Kager 95 Metrical Theory
Kager 95 Metrical Theory
Word Stress
RENE KAGER
0 Introduction
(1) word
AA
swsw
A la ba ma
Stress, as represented in the metrical tree, is a relational property: a node is
strong only by virtue of the fact that it is the sister of a weak node. Thus in
(1), the first syllable is stronger than the second, while the third is stronger
than the fourth. The superior nodes are themselves in a weak-strong
Metrical Theory of Word Stress 369
I
I relationship, which represents the relative prominence of the first and third
syllables.
(2) *
* *
** * *
Alabama
The height of the grid columns represents the degree of prominence. Thus
in (2) the third syllable is the most prominent, the initial one is less prominent
by one degree, while the second and fourth are the least prominent. The grid
perspicuously depicts the rhythmic alternation of strong and weak syllables.
Early metrical theory derived the grid from the tree by a mapping rule, which
imposes a prominence relation between syllables dominated by pairs of sister
nodes.
o Fw
A
as Cc as
/\
Fs
Ow
Fs
a
I
F,
I
a
Z\
as
F
G,
A la ba ma con test tern pest
1
370 René Kager
1.1.1 Boundedness
A major distinction can be drawn between systems in which stresses fall within
limited distances both from each other and from word edges, and systems
where the distribution of stresses is not restricted in this way. The relevant
parameter of boundedness has two values: bounded and unbounded. Bounded
feet contain no more than two syllables, while unbounded feet are not subject
to any restrictions on size. We illustrate this with head-initial feet, in (4).
Metrical Theory of Word Stress 371
Aa
F F
1
a
(\
A
s
0, aw
A
as av., aw
A
F
as 0,
F
a
i
as Gs aw
s s
s
as Ow ow Ow a, Ow Ow as
1.1.3 Quantity-sensitivity
The third foot shape parameter, quantity-sensitivity, governs the distribution of
light and heavy syllables in terminal nodes of feet. In quantity-insensitive feet,
372 René Kager
no restrictions hold, so that all syllables are treated as light (or equally heavy).
In quantity-sensitive feet, heavy syllables may not occur in recessive positions,
and are stressed. Quantity-determined (or Obligatory Branching) feet are
quantity-sensitive, with the extra requirement that dominant terminal nodes
must dominate heavy syllables. The three types are shown below with left-
dominant, bounded feet in which dominant nodes are strong. We indicate
heavy syllables as H, and light syllables as L in (6). Where either H or L is
indicated, the template indicated refers specifically to patterns possessing the
requisite H or L; when a simple a is indicated, the template is appropriate for
either an H or L syllable, with the more specific template taking precedence
over the more general, in this informal presentation.
(6) (a) Q-insensitive (b) Q-sensitive (c) Q-determined
/\
Gs
F
0
or F
I
a
/\
6,
F
6
or F
I
a as
A
F
av,
or F
I
a
L H L H
We made reference above to an unmarked labeling convention. Here we
observe the marked convention, according to which dominant nodes are marked
as strong iff they dominate a branching node (heavy syllables count as
branching, as we will see shortly). This produces one more quantity-sensitive
foot, the Labeling Based on Branching (LBOB) foot. Its left-dominant version is
in (7).3
(7) F or F or F
as aw Gv,
A as a
I
H L L L
In the geometrical spirit of early metrical theory, Hayes proposes that syllable
weight is tied essentially to whether certain syllable-internal constituents do or
do not branch. The constituents in question are the rhyme and the nucleus.'
Foot construction inspects branchingness on one of two projections. On the
rhyme projection (8), both long-voweled and closed syllables are heavy, as
opposed to open short-voweled syllables. On the nucleus projection (9), long-
voweled syllables are heavy as opposed to all others.
(8) (a) R (b) R (c) R
V
A
VV
/\ (includes Coda C)
VC
(9) (a) N (b) N (c) N
V
/\
VV
I
V
(excludes Coda C)
i
/s
(10) (a) Wd (b) Wd (c) Wd (d) Wd
/\
F, F Fs
/\ F, F,, Fs
I
F, F,
/\
Fs F Fs F,,
I
Word-level labeling may refer to the internal structure of feet, but never to
that of syllables. More generally, the Metrical Locality principle (Hammond
1982) states that rules may refer only to elements at the same or adjacent layers
of metrical structure.
/\ /\
as
F
ow as ow
F
as
F
ow a
F F
a as
F
ow as
F
aw as
F
ow
aw as aw as ow as a a ow as ow as ow as
Hungarian (Kerek 1971) exemplifies (11a). Main stress is initial and secondary
stresses fall on all odd-numbered syllables. A left-dominant word tree produces
initial main stress, as in (12).
The pattern of (11d) occurs in Weri (Boxwell and Boxwell 1966). Main stress
is on the final syllable, and secondaries are on preceding odd-numbered syl-
lables counting from the word end. The word tree is right-dominant. See (15).
1
(16) Wd
1.--ss
rzsN
A AIA
F, F, F,
as a, as Ow a as a,
Fs
The word tree is right-dominant, and the weak degenerate foot preceding the
main stress foot is eliminated.
In all systems discussed so far, main stress falls at the edge where foot
construction starts. Hammond (1985) states this in his Directionality Dominance
Hypothesis, according to which the first application of foot assignment uniquely
determines word tree dominance.' The Directionality Dominance Hypothesis
seems to be falsified by Creek (Hayes 1981) and Cairene Arabic (McCarthy
1979), where rightward foot construction combines with a right-dominant word
tree. Hammond, observing that both systems lack overt secondary stresses,
suggests that main stress and secondary stresses are on distinct parallel metrical
planes, a situation which renders them immune to the Directionality Dominance
Hypothesis. However, overt secondary stresses running towards the main stress
do occur in systems such as Wargamay (Dixon 1981) and Cayuga (Foster 1982),
which seems to reduce the Directional Dominance Hypothesis to a statement
regarding frequency, rather than a firm metrical universal.
IA I A IIA
F F F
A IA IIA
F
as a. a as aw a a as Ow a
I
0 as Ow a as aw a a as aw
L LLHL LHHL L LL L H LLHHL L
376 René Kager
L L L HL LHHL L L LLHLLHHLL
Central Siberian Yupik (Jacobson 1985) has rightward iambs (the final syl-
lable is never stressed, see section 1.4 on extrametricality) (see 18a), while
Tubatulabal (Voegelin 1935) has leftward iambs (see 18b).
FF
(18) (a)
aw
F
A Iaas
F (b)
0 a 0,
I I /\F
a,
L L HL HL L L
sa gti yaa ni taa ha wi la
"in his (another's) drum" "the summer(obj.)"
Both languages seem to lack prominence distinctions between stresses, which
is accounted for by not assigning a word tree. Iterative quantity-sensitive
trochaic systems are extremely rare, an observation to which we will return in
section 5.1. A noniterative example is Latin, as we see in section 1.4.1.
F,
Wd (b)
/\
Wd
F,
a
1 A
as aw
Fr
a
I
a
H LL H L
mux ta li <fa> mar ta <ba>
1
(c) Wd (d)
Z\ Wd
F,
AA
Z--------_____s.,,,,
F, F,
a
I
/\
a,
L
F,
ow
LH
F,
a
I
as aw
LLLLta Lhu <maa>
as cr.
sa ka kii <n>
4a ja ra
Yapese (Jensen 1977) has final stress except in words whose final vowel is
short and whose penultimate vowel is long. A bounded left-dominant
Obligatory Branching (OB) foot at the right edge of the word produces this
pattern. In (21c) we have a word that has no heavy syllables, and thus no OB
foot can be constructed; as a result, a right-dominant word tree is constructed
directly over syllables; see (21).
F F
A 0 a
I
as
as aw Ow
HL LH LL
sAalap "expert" magpda? "wedding" pa?ag "my hand"
AAA
F,
Gs Ow
L L HLHL
Gs
Fw F,
Ow as Ow
A
Gs Ow Ow aw
L LL L
Default-to-same systems stress a heavy syllable closest to an edge, else the
syllable at the same edge. Again, two mirror-image variants occur. Aguacatec
Mayan (McArthur and McArthur 1956) stresses the rightmost heavy syllable,
else the final syllable, Khalka Mongolian (Street 1963) the leftmost heavy
syllable, else the initial syllable. Halle and Vergnaud (1978) employ unbounded
Obligatory Branching feet. In words that have no heavy syllables, and hence
no feet, the word tree is constructed directly over syllables. Word tree
dominance matches the default side, as in (23), which represents the analysis
of Aguacatec Mayan.
ZNi
AAA Fw
GS GW
Fw
aW GS aw
F,
ow ow ow as
L L HLHL
GS
L LL L
Peripheral-plus-heavies systems stress a peripheral syllable and all heavy syl-
lables. The mirror-image variants are initial main stress plus heavies (Papago,
see Saxton 1963), and final main stress and heavies (Western Greenlandic
Eskimo, see Schultz-Lorentzen 1945). Here, the dominance of feet and word
trees match, as shown in (24), which represents the analysis of Papago.
F, F,,, Fw
s/\
(\
F
A A A
Gs aw as Ow Cc a
L asHLHL
Gs Ow
Ow
L L LL L
Metrical Theory of Word Stress 379
1.4 Extrametricality
The concept of extrametricality, introduced by Liberman and Prince (1977),
became a cornerstone of metrical theory in Hayes (1981). Extrametrical elements
are not analyzed by the metrical stress rules, neither regarding its structural
descriptions nor its structural change; informally speaking, rules may be said
to be "blind" to extrametrical elements, and those extrametrical elements may
be said to be "invisible" to the rules. Extrametricality is restricted to peripheral
elements, and has three types of motivation: (a) at word edges, it avoids foot
types that are otherwise rare or not found; (b) it functions to analyze
stresslessness of peripheral syllables, and (c) it marks exceptions to the stress
rules.
aAa a
1
a
1
L L H L
re fi<cit> re fe:<cit> fa<cit>
a
A
am, as
I
0 a
I
H LL H L
?acve<wa> qö tosom<pi> taa<vo> ko<ho>
1
Metrical Theory of Word Stress 381
(30)
rsr
feet are deleted, as in (30).
Wd
s
Wd
s
________-------....
AA
as
F,
Ow
Fw
as C7w
Fw
I A0,
0 as
nit ká na
Fs
, 6,
AAAF,
ths lu
aw
Fw
as
no
Gw a
ti nit kg na
as
Fs
aw
tits lu no ti
The output of destressing in (30) violates the prosodic exhaustivity requirement.
Metrical theory assumes that repair is automatic, in the form of a universal
convention of Stray Syllable Adjunction. Hayes suggests that Stray Syllable
Adjunction is structure-preserving: the dominance of derived feet matches
the system's parametric value, when possible. In (30) this makes the stray
syllable adjoin leftward under the preceding foot. Where this is impossible,
stray syllables are adjoined directly under the word tree.
Foot deletion renders the surface pattern stress opaque with respect to foot
assignment rules. Familiar considerations of learnability thus necessitate
constraints on destressing rules, an example of which is Hayes's condition that
destressing may not affect the main stress foot.
2 Grid Theory
Tree theory came under attack when Prince (1983) and Selkirk (1984) introduced
a pure grid theory. They showed that rhythmic notions such as alternation and
clash are best represented in grids. They also argued that metrical theory is
simplified by eliminating constituency altogether, since parametric theory can
be stated equally well in terms of pure grids.
382 René Kager
(31) * Wd
* *
* *F
******* a
Let us now focus on some formal properties of grid notation: (1) The grid
represents stress as a hierarchical rather than a relational property. (2) Grid
structure is subject to a constraint that forms the analogue of the closed prosodic
hierarchy in tree theory:
V I
a a
Thus grid theory marks heavy syllables as inherently stressed. In contrast, tree
theory marks heavy syllables as stressed only if they are heads of feet, and
unfooted heavy syllables are stressless.
Starting with a trough at the right edge, or with a peak at the left edge,
produces "trochaic" rhythm (36a, d). Starting with a trough at the left edge, or
with a peak at the right edge, produces "iambic" rhythm (36b, c). Thus Perfect
Grid makes a notion such as trochaic stress rule undefinable, since the starting
edge has to be taken into account. By the strictly alternating clash-avoiding
nature of Perfect Grid, no additional rules are needed to eliminate analogues
of degenerate feet in clashing positions. Compare (36a) to (11b), and (36b) to
(11c).
Since Perfect Grid only fills out portions of the grid that have been left blank
by the rule Quantity-sensitivity, quantity and rhythm become separate notions.
In contrast, tree theory integrates both into the concept of Foot.
(38) (a) *
(b) * ER(F;Wd)
* *
ER(I;F)
* * ** * * ** * * * * * * * *
LLHLLHL LLLLLLL
For Default-to-same systems, tree theory constructs the word tree over syllables
without intervening feet in the default case (see section 1.3.2). Analogously, in
Metrical Theory of Word Stress 385
grid theory, the End Rule defaults one layer down if no proper Foot layer
landing site is found, as in (39).
* * ** * * ** * * * * * * * *
LLHLLHL LLLLLLL
Peripheral-plus-heavies systems require End Rules at Foot and Word layers, at
identical edges, as in (40).
LLHLLHL LLLLLLL
2.3.1 Delete x
Destressing rules can be written in a simple format: Delete x. Three advantages
come from this. (a) This is a local operation, requiring no deletion of a prosodic
category, nor stray adjunction. (b) The triggering clash is directly represented.
A dominance parameter specifies whether to delete the first or the second of
two clashing grid marks, as in (41). (c) The integrity of the main stress needs
no stipulation, because the Continuous Column Constraint blocks deletion of
a grid mark supporting another on the next layer up.
2.3.2 Insert x
The second type of adjustment is the insertion of a grid mark to resolve a
lapse. Insert x is parametrized for dominance in much the same way as Delete
x, yielding two basic types, those in (42a) and (b).
(43) *
* * * * *
* * * * * * * * *
*** *** *** *** *** ***
Apalachicola Apalachicola (not Apalachicola)
2.3.3 Move x
Move x involves a (leftward or rightward) shift of a mark to resolve a clash,
as in (44).
(44) *
(a) * * (b) *
* * * * ** * *
*** _4 *** *** ___)
***
(45) (a) * * ** * *
(b)
** * * ** **
*** -) * ** *** -4 ***
Prince and Selkirk suggest that Move x may be decomposed into Delete x and
Insert x. Delete x resolves the clash, while Insert x assigns the rhythmic "antipole."
(47) F F
N a
I
asaw
?inkasa<rat> > ?ink-sa<rat>
This analysis has two interesting implications, both of which have been
confirmed by studies of similar phenomena in other languages, including
Tiberian Hebrew (Prince 1975), various Arabic dialects (Kenstowicz 1983; Hayes
1994), Russian (Halle and Vergnaud 1987), and Sanskrit (Halle and Vergnaud
1987). First, the deletion of a stressed vowel does not result in the deletion of
the stress, but rather into its migration to an adjacent vowel. Thus, stress
seems to display a stability effect that hitherto had been observed only in
autosegmental phenomena such as tone and length. Second, the direction of
the stress shift is predictable from the dominance of the foot whose head is
deleted: stress shifts rightward in trochees, leftward in iambs. More generally,
388 René Kager
within the foot, the stress shifts to the nonhead syllable. Stability follows from
the integrity of constituency, and the assumption that every constituent must
have a head.
(48) (a)
/
w
AAA
w s
------1:
NNN -
(c)
*
*
* *
**
*
** *
Wd
F
(50) (a) A I
(b)
N I
! I\ . .. IN ! .
F F F
N N FN
.
N
. . .
LLLHLLLHLL LLLLLL
The exhaustivity requirement makes primitive bounded feet expand into
derived unbounded feet by Stray Syllable Adjunction. Elimination of primitive
unbounded feet is supported by the observation that they are hard to motivate
as prosodic constituents by familiar diagnostics such as stress shifts, foot-
domain rules, and prosodic morphology (Prince 1983; Kager 1989).
390 René Kager
(54) line 2
(* * *) line 1
(* *) (* *) (* *) line 0
Apa lachi cola
(55) * line 2
* * *) line 1
(*
*)
(*) (*) (*) line 0
In Halle and Vergnaud's terminology, the rule set that constructs bounded
constituents on line 0 and locates their heads on line 1 is the Alternator, similar
to Perfect Grid, discussed above. It must be iterative by the Exhaustivity
Condition, requiring all line 0 elements to be in a constituent, and which they
construe as a condition on foot construction, i.e., on rule application. Thus
Halle and Vergnaud reject the iterativity parameter.
392 René Kager
In (57), an accent blocks construction of a left-headed foot over the first and
second syllable (under rightward application).
(57) * * * *
QS line 1
* * * * * * * * * *
--> -4 (*) (* *) (* *) line 0
LHLLL LHLLL LH LL L
Decomposition of quantity-sensitivity and rhythm unifies all bounded feet
construction by a single rule, the Alternator. We will see advantages of this in
section 4.3 on bidirectionality.
Another way in which heavy syllables can be marked off is by preassigning
a bracket at line 0, a mechanism introduced in Halle (1990). This device may
be employed in systems where stress-bearing units are rhyme segments (moras),
as in Cairene Arabic (see section 1.2.3). A preassigned left bracket "[" before
a heavy syllable blocks the construction of a line 0 constituent over the first
and second moras in (58):
(58) * * *
* ** * * * * [** * * *
__>
> (*) [**) (* *) (*)
LHLLL LHLLL L H LL L
If rhyme segments (moras) can be stress-bearing units, it is predicted that
foot boundaries may occur inside heavy syllables. Halle and Vergnaud argue
that this is the case in systems such as Winnebago. In words starting with a
sequence of light syllables, stress is on the third syllable, while in words starting
with a heavy syllable, stress is on the second syllable. The third mora is stressed
by initial mora extrametricality, and an initial right-headed bounded foot, as
in (59).
4.2.3 Ternarity
Hayes (1980) and Levin (1988a) draw attention to the stress pattern of Cayuvava
(Key 1961), where stresses are on the antepenultimate syllable and on every
third syllable preceding it. For such ternary systems, Halle and Vergnaud
introduce a parameter (+ / Head-Terminal). If the parameter is set negatively,
one nonhead element is allowed between a foot bracket and the head. The
result is a ternary, head-medial, amphibrach. The Cayuvava pattern is generated
by marking final syllables extrametrical and a leftward application of bounded
[HT] feet:
* * * line 1
(60)
********** (* * *) (* * *) (* * *) e> line 0
-->
(61)
* * line 2
*) line 1
* *) (
CE
*) (* * * * * * * * (* *) line 0
r* *)(* * * *)
LLLHLLLHL
LLL HLLLHL
Line Conflation also functions to analyze bidirectionality. Rejecting the
iterativity parameter, HV reanalyze bidirectional systems by means of two
iterative rules of opposing directionality. Main stress is generated by one
iterative pass, the output of which is subject to Line Conflation. A second
iterative pass from the opposite edge generates secondary stresses as in (62).10
(62) * * * line 2
r(*)*(* *)*)(* *) > (
* * * (* *) >
*)
(*
* *)
(* *) (*) (* *)
line 1
line 0
Finally, Halle and Vergnaud use Line Conflation for systems such as Eng-
lish, that have a quantity-sensitive main stress rule and a quantity-insensitive
secondary stress rule. The rules can be identified if the Alternator applies in
two strata. In the cyclic stratum, where the Alternator is preceded by Quantity
Sensitivity, Line Conflation eliminates all stresses but the primary. The noncyclic
Alternator assigns secondary stress quantity-insensitively, since Quantity
Sensitivity is not in the noncyclic stratum.
394 René Kager
,i
aZN
1-1
aa aaa
N/
1.1
P2
0
Y
a 0
\/
a a_
P2
The stress rules of the cyclic stratum apply to each of the planes P,, P2. Halle
and Vergnaud propose that information about stress recorded on the stem
plane is not carried over in the plane-copying;" see (64).
Let us see how the Vedic stress data are analyzed under this proposal. The
Basic Accentuation Principle can be formalized by a rule set which essentially
functions as the analysis of default-to-same systems (see section 1.3). In the
noncyclic stratum this rule set accounts for words with only recessive suffixes.
The same rule set is applied in the cyclic stratum to words containing dominant
cyclic suffixes. Here, stress erasure neutralizes any contrasts between accented
and unaccented stems before dominant suffixes. When the last dominant suffix
is accented, it ends up as the only accent surviving erasure, and it attracts
word stress. When the last dominant suffix is unaccented no accents survive
at all, and stress defaults to the initial syllable; see (65).
The patterns of the enclitic forms do not match the basic generalization on
stress, which is that stress is antepenultimate (instead of penultimate) when
396 René Kager
the penult is light. The opacity of stress is explained if the stress rules reapply
to enclitic forms while respecting the metrical structure of the base (cf. the Free
Element Condition discussed in section 3.3). Nonperipheral base-final syllables
lose their extrametricality:
(67) (a) * . * * *
(* *)
(*. *) (*) (*) (* *)
li:mina<que> > li:miná<que> not *li:mina<que>
* *
(b) *
(*) . (*) (*) (*. *)
mii:sa<que> > mu:sá<que> not *mii:sa<que>
Hayes (1985, 1987, 1994), McCarthy and Prince (1986), and Prince (1990)
develop a theory based on an asymmetric inventory of foot templates. It is
motivated by the typology of iterative bounded systems, as well as by pro-
cesses that change syllable quantity in foot-governed contexts. Another field of
motivation, prosodic morphology, is discussed in chapter 9 this volume.
5.1.3 lambs
The iamb produces patterns such as those below:
(72) (a) Iambs (left-to-right) (b) Iambs (right-to-left)
( *) ( *) (*) ( *) . ( *) . ( *) . ( *)
LH LLH LLL LHLLHLLL
Absence of degenerate feet is motivated by the stress patterns of final syllables
in systems with rightward iambs, which form the great majority of iambic
systems. The few leftward iambic systems (such as Tilbatulabal [Voegelin 1935],
18b) apparently require degenerate feet. Kager (1989), however, shows that
these can be reanalyzed by moraic trochees.
Most iambic systems have underlying quantitative contrasts, and are what
we might call truly quantity-sensitive. However, iambic rhythms also occur in
Metrical Theory of Word Stress 399
a few systems lacking weight distinctions, such as Weri. Hayes argues that such
systems are formally within the scope of the iambic expansion (L L)F, even
though they lack the uneven expansion (L H)F. Moreover, some of these systems
establish unevenness at the surface by rhythmic lengthening (see section 5.3).
(75) (a) (* .) - (* .)
6600606<a>
(b) ( *) ( *) . > ( *) ( *) (- *)
LLLLL LL LL LLLLL
6 Conclusion
After a decade of theoretical work on metrical systems, a consensus has emerged
on a number of points. First, stress requires hierarchical representation in
order to capture culminativity and prominence differences between stresses.
Second, the rhythmic nature of stress is most adequately represented by the
grid. Third, the grid is enriched by metrical constituency in order to capture
stress shifts, requirements of prosodic morphology, and template-governed
phenomena such as quantitative asymmetries. Researchers still seem to differ
in opinion about the symmetrical nature of the foot inventory, the status of
degenerate feet, exhaustivity, and the issue of what may constitute stress-
bearing units.
4F
402 René Kager
NOTES
This research was partially supported Estonian, Levin (1988a) for
by the Linguistic Research Foundation, Cayuvava, Woodbury (1987) for
which is funded by the Netherlands Yupik, and Dresher and Lahiri
organization for scientific research, (1991) for Germanic. See also
NWO, grant no. 300-171-023. For sections 4.2.1 and 5.4 on ternarity.
valuable comments on earlier versions 7 For a discussion of interactions
of this paper, I wish to thank John between metrical structure and
Goldsmith, Harry van der Hulst, and lexical phonology, see Kiparsky
Wim Zonneveld. (1982a, 1985) and chapters 2 and 3,
1 The learnability of stress systems is this volume.
studied from a parametric 8 Hammond (1987) claims that this
viewpoint by Dresher and Kaye power is not crucially needed.
(1990) and Hammond (1990). 9 There is thus no inherent
2 Other applications are dialectal connection between internal syllable
variation (Kenstowicz 1983) and structure and prosodic prominence,
diacronic phonology (Wheeler a link whose absence has been
1980). noticed.
3 Hammond (1986) argues for a foot 10 HV's analysis of bidirectionality is
type that restricts its dominant disputed by Levin (1988b). For an
nodes (to heavy syllables), without answer, see Halle (1990).
restricting its recessive nodes. He 11 See for discussion of the SEC,
proposes that this Revised Obligatory Harris (1989) and Halle, Harris, and
Branching foot should replace the Vergnaud (1991).
Obligatory Branching foot. The 12 Hayes cites experimental evidence
complex arguments for Revised from Woodrow (1951), who found
Obligatory Branching feet will not that rhythmically alternating stimuli
be reviewed here. with durational prominence
4 Onsets fail to contribute to weight, marking were perceived as iambic,
or only hardly ever do, though the and those with intensity marking
reader may see Everett and Everett were perceived as trochaic.
(1984) on Piraha, and Davis (1988) 13 Here we follow Hayes (1994), who
on Western Aranda, Madimadi, eliminates the degenerate stressless
Italian, and English. feet of Hayes (1987).
5 Van der Hulst (1984) proposes a 14 Recently, the asymmetric foot
more radical Main Stress First inventory has been challenged by
theory: Main stress is assigned proposals that advocate a
first, and secondary stresses run symmetric foot inventory, and
from the main stress, or the derive the rhythmic asymmetry by
opposite edge. independent means, cf. Jacobs
6 Nonbinary bounded feet have been (1990), Hammond (1990), Kager
proposed by Prince (1980) for (1993).