Pre-Design Experiment Guide Sheet
Pre-Design Experiment Guide Sheet
The use of pre-design experiment guide sheets provides a way to systematize the process
by which an experimentation team does this planning, to help people to (a) more clearly
define the objectives and scope of an experiment and (b) gather information needed to
design an experiment.
Steps of Experimentation
1. Recognition of and statement of the problem
2. Choice of factors and levels
3. Selection of the response variable(s)
4. Choice of experimental design
5. Conduction of the experiment
6. Data analysis
7. Conclusions and recommendations
1
Adapted by Erik Barry Erhardt (2007/02/23) from Technometrics, February 1993, Vol. 35, No. 1, A
Systematic Approach to Planning for a Designed Industrial Experiment, David E. Coleman, Douglas C.
Montgomery
1
Pre-design Master Guide Sheet
1. Experimenter’s Name and Organization:
2
3. Relevant background on response and control variables: (a) theoretical relationships;
(b) expert knowledge/experience; (C) previous experiments. Where does this experiment
fit into the study of the process or system?
((a) to establish a context for the experiment to clearly understand what new knowledge can be gained; (b)
to motivate discussion about the relevant domain knowledge, since such discussion may change the
consensus of the group, hence the experiment; and (c) to uncover possible experimental regions of
particular interest and others that should be avoided. With this background, we reduce the risks of naive
empiricism and duplication of effort.)
3
4. List: (a) each response variable, (b) the normal response variable level at which the
process runs, the distribution or range of normal operation, (C) the precision or range to
which it can be measured (and how):
1. It is preferably a continuous variable.
2. Should capture, as much as possible, a quantity or quality of interest for the experimental unit
3. Should be in appropriate units
4. Should be associated with a target or desirable condition (which motivates the experiment).
5. Is preferably obtained by nondestructive and nondamaging methods so that repeated measures can
be made and measurement error can be quantified.
6. Should not be near a natural boundary.
7. Preferably has constant variance over the range of experimentation.
4
Different categories of factors that affect response variables, based on three key
characteristics—magnitude of influence on response variables, degree of controllability,
and measurability (e.g., precision and accuracy).
A description of the diagram is as follows:
1. Control variables are measurable, controllable, and thought to be (very)
influential.
2. Held-constant factors tire controlled.
3. Nuisance factors are uncontrolled factors (either they cannot be controlled, or they
are allowed to vary).
5
5. List: (a) each control variable, (b) the normal control variable level at which the
process is run, and the distribution or range of normal operation, (the precision (s) or
range to which it can be set (for the experiment, not ordinary operations) and the
precision to which it can be measured, (d) the proposed control variable settings, and (e)
the predicted effect (at least qualitative) that the settings will have on each response
variable:
other issues and constraints taken into account when settings are selected—safety, discreteness of settings,
process constraints, ease of changing a setting, and so forth.
attempting to predict the outcome of the experiment before it is run can foster good interaction within the
experimentation team and often leads to revised choices of settings. An additional advantage is that the
predictions will always be wrong, so it is easier to see what knowledge has been gained through
experimentation.
• Current Use (col. 2)
o historical process data can be used to gain relevant knowledge may be revealed.
o select a range large enough to produce an observable effect and to span a good proportion
of the operating range, yet not choose so great a range that no empirical model can be
postulated for the region.
• Ability to Measure and Set (col. 3)
o know how measurements will be obtained and the precision of measurement
o how the control variable settings will be obtained and “setting error.”
Control variable Normal level and Measurement Proposed settings, Predicted effects
(units) range (current use) precision and based on (for various
setting error— predicted effects responses)
how known? (knowledge (knowledge
(ability to sought through sought through
measure and set) experimentation) experimentation)
6
6. List: (a) each factor to be “held constant” in the experiment, (b) its desired level and
allowable s or range of variation, (c) the precision or range to which it can measured (and
how), (d) how it can be controlled, and (e) its expected impact, if any, on each of the
responses:
Held-constant factors are controllable factors whose effects are not of interest in this experiment.
to help ensure that there are no extraneous factors distorting the results.
can force helpful discussion about which factors are adequately controlled and which factors are not. In so
doing, it is often necessary to consult experts to help prioritize factors, recommend preexperiment studies to
assess control, or develop control strategy.
7
7. List: (a) each nuisance factor (perhaps time-varying), (b) measurement precision, (c)
strategy (e.g. blocking, randomization, or selection), and (d) anticipated effect:
Processes and experimental conditions vary over time.
Some of these can be measured and monitored to at least ensure that they are within limits; others must be
assessed subjectively by experts; still others are unmeasured. If the level can be selected for any
experimental unit, however, blocking or randomization might be appropriate. If levels cannot be selected
(i.e., the levels of the factor are unpredictable, perhaps continuous), then the nuisance factor becomes a
covariate in the analysis. If a nuisance factor is not measurable and thought to be very influential, it may
also be called an experimental risk factor. Such factors can inflate experimental error, making it more
difficult to assess the significance of control variables. They can also bias the results.
strategies (randomization, blocking, analysis of covariance, stratified analysis) to reduce the impact of
nuisance factors.
Blocking will introduce a bias in the estimates confounded with the blocking van- able(s). whereas
randomization will inflate the experimental error.
8
8. List and label known or suspected interactions:
For interactions tutorial, see Figure 2 on next page.
The interactions table explicitly recognizes only pairwise interactions of linear terms.
This input is helpful when the experiment is later designed—to choose resolution, or more generally to
choose which effects should or should not be confounded.
Use the process of elimination or inclusion. If interactions are generally important: “Are there any
interactions that are arguably not present?” If main effects dominate interactions: “Are there any
interactions that must be estimated clear of main effects?”
Control A B C D E F G H I J
variable
A —
B —
C —
D —
E —
F —
G —
H —
I —
J —
Use: Along either the upper or lower triangular region, place a dash “—” to indicate no predicted
interaction. Place a letter, such as X, to indicate a predicted interaction between two control variables on
response X. If multiple response variables, can use X, Y, Z, etc.
9
Figure 2 Interactions tutorial
10
9. List restrictions on the experiment, e.g., ease of changing control variables, methods
of data acquisition, materials, duration, number of runs, type of experimental unit (need
for a split-plot design), “illegal” or irrelevant experimental regions, limits to
randomization, run order, cost of changing a control variable setting, etc.:
Theoretical optimal experimental design and practical experimental design are often worlds apart, and
restrictions often make the difference.
Put these limitations and pitfalls in the open.
11
10. Give current design preferences, if any, and reasons for preference, including
blocking and randomization:
When experimenters are statistically sophisticated and have a good idea of appropriate designs or analysis
techniques.
When the experiment has been preceded by experiments in which a particular design or technique proved
to be useful.
Discuss problem-specific issues that will affect the experimental design, such as multilevel factors,
different sizes of experimental units, and logistics.
(a) choose candidate designs, (b) review them with the experimenters in the context of the collected
information to determine if any of the designs should be dropped from further consideration, and (c) write
an experimental design proposal that contains (at least) one or more proposed designs; a comparative
analysis of the designs with respect to number of runs, resolution (or aliased effects), number of distinct
control variable combinations, prediction error standard deviation, and so forth; a design recommendation
with justification; and copies of the completed guide sheets.
12
11. If possible, propose analysis and presentation techniques, e.g., plots, ANOVA,
regression, plots, t-tests, etc.:
13
Work diligently to bridge the gap in knowledge and experience between the statistician
and experimenter. The consequences of not bridging this gap can be serious. Help
ensure the experiment goes as planned by overcoming the challenges below.
Hahn advised, “The statistician’s major functions are to help structure the problem, to
identify important issues and practical constraints, and to indicate the effect of various
compromises on the inferences that can be validly drawn for the experimental data”.
14