5675 1 15896 1 10 20170703
5675 1 15896 1 10 20170703
Abstract
reinforce the ideas of the message source. On the other hand, the
artistic category of persuasion is classified into three: ethos, pathos, and
logos (Bulan and de Leon 2002, 60; Tompkins 1982, 31-32). Simply,
ethos pertains to the communicator’s source credibility; pathos, to the
communicator’s emotional appeal; and logos, to the communicator’s
method of reasoning and argumentation (Bulan and de Leon 2002, 60).
Among the three modes, it is assumed that ethos is the most potent.
Moreover, it may be removed from the triadic association it maintains with
pathos and logos since it operates at a different level or plane of classification
(Rosenthal 1966, 26). While this view of the importance of ethos has
undoubtedly acceptable basis, it may be supererogatory as more recent
interrogations of the concept contend that credibility cannot be totally
detached from emotion and logic primarily because their manifestations
and functions may also be exemplified and embodied in ethos (Cheng
2012, 428); perceptions of ethos may largely stem from pathos and logos
(in the same way that the values of pathos and logos may as well be
dependent on ethos—emotion and logic in the absence of credibility
cannot be persuasion). Moreover, the conceptualization of ethos as a
construct that is connected with empathy and intelligence (among other
concepts linked with credibility) (McCroskey and Teven 1999, 95-96; Niu
and Ying 2016, 45) may imply its inherent relations with pathos and logos,
hence, questioning its absolute independence from the two other artistic
persuasion measures. Despite these seemingly contradicting takes on
ethos’ interactions with pathos and logos, none of them refute the former’s
vital position in communication transactions that necessitate persuasive
appeals. Following, then, the notion that ethos plays an extremely crucial
role in persuasion, it may be said that if credibility is presented well and
perceived positively by the message receivers, it is easier for the source to
persuade the listeners, rendering higher probability of the receivers to act
the way the source wishes. In the context of the study, it can be claimed
that credibility of politicians may, to a great extent, hone the publics’
voting behaviors, especially those of the youth who can significantly
influence election results, considering their substantial numbers.
(Bulan and de Leon 2002, 63; McCroskey 1986, 62-66). These three facets
of ethos are all essential in measuring credibility. In fact, empirical data
suggest that ethos may not be quantified using only one category rating;
meaning, three ratings, one for each dimension, should be computed
when accounting for credibility (McCroskey and Teven 1999, 99). In this
paper, then, these concepts are variables that are necessary in gauging the
effects of political candidates’ credibility on public’s voting behaviors.
Ethos as a Construct
The third stage, the final one, is the terminal ethos. This is the
source’s credibility upon the completion of the communicative act, the
sum total of the first two ethos levels, the product of extrinsic and intrinsic
credibility (Bulan and de Leon 2002, 64-65; McCroskey 1986, 77-78). It
may be assumed that this is the most critical ethos stage since it can greatly
affect the source’s initial ethos in future communication interactions.
knowing its stages as its dimensions are equally important, if not more
so, especially in gauging its effects on different components in various
communication settings. Aristotle’s rhetoric proposes three universal
ethos facets: competence or authoritativeness (source’s expertise,
training, and intelligence), trustworthiness or character (source’s honor
and moral qualifications), and goodwill or intention (source’s genuineness
and sensitivity) (McCroskey 1986, 63-66). These dimensions are said to
be concrete determinants of communicator’s total credibility (McCroksey
and Teven 1999, 95-96; Teven 2008, 389-94), which means that they
stand on equal footing and are evenly important; hence, the three must
always be considered in any attempt to gauge one’s credibility so as to
preserve the triadic nature of ethos. In determining, then, the effects of
political candidates’ credibility on Filipino youth’s voting behaviors, it is of
paramount importance that all the three above-said aspects are assessed;
otherwise the quantitative evaluation of ethos is compromised.
The factor model discusses the extent to which the receivers deem
the source as credible. It is the first mechanism that affects the degree of
persuasion, in the manner that, if the judgment of the receivers is favorable
8 | DE PANO
While the paper, through the studies reviewed above, aims to solidify
the grounds of credibility in the turf of political persuasion, it cannot be
denied that there are also data that somewhat negate or downplay ethos’
weight in assessment of one’s persuasive faculty. Claims that credibility
does not enjoy significant effects on behavioral change in the context of
persuasive political communication is found in existing literature (Morin,
Ivory, and Tubbs 2012, 418-19). It is suggested that though ethos and its
facets may sustain relationships with the public’s electoral preference, the
significant influences of the former on the latter can not be categorically
inferred. Although this does not support the paper’s claims on the
centrality of ethos dimensions in Filipinos’ voting attitudes, it nevertheless,
and in fact, all the more, necessitates examination or reexamination
of said variable primarily because of the incongruent view it exhibits.
While voting behaviors carry various definitions, in this paper, they are
operationalized as either positive or negative. Positive voting behavior indicates
favorable voting decision (voted for); negative voting behavior, unfavorable
voting decision (did not vote for and would have least likely voted for).
can range between .80 and .94 (McCroskey and Teven 1999, 95).
Findings show that 94.06% of the youth respondents who rated the
credibility of the presidential candidates they voted for assigned them high
competence ratings. These are consistent with the perceived competence
mean score of 38.61, also categorized as high. In the cases of trustworthiness
and goodwill dimensions, the same findings are observed; 82.18% of the
same respondents gave their presidential bets high trustworthiness ratings
(mean score of 35.12, high); and 64.36% assigned them high goodwill
ratings (mean score of 32.15, high). Table 1 summarizes these results.
Ethos Dimensions
Competence Trustworthiness Goodwill
Ethos Dimensions
Competence Trustworthiness Goodwill
they voted for (exhibiting positive voting behavior) and political candidates
whom they did not vote for (exhibiting negative voting behavior).
Lastly, for the goodwill dimension, the same findings are recorded.
The difference between the evaluations of the respondents who displayed
positive voting behavior (perceived goodwill mean score of 32.15, high)
and those who presented negative voting behavior (perceived goodwill
mean score of 16.24, low) is significant (p-value: 0.000), accepting the
third hypothesis that there would be a significant difference between the
Filipino youth’s perceived goodwill levels of political candidates whom
they voted for (exhibiting positive voting behavior) and political candidates
whom they did not vote for (exhibiting negative voting behavior).
Table 3 shows the results of the statistical test for difference run to
examine and measure the effect of credibility dimensions on youth’s voting
patterns.
Table 3: Summary of the Statistical Differences Measuring the Effects of Credibility Dimensions on
Youth’s Voting Behaviors
*significant at .05 alpha level
Table 4: Summary of the Logistic Regression Test Results Examining Each Credibility Dimension as a
Predictor of Voting Behaviors
*significant at .05 alpha level
Number of
Responses Respondents %
Would have given the same ratings
164 79.61
(Gave scores of 3, 4, and 5)
Would not have given the same ratings
42 20.39
(Gave scores of 1 and 2)
Table 5: Summary of Ratings on the Likeliness of Giving the Same Credibility Evaluations to Other
Political Candidates in the 2016 National Elections
18 | DE PANO
389-94). Having said this, two other assumptions may be forwarded: first,
that whatever happens to politicians’ credibility levels will reflect in their
ability to market themselves to the voting populace; and second, since
credibility can partially forecast voting behaviors, the factors that hone ethos
at its various states and stages may well have bearing on polling verdicts.
While not a primary objective of the study, the empirical data may
as well offer basis for acceptance of goodwill’s legitimacy as a measure of
Aristotelian ethos. The results also reconfirm the statistical validity and reliability
of the constructs included in the instrument to quantitatively assess credibility.
constitute a big chunk of the electing population and play a vital role in
any Philippine election, since they constitute a big chunk of the electing
population, this paper provides political candidates an effectual means to
secure affirmative voting attitudes. Indeed, the study, in general, stresses
the strength of ethos in the field of persuasion and communication.
It must be noted that while the study results advance the legitimacy
of goodwill, along with competence and trustworthiness, as ethos criterion,
they do not discredit other alternative hypotheses that expect isolations of
more constructs that can likewise determine ethos. Meaning, it is possible
that apart from the three universally accepted dimensions, credibility may
be shaped by other variables. In a nutshell, the findings acknowledge
and demonstrate the importance of Aristotelian ethos constructs, but
22 | DE PANO
References
Cheng, Martha. “Colin Powell’s Speech to the UN: A discourse analytic study of
reconstituted ethos.” Rhetoric Society Quarterly. 2012.
24 | DE PANO
Gotinga, JC. “Poll watchdog PPCRV: Candidates now buy votes by wholesale.”
CNN News Site, October 26, 2015. http://cnnphilippines.com/
news/2015/10/26/vote-buying-PPCRV-2016-elections.html (accessed May 19,
2016).
Hovland, Carl, Irving Janis, and Harold Kelley. Communication and Persuasion:
Psychological studies of opinion change. New Haven: Yale university Press.
1953.
Morin, David, James Ivory, and Meghan Tubbs. “Celebrity and politics: Effects of
endorser credibility and sex on voter attitudes, perceptions, and behaviors.” The
Social Science Journal. 2012.
Nicolas, Fiona and David Santos. “The youth ask, who deserves our vote?”
CNN Philippines News Site, August 14, 2015. http://cnnphilippines.com/
news/2015/08/12/youth-ask-who-deserves-our-vote.html (accessed July 19,
2016). 2015.
Niu, Wang and Yuan Ying. “Reframing ethos rhetorical criticism.” Linguistics and
Literature Studies. 2016.
Relao, Eileen Kae. “Politicians in the public eye: A study on the relationship
between the source credibility and parasociability of selected politician endorsers
and the publics’ voting behaviour during the May 2010 national elections.”
Underraduate thesis, University of the Philippines Diliman. 2011.
Rosenthal, Paul. “The concept of ethos and the structure of persuasion.” Speech
Monographs. 1966.
Wayne, Stephen. The road to white house. New York: St. Martin’s Press. Cited in
Teven, Jason. 2008. An examination of perceived credibility of the 2008
presidential candidates: Relationships with believability, likeability, and
deceptiveness. Human Communication. 2002.