Culture and Emotion - Noba
Culture and Emotion - Noba
Toggle Navigation
Log InSign Up
●
●
● Blog
● Browse
1. Browse Content
2. Culture and Emotion
By Jeanne Tsai
Stanford University
How do people’s cultural ideas and practices shape their emotions (and other types of feelings)?
In this module, we will discuss findings from studies comparing North American (United States,
Canada) and East Asian (Chinese, Japanese, Korean) contexts. These studies reveal both
cultural similarities and differences in various aspects of emotional life. Throughout, we will
highlight the scientific and practical importance of these findings and conclude with
recommendations for future research.
Share
●
●
● ×
Close Dialog
Culture and Emotion
Share this module to:
Share this URL
● Collectivism
● Culture
● Emotion
● ethnicity
● facial expression
● Happiness
● ideal affect
● Individualism
● Self
● well-being
Learning Objectives
● Review the history of cross-cultural studies of emotion
● Learn about recent empirical findings and theories of culture and emotion
● Understand why cultural differences in emotion matter
● Explore current and future directions in culture and emotion research
Take a moment and imagine you are traveling in a country you’ve never been to before.
Everything—the sights, the smells, the sounds—seems strange. People are speaking a
language you don’t understand and wearing clothes unlike yours. But they greet you with a
smile and you sense that, despite the differences you observe, deep down inside these people
have the same feelings as you. But is this true? Do people from opposite ends of the world
really feel the same emotions? While most scholars agree that members of different cultures
may vary in the foods they eat, the languages they speak, and the holidays they celebrate, there
is disagreement about the extent to which culture shapes people’s emotions and
feelings—including what people feel, what they express, and what they do during an emotional
event. Understanding how culture shapes people’s emotional lives and what impact emotion has
on psychological health and well-being in different cultures will not only advance the study of
human behavior but will also benefit multicultural societies. Across a variety of
settings—academic, business, medical—people worldwide are coming into more contact with
people from foreign cultures. In order to communicate and function effectively in such situations,
we must understand the ways cultural ideas and practices shape our emotions.
Historical Background
In the 1950s and 1960s, social scientists tended to fall into either one of two camps. The
universalist camp claimed that, despite cultural differences in customs and traditions, at a
fundamental level all humans feel similarly. These universalists believed that emotions evolved
as a response to the environments of our primordial ancestors, so they are the same across all
cultures. Indeed, people often describe their emotions as “automatic,” “natural,” “physiological,”
and “instinctual,” supporting the view that emotions are hard-wired and universal.
Universalists point to our prehistoric ancestors as the source of emotions that all humans share.
[Image: Stefan Sheer, https://goo.gl/x56mw9, CC BY-SA 3.0, https://goo.gl/tCiqlm]
The social constructivist camp, however, claimed that despite a common evolutionary heritage,
different groups of humans evolved to adapt to their distinctive environments. And because
human environments vary so widely, people’s emotions are also culturally variable. For
instance, Lutz (1988) argued that many Western views of emotion assume that emotions are
“singular events situated within individuals.” However, people from Ifaluk (a small island near
Micronesia) view emotions as “exchanges between individuals” (p. 212). Social constructivists
contended that because cultural ideas and practices are all-encompassing, people are often
unaware of how their feelings are shaped by their culture. Therefore emotions can feel
automatic, natural, physiological, and instinctual, and yet still be primarily culturally shaped.
In the 1970s, Paul Ekman conducted one of the first scientific studies to address the
universalist–social constructivist debate. He and Wallace Friesen devised a system to measure
people’s facial muscle activity, called the Facial Action Coding System (FACS; Ekman &
Friesen, 1978). Using FACS, Ekman and Friesen analyzed people’s facial expressions and
identified specific facial muscle configurations associated with specific emotions, such as
happiness, anger, sadness, fear, disgust. Ekman and Friesen then took photos of people posing
with these different expressions (Figure 1). With the help of colleagues at different universities
around the world, Ekman and Friesen showed these pictures to members of vastly different
cultures, gave them a list of emotion words (translated into the relevant languages), and asked
them to match the facial expressions in the photos with their corresponding emotion words on
the list (Ekman & Friesen, 1971; Ekman et al., 1987).
Across cultures, participants “recognized” the emotional facial expressions, matching each
picture with its “correct” emotion word at levels greater than chance. This led Ekman and his
colleagues to conclude that there are universally recognized emotional facial expressions. At
the same time, though, they found considerable variability across cultures in recognition rates.
For instance, whereas 95% of U.S. participants associated a smile with “happiness,” only 69%
of Sumatran participants did. Similarly, 86% of U.S. participants associated wrinkling of the nose
with “disgust,” but only 60% of Japanese did (Ekman et al., 1987). Ekman and colleagues
interpreted this variation as demonstrating cultural differences in “display rules,” or rules about
what emotions are appropriate to show in a given situation (Ekman, 1972). Indeed, since this
initial work, Matsumoto and his colleagues have demonstrated widespread cultural differences
in display rules (Safdar et al., 2009). One prominent example of such differences is biting one’s
tongue. In India, this signals embarrassment; however, in the U.S. this expression has no such
meaning (Haidt & Keltner, 1999).
Figure 1. Facial expressions associated with happiness, sadness, disgust, and anger based on
the Facial Action Coding System. [Image: Paul Eckman, used with permission]
These findings suggest both cultural similarities and differences in the recognition of emotional
facial expressions (although see Russell, 1994, for criticism of this work). Interestingly, since the
mid-2000s, increasing research has demonstrated cultural differences not only in display rules,
but also the degree to which people focus on the face (versus other aspects of the social
context; Masuda, Ellsworth, Mesquita, Leu, Tanida, & Van de Veerdonk, 2008), and on different
features of the face (Yuki, Maddux, & Matsuda, 2007) when perceiving others’ emotions. For
example, people from the United States tend to focus on the mouth when interpreting others’
emotions, whereas people from Japan tend to focus on the eyes.
But how does culture shape other aspects of emotional life—such as how people emotionally
respond to different situations, how they want to feel generally, and what makes them happy?
Today, most scholars agree that emotions and other related states are multifaceted, and that
cultural similarities and differences exist for each facet. Thus, rather than classifying emotions
as either universal or socially-constructed, scholars are now attempting to identify the specific
similarities and differences of emotional life across cultures. These endeavors are yielding new
insights into the effects of cultural on emotion.
Based on studies comparing North American and East Asian contexts, there is clear evidence
for cultural similarities and differences in emotions, and most of the differences can be traced to
different cultural models of the self.
Consider your own concept of self for a moment. What kinds of pastimes do you
prefer—activities that make you excited, or ones that make you calm? What kinds of feelings do
you strive for? What is your ideal affect? Because emotions seem and feel so instinctual to us,
it’s hard to imagine that the way we experience them and the ones we desire are anything other
than biologically programmed into us. However, as current research has shown (and as future
research will continue to explore), there are myriad ways in which culture, both consciously and
unconsciously, shapes people’s emotional lives.
Outside Resources
Audio Interview: The Really Big Questions “What Are Emotions?” Interview with Paul Ekman,
Martha Nussbaum, Dominique Moisi, and William
Reddyhttp://www.trbq.org/index.php?option=com_content&view=category&layout=blog&id=16&I
temid=43Book: Ed Diener and Robert Biswas-Diener: Happiness: Unlocking the Mysteries of
Psychological WealthBook: Eric Weiner: The Geography of BlissBook: Eva Hoffmann: Lost in
Translation: Life in a New LanguageBook: Hazel Markus: Clash: 8 Cultural Conflicts That Make
Us Who We AreVideo: Social Psychology
Alivehttp://psychology.stanford.edu/~tsailab/PDF/socpsychalive.wmvVideo: The Really Big
Questions “Culture and Emotion,” Dr. Jeanne Tsai
Video: Tsai’s description of cultural differences in emotion
1. What cultural ideas and practices related to emotion were you exposed to when you were
a child? What cultural ideas and practices related to emotion are you currently exposed to
as an adult? How do you think they shape your emotional experiences and expressions?
2. How can researchers avoid inserting their own beliefs about emotion in their research?
3. Most of the studies described above are based on self-report measures. What are some
of the advantages and disadvantages of using self-report measures to understand the
cultural shaping of emotion? How might the use of other behavioral methods (e.g.,
neuroimaging) address some of these limitations?
4. Do the empirical findings described above change your beliefs about emotion? How?
5. Imagine you are a manager of a large American company that is beginning to do work in
China and Japan. How will you apply your current knowledge about culture and emotion
to prevent misunderstandings between you and your Chinese and Japanese employees?
Vocabulary
AffectFeelings that can be described in terms of two
dimensions, the dimensions of arousal and valence
(Figure 2). For example, high arousal positive states
refer to excitement, elation, and enthusiasm. Low
arousal positive states refer to calm, peacefulness,
and relaxation. Whereas “actual affect” refers to the
states that people actually feel, “ideal affect” refers to
the states that people ideally want to
feel.CultureShared, socially transmitted ideas (e.g.,
values, beliefs, attitudes) that are reflected in and
reinforced by institutions, products, and
rituals.EmotionsChanges in subjective experience,
physiological responding, and behavior in response
to a meaningful event. Emotions tend to occur on the
order of seconds (in contract to moods which may
last for days).FeelingsA general term used to describe
a wide range of states that include emotions, moods,
traits and that typically involve changes in subjective
experience, physiological responding, and behavior in
response to a meaningful event. Emotions typically
occur on the order of seconds, whereas moods may
last for days, and traits are tendencies to respond a
certain way across various situations.Independent
selfA model or view of the self as distinct from others
and as stable across different situations. The goal of
the independent self is to express and assert the self,
and to influence others. This model of self is prevalent
in many individualistic, Western contexts (e.g., the
United States, Australia, Western
Europe).Interdependent selfA model or view of the self
as connected to others and as changing in response
to different situations. The goal of the interdependent
self is to suppress personal preferences and desires,
and to adjust to others. This model of self is prevalent
in many collectivistic, East Asian contexts (e.g.,
China, Japan, Korea).Social constructivismSocial
constructivism proposes that knowledge is first
created and learned within a social context and is then
adopted by individuals.UniversalismUniversalism
proposes that there are single objective standards,
independent of culture, in basic domains such as
learning, reasoning, and emotion that are a part of all
human experience.
References
● Bylsma, L., Morris, B., & Rottenberg, J. (2008). A meta-analysis of emotional reactivity in
major depressive disorder. Clinical Psychology Review, 28(4), 676–691.
● Chentsova-Dutton, Y. E., Chu, J. P., Tsai, J. L., Rottenberg, J., Gross, J. J., & Gotlib, I. H.
(2007). Depression and emotional reactivity: Variation among Asian Americans of East
Asian descent and European Americans. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 116(4),
776–785.
● Chentsova-Dutton, Y. E., Tsai, J. L., & Gotlib, I. (2010). Further evidence for the cultural
norm hypothesis: Positive emotion in depressed and control European American and
Asian American women. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 16, 284–295.
● Cheryan, S., & Monin, B. (2005). Where are you really from?: Asian Americans and
identity denial. Journal of personality and social psychology, 89(5), 717–731.
● Chim, L., Moon, A., Ang, J., & Tsai, J. L. (2013). Magazine ads, Facebook pages, and
company websites reflect cultural differences in ideal affect. In T. Masuda (Chair). Culture
and mind: Implications for art, design, and advertising. Symposium held at International
Association for Cross-Cultural Psychology, Los Angeles, CA.
● Chim, L., Tsai, J.L., Hogan, C., & Fung, H. H. (2013). Enhancing happiness by valuing
calm. Manuscript in preparation.
● Cousins, S. D. (1989). Culture and self-perception in Japan and the United States.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56(1), 124–131.
● Curhan, K., Sims, T., Markus, H., Kitayama, S., Karasawa, M., Kawakami, N., . . . Ryff, C.
(2013). Negative affect predicts worse physical and mental health in the U.S. than in
Japan. Manuscript under review.
● De Leersnyder, J., Mesquita, B., & Kim, H. S. (2011). Where do my emotions belong? A
study of immigrants’ emotional acculturation. Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin, 37(4), 451–463.
● Ekman, P. (1972). Universals and Cultural Differences in Facial Expressions of Emotion.
Paper presented at the Nebraska Symposium on Motivation.
● Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. (1978). Facial Action Coding System: A Technique for the
Measurement of Facial Movement. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
● Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. (1971). Constants across cultures in the face and emotion.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 17(2), 124–129. doi: 10.1037/h0030377
● Ekman, P., Friesen, W., O\'Sullivan, M., Chan, D., Diacoyanni-Tarlatzis, I., Heider, K., . . .
Tzavaras, A. (1987). Universals and cultural differences in the judgments of facial
expressions of emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 712–717.
● Freud, S. (1910). Five lectures on psycho-analysis. (Vol. XI). London: Hogarth Press.
● Gross, J. J. (1998). Antecedent- and response-focused emotion regulation: Divergent
consequences for experience, expression, and physiology. Journal of personality and
social psychology, 74(1), 224–237.
● Haidt, J., & Keltner, D . (1999). Culture an d facial expression : Open-ended methods find
more faces and a gradient of recognition. Cognition and Emotion, 13, 225-266.
● Heine, S., Lehman, D., Markus, H., & Kitayama, S. (1999). Is there a universal need for
positive self-regard? Psychological Review, 106(4), 766–794.
● Hofstede, G. (2001). Culture\'s Consequences: comparing values, behaviors, institutions,
and organizations across nations (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications.
● Hyun, J. (2005). Breaking the bamboo ceiling: Career strategies for Asians. New York.
Harper Collins.
● Koopmann-Holm, B., & Matsumoto, D. (2011). Values and display rules for specific
emotions. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 42(3), 355–371.
● Kroeber, A.L. & Kluckhohn, C. (1952). Culture: A critical review of concepts and
definitions. Papers. Peabody Museum of Archaeology & Ethnology, Harvard University,
47, (viii), 223.
● Kwan, V., Bond, M., & Singelis, T. (1997). Pancultural explanations for life satisfaction:
Adding relationship harmony to self-esteem. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 73(5), 1038–1051.
● Lutz, C. (1988). Unnatural emotions: Everyday sentiments on a Micronesian atoll and
their challenge to Western theory. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
● Markus, H., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition,
emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98(2), 224–253. doi:
10.1037/0033-295X.98.2.224
● Masuda, T., Ellsworth, P. C., Mesquita, B., Leu, J., Tanida, S., & Van de Veerdonk, E.
(2008). Placing the face in context: Cultural differences in the perception of facial emotion.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94(3), 365–381. doi:
10.1037/0022-3514.94.3.365
● Miyamoto, Y., Uchida, Y., & Ellsworth, P. C. (2010). Culture and mixed emotions:
Co-occurrence of positive and negative emotions in Japan and the United States.
Emotion, 10(3), 404–415. doi: 10.1037/a0018430
● Morling, B., Kitayama, S., & Miyamoto, Y. (2002). Cultural practices emphasize influence
in the United States and adjustment in Japan. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
28, 311–323.
● Oishi, S., Diener, E. F., Lucas, R. E., & Suh, E. M. (1999). Cross-cultural variations in
predictors of life satisfaction: Perspectives from needs and values. Personality and Social
Psychology Bulletin, 25(8), 980–990.
● Perunovic, W., Heller, D., & Rafaeli, E. (2007). Within-person changes in the structure of
emotion: The role of cultural identification and language. Psychological Science, 18,
607–613.
● Russell, J. A. (1994). Is there universal recognition of emotion from facial expressions? A
review of the cross-cultural studies. Psychological Bulletin, 115(1), 102–141. doi:
10.1037/0033-2909.115.1.102
● Safdar, S., Friedlmeier, W., Matsumoto, D., Yoo, S. H., Kwantes, C. T., Kakai, H., &
Shigemasu, E. (2009). Variations of emotional display rules within and across cultures: A
comparison between Canada, USA, and Japan. Canadian Journal of Behavioural
Science/Revue canadienne des sciences du comportement, 41(1), 1–10.
● Sims, T., Tsai, J., Wang, I., Fung, H. H., & Zhang, X. L. (2013). Whether you experience
the bad with the good depends on how you want to feel: Understanding cultural
differences in the relationship between positive and negative affect. Manuscript in
progress.
● Soto, J., Perez, C., Kim, Y. H., Lee, E., & Minnick, M. (2011). Is expressive suppression
always associated with poorer psychological functioning? A cross-cultural comparison
between European Americans and Hong Kong Chinese. Emotion, 11(6), 1450–1455.
● Suh, E., Diener, E., Oishi, S., & Triandis, H. (1998). The shifting basis of life satisfaction
judgments across cultures: Emotions versus norms. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 74(2), 482.
● Tsai, J. L. (2007). Ideal affect: Cultural causes and behavioral consequences.
Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2, 242–259.
● Tsai, J. L., Chentsova-Dutton, Y., Freire-Bebeau, L., & Przymus, D. (2002). Emotional
expression and physiology in European Americans and Hmong Americans. Emotion, 2(4),
380–397. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.2.4.380
● Tsai, J. L., Knutson, B., & Fung, H. H. (2006). Cultural variation in affect valuation. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 90, 288–307.
● Tsai, J. L., Levenson, R., & McCoy, K. (2006). Cultural and temperamental variation in
emotional response. Emotion, 6(3), 484–497. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.6.3.484
● Tsai, J. L., Levenson, R. W., & Carstensen, L. L. (2000). Autonomic, subjective, and
expressive responses to emotional films in older and younger Chinese Americans and
European Americans. Psychology and Aging, 15(4), 684–693.
● Tsai, J. L., Louie, J., Chen, E. E., & Uchida, Y. (2007). Learning what feelings to desire:
Socialization of ideal affect through children’s storybooks. Personality and Social
Psychology Bulletin, 33, 17–30.
● Tsai, J. L., Miao, F. F., Seppala, E., Fung, H. H., & Yeung, D. (2007). Influence and
adjustment goals: Sources of cultural differences in ideal affect. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 92, 1102–1117.
● Tsai, J. L., Sims, T., Thomas, E., & Fung, H. H. (2013). Ideal affect across the life span: A
comparison of European American, Chinese American, and Hong Kong Chinese.
Manuscript in progress.
● Weisz, J., Rothbaum, F., & Blackburn, T. (1984). Standing out and standing in: The
psychology of control in American and Japan. American Psychologist, 39, 955–969.
● Yuki, M., Maddux, W. W., & Masuda, T. (2007). Are the windows to the soul the same in
the East and West? Cultural differences in using the eyes and mouth as cues to recognize
emotions in Japan and the United States. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,
43(2), 303–311.
Authors
● Jeanne TsaiJeanne Tsai is associate professor of psychology at Stanford University and
director of the Stanford Culture and Emotion Lab. She has received numerous awards
and grants for her work on culture and emotion and on the implications of cultural
differences in emotion for mental health, decision-making, and person perception.
Tsai, J. (2023). Culture and emotion. In R. Biswas-Diener & E. Diener (Eds), Noba textbook
series: Psychology. Champaign, IL: DEF publishers. Retrieved from http://noba.to/gfqmxtyw
Sections
● Abstract
● Learning Objectives
● Historical Background
● Current and Research Theory
● Cultural Models of Self in North American and East Asian Contexts
● Cultural Similarities and Differences in Emotion: Comparisons of North American and East
Asian Contexts
○ People’s Physiological Responses to Emotional Events Are Similar Across
Cultures, but Culture Influences People’s Facial Expressive Behavior
○ People Suppress Their Emotions Across Cultures, but Culture Influences the
Consequences of Suppression for Psychological Well-Being
○ People Feel Good During Positive Events, but Culture Influences Whether People
Feel Bad During Positive Events
○ People Want to Feel Good Across Cultures, but Culture Influences the Specific
Good States People Want to Feel (Their “Ideal Affect”)
○ People Base Their Happiness on Similar Factors Across Cultures, but Culture
Influences the Weight Placed on Each Factor
● Why Do Cultural Similarities And Differences In Emotion Matter?
● Current Directions In Culture And Emotion Research
○ What About Other Cultures?
○ How Are Cultural Differences in Beliefs About Emotion Transmitted?
○ Could These Cultural Differences Be Due to Temperament?
● Summary
● Outside Resources
● Discussion Questions
● Vocabulary
● References
● Authors
● Creative Commons License
● About
● Privacy
● Terms
● Licensing
● Contact
● © 2023 Diener Education Fund
● Comments or questions?
×
Close Dialog
Sign up using your email
Email Address
Password
First Name
Last Name
By joining Noba, you agree to the privacy policy, terms of service and licensing agreement.
×
Close Dialog
Log in using your email
Email Address
PasswordForgot your password?
Remember me for two weeks
×
Close Dialog