Theories of Second-Language Acquisition - Wikipedia
Theories of Second-Language Acquisition - Wikipedia
language acquisition
History
As second-language acquisition began as
an interdisciplinary field, it is hard to pin
down a precise starting date.[1] However,
there are two publications in particular
that are seen as instrumental to the
development of the modern study of SLA:
(1) Corder's 1967 essay The Significance of
Learners' Errors, and (2) Selinker's 1972
article Interlanguage. Corder's essay
rejected a behaviorist account of SLA and
suggested that learners made use of
intrinsic internal linguistic processes;
Selinker's article argued that second-
language learners possess their own
individual linguistic systems that are
independent from both the first and
second languages.[2]
Universal grammar
From the field of linguistics, the most
influential theory by far has been
Chomsky's theory of Universal Grammar
(UG). The core of this theory lies on the
existence of an innate universal grammar,
grounded on the poverty of the stimulus.[3]
The UG model of principles, basic
properties which all languages share, and
parameters, properties which can vary
between languages, has been the basis for
much second-language research.
Input hypothesis
Learners' most direct source of
information about the target language is
the target language itself. When they come
into direct contact with the target
language, this is referred to as "input."
When learners process that language in a
way that can contribute to learning, this is
referred to as "intake". However, it must be
at a level that is comprehensible to them.
In his monitor theory, Krashen advanced
the concept that language input should be
at the "i+1" level, just beyond what the
learner can fully understand; this input is
comprehensible, but contains structures
that are not yet fully understood. This has
been criticized on the basis that there is no
clear definition of i+1, and that factors
other than structural difficulty (such as
interest or presentation) can affect
whether input is actually turned into
intake. The concept has been quantified,
however, in vocabulary acquisition
research; Nation reviews various studies
which indicate that about 98% of the
words in running text should be previously
known in order for extensive reading to be
effective.[6]
Monitor model
Other concepts have also been influential
in the speculation about the processes of
building internal systems of second-
language information. Some thinkers hold
that language processing handles distinct
types of knowledge. For instance, one
component of the Monitor Model,
propounded by Krashen, posits a
distinction between “acquisition” and
“learning.”[7] According to Krashen, L2
acquisition is a subconscious process of
incidentally “picking up” a language, as
children do when becoming proficient in
their first languages. Language learning,
on the other hand, is studying, consciously
and intentionally, the features of a
language, as is common in traditional
classrooms. Krashen sees these two
processes as fundamentally different, with
little or no interface between them. In
common with connectionism, Krashen
sees input as essential to language
acquisition.[7]
Interaction hypothesis
Long's interaction hypothesis proposes
that language acquisition is strongly
facilitated by the use of the target
language in interaction. Similarly to
Krashen's Input Hypothesis, the Interaction
Hypothesis claims that comprehensible
input is important for language learning. In
addition, it claims that the effectiveness of
comprehensible input is greatly increased
when learners have to negotiate for
meaning.[10]
Output hypothesis
In the 1980s, Canadian SLA researcher
Merrill Swain advanced the output
hypothesis, that meaningful output is as
necessary to language learning as
meaningful input. However, most studies
have shown little if any correlation
between learning and quantity of output.
Today, most scholars contend that small
amounts of meaningful output are
important to language learning, but
primarily because the experience of
producing language leads to more
effective processing of input.
Competition model
Some of the major cognitive theories of
how learners organize language
knowledge are based on analyses of how
speakers of various languages analyze
sentences for meaning. MacWhinney,
Bates, and Kliegl found that speakers of
English, German, and Italian showed
varying patterns in identifying the subjects
of transitive sentences containing more
than one noun.[13] English speakers relied
heavily on word order; German speakers
used morphological agreement, the
animacy status of noun referents, and
stress; and speakers of Italian relied on
agreement and stress. MacWhinney et al.
interpreted these results as supporting the
Competition Model, which states that
individuals use linguistic cues to get
meaning from language, rather than
relying on linguistic universals.[13]
According to this theory, when acquiring
an L2, learners sometimes receive
competing cues and must decide which
cue(s) is most relevant for determining
meaning.
Connectionism
These findings also relate to
Connectionism. Connectionism attempts
to model the cognitive language
processing of the human brain, using
computer architectures that make
associations between elements of
language, based on frequency of co-
occurrence in the language input.[14]
Frequency has been found to be a factor in
various linguistic domains of language
learning.[15] Connectionism posits that
learners form mental connections
between items that co-occur, using
exemplars found in language input. From
this input, learners extract the rules of the
language through cognitive processes
common to other areas of cognitive skill
acquisition. Since connectionism denies
both innate rules and the existence of any
innate language-learning module, L2 input
is of greater importance than it is in
processing models based on innate
approaches, since, in connectionism, input
is the source of both the units and the
rules of language.
Noticing hypothesis
Attention is another characteristic that
some believe to have a role in determining
the success or failure of language
processing. Richard Schmidt states that
although explicit metalinguistic knowledge
of a language is not always essential for
acquisition, the learner must be aware of
L2 input in order to gain from it.[16] In his
“noticing hypothesis,” Schmidt posits that
learners must notice the ways in which
their interlanguage structures differ from
target norms. This noticing of the gap
allows the learner's internal language
processing to restructure the learner's
internal representation of the rules of the
L2 in order to bring the learner's
production closer to the target. In this
respect, Schmidt's understanding is
consistent with the ongoing process of
rule formation found in emergentism and
connectionism.
Processability
Some theorists and researchers have
contributed to the cognitive approach to
second-language acquisition by increasing
understanding of the ways L2 learners
restructure their interlanguage knowledge
systems to be in greater conformity to L2
structures. Processability theory states
that learners restructure their L2
knowledge systems in an order of which
they are capable at their stage of
development.[17] For instance, In order to
acquire the correct morphological and
syntactic forms for English questions,
learners must transform declarative
English sentences. They do so by a series
of stages, consistent across learners.
Clahsen proposed that certain processing
principles determine this order of
restructuring.[18] Specifically, he stated
that learners first, maintain declarative
word order while changing other aspects
of the utterances, second, move words to
the beginning and end of sentences, and
third, move elements within main clauses
before subordinate clauses.
Automaticity
Thinkers have produced several theories
concerning how learners use their internal
L2 knowledge structures to comprehend
L2 input and produce L2 output. One idea
is that learners acquire proficiency in an L2
in the same way that people acquire other
complex cognitive skills. Automaticity is
the performance of a skill without
conscious control. It results from the
gradated process of proceduralization. In
the field of cognitive psychology, Anderson
expounds a model of skill acquisition,
according to which persons use
procedures to apply their declarative
knowledge about a subject in order to
solve problems.[19] On repeated practice,
these procedures develop into production
rules that the individual can use to solve
the problem, without accessing long-term
declarative memory. Performance speed
and accuracy improve as the learner
implements these production rules.
DeKeyser tested the application of this
model to L2 language automaticity.[20] He
found that subjects developed increasing
proficiency in performing tasks related to
the morphosyntax of an artificial language,
Autopractan, and performed on a learning
curve typical of the acquisition of non-
language cognitive skills. This evidence
conforms to Anderson's general model of
cognitive skill acquisition, supports the
idea that declarative knowledge can be
transformed into procedural knowledge,
and tends to undermine the idea of
Krashen[7] that knowledge gained through
language “learning” cannot be used to
initiate speech production.
Declarative/procedural model
An example of declarative
knowledge, procedural knowledge,
and conditional knowledge
Semantic theory
For the second-language learner, the
acquisition of meaning is arguably the
most important task. Meaning is at the
heart of a language, not the exotic sounds
or elegant sentence structure. There are
several types of meanings: lexical,
grammatical, semantic, and pragmatic. All
the different meanings contribute to the
acquisition of meaning resulting in the
integrated second language
possession.[23]
Larsen-Freeman
Notes
1. Gass & Selinker 2008.
2. VanPatten & Benati 2010, pp. 2–5.
3. Tomasello 2005, pp. 183–197.
4. Cook 2008, p. 35.
5. Schachter 1988, pp. 219–235
6. Nation 2001.
7. Krashen 1982.
8. Bialystok & Sharwood Smith 1985
9. Bialystok 1994.
10. Ellis 1997.
11. Schmidt 2002.
12. Newport 1990.
13. MacWhinney, Bates & Kliegl 1984.
14. Christiansen & Chater 2001.
15. Ellis 2002.
16. Schmidt 1990.
17. Pienemann 1998.
18. Clahsen 1984.
19. Anderson 1992.
20. DeKeyser 1997.
21. Ullman 2001.
22. Williams 1999.
23. Slabakova 2010.
24. Lantolf & Beckett 2009.
25. Norton 2011.
26. Larsen-Freeman 2011.
References
Anderson, J. R. (1992). "Automaticity and the
ACT* theory". American Journal of
Psychology. 105 (2): 165–180.
doi:10.2307/1423026 (https://doi.org/10.230
7%2F1423026) . JSTOR 1423026 (https://w
ww.jstor.org/stable/1423026) .
PMID 1621879 (https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.
gov/1621879) .
Bialystok, E.; Sharwood Smith, M. (1985).
"Interlanguage is not a state of mind: An
evaluation of the construct for second-
language acquisition". Applied Linguistics. 6
(2): 101–117. doi:10.1093/applin/6.2.101 (ht
tps://doi.org/10.1093%2Fapplin%2F6.2.101)
.
Bialystok, E. (1994). "Analysis and control in
the development of second language
proficiency". Studies in Second Language
Acquisition. 16 (2): 157–168.
doi:10.1017/S0272263100012857 (https://d
oi.org/10.1017%2FS0272263100012857) .
S2CID 145296687 (https://api.semanticschol
ar.org/CorpusID:145296687) .
Christiansen, M. H.; Chater, N. (2001).
"Connectionist psycholinguistics: Capturing
the empirical data". Trends in Cognitive
Sciences. 5 (2): 82–88.
CiteSeerX 10.1.1.22.3502 (https://citeseerx.i
st.psu.edu/viewdoc/summary?doi=10.1.1.2
2.3502) . doi:10.1016/S1364-
6613(00)01600-4 (https://doi.org/10.1016%2
FS1364-6613%2800%2901600-4) .
PMID 11166638 (https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.ni
h.gov/11166638) . S2CID 7279603 (https://a
pi.semanticscholar.org/CorpusID:7279603) .
Clahsen, H. (1984). "The acquisition of
German word order: a test case for cognitive
approaches to second language acquisition".
In Andersen, R. (ed.). Second languages: a
cross-linguistic perspective. Rowley, MA:
Newbury House. pp. 219–242. ISBN 978-0-
88377-440-3.
Cook, Vivian (2008). Second language
learning and language teaching. London:
Arnold. ISBN 978-0-340-95876-6.
DeKeyser, R. M. (1997). "Beyond explicit rule
learning: Automatizing second language
morphosyntax". Studies in Second Language
Acquisition. 19 (2): 195–222.
doi:10.1017/s0272263197002040 (https://d
oi.org/10.1017%2Fs0272263197002040) .
S2CID 146769886 (https://api.semanticschol
ar.org/CorpusID:146769886) .
Ellis, R. (1997). Second language acquisition
(https://archive.org/details/secondlanguage
ac00rode) . Oxford, New York: Oxford
University Press. ISBN 978-0-19-437212-1.
Ellis, N. (2002). "Frequency effects in
language processing" (https://doi.org/10.101
7%2Fs0272263102002024) . Studies in
Second Language Acquisition. 24 (2): 143–
188. doi:10.1017/s0272263102002024 (http
s://doi.org/10.1017%2Fs027226310200202
4) .
Ellis, R. (2005). "Measuring implicit and
explicit knowledge of a second language: A
psychometric study" (https://doi.org/10.101
7%2Fs0272263105050096) . Studies in
Second Language Acquisition. 27 (2): 141–
172. doi:10.1017/s0272263105050096 (http
s://doi.org/10.1017%2Fs027226310505009
6) .
Gass, S.; Selinker, L. (2008). Second language
acquisition: An introductory course (https://ar
chive.org/details/secondlanguageac00gass
_0) . New York, NY: Routledge. ISBN 978-0-
8058-5497-8.
Hulstijn, J. H. (2005). "Theoretical and
empirical issues in the study of implicit and
explicit second-language learning" (https://d
oi.org/10.1017%2Fs0272263105050084) .
Studies in Second Language Acquisition. 27
(2): 129–140.
doi:10.1017/s0272263105050084 (https://d
oi.org/10.1017%2Fs0272263105050084) .
Krashen, S. (1982). Principles and practice in
second language acquisition (https://web.arc
hive.org/web/20110716023230/http://www.
sdkrashen.com/Principles_and_Practice/ind
ex.html) . Pergamon Press. ISBN 978-0-08-
028628-0. Archived from the original (http://
www.sdkrashen.com/Principles_and_Practic
e/index.html) on 2011-07-16. Retrieved
2010-11-25.
Lantolf, J. P.; Beckett, T. G. (2009).
"Sociocultural theory and second language
acquisition" (https://publishup.uni-potsdam.d
e/opus4-ubp/files/1838/kliegl_etal_1984.pd
f) (PDF). Language Teaching. 42 (4): 459–
475. doi:10.1017/S0261444809990048 (http
s://doi.org/10.1017%2FS026144480999004
8) . S2CID 145202925 (https://api.semantics
cholar.org/CorpusID:145202925) .
MacWhinney, B.; Bates, E.; Kliegl, R. (1984).
"Cue validity and sentence interpretation in
English, German, and Italian" (https://publish
up.uni-potsdam.de/opus4-ubp/files/1838/kli
egl_etal_1984.pdf) (PDF). Journal of Verbal
Learning and Verbal Behavior. 23 (2): 127–
150. doi:10.1016/S0022-5371(84)90093-8 (h
ttps://doi.org/10.1016%2FS0022-5371%288
4%2990093-8) .
Nation, P. (2001). Learning vocabulary in
another language. Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press. ISBN 978-0-521-80498-1.
Pienemann, M. (1998). Language processing
and second language development:
Processability theory. Amsterdam: John
Benjamins. ISBN 978-1-55619-549-5.
Slabakova, R. (2010). "Semantic theory and
second language acquisition". Annual Review
of Applied Linguistics. 30: 231–247.
doi:10.1017/s0267190510000139 (https://d
oi.org/10.1017%2Fs0267190510000139) .
S2CID 145691099 (https://api.semanticschol
ar.org/CorpusID:145691099) .
Tomasello, M. (2005). "Beyond formalities:
The case of language acquisition". The
Linguistic Review. 22 (2–4): 183–197.
doi:10.1515/tlir.2005.22.2-4.183 (https://doi.
org/10.1515%2Ftlir.2005.22.2-4.183) .
S2CID 15476748 (https://api.semanticschola
r.org/CorpusID:15476748) .
Ullman, M. T. (2001). "The
declarative/procedural model of lexicon and
grammar". Journal of Psycholinguistic
Research. 30 (1): 37–69.
doi:10.1023/A:1005204207369 (https://doi.o
rg/10.1023%2FA%3A1005204207369) .
PMID 11291183 (https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.ni
h.gov/11291183) . S2CID 17654813 (https://
api.semanticscholar.org/CorpusID:1765481
3) .
VanPatten, B.; Benati, A.G. (2010). Key terms
in second language acquisition. London:
Continuum. ISBN 978-0-8264-9914-1.
Williams, J. (1999). "Memory, attention and
inductive learning". Studies in Second
Language Acquisition. 21: 1–48.
doi:10.1017/s0272263199001011 (https://d
oi.org/10.1017%2Fs0272263199001011) .
S2CID 144518733 (https://api.semanticschol
ar.org/CorpusID:144518733) .
Schmidt, R. (1990). "The role of
consciousness in second language learning".
Applied Linguistics. 11 (2): 129–158.
doi:10.1093/applin/11.2.129 (https://doi.org/
10.1093%2Fapplin%2F11.2.129) .
Schmidt, R. (2002). "Interaction hypothesis".
In Richards, J.; Schmidt, R. (eds.). Longman
dictionary of language teaching and applied
linguistics. London New York: Longman.
p. 264. ISBN 978-0-582-43825-5.
Schachter, J. (1988). "Second language
acquisition and its relationship to universal
grammar". Applied Linguistics. 9 (3): 219–
235. doi:10.1093/applin/9.3.219 (https://doi.
org/10.1093%2Fapplin%2F9.3.219) .
Newport, E. (1990). "Maturational constraints
on language learning". Cognitive Science. 14
(1): 11–28.
doi:10.1207/s15516709cog1401_2 (https://d
oi.org/10.1207%2Fs15516709cog1401_2) .
S2CID 207056257 (https://api.semanticschol
ar.org/CorpusID:207056257) .
Norton, B. (2002). "Identity". In Simpson, J.
(ed.). Routledge handbooks in applied
linguistics: the Routledge handbook of applied
linguistics. London, UK: Routledge.
Retrieved from
"https://en.wikipedia.org/w/index.php?
title=Theories_of_second-
language_acquisition&oldid=1168786332"