Formative Assessment A Critical Review
Formative Assessment A Critical Review
To cite this article: Randy Elliot Bennett (2011) Formative assessment: a critical
review, Assessment in Education: Principles, Policy & Practice, 18:1, 5-25, DOI:
10.1080/0969594X.2010.513678
In primary and secondary education, formative assessment is, without doubt, in vogue.
It has become a common theme at educational conferences, a standard offering in test-
company catalogues, the subject of government tenders, and a focus for teacher in-
service training.
This paper examines six interrelated topics, denoted as follows: the definitional
issue, the effectiveness issue, the domain dependency issue, the measurement issue,
the professional development issue, and the system issue. Collectively, these topics
are important in understanding what formative assessment is and what claims we, as
responsible professionals, should be making about it. The purpose of the paper is to
encourage something largely missing from the discourse around formative assessment
today: that is, a frank and judicious dialogue, one that is necessary for moving this
promising concept forward.
*Email: rbennett@ets.org
regardless of its original intended purpose (e.g., Weeden, Winter, and Broadfoot 2002,
28; Wiliam and Thompson 2008, 71).
Arguably, each position is an oversimplification. It is an oversimplification to
define formative assessment as an instrument because even the most carefully
constructed, scientifically supported instrument is unlikely to be effective instruction-
ally if the process surrounding its use is flawed. Similarly, it is an oversimplification
to define formative assessment as a process since even the most carefully constructed
process is unlikely to work if the ‘instrumentation’, or methodology, being used in that
process is not well-suited for the intended purpose. ‘Process’ cannot somehow rescue
unsuitable instrumentation, nor can instrumentation save an unsuitable process. A
strong conceptualisation needs to give careful attention to each component, as well as
to how the two components work together to provide useful feedback.
As noted, EdWeek reported that Richard Stiggins has stopped using the term ‘forma-
tive assessment’, presumably because that phraseology had lost its meaning. Many
advocates of the process view appear to prefer, ‘assessment for learning’, employing
‘assessment of learning’ to denote ‘summative assessment’. From a definitional
perspective, however, this substitution is potentially problematic in that it absolves
summative assessment from any responsibility for supporting learning. Further, it, too,
potentially leads to oversimplifying what is, in fact, a more complex relationship.
The relationship is more complex because a summative assessment should fulfil
its primary purpose of documenting what students know and can do but, if carefully
crafted, should also successfully meet a secondary purpose of support for learning.
Such support may be provided in at least three ways. First, if the content, format and
design of the test offer a sufficiently rich domain representation, preparing for the
summative test can be a valuable learning experience (Shepard 2006). When students
are motivated to prepare, studying encourages consolidation and organisation of
knowledge, rehearsal of domain-relevant processes and strategies, stronger links to
conditions of use, and greater automaticity in execution; in other words, the develop-
ment of expertise. Second, recent research suggests that taking a test can both enhance
learning by strengthening the representation of information retrieved during the test
and also slow the rate of forgetting (Rohrer and Pashler 2010). A final way in which
summative assessment may support learning is by providing a limited type of forma-
tive information. There is no claim that just any summative assessment can support
learning effectively; only those summative tests that are designed to fulfil that subsid-
iary purpose by, for example, linking performance to theoretically or empirically based
learning progressions (Corcoran, Mosher, and Rogat 2009; Popham 2008, 23), or
mapping key tasks to the score scale (Zwick et al. 2001). With such a test, the teacher
may be able to identify particular students needing more focused formative follow-up
or content that may need to be re-taught presently, or taught differently next cycle.
By the same token, well-designed and implemented formative assessment should
be able to suggest how instruction should be modified, as well as suggest impression-
istically to the teacher what students know and can do. Thus, we should be able to
design assessment systems in which summative tests, besides fulfilling their primary
purposes, routinely advance learning, and formative assessments routinely add to the
teacher’s overall informal judgments of student achievement (see Table 1).
Formative assessment then might be best conceived as neither a test nor a process,
but some thoughtful integration of process and purposefully designed methodology or
instrumentation. Also, calling formative assessment by another name may only exac-
erbate, rather than resolve, the definitional issue.2
8 R.E. Bennett
Table 1. A more nuanced view of the relationship between assessment purpose and
assessment type.
Purpose
Assessment Assessment
Type Of Learning For Learning
Summative X x
Formative x X
Note: X = primary purpose; x = secondary purpose.
But why is definition important in the first place? Definition is important because
if we can’t clearly define an innovation, we can’t meaningfully document its effective-
ness. Part of that documentation needs to be an evaluation of whether the formative
assessment was implemented as intended, which we cannot accomplish if we don’t
know what was supposed to be implemented. Similarly, if we can’t clearly define an
innovation, we can’t meaningfully summarise results across studies because we won’t
know which instances to include in our summary. Last, we won’t be able to transport
it to our own context, for how will we know the characteristics on which to focus in
doing the transport?
For a meaningful definition of formative assessment, we need at least two things:
a theory of action and a concrete instantiation. Among other things, the theory of
action: (1) identifies the characteristics and components of the entity we are claiming
is ‘formative assessment’, along with the rationale for each of those characteristics and
components; and (2) postulates how these characteristics and components work
together to create some desired set of outcomes (Bennett 2010). The concrete instan-
tiation illustrates what formative assessment built to the theory looks like and how it
might work in a real setting.
In this regard, the Keeping Learning on Track® Program (ETS 2010), or KLT, is
a provocative example because it contains a rudimentary theory of action and a
concrete instance to illustrate at least one type of ‘formative assessment’. The theory
of action revolves around ‘one big idea and five key strategies’, based in substantial
part on the work of Black and Wiliam (1998c, 2009). The big idea is of ‘students and
teachers using evidence … to adapt teaching and learning to meet immediate learning
needs minute-by-minute and day-by-day’ (ETS 2010).
The five key strategies are Sharing Learning Expectations (i.e., clarifying and
sharing learning intentions and criteria for success), Questioning (i.e., engineering
effective classroom discussions, questions and learning tasks that elicit evidence of
learning), Feedback, Self Assessment (i.e., activating students as the owners of their
own learning), and Peer Assessment (i.e., activating students as instructional resources
for one another). These strategies are used to direct the instructional processes of
establishing where learners are (e.g., through questioning), where they are going (by
sharing learning expectations), and how to get them there (through feedback) (Wiliam
and Thompson 2008). The KLT strategies are implemented through teacher- and
student-use of a large catalogue of techniques, including ones like, ‘Three stars and a
wish’, and ‘Traffic lights’. In ‘Three stars …’, students exchange work and each
student is expected to indicate three things he or she liked about his or her peer’s work
and one thing that he or she wished could be made better. In ‘Traffic lights …’, each
student is given a red, a yellow, and a green cup, and asked to display at key points in
Assessment in Education: Principles, Policy & Practice 9
the class lesson the cup indicating his or her level of understanding (i.e., red for ‘don’t
understand’, yellow for ‘unsure’, and green for ‘please proceed’).
Anticipating an issue to be discussed later, it is worth pointing out that the five key
strategies are intended to be general, domain-independent ones. The strategies have
links to cognitive-scientific theory, particularly that segment of the field concerned
with learning through social interaction (e.g., Vygotsky 1978, as cited in Shepard
2006). ‘Sociocultural’ theories postulate that students learn most effectively through
interchange with others, especially more proficient domain practitioners who can
model the internal standards and habits of mind that define advanced competency.
Sharing expectations, questioning, feedback, self-assessment, and peer assessment are
intended to, among other things, help students develop internal standards for their
work, reflect upon it, and take ownership of learning.
A logic model depicting the KLT theory of action is shown in Figure 1 (ETS
2009). The model is read from left to right. In broad strokes, the KLT components are
postulated to cause change in teacher practice (shown in the centre area) that, in turn,
influences student behaviour and increases achievement (shown in the right-hand
portion).
The KLT components are focused on training teachers in formative assessment. The
FigureFrom
Note: 1. The
Lyon
KLT
andtheory
Leusner
of action.
(2008). Used by permission. © Educational Testing Service. All rights reserved.
components include both materials and facilitated events. One of the events is a work-
shop for school staff members who will, in turn, support local teachers by helping them
establish a ‘learning community’ for themselves. The role of the learning community
performed middling on international assessments like PISA or TIMSS to the top of the
pack (e.g., Chappuis, Chappuis, and Stiggins 2009, 56).
Regardless of the metric used, the essential argument put forth by these and
numerous other advocates is that empirical research proves formative assessment
causes medium-to-very large achievement gains and that these results come from
trustworthy sources. In particular, the sources are said to include meta-analyses, as
well as noteworthy individual studies.
These claims deserve a closer look. The idea of meta-analysis is a sensible place
to start because it has been so frequently cited in the effectiveness claims to connote
methodological rigour. Meta-analysis was originally conceived as a method for
describing the empirical results observed in a research literature, though it has since
been extended for inferring underlying population parameters (Hunter and Schmidt
2004, 512). In its simplest form, the method is essentially a pooling of results from a
set of comparable studies that yields one or more summary statistics, including what
is commonly called an ‘effect size’. (See, for example, Glass, McGaw, and Smith
1981, for a detailed classic introduction to the method.) For experimental studies, the
effect size is typically computed as the difference between the treatment-group and
control-group means, divided by the standard deviation (of the control group or appro-
priately pooled across the groups).5
Like any method, however, meta-analysis can produce meaningless results. The
results should be considered suspect when, for example:
In this regard, a major concern with the original Black and Wiliam (1998c) review
is that the research covered is too disparate to be summarised meaningfully through
meta-analysis. That research includes studies related to feedback, student goal orien-
tation, self-perception, peer assessment, self assessment, teacher choice of assessment
task, teacher questioning behaviour, teacher use of tests, and mastery learning
systems. That collection is simply too diverse to be sensibly combined and
summarised by a single, mean effect-size statistic (or range of mean statistics).
This fact might be better appreciated if more advocates of formative assessment
carefully read the original article. In a section titled, ‘No Meta Analysis’, Black and
Wiliam (1998c) state the following:
It might be seen desirable … for a review of this type to attempt a meta-analysis of the
quantitative studies that have been reported… Individual quantitative studies which look
at formative assessment as a whole do exist …, although the number with adequate and
comparable quantitative rigour would be of the order of 20 at most. However, whilst
these [studies] are rigorous within their own frameworks and purposes, … the underlying
differences between the studies are such that any amalgamations of their results would
have little meaning. (53)
12 R.E. Bennett
In their review article, then, Black and Wiliam report no meta-analysis of their
own doing, nor any quantitative results of their own making. The confusion may occur
because, in their brief pamphlet and Phi Delta Kappan position paper, Black and
Wiliam (1998a, 1998b) do, in fact, attribute a range of effect sizes to formative assess-
ment. However, no source for those values is ever given. As such, these effect sizes
are not the ‘quantitative result’, meta-analytical or otherwise, of the 1998 Assessment
in Education review but, rather, a mischaracterisation that has essentially become the
educational equivalent of urban legend.6 Even so, the review provides a very valuable
qualitative synthesis, though of a broad array of literatures and not of a single, well-
defined class of treatments that could be called, ‘formative assessment’.
Whereas the Black and Wiliam articles are probably the most frequent derivation
for the claimed large impact of formative assessment, as suggested earlier there are a
number of other commonly referenced sources. But each source raises concerns that
might call the size of the claimed effects into question. We discuss here several
frequently cited examples to illustrate the nature of the concerns posed.
Let’s start with the Bloom studies that reputedly found effects of between 1 and 2
standard deviations, somewhere between ‘large’ and ‘huge’. That claim comes from a
summary article (Bloom 1984), based principally on (now quite dated) dissertations
conducted by Bloom’s students. In a comprehensive literature review that included
those same studies, Slavin (1987) wrote:
Bloom’s claim that mastery learning can improve achievement by more than 1 sigma is
based on brief, small, artificial studies that provided additional instructional time to the
experimental classes [and not to controls]. In longer term and larger studies with exper-
imenter-made measures, effects of group-based mastery learning are much closer to 1/4
sigma, and in studies with standardised measures there is no indication of any positive
effect at all. [The] 1-sigma claim is misleading … and potentially damaging … as it may
lead researchers to belittle true, replicable, and generalisable achievement effects in the
more realistic range of 20–50% of [a] standard deviation. (207)
A second commonly referenced source, by Nyquist (2003), is far more recent. The
relevance of this source to the school context can be immediately questioned because,
although rarely noted in advocates’ invocations, it focuses on the college-level popu-
lation (19). Second, the study is an unpublished master’s thesis and, as such, is not
generally available (including on the Internet). The fact that it is unpublished lessens
its value as backing for the general efficacy of formative assessment since it has not
been subjected to peer review – a hallmark of the scientific process – nor has it been
readily accessible for purposes of challenge and rejoinder. Finally, as might be
expected for a master’s thesis, it has significant limitations.7
Two individual studies, by Meisels et al. (2003) and by Rodriguez (2004), also
have figured among advocates’ evidentiary sources (e.g., Arter 2006, 42; Davies
n.d.; Glasson 2008; Kahl 2007; Love 2009, 15; Stiggins 2006). Of note is that both
studies were observational, so it is not possible to rule out alternative explanations
for treatment effects. The design of the Meisels et al. study is of particular concern
since it seems to have used a volunteer treatment group (ostensibly more motivated
than the comparison group), and because other curricular innovations were being
implemented during the study period. No accounting was apparently made for
either the potential selection bias or the confound with other innovations, so defen-
sible assertions about the impact of formative assessment are very difficult to
make.
Assessment in Education: Principles, Policy & Practice 13
KLT, that teachers attended TLC meetings and spent time sharing and critiquing their
formative assessment practices). Second, that backing should include data suggesting
that other intermediate outcomes stipulated by the theory of action were achieved
(e.g., that teachers actually shared learning intentions, structured opportunities to acti-
vate students as instructional resources for one another). Finally, the backing should
entail data indicating that students participating in formative assessment changed
more in a positive direction on outcomes of interest than those participating in some
alternative practice (e.g., that students do, in fact, act on feedback, become more
engaged, and learn more).
It should be obvious, then, that data are required to support the theory of action
underlying any specific approach to formative assessment. It should be equally obvi-
ous that every user of formative assessment need not collect such data. To evaluate the
theory, data need only be gathered from a reasonably representative subset of those
using the approach in question. The goal is to obtain sufficient data from enough
contexts to make the validity and efficacy arguments credible, thereby allowing gener-
alised claims for the meaning of formative assessment results and for the impact on
learning of using those results instructionally. The standard of rigour being advocated
is a scientific one, similar to that required for the effectiveness claims behind any
educational intervention.
If we accept that formative assessment programmes do, in fact, require an efficacy
argument and an encapsulated validity argument, a related question concerns whether
or not those arguments must have a specific substantive focus, the subject of our next
section.
teachers may differ significantly from one domain to the next because they ought to
be specifically tuned for the domain in question (Hodgen and Marshall 2005).
A possible approach to dealing with the domain dependency issue is to conceptu-
alise and instantiate formative assessment within the context of specific domains. Any
such instantiation would include a cognitive-domain model to guide the substance of
formative assessment, learning progressions to indicate steps toward mastery on key
components of the cognitive-domain model, tasks to provide evidence about student
standing with respect to those learning progressions, techniques fit to that substantive
area, and a process for teachers to implement that is closely linked to the preceding
materials and, therefore, to the domain in question.
In reading, for example, the cognitive-domain model created by O’Reilly and
Sheehan (2009) suggests that one key component of proficiency is the ability to use
and understand text conventions for various genres – persuasive, literary, informative.
For the literary genre, such a convention would be the ability to use and understand
plot structure as a comprehension aid. A hypothesised learning progression for that
ability would include the following steps: (1) determine the basic idea of plot; (2)
identify key plot elements (e.g., climax, resolution); and (3) understand how events
related to the plot advance the author’s goals. A question linked to the first step might
ask students to summarise the plot for a given text, and a domain-specific technique
for gathering additional evidence might be to have students complete a graphic orga-
niser calling for the identification of plot elements for text of the teacher’s choosing.
This approach implies that formative assessment should be essentially curriculum
embedded, a position that Shepard (2006, 2008) has espoused and Shavelson (2008)
has illustrated. But how tightly linked formative assessment must be to any given
curriculum is unresolved. It may be workable, for instance, to provide formative
assessment materials for the key ideas or core understandings in a domain, which
should be common across curricula. That would leave teachers to either apply poten-
tially weaker, domain-general strategies to the remaining topics or, working through
the teacher learning communities, create their own formative materials, using the
provided ones as models.
from such things as class participation, class work, homework, and test performance.
Backing for the validity of our conjectures is stronger to the extent we observe reason-
able consistency in student behaviour across multiple sources, occasions, and
contexts. Thus, each teacher-student interaction becomes an opportunity for posing
and refining our conjectures, or hypotheses, about what a student knows and can do,
where he or she needs to improve, and what might be done to achieve that change.
In her ‘Classroom Assessment’ chapter from the fourth edition of Educational
Measurement, Shepard (2006) touches on what might be called a ‘formative hypoth-
esis’ (Bennett and Gitomer 2009). Shepard writes:
Kane (2006), in the ‘Validation’ chapter from the same volume, echoes the idea:
By examining … student work…, the teacher can form hypotheses about the student’s
competencies and about gaps in … understanding … If a particular set of conjectures …
does account for the student’s pattern of performance (including mistakes), and no plau-
sible alternative hypothesis does as well, the proposed conjectures can be accepted as a
reasonable conclusion about the student. (49)
A related issue is time. Even if we can find a practical way to help teachers build
pedagogical skill, deep domain understanding, and a sense of the measurement funda-
mentals, teachers need significant time. They need time to put that knowledge, skill,
and understanding to practice, for example, to learn to use or adapt purposefully
constructed, domain-based, formative-assessment materials. Such materials might
include items, integrated task sets, projects, diagnostic tests, and observational and
interpretive guides. Teachers also need time to reflect upon their experiences with
these materials. If we can get teachers to engage in iterative cycles of use, reflection,
adaptation, and eventual creation – all firmly rooted in meaningful cognitive-domain
models – we may have a potential mechanism for helping teachers better integrate the
process and methodology of formative assessment with deep domain understanding.
Conclusion
The term, ‘formative assessment’, does not yet represent a well-defined set of artefacts
or practices. A meaningful definition requires a theory of action and one or more
concrete instantiations. When we have those components in place, we have something
20 R.E. Bennett
useful to implement and to study. The KLT Program (ETS 2010) offers such a defini-
tion for one category of formative assessment. More work like that is needed to push
the field forward.
Second, a more circumspect interpretation of the effectiveness research would be
that the general practices associated with formative assessment can, under the right
conditions, facilitate learning. However, the benefits may vary widely in kind and size
from one specific implementation of formative assessment to the next, and from one
subpopulation of students to the next. (As an example, consider the extensive variation
in the effectiveness of feedback.) Also, the magnitude of commonly made quantitative
claims for the efficacy of formative assessment is suspect, to say the least. The most
frequently cited effect-size claim of .4 – .7 standard deviations is neither meaningful
as a representation of the impact of a single well-defined class of treatments, nor
readily traceable to any inspectible, empirical source. Other empirical sources are
dated, unpublished, methodologically flawed, target older populations, or show
smaller effects than advocates cite. Finally, the validity argument, and evidence to
support it – both of which should logically be key to any theory of action of formative
assessment – are generally absent. Given these facts, as researchers we need to be
more responsible in our efficacy claims and, as educators, less immediately accepting
of those who push too self-assuredly for quick adoption.
Third, rooting formative assessment in pedagogical skills alone is probably insuf-
ficient. Rather, formative assessment would be more profitably conceptualised and
instantiated within specific domains. For example, in a special issue of Applied
Measurement in Education, Shavelson and his colleagues describe embedding forma-
tive assessment in a widely used curriculum, Foundational Approaches in Science
Teaching (Shavelson 2008). ETS’ CBAL (Cognitively Based Assessment of, for, and
as Learning) initiative, which is building assessments from cross-curricular cognitive-
domain models, offers a second example (Bennett 2010).
Fourth, formative assessment entails making inferences about what students know
and can do. Therefore, formative assessment is assessment, at least in part. This fact
implies that relevant measurement principles should figure centrally in its conceptual-
isation and instantiation. Incorporating measurement principles doesn’t mean that
validity should be sacrificed for reliability, as some advocates fear, or that inappropri-
ate psychometric concepts, methods, or standards of rigour intended for other assess-
ment purposes should be applied. But it does mean we should incorporate, rather than
ignore, the relevant fundamental principles.
Fifth, teachers need substantial knowledge to implement formative assessment
effectively in classrooms. It is doubtful that the average teacher has that knowledge,
so most teachers will need substantial time and support to develop it. Additionally,
teachers will need useful classroom materials that model the integration of pedagogi-
cal, domain, and measurement knowledge (e.g., developmentally sequenced tasks that
can help them make inferences about what students know with respect to key domain
competencies, and about what next to target for instruction).
Finally, we must account for the fact that formative assessment exists in an educa-
tional context. Ultimately, we have to rethink assessment from the ground up as a
coherent system, in which formative assessment is a critical part, but not the only crit-
ical part.
A suitable closing for this paper comes from Shavelson (2008). Referring to his
experience creating, implementing, and studying the effects of formative assessment,
he writes:
Assessment in Education: Principles, Policy & Practice 21
After five years of work, our euphoria devolved into a reality that formative assessment,
like so many other education reforms, has a long way to go before it can be wielded
masterfully by a majority of teachers to positive ends. (294)
Acknowledgements
I am grateful to Steve Chappuis, Joe Ciofalo, Terry Egan, Dan Eignor, Drew Gitomer, Steve
Lazer, Christy Lyon, Yasuyo Sawaki, Cindy Tocci, Caroline Wylie, and two anonymous
reviewers for their helpful comments on earlier drafts of this paper or the presentation upon
which the paper was based; to Brent Bridgeman, Shelby Haberman, and Don Powers for their
critique of selected effectiveness studies; to Dylan Wiliam, Jim Popham and Rick Stiggins for
their willingness to consider differing points of view; and to Caroline Gipps for suggesting
(however unintentionally) the need for a paper such as this one.
Notes
1. Influential members of the group have included Paul Black, Patricia Broadfoot, Caroline
Gipps, Wynne Harlen, Gordon Stobart, and Dylan Wiliam. See http://www.assessment-
reform-group.org/ for more information on the Assessment Reform Group.
2. How does formative assessment differ from diagnostic assessment? Wiliam and Thompson
(2008, 62) consider an assessment to be diagnostic when it provides information about
what is going amiss and formative when it provides guidance about what action to take.
They note that not all diagnoses are instructionally actionable. Black (1998, 26) offers a
somewhat different view, stating that: ‘… diagnostic assessment is an expert and detailed
enquiry into underlying difficulties, and can lead to a radical re-appraisal of a pupil’s needs,
whereas formative assessment is more superficial in assessing problems with particular
classwork, and can lead to short-term and local changes in the learning work of a pupil’.
3. Expected growth was calculated from the norms of the Metropolitan Achievement Test
Eighth Edition (Harcourt Educational Measurement 2002), the Iowa Tests of Basic Skills
Complete Battery (Hoover, Dunbar, and Frisbie 2001), and the Stanford Achievement Test
Series Tenth Edition (Pearson 2004).
4. Stiggins is reported to no longer stand by the claims quoted here (S. Chappuis, April 6,
2009, personal communication). I have included them because they are published ones still
frequently taken by others as fact. See Kahl (2007) for an example.
5. Cohen (1988, 25–7) considers effects of .2 to be small, .5 to be medium, and .8 to be large.
6. It is possible that these values represent Black and Wiliam’s retrospective extraction from
the 1998 review of the range of mean effects found across multiple meta-analytical stud-
ies done by other investigators on different topics (i.e., the mean effect found in a meta-
analysis on one topic was .4 and the mean effect found in a meta-analysis on a second
topic was .7). If so, the range of observed effects across individual studies would, in fact,
be wider than the oft-quoted .4 to .7 range of effects, as each meta-analytic mean itself
represents a distribution of study effects. But more fundamentally, the construction of any
such range would seem specious according to Black and Wiliam’s (1998c) very own
critique – i.e., ‘… the underlying differences between the studies are such that any amal-
gamations of their results would have little meaning’ (53).
7. A partial list of concerns includes confusing association with causation in the interpretation
of results, ignoring in the interpretation the finding that results could be explained by (irrel-
evant) method factors, seemingly computing effect sizes before coding the same studies for
the extent of use of formative assessment (introducing the possibility of bias in coding),
giving no information on the reliability of the coding, and including many dated studies (57
of the 86 included articles were 30 or more years old) without considering publication date
as a moderator variable.
22 R.E. Bennett
Notes on contributor
Randy Elliot Bennett is Norman O. Frederiksen Chair in Assessment Innovation in the
Research & Development Division at Educational Testing Service in Princeton, New Jersey.
He has conducted research on integrating advances in cognitive science, technology, and
measurement to create new approaches to assessment. Since 2007, Bennett has directed an inte-
grated research initiative titled: Cognitively-Based Assessment of, for, and as Learning
(CBAL) (http://www.ets.org/research/topics/cbal/initiative). This initiative is attempting to
create a model for a balanced system of K–12 assessment that provides accountability infor-
mation and supports classroom learning. His and his colleagues’ work has been described in
the George Lucas Educational Foundation publication Edutopia, in Education Week’s Teacher
Beat online blog, in a Science review of innovative approaches to educational assessment, and
in the US Department of Education’s National Educational Technology Plan 2010.
References
Arter, J. 2006. Making use of data: What educators need to know and be able to do. In Beyond
NCLB: From measuring status to informing improvement, ed. J. O’Reilly, 39–72.
Proceedings of the National Association of Test Directors 2006 symposium. Boone, NC:
National Association of Test Directors. http://natd.org/files/uplink/2006proceedings.pdf
(accessed February 25, 2009).
Ball, D.L., M.H. Thames, and G. Phelps. 2008. Content knowledge for teaching: What makes
it special? Journal of Teacher Education 59, no. 5: 389–407.
Bennett, R.E. 2010. Cognitively based assessment of, for, and as learning: A preliminary
theory of action for summative and formative assessment. Measurement: Interdisciplinary
Research and Perspectives 8, nos. 2–3: 70–91.
Bennett, R.E., and D.H. Gitomer. 2009. Transforming K-12 assessment: Integrating account-
ability testing, formative assessment, and professional support. In Educational assessment
in the 21st century, ed. C. Wyatt-Smith and J. Cumming, 43–61. New York: Springer.
Bennett, R.E., R.L. Gottesman, D.A. Rock, and F.M. Cerullo. 1993. The influence of behavior
and gender on teachers’ judgments of students’ academic skill. Journal of Educational
Psychology 85, no. 2: 347–56.
Assessment in Education: Principles, Policy & Practice 23
Black, P. 1998. Testing, friend or foe? The theory and practice of assessment and testing.
London: Routledge/Falmer Press.
Black, P., and D. Wiliam. 1998a. Inside the black box: Raising standards through classroom
assessment. Phi Delta Kappan 80, no. 2: 139–48.
Black, P., and D. Wiliam. 1998b. Inside the black box: Raising standards through classroom
assessment. London, UK: Kings College, London School of Education.
Black, P., and D. Wiliam. 1998c. Assessment and classroom learning. Assessment in Educa-
tion: Principles, Policy & Practice 5, no. 1: 7–74.
Black, P., and D. Wiliam. 2003. ‘In praise of educational research’: Formative assessment.
British Educational Research Journal 29, no. 5: 623–37.
Black, P., and D. Wiliam. 2009. Developing a theory of formative assessment. Educational
Assessment, Evaluation and Accountability 21, no. 1: 5–31.
Bloom, B.S. 1969. Some theoretical issues relating to educational evaluation. In Educational
evaluation: New roles, new means. The 63rd yearbook of the National Society for the
Study of Education, part 2 (Vol. 69), ed. R.W. Tyler, 26–50. Chicago, IL: University of
Chicago Press.
Bloom, B.S. 1984. The 2 sigma problem: The search for methods of group instruction as
effective as one-to-one tutoring. Educational Researcher 13, no. 6: 4–16.
Boekaerts, M., and L. Corno. 2005. Self-regulation in the classroom: A perspective on
assessment and intervention. Applied Psychology: An International Review 54, no. 2:
199–231.
Cech, S.J. 2007. Test industry split over ‘formative’ assessment. Edweek 28, no. 4:
1, 15. http://www.edweek.org/ew/articles/2008/09/17/04formative_ep.h28.html (accessed
February 6, 2009).
Chappuis, J., S. Chappuis, and R. Stiggins. 2009. Formative assessment and assessment for
learning. In Meaningful measurement: The role of assessments in improving high school
education in the twenty-first century, ed. L.M. Pinkus, 55–76. Washington, DC: Alliance
for Excellent Education. http://www.all4ed.org/files/MeanMeasCh3Chappuis Stiggins.pdf
(accessed August 3, 2009).
Cohen, J. 1988. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. 2nd ed. Hillsdale, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Corcoran, T., F.A. Mosher, and A. Rogat. 2009. Learning progressions in science: An
evidence-based approach to reform (RR-63). New York: Consortium for Policy Research
in Education (CPRE), Teachers College, Columbia University.
Davies, A. N.d. Summary of classroom assessment research. Assessment for learning: An
online resource for educators. http://annedavies.com/assessment_for_learning_arc.html
(accessed February 25, 2009).
ETS (Educational Testing Service). 2009. Research rationale for the Keeping Learning on
Track® program. Princeton, NJ: Author. http://www.ets.org/Media/Campaign/12652/rsc/
pdf/KLT-Resource-Rationale.pdf (accessed December 17, 2010).
ETS (Educational Testing Service). 2010. About the KLT program. Princeton, NJ: Author.
http://www.ets.org/Media/Campaign/12652/about.html (accessed December 17, 2010).
Furtak, E.M., M.A. Ruiz-Primo, J.T. Shemwell, C.C. Ayala, P.R. Brandon, R.J. Shavelson,
and Y. Yin. 2008. On the fidelity of implementing formative assessments and its relation
to student learning. Applied Measurement in Education 21, no. 4: 360–89.
Gitomer, D.H., and R.A. Duschl. 2007. Establishing multilevel coherence in assessment. In
Evidence and decision making. The 106th yearbook of the National Society for the Study
of Education, Part I, ed. P.A. Moss, 288–320. Chicago, IL: National Society for the Study
of Education.
Glass, G.V., B. McGaw, and M.L. Smith. 1981. Meta-analysis in social research. Beverly
Hills, CA: Sage.
Glasson, T. 2008. Improving student achievement through assessment for learning. Curriculum
Leadership 6, no. 31. http://www.curriculum.edu.au/leader/improving_student
_achievement,25374.html?issueID=11603 (accessed February 25, 2009).
Harcourt Educational Measurement. 2002. Metropolitan8: Technical manual. San Antonio,
TX: Author.
Harrison, C. 2005. Teachers developing assessment for learning: Mapping teacher change.
Teacher Development 9, no. 2: 255–63.
24 R.E. Bennett
Hodgen, J., and B. Marshall. 2005. Assessment for learning in English and mathematics: A
comparison. The Curriculum Journal 16, no. 2: 153–76.
Hoover, H.D., S.B. Dunbar, and D.A. Frisbie. 2001. Iowa Tests of Basic Skills Complete/Core
Battery: Spring norms and score conversions with technical information. Itasca, IL:
Riverside.
Hunter, J.E., and F.L. Schmidt. 2004. Methods of meta-analysis: Correcting error and bias in
research findings. 2nd ed. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Kahl, S. 2007. Formative assessment: An overview. Presentation at the Montana Office of Public
Instruction ‘Assessment Toolkit’ conference, April 23, in Helena, MT. http://opi.mt.gov/
PDF/Assessment/conf/Presentations/07MON_FormAssmt.ppt (accessed February 11,
2009).
Kane, M.T. 2006. Validation. In Educational measurement, 4th ed., ed. R.L. Brennan, 17–64.
Westport, CT: American Council on Education/Praeger.
Kluger, A.N., and A. DeNisi. 1996. The effects of feedback interventions on performance: A
historical review, a meta-analysis, and a preliminary feedback intervention theory.
Psychological Bulletin 119, no. 2: 254–84.
Lee, C., and D. Wiliam. 2005. Studying changes in the practice of two teachers developing
assessment for learning. Teacher Development 9, no. 2: 265–83.
Love, N. 2009. Building a high-performance data culture. In Using data to improve learning
for all: A collaborative inquiry approach, ed. N. Love, 2–24. Thousand Oaks, CA:
Corwin Press.
Martiniello, M. 2008. Language and the performance of English-language learners in math
word problems. Harvard Educational Review 78, no. 2: 333–68.
McManus, S. 2008. Attributes of effective formative assessment. Washington, DC: Council for
Chief State School Officers. http://www.ccsso.org/publications/details.cfm?PublicationID
=362 (accessed March 2, 2009).
Meisels, S.J., S. Atkins-Burnett, Y. Xue, D.D. Bickel, and S. Son. 2003. Creating a system of
accountability: The impact of instructional assessment on elementary children’s achieve-
ment test scores. Education Policy Analysis Archives 11, no. 9. http://epaa.asu.edu/epaa/
v11n9/ (accessed February 11, 2009).
Nyquist, J.B. 2003. The benefits of reconstruing feedback as a larger system of formative
assessment: A meta-analysis. Master’s thesis, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN.
O’Reilly, T., and K.M. Sheehan. 2009. Cognitively based assessment of, for and as learning:
A framework for assessing reading competency (RR-09-26). Princeton, NJ: Educational
Testing Service.
Pearson. 2004. Stanford Achievement Test Series Tenth Edition: Technical data report. Iowa
City, IA: Author.
Pearson. 2005. Achieving student progress with scientifically based formative assessment: A
white paper from Pearson. http://www.pearsoned.com/RESRPTS_FOR_POSTING/
PASeries_RESEARCH/PA1.%20Scientific_Basis_PASeries%206.05.pdf (accessed June
26, 2009).
Pellegrino, J.W., N. Chudowsky, and R. Glaser. 2001. Knowing what students know: The science
and design of educational assessment. Washington, DC: National Academies Press.
Perkins, D.N., and G. Salomon. 1989. Are cognitive skills context bound? Educational
Researcher 18, no. 1: 16–25.
Popham, W.J. 2006. Phony formative assessments: Buyer beware! Educational Leadership
64, no. 3: 86–7.
Popham, W.J. 2008. Transformative assessment. Alexandria, VA: ASCD.
Rodriguez, M.C. 2004. The role of classroom assessment in student performance on TIMSS.
Applied Measurement in Education 17, no. 1: 1–24.
Rohrer, D., and H. Pashler. 2010. Recent research on human learning challenges conventional
instructional strategies. Educational Researcher 39, no. 5: 406–12.
Sadler, D.R. 1989. Formative assessment and the design of instructional systems. Instructional
Science 18, no. 2: 119–44.
Scriven, M. 1967. The methodology of evaluation. In Perspectives of curriculum evaluation,
ed. R.W. Tyler, R.M. Gagne, and M. Scriven, 39–83. Chicago, IL: Rand McNally.
Shavelson, R.J. 2008. Guest editor’s introduction. Applied Measurement in Education 21, no.
4: 293–4.
Assessment in Education: Principles, Policy & Practice 25
Shepard, L.A. 2006. Classroom assessment. In Educational measurement, 4th ed., ed. R.L.
Brennan, 623–46. Westport, CT: American Council on Education/Praeger.
Shepard, L.A. 2008. Formative assessment: Caveat emptor. In The future of assessment:
Shaping teaching and learning, ed. C.A. Dwyer, 279–303. New York: Erlbaum.
Shulman, L.S. 1986. Those who understand: Knowledge growth in teaching. Educational
Researcher 15, no. 2: 4–14.
Shute, V.J. 2008. Focus on formative feedback. Review of Educational Research 78, no. 1:
153–89.
Slavin, R.E. 1987. Mastery learning reconsidered. Review of Educational Research 57, no. 2:
175–213.
Stiggins, R.J. 1999. Assessment, student confidence, and school success. Phi Delta Kappan
81, no. 3: 191–8. http://www.pdkintl.org/kappan/k9911sti.htm (accessed February 11,
2009).
Stiggins, R. 2006. Assessment for learning: A key to motivation and achievement. Edge 2, no.
2: 3–19. http://www.michigan.gov/documents/mde/Kappan_Edge_Article_188578_7.pdf
(accessed February 11, 2009).
Stiggins, R.J., J.A. Arter, J. Chappuis, and S. Chappuis. 2006. Classroom assessment for
learning: Doing it right—using it well. Princeton, NJ: Educational Testing Service.
Weeden, P., J. Winter, and P. Broadfoot. 2002. Assessment: What’s in it for schools? London:
Routledge/Falmer.
Wiliam, D., and M. Thompson. 2008. Integrating assessment with learning: What will it take
to make it work? In The future of assessment: Shaping teaching and learning, ed. C.A.
Dwyer, 53–82. New York: Erlbaum.
Zwick, R., D. Senturk, J. Wang, and S.C. Loomis. 2001. An investigation of alternative meth-
ods for item mapping in the National Assessment of Educational Progress. Educational
Measurement: Issues and Practice 20, no. 2: 15–25.