Scam
Scam
Funding:
Acknowledgments
The authors recognize the FINRA Investor Education Foundation for sponsoring this project, as
well as Melissa Trumpower, Rubens Pessanha, Craig Honik and Emma Fletcher for
collaborating on the survey design and data collection. We are grateful to the Better Business
Bureau’s Institute for Marketplace Trust for providing access to consumers who reported a scam
to BBB Scam Tracker. Thank you to the American and Canadian consumers who responded to
the survey and provided details about their experience. We sincerely appreciate their
commitment to raising awareness and protecting fellow consumers against scams.
Consumer fraud reports in the United States have increased each year along with median
losses. Using survey data from 1,175 American and Canadian consumers who reported a scam to
a North American consumer complaint organization, this study examines the correlates of
engaging with (i.e., responding to) and complying with three types of consumer fraud:
opportunity-based scams, threat-based scams, and consumer purchase scams. Consumers were
less likely to engage with and lose money in threat-based scam solicitations relative to
opportunity-based and consumer purchase scams. Risk factors, including household income,
loneliness, financial fragility, and financial literacy, varied across scam categories. Different risk
factors were associated with engaging in the scam than were associated with actually losing
money. Having advance knowledge of fraud prior to being targeted was protective across scam
types. Results derived from this unique data set that combined fraud reports from a consumer
complaint organization with survey responses suggest that education about specific scams is
Keywords: consumer education, deception, financial fragility, financial literacy, fraud, fraud
Introduction
Consumer fraud is an international crime that results in billions of dollars in losses each
concealing, or omitting facts about promised goods, services, or other benefits and consequences
that are nonexistent, unnecessary, never intended to be provided, or deliberately distorted for the
purpose of monetary gain” (Beals, DeLiema, & Deevy, 2015, pg. 7). In the United States, an
estimated 15.9% consumers fell victim to a scam in 2017 (Anderson, 2019). A total of $906
million in losses were reported that year by those who submitted a fraud complaint to consumer
protection agencies (Federal Trade Commission (FTC), 2018). Reported losses more than
doubled to $1.9 billion just two years later (FTC, 2020), yet the majority of incidents are never
reported to authorities or acknowledged by victims in surveys (DeLiema, Shadel, & Pak, 2020;
Raval, 2020). Researchers have estimated that true fraud losses are between $40 and $50 billion
COVID-19 has exacerbated financial scams by creating an environment ripe for fraud
(Ma & McKinnon, 2020). Scammers capitalize on economic disruption, fear, and misinformation
to deceive their targets (Balleisen, 2018). The pandemic economy has deepened financial
insecurity for many, and fear and uncertainty permeate the globe as nations struggle to control
the disease and respond to high levels of unemployment. Social distancing brought on by the
pandemic has increased loneliness for many Americans, particularly the elderly, which as prior
work suggests, may lead to increased fraud susceptibility. To safeguard those who are most
vulnerable, consumer protection agencies and advocacy organizations need guidance on where to
Better Business Bureau, this study examines the demographic, psychological, and contextual
factors associated with responding to a scam and losing money. We test whether risk factors
differ based on the type of scam that was reported. Frauds are categorized as either (1)
opportunity-based scams involving positive financial or social rewards, (2) threat-based scams
involving harmful threats of negative consequences, or (3) consumer purchase scams involving
fake or unnecessary products and services. The goal is to inform more targeted consumer
Age
Consumer fraud complaint data and random sample surveys typically find that young and
middle-aged adults are more likely than older adults to report fraud (Anderson, 2019; FTC
2020). However, studies that assess behaviors associated with scam susceptibility find that
susceptibility increases with age-related declines in cognitive functioning and financial decision-
making (Han et al., 2016a; 2016b). More research is needed to determine whether the
relationship between age and fraud victimization varies across scam types which may help
Social isolation—the extent to which a person spends their time alone—and loneliness,
an emotional state in which a person subjectively feels alone (Holt-Lunstad et al., 2015), are
hypothesized risk factors for fraud. A recent study found that loneliness was associated with
poorer financial and healthcare decision-making, but only among subjects with low cognitive
ability (Stewart et al., 2020). Qualitative studies also suggest a link between loneliness, social
isolation, and fraud victimization (Alves & Wilson, 2008; Cross, 2016; Lee & Soberon-Ferrer,
1997). Lonely consumers may be particularly susceptible to scams that offer a chance to feel
special or important, such as lottery and sweepstakes scams, or the feeling of being loved and
needed, such as romance scams (Buchanan & Whitty, 2014). Scammers also use social isolation
as a tactic to actively manipulate targets away from trusted friends and family members who
could keep them safe (DeLiema, 2018). In the “grandparent scam” and government imposter
scams, for example, scammers instruct their targets to keep the interaction confidential to stop
them from seeking advice or a second opinion that could foil the scheme. The extent to which
loneliness and isolation increase susceptibility to different categories of scams is not known.
Cacioppo, 1986), individuals process persuasive information through either a “central route” that
involves dedicating attentional resources to carefully scrutinize and cross-reference the message
with prior knowledge, or a “peripheral route” which involves using heuristic shortcuts and
reliance on basic cues to judge the legitimacy of the message. Incidental affect—sadness,
excitement, etc.—can influence whether a persuasion message is processed through the central or
peripheral route (Petty & Briñol, 2015). In decision-making tasks in the laboratory, incidental
emotions have been shown to produce peripheral thinking and poor decision outcomes. For
example, Duclos, Wan, and Jiang (2013) found that subjects who felt isolated or ostracized were
more likely to pursue risky decisions that had a higher potential payout, and Baumeister and
colleagues (2002) showed that social exclusion produces a decline in cognitive performance. In
another study, subjects who were manipulated to feel sad were more impatient and chose to
accept an immediate payout of less money instead of waiting to receive more money in the future
Many frauds incorporate powerful visceral appeals to put the target in a state of high
emotional arousal and inhibit them from carefully scrutinizing the offer or request. Some scams
use positive emotional arousal, promising the target an opportunity to make money, find
romance, or get out of debt easily. Other scams use fear to convince the target that they must pay
money in order to avoid a negative consequence, such as a computer virus, legal problems, or to
get a loved one out of serious trouble. Kircanski et al. (2018) found older adults in positive and
negative arousal states were more likely to want to purchase a product in a misleading
advertisement than older adults in a neutral emotional state. More research is needed to identify
how these emotions influence decision-making in an applied high-risk consumer context, such as
the choice to comply with a scam, and whether positive emotions—generated by the promise of
Financial Literacy
Financial literacy is a broad concept which assess one’s “ability to use knowledge and
skills to manage financial resources effectively for lifetime financial security” (Goyal & Kumar,
2020, p. 81). The consequences of poor financial literacy are serious in other financial decision-
making contexts such as saving, investing, and managing debt (Lusardi, 2012). Researchers have
proposed that poor financial literacy plays a role in fraud susceptibility as well, but findings are
mixed. Pak and Shadel (2011) found that older lottery fraud victims scored poorly on a test of
financial literacy whereas investment fraud victims scored significantly higher than the general
U.S. population age 50 and older. Even within the category of investment scams, findings are
mixed. DeLiema et al. (2018) found that financial literacy was negatively associated with
investing with an unknown person, but positively associated with investing based on a free meal
seminar, while Kieffer and Mottola (2017) found that financial literacy was positively associated
with investment fraud victimization. More investigation is needed to determine how actual and
Financial Fragility
Many scammers use phantom fixation—dangling the promise of rewards—to entice their
targets to comply. “Get rich quick” schemes and other scams that promise a windfall of money
or an easy way to get rid of debt may appeal specifically to those facing financial hardship.
Research is needed to determine whether the condition of being financially fragile – having little
to no access to funds to cover an unexpected emergency expense – is a risk factor for engaging
with and losing money in scams, particularly scams that involve attractive financial
opportunities.
Prior Victimization
Titus and Gover (2001) found that becoming a victim of fraud increases the risk of future
victimization. One reason is that victims’ contact information is added to “sucker” lists and then
sold to other scam artists (FTC, 2016), resulting in increased targeting. However, some victims
might become more vigilant in future consumer interactions after losing money in a scam.
Further study is needed to determine the relationship between past victimization and the
Scam Knowledge
whether efforts to educate and inform the public about fraud are effective. According to the
Persuasion Knowledge Model (Campbell & Kirmani, 2000; Friestad & Wright, 1994), a target’s
assessment of a sales interaction depends on his or her prior knowledge about the product or
offer, ability to recognize persuasion attempts, and beliefs about the influencer’s motives.
Knowing about a scam may stop a person from responding to a solicitation, but if they do
engage, they encounter additional persuasion tactics that make it challenging to withdraw from
the interaction. Common persuasion tactics scammers use include scarcity (the pressure to act
right away), liking (presenting oneself as kind and friendly and thus harder to refuse), and
(Cialdini, 2007; DeLiema, Yon, & Wilber, 2016). No studies have assessed whether consumers’
psychological responses to common persuasion tactics differ by scam type or to what extent they
limitations. First, victim characteristics appear to differ by scam type (see Pak & Shadel, 2011),
indicating that different types of people are uniquely susceptible to specific forms of fraud.
Studies that group victims of very different scams together in statistical models (e.g., Schoepfer
& Piquero, 2009; Titus, Heinzelmann, & Boyle, 1995) can obscure victim risk factors that vary
by fraud type. For consumer education to be effective at protecting the most vulnerable
consumers, risk factors must be analyzed separately based on the category of the scam.
Another limitation is that aside from a handful of surveys (see AARP Foundation (2003),
Financial Industry Regulatory Authority (2006), FINRA Foundation (2007), DeLiema, Pak, &
Shadel (2020)), the majority of victim profiling studies use general population samples where
survey respondents are asked to self-report fraud victimization (e.g., Anderson, 2004, 2013,
2019; FINRA, 2013; Schoepfer & Piquero, 2009; Titus, Heinzelmann, & Boyle, 1995). The
problem with this approach is that fraud is under-acknowledged and underreported in survey
research, likely due to social-desirability bias. This problem is illustrated by studies surveying
independently identified fraud victims (“known” victims) using samples provided by law
enforcement agencies or victim lists. In the Senior Fraud Risk Survey, FINRA Foundation
(2007) found that only half of 101 known victims admitted losing money after being misled or
defrauded by a broker. This finding is similar to that of DeLiema, Shadel, and Pak (2020) who
found only 48% of known investment fraud victims admitted that they invested and lost money
in a scam. Pak and Shadel (2011) found that only 36.9% of known victims age 55 and older
non-victims.
The third limitation of prior research is that it fails to differentiate non-victims who were
targeted by a scam but did not comply from those who were never targeted in the first place. The
latter group of non-victims may share characteristics with victims but were never presented an
opportunity to engage or pay money. One survey study found that as targeting increased so did
investment fraud victimization (Kieffer et al., 2017), but overall, little is known about the role of
victimization in surveys and failing to separate risk factors by scam type, limits our
Study Purpose
The present study addresses these limitations by surveying 1,175 consumers who were all
targeted by scams, and of these targets, nearly a quarter lost money. These consumers reported
the incident to the Better Business Bureau (BBB). Reports were reviewed by BBB staff and
confirmed as fraud prior to being publicly posted on the BBB Scam Tracker website. This is the
first study in the U.S. to survey individuals who previously filed a fraud report with a consumer
protection organization. The advantage of surveying this population is that the study controls for
non-victims. Another advantage is that a range of different scams are reported, allowing risk
factors to be assessed separately by the category of scam. We identify the correlates of engaging
with and losing money across three major categories of fraud—opportunity-based, threat-based,
and consumer purchase scams, determining how risk factors vary by scam type and whether
there are consistent characteristics that offer promising opportunities for consumer protection
and education.
Methods
In 2017 and 2018, the FINRA Investor Education Foundation, in concert with BBB
Institute for Marketplace Trust and the Stanford Center on Longevity, sponsored and conducted a
10
study to uncover the process of fraud victimization and understand the factors associated with
losing money. To inform survey development, 18 in-depth interviews were conducted with
individuals who were targeted by fraud. Half of these participants lost money and half did not.
Interviews revealed situational characteristics of the scam encounters and surfaced the personal
knowledge, beliefs, and values of the targets themselves. Interview findings were used to inform
The 89-item online survey was fielded in August 2018. More than 90,000 individuals
who resided in the U.S. and Canada were invited by email to participate in a 15-minute survey
seeking to understand why people are targeted for scams. All email recipients had submitted a
fraud report between 2015 and 2018 using the BBB Scam Tracker website
(BBB.org/ScamTracker), an online reporting tool for consumers to inform the BBB and the
general public about scams they experienced. A reminder email was sent one week later yielding
Before entering the survey, participants read an online consent form and agreed to
participate. The study protocol was reviewed and approved by Sterling IRB, ID6442. No
personally identifying information was collected. Respondents who initially submitted a fraud
report to BBB Scam Tracker on behalf of someone else were discontinued because they were not
Survey Items
questions about the scam that was reported to BBB Scam Tracker. Respondents could select
from a dropdown list of the 12 most common reported scams or could select “other” and self-
11
describe the scam they experienced. These descriptions were read and categorized by a member
of the research team, generating a new total of 27 distinct scam types (including the original 12).
Scams were divided into three categories based on the primary “story” the scammers
used to convince targets to comply with the solicitation. Opportunity-based scams (n=387) are
frauds that promise the target something positive and rewarding—lottery winnings, a prize, a
chance to earn significant money either through a job opportunity, grant, or investment; the
chance to reduce or eradicate debt; or the promise of romance/partnership. Scam types in this
loan, fake check/money order, debt consolidation, secret shopper, and sweetheart/romance
scams. Threat-based scams (n=562) are frauds in which scammers convince the target that
something bad has happened and they must pay money to avoid a negative consequence. These
frauds include bogus tax collection, government impostor, debt collection, tech support,
grandparent scam, extortion, and unnecessary home repairs. Consumer purchase scams (n=226)
involve paying for products and services that do not exist or that were intentionally
misrepresented to prospective buyers. They include online marketplace fraud, pet adoption,
health insurance enrollment, bogus charities, and other general consumer scams like billing for
non-existent subscriptions. A final group of reported scams were uncategorizable because the
respondent did not provide sufficient information for assignment or because the target was a
business and not an individual. The majority of uncategorizable scams were email or website
phishing attempts (n=200), followed by fake invoices, credit card fraud, and all scams recorded
as “other”. A total of 232 uncategorizable reports were removed from the analysis.
Survey respondents next answered scam-specific questions that included the method of
contact, whether they engaged or interacted with the scammer/solicitation (versus ended the
12
interaction immediately), whether they lost money (became victims), and if so, the dollar amount
of direct losses. They were asked whether they knew about the specific scam before they were
targeted, such as from friends/family, the news, and social media, and whether they have lost
Respondents who engaged with the scam were asked whether anyone had tried to
intervene and the extent to which they agreed with 14 items that characterize many consumer
interactions with fraud, including targets’ perceptions of the situation, their personal
circumstances at the time, and their emotions. Examples include, “I felt under time pressure,” “I
felt that I had an opportunity to get ahead financially,” “There was no one available to talk to
about the offer at the time,” and “I thought the person was nice.” Items were rated on a scale
from 1 (completely disagree) to 7 (completely agree). An exploratory factor analysis (EFA) was
conducted to examine the factor structure that emerged from these series of 14 questions and
In addition to questions specific to the scam interaction, the survey included items that
measured psychological and financial characteristics that may increase the risk of scam
engagement and victimization. Loneliness was assessed using 3-items from Hughes, Waite,
Hawkley and Cacioppo (2004). Participants are asked how often they (1) lack companionship,
(2) feel left out, and (3) feel isolated from others, where 1=often; 2=some of the time; and
3=hardly ever or never. Items were reverse scored. Total loneliness was calculated by summing
expense (Hasler, Lusardi & Oggero, 2018). Participants were asked if they would be able to
come up with $2,000 if an unexpected need arose in the next month. The item was dichotomized
13
such that those who said they probably could not or certainly could not come up with $2,000
were coded as financially fragile. Financial literacy was scored by summing the number of
correct responses to five multiple choice and true/false questions that assess fundamental
personal finance concepts, including compound interest, inflation, and diversification (Lusardi,
Mitchell, & Curto, 2014). Responses ranged from 0 (none correct) to 5 (all correct). Self-rated
financial competency was measured by asking respondents to rate the extent to which they are
good at dealing with day-to-day financial matters, such as checking accounts, credit and debit
Analytic Approach
Principal axis factoring with a promax rotation was used to identify relationships between
14 items that measured the consumer’s perception of the scam interaction, their feelings, beliefs,
and other situational characteristics specific to the interaction. These questions were only asked
of those who engaged (n=673). The Keiser-Meyer-Olkin (KMO) measure of sampling adequacy
(0.737) and Bartlett’s test of Sphericity (p < .001) suggest that the sample was appropriate for
EFA (Howell, 2012; Leech, Barrett, & Morgan, 2005). Items with low communalities (< .30)
and factor loadings less than 0.50 were eliminated from the final factor solution (Fabrigar et al.,
1999).
Using Mplus 8 statistical computing software, logistic regression was used to estimate the
correlates of engaging in a scam (engaging=1, N=1,175), and separately, losing money in a scam
(victim=1, N=1,150). Separate analyses were performed for each category of scam reported:
opportunity-based scams (N=387), threat-based scams (N=562), and consumer purchase scams
(N=226); in addition to models that included all respondents together. Independent variables
included sex (male=1), age (continuous), race (non-Latino White=1), education (high school or
14
less (reference), some college/associate degree, bachelor’s degree or higher), annual household
than $100,000), loneliness (range =3-9), financially fragile (yes=1), financial literacy score
(range 0-5), self-rated financial competency (range 0-7), whether the consumer heard about the
exact scam before being targeted (yes=1), and whether they had lost money in a prior scam
(yes=1). Models including all respondents controlled for scam type, where opportunity-based
In another set of models assessing risk of victimization, the sample is restricted to only
those who engaged with the scammer or scam solicitation (N=673) to determine whether
characteristics unique to the interaction drive compliance. These models included correlates
specific to the interaction and how the target felt about and interpreted the situation. Items
include the extent to which the target was “opportunity seeking” and “felt intimidated” (both
factor scores), as well the extent to which they “chose not to discuss it [the offer/threat] with
anyone,” “no one available to discuss it [the offer/threat] with,” “had little knowledge about the
“person/organization knew personal details about me,” and “someone tried to intervene.”
Results
Demographic Characteristics
A total of 1,175 individuals were included in the current study. The majority of
participants were female (67%), 81% self-identified as white/Caucasian, and 60% were married
15
or partnered. Nearly half were 60 years and older (46%). Forty-six percent reported having
obtained a bachelor’s degree or higher, 42% work for an employer, and 48% and live in a
suburban area. Threat/fear-based scams (48%) were reported more often than opportunity-based
A total of 492 participants reported that they did not engage in the scam they reported to
BBB Scam Tracker. Participants who did not engage had slightly higher educational attainment
(43% bachelor’s or higher), were more likely to report working for an employer (43%), and
living in a suburban area (53%). Regarding the scam type, more than half of the participants who
reported threat/fear-based scams did not engage (68%) compared to minority of those who
Among those who engaged, 300 participants reported they were victims, meaning they
engaged and lost money. On average, victims lost $7,158.50 (Standard Deviation (SD)=
$58,435.70) per incident (median =$625.00). Those who engaged in the scams, including both
non-victims who engaged and victims who lost money, reported a higher percentage of
opportunity-based scams (44% and 37% respectively) and consumer purchase scams (22% and
31% respectively) compared to those who did not engage with the scam they reported.
Factor Analysis
Two factors emerged that explain 49% of the variance for the set of 14 items. Factor 1
was labeled as “I was seeking and opportunity” and had high loadings on the following
variables: (a) I felt that I had an opportunity to get ahead financially; (b) I felt that I had an
opportunity to make good on past mistakes; (c) I felt that it was "my time" and I deserved to be
rewarded; and (d) I worried about missing out on an opportunity. This factor explained 33% of
the variance and reliability (ɑ) was 0.78. Factor 2 was labeled “I felt intimidated” due to high
16
loadings on the following items: (a) I felt afraid of being punished for something I had done; (b)
I felt under time pressure; and (c) I felt intimidated by the person I was dealing with. This factor
explained 16% of the variance with ɑ = 0.71. See Appendix C for details.
Regression Results
Engaging with scams. Table 2 presents the correlates of engaging with fraud where
engaged is regressed on demographic, social, financial, and other potential risk and protective
factors (N=1,175). Relative to opportunity-based scams, individuals were nearly 70% less likely
to engage when targeted by a threat-based scam (e.g., government imposter, tech support) (Odds
ratio (OR)=0.32; 95% Confidence Interval (95%CI)=0.24, 0.44). Loneliness was a statistically
significant risk factor for engaging in all scam types combined (Model 1; OR=1.16,
95%CI=1.06, 1.26). Loneliness was also associated with engaging specifically in opportunity-
based scams—e.g., lottery, prize, investment, debt reduction, and romance scams (Model 2;
OR=1.20, 95%CI=1.02, 1.42). Among those who reported threat-based scams, college graduates
were half as likely to engage than those with a high school education or less (Model 3; OR=0.49,
95%CI=0.27, 0.88). The only statistically significant factor for consumer purchase scams was
having heard about the exact scam before being targeted (Model 4; OR=0.37, 95%CI=0.19,
0.73), which was also significantly protective against engaging with other categories of scams.
Individuals were between 43% and 85% less likely to engage if they knew about the scam
beforehand.
Losing money in scams. Regressing the same factors on risk on victimization (Table 3)
shows that among all respondents with data on victimization (n=1,150), loneliness is a
significant risk factor (Model 1; OR=1.15, 95%CI=1.05, 1.26), as is poor financial literacy
(Model 1; OR=0.84, 95%CI=0.76, 0.94). Those who were targeted by consumer purchase scams
17
were significantly more likely to lose money relative to opportunity-based scams (Model 1;
OR=2.19, 95%CI=1.48, 3.23). Respondents who were financially fragile were twice as likely to
lose money (Model 1; OR=2.01, 95%CI=1.33, 3.04) compared to those with access to $2,000 in
emergency funds. Those who knew about the specific scam before they were targeted were 75%
less likely to comply and lose money (Model 2; OR=0.25, 95%CI=0.17, 0.35).
Among those targeted by opportunity-based scams (Model 2), males were more likely to
likely to report victimization compared to those who were single, widowed, or divorced (Model
2; OR=0.46, 95%CI=0.25, 0.86). Individuals who reported a household income between $50,001
and $100,000 were more likely to comply with opportunity-based scams than those with incomes
of $25,00 or less (Model 2; OR=2.41, 95%CI=1.01, 5.78); but even controlling on income, being
financially fragile significantly increased risk of victimization by more than four times (Model 2;
OR=4.57, 95%CI=2.27, 9.20). Financial literacy was negatively associated with victimization by
opportunity-based scams (Model 2; OR=0.75, 95%CI=0.62, 0.92) and with losing money in
threat-based scams although the effect sizes are small (Model 3; OR=0.83, 95%CI=0.69, 0.99).
Despite lower financial literacy, self-rated financial competency was positively correlated with
Having prior knowledge of the exact scam was significantly protective for opportunity-
based scams (Model 2; OR=0.20, 95%CI=0.10, 0.41) and threat-based scams (Model 3;
OR=0.11, 95%CI=0.06, 0.21), but not consumer purchase scams (Model 4; OR=0.60,
95%CI=0.31, 1.16). The only significant risk factor for consumer purchase scam victimization
18
Losing money among those who engaged. Table 4 reports the results of regressing
victimization on risk and protective factors among only those who engaged with the scam
(N=673). These models include measures of the respondents’ assumptions, feelings, and
interpretations of the interaction while they were engaging. In Model 1 (all scam types), being
financially fragile was significantly associated with losing money following scam engagement
(Model 1; OR=1.86, 95%CI=1.16, 3.00). Knowing about the exact scam reduced risk of
victimization by nearly 40% (Model 1; OR=0.62, 95%CI=0.40, 0.97). Respondents who were
more likely to be seeking a financial opportunity at the time they were targeted were more likely
to lose money (Model 1; OR=1.35, 95%CI=1.03, 1.77). Choosing not to discuss the offer/threat
with others significantly increased the odds of losing money (Model 1; OR=1.19, 95%CI=1.09,
1.30), as did agreeing that the scammer or offer seemed official (Model 1; OR=1.41,
95%CI=1.26, 1.58). The more the respondent agreed that the scammer seemed nice, the higher
scams following engagement (Model 2; OR=2.74, 95%CI=1.37, 5.47), but not with other scam
types. Interestingly, feeling intimidated during the interaction was a risk factor for losing money
in opportunity-based scams (Model 2; OR=1.68, 95%CI=1.08, 2.62) but not threat-based scams
that are more likely to incorporate fear tactics. Among those who engaged, higher self-rated
financial competency was a significant risk factor for victimization, but only for threat-based
scams (Model 3; OR=1.40, 95%CI=1.03, 1.92). Having heard about the exact scam prior to
being targeted was still protective even after consumers engaged in opportunity-based and threat-
based scams, but not consumer purchase scams. Agreeing that the scammer or scam offer
19
seemed official was a significant risk factor for all categories of scams, increasing risk of
Discussion
The present study examines the correlates associated with both engaging in and losing
purchase scams. We uncovered which risk factors differ and which characteristics are common
across scam types. The study contributes to the literature in several ways. First, our findings are
not subject to underreporting bias as all participants acknowledged and reported a scam to BBB
Scam Tracker and had their entries reviewed and confirmed as scams by BBB staff. Second, this
study controls for targeting, solving a common survey limitation in which some respondents who
are classified as non-victims were never targeted to begin with. Third, we performed separate
purchase scams.
Relative to threat-based scams, consumers were significantly more likely to engage with
opportunity-based scams and were more likely to lose money following engagement. One
relative to non-victims, report these scams because it takes losing money for the consumer to
know they were deceived. Relatedly, more non-victims may choose to report targeting attempts
by threat-based scams because they felt intimidated, violated, and upset by the scammer’s
threats. These feelings may motivate them to contact a consumer protection organization despite
20
emotions have opposing effects on information processing. Positive emotions tend to promote
simple, Type II heuristic processing of a persuasion message whereas negative emotions tend to
facilitate more systematic scrutiny (see Griskevicius, Shiota, & Neufeld (2010)). In the context
of consumer fraud, this differential processing of persuasion messages suggests that positive
scams are more likely to result in a financial loss than negative and emotionally neutral scams.
Therefore, another possibility is that opportunity-based scams are simply more effective at
deceiving consumers because they play into desires for wealth, recognition, security, or
companionship. However, in a lab-based study that tested the impact of emotional arousal on
negative arousal (Kirkanski et al., 2018). Relative to participants in a neutral emotional state,
participants in both the positive and negative emotional states reported a greater intention to
purchase falsely advertised items, however these emotions were incidental (elicited by a
laboratory task) and not stimulated by the advertisements themselves. Future research must move
beyond laboratory studies and self-report data to determine whether opportunity-based scams are
actually more likely to result in victimization relative to other scam types and how emotions
contribute to compliance.
In the present study, we did not find that respondents who identified as seeking
opportunity during the interaction (Factor 1) were more likely to comply with opportunity-based
scams, nor did we find that respondents who felt intimidated (Factor 2) were more likely to
comply with threat-based scams. However, bivariate analyses show that victims score
significantly higher on the individual items that comprise these factors relative to non-victims
21
In addition to differences in the likelihood of compliance across the three scam types,
there are also important differences between the correlates of engagement and victimization by
scam type. For example, those who are male, unmarried, have higher household income but who
also are financially fragile were more likely to report losing money in opportunity-based scams,
but this profile did not emerge for any of the other categories. Low educational attainment was
only significant for engaging in threat-based scams and choosing not to discuss the offer with
anyone was only associated with consumer purchase scams. This variation in correlates adds to
previous literature showing that people are uniquely susceptible to different scams based on their
financial and psychological characteristics, as well as the social context. The products and
services scammers’ promise to provide and the elements of persuasion are each tailored to appeal
to certain targets more than others. For example, someone who is financially insecure may be
seeking money-making opportunities that will help improve their financial status, making them
more prone to opportunity-based frauds. Having financial troubles, however, would not
necessarily make that person more likely to believe they were in trouble with the government or
We found relatively few correlates of consumer products fraud. It is possible that this
category is too broadly defined. It includes online marketplace scams where consumers
automatically engage with a fraudulent offer simply by viewing products online, and also scams
where the consumer is solicited by salespeople (e.g., door-to-door solar panel installation).
Another issue is that the types of fraudulent products and services vary widely—pets, medical
insurance, car repair, etc., thereby appealing to consumers from various demographic groups.
Additional covariates, such as frequency of online shopping, may help shed light on who is most
22
susceptible to these scams. Further, this fraud category was the smallest of the three and the
sample sizes for the regression analyses were relatively small. The reduced power associated
with the smaller sample sizes for the consumer product fraud regressions (i.e., n=226, 219, and
Distinct Risk Factors for Engagement versus Victimization within Each Scam Type
Results suggest that the risk factors for engaging in specific scams differ from the risk
factors for losing money in those scams. For example, loneliness was significantly associated
with engaging in opportunity-based scams, but not with losing money. For victimization,
loneliness was associated with complying in consumer purchase scams and threat-based scams.
It is possible that the promises scammers offer in opportunity-based scams and the ways in
which they cultivate relationships with their targets may be particularly effective at engaging
those who feel isolated, however once the consumer has made the choice to respond, loneliness
does not further increase the risk of losing money. In consumer purchase scams on the other
hand, loneliness may act to decrease attention to the red flags surrounding fake products and
services, or to decrease resistance to the appeals used in advertising. Other studies have found a
link between feeling lonely and the desire to go shopping (Kim, Kang, & Kim, 2005), and also a
link between loneliness and parasocial connections to TV home shopping hosts (Lim & Kim,
2011). This suggests that lonely consumers may be inclined to spend their money on products
and services as a mood enhancement or coping strategy. Although subjective loneliness was
significant in some models, we found that once the respondent had engaged with the scam,
having no one available to talk to about the offer did not increase the risk of victimization. In
other words, being physically alone did not increase the odds of complying, controlling for other
23
factors. The survey did not ask those who chose not to engage if there was someone present who
deterred them from responding in the first place. This should be explored in future research.
Financial literacy emerged as a protective factor against losing money in opportunity- and
threat-based scam victimization among survey respondents, but was not significant for consumer
purchase scams, nor was it significant once the target engaged. Those who understand financial
concepts such as inflation and investing may be more skeptical of “get rich quick” opportunities,
but it is unclear how financial literacy increases resilience in the face of scams that use fear to
Despite large variation in correlates across scam categories, two factors were fairly
reliable across all scam types. Knowing about the specific scam before being targeted
significantly reduced the odds of engaging for all scam categories and the odds of victimization
for opportunity and threat-based scams in particular. This was the first study to use self-report
data to examine the impact of prior scam knowledge on susceptibility and suggests that efforts to
The second consistent factor associated with victimization was “seeming official.” Once
engaged, consumers were between 34% and 67% more likely to comply with the scam for each
degree they perceived it to be more official. The use of authority is a common persuasion tactic
in marketing and advertising (see Cialdini, 2001) and is the sine qua non for scammers. Research
on phishing emails (Ferreira & Teles, 2019), deceptive annuity sales (DeLiema, Yon, & Wilber,
2016), and advance fee fraud (Chang, 2008) show that fraudulent communications mimic or
reference well known people and organizations (e.g., Publisher’s Clearinghouse, Microsoft,
Social Security Administration), use professional titles and insignias (e.g., “Special Agent”,
24
“Financial Accounts Manager”), and present other indicators of legitimacy to appear trustworthy.
Our survey did not specifically ask respondents to describe what about the solicitation made it
seem official, but in the qualitative responses, consumers provided comments such as “[He]
sounded like a sheriff’s deputy and he was threatening me with immediate arrest if I didn’t
comply,” and “The phone ID had said 'Apple' and I had been having trouble with my computer.”
protecting people, reducing vulnerability by up to 85%, yet results also suggest that different
scams. This means there is not a one-size-fits-all victim profile for which to direct fraud
those who have a set of characteristics that make them uniquely vulnerable to that scam. For
example, when informing people about how to detect and resist opportunity-based scams, it may
be wise to focus on individuals who are financially insecure. For threat-based scams it may be
Given that victim profiles vary by scam type, the challenge for protection agencies and
consumer advocates is deciding which scams to prioritize in education and awareness efforts—
the scams that are the most common or the scams that result in the greatest losses? Another open
question is where to broadcast information about fraud. This study did not ask consumers who
knew about the scam before they were targeted to share where this knowledge came from, but in
general, the most common sources of fraud awareness are news stories and word of mouth
25
category than regression results. For example, threat-based scam targets were older, on average,
than targets in the other categories, although age differences in rates of engagement and
victimization were not statistically significant in the regression models. Still, altering the
message content and using different mediums to deliver fraud awareness information to older
Future research should determine the specific aspects of fraud awareness education that
make it memorable and effective. As scams continue to evolve, consumers are challenged to
transfer their acquired knowledge about persuasion and other red flags to new variations of
scams. Scheibe and colleagues (2014) conducted a forewarning study where former fraud
victims were pitched a mock scam two weeks or four weeks after they received a forewarning
phone call about the same scam or an entirely different scam. Although forewarning reduced
compliance with the mock scam in both conditions, outright refusals were more frequent among
those who were forewarned about the same scam they were later pitched. This indicates that
consumers are poor at transferring general fraud knowledge to other scams, yet a report by
DeLiema et al. (2019) suggests that having knowledge about the methods of scammers in general
and having experience with scams is greater among those who did not engage with fraud relative
to those who engaged but did not comply. This descriptive data suggests that general knowledge
Efforts to delegitimize scam solicitations so that targets do not perceive them as official
are much more difficult. Scammers use a variety of tricks to make their communications seem
valid, borrowing all the marketing and persuasion tools used in the legitimate consumer
marketplace. Perhaps the only way to successfully inoculate consumers against these tricks is to
26
educate people about the elements of persuasion and to encourage them to question and
interrogate all marketing claims before making a purchase or payment decision. To this end, a
recent randomized control trial found that concise, online educational interventions aimed at
educating subjects about the persuasion tactics used in fraud reduced susceptibility to investment
While this study examined the correlates of fraud victimization and monetary losses by
type of fraud, the field could benefit from research that examines the non-financial costs of
victimization by type of fraud. For example, do threat-based fraud victims experience more
Are stress levels and depression rates higher for threat-based fraud victims compared to
opportunity-based victims? Differences in how consumers respond might be driven by the way
society perceives victimization by fraud type. For instance, victims of opportunity fraud may be
perceived as greedy, whereas victims of consumer purchase and threat-based scams may be
perceived as naïve. Research in this vein could also examine if type of fraud is related to
recovery and reporting decisions following victimization, including financial and psychological
outcomes.
Limitations
Survey data is self-reported, cross sectional, and retrospective, limiting the conclusions
we can draw from the study. Because respondents completed the survey following the incident,
data does not allow us to determine whether certain situational factors, like loneliness and
financial fragility, precede or follow the scam. Longitudinal research is needed to separate the
27
Second, the response rate to this voluntary survey was poor. Only 2.3% completed the
survey following sequential email solicitations from BBB. Survey respondents were slightly
older than Scam Tracker reporters on average, and median losses were $448 higher than the
median losses reported by victims in the full 2018 Scam Tracker database. (For comparison, see
BBB Scam Tracker Risk Report 2018). This suggests that higher financial losses may have
motivated individuals to respond to the survey and share additional details about the incident.
Third, survey respondents are not representative of the true universe of fraud targets and
victims. Fraud is ubiquitous and the vast majority of individuals do not file reports when they are
targeted. Victims also underreport, including those who do not recognize their experience as
fraud or who feel too ashamed to convey the details of their experience to others. Future research
may attempt to survey individuals who are listed on scammer’s lead lists to better represent those
who are targeted and deceived by fraudsters. This approach would also control on targeting but
Last, this study included only American and Canadian consumers. Future work should
focus on consumers from different regions to identify whether there are variations in risk and
protective factors.
Conclusion
Using data from a unique survey of consumers who reported fraud to a consumer
complaint and advocacy organization, our study shows that the factors associated with engaging
with fraud differ from the factors associated with complying. Similarly, risk factors such as
socioeconomic characteristics, financial fragility, loneliness, and financial literacy vary by the
category of scam, although having advance knowledge of the exact scam prior to being targeted
is protective. Additional research is needed to better understand risk factors related to consumer
28
purchase scams and how to deliver fraud education that is most effective for all subtypes. Our
findings and other research are consistent in showing that fraud is a pernicious social problem,
and that consumers would benefit from widespread consumer awareness campaigns from
29
References
AARP Foundation (2003). Off the hook: Reducing participation in telemarketing fraud.
Alves, L. M., & Wilson, S. R. (2008). The effects of loneliness on telemarketing fraud
vulnerability among older adults. Journal of Elder Abuse & Neglect, 20(1), 63-85.
https://doi.org/10.1300/J084v20n01_04
Anderson, K. B. (2004). Consumer fraud in the United States: An FTC survey. Washington, DC:
Anderson, K. B. (2013). Consumer fraud in the United States, 2011: The third FTC
Anderson, K. B. (2019). Mass-market consumer fraud in the United States: A 2017 update.
consumer-fraud-united-states-2017-
update/p105502massmarketconsumerfraud2017report.pdf
Baumeister, R. F., Twenge, J. M., & Nuss, C. K. (2002). Effects of social exclusion on cognitive
Beals, M., DeLiema, M., & Deevy, M. (2015). Framework for a taxonomy of fraud. Financial
content/uploads/2016/03/Full-Taxonomy-report.pdf
Buchanan, T., & Whitty, M. T. (2014). The online dating romance scam: Causes and
doi:10.1080/1068316X.2013.772180
Burke, J., Kieffer, C., Mottola, G., & Perez-Arce, F. (2020). Can educational interventions
Campbell, M. C., & Kirmani, A. (2000). Consumers' use of persuasion knowledge: The effects
Chang, J. J. (2008). An analysis of advance fee fraud on the internet. Journal of Financial
Cialdini, R. B. (2001). The science of persuasion. Scientific American, 284(2), 76-81. Retrieved
from http://www.jstor.org/stable/26059056
Cross, C. (2016). ‘They’re very lonely’: Understanding the fraud victimization of seniors.
International Journal for Crime, Justice and Social Democracy, 5(4), 60-75.
doi:10.5204/ijcjsd.v5i4.268
Deevy, M. & Beals, M. (2013). The scope of the problem: An overview of fraud prevalence
https://longevity.stanford.edu/the-scope-of-the-problem-an-overview-of-fraud-
prevalence-measurement/
DeLiema, M. (2018). Fraud versus financial abuse and the influence of social relationships.
content/uploads/2017/08/Fraud-versus-financial-abuse-and-the-influence-of-social-
relationships.pdf
DeLiema, M., Shadel, D., & Pak, K. (2020). Profiling victims of investment fraud: Mindsets and
DeLiema, M., Yonashiro-Cho, J., Gassoumis, Z. D., Yon, Y., & Conrad, K. J. (2018). Using
latent class analysis to identify profiles of elder abuse perpetrators. The Journals of
DeLiema, M., Yon, Y., & Wilber, K. H. (2016). Tricks of the trade: Motivating sales agents to
31
Duclos, R., Wan, E. W., & Jiang, Y. (2013). Show me the honey! Effects of social exclusion on
doi:10.1086/668900
Fabrigar, L. R., Wegener, D. T., MacCallum, R. C., & Strahan, E. J. (1999). Evaluating the use
299. doi:10.1037/1082-989X.4.3.272
Ferreira, A., & Teles, S. (2019). Persuasion: How phishing emails can influence users and bypass
doi:10.1016/j.ijhcs.2018.12.004
Friestad, M., & Wright, P. (1994). The persuasion knowledge model: How people cope with
Financial Industry Regulatory Authority (2006). Investor fraud study: Final report. NASD
Investor Education Foundation and WISE Senior Services. The Consumer Fraud
FINRA Investor Education Foundation (2007). Senior Fraud Risk Survey. Retrieved from
https://www.finra.org/sites/default/files/Senior%20Fraud%20Risk%20Survey%20Findin
gs_0.pdf.
FINRA Investor Education Foundation (2013). Financial fraud and fraud susceptibility in the
United States. Retrieved from https://www.finrafoundation.org/files/financial-fraud-and-
fraud-susceptibility-united-states-research-report-2012-national-survey
Federal Trade Commission. (2016). Refund and recovery scams. Retrieved from
https://www.consumer.ftc.gov/articles/0102-refund-and-recovery-scams
Federal Trade Commission. (2018). Consumer sentinel network data book 2017. Retrieved from
https://www.ftc.gov/system/files/documents/reports/consumer-sentinel-network-data-
book-2017/consumer_sentinel_data_book_2017.pdf
32
Federal Trade Commission. (2020). Consumer Sentinel Network Data Book 2019. Retrieved
from https://www.ftc.gov/system/files/documents/reports/consumer-sentinel-network-
data-book-2019/consumer_sentinel_network_data_book_2019.pdf
Goyal, K., & Kumar, S. (2020). Financial literacy: A systematic review and bibliometric
Griskevicius, V., Shiota, M. N., & Neufeld, S. L. (2010). Influence of different positive emotions
doi:10.1037/a0018421
Han, S. D., Boyle, P. A., James, B. D., Yu, L., & Bennett, D. A. (2016). Mild cognitive
impairment and susceptibility to scams in old age. Journal of Alzheimer's Disease, 49(3),
845-851. doi:10.3233/JAD-150442
Han, S. D., Boyle, P. A., Yu, L., Arfanakis, K., James, B. D., Fleischman, D. A., & Bennett, D.
Hasler, A., Lusardi, A. & Oggero, N. (2018). Financial fragility in the U.S.: Evidence and
content/uploads/2018/04/Financial-Fragility-Research-Paper-04-16-2018-Final.pdf
Holt-Lunstad, J., Smith, T. B., Baker, M., Harris, T., & Stephenson, D. (2015). Loneliness and
Howell, D. C. (2012). Statistical methods for psychology. Independence, KY: Cengage Learning.
Hughes, M. E., Waite, L. J., Hawkley, L. C., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2004). A short scale for
Kieffer, C. & Mottola, G. (2017). Understanding and combating investment fraud. Mitchell, O.
S., P. B. Hammond, and S. P. Utkus (Eds.) Financial Decision Making and Retirement
33
Security in an Aging World, pp. 185-213, Oxford University Press. Retrieved from
https://repository.upenn.edu/cgi/viewcontent.cgi?article=1060&context=prc_papers
Kim, Y. K., Kang, J., & Kim, M. (2005). The relationships among family and social interaction,
loneliness, mall shopping motivation, and mall spending of older consumers. Psychology
Kircanski, K., Notthoff, N., DeLiema, M., Samanez-Larkin, G. R., Shadel, D., Mottola, G., &
Gotlib, I. H. (2018). Emotional arousal may increase susceptibility to fraud in older and
Lee, J., & Soberon‐Ferrer, H. (1997). Consumer vulnerability to fraud: Influencing factors.
Leech, N. L., Barrett, K. C., & Morgan, G. A. (2005). SPSS for intermediate statistics: Use and
Lerner, J. S., Li, Y., & Weber, E. U. (2013). The financial costs of sadness. Psychological
Lim, C. M., & Kim, Y. K. (2011). Older consumers' TV home shopping: Loneliness, parasocial
doi:10.1002/mar.20411
Lusardi, A., Mitchell, O. S., & Curto, V. (2014). Financial literacy and financial sophistication in
the older population. Journal of Pension Economics & Finance, 13(4), 347-366.
doi:10.1017/S1474747214000031
Lusardi, A. (2012). Numeracy, financial literacy, and financial decision-making. Numeracy, 5(1),
https://www.nber.org/system/files/working_papers/w17821/w17821.pdf
Ma, K., & McKinnon, T. (2020). COVID-19 and cyber fraud: Emerging threats during the
https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Katelyn_Wan_Fei_Ma/publication/344959656_CO
VID-
34
19_and_Cyber_Fraud_Emerging_Threats_During_the_Pandemic/links/5f9b3ce992851c1
4bcf2cfec/COVID-19-and-Cyber-Fraud-Emerging-Threats-During-the-Pandemic.pdf
Pak, K. & Shadel, D. (2011). AARP Foundation National Fraud Victimization Study.
http://assets.aarp.org/rgcenter/econ/fraudvictims-11.pdf.
Petty, R. E., & Briñol, P. (2015). Emotion and persuasion: Cognitive and meta-cognitive
doi:10.1080/02699931.2014.967183
Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1986). The elaboration likelihood model of persuasion. Advances
Raval, D. (2020). Whose voice do we hear in the marketplace? Evidence from consumer
Scheibe, S., Notthoff, N., Menkin, J., Ross, L., Shadel, D., Deevy, M., & Carstensen, L. L.
Schoepfer, A., & Piquero, N. L. (2009). Studying the correlates of fraud victimization and
doi:10.1016/j.jcrimjus.2009.02.003
Stewart, C., Yu, L., Glover, C., Mottola, G., Bennett, D., Wilson, R. & Boyle, P. (2020).
doi:10.1093/geront/gnaa078
Titus, R. M., & Gover, A. R. (2001). Personal fraud: The victims and the scams. Crime
http://www.popcenter.org/library/crimeprevention/volume_12/08-Titus.pdf
Titus, R. M., Heinzelmann, F., & Boyle, J. M. (1995). Victimization of persons by fraud. Crime
36
Self-rated financial competency (range 0-7) 6.0 (1.4) 6.2 (1.2) 5.9 (1.5) 6.0 (1.5)
Heard about the scam before targeted 446 (42.1) 296 (66.1) 97 (29.7) 46 (17.6)
Prior scam victim 228 (20.4) 90 (19.6) 66 (18.6) 64 (22.5)
Mean (SD) agreement with factors associated
with scam interactions (range 1-7)
I was seeking an opportunity (Factor 1) — N/A 2.6 (1.8) 3.2 (1.9)
I felt intimidated (Factor 2) — N/A 2.7 (1.7) 3.3 (1.8)
I choose not to discuss the offer with anyone — N/A 2.7 (2.2) 3.8 (2.3)
No one was available to discuss offer with — N/A 2.1 (1.7) 2.6 (2.0)
I had little knowledge about the offer — N/A 4.5 (2.1) 6.0 (1.5)
Person/organization seemed official — N/A 3.0 (2.2) 3.4 (2.3)
Person/organization knew my personal — N/A 3.1 (2.0) 4.2 (2.1)
details
Person/organization seemed nice — N/A 0.2 (0.4) 0.2 (0.4)
Someone tried to intervene — N/A 68 (18.7) 62 (21.0)
37
Table 2. Correlates of deciding to engage with a scam solicitation among all respondents and by scam category.
Note: Bolded values are statistically significant (p<.05); OR= Odds ratio; CI= Confidence Interval
38
Table 3. Correlates of scam victimization (financial loss) among all respondents and by scam category
Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4
All scam types Opportunity-based Threat-based scams Consumer purchase
(N=1,150) scams (N=380) (N=551) scams (n=219)
Note: Bolded values are statistically significant (p<.05); OR= Odds ratio; CI= Confidence Interval
39
Table 4. Correlates of victimization among those who engaged with a scam solicitation (all types and by scam category)
Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4
40
Note: Survey data is publicly available by request through BBB Institute for Marketplace
Trust, the Better Business Bureau’s educational foundation. Researchers interested in viewing
and using the data can email Institute@IABBB.org.
41
that banks make funds available within days of a deposit, but can take weeks to detect a
fake check.
TRAVEL/VACATIONS - Scammers post listings for properties that either are not for rent,
do not exist, or are significantly different than pictured. In another variation, scammers
claim to specialize in timeshare resales and promise they have buyers ready to
purchase.
TAX COLLECTION - In this scam, imposters pose as government tax collection agents
and use threats of immediate arrest or other scare tactics to convince their targets to pay,
often requesting that the target load money onto gift cards as payment.
PHISHING- Communication impersonating a trustworthy entity, such as a bank or
mortgage company, intended to mislead the recipient into providing personal information
or passwords.
ADVANCE FEE LOAN - In this scam, a loan is guaranteed but, once the victim pays
upfront charges such as taxes or a “processing fee,” the loan never materializes.
Other (please specify):
3. Which of the following best describes the incident you reported to the BBB Scam Tracker?
Select one.
42
4. Which of the following best describes how the scam attempt began?
Select one.
43
6.2 Did you receive any portion of your money back or whatever was promised to you by
engaging in the transaction?
Select one.
Yes
No
6.3 From the options below, what helped you to avoid being scammed? (Check all that
apply.)
Select all that apply.
7. (This item is included so we know you're paying attention and you're not a robot.) Which
word begins with the letter "T".
Select one.
Foot
Hand
Toe
Head
Neck
44
*8. Which of the following best describes your experience with the scam you
reported?(*Required)
Select one.
I immediately knew it was a scam and did not engage at all (ignored the
email/letter/phone call/sales person, hung up the phone, etc.)
I suspected it might be a scam but (Answer question number 8.1, 8.2, 8.3, 8.4, 8.5,
continued 8.6, 8.7, 8.8.)
I didn't know or suspect it was a (Answer question number 8.5, 8.7, 8.8.)
scam
8.1 During the scam, at what point did you suspect it might be a scam?
Select one.
8.2 What do you think caused you to engage, or continue to engage, even after you
suspected it might be a scam?
45
8.3 What do you think might have stopped you from engaging?
8.4 Did you talk to someone you know about the offer or situation while it was happening, that
is, before it concluded?
Select one.
Yes
No
Can't recall
8.5 Did any organization, company, or agency intervene or try to intervene to stop the
transaction? This may include a wire transfer company (e.g., Western Union), your financial
institution, a retail store worker, etc.
Select one.
Yes
No
8.6 If YES, please describe the intervention and be sure to name the organization, company
or agency that intervened:
46
8.7 Thinking back, when you were experiencing the scam you reported, which of the following
were true? Please give your answer on a 1-7 scale, where 1="Strongly disagree" 7="Strongly
agree" and 4="Neither agree nor disagree." You can use any number from 1 to 7.
Select one per row.
1 = Strongly 7 = Strongly
2 3 4 5 6
disagree agree
47
8.8 Thinking back, when you were experiencing the scam you reported, which of the following
were true? Please give your answer on a 1-7 scale, where 1="Strongly disagree" 7="Strongly
agree" and 4="Neither agree nor disagree." You can use any number from 1 to 7.
Select one per row.
1 = Strongly 7 = Strongly
2 3 4 5 6
disagree agree
9. Before you were targeted by the scam, had you heard about this type of scam before (e.g.,
from friends/family, on the news, from social media, etc.)?
Select one.
Yes
No
Not sure
48
10. Of the following sources of information, choose the top three that you believe would be
most helpful to you to learn about scams.
Select at most 3 choices.
11. Think about what you know about scams. Where did this information come from? (Select
all that apply.)
Select all that apply.
49
12. If I had been targeted by the same scam when I was younger, I would have been:
Select one.
13. Please indicate how much you agree or disagree with the following statements. Please
give your answer on a 1-7 scale, where 1="Strongly disagree" 7="Strongly agree" and
4="Neither agree nor disagree" You can use any number from 1 to 7. Scammers typically will
try to target...
Select one per row.
1= Strongly 7 = Strongly
2 3 4 5 6
disagree agree
wealthy people
pregnant women
the disabled
older adults
young adults
50
14. The next questions are about how you feel about aspects of your life. Please answer as
honestly as possible. How much of the time do you feel...
Select one per row.
left out?
15. Please indicate how much you agree or disagree with the following statements. Please
give your answer on a 1-7 scale, where 1="Strongly disagree" 7="Strongly agree" and
4="Neither agree nor disagree" You can use any number from 1 to 7.
Select one per row.
7=
1 = Strongly
2 3 4 5 6 Strongly
disagree
agree
I am good at math
51
16. How strongly do you agree or disagree with the following statement? Please give your
answer on a 1-7 scale, where 1= "Strongly disagree" 7="Strongly agree" and 4="Neither
agree nor disagree" You can use any number from 1 to 7.
Select one per row.
1 = Strongly 7 = Strongly
2 3 4 5 6
disagree agree
17. How confident are you that you could come up with $2,000 if an unexpected need arose
within the next month?
Select one.
18. Suppose you had $100 in a savings account and the interest rate was 2% per year. After
5 years, how much do you think you would have in the account if you left the money to grow?
Select one.
52
19. Imagine that the interest rate on your savings account was 1% per year and inflation was
2% per year. After 1 year, how much would you be able to buy with the money in this
account?
Select one.
20. If interest rates rise, what will typically happen to bond prices?
Select one.
21. A 15-year mortgage typically requires higher monthly payments than a 30-year mortgage,
but the total interest paid over the life of the loan will be less.
Select one.
True
False
Don’t know
Prefer not to say
53
22. Buying a single company’s stock usually provides a safer return than a stock mutual fund.
Select one.
True
False
Don’t know
Prefer not to say
23. Over the past year, would you say your household's spending was less than, more than,
or equal to your household's income? Please do not include the purchase of a house or new
car, or any other large investments you may have made.
Select one.
54
24. Please indicate how much you agree or disagree with the following statements. Please
give your answer on a 1-7 scale, where 1="Strongly disagree" 7="Strongly agree" and
4="Neither agree nor disagree" You can use any number from 1 to 7.
Select one per row.
7=
1 = Strongly
2 3 4 5 6 Strongly
disagree
agree
55
25. How well do the following statements describe your personality? I see myself as someone
who . . .
Select one per row.
is reserved
is relaxed, handles
stress well
is outgoing,
sociable
tends to be lazy
does a thorough
job
has an active
imagination
is generally trusting
Yes
No
Not sure
56
28. Is there anything else you'd like to tell us about the scam you reported to BBB Scam
Tracker?
29. How well do you recall the details of the scam you reported?
Select one.
57
2. Age .10** -
3. Non-Latino white
0.05 .18** -
4.Married/partnered .13** 0.06 .10** -
9. Financial literacy .22** .19** .16** .10** -.07* .35** -.11** -.31** -
13. I felt intimidated -0.06 0.04 -0.07 -.10* 0.04 -.11** .12** .12** -.08* -0.03 -0.07 0.08 -
14. I was seeking an
0.07 -.21** -.13** -0.02 -0.01 -.15** .20** .22** -.10** -.14** -.10* .11** 0.05 -
opportunity
15. I chose not to discuss
0.045 .11** 0.02 -.11** -0.01 -.13** .17** .15** -0.05 -0.07 -0.05 .15** .21** .20** -
w/anyone
16. No one was available to
0.02 .11** -0.01 -.16** 0.02 -.17** .26** .14** -.18** -.08* -0.04 0.07 .33** .10* .38** -
discuss offer with
22. Opportunity-based scams -0.02 -.15** -.12** -.07* -0.03 -.13** .11** .17** -.09** -.11** -.13** 0.05 -.11** .53** .13** -0.06 0.06 -0.01 -0.03 .20** -0.04 -
23. Threat-based scams 0.04 .21** 0.05 0.04 0.06 .11** -.08** -.14** .07* .11** .21** -0.05 .30** -.43** -0.06 .14** .12** 0.02 .27** -.11** -0.01 -.67** -
24. Consumer purchase scams -0.03 -.09** .08** 0.03 -0.03 0.01 -0.03 -0.02 0.02 -0.008 -.11** 0.00 -.20** -.14** -.08* -.08* -.20** -0.02 -.25** -.10** 0.05 -.34** -.47** -
58
Appendix C. Factor loadings and communalities based on a principal axis factoring analysis with promax
rotation and Kaiser Normalization for 7 items (n = 673)
Factor 1 Factor 2
Items I was seeking I felt
opportunity intimidated
I felt that I had an opportunity to make good on past mistakes. .60 .29
I felt that it was "my time" and I deserved to be rewarded .76 .10
I felt afraid of being punished for something I had done .05 .75
Note: Items in which communalities were lower than 0.3 were removed (n=7).
59