0% found this document useful (0 votes)
79 views30 pages

The Evidence Is in The Design

This document summarizes research on the instructional design principles of Direct Instruction (DI). It discusses four key design principles of DI: 1) explicit teaching of rules and strategies, 2) example selection, 3) example sequencing, and 4) covertization. The document analyzes the empirical evidence supporting each design principle and how they contribute to DI's effectiveness across content areas and learners. Overall, the research indicates DI achieves strong learning outcomes through its systematic approach to breaking down concepts, carefully selecting and ordering examples, and ensuring students understand generalizable strategies.

Uploaded by

Emma
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
0% found this document useful (0 votes)
79 views30 pages

The Evidence Is in The Design

This document summarizes research on the instructional design principles of Direct Instruction (DI). It discusses four key design principles of DI: 1) explicit teaching of rules and strategies, 2) example selection, 3) example sequencing, and 4) covertization. The document analyzes the empirical evidence supporting each design principle and how they contribute to DI's effectiveness across content areas and learners. Overall, the research indicates DI achieves strong learning outcomes through its systematic approach to breaking down concepts, carefully selecting and ordering examples, and ensuring students understand generalizable strategies.

Uploaded by

Emma
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
You are on page 1/ 30

Ball State University Libraries

A destination for research, learning, and friends

Notice: Warning Concerning Copyright Restrictions


The copyright Law of the United States (Title 17, United States
Code) governs the making of photocopies or other reproductions of
copyrighted material.

Any electronic copy or copies, photocopies or any other type of


Reproduction of this article or other distribution of this copyrighted
Material may be an infringement of the Copyright Law. This copy is
not to be “used for any purpose other than private study, scholarship,
or research [section 107]. “If a user makes or later uses any form of
reproduction of this copyrighted work for purposes in excess of section
107, Fair use, that user may be liable for copyright infringement.

This material is provided by the Ball State University Libraries. If you have
questions concerning this material or are unable to access an electronic document,
contact Interlibrary Loan Services via email at interlib@bsu.edu or by telephone at
765-285-1324 between 8:00 am – 5:00 pm during the academic year.

University Libraries
Perspectives on Behavior Science (2021) 44:195–223
https://doi.org/10.1007/s40614-021-00309-8
SPECIAL ISSUE: DIRECT INSTRUCTION

The Evidence is in the Design

Janet S. Twyman 1

Accepted: 14 July 2021/ Published online: 3 September 2021


# Association for Behavior Analysis International 2021

Abstract
To better understand the effectiveness of Direct Instruction (DI), the empirical base
related to DI’s instructional design components (explicit teaching, judicious selection
and sequencing of examples) and principles (identifying big ideas, teaching generaliz-
able strategies, providing mediated instruction, integrating skills and concepts, priming
background knowledge, and providing ample review) are analyzed. Attention is given
to the converging evidence supporting the design characteristics of DI, which has broad
applicability across different disciplines, teaching methodologies, and perspectives.

Keywords Direct instruction . Instructional design . Empirical evidence

Direct Instruction (DI) teaches so efficiently that all students learn the material in a minimal
amount of time. Such highly effective teaching requires a laser-like focus on the design,
organization, and delivery of instruction. Each and every DI program identifies concepts,
rules, strategies, and “big ideas” to be taught, clearly communicates these through carefully
crafted instructional sequences, organizes instruction to provide each student with appropri-
ate and sufficient practice (through scheduling, grouping, and ongoing progress monitoring),
and ensures active student engagement and lesson mastery via specific forms of student–
teacher interactions. Every DI strategy, tactic, technique, or procedure involves careful
design and testing. In Theory of Instruction: Principles and Applications (hereafter, Theory
of Instruction), Engelmann and Carnine (1982, 1991) lay out how DI, and perhaps all
instructional programs, should be designed.

What is Instructional Design?

Simply put, instructional design (ID) is the creation of materials and experiences
that result in a learner’s acquisition of new knowledge and skills. A more formal

* Janet S. Twyman
jstwyman@gmail.com

1
blast, 3075 South Morningsky Drive, Cottonwood, AZ 86326, USA
196 Perspectives on Behavior Science (2021) 44:195–223

description is that ID is “the process of systematically applying instructional


theory and empirical findings to the planning of instruction” (Dick, 1987, p.
183) “in a consistent and reliable fashion” (Reiser & Dempsey, 2007, p. 11). As
a discipline, ID is concerned with understanding, developing, improving, and
applying methods of instruction, thus creating optimal models, blueprints, or
programs for teaching (Reigeluth, 1983). Although ID is a field in its own right,
other diverse fields (e.g., education, psychology, philosophy, and performance
improvement) concern themselves with thinking, teaching, and learning, giving
rise to a range of theories on how people learn.
Learning theories identify the conditions of learning and provide a conceptual
framework and vocabulary for interpreting examples of learning. Instructional
designers turn to learning theories as a source of strategies for facilitating learning
and suggested solutions to learning problems (Ertmer & Newby, 2013; Joyce
et al., 1997; Markle, 1978). Learning theories abound, and typically represent
one of three philosophical frameworks: behaviorism, cognitivism, and
constructivism (Ertmer & Newby, 2013; Schunk, 2007). In summary, behavioral
learning theory learning outcomes are indicated by observable, measurable behav-
ior and greatly influenced by “environmental events” (see Skinner, 1954). Cogni-
tive theory traditionally focuses on the thought process behind the behavior;
changes in behavior are indicators of activity in the learner's mind (see Phillips,
1995). Constructivist theory contends that learner construct their own individual
perspectives of the world is based on experience and schema (internal knowledge
structure; see Anderson, 1983). Although distinctions often made among these
three types of learning theory, the instructional design procedures derived from
behavioral, cognitive, and constructivist approaches have more in common than
what is typically described—as evidenced by each field’s work in analytical
thinking, reasoning, and problem solving (for examples, see Markle, 1981;
Markle & Droege, 1980; Mechner et al., 2013; Robbins, 2011).
We concern ourselves with the design of instruction because we want our
teaching to be effective. How people learn and how they learn best form the basis
of instructional design. With the expanding multitude of instructional design
theories, systems, products, and graduate programs, it would seem logical to
simply access the evidence-base of best practices in instructional design to deter-
mine which model or program to use. However, the field of instructional design
(or the process of systematically applying instructional theory and empirical
findings to the design and delivery of instruction in particular) has yet to garner
that level of empirical consensus. Although numerous theories abound, few have
comprehensively undergone either a formative or summative evaluation process
replicated across learners, content, or context. As a model, Direct Instruction (DI)
may be one of the few exceptions.
Over 50 years of research shows DI to be highly effective with diverse learners
across a wide range of content, including basic skills, complex learning tasks, and
social emotional skills such as self-concept and confidence (see Mason & Otero, 2021;
Stockard et al., 2018; and Stockard, 2021). Why is DI so successful? An analysis of
instructional design components and principles essential to DI programs may contribute
towards an understanding of the model’s overall effectiveness. This article focuses on
research investigating the design features of DI.
Perspectives on Behavior Science (2021) 44:195–223 197

Design Principles Evident in Direct Instruction

Direct Instruction programs feature obvious teaching practices, regardless of academic


content, that define DI for many educators. DI teachers use scripted lessons, clear
instructional language, signals for students to respond in unison, frequent questioning
to achieve high rates of meaningful responding, and specific feedback with targeted
error correction (Binder & Watkins, 1990; Stein et al., 1998; Watkins & Slocum,
2004). Other salient features include the identification of teaching goals, ongoing
monitoring of student performance, and small-group instruction to ensure teachers
are working with students with similar skills.
Each of these teaching practices offers a robust evidence base on their own,
regardless of the name of the instructional model in which they are embedded (see
Embry & Biglan, 2008, for an analysis of evidence-based educational tactics). Each of
these teaching strategies also contributes to the DI research-base, because all DI
programs contain these highly effective elements. As Carnine et al. (1988) noted,
student perform better when teachers use materials that devote substantial time to
active instruction and distill complex skills and concepts into smaller, easy to under-
stand steps (ideally the largest attainable step). Student progress is often seen when
teachers teach in a systematic fashion, provide immediate feedback, and conduct much
of their instruction in small groups to allow for frequent interactions (Brody & Good,
1986; Rosenshine & Stevens, 1986).
DI is more than a container for individually effective instructional practices. It
integrates such practices with a careful and logical analysis of content so that learners
can accurately and systematically develop important background knowledge and apply
it to building new knowledge and knowledge frameworks (see Kame’enui, 2021; Stein
et al., 1998). In a comprehensive review of DI programs, Adams and Engelmann
(1996) noted, “‘effective’ practices without a systematic instructional sequence will not
necessarily lead to highly effective teaching” (p. 31). One of the most significant facets
of DI is its design and sequencing. With Theory of Instruction as its foundation, DI
reflects a meticulous and sophisticated analysis of curriculum, organization, and
sequence of instructional content. Kinder and Carnine (1991) identified four design
principles supporting DI: (1) explicit teaching of rules and strategies, (2) example
selection, (3) example sequencing, and (4) covertization. Each of these principles and
a review of the related research are described below.

Explicit Teaching of Rules and Strategies

DI programs make learning maximally effective through carefully designed instruc-


tional “communications” (i.e., what the teacher presents to the student) that do not rely
on inherent characteristics of the learner (Carnine, 1983). The goal of DI is “faultless
communication” between teachers and students; the communication is precise if and
only if the learner “understands” the information conveyed (Carnine, 1983; Kinder &
Carnine, 1991; Twyman, 2021).
Explicit teaching involves guiding student attention and responding toward a spe-
cific learning objective within a structured teaching environment. Curriculum content is
typically taught in a logical order with the teacher providing demonstration, explana-
tion, and opportunities for practice (often referred to as “Model, Lead, Test,” “I do, You
198 Perspectives on Behavior Science (2021) 44:195–223

do, We do,” or some variation). As noted in this example provided by Joseph et al.
(2016), many of the teaching practices used within explicit instruction (e.g., modeling,
prompting, frequent opportunities to respond with targeted feedback) are closely
aligned with both DI principles and applied behavior analytic teaching:

. . . teaching students to spell words correctly may involve the following learning
trials: (1) the teacher orally presents a word printed on a flashcard and prompts
the student to look at the word and spell it along with him or her (stimulus), (2)
the student spells the word with the teacher (response), and (3) the teacher
provides immediate performance feedback (consequence). (p. 2)

DI’s explicit teaching also involves specific design components, which are discussed
individually below. Since the late 1960s, research on explicit instruction has shown
positive effects when rules and strategies are openly, overtly, and clearly taught (e.g.,
step-by-step instructions for a science experiment or how to think “out loud” when
solving a problem, aka “talk aloud problem solving” [TAPS]; Whimby & Lochhead,
1982).
Improved outcomes have been demonstrated across grades (preschool through
adult), student abilities (in special and general education settings), and subject matter
(e.g., mathematics, language arts, social studies, science, art, and music; Archer &
Hughes, 2010; Hall, 2002; Hughes et al., 2017). Pearson and Dole (1987) synthesized
the research on three explicit instruction strategies for comprehension instruction
(reciprocal teaching, process training, and inference training) and found explicit in-
struction taught comprehension more effectively traditional basal reading approaches
(i.e., mentioning, practicing, and assessing). In a meta-analysis summarizing explicit
teaching strategies to teach critical thinking, Bangert-Drowns and Bankert (1990)
report 18 out of the 20 studies that met the review criteria consistently showed the
positive effects of explicit instruction on critical thinking.
Other components of explicit instruction, such as those found in DI programs, have
been shown to be highly successful with a range of student and teacher populations and
classroom settings (Adams & Engelmann, 1996; Ellis & Worthington, 1994;
Rosenshine & Stevens, 1986). Results from this extensive research base indicate that
all students, not only those with learning difficulties or in special classrooms, benefit
from well-designed explicit instruction (Hall, 2002). The explicit teaching of rules and
strategies component of DI is well-supported in the research, and further adds to the
support of DI as an evidence-based practice.

Example Selection

As noted by Layng (2016), our sense of the world is founded on our understanding of
simple and complex concepts. The preponderance of curriculum content and instruction
in K–12 and higher education involves concept teaching. A concept is defined the
common attributes found in each example of the concept. Concept teaching relies on the
analysis of examples and nonexamples (see Layng, 2019, for a tutorial). The selection of
examples (and how the range of examples could be improved upon) is fundamental to
DI. Consistent instructions communicate that the examples are the same and prompt the
learner to induce the ways in which the examples are the same (Twyman, 2021).
Perspectives on Behavior Science (2021) 44:195–223 199

A concept cannot be taught with a single example (Markle & Tiemann, 1970, 1971).
A wide range of examples (is the concept) must be presented. These should vary
greatly, yet are treated the same way (as a member of the concept). Negative examples
(is not the concept) are also required; they help identify the boundaries of accurate
generalization. Although overlapping with positive instances in various ways, the
differences that make examples negative (aka nonexamples) should be clear.
Theory of Instruction explains four critical facts about presenting examples:

1. It is impossible to teach a concept through the presentation of one example. A


single example allows for any number of possible interpretations.
2. It is impossible to present a group of positive examples that communicates only
one interpretation. A set of positive examples still allows for more than one
possible interpretation.
3. Any sameness share by both positive and negative examples rules out a possible
interpretation. The communication given with positive and negative examples
differs (e.g., “This is red” vs. “This is not red.”), thus any sameness observed
between the positive and negative examples narrows the possible interpretations by
ruling out the shared characteristic.
4. A negative example rules out the maximum number of interpretations when the
negative example is at least different from some example. The more shared
characteristics between positive and negative examples, the narrower the possible
interpretation (more interpretations get ruled out). (Engelmann & Carnine, 1991, p.
37)

The emphasis on selecting positive and negative examples in Theory of Instruction


overlaps greatly with an analysis of “critical and variable attributes.” To be considered
an instance of the concept, an example must have certain defining, critical attributes.
An example missing any of the critical attributes is not an instance of the concept.
Variable attributes are characteristics or features that an example may have, but which
have no effect on the example’s membership within a concept (Layng et al., 2011;
Markle & Tiemann, 1969; Tiemann & Markle, 1990).
Harlow’s (1949) work with humans and primates in the development and testing of
“learning sets” provides early model of the careful selection of examples. Harlow
proposed that humans and other highly intelligent mammals not only learned isolated
relationships but also noticed patterns that made learning more efficient, thus becoming
faster at solving new problems upon experience solving similar classes of problems
(Harlow & Warren, 1952; see also Critchfield & Twyman, 2014). This “learning to
learn” phenomenon seems to be directly related to the “sameness” principle described
in Theory of Instruction. By treating various examples the same way in instruction, the
learner begins to identify what is the same about them and begins to treat new instances
the same way—this is true even for problem-solving behavior.
Several empirical investigations have tested methods of example selection, espe-
cially during program design or revision. Working with 47 first- through fifth-grade
students who had some knowledge of fractions but none of decimals, Carnine (1980a)
found significantly greater transfer of accurately converting fractions with a denomi-
nator of 100 to decimals when demonstrations included a range of positive examples of
fractions with single, double, and triple digit numerators (e.g., 7/100 = .07, 26/100 =
200 Perspectives on Behavior Science (2021) 44:195–223

.26, and 159/100 = 1.59) compared to a similar group of students who received the
same training but with only one type of positive example (a double digit numerator,
e.g., 26/100 = .26, 43/100 = .43, 78/100 = .78). The selection of negative examples is
also integral to concept learning (Tennyson, 1973). In a series of studies involving
teaching line angles to preschoolers, Williams and Carnine (1981) found clear differ-
ences in skill transfer when example sets contained positive and negative examples of
angles compared to positive examples alone.
To investigate the effects of the degree of difference between positive and negative
examples, Carnine (1980b) developed five sets of examples, each of which contained
the same positive examples but different negative examples. The negative examples
varied to the degree to which they were different from the positive example (least to
most different). The data showed the example set with the least differences between
positive and negative examples resulted in fewer training trials to criterion (7.6)
compared to maximum difference between positive and negative examples group
(26.0), supporting the design component of using close-in negative examples to reduce
unintended interpretations of a concept.
Regarding concept teaching, Becker et al. (1975) analyzed four procedures designed
to establish stimulus control of operant behavior (i.e., teaching general case), and
demonstrated that a concept cannot be established with a single positive or a single
negative example. Rather a set of positive examples that exhibited all the relevant
concept characteristics as well as a negative example set that exhibited some or none of
the characteristics were both necessary to teach the concept. Irrelevant characteristics
also had to be varied across positive and negative examples. As noted by Carnine
(1980a, p. 109), “Minimal stimulus variations between positive and negative examples
can clearly define a concept boundary, and maximal relevant variations among positive
examples shows how the full range of positive examples should be treated.”

General case instruction Engelmann and Carnine (1991) stipulated that generalized
responding requires the systematic selection of multiple examples that sample the range
of stimulus and response variation. Often called “general case instruction,” this meth-
odology has shown great utility in the applied research literature, perhaps because
general case strategies use the smallest possible number of examples to produce the
largest possible gains in learning (Horner et al., 2005). In general case instruction,
teachers use a wide range of comparative examples (e.g., different species of plants,
different minerals) to prepare students for novel situations outside of the classroom
(Kozloff et al., 1999). The general case model evolved from DI principles and
procedures and shares its careful use and sequencing of examples (viz. Becker et al.,
1975; O’Neill, 1990).
In addition to its successful use in DI programs, general case programming has been
shown to be effective with students with moderate (Gersten & Maggs, 1982) to severe
disabilities (Horner et al., 1982; Horner & Albin, 1988), and adults (Ducharme &
Feldman, 1992) across a variety of concepts and skills such as solving math story
problems (Neef et al., 2003), requesting (Chadsey-Rusch & Halle, 1992), cooking
(Trask-Tyler et al., 1994), operating vending machines (Sprague & Horner, 1984),
vocational skills (Horner & McDonald, 1982), and dressing (Day & Horner, 1986).
Applying general case instruction not only supports careful identification and sequenc-
ing of teaching examples, it also promotes generalization and more efficient learning.
Perspectives on Behavior Science (2021) 44:195–223 201

For example, by using general case strategies to teach 40 sounds and blending skills,
students can learn a generalized decoding skill that applies to at least half of the words
used in the English written language (Binder & Watkins, 1990).
The example selection and sameness-and-different approach is used in many models
of concept formation, knowledge instruction, and extension to “real world” activities.
As Markle and Tiemann (1970) noted, “[t]he key to producing the kind of generaliza-
tion and discrimination that we want lies in the selection of a special rational set of
examples and non-example” (p. 43) based on an analysis of how subject matter experts
respond to the concept in the real world.
Likewise, Klausmeier (1980) focused on the attributes of examples and
nonexamples of concepts, identifying four levels of concept learning: (1)
concrete—recall of critical attributes, (2) identity—recall of examples, (3)
classification—generalizing to new examples, and (4) formalization—
discriminating new instances. Earlier work with colleagues identified a schema
for testing concept mastery that focused not only on the examples and
nonexamples of members of the concept, but also examples and nonexamples of
each critical attribute as well (Frayer et al., 1969; Klausmeier et al., 1974).
Concept learning is considered to have occurred when a learner is able to
discriminate attributes of a concept and to evaluate new examples based on
inclusion in the concept category (Klausmeier & Feldman, 1975).
A thorough analysis has gone into the selection of examples and nonexamples used
within DI programs. The method and rationale for example selection not only has
support via direct empirical investigations (providing research-guided and research-
tested evidence), but also strong parallels in the concept building literature (providing
research-informed support). These findings provide strong empirical support for the
example selection methodology used within DI.

Example Sequencing

Although careful example selection is essential for teaching concepts, the sequence
with which those examples and nonexamples are presented is also critical. Early
research on concept teaching revealed that the presentation order of set of examples
influences what is perceived about the examples, such as how things are the same or
different (Klausmeier & Feldman, 1975; Markle, 1970; Tennyson & Park, 1980).
Theory of Instruction provides five guidelines (called juxtaposition principles) for
effectively presenting examples and nonexamples:

1. The wording principle. To make the sequence of examples as clear as possible, use
the same wording on juxtaposed examples (or wording that is as similar as
possible).
2. The set-up principle. To minimize the number of examples needed to demonstrate
a concept, juxtapose examples that share the greatest possible number of features.
3. The difference principle. To show differences between examples, juxtapose exam-
ples that are minimally different and treat the examples differently.
4. The sameness principle. To show sameness across examples, juxtapose examples
that are greatly different and indicated that the examples have the same label.
202 Perspectives on Behavior Science (2021) 44:195–223

5. The testing principle. To test the learner, juxtapose examples that bear no predict-
able relationship to each other (Engelmann & Carnine, 1991, pp. 38–40).

Readers interested in example sequencing and related topics from an applied perspec-
tive are encouraged to view the articles by Johnson and Bulla, Slocum and Rolf, Rolf
and Slocum, Spencer, and Twyman and Hockman in the special section on Direct
Instruction in the September 2021 issue of Behavior Analysis in Practice.
An effective means of juxtaposing examples is through continuous conversion (i.e.,
how examples are changed from one example to the next). Creating examples using
continuous conversion focuses the learner’s attention on the relevant changes between
the examples (e.g., holding ones hands apart, then moving them further apart to show
“wider”). Continuous conversion emphasizes change, allowing learners to focus on the
critical details involved in the change (Engelmann & Carnine, 1991). Also known as
the adjustment method, continuous conversion may the oldest and perhaps most
effective procedure to teach dimensional concepts where a comparison is required
(see Goldiamond & Thompson, 2004). In laboratory experiments the adjustment
response occurs when the participant adjusts a stimulus until it reaches some criterion
or makes an indicator response when a continually varying stimulus matches a constant
standard.
These rules also influence the methods used to broaden the members of a concept,
class, or set. Although demonstrating sameness and difference remain critical, the
operations of interpolation, extrapolation, and stipulation help set the occasion for
generalization (see Engelmann & Carnine, 1991, Ch. 14). In brief, interpolation
involves changes along a single stimulus dimension, such as color hue or the addition
or removal of parts. When a learner is presented with three examples differentiated
along the single dimension, the learner will include any intermediate example as part of
the set (e.g., when shown examples of light blue, dark blue, and blue, all identified as
“blue” any example of a blue whose hue is on the continuum of the examples).
Extrapolation involves changes from positive to negative examples (or vice versa). If
a small change makes a positive example negative, then any greater change along the
same dimension will also make the example negative. For example, if two pictures are
hung on the same horizontal plane (signaling “level”), moving one picture up 2 inches
signals “not level.” Moving the picture up 2 feet would provoke the generalization of
“not level.”
Stipulation requires repeated demonstrations of select examples that are highly
similar, and presenting only those examples. This implies that any example outside
the range is not the same (and should be treated differently). Stipulation relies on
multiple presentations of the selected examples (such as numerous presentation of long
objects held upright, signaling “vertical”). When one of the objects is presented at a
discernable angle it would be treated differently (“not vertical”). This is somewhat
related to the procedures of responding by exclusion first identified by Dixon (1977),
involving a match-to-sample conditional discrimination procedure in which a novel
sample stimulus (one not previously related to any other stimulus) is presented with a
novel comparison stimulus and at least one previously known comparison stimulus.
Across basic and applied studies, participants almost consistently select the novel
stimulus while excluding sample-comparison pairs previously learned (e.g., Grassmann
et al., 2015; Langsdorff et al., 2017).
Perspectives on Behavior Science (2021) 44:195–223 203

Carnine and colleagues formatively evaluated variables related to continuous con-


version, juxtaposition and other procedures related to example sequencing prior to and
during the development of DI curricula. These procedures also have a history in the
basic work of behavioral experimenters (for example see Azrin et al., 1961; Baum,
2012; Goldiamond & Malpass, 1961). Carnine (1980b) compared the continuous
conversion procedure for presenting examples to procedures recommended by other
researchers who suggested that examples are best presented by changing the empha-
sized features after every second set of examples. When comparing the Tennyson set up
procedure (Tennyson, 1973; Tennyson et al., 1972; both cited in Carnine, 1980b) and a
noncontinuous conversion procedure, Carnine found continuous conversion resulted in
statistically significant training efficiency (i.e., fewer trials to criterion). In two separate
experiments evaluating the effects of juxtaposition principles, Granzin and Carnine
(1977), found that minimally different positive and negative training examples resulted
in fewer trials to criteria when compared to maximally different examples. Carnine and
Stein (1981) demonstrated the importance of juxtaposition in inducing a single trans-
formation relationship in teaching 6-year-olds related math facts, showing that properly
juxtaposed transformation procedures resulted in greater efficiency and generality.
Stipulation procedures have been used effectively with students with severe intellectual
disabilities (Sprague & Horner, 1984), early elementary students (L. Carnine, 1979,
cited in Carnine, 1983), and students with motor skills deficits (Horner & McDonald,
1982), all demonstrating that students can learn that examples outside a predetermined
range are not the same and are thus treated differently.
A clear set of logical rules that can be applied to any interaction involving commu-
nicating through examples is essential for any well-designed instructional program.
This foundational, consistent rationale for selecting and sequencing examples was
outlined by Markle (1969) and is evident in Gilbert’s (1912, 1969) mathetics, and
Mechner's (1967) content and concept analysis protocols used in programmed instruc-
tion. More recent examples can be seen in internet-delivered instruction, such
Headsprout Early Reading and Headsprout Reading Comprehension (see Layng
et al., 2003, 2004; Leon et al., 2011).
The methods of analytical example sequencing in DI programs have strong research-
tested support in the education literature. These five juxtaposition principles along with
the use of continuous conversion, extrapolation, interpolation, and stipulation illustrate
how DI programming principles guide the design and implementation of instruction
(Binder & Watkins, 1990; Twyman & Hockman, 2021).

Covertization

Although explicit teaching procedures such as talk aloud problem-solving strategies are
effective in teaching problem solving and other reasoning skills (Robbins, 2011;
Whimby & Lochhead (1982, 1999), strategies that are made explicit during teaching
do not have to remain explicit. The overt routines established during initial instruction
make problem solving easier, however these routines should be shifted to interactions
that contain few instructions or prompts and ultimately to covert responding by the
learner.
Engelmann and Carnine (1991) provide guidelines for “covertizing” routines, in-
cluding systematic reduction of overt responses over time and keeping the transition
204 Perspectives on Behavior Science (2021) 44:195–223

simple and relatively errorless from step to step. This can be done by dropping steps,
regrouping a chain of instructions and responses, providing new “inclusive” wording
(that provides less detail or combines steps), or providing equivalent pairs of instruc-
tions (used during covertization when later instructions do not appear earlier in the
routine). Specific procedures may be governed by the type of task and can be
combined. For a comprehension description of covertization routines, see Engelmann
and Carnine (1991), Chapter 21.
The process of covertization has been empirically evaluated. In a study looking at
miscues in early reading by kindergarten through third-grade children, Carnine et al.
(1984) found that teacher directed exercises on sentence and story reading promoted
covertization of a specific set of reading skills (such as anticipating meaning or self-
correction of nonsensical words). Paine et al. (1982) investigated the need for
covertization with third graders who were asked to generalize a math operation to a
new situation. The researchers found that novel, independent student performance
returned to mastery levels only after working through covertization routines.

Evidence-Based Design Components of Direct Instruction

Within a DI program, explicit teaching relies upon specific design components. Stein
et al. (1998) and later Carnine and Kame’enui (1992) described several design practices
central to DI: (1) identify big ideas; (2) teach explicit and generalizable (conspicuous)
strategies; (3) provide mediated or scaffolded instruction; (4) integrate skills and
concepts strategically; (5) prime background knowledge; and (6) provide adequate
judicious review.

Identify Big Ideas

Direct Instruction focuses on “big ideas.” Big ideas are essential concepts within a
content area are concepts or principles that facilitate the broadest acquisition of
knowledge in the most efficient manner, and aide in the organization and interrelation
of information (Carnine, 1997; Carnine et al., 1996; Slocum, 2004; Stein et al., 1998).
Big ideas untangle a content area for a broad range of diverse learners by providing
thorough knowledge of the most important aspects of a given content area (Kame'enui
et al., 1998). Kame’enui and Carnine (1998) provide the following example as a big
idea in a DI science curriculum: "the nature of science, energy transformations, forces
of nature, flow of matter and energy in ecosystems, and the interdependence of life" (p.
119). Big ideas “are essential in building a level of scientific literacy among all students
that is necessary for understanding and problem-solving within the natural and created
world" (pp. 121–122). For example, to understand how a big idea is integrated across
concepts, consider instruction on each of the properties of density, heat, and pressure as
a strand (see Fig. 1). These strands overlap, leading to a larger integrated concept (e.g.,
convection cell). As a big idea, convection is illustrated as air circulating in a room.
Integration of the concept is shown when convection is applied to liquid boiling in a
pot, or to mantle, ocean, and ocean–land convection (Grossen et al., 2011).
Another comprehensive example of the integration of a big idea is illustrated within
a DI history curriculum. “Change” is a common theme through history and the big idea
of “problem-solution-effect” primes and generates understanding of numerous
Perspectives on Behavior Science (2021) 44:195–223 205

Fig. 1 Convection as a Big Idea

historical movements (Carnine et al., 1994). Big ideas facilitate the generalization of
knowledge to other areas and become part of the context of prior knowledge to which
students integrate new learning (Kozloff et al., 1999). Big ideas enable instructional
designers to organize instruction in ways that promote conceptual understanding (Stein
et al., 1998). The goal is for students to acquire knowledge that is comprehensive, rich
in detail, cohesive, and generalizable.
An overabundance of standards, educational goals, and teaching objectives are now
available to educators. Early methods of determining what to teach emphasized an
analysis of the knowledge or verbal repertoire of experts on a given topic (Markle,
1969). Subject matter expertise and the identification of critical knowledge came from
this approach. Subject matter in any given area can be quite vast. In the 1960s,
psychologists such as Gagné and Bruner postulated schemas for dealing with classes
(concepts), hierarchies of classes (conceptual structures), and relationships between the
classes (Bruner, 1960, 1966; Gagné, 1962, 1965, 1985; see also Smith, 2002). Gagné’s
(1985) “knowledge hierarchy” proposes a ranked list of all knowledge; a list of all
intellectual skills and all learning that progresses from the simplest to the most
206 Perspectives on Behavior Science (2021) 44:195–223

complex. Pieces of knowledge have associations and chains, discriminations, concepts,


rules and generalizations, and higher-order rules; each one a prerequisite for the one
that follows. In Gagné’s hierarchical model higher-level information can only be
learned or understood when all the appropriate lower-level knowledge is mastered.
This is similar to one of the most frequently used hierarchies in education for the past
50 years: Bloom’s taxonomy of educational objectives (Bloom, 1956). Bloom et al.,
(2001) categorized educational goals in a continuum from simple to complex and
concrete to abstract, along six major categories: knowledge, comprehension, applica-
tion, analysis, synthesis, and evaluation. Big ideas, as theorized in DI, endeavors to
support all six.
Although there are many theories surrounding content organization and concept
teaching (such as Merrill et al., 1992; Prater, 1993; Reigeluth & Darwazeh, 1982;
Rowley & Hartley, 2008; Tennyson & Park, 1980), the research empirically evaluating
the effectiveness of these theories is relatively thin. Most evaluations entail a compar-
ison to theoretical framework or a set of design criteria (see Jitendra & Kame’enui,
1994). Johnson and Stratton (1966) evaluated concept learning using logical classifi-
cation, four teacher-derived strategies (defining the concept, using it in a sentence, and
providing synonyms) and a mixed method. The mixed method resulted in the greatest
learning of new concepts, with each of the four independent methods showing equal
effectiveness. In a review of concept-teaching procedures in five basal language
programs for the early grades, Jitendra and Kame’enui (1988) found the programs
rarely met 11 design-of-instruction criteria drawn from empirically based models of
concept teaching. The lack of well-controlled studies highlights the need for empirical
evaluations of both the effectiveness and the efficiency of teaching “big ideas” or other
forms of concept learning.

Teach Explicit (Conspicuous) and Generalizable Strategies

Strategies are one way in which learners apply knowledge to a novel context. A
strategy is a general framework or process, used by learners to analyze content and
solve problems. For example, students may use the TINS method (Owen, 2003) when
learning to solve math word problems. TINS stands for the overt steps used to solve the
equation: T-Think (Think about what operation you need to do to solve this problem
and circle the key words); I-Information (Write the information needed to solve the
problem/draw a picture to represent the problem); N-Number (Write a number sentence
to represent the problem); S-Solution (Write a solution sentence that explains your
answer).
When learned overtly, cognitive routines aid in solving complex problems. DI
programs teach generalizable strategies in visible and unambiguous ways. Cognitive
routines are presented in explicit fashion, making them more similar to physical
routines in that they can be observed, prompted, corrected, or reinforced. Once learned
and applied to various (cross curriculum) examples, the overt instruction is gradually
withdrawn and the learner moves to performing the strategy covertly (Carnine, 1983;
Carnine et al., 1988; Carnine et al., 1996).
When converting thinking into doing, the instructional communication becomes
clear. However, explicit cognitive routines are much more than simply making the
covert overt. To successfully serve as a bridge the overt routine must completely and
Perspectives on Behavior Science (2021) 44:195–223 207

solely account for the outcome or solution to the problem. In other words it must be
consistent with a single generalization and cover the full range of problem types of
which it is to be applied. Generalizable strategies not only support student across a
range of problem types, they assist learners in gaining new knowledge. (For a
description of the formation of explicit strategies in DI see Carnine, 1983, Carnine
et al., 1988; Carnine et al., 1996; or Stein et al., 1998.)
There is a sizable body of research evidence supporting DI’s use of explicit
strategies. Following a survey of the existing literature, Falk and Wehby (2005)
reported that the type of explicit and systematic instruction found in DI can improve
academic and school outcomes for with emotional/behavioral disorders. Darch et al.
(1984) looked at the effectiveness of teaching fourth graders to write mathematical
equations from word story problems. They compared an explicit step-by-step teaching
procedure to procedures identified in basal textbooks and found significant positive
effects for the explicit teaching procedure.
In a controlled demonstration of the utility of conspicuous strategies, Ross and
Carnine (1982) investigated the role of making strategies overt by comparing three
teaching strategies: discovery, rule-plus-discovery, and rule-plus-overt-steps, within
and across groups of second, third, and fifth graders. The discovery groups were given
an example and asked if it fit the rule (with no explanation given). Students in the rule-
plus-discovery groups were given the same examples but told the rule/explanation
beforehand. The rule-plus-overt-steps groups were presented the rule and for each
example, asked and told specifically why it did or did not fit the rule. In all grades
the discovery strategy did significantly poorer on the training criterion. In the younger
grades, 20% of the rule-plus-discovery children met the criterion compared to 83% of
children who received the rule-plus-overt-steps. Carnine et al. (1982) demonstrated that
with “sophisticated” learners the presentation of a rule without application may be
sufficient, however, to make the learning perfectly clear the presentation of the rule
should be followed with its application to a (carefully identified and sequenced) range
of examples. These findings further reiterate the effectiveness of the “Ruleg” procedure
of presenting a rule followed by examples and nonexamples first advocated by Evans
et al. (1962); also see Hermann, 1971, for a comparison of “Ruleg” vs. “EgRule”).
The research base also informs us about the positive effects of explicit strategies in
general. In an investigation of explicit-strategy instruction, whole-language instruction,
and a combination of the two, Butyniec-Thomas and Woloshyn (1997) found exper-
imental conditions that included explicit-strategy instruction generally were superior to
the whole-language condition, suggesting that third-grade children learn to spell best
when they are explicitly taught a repertoire of effective spelling strategies in a mean-
ingful context. Reading instruction that provides direct and explicit teaching in specific
skills and strategies have been shown to be most effective, both across grades and ages
(Biancarosa & Snow, 2006; National Institute of Child Health & Human Development,
2000; Torgesen et al., 2007).
In an experiment examining the impact that explicit instruction in heuristic strategies,
Schoenfeld (1979) carefully controlled conditions (training in problem solving, equal
time working on the same problems, identical problem solutions) with half the students
show explicitly how five strategies were used. The heuristics group significantly
outperformed the other students. Hollingsworth and Woodward (1993) found explicit
strategy group performed significantly better on transfer measures when evaluating
208 Perspectives on Behavior Science (2021) 44:195–223

secondary students’ ability to link facts, concepts, and problem solving in an unfamiliar
learning domain. Lastly, in an extensive and important review of the literature evaluat-
ing guided models of instruction (e.g., DI) to minimally guided models (i.e., discovery,
inquiry, experiential, and constructivist learning), Kirschner et al. (2006) found that
providing only minimal guidance during instruction “simply does not work” (as
paraphrased from the title: Why Minimal Guidance During Instruction Does Not Work:
An Analysis of the Failure of Constructivist, Discovery, Problem-Based, Experiential,
and Inquiry-Based Teaching). There is a substantial, clear empirical base suggesting that
all students benefit from having effective strategies made apparent for them during
instruction (Adams & Engelmann, 1996; Hall, 2002; Hattie, 2009, 2012; Hattie &
Yates, 2013; Pearson & Dole, 1987). The explicit teaching of strategies is a critical
design component of DI; its empirical base provides ample evidence of the strength of
DI programs.

Scaffolded or Mediated Instruction

DI programs move instruction from a more teacher-led format to one requiring less
teacher guidance. This process is often called mediated scaffolding (Kame'enui et al.,
1998). Scaffolding reduces task complexity by structuring knowledge into manageable
chunks to increase successful task completion, and supports the learner’s development
just beyond the level of what the learner can do alone (Larkin, 2001; Olson & Platt,
2000; Raymond, 2000). Scaffolds occur most often during initial learning and may be
in the form of prompts, cues, steps, or tasks provided either in-person (by teachers or
peers) or embedded in the materials (via the sequence and selection of tasks). Mediated
scaffolding calibrates ongoing modification of teacher assistance and the design of the
examples used in teaching based on the student’s current knowledge or abilities (Coyne
et al., 2007). Scaffolding assists students by teaching problem-solving strategies,
helping students distinguish essential and nonessential details, and fading assistance
provided from the teacher or by the materials (Becker & Carnine, 1981). An important
consideration when scaffolding instruction is that the scaffolds are temporary; the type
or degree of scaffolding is individualized and thus varies with the evolving abilities of
the learner, goals of instruction, and complexities of the task. As the learner’s knowl-
edge competency increases, the teacher-provided support decreases. The purpose of
scaffolding is to help all students to become independently successful in activities
(Hall, 2002; Kame'enui et al., 2002).
Instructional scaffolding enjoys broad support in the education literature.
Scaffolding theory was first introduced by Bruner (1978) to describe how parents help
their young children acquire oral language. As a teaching strategy, most historians
associate scaffolding with Vygotsky’s sociocultural theory and the concept of the zone
of proximal development. Vygotsky wrote “The zone of proximal development is the
distance between what children can do by themselves and the next learning that they
can be helped to achieve with competent assistance” (quoted in Raymond, 2000, p.
176). Scaffolds help learners through the zone of proximal development by supporting
their ability to build on prior knowledge and internalize new information (Chang et al.,
2002). According to Vygotsky, external scaffolds can be removed because the learner
has gained “. . . more sophisticated cognitive systems, related to fields of learning such
as mathematics or language, the system of knowledge itself becomes part of the
Perspectives on Behavior Science (2021) 44:195–223 209

scaffold or social support for the new learning” (Raymond, 2000, p. 176). The
constructivist literature recently repackaged instructional scaffolding as “cognitive
apprenticeship.” Building on the practice of apprenticeship and its long history of use
in transmitting physical expertise (e.g., in learning a trade or a craft), a cognitive
apprenticeship facilitates learning by using experts to help novices develop cognitive
skills through participating in “authentic” learning experiences. Collins et al. (1989, p.
456, cited in Dennon, 2004) describe it as “learning-through-guided-experience on
cognitive and metacognitive, rather than physical, skills and processes” (see also
Collins et al., 1991).
With regard to prompt and cues within instructional and the fading of these stimuli,
the behavior analytic literature is replete with studies of procedural and overall effec-
tiveness (Cooper et al., 2020; MacDuff et al., 2001). The goal of these tactics is the
transfer of stimulus control from the prompt or cue to the stimulus itself. Early
behavioral research began with Terrace’s classic work on the transfer of stimulus
control using fading and the superimposition of stimuli (Terrace, 1963) and continued
with Touchette’s work on the systematic delay of the delivery of prompts or cues
(Touchette, 1971).
Empirical studies have specifically investigated scaffolding procedures and con-
firmed the utility of specific, concise, structured methods that translate to independent
skills (Darch & Carnine, 1986). In a study investigating an interactive teaching strategy
designed to expedite reading comprehension, Bos et al. (1989) found that when junior
high students with learning disabilities were given a prompt to self-monitor compre-
hension, their performance increased to equal that of average students. Within a one-to-
one setting, Wertsch and Stone (1984) demonstrated how adult language directed a
child with learning disabilities to strategically monitor his actions and clearly showed
the transfer of responsibility from teacher-regulated to student-regulated behaviors.
Given the spread of online education, Jumaat and Tasir (2014), provide an interesting
meta-analysis of the types of scaffolding that could be used in an online learning
environment and offer suggestions for validating students’ success in an online learning
environment.
The research base on scaffolding shows a sizeable body of evidence indicating
improved student learning with thoughtfully guided instruction (McKeough et al.,
1995; Moreno, 2004; Rosenshine & Meister, 1995). Additional research or parametric
studies investigating the various types of scaffolding (teacher delivered, via materials,
or embedded within computer-based instruction) and how scaffolds are mediated or
faded over time would be beneficial. Although additional investigations would be
helpful in evaluating specific scaffolding procedures within DI programs, the existing
empirical data on scaffolding clearly indicate it is another solid research-based com-
ponent of DI programs.

Integrate Skills and Concepts Strategically

As noted previously, DI relies on clear, logical, and unambiguous communications tied


to “big ideas” and their related “systems of knowledge classification” (Carnine et al.,
1988). Concepts are not taught in isolation but instead are integrated within and across
content areas. Strategic integration is used to illustrate how these analyses and appli-
cations can direct student progression toward higher-order thinking skills. Strategic
210 Perspectives on Behavior Science (2021) 44:195–223

integration links critical information together to promote new and more complex
knowledge resulting in multiple benefits for the learner including learning when to
apply knowledge.
Characteristics of strategic integration include (1) curriculum design that provides
opportunities to combine several big ideas, (2) content applicable to multiple contexts,
and (3) incorporation of concepts once mastered (Hall, 2002). Integration allows
students to examine relationships among various concepts, and ultimately to combine
relevant information to acquire new and more complex knowledge. Once a skill has
been identified as important for teaching, designers must consider how to integrate it
into a meaningful context. As noted by Stein et al. (1998), “A key role of an
instructional designer is to determine those concepts that highlight relationships both
within a content area and across different disciplines and to design instruction that
facilitates understanding of those relationships” (p. 231). High quality, strategic inte-
gration (1) facilitates the assimilation of concepts and knowledge, and (2) aids the
learner in applying new knowledge and “big ideas” to multiple contexts, including
those beyond classroom applications (Hall, 2002; Kame'enui et al., 1998).
Graphic organizers—tables, charts, maps, or other visual schema—are frequently
used within DI programs to convey big ideas and illustrate how information is
interrelated. Early work in this type of organization can be traced back to Ausubel’s
(1960, 1968) “advanced organizer” system. Used to convey large amounts of informa-
tion within a subject area, advanced organizers start with a visual representation of the
most general ideas within the concept and then proceed to introduce more detail and
specificity. Graphic organizers often provide templates or frames for highlighting
essential content, organizing information, and recording connections between facts,
concepts, and ideas (Fountas & Pinnell, 2001; Lovitt & Horton, 1994).
There is a relatively strong research base supporting the use of graphic or other
visual organizers to enhance content comprehension (Barton-Arwood & Little, 2013;
Boyle & Weishaar, 1997; Gardill & Jitendra, 1999; Horton et al., 1990). Students at all
grades and levels have been shown to benefit from the use of graphic organizers across
a variety of content areas, because they provide support when previous information is
reviewed and new information is presented (Dye, 2000; Gagnon & Maccini, 2000).
When investigating the use of graphical organizers to teach conceptual content, Darch
and Carnine (1986) and Darch et al. (1986) found such systems were highly related to
increased student performance. In a study examining the relative effectiveness of visual
spatial displays in enhancing comprehension during instruction, Darch and Eaves
(1986) found that adolescent students with learning disabilities showed greater perfor-
mance on short-term recall tests when taught with the visual displays (no differences
were found with transfer or maintenance tests). Likewise, Carnine and Kinder (1985)
found low-performing students made sizable improvements in generative and schema
strategies for narrative and expository materials even with short use of these strategies.
The authors emphasized the importance of carefully designed, concentrated instruction
that increases students’ attention to text.
A robust empirical research base supports the integration of skills and concepts, in
particular with the use of visual and graphic organizers (see Hall & Strangman, 2002).
The research has grown to include the computer-assisted use of visual organizers (e.g.,
Anderson-Inman & Horney, 1997; Lin et al., 2004). Further study is needed to
determine the transfer and generalization of these types of integration techniques
Perspectives on Behavior Science (2021) 44:195–223 211

presented via the computer. Advance organizers go beyond chapter summaries by


using visual systems to bridge the gap between new material and the knowledge the
student already has. Advance organizers help prime the student for learning and are a
frequently used design component within DI.

Prime Background Knowledge

Direct Instruction programs stress the importance of building new knowledge from
what is already known or just taught. “The primary goal of [DI] is to increase not only
the amount of student learning but also the quality of that learning by systematically
developing important background information and explicitly applying it to new knowl-
edge” (Stein et al., 1998, p. 228). Research indicates that learning new skills or gaining
new knowledge is largely dependent upon the knowledge a learner brings to the
situation (Carnine et al., 1996; Marzano, 2004), making prior knowledge an essential
variable in learning (Dochy et al., 1999). The utility of that previous knowledge is
directly related to the accuracy of the information and the learner’s ability to use that
information (Hall, 2002). Students with insufficient background knowledge (or those
unable use this knowledge) often have difficulty progressing within the general curric-
ulum (Strangman & Hall, 2004). To improve knowledge acquisition for all learners, the
effective priming of background knowledge is an important design component of DI.
Direct Instruction programs prime background knowledge by connecting new
information to what students have previously learned, by making comparisons between
known items and the new concept or relate the topic to events or knowledge that are
familiar to students. Some see priming as a method of addressing any memory or
strategy deficits learners may bring to new tasks, as priming emphasizes critical
features, activates current knowledge, and thus increases the likelihood of learner
success on new tasks (Hall, 2002). In fact, priming background knowledge may be
essential for many students, as an “analysis of history textbooks showed that texts
assumed unrealistic levels of students’ prior knowledge and these unrealistic levels
negatively influenced student understanding” (McKeown& Beck, 1990). It also has an
interesting connection to analyses of dimensional versus instructional guidance of
behavior (see Goldiamond, 1966), where dimensional control (SDd) could be consid-
ered a “what” discrimination whereas instructional control (SDi) could be considered a
“how” discrimination (Layng et al., 2011). When presented with a hat, a learner may
say “hat” (a tact); however, when the presentation is paired with varying instructions,
such as “What color is it?,” “Is it soft or hard?,” or “Who wears a hat like this?,” the
responses may change to “red,” “hard,” and “a fireman.” In each case the SDi (the
instruction) narrows the response alternatives without changing the SDd (the hat;
example derived from Layng et al., 2011). It would be interesting to determine if and
how manipulation of SDi’s might assist in priming a learner’s background knowledge
by manipulating SDi guidance and highlighting connections to prior knowledge.
Preteaching, or emphasizing critical concepts and how they relate to what is already
known prior to teaching new material, is one way of priming background knowledge. A
number of applied studies have shown that explicit preteaching using background
knowledge substantially increases student understanding of relevant reading material
(Graves et al., 1983; McKeown et al., 1992; Stevens, 1980). Stevens (1982) found that
students who received specific instruction on the pertinent background related to an
212 Perspectives on Behavior Science (2021) 44:195–223

expository text demonstrated significantly greater reading comprehension of the text


than those that received irrelevant instruction. Likewise, Dole et al. (1991) showed that
students who learned relevant background information had significantly greater per-
formance on comprehension questions than students without background knowledge
instruction. In a component analysis of a DI math program, Kame’enui and Carnine
(1986) found preteaching improved the subtraction performance of second graders, as
compared to teaching component skills concurrently. Pearson et al. (1979) evaluated
the comprehension of textually explicit and inferable information by above-average
second grade readers and found a significant prior-knowledge effect on inferable
knowledge but not on explicitly stated information. Thus, priming background knowl-
edge seems to promote the development of inferential knowledge based on new
material. With regard to concept formation, Pazzani (1991) demonstrated that prior
knowledge influences the rate of concept learning by college undergraduates and also
found that prior causal knowledge can influence the logical form being acquired.
Numerous classroom-based studies support the positive effects of priming back-
ground knowledge, and various techniques have been developed to do so effectively
and efficiently (Dochy et al., 1999; Singleton & Filce, 2015). These include instruction
on background knowledge, student recording of background knowledge, and contact
with background knowledge through questioning (Strangman & Hall, 2004). A poten-
tial line of research could involve the use of augmented and virtual reality applications
to create new experiences for learners, which then become “background knowledge”
for a new concept or skill to be learned. Additionally, research is needed in the areas of
student characteristics (e.g., familiarity with a content area and the accuracy of their
prior knowledge) and how to provide instruction that clarifies and expands prior
knowledge. The current research base provides ample direct support for instructional
strategies that build and activate prior knowledge, making it a viable research-tested
component of DI’s instructional design.

Provide Adequate Judicious Review

When building concepts and acquiring new knowledge, effective review of new
information to novel contexts or at different times aids in the transfer, maintenance,
and generalization of learning (Hall, 2002). Planned, thoughtful, specific (i.e., judi-
cious) review is crucial to the solid understanding and application of the “big ideas”
learned throughout DI programs. These programs carefully embed review into the
teaching sequence, so that it is cumulative across knowledge, distributed across content,
varied for practice across contexts (Carnine et al., 1996), and sufficient to cover the
material presented (Stein et al., 1998). Regular progress monitoring allows teachers to
individualize when, what, and how much information to review.
Computers can for provide effective opportunities for review. Johnson et al. (1987)
compared two computer-based vocabulary programs to teach 50 vocabulary words to
high school students identified as learning disabled. Twenty-five students were ran-
domly assigned to either the small set condition (teaching and practice exercises on
small sets 10 with cumulative review exercises on all words) or the large set condition
(teaching and practice exercises on two sets of 25 words and no cumulative review). Of
the 12 students assigned to the small teaching set, 10 achieved mastery more quickly
than those with the larger teaching set, indicating frequent review to be more
Perspectives on Behavior Science (2021) 44:195–223 213

efficacious. In another computer-based investigation related to the review of chunks of


information, Gleason et al. (1991) found the cumulative presentation of smaller chunks
of information, where compared to the rapid introduction of new information, resulted
in shorter learning time to mastery, fewer errors, and less student frustration for 60
elementary and middle school students (many with learning disabilities).
Mayfield and Chase (2002) compared three methods of teaching basic algebra rules
to college students. The same procedures were used to teach the rules, with the kinds of
practice provided during the four review sessions as the only differences between
methods. On the final test, the cumulative review group scored highest on application
and problem-solving items and completed the problem-solving items significantly more
quickly than the two other groups. The authors suggest cumulative practice of compo-
nent skills to be an effective method of training problem solving. Additional studies
support the use of cumulative practice and review to promote new knowledge, espe-
cially within traditional academic domains. Frequent, cumulative review has become
commonly recommend as an instructional methodology, especially for students who
struggle to learn (as evidenced by its recommendation as a “best practice” by the
Institute for Educational Sciences [Gersten et al., 2009]).
Although the benefits of frequent, cumulative review have been established within the
education community, the specific parameters of cumulative, distributed, varied, and suffi-
cient review have received less direct empirical scrutiny. Although derived in general from
the research base on review, these specific forms of review used in DI programs would
benefit from a more direct investigation of the efficacy of each of the components.

Conclusion

The theory of instruction underpinning DI rests on fundamental philosophical notions


about knowledge, meaning, logic, and rigor, including the fundamental idea that
“instruction is inductive in nature” (Engelmann & Carnine, 1991, p. 368). DI programs
make these notions actionable through a careful, systematic blend of content analysis,
logical analysis, and behavior analysis.
A critically important, yet often underused, form of instructional design comes from
empirical evaluations of principles and procedures during program development (Layng
et al., 2006; Markle, 1967). Engelmann and Carnine developed the principles and proce-
dures elucidated in Theory of Instruction based on logic. However, logic by itself was (and
is) insufficient for developing a cohesive, workable, effecting instructional technology.
Empirical validation is vital. Engelmann (2009) reported that each time the authors formu-
lated a principle, strategy, or tactic, they asked each other, “Do you know of any studies
confirming this?” If not, Carnine and colleagues conducted formal experiments testing the
logic, resulting in nearly 50 published studies evaluating components described in Theory of
Instruction (see reviews or summaries in Becker, 1992; Becker & Carnine, 1980, 1981;
Weisberg et al., 1981). Thus, they continuously validated the logical analyses1 via applied

1
Engelmann (2009) reports that only one component was not validated, that of beginning instruction with a
negative example (e.g., “This is not glurm”). Effectiveness for the procedure produced mixed results in that
law students learned with the procedure whereas preschoolers did not (reportedly because preschool partic-
ipants did not attend to the negative example).
214 Perspectives on Behavior Science (2021) 44:195–223

and field test studies, providing empirical support for specific design principles and teaching
practices, and ultimately using the results to determine if communications are having their
intended effect . . . or if, where, or how they might need to be refined (Binder & Watkins,
1990; Engelmann, 2009).
Numerous meta-analyses of the research base firmly support DI components
or teaching strategies (Adams & Engelmann, 1996, Becker & Gersten, 1982;
Ehri et al., 2001, Gersten, 1985; Marzano et al., 2001; Mason & Otero, 2021;
White, 1988). As noted by Binder and Watkins (1990), “Direct Instruction
principles and procedures are supported by general principles derived from
basic behavioral research as well as by the literature of effective teaching
practices” (p. 90). This type of scientifically validated instruction is critical
for helping resolve our educational crisis (NICHHD, 2000; No Child Left
Behind Act, 2001). However, the merit of any instructional program is not
based on how rigorously it was developed. A program’s ultimate value is
determined by the outcomes it produces in educational settings across the range
of teachers and students found within our schools. A summary of the research
reviews related to DI indicates that it (1) is consistent with known effective
instructional practices; (2) has been found by independent evaluation to have
strong positive effects; and (3) has been shown effective across student popu-
lations, grade levels, and content areas.
Reform in education often has focused on the goals of teaching and less on
how those goals can be attained (Cuban, 1990). The attention on evidence-based
practices has moved the spotlight to specific teaching practices, and determining
which practices are most likely to be effective with what populations under what
conditions (see Layng et al., 2006). This shift has tremendous implications for
those who design curricula and instructional programs, underscoring the critical
need to:

(1) base instructional design and curriculum development on the best “state-of –the
science” practices known to date (i.e., research-informed);
(2) conduct multiple iterations of “formative evaluation” assessments of each com-
ponent of instruction (i.e., research-guided); and
(3) subject the instructional program to multiple variations of “summative evaluation”
to determine the field conditions under which it does, and doesn’t work (i.e.,
research-tested).

It is clear from the evidence base that the theory, model, programs, and practices of DI
have addressed all three.
The effectiveness of an instructional program should be determined by from
its empirically derived evidence base. The research base associated with the
principles and design features of DI reveals verification of effects across all
three forms of evidence (research-tested, research-informed, and research-guided
instruction), solidifying Direct Instruction as an evidence-based practice.

Declarations

Conflicts of Interest The author has no known conflicts of interest to disclose.


Perspectives on Behavior Science (2021) 44:195–223 215

References

Adams, G. L., & Engelmann, S. (1996). Research in Direct Instruction: 25 years beyond DISTAR.
Educational Achievement Systems.
Anderson, J. R. (1983). The architecture of cognition. Harvard University Press.
Anderson-Inman, L., & Horney, M. (1997). Computer-based concept mapping: Enhancing literacy with tools
for visual thinking. Journal of Adolescent & Adult Literacy, 40(4), 302–306 www.jstor.org/stable/
40013436.
Archer, A. L., & Hughes, C. A. (2010). Explicit instruction: Effective and efficient teaching. Guilford Press.
Ausubel, D. P. (1960). The use of advance organizers in the learning and retention of meaningful verbal
material. Journal of Educational Psychology, 51(5), 267–272. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0046669.
Ausubel, D. P. (1968). Educational psychology: A cognitive view. Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
Azrin, N. H., Holz, W., Ulrich, R., & Goldiamond, I. (1961). The control of the content of conversation
through reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 4(1), 25. https://doi.org/10.
1901/jeab.1961.4-25.
Bangert-Drowns, R. L., & Bankert, E. (1990). Meta-analysis of effects of explicit instruction for critical
thinking (ED328614). ERIC https://files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/ED328614.pdf.
Barton-Arwood, S. M., & Little, M. A. (2013). Using graphic organizers to access the general curriculum at
the secondary level. Intervention in School & Clinic, 49(1), 6–13. https://doi.org/10.1177/
1053451213480025.
Baum, W. M. (2012). Rethinking reinforcement: allocation, induction, and contingency. Journal of the
Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 97(1), 101–124. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.2012.97-101.
Becker, W. C. (1992). Direct Instruction: A twenty-year review. In R. P. West & L. A. Hamerlynck (Eds.),
Designs for excellence in education: The legacy of B. F. Skinner (pp. 71–112) Sopris West.
Becker, W. C., & Carnine, D. W. (1980). Direct Instruction: An effective approach to educational intervention
with the disadvantaged and low performers. In B. B. Lahey & A. K. Kazdin (Eds.), Advances in clinical
and child psychology. Plenum.
Becker, W. C., & Carnine, D. W. (1981). Direct Instruction: A behavior theory model for comprehensive
educational intervention with the disadvantaged. In S. W. Bijou & R. Ruiz (Eds.), Behavior modification:
Contributions to education. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Becker, W. C., & Gersten, R. (1982). A follow-up of follow through: The later effects of the direct instruction
model on children in fifth and sixth grades. American Educational Research Journal, 19(1), 75–92.
https://doi.org/10.3102/00028312019001075.
Becker, W. C., Engelmann, S., & Thomas, D. R. (1975). Teaching 2: Cognitive learning and instruction.
Science Research Associates.
Biancarosa, C., & Snow, C. (2006). Reading next—A vision for action and research in middle and high school
literacy: A report to the Carnegie Corporation of New York (2nd ed.). Alliance for Excellent Education.
Binder, C., & Watkins, C. L. (1990). Precision Teaching and Direct Instruction: Measurably superior
instructional technology in schools. Performance Improvement Quarterly, 3(4), 74–96. https://doi.org/
10.1111/j.1937-8327.1990.tb00478.x.
Bloom, B. S. (1956). Taxonomy of educational objectives. Vol. 1: Cognitive domain. David McKay Co Inc.
Bloom, B. S., Airasian, P., Cruikshank, K., Mayer, R., Pintrich, P., Raths, J., & Wittrock, M. (2001). A
taxonomy for learning, teaching, and assessing: A revision of Bloom's taxonomy of educational objec-
tives. Prentice Hall.
Bos, C. S., Anders, P. L., Filip, D., & Jaffe, L. E. (1989). The effects of an interactive instructional strategy for
enhancing reading comprehension and content area learning for students with learning disabilities.
Journal of Learning Disabilities, 22(5), 384–390. https://doi.org/10.1177/002221948902200611.
Boyle, J. R., & Weishaar, M. (1997). The effects of expert-generated versus student-generated cognitive
organizers on the reading comprehension of students with learning disabilities. Learning Disabilities
Research & Practice, 12(4), 228–235.
Brody, J., & Good, T. (1986). Teacher behavior and student achievement. In M. Wittrock (Ed.), The third
handbook of research on teaching (pp. 328–375). Macmillan.
Bruner, J. (1960). The process of education. Harvard University Press.
Bruner, J. S. (1966). Toward a theory of instruction. Belknap Press of Harvard University Press.
Butyniec-Thomas, B., & Woloshyn, V. E. (1997). The effects of explicit-strategy and whole-language
instruction on students' spelling ability. Journal of Experimental Education, 65(4), 293–302 Abstract
retrieved on January 11, 2011, from http://www.jstor.org/pss/20152531.
216 Perspectives on Behavior Science (2021) 44:195–223

Carnine, D. (1980a). Relationships between stimulus variation and the formation of misconceptions. Journal
of Educational Research, 74(2), 106–110. https://doi.org/10.1080/00220671.1980.10885292.
Carnine, D. (1980b). Three procedures for presenting minimally different positive and negative instances.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 72(4), 452–456. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.72.4.452.
Carnine, D. (1983). Direct Instruction: In search of instructional solutions for educational problems. In D.
Carnine, D. Elkind, D. Melchenbaum, R. Lisieben, & F. Smith (Eds.), Interdisciplinary voices in learning
disabilities and remedial education (pp. 1–66). Pro-Ed.
Carnine, D. (1997). Instructional design in mathematics for students with learning disabilities. Journal of
Learning Disabilities, 30(2), 130–141. https://doi.org/10.1177/002221949703000201.
Carnine, D., & Kame’enui, E. J. (1992). Higher order thinking: Designing curriculum for mainstreamed
students. Pro-Ed.
Carnine, D., & Kinder, D. (1985). Teaching low-performing students to apply generative and schema
strategies to narrative and expository material. Remedial & Special Education, 6(1), 20–30. https://doi.
org/10.1177/074193258500600104.
Carnine, D. W., & Stein, M. (1981). Organizational strategies and practice procedures for teaching basic facts.
Journal of Research in Mathematics Education, 12(1), 65–69. https://doi.org/10.2307/748659.
Carnine, D. W., Kame’enui, E. J., & Maggs, A. (1982). Components of analytic assistance: Statement saying,
concept training and strategy training. Journal of Educational Research, 75(6), 374–377. https://doi.org/
10.1080/00220671.1982.10885412.
Carnine, L. M., Carnine, D., & Gersten, R. (1984). Analysis of oral reading errors economically disadvantaged
students taught with a synthetic phonics approach. Reading Research Quarterly, 19(3), 343–356. https://
doi.org/10.2307/747825.
Carnine, D., Granzin, A., & Becker, W. (1988). Direct Instruction. In J. Graden, J. Zins, & M. Curtis (Eds.),
Alternative educational delivery systems: Enhancing instructional options for all students (pp. 327–349).
National Association of School Psychologists.
Carnine, D., Crawford, D., Harniss, M. K., & Hollenbeck, K. (1994). Understanding U.S. history: Vol.1
Through the Civil War. Considerate Publishing.
Carnine, D., Caros, J., Crawford, D., Hollenbeck, K., & Harniss, M. K. (1996). Designing effective U.S.
history curricula for all students. Advances in Research on Teaching, 6, 207–256.
Chadsey-Rusch, J., & Halle, J. (1992). The application of general-case instruction to the requesting repertoires
of learners with severe disabilities. Journal of the Association for Persons with Severe Handicaps, 17(3),
121–132. https://doi.org/10.1177/154079699201700301.
Chang, K., Sung, Y., & Chen, I. (2002). The effect of concept mapping to enhance text comprehension and
summarization. Journal of Experimental Education, 71(1), 5–23. https://doi.org/10.1080/
00220970209602054.
Collins, A., Brown, J. S., & Holum, A. (1991). Cognitive apprenticeship: Making thinking visible. American
Educator, 15(3), 6–11 https://www.aft.org/ae/winter1991/collins_brown_holum.
Cooper, J. O., Heron, T. E., & Heward, W. L. (2020). Applied behavior analysis (3rd ed.). Pearson Education.
Coyne, M. D., Kame’enui, E. J., & Carnine, D. W. (2007). Effective teaching strategies that accommodate
diverse learners (3rd ed.). Pearson Education.
Critchfield, T. S., & Twyman, J. S. (2014). Prospective instructional design: Establishing conditions for
emergent learning. Journal of Cognitive Education & Psychology, 13(2), 201–217. https://doi.org/10.
1891/1945-8959.13.2.201.
Cuban, L. (1990). Reforming again, again, and again. Educational Researcher, 19(1), 3–13. https://doi.org/10.
2307/1176529.
Darch, C., & Carnine, D. (1986). Teaching content area material to learning disabled students. Exceptional
Children, 53(3), 240–246. https://doi.org/10.1177/001440298605300307.
Darch, C., & Eaves, R. C. (1986). Visual displays to increase comprehension of high school learning-disabled
students. Journal of Special Education, 20(3), 309–318. https://doi.org/10.1177/002246698602000305.
Darch, C., Carnine, D., & Gersten, R. (1984). Explicit instruction in mathematics problem solving. Journal of
Educational Research, 77(66), 350–359. https://doi.org/10.1080/00220671.1984.10885555.
Darch, C., Carnine, D., & Kame’enui, E. (1986). The role of graphic organizers and social structure in content
area instruction. Journal of Reading Behavior, 18(4), 275–295. https://doi.org/10.1080/
10862968609547576.
Day, H. M., & Horner, R. H. (1986). Response variation and the generalization of a dressing skill: Comparison
of single instance and general case instruction. Applied Research in Mental Retardation, 7(2), 189–202.
https://doi.org/10.1016/0270-3092(86)90005-6.
Dennon, V. P. (2004). Cognitive apprenticeship in educational practice: Research on scaffolding, modeling,
mentoring, and coaching as instructional strategies. In D. H. Jonassen (Ed.), Handbook of research on
Perspectives on Behavior Science (2021) 44:195–223 217

educational communications and technology (2nd ed., pp. 813–828). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
http://www.aect.org/edtech/ed1/31.pdf.
Dick, W. (1987). A history of instructional design and its impact on educational psychology. In J. A. Glover &
R. R. Ronning (Eds.), Historical foundations of educational psychology (pp. 183–202). Plenum Press.
Dixon, L. S. (1977). The nature of control by spoken words over visual stimuli selection. Journal of
Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 27(3), 433–442. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1977.27-433.
Dochy, F., Segers, M., & Buehl, M. M. (1999). The relation between assessment practices and outcomes of
studies: The case of research on prior knowledge. Review of Educational Research, 69(2), 145–186.
https://doi.org/10.3102/00346543069002145.
Dole, J. A., Valencia, S. W., Greer, E. A., & Wardrop, J. L. (1991). Effects of two types of prereading
instruction on the comprehension of narrative and expository text. Reading Research Quarterly, 26(2),
142–159. https://doi.org/10.2307/747979.
Ducharme, J. M., & Feldman, M. A. (1992). Comparison of staff training strategies to promote generalized
teaching skills. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 25(1), 165–179. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1992.
25-165.
Dye, G. (2000). Graphic organizers to the rescue! Teaching Exceptional Children, 32(1), 1–5. https://doi.org/
10.1177/004005990003200311.
Ehri, L. C., Nunes, S. R., Stahl, S. A., & Willows, D. M. (2001). Systematic phonics instruction helps students
learn to read: Evidence from the National Reading Panel’s meta-analysis. Review of Educational
Research, 71(3), 393–447. https://doi.org/10.3102/00346543071003393.
Ellis, E. S., & Worthington, L. A. (1994). Research synthesis of effective teaching principles and the design of
quality tools for educators (Technical Report No. 5). University of Oregon, National Center to Improve
the Tools of Educators.
Embry, D. D., & Biglan, A. (2008). Evidence-based kernels: Fundamental units of behavioral influence.
Clinical Child & Family Psychology Review, 11(3), 75–113. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10567-008-0036-x.
Engelmann, S. (2009, July). Theory of direct instruction. Address presented at the 35th annual National Direct
Instruction Conference, Eugene, OR. Video podcast: http://www.zigsite.com/video/theory_of_direct_
instruction_2009.html
Engelmann, S., & Carnine, D. (1982). Theory of instruction: Principles and applications. Irvington
Publishers.
Engelmann, S., & Carnine, D. (1991). Theory of instruction: Principles and applications (rev. ed.). ADI Press.
Ertmer, P. A., & Newby, T. J. (2013). Behaviorism, cognitivism, constructivism: Comparing critical features
from an instructional design perspective. Performance Improvement Quarterly, 26(2), 43–71. https://doi.
org/10.1002/piq.21143.
Evans, J., Homme, L., & Glasser, R. (1962). The RULEG system for the construction of programmed verbal
learning sequences. Journal of Educational Research, 55(9), 513–518. https://doi.org/10.1080/00220671.
1962.10882862.
Falk, K. B., & Wehby, J. H. (2005, Spring). The impact of explicit reading instruction on student achievement:
study findings. Impact, 18(2), 30 & 39.
Fountas, I., & Pinnell, G. (2001). Guiding readers and writers grades 3-6: Teaching comprehension, genre,
and content literacy. Heinemann.
Frayer, D. A., Fredrick, W. C., & Klausmeier, H. J. (1969). A schema for testing the level of concept mastery.
Wisconsin Univ. Research & Development Center for Cognitive Learning.
Gagné, R. M. (1962). The acquisition of knowledge. Psychological Review, 69(4), 355–365. https://doi.org/
10.1037/h0042650.
Gagné, R. M. (1965). The conditions of learning and theory of instruction. Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
Gagné, R. M. (1985). The conditions of learning and theory of instruction (4th ed.). Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
Gagnon, J., & Maccini, P. (2000). Best practices for teaching mathematics to secondary students with special
needs: Implications from teacher perceptions and a review of the literature. Focus on Exceptional
Children, 32(5), 1–22. https://doi.org/10.17161/foec.v32i5.6919.
Gardill, M. C., & Jitendra, A. K. (1999). Advanced story map instruction: Effects on the reading compre-
hension of students with learning disabilities. Journal of Special Education, 33(1), 2–17. https://doi.org/
10.1177/002246699903300101.
Gersten, R. (1985). Direct instruction with special education students: A review of evaluation research.
Journal of Special Education, 9(1), 41–58. https://doi.org/10.1177/002246698501900104.
Gersten, R., & Maggs, A. (1982). Teaching the general case to moderately retarded children: Evaluation of a
five-year project. Analysis & Intervention in Developmental Disabilities, 2(4), 329–343. https://doi.org/
10.1016/0270-4684(82)90028-3.
218 Perspectives on Behavior Science (2021) 44:195–223

Gersten, R., Beckmann, S., Clarke, B., Foegen, A., Marsh, L., Star, J. R., & Witzel, B. (2009). Assisting
students struggling with mathematics: Response to Intervention (RtI) for elementary and middle schools
(NCEE 2009-4060). National Center for Education Evaluation and Regional Assistance, Institute of
Education Sciences, U.S. Department of Education. http://ies.ed.gov/ncee/wwc/publications/
practiceguides/
Gilbert, T. E. (1962). Mathetics: The technology of education. Journal of Mathetics, 1(1), 7–74.
Gilbert, T. F. (1969). Some issues in mathetics. I. Saying what a subject matter is. NSPI Journal, 8(2), 4–19.
https://doi.org/10.1002/pfi.4180080203.
Gleason, M., Carnine, D., & Valla, N. (1991). Cumulative versus rapid introduction of new information.
Exceptional Children, 57(4), 353–358.
Goldiamond, I. (1966). Perception, language, and conceptualization rules. In B. Kleinmuntz (Ed.), Problem
solving (pp. 183–224). Wiley.
Goldiamond, I., & Malpass, L. F. (1961). Locus of hypnotically induced changes in color vision responses.
Journal of the Optical Society of America, 51, 1117–1121. https://doi.org/10.1364/josa.51.001117.
Goldiamond, I., & Thompson, D. (2004). The blue books: Goldiamond & Thompson's the functional analysis
of behavior. (updated ed.; P. T. Andronis, ed.). Cambridge Center for Behavioral Studies. (Original work
published 1967)
Granzin, A. C., & Carnine, D. W. (1977). Child performance on discrimination tasks: Effects of amount of
stimulus variation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 24(4), 332–342. https://doi.org/10.1016/
0022-0965(77)90011-x.
Grassmann, S., Schulze, C., & Tomasello, M. (2015). Children’s level of word knowledge predicts their
exclusion of familiar objects as referents of novel words. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1200. https://doi.org/
10.3389/fpsyq.2015.01200.
Graves, M. F., Cooke, C. L., & Laberge, M. J. (1983). Effects of previewing difficult short stories on low
ability junior high school students' comprehension, recall, and attitudes. Reading Research Quarterly,
18(3), 262–276. https://doi.org/10.2307/747388.
Grossen, B., Carnine, D. W., Romance, N. R., & Vitale, M. R. (2011). Effective strategies for teaching
science. In M. D. Coyne, E. J. Kame’enui, & D. W. Carnine (Eds.), Effective teaching strategies that
accommodate diverse learners (4th ed., pp. 181–212). Pearson Education.
Hall, T. (2002). Explicit instruction. National Center on Accessing the General Curriculum. http://aim.cast.org/
learn/historyarchive/backgroundpapers/explicit_instruction.
Hall, T., & Strangman, N. (2002). Graphic organizers. National Center on Accessing the General Curriculum.
https://www.northernhighlands.org/cms/lib5/NJ01000179/Centricity/Domain/18/Graphic_Organizers_
2008.pdf
Harlow, H. F. (1949). The formation of learning sets. Psychological Review, 56(1), 51–65. https://doi.org/10.
1037/h0062474.
Harlow, H. F., & Warren, J. M. (1952). Formation and transfer of discrimination learning sets. Journal of
Comparative & Physiological Psychology, 45(5), 482–489. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0057267.
Hattie, J. C. (2009). Visible learning: A synthesis of over 800 meta-analyses relating to achievement.
Routledge.
Hattie, J. (2012). Visible learning for teachers: Maximizing impact on learning. Routledge.
Hattie, J., & Yates, G. C. (2013). Visible learning and the science of how we learn. Routledge.
Hermann, G. D. (1971). Egrule vs. ruleg teaching methods: Grade, intelligence, and category of learning.
Journal of Experimental Education, 39(3), 22–33. https://doi.org/10.1080/00220973.1971.11011261.
Hollingsworth, M., & Woodward, J. (1993). Integrated learning: Explicit strategies and their role in problem-
solving instruction for students with learning disabilities. Exceptional Children, 59(5), 444–455. https://
doi.org/10.1177/001440299305900507.
Horner, R. H., & Albin, R. W. (1988). Research on general-case procedures for learners with severe
disabilities. Education & Treatment of Children, 11(4), 375–388.
Horner, R. H., & McDonald, R. S. (1982). Comparison of single instance and general case instruction in
teaching a generalized vocational skill. Journal of the Association for the Severely Handicapped, 7(3), 7–
20. https://doi.org/10.1177/154079698200700302.
Horner, R. H., Sprague, J., & Wilcox, B. (1982). Constructing general case programs for community activities.
In B. Wilcox & T. Bellamy (Eds.), Design of high school for severely handicapped students (pp. 61–98).
Brookes.
Horner, R., Sprague, J., & Wilcox, B. (2005). General case programming. In Hersen, M., Rosqvist, J., Gross,
A. M., Drabman, R. S., Sugai, G., & Horner, R. (Eds.), Encyclopedia of behavior modification and
cognitive behavior therapy (Vol. 1; pp 1343–1348). https://doi.org/10.4135/9781412950534.n3071
Perspectives on Behavior Science (2021) 44:195–223 219

Horton, S., Lovitt, T., & Bergerud, D. (1990). The effectiveness of graphic organizers for three classifications
of secondary students in content area classes. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 23(1), 12–22. https://doi.
org/10.1177/002221949002300107.
Hughes, C. A., Morris, J. R., Therrien, W. J., & Benson, S. K. (2017). Explicit instruction: Historical and
contemporary contexts. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 32(3), 140–148. https://doi.org/10.
1111/ldrp.12142.
Jitendra, A. K., & Kame’enui, E. J. (1988). A design-of-instruction analysis of concept teaching in five basal
language programs: Violations from the bottom up. Journal of Special Education, 22(2), 199–219. https://
doi.org/10.1177/002246698802200206.
Jitendra, A. K., & Kame’enui, E. J. (1994). Review of concept learning models: Implications for special
education practitioners. Intervention in School & Clinic, 30(2), 91–98. https://doi.org/10.1177/
105345129403000205.
Johnson, D. M., & Stratton, R. P. (1966). Evaluation of five methods of teaching concepts. Journal of
Educational Psychology, 57(1), 48–53. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0022925.
Johnson, G., Gersten, R., & Carnine, D. (1987). Effects of instructional design variables on vocabulary
acquisition of LD students: A study of computer-assisted instruction. Journal of Learning Disabilities,
20(4), 206–212. https://doi.org/10.1177/002221948702000402.
Joseph, L. M., Alber-Morgan, S., & Neef, N. (2016). Applying behavior analytic procedures to effectively
teach literacy skills in the classroom. Psychology in the Schools, 53(1), 73–88. https://doi.org/10.1002/
pits.21883.
Joyce, B., Calhoun, E., & Hopkins, D. (1997). Models of learning—Tools for teaching. Open University
Press.
Jumaat, N. F., & Tasir, Z. (2014, April 11–13). Instructional scaffolding in online learning environment: A
meta-analysis. Paper presented at the International Conference on Teaching and Learning in Computing
and Engineering (LaTiCE) (pp. 74–77). https://doi.org/10.1109/latice.2014.22
Kame’enui, E. (2021) Ode to Zig (and the Bard): In Support of an incomplete logical-empirical model of direct
instruction. Perspectives on Behavior Science. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40614-021-00302-1.
Kame’enui, E. J., & Carnine, D. (1986). Preteaching versus concurrent teaching of the component skills of a
subtraction algorithm to skill-deficient second graders: A component analysis of direct instruction. The
Exceptional Child, 33(2), 103–115.
Kame’enui, E. J., & Carnine, D. W. (1998). Effective teaching strategies that accommodate diverse learners.
Merrill Prentice Hall.
Kame'enui, E. J., Carnine, D. W., & Dixon, R. C. (1998). Introduction: Effective teaching strategies that
accommodate diverse learners. In E. J. Kame’enui & D. W. Carnine (Eds.), Effective teaching strategies
that accommodate diverse learners (pp. 1–17). Merrill Prentice Hall.
Kame'enui, E. J., Carnine, D. W., Dixon, R. C., Simmons, D. C., & Coyne, M. D. (2002). Effective teaching
strategies that accommodate diverse learners (2nd ed.). Merrill Prentice Hall.
Kinder, D., & Carnine, D. (1991). Direct Instruction: What it is and what it is becoming. Journal of Behavioral
Education, 1(2), 193–213. https://doi.org/10.1007/bf00957004.
Kirschner, P. A., Sweller, J., & Clark, R. E. (2006). Why minimal guidance during instruction does not work:
An analysis of the failure of constructivist, discovery, problem-based, experiential, and inquiry-based
teaching. Educational Psychologist, 41(2), 75–86. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326985ep4102_1.
Klausmeier, H. J. (1980). Learning and teaching concepts. Academic Press.
Klausmeier, H. J., & Feldman, K. V. (1975). Effects of a definition and a varying number of examples and
nonexamples on concept attainment. Journal of Educational Psychology, 67(2), 174–178. https://doi.org/
10.1037/h0076998.
Klausmeier, H. J., Ghatala, E. S., & Frayer, D. A. (1974). Conceptual learning and development: A cognitive
view. Academic Press.
Kozloff, M. A., LaNunziata, L., & Cowardin, J. (1999, January). Direct Instruction in education. http://people.
uncw.edu/kozloffm/diarticle.html.
Langsdorff, L. C., Domeniconi, C., Schmidt, A., Gomes, C. G., & das Graças de Souza, D. (2017). Learning
by exclusion in individuals with autism and Down syndrome. Psicologia: Reflexão e Crítica, 30(1).
Retrived July 9, 2021 from https://www.scielo.br/j/prc/a/mtrtkQWW7w86MxWd4tVtqNz/?lang=
en&format=html
Larkin, M. J. (2001). Providing support for student independence through scaffolded instruction. Teaching
Exceptional Children, 34(1), 30–34. https://doi.org/10.1177/004005990103400104.
Layng, T. V. J. (2016). Thirty million words––and even more functional relations: A review of Suskind’s
Thirty million words. The Behavior Analyst, 39, 339–350. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40614-016-0069-2.
220 Perspectives on Behavior Science (2021) 44:195–223

Layng, T. V. J. (2019). Tutorial: Understanding concepts: Implications for behavior analysts and educators.
Perspectives on Behavior Science, 42(2), 345–363. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40614-018-00188-6.
Layng, T. V. J., Twyman, J. S., & Stikeleather, G. (2003). Headsprout Early ReadingTM: Reliably teaching
children to read. Behavioral Technology Today, 3, 7–20.
Layng, T. V. J., Twyman, J. S., & Stikeleather, G. (2004). Selected for success: How Headsprout Reading
Basics™ teaches children to read. In D. J. Moran & R. W. Malott (Eds.), Evidence based education
methods (pp. 171–197). Elsevier/Academic Press.
Layng, T. V. J., Stikeleather, G., & Twyman, J. S. (2006). Scientific formative evaluation: The role of
individual learners in generating and predicting successful educational outcomes. In R. F. Subotnik & H.
Walberg (Eds.), The scientific basis of educational productivity (pp. 29–44). Information Age.
Layng, T. V. J., Sota, M., & Leon, M. (2011). Thinking through text comprehension I: Foundation and
guiding relations. The Behavior Analyst Today, 12, 1–10. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0100706.
Leon, M., Layng, T. V., & Sota, M. (2011). Thinking through text comprehension III: The programing of
verbal and investigative repertoires. The Behavior Analyst Today, 12(1), 22–33. https://doi.org/10.1037/
h0100708.
Lin, S. Y., Strickland, J., Ray, B., & Denner, P. (2004). Computer-based concept mapping as a prewriting
strategy for middle school students. Meridian: A Middle School Computer Technologies Journal, 7(2), 1–
17.
Lovitt, T. C., & Horton, S. V. (1994). Strategies for adapting science textbooks for youth with learning
disabilities. Remedial & Special Education, 15(2), 105–116. https://doi.org/10.1177/
074193259401500206.
MacDuff, G. S., Krantz, P. J., & McClannahan, L. E. (2001). Prompts and prompt-fading strategies for people
with autism. In C. Maurice, G. Green, & R. M. Foxx (Eds.), Making a difference: Behavioral intervention
for autism (pp. 37–50). Pro-Ed http://dddc.rutgers.edu/files/prompting.pdf.
Markle, S. M. (1967). Empirical testing of programs. In P. C. Lange (Ed.), Programmed instruction: Sixty-
sixth yearbook of the National Society for the Study of Education: 2 (pp. 104–138). University of Chicago
Press.
Markle, S. M. (1969). Good frames and bad (2nd ed.). John Wiley & Sons.
Markle, S. M. (1970). Programing and programed instruction. Academy for Educational Development
https://files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/ED039711.pdf.
Markle, S. M. (1978). Teaching conceptual networks. NSPI Journal, 17(1), 4–7. https://doi.org/10.1002/pfi.
4180170107.
Markle, S. M. (1981). Training designers to think about thinking. Journal of Instructional Development, 4(3),
24–27. https://doi.org/10.1007/bf02905321.
Markle, S. M., & Droege, S. (1980) Solving the problem of problem solving domains, NSPI Journal, 30–33.
https://doi.org/10.1002/pfi.4180190112
Markle, S. M., & Tiemann, P. W. (1969). Really understanding concepts: Or in frumious pursuit of the
Jabberwock. (Slide/Tape instructional program) Tiemann Associates.
Markle, S. M., & Tiemann, P. W. (1970). “Behavioral” analysis of “cognitive” content. Educational
Technology, 10(1), 41–45.
Markle, S. M., & Tiemann, P. W. (1971). Conceptual learning and instructional design. In M. D. (Ed.),
Instructional design: Readings. Prentice Hall.
Marzano, R. J. (2004). Building background knowledge for academic achievement: research on what works in
schools. Association for Supervision & Curriculum Development.
Marzano, R. J., Pickering, D. J., & Pollock, J. E. (2001). Classroom instruction that works: Research-based
strategies for increasing student achievement. Association for Supervision and Curriculum Development.
Mason, L. & Otero, M. (2021). Just How effective is Direct Instruction? Perspectives on Behavior Science.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s40614-021-00295-x (in this issue).
Mayfield, K. H., & Chase, P. N. (2002). The effects of cumulative practice on mathematics problem solving.
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 34(2), 105–123. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2002.35-105.
McKeough, A., Lupart, J., & Marini, A. (Eds.). (1995). Teaching for transfer: Fostering generalization in
learning. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
McKeown, M. G., & Beck, I. L. (1990). The assessment and characterization of young learners’ knowledge of
a topic in history. American Educational Research Journal, 27(4), 688–726. https://doi.org/10.3102/
00028312027004688
McKeown, M. G., Beck, I. L., Sinatra, G. M., & Loxterman, J. A. (1992). The relative contribution of prior
knowledge and coherent text to comprehension. Reading Research Quarterly, 27(1), 78–93. https://doi.
org/10.2307/747834.
Perspectives on Behavior Science (2021) 44:195–223 221

Mechner, F. (1967). Behavioral analysis and instructional sequencing. In P. C. Lange (Ed.), Programmed
instruction: The sixty-sixth yearbook of the National Society for the Study of Education (pp. 81–103).
University of Chicago Press.
Mechner, F., Fredrick, T., & Jenkins, T. (2013). How can one specify and teach thinking skills? European
Journal of Behavior Analysis, 14(2), 285–293. https://doi.org/10.1080/15021149.2013.11434461.
Merrill, D. M., Tennyson, R. D., & Posey, L. O. (1992). Teaching concepts: An instructional design guide
[Electronic version]. Educational Technology Publications.
Moreno, R. (2004). Decreasing cognitive load in novice students: Effects of explanatory versus corrective
feedback in discovery-based multimedia. Instructional Science, 32(2), 99–113. https://doi.org/10.1023/b:
truc.0000021811.66966.1d.
National Institute of Child Health & Human Development (NICHHD) (2000). Report of the National Reading
Panel. Teaching children to read: An evidence-based assessment of the scientific research literature on
reading and its implications for reading instruction: Reports of the subgroups (NIH Publication No. 00-
4754). U.S. Government Printing Office.
Neef, N. A., Nelles, D., Iwata, B. A., & Page, T. J. (2003). Analysis of precurrent skills in solving mathematics
story problems. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 36(1), 21–33. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2003.
36-21.
No Child Left Behind Act of 2001, Pub. L. No. 107-110, § 115, Stat. 1425 (2002).
O’Neill, R. E. (1990). Establishing verbal repertoires: Toward the application of general case analysis and
programming. Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 8(1), 113–126. https://doi.org/10.1007/bf03392852.
Olson, J., & Platt, J. (2000). The instructional cycle: Teaching children and adolescents with special needs.
Prentice Hall.
Owen, M. J. (2003). It’s elementary! 275 math word problems (Book 3). Educator Publishing Service.
Paine, S., Carnine, D., & White, W. A. T. (1982). Effects of fading teacher presentation structure
(covertization) on acquisition and maintenance of arithmetic problem-solving skills. Education &
Treatment of Children, 5(2), 93–107 https://www.jstor.org/stable/42900378.
Pazzani, M. J. (1991). Influence of prior knowledge on concept acquisition: Experimental and computational
results. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 17(3), 416–432. https://
doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.17.3.416.
Pearson, P. D., & Dole, J. A. (1987). Explicit comprehension instruction: A review of research and new
conceptualization of instruction. Elementary School Journal, 88(2), 151–165. https://doi.org/10.1086/
461530.
Pearson, P. D., Hansen, J., & Gordon, C. (1979). The effect of background knowledge on young children's
comprehension of explicit and implicit information. (Technical Report No. 116). Center for the Study of
Reading, University of Illinois.
Phillips, D. C. (1995). The good, the bad, the ugly: The many faces of constructivism. Educational
Researcher, 24(7), 5–12. https://doi.org/10.3102/0013189x024007005.
Prater, M. A. (1993). Teaching concepts: Procedures for the design and delivery of instruction. Remedial &
Special Education, 14(5), 51–62. https://doi.org/10.1177/074193259301400508.
Raymond, E. (2000). Cognitive characteristics. Learners with mild disabilities. Allyn & Bacon.
Reigeluth, C. M. (1983). Instructional design: What is it and why is it. In C. M. Reigeluth (Ed.), Instructional-
design theories and models. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Reigeluth, C. M., & Darwazeh, A. (1982). The elaboration theory’s procedure for designing instruction: A
conceptual approach. Journal of Instructional Development, 5(3), 22–32. https://doi.org/10.1007/
bf02905492.
Reiser, R. A., & Dempsey, J. V. (2007). Trends and issues in instructional design (2nd ed.). Pearson
Education.
Robbins, J. K. (2011). Problem solving, reasoning, and analytical thinking in a classroom environment. The
Behavior Analyst Today, 12(1), 41–48. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0100710.
Rosenshine, B., & Meister, C. (1995). Scaffolds for teaching higher-order cognitive strategies. In A. C.
Ornstein (Ed.), Teaching: Theory into practice. Allyn & Bacon.
Rosenshine, B., & Stevens, R. (1986). Teaching functions. In M. C. Wittrock (Ed.), Handbook of research on
teaching (3rd ed., pp. 376–391). Macmillan.
Ross, D., & Carnine, D. W. (1982). Analytic assistance: Effects of example selection, subjects' age, and
syntactic complexity. Journal of Educational Research, 75(5), 294–298. https://doi.org/10.1080/
00220671.1982.10885397.
Rowley, J., & Hartley, R. (2008). Organizing knowledge: An introduction to managing access to information
(4th ed.). Routledge. https://doi.org/10.1108/01435120910937393.
222 Perspectives on Behavior Science (2021) 44:195–223

Schoenfeld, A. H. (1979). Explicit heuristic training as a variable in problem-solving performance. Journal for
Research in Mathematics Education, 10(3), 173–187. https://doi.org/10.2307/748805.
Schunk, D. H. (2007). Learning theories: An educational perspective (5th ed.). Prentice Hall.
Singleton, S. M., & Filce, H. G. (2015). Graphic organizers for secondary students with learning disabilities.
Teaching Exceptional Children, 48(2), 110–117. https://doi.org/10.1177/0040059915605799.
Skinner, B. F. (1954). The science of learning and the art of teaching. Harvard Educational Review, 24(2), 86–
97.
Slocum, T. A. (2004). Direct Instruction: The big ideas. In D. J. Moran & R. W. Malott (Eds.), Empirically
supported educational methods. Elsevier Academic Press.
Smith, M. K. (2002). Jerome S. Bruner and the process of education. In The encyclopedia of pedagogy and
informal education. Retrieved July 9, 2021 from http://www.infed.org/thinkers/bruner.htm.
Sprague, J. R., & Horner, R. H. (1984). The effects of single instance, multiple instance, and general case
training on generalized vending machine use by moderately and severely handicapped students. Journal
of Applied Behavior Analysis, 17(2), 273–278. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1984.17-273.
Stein, M., Carnine, D., & Dixon, R. (1998). Direct Instruction: Integrating curriculum design and effective
teaching practice. Intervention in School & Clinic, 33(4), 227–233. https://doi.org/10.1177/
105345129803300405.
Stevens, K. C. (1980). The effect of background knowledge on the reading comprehension of ninth graders.
Journal of Reading Behavior, 12(2), 151–154. https://doi.org/10.1080/10862968009547365.
Stevens, K. C. (1982). Can we improve reading by teaching background information? Journal of Reading
Behavior, 14(4), 326–329 https://www.jstor.org/stable/40030380.
Stockard, J. (2021). Direct instruction: An effective and humane method to attain equality in education.
Perspectives on Behavior Science. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40614-021-00287-x
Stockard, J., Wood, T. W., Coughlin, C., & Rasplica Khoury, C. (2018). The effectiveness of Direct
Instruction curricula: A meta-analysis of a half century of research. Review of Educational Research,
88(4), 479–507. https://doi.org/10.3102/0034654317751919.
Strangman, N., & Hall, T. (2004). Background knowledge. National Center on Accessing the General
Curriculum. Retrieved July 9, 2021 from http://www.aim.cast.org/learn/historyarchive/
backgroundpapers/background_knowledge.
Tennyson, R. D. (1973). Effects of negative instances in concept acquisition using a verbal learning task.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 64(2), 247–260. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0034400
Tennyson, R. D., & Park, O. (1980). The teaching of concepts: A review of instructional design research
literature. Review of Educational Research, 50(1), 55–70. https://doi.org/10.3102/00346543050001055.
Terrace, H. S. (1963). Discrimination learning with and without "errors.". Journal of the Experimental
Analysis of Behavior, 6(1), 1–27. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1963.6-1.
Tiemann, P. W., & Markle, S. M. (1990). Analyzing instructional content: A guide to instruction and
evaluation (4th ed.). Morningside Press.
Torgesen, J. K., Houston, D. D., Rissman, L. M., Decker, S. M., Roberts, G., Vaughn, S., Wexler, J., Francis,
D. J., Rivera, M. O., & Lesaux, N. (2007). Academic literacy instruction for adolescents: A guidance
document from the Center on Instruction. RMC Research Corporation, Center on Instruction. Retrieved
July 9, 2021 from www.centeroninstruction.org.
Touchette, P. (1971). Transfer of stimulus control: Measuring the moment of transfer. Journal of the
Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 15(3), 347–354. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1971.15-347.
Trask-Tyler, S. A., Grossi, T. A., & Heward, W. L. (1994). Teaching young adults with developmental
disabilities and visual impairments to use tape-recorded recipes: Acquisition, generalization, and main-
tenance of cooking skills. Journal of Behavioral Education, 4, 283–311. https://doi.org/10.1007/
bf01531984.
Twyman, J. S. (2021). Faultless Communication: The Heart and Soul of Direct Instruction (in this issue).
Twyman, J. S., & Hockman, A. (2021). You have the big idea, concept, and some examples. . . . Now what?
Behavior Analysis in Practice.
Watkins, C. L., & Slocum, T. A. (2004). The components of Direct Instruction. In N. E. Marchand-Martella,
T. A. Slocum, & R. C. Martella (Eds.), Introduction to Direct Instruction (pp. 28–65). Allyn & Bacon.
Weisberg, P., Packer, R. A., & Weisberg, R. S. (1981). Academic training. In J. L. Matson & J. R. McCartney
(Eds.), Handbook of behavior modification with the mentally retarded. Applied clinical psychology.
Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4615-7130-8_10.
Wertsch, J. V., & Stone, C. (1984). A social interactional analysis of learning disabilities remediation. Journal
of Learning Disabilities, 17(4), 194–199.
Whimby, A., & Lochhead, J. (1982). Problem solving and comprehension. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Perspectives on Behavior Science (2021) 44:195–223 223

Whimby, A., & Lockhead, J. (1999). Problem solving and comprehension (6th ed.). Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates.
White, W. A. (1988). Meta-analysis of the effects of Direct Instruction in special education. Education &
Treatment of Children, 11(4), 364–374. Retrieved July 9, 2021, from https://www.jstor.org/stable/
42899084.
Williams, P., & Carnine, D. (1981). Relationships between range of examples and of instructions and attention
in concept attainment. Journal of Educational Research, 74(3), 144–148. https://doi.org/10.1080/
00220671.1981.10885300.

Publisher’s Note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and
institutional affiliations.

You might also like

pFad - Phonifier reborn

Pfad - The Proxy pFad of © 2024 Garber Painting. All rights reserved.

Note: This service is not intended for secure transactions such as banking, social media, email, or purchasing. Use at your own risk. We assume no liability whatsoever for broken pages.


Alternative Proxies:

Alternative Proxy

pFad Proxy

pFad v3 Proxy

pFad v4 Proxy