbitrary symbols: there is
no particular reason why a straight line with a circle attached to the bottom right-
hand side represents the first sound in boy, other than the fact that the English
alphabet happened to evolve that way.

Children beginning to read, however, may take their cues about letters from their
understanding of how physical objects behave. They know that if, say, a chair or
a spoon is flipped upside down, it does not stop being a chair or a spoon. They
may then apply the same principle to letters, not grasping the idea that a "b"
becomes a "d" when the circle is moved to the other side of the line, or a "p" when
it is moved to the top. Furthermore, more English letters face right than left,
leading children to treat right-facing letters as the default option.

One way to address this issue is to play a "game" in which you rotate/flip a letter
in different directions, and ask the child to say when it is and is not the letter.
It is important to begin this exercise using only one letter/sound at a time,
because working with multiple letters simultaneously is likely to place too
much of a strain on the child's working memory and add to their
confusion. As the child becomes more adept at recognizing individual letters,
you can gradually add more letters into the mix.

You can also try the "b-hand" method shown by Linda Farrell of Readsters in this
wonderful video:

https://www.readingrockets.org/shows/reading-interventions/letter-reversals-b-d-
with-aiko-second-grade

29
X. Dyslexia

Dyslexia is a brain-based disorder characterized by difficulty perceiving and


discriminating between sounds, e.g., "ch" and "sh" (phonological
awareness). It can also involve trouble retrieving known words from
memory. Individuals with dyslexia typically struggle to match sounds to letters,
decode accurately and fluently, and spell correctly. Diagnostic criteria vary, but
the percentage of the population that is dyslexic is estimated at around 10%.

One of the hallmarks of dyslexia is that these problems are generally


inconsistent with a person's overall level of intelligence; they are not
predicted by difficulties in other areas.

Despite this knowledge, dyslexia remains poorly understood by many members of


the education community and continues to be surrounded by myths.

Dyslexia Myths

Dyslexia is primarily a visual problem characterized by switching letters


and/or writing them backwards.

Dyslexia is a disorder that children naturally grow out of.

Dyslexia cannot be diagnosed until after children have learned to read.

Dyslexics are just lazy and/or unintelligent.

Poor reading skills cause dyslexia.

Phonics alone can cure dyslexia.

Dyslexia does not exist.

30
Dyslexia and Phonics

While children with dyslexia will struggle disproportionately in reading programs


based on the assumption that children will naturally intuit sound-letter
relationships, or that teach phonics in a haphazard way (e.g., by calling attention
to letter-sound relationships only when children struggle with a particular word),
they also have trouble in highly systematic phonics programs that are letter-based
from the start.

The underlying problem in dyslexia primarily involves the ability to process


sounds, and it is impossible to match letters to sounds that one cannot
distinguish. As a result, remediation for children with dyslexia must begin by
systematically teaching them to identify sounds.

Although dyslexia becomes apparent when children begin learning to read,


dyslexics show processing differences when compared to non-dyslexics long
before they enter school and can be identified via aural letter-naming,
phoneme/rhyme-identification, and blending/segmenting tasks well before they
begin to experience difficulties with print. Early detection is important because it
can help avert years of unnecessary struggle.

Dyslexia vs. "Dysteachia"

Note that children who are taught to read poorly may display some of the same
compensatory behaviors as dyslexics (skipping, guessing, relying on images) but
do not actually have the underlying problem with phonological awareness: if they
are given appropriate remediation, they will pick up sound-letter correspondences
much more quickly and easily, and will require far less repetition to hear subtle
distinctions between sounds.

For more information about dyslexia, see: https://www.mayoclinic.org/diseases-


conditions/dyslexia/symptoms-causes/syc-20353552

31
Sources

Adams, Marilyn J. "The Three Cueing System." 1998 https://phonicsintervention.org/2017/01/16/marilyn-jager-


adams-three-cueing-system-origins-tragedy-described-summary/

-----"Improve the Quality of Learning" (Interview with Siegfried Engelmann).


https://childrenofthecode.org/interviews/engelmann2.htm

Ehri, Linnea. "Development of Sight Word Reading: Phases and Findings." 2005.
http://www.pitt.edu/~perfetti/PDF/Ehri.pdf

Gough, Philip; Tunmer, William. "Decoding, Reading, and Reading Disability." RASE 7(1), 6-10 (1986).
http://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/viewdoc/download?doi=10.1.1.905.7606&rep=rep1&type=pdf

Hanford, Emily. "How a Flawed Idea Is Teaching Millions of Kids to Be Poor Readers." American Public Media,
August 22, 2019. https://www.apmreports.org/story/2019/08/22/whats-wrong-how-schools-teach-reading

-----"Why Aren't Kids Being Taught to Read?" American Public Media, September 10, 2018.
https://www.apmreports.org/story/2018/09/10/hard-words-why-american-kids-arent-being-taught-to-read

Lyon, G. Reid. "Converging Evidence—Reading Research What It Takes to Read (Interview). Reading Rockets,
https://childrenofthecode.org/interviews/lyon.htm

Moats, Louisa. "How Spelling Supports Reading." American Educator, Winter 2005/06, 12-43.
https://www.readingrockets.org/article/how-spelling-supports-reading

---"Teaching Teachers to Teach Reading" (Interview). Children of the Code, October 30,
2003. https://childrenofthecode.org/interviews/moats.htm

Moats, Louisa; Tolman, Carol. "English Gets a Bad Rap," 2009. Excerpted from Language Essentials for
Teachers of Reading and Spelling (LETRS): Spellography for Teachers: How English Spelling Works (Module 3).
Boston: Sopris West. https://www.readingrockets.org/article/english-gets-bad-rap

Seidenberg, Mark. "Blue Cell Dyslexia." October 27, 2017 @ 12:53 am. https://languagelog.ldc.upenn.edu/nll/?
p=35144

-----"This is why we don’t have better readers: Response to Lucy Calkins." Reading Matters, December 6, 2019.
https://seidenbergreading.net/2019/12/06/lucy-calkins-on-the-attack/

Young, Nancy. "The Ladder of Reading" (Infographic), 2012, updated 2017. https://dyslexiaida.org/ladder-of-
reading-infographic-structured-literacy-helps-all-students/. (Statistics based on https://dyslexiaida.org/wp-
content/uploads/2018/07/the-ladder-of-reading-statistics-2.pdf)

32
For more information about
teaching reading, or to find out
about instructor training, please
visit www.breakingthecode.com

33

You might also like

pFad - Phonifier reborn

Pfad - The Proxy pFad of © 2024 Garber Painting. All rights reserved.

Note: This service is not intended for secure transactions such as banking, social media, email, or purchasing. Use at your own risk. We assume no liability whatsoever for broken pages.


Alternative Proxies:

Alternative Proxy

pFad Proxy

pFad v3 Proxy

pFad v4 Proxy