The Role of Motivation in EFL Students ' Perceptions of Teacher/learner Responsibilities and Learner Abilities
The Role of Motivation in EFL Students ' Perceptions of Teacher/learner Responsibilities and Learner Abilities
* Correspondence:
aysegulokay@hotmail.com Abstract
1
Yıldız Teknik Üniversitesi, İstanbul,
Turkey It is the aim of this study to investigate EFL (English as a Foreign Language) students’
Full list of author information is perceptions of teacher/learner responsibilities and learner decision-making abilities at
available at the end of the article tertiary level - questioning whether motivation has a role in these perceptions. To
this end, 144 students who studied in the preparatory school of a state university
took part in this study. In a mixed-methods research design, two questionnaires were
used to collect quantitative data on students’ perceptions and motivation, whereas
an open-ended questionnaire was used to gather qualitative data to gain a deeper
insight about the perceptions of the students. To analyze the quantitative and qualitative
data, statistical analyses via SPSS Version 18 and latent level analyses were performed,
respectively. The results suggested that high-motivated students do rate themselves
good/very good in their decision-making abilities than their low-motivated peers.
However, motivation or ability was not found to be related to students’ perceptions of
teacher/learner responsibilities. Since high-motivated students were good/very good at
their decision-making abilities, but not more willing to share responsibilities with their
teachers, it could be argued that motivation or ability alone does not ensure learner
autonomy.
Keywords: Learner autonomy, Motivation, Perception, Responsibility
Introduction
The contemporary view of education today takes students as ‘agents’ of their own edu-
cational destiny (van Lier, 2008, p. 47); and teachers, in accordance, have begun to
change their roles from teaching to facilitating, helping, coordinating, counselling, con-
sulting, advising, knowing, resourcing and so on. One of the main reasons for this shift
is the advent of the term ‘autonomy’ into the area of language teaching and learning.
Although learner autonomy is commonly defined as “the ability to take charge of one’s
own learning” (Holec, 1981, p. 3), there have been many other definitions of learner
autonomy since then. Little (1991), for instance, uses ‘capacity’ instead of ‘ability’ and
states that “autonomy is essentially a capacity for detachment, critical reflection,
decision-making, and independent action” (p. 4). The famous ‘Bergen definition’, as
Dam (1995) explains, associates autonomy with “readiness to take charge of one’s own
learning in the service of one’s needs and purposes – which entails a capacity and
© The Author(s). 2017 Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International
License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium,
provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and
indicate if changes were made.
Okay and Balçıkanlı Asian-Pacific Journal of Second and Foreign Language Education (2017) 2:8 Page 2 of 12
Methods
Participants
The present study took place at a preparatory school of a state university in Turkey.
The participants were chosen by convenient sampling procedure. A total number of
144 students (61 males, 83 females) took part in the quantitative data collection process
of the study. Thirty-eight of them also took part in the qualitative data collection
process. At the time of data collection, they had all been studying English for 26 weeks
and they were all at CEFR B1+ and B2 levels.
(a) their perceptions of the English teacher’s responsibilities and their own;
(b)their perceptions of their decision-making abilities;
(c)their motivation to study English; and
(d)how often they carried out different autonomous activities in and outside class.
Section (a) asked students to rate both their and their teachers’ responsibilities in the
Likert-type options ranging from “not at all” to “completely”. In her study, Üstünlüoğlu
(2009) adapted this first section by removing the Likert-type options and only asking
student to decide whose responsibility a certain action is: theirs, their teachers’, or both.
In the present study, this adapted version of Section (a) was administered in the data
collection process. The questions in Section (b) were Likert-type and asked students
how able they think they are about their decision-making abilities. These abilities were
related to the in-and-out-of-class responsibilities asked in the Section (a) of the ques-
tionnaire to gather more information. Sections (c) and (d) were not used for data col-
lection because Section (c) measured the motivation level of students only by asking
them “how motivated they think they are”. However, depending merely on students’
own evaluations of their own motivation levels might not have been reasonable. There-
fore, their motivation levels were measured separately by another data collection tool.
The reason why Section (d) was also decided to be excluded from the data collection
procedure was that it asked students “how often they did inside and outside class activ-
ities” in an academic year. Chan, Spratt, and Humphreys (2002) explain that these ac-
tivities could be thought of as “manifestations of autonomous language learning
behavior” (p. 5). However, it may be argued that when a teacher assigns students to
‘read a grammar book’ (Item 26 in the original questionnaire) or ‘do grammar exercises’
(Item 40 in the original questionnaire), these activities may become compulsory and
Okay and Balçıkanlı Asian-Pacific Journal of Second and Foreign Language Education (2017) 2:8 Page 5 of 12
1. Motivational Intensity. This measure consists of ten multiple choice items which are
designed to measure the intensity of a student’s motivation to learn English in
terms of work done for classroom assignments, future plans to make use of and
study the language, etc. A high score represents a student’s self-report of a high de-
gree of effort being spent in acquiring the language.
2. Desire to Learn English. Ten multiple choice items are included in this scale with a
high score expressing a strong desire to learn English.
3. Attitudes toward Learning English. This is a ten item scale, higher score from which
indicates a positive attitude toward learning English.
4. Instrumental Orientation. Students are presented with four items which stress the
pragmatic or utilitarian value of learning English. A high score indicates that the
student endorses instrumental reasons for learning English.
259). To this end, after the quantitative data were collected through two questionnaires, the
qualitative data were gathered via an open-ended questionnaire to gain a deeper insight
about the constructs being investigated and thereby to ensure research validity.
Quantitative data were analyzed statistically by SPSS version 18. The test for normal-
ity examining standardized skewness and the Shapiro-Wilks test indicated the data
were normally distributed. In order to answer the first two research questions, [(1)
What are the students’ perceptions of their responsibilities? (2) What are the students’
perceptions of their abilities?], frequency analyses were performed. For the third and
fourth research questions [(3) Is there a relationship between students’ perceptions of
their English teachers’ responsibilities and their own and their decision-making abil-
ities? (4) Is there a relationship between motivation levels and perceptions of students
in terms of their responsibilities and abilities?], Pearson correlation and One-way
ANOVA analyses were carried out.
Qualitative data, on the other hand, were subjected to latent level analysis, which
Dörnyei (2007) describes as ‘a second-level, interpretive analysis of the underlying
deeper meaning of the data’ (p. 246). This type of analysis includes coding, looking for
patterns, making interpretations and building theories from the data.
Results
Perceptions of responsibilities
Table 1 below summarizes the results of students’ perceptions of their and their English
teacher’s responsibilities. Out of 13 responsibilities, students give the responsibility only
to their teachers in 4 tasks, and only to themselves in 2 tasks, while the rest are stated
as the responsibilities shared by both themselves and their teachers.
Table 1 Students’ perceptions of their own and their teachers’ responsibilities in percentages
Item Yours Your Teacher’s Both
Learning Objectives
6 Deciding the objectives of the English course 18.8 38.9 42.4
7 Deciding what you should learn next in your English lessons 11.1 61.8 27.1
Learning process (in class)
10 Choose what materials to use to learn English in your English lessons 0.7 59.7 39.6
8 Choose what activities to use to learn English in your English lessons 1.4 52.8 45.8
9 Decide how long to spend on each activity 5.6 62.5 31.9
3 Stimulate your interest in learning English 11.1 29.9 59.0
1 Make sure you make progress during lessons 15.3 12.5 72.2
5 Make you work harder 31.9 20.1 47.9
Learning process (outside class)
13 Decide what you learn outside class 74.3 4.2 21.5
4 Identify your weaknesses in English 44.4 6.9 48.6
2 Make sure you make progress outside class 79.9 4.2 16.0
Outcome
11 Evaluate your learning 12.5 26.4 61.1
12 Evaluate your course 16.0 16.7 67.4
Bold figures = categories with highest score
Okay and Balçıkanlı Asian-Pacific Journal of Second and Foreign Language Education (2017) 2:8 Page 7 of 12
It is clear that students believe that they should decide the objectives of the lesson to-
gether with their teachers, but the materials, the activities, and the time allocated to
these practices should be decided by their teachers. When it comes to making progress
outside the class, most students take the responsibility on themselves, leaving almost
none to their teachers. Students also tend to share their teachers’ responsibilities in
terms of evaluating their learning and the course. Most of them state that they should
evaluate the course and their learning together with their teachers.
The open-ended questionnaire, which asks the students “why” they believe so, also
supports these results:
Learning Objectives. The students believe that they should decide the objectives of
the English course together with their teacher. They mostly acknowledge that the
teacher has knowledge and experience; but the students add that since they are the
ones who want to learn English, they should learn what they need and thus they should
decide the objectives together with the teacher in accordance with their own educa-
tional aims. Additionally, the students state that their teachers should decide what they
should learn next in their English lessons. Again they mostly explain that it is the
teacher who is expert, knowledgeable, and experienced, and thus it is the teacher’s re-
sponsibility to ‘plan’ and it’s their responsibility to ‘stick to’ the plan. A few students
even put forward that students might ‘abuse’ this or there might be ‘chaos’ if they were
given the chance to do so. A few students question with sarcasm what their teachers
would do if they took this responsibility on themselves as well.
Learning Process in Class. The students’ general impression is that, after deciding
the objectives together with their teachers, their teachers should be the ones to decide
the materials, the activities and the time spent on these practices. The students usually
mention their teachers’ experience, education and knowledge. A few students, however,
state that it could be better if the teacher provided them with a set of materials to
choose from and then they could decide on the materials which appeal to their
interests.
Learning Process outside Class. Nearly all of the students state that it is their own
responsibility to decide what they learn outside the class and make sure they progress
outside the class. The commonly put forward reason is that when they are outside the
classroom, they are alone and since English cannot only be taught by the teacher in the
classroom, it should be them who are responsible for learning outside the class. A
number of students also mention that because they have a lot of students, it would be
impossible for teachers to take interest in or identify weaknesses of each and every
student. However, some of the students also indicate that they may not identify their
weakness on their own because they might not know how to do so, so they suggest
getting some ‘help’, ‘advice’, or ‘counselling’ from their teachers in this regard.
Learning Outcome. The common belief among students is that evaluating the
course is a responsibility which should be shared by both parties. They say that a
teacher should evaluate his/her own lesson and the students should evaluate the
teacher’s lesson. The students further state that the course would then be much more
beneficial if it were evaluated both by them and by their teachers. In terms of evaluat-
ing their own learning, students tend to share the responsibility with their teachers,
stating that they should be able to evaluate their own learning by continuously moni-
toring themselves and watching their progress; however, since they lack knowledge and
Okay and Balçıkanlı Asian-Pacific Journal of Second and Foreign Language Education (2017) 2:8 Page 8 of 12
experience in doing so, they may be incapable of ‘objectively’ evaluating their own
learning, so they state that they need their teachers to ‘control’ and to ‘check’ their
learning outcome.
Perceptions of abilities
As Table 2 shows, the second section of the questionnaire asks students to evaluate
their own decision-making abilities in a range of activities and processes. These activ-
ities and processes are also the ones for which the students are asked about responsibil-
ities in the first part of the questionnaire.
Among 9 activities, the students rate themselves having ‘very good/good’ abilities for
7 of them; and they rate their abilities ‘OK’ for only two of them. They do not regard
themselves ‘poor’ in any of these activities. Here it is important to note that the major-
ity of students rate themselves having ‘very good/good’ or ‘OK’ decision-making abil-
ities to perform in-class, outside-class and evaluation tasks. It is also easy to notice that
students rate themselves good/very good when it comes to out-of-class activities or
evaluation. They only do not seem to trust their abilities about in-class activities such
as choosing learning materials or deciding on the time spent on each activity.
Discussion
The data analyses of this study reveal that Turkish students at tertiary level give most in-
class responsibilities such as choosing the materials and activities and deciding on how
long to spend in these activities during class to their teachers because they think that it is
their teachers who are experienced and who have the necessary skills and knowledge to
do so. These findings are in line with those found in Chan, Spratt and Humphreys’ (2002)
research with Chinese students in Hong Kong. On the one hand, this resemblance of per-
ceptions between Chinese and Turkish students supports the cultural aspect of learner
autonomy, which suggests that autonomy is a “western construct” (Littlewood, 1999) be-
cause eastern countries tend to regard teachers as a “source of knowledge” (Littlewood,
2000). Pennycook (1997) also claims that “autonomy is a western concept which may not
suitable to the eastern world”, and he argues that this is especially true when autonomous
behaviors require “questioning” (as cited in Humphreys & Wyatt, 2014, p. 52). On the
other hand, another finding of the present study contradicts this idea of autonomy being
a western concept because Turkish students are actually found to be eager to share re-
sponsibilities with their teachers about out-of-class responsibilities such as “deciding what
they learn outside class” stating that there is no teacher outside the class and they should
be taking their own responsibility for their own learning. In other words, this study reveals
that the reason why Turkish students do not show autonomous behaviors is not that they
avoid questioning, but that they find themselves lacking the capacity and the skills of car-
rying out that particular autonomous behavior.
Secondly, although the levels of students’ motivation and their rating themselves
good/very good are found to be related, the data analyses of this study reveal no mean-
ingful relationship between students’ motivation levels and their abilities about their
and their teachers’ responsibilities. In other words, this study fails to claim that high-
motivated students are likely to share more responsibilities with their teachers; or those
who rated themselves poor/very poor tend to put more responsibilities on their
Okay and Balçıkanlı Asian-Pacific Journal of Second and Foreign Language Education (2017) 2:8 Page 10 of 12
teachers. These results do not match those found by Spratt, Humphreys and Chan
(2002) who state that “motivation may lead to autonomy or be a precondition for it
[and] absence of motivation seems to inhibit practice of learner autonomy” (p. 262). In
contrast, this study claims that students’ motivation does not ensure students’ taking
on responsibilities in-and-out of the classroom, and thus it can be argued that motiv-
ation only might not guarantee learner autonomy.
The reason might be that since the participants in this study are the students who were
neither introduced to the concept of autonomy nor were given the chance to act so, their
motivational characteristics and their perceptions of responsibilities and decision-making
abilities might have shown no meaningful relationship with their perceptions of autonomy
in this particular study. As Fazey and Fazey (2001) explain, while the potential for auton-
omy appears to exist at the individual level, students need to be given permission to dem-
onstrate their capacities and develop them (p. 358); and as Chan, Spratt and Humphreys
(2002) exert, students’ attitudes may not always be apparent in actual autonomous behav-
ior. From another viewpoint, Deci, et al. (1991) explain that “one can be highly competent
and highly motivated, but be regulated externally or by introjects and thus not be autono-
mous or self-determined” (p. 339). Therefore, although the students in this study were
found to be motivated, they might not have shown any indication of autonomous behav-
iors because they might not have had any chance to ever act so, or their motivation might
not be internally driven to lead them to act autonomously.
indicate any relationship between students’ rating themselves good/very good in their
decision-making abilities and their perceptions on their and their teachers’ responsibilities.
Thus, this study failed to prove that the students who rated themselves more able to make
educational decisions also shared more responsibilities with their teachers, or vice-versa.
Therefore, this study has a number of implications in terms of autonomous learning
and teaching. First of all, the results of this study suggest that the cultural diversifica-
tions made under the term ‘learner autonomy’ might not always turn out to be true for
all countries. Although eastern cultures are usually thought to be favoring “reactive
autonomy” (Littlewood, 1999), this study reveals that Turkish students contrarily
demand “proactive” autonomy because they state that rather than following the objec-
tives already decided by the teacher, they would rather decide the objectives together
with their teachers, since it is them - the students themselves - who know what they
need and what they want in terms of learning English.
Second, the most important result of this study is that motivation or ability only
does not guarantee learner autonomy. As Chan, Spratt and Humphreys (2002) ex-
plain, students might not always have “the knowledge or the skills to make the
right choices” (p. 8). Therefore, when it comes to making decisions inside or out-
side the classroom, it is evident that they need training to become autonomous
learners no matter how motivated students might be or no matter how able they
think they are. Thus, this study supports Smith’s (2003) weak version of autonomy,
which implies that autonomy is a capacity which students lack and therefore need
training. As Fazey and Fazey (2001) also agree, while the potential for autonomy
appears to exist at the individual level, students need to be given permission to
demonstrate their capacities and develop them. Thus, students should not only be
trained on how to become autonomous learners, but also be given the chance to
take decisions together with their teachers.
For students to take decisions together with their teachers requires teachers to be au-
tonomous enough to share this power of decision-making with their students, so it
could be argued that “teacher autonomy is a prerequisite for learner autonomy”
(McGrath, 2000, p. 109). However, as Little (2000) explains, “it is unreasonable to ex-
pect teachers to foster the growth of autonomy in their learners if they themselves do
not know what it is to be an autonomous learner” (p. 45). As for the case of Turkey,
Erdoğan (2003), for example, found out in her study at a Turkish secondary school that
the factors of teacher behavior hinder the development of learner autonomy because
the teachers themselves were within the same education system and thus were unable
to change their habits (Üstünlüoğlu, 2009).
Whether autonomy precedes motivation or motivation precedes autonomy, this mu-
tual practice of decision-making would doubtlessly have a positive effect on students’
motivational and autonomous behaviors. Therefore, the most important implication of
this study is thatwe need to raise autonomous teachers to raise autonomous learners,
and the schools or the institutions should allow room for teachers to act autonomously
enough to raise autonomous students (Balçıkanlı, 2010; Çakır & Balçıkanlı, 2012).
Although this is a small scale study and the results are non-generalizable to other
contexts, it should be kept in mind that since autonomy is not an all-or-nothing con-
cept, every small step taken towards autonomy would help students to get more and
more autonomous.
Okay and Balçıkanlı Asian-Pacific Journal of Second and Foreign Language Education (2017) 2:8 Page 12 of 12
Authors’ contributions
This study was carried out with the mutual work of two authors. AO (first author) designed the study, reviewed the
literature, collected the data and wrote the first draft. CB (second author) supervised the work, provided sources,
helped with the data analyses, and revised the first draft. Both authors read and approved the final manuscript.
Competing interests
The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Publisher’s Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Author details
1
Yıldız Teknik Üniversitesi, İstanbul, Turkey. 2Gazi Üniversitesi, Ankara, Turkey.
References
Balçıkanlı, C. (2010). Learner autonomy in language learning: Student Teachers’ beliefs. Australian Journal of Teacher
Education, 35(1), 90–103.
Bao, X. H., & Lam, S. F. (2008). Who makes the choice? Rethinking the role of autonomy and relatedness in Chinese
children's motivation. Child Development, 79, 269–283.
Benson, P. (1997). The philosophy and politics of learner autonomy. In P. Benson & P. Voller (Eds.), Autonomy and
independence in language learning. London: Longman.
Benson, P. (2001). Teaching and researching autonomy in language learning. London: Longman.
Çakır, A., & Balçıkanlı, C. (2012). The use of the EPOSTL to foster teacher autonomy: ELT student teachers’ and teacher
trainers’ views. Australian Journal of Teacher Education, 37(3), 1–16.
Chan, V., Spratt, M., & Humphreys, G. (2002). Autonomous language learning: Hong Kong tertiary students’ attitudes
and behaviors. Evaluation and Research in Education, 16(1), 1–18.
Dam, L. (1995). Learner autonomy 3 - from theory to classroom practice. Dublin: Authentik.
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human behavior. New York: Plenum.
Deci, E. L., Vallerand, R. J., Pelletier, L. G., & Ryan, R. M. (1991). Motivation and education: The self-determination
perspective. Educational Psychologist, 26, 325–346.
Dickinson, L. (1995). Autonomy and motivation. System, 23(2), 165–174.
Dörnyei, Z. (2007). Research methods in applied linguistics: Quantitative, qualitative and mixed methodologies. Oxford:
Oxford University Press.
Dörnyei, Z., & Csizér, K. (1998). Ten commandments for motivating language learners: Results of an empirical study.
Language Teaching Research, 2(3), 203–229.
Erdoğan, S. (2003). Learner training via course books and teacher autonomy: A case of need. Retrieved March 7, 2017,
from https://drive.google.com/file/d/0BwDvAVEemzNwODA3MDI0YzYtNTAxNy00YWJiLTlmMTAtMTBmZGE1MDI3Y
mNm/view?ddrp=1&hl=en#.
Fazey, D., & Fazey, J. (2001). The potential for autonomy in learning. Studies in Higher Education, 26(3), 345.
Garcia, T., & Pintrich, P. R. (1996). The effects of autonomy on motivation and performance in the college classroom.
Contemporary Educational Psychology, 21, 477–486.
Greene, J. C., Caracelli, V. J., & Graham, W. F. (1989). Toward a conceptual framework for mixed-method evaluation
designs. Educational Evaluation and Policy Analysis, 11(3), 255–274.
Holec, H. (1981). Autonomy and foreign language learning. Oxford: Pergamon.
Humphreys, G., & Wyatt, M. (2014). Helping Vietnamese university learners to become more autonomous. ELT Journal,
68(1), 52–63.
Little, D. (1991). Learner autonomy 1: Definitions, issues and problems. In Dublin: Authentik autonomy and independence
in language learning (pp. 18–34). London: Longman.
Little, D. (2000) We’re all in it together: Exploring the interdependence of teacher and learner autonomy. Paper
presented at Autonomy 2000, University of Helsinki Language Centre, 7–9 September 2000. In Newsletter of the
IATEFL PL Autonomous Learning SIG 4.
Littlewood, W. (1999). Defining and developing autonomy in east Asian contexts. Applied Linguistics, 20(1), 71–94.
Littlewood, W. (2000). Do Asian students really want to listen and obey? ELT Journal, 54(1), 31–35.
McGrath, I. (2000). Teacher autonomy. In B. Sinclair, I. McGrath, & T. Lamb (Eds.), Learner autonomy, teacher autonomy:
Future directions (pp. 100–110). London: Longman.
Pennycook, A. (1997). Cultural alternatives and autonomy. In P. Benson, & P. Voller. Autonomy and Independence in
Language Learning. Harlow: Longman. 35–53.
Smith, R. (2003). Pedagogy for autonomy as (becoming-)appropriate methodology, and ‘Postscript: Implications for
language education’. In R. C. Smith & D. Palfreyman (Eds.), Learner autonomy across cultures: Language education
perspectives. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.
Spratt, M., Humphreys, G., & Chan, V. (2002). Autonomy and motivation: Which comes first? Language Teaching
Research, 6(3), 245–266.
Üstünlüoğlu, E. (2009). Autonomy in language learning: Do students take responsibility for their learning? Journal of
Theory and Practice in Education., 5(2), 148–169.
van Lier, L. (2008). Agency in the classroom. In J. P. Lantolf & M. E. Poehner (Eds.), Sociocultural theory and the teaching
of second languages (pp. 163–186). London: Equinox.