The Inflectional Structure of Lubukusu Verbs
The Inflectional Structure of Lubukusu Verbs
OCTOBER 2018
ii
DECLARATION
iii
DEDICATION
In memory of my dear late, mum Edith Nekoye, my late uncles Jeff Watulo and
Fred Wenyaa for being my mentors. To my late grand mums Rosa and Rasoa
who took good care of me after the demise of my mum. Lastly, to my dear wife
Naomi who with unwavering support took good care of our lovely sons Ken and
Mike while I was busy connecting dots during mid night and day time to make
my writing scholarly.
iv
ACKNOWLEDGEMENT
With a lot of humility, I appreciate our Almighty God for enriching me with
sufficient grace and patience until this moment. I would not have travelled this
long journey had it not been for God‟s mercy and guidance in all the activities I
to read the many drafts I send to him. I am deeply indebted to Dr. Nandelenga‟s
passionate guidance and advice during the time I was struggling to read and write
my work.
my course work. To Dr. Kirigia, Prof. Khasandi and Dr. Wathika thank you for
taking me through course work. I have special regards for Professor Marlo of
Missouri University for the pain-staking task he took for sending soft copy of
The great role that my dad played cannot be gainsaid up to this final stage of my
project. He gave me the most adequate advice to study language up to this level.
TABLE OF CONTENTS
DECLARATION .................................................................................................... ii
ACKNOWLEDGEMENT ..................................................................................... iv
CHAPTER TWO................................................................................................. 15
RESEARCH METHODOLOGY....................................................................... 32
LIST OF TABLES
aspect………………………………………………………………...45
AGR Agreement
FV Final Vowel
INTER Intermediate
Neg Negative
NUM Number
OM Object Marker
PER Person
PL Plural
POL Polarity
PST Past
SG Singular
SM Subject Marker
SU Subject
TNS Tense
VR Verb Root
X Root
xi
ABSTRACT
CHAPTER ONE
1.0 Introduction
The chapter provides details on the background of the study, statement of the
and significance of the study. Lastly, it presents information on the scope and
world are classified into the Niger-Congo group while 522 are narrow Bantu. The
word Bantu is a sub-family that comprises of about 500 and 800 languages
spoken by approximately 240 million people in the sub Saharan (Nurse &
and 7.5 percent are Bantu. Bantu languages are a group of the largest African
2007). Botne and Kershner (2008) argue that the languages have a rich tense and
aspect systems. The factors towards this richness, is because of a large number of
languages. The present study sought to investigate the complexity of tense system
evident in Lubukusu.
2
Guthrie (1948) originally classified Lubukusu as E31 but current studies have
under J 30.
Lubukusu spoken in Bungoma and parts of Rift valley. Apart from Lubukusu, the
following are the other dialects of Luhya spoken in Western region: Lutiriki,
east, Lutura and Lwisukha (Angogo, 1980). In Eastern Uganda, there exist four
(Were, 1967). Were argues that Lubukusu dialect is spoken in western Kenya in
the larger Bungoma district and Eastern region in Uganda in Manafwa, Bududa,
Mbale and Sironko districts. Marlo (2009) as quoted in Diercks (2010) argues
that there are twenty-three dialects of Luhya spoken in Western Kenya and
Eastern Uganda.
Bryan (1959) questioned whether Lubukusu, Lugisu and Lukisu (also spoken in
Lugisu and Lukisu were later considered as similar languages. She recognized
three divisions within the Kavirondo area (Gisu, Luhya and Gusii groups).
Guthrie (1967) argued that the more southerly, languages should be considered
North –Eastern Victoria Bantu (NEV): Northern Central and Southern. He further
argued that the Northern subgroup comprised the Gisu and Bukusu. The Southern
regarded as Lulogooli.
northern most dialect of the Luhya language, spoken mostly in Bungoma County
and parts of Transnzoia County and some areas of Uasin Gishu County
(Nandelenga, 2000; 2013). The Lubukusu spoken in Bungoma District has three
main variations according to (Makila, 1978). There are minor variations as you
go further from Bukusu heartland (Bumula) to the north of Kimilili area towards
Transnzoia and east of Webuye towards Kakamega and Lugari. These variations
from those who hail Bumula which is the heartland of Lubukusu dialect.
4
The pronunciation affects the transcription especially the tone patterns on given
syllables. The Babukusu are a Bantu cultural sub-entity who belongs to a larger
cultural entity known as Abaluhya. They speak the dialect called Lubukusu.
Coming to the linguistic subfield under which Lubukusu will be studied Booij
(2007), argues that morphology is a sub discipline of linguistics but it can also
mean part of the grammar of a language. It contains rules for inflection and word
formation also called derivation. He further notes that morphology aims to probe
into the nature of linguistic systems and human natural language. Hence it serves
to get a good understanding of the given linguistic rules and even internal
gives an insight into how linguistic rules function in language perception and
The morphology of any given language is governed by rules that explain the
attachment of a given affix to the stem. The rule-governed morphology forms the
field for linguistic investigation of verbal inflectional rules to account for the
argues that for a given morphological operation there is need to define a group of
process whose role is to create words that have a high level of transparency,
which means that words have a formal morphological structure that relates to
their semantic interpretation (Booij, 2007, pp. 34). He argues that words are
grouped into various roots and affixes called morphemes. Therefore, morphemes
provides rules that account for the sub-divided parts (Haspelmath, 2002). The
into different types (Payne, 1997). He notes that the syntactic environment
requires any inflectional operations where the root appears (Payne, 1997). These
operations do not usually change the basic meaning of the concept expressed.
They specify the time the activity or situation took place, the participant or
participants and at other times where the activity took place. The present study on
analysis will utilize inflectional rules and reconstruct some, which will explain
Booij (2007) posits that agglutinative languages are always studied in a model
the inflected morphemes. Nurse (2008) argues that Bantu languages are “verby”
Bantu languages.
Verbs have several affixes, and a verb plays a pivotal role in a sentence by
incorporating a lot of information and it can make a complete sentence (Nurse &
Philippson, 2003). In this sense, single word verbs initially consisted of eleven
slots, centered on a root. The verb comprises the root with optional derivational
suffixes and inflectional prefixes (Van der wal, 2015). The eleven slots do not
morphemes to co-occur. Therefore, to the left are inflectional prefixes that mark
relative, negation, subject, tense, aspect and object while on the right exists
mood among other categories. Most Bantu languages still have this or a similar
structure, others a modified form and a few have shortened it as others have made
it long.
Buell (2011) argues that object marking is widely realized using a prefix that is
morphologically close to the verb root. Subject marking is always obligatory but
object marking is not always available in a verb paradigm (Buell, 2011). On the
The study in Lubukusu sought to find out the order of morphemes of Lubukusu
Morrison (2011) posits that the Bena language has a rich morphology compared
to other Bantu languages in Africa. The verb can have a set of prefixes that are
entirely inflectional while suffixes are purely derivational although at other levels
inflectional suffixes exist. The tense and aspect configuration within the language
is quite complex. Within the language tense and aspect is marked using prefixes,
mostly before the verbal root while aspect morphemes occur before and after the
verbal root.
In the Bena language aspect is mostly indicated using verbal suffixes, some
aspectual prefixes and to some extent auxiliaries followed by a main verb. She
argues that the progressive aspect indicates ongoing activities and perfective
marks an event as a whole. The past perfective is marked using the suffix [–ile] in
the verb such as „tugonile’ (we slept). In her study, she discusses present
perfective and past perfective along with morphemes that mark the two
morphological aspects. Our present study handled tense and aspect then provided
On the other hand, mood in the Bena language is imperative and subjunctive.
Morrison (2011) argues that the imperative takes the verbal stem (verbal root and
any verbal extensions, with a final vowel [–a]. The vowel is used with the second
subjunctive is formed with the subject marker followed by the verbal base (verbal
root with any extensions) and the final vowel [–e]. Moreover, the object prefixes
are evident with the subjunctive. Apart from the exhortation marker [–ha-] used
in commands there are no other prefixes used to form the subjunctives. The
object marker + verbal base + [-e]. The study by Morrison on the template
informed the present study to generate a verbal template structure for Lubukusu
Negation in the Bena is quite straightforward. Morrison (2011) argues that there
are three negative morphemes the negative prefix [si-] with all inflected finite
forms. Secondly, the prefix [ta-] negates verbal infinitives. The third form of
negation involves the verb tanaa, which negates the subjunctive. The morpheme
[si-] occurs in one of the positions. The prefix immediately precedes the subject
marker in the pre- SM slot or it immediately follows the subject marker in the
The negated infinitives have the negative morpheme ta- before the verbal stem.
The high tone occurs on the morpheme ta-. The study on negation in Bena
a tensed verb.
number of a given subject. Nurse and Philippson (2003) argue that in most Bantu
cease playing the role of a sentence. In this case, a finite verb acts as a basic
Just like other Luhya dialects, the morphology of Lubukusu is quite rich in
agreement both at affixal and sentential level. Diercks (2010) argues that
prediction that agreement is upward, nominal and verbal among others. The study
rules and agreement. The studies conducted in the area of morphology and
morphosyntax; had either neglected or left out some core aspects like; verbal
inflectional rules, agreement at a level of subject and object prefixes within verbs.
Though the template structure of Bantu verbs seems to be somehow similar, the
study on Lubukusu template verbal structure has not been studied by the previous
researchers. The starting point for verbal inflectional study is a sub division of a
verbal paradigm into smallest units to offer a step forward for a detailed
morphological analysis. The study focuses on 1st, 2nd and 3rd person forms of
verbs in both singular and plural forms within Lubukusu. The study takes up a
formal analysis and a rule based approach to the study of verbal morphemes in
Lubukusu.
The morphological structure of Bantu verbs is the same cross linguistically but
some Bantu languages do not adopt such a general and similar structure, hence
widely and this provides a firm basis for studying Lubukusu. Through a formal
analysis, the study utilizes Bantu morphological rules and reconstructs some that
Additionally, the study probes into the order of selected morphemes in few
morphemes.
verbs.
morphemes?
3. What are the inflectional rules that govern the inflectional morphology of
Lubukusu verbs?
1.5 Assumptions
inflectional rules.
Diercks (2010, p.7) argues that “there are strong reasons as to why Lubukusu as a
into some aspects of syntax and phonology hence leaving out a key component-
Although studies on morphology are ongoing but there are strong reasons why
Realizational theory was to be conducted to unearth some of the issues that the
previous researchers have not given a thorough look in the area of verbal
languages and hence acts as a basis for documentation of the language within
have been conducted but little has been studied in the area of verbal inflectional
study handled the interface between morphology and syntax through agreement.
marking. The study also discusses agreement within simple predicates, negatives,
syntax, which do not bear a relation with morphology. The study does not tackle
syntactical trees because the theoretical framework does not provide details on
such a phenomena. The syntactical trees though they are key aspects of Syntax
but they do not offer an in-depth opportunity for interaction with inflectional
morphology. Further, the theory is only limited to two sets of tenses (remote and
immediate future) within the same category in order to reconstruct the realization
rules.
14
Moreover, the study is limited to introspected data, corpora data and elicited data.
The researcher used the native speaker‟s intuitions to arrive at introspected data.
Additionally elicited data was generated from three native speakers using an
interview schedule. Despite the fact that there were scanty published sources on
Lubukusu literature the researcher used his on intuitions with the aid of other
native speakers to confirm the data collected. On the other hand, sampling is
limited to subjects from Bumula sub-county. Makila (1978) argues that the purest
1.8 Summary
In summary, this chapter has focused on the background of the study, statement
and significance of the study and finally the scope and limitation. In the next
chapter, our discussion deals with the literature review and the theoretical
framework.
15
CHAPTER TWO
2.0 Introduction
The chapter handles a review of related literature to the study and Stump‟s theory
of 2000, which tackles pertinent issues of the research. The literature review is
field and area of study. The literature is also linked tightly to the objectives of the
Nurse (2008) suggests five common types of verbal structures evident in Bantu.
They are single imperatives, inflected single words, two word structures
inflected auxiliaries. Lastly, the inflected main verb two-word structures with
inflected form of the verb. Morrison (2011) argues that the Bena language
comprises the five types although some structures are more common. The
imperatives in singular and plural are singleword of the structure SM- root-FV.
16
Bena language, which is spoken in South Western Tanzania, has the verbal
structures comprising of one or two forms: they can be inflected single words or
combining inflected auxiliaries and inflected main verbs. She adds that the
structure consisting of an inflected auxiliary and infinitive are not common in the
Bena. The last structure has an infinitive followed by an inflected form of the
same verb. This study bears a relationship with the current because it deals with
aspects of verbal morphology that are discussed in our study. The study on
that when there are no other prefixes the subject marker precedes the root but if
the tense marker or the object marker is present the subject marker cannot
immediately precede the root since the position is occupied by the prefixes. In the
immediate future continuous and future continuous tense, tense marking comes
after the subject marker, which is followed up by the object marker. Within the
language there exists a special type of object marking by the morpheme [zi-] that
indicates the relationship between an object and the verb. Nikki (2010) agrees
with other researchers that the Zulu verb structure is quite similar to that of other
Bantu languages. The Zulu verbal template has the following structure: neg
He argues that negation is presented at the left side of the verb in the form of an
initial a-. It can also be split and expressed not only with a- but also with a
different final vowel. Nikki (2010) states that the Zulu language compared to
many Bantu languages allow object marking within the verb. The object marker
is optional and not always available like the subject markers. The object markers
Ndebele on inflectional markings has a similarity with the current because they
sought to explore the order of morphemes on a verb template. The current study
the negative marker /ka-/ at the beginning of a verb. This marker is evident with
the present progressive, future tense and the conditionals, which is a combination
of the past tense marker-a- and future marker-tso-. The study explored the
position of the negative mark on the root and its interaction with tense and aspect,
concords before the verb stems or infinitive and several tense markers. The
subjunctive mood expresses ideas that communicate desire, wish and even doubt.
18
Such ideas are realized using a stem that ends in [–e] with or without future
formative [–ka], affix [–ki], continuous suffix [–nga] and any other object infixes
and suffixes. Sibanda (2004) explains that the infinitive mood Ndebele language
is realised through the prefix uku-. Besides [uku-] the infinitive can also take the
object marker, which comes before the root. The studies on Bena, Ndebele and
Mchombo (2004) notes that Chichewa is a tone language, which is, represented
in two levels with high and low tones. The verb structure comprises a stem onto
which proclitics are added which carry syntactic information. This information
and directional markers. He further asserts that object marking is not a mandatory
feature but subject marking is obligatorily present within the verb structure. In
subject marking. In a study by Spencer and Zwicky 1998, they argue that the
contains various prefixes and suffixes that play different grammatical roles. They
further argue that the regular verbal radical interchanges with the suffix [a],
which is referred to, as the final vowel. If it occurs in simple sentences, the verb
root is always prefixed hence marking the subject, person, number, tense and
aspect.
19
They further note that person numbers within the language are ndi-(1st plural), u-
2nd singular, a- 3rd singular and plural, ti-1st plural and mu-2ndplural. The tense
Lastly, the [–a-] marks perfective while [– na-] and [naa-] indicates past and
remote past respectively. They argue that a few tense aspect markers toned for
example immediate future. The study on Chichewa informs the current study to
discuss the 1st, 2nd, and 3rd person, tense, aspect categories and negation within
Lubukusu.
Kioko (2005) posits that the verbal morphology of Kikamba is quite complex.
The verb has a verb root, one or more prefixes and one or more suffixes (Kioko
2005, p. 24). The morphology of the verb has the pre root affixes, root,
derivational affixes and post stem affixes (Kioko 2005, p. 40). The pre root
affixes include the focus marker, the subject agreement, negative marker and the
tense marker. On the other hand, derivational affixes are verbal extensions while
the post stem markers are aspectual marker, final vowel and lastly the imperative
plural affix.
Kioko (2005) further argues that the Kikamba imperative is the simplest verbal
form within the language. She discusses forms of imperative used to address one
person, two people and many people with their corresponding negatives. She
inflections, modal auxiliaries and phonologically through high and low tones.
20
marking and its usage in reference to one or many people. Onyango (2006)
explains tha Olunyala language is verby just like other Luhya languages. The
tenses as follows present, have just, immediate past, past, remote past, immediate
future, and future. The tense and aspect system has inflections and tone patterns
tense and aspect system in Lubukusu through inflection marking and high tones
Nasiombe (2000) discusses tense system in Lubukusu verbs and provides affixal
markers for tense, aspect and negation in the language. He classifies the future
presents events that will happen today while the intermediate talks about the
activities that will take place after today. The affixal marker for immediate future
is the affix-ta- while the intermediate future is [–xa-] and the remote future is
and remote past just as he does with the progressive aspect (Nasiombe, 2000,
Our study sets out to provide specific morphological rules that account for
but it happens that these elements are not placed on the verbal template with their
uses the prefixes [se-] and [sa-] to mark negation in selected paradigms in
Lubukusu. The particle [–ta] is attached to the verb. The current study will
negation but he leaves out information concerning rules that explain the
omitted or left out the prefix so- in marking negation. The present study delved
into a discussion of the negative prefixes sa-, se- and so- which mark negation
Nasiombe (2000) in his study discusses the Lubukusu imperative forms which
take two forms depending on if the order is being given to one person or many.
The examples of tehse forms are /lima/ (cultivate) addressing a single person
while /limé/ is used with the plural. The negative paradigms of the imperatives
are realized through attaching the negative prefix [xa] to a verb stem.
22
The subject marker is not a mandatory component in the negated verbal forms but
if it is present it must preced the negative marker [xa]. On the other hand, he
states that the subjunctives are combined with another verb for example [nééɲa]
(I want). A subject marker is obligatorily present with the final vowel [-e]. a high
tone is further attached to the first two moras of the verb to realize the following
[nééɲa múlime] (I want you to cultivate). The study by Nasiombe is quite related
Buell (2011) argues that Bantu languages use their subject markers, object
markers and agreeing prefixes that appear in the verb. Bantu verb word can be
Bearth (2003) posits that syntax creates an interaction with the verbal
verb stem. The subject marker appears earlier in the word, often to the left of
certain tense and aspect prefixes. Subject marking is always obligatory while
object marking is not always present in a verb. The study is related with the
present because it deals with conditions for subject and object marking. Our study
Kikamba just like most Bantu languages, there are subject and object agreement
the other hand, the lack of object markers is dependent partly on the presence and
(Muchina, 1981). Subject-verb agreement in Kikamba is after the focus -ni in the
verb complex (Kioko, 2005, p. 55). In Chingoni language, agreement between the
subject and the verb is in the form of an affix that precedes the tense marker
(Ngonyani, 2003). Subject concord is obligatory except for the imperative verbs
only. He adds that object markers are obligatory for human objects and not other
nouns. In Chingoni, the object concord marker appears after the tense marker and
the verb stem. The current study sought to discuss manifestation of agreement
and argues that a template between subject concord and the stem can best serve to
analyze the Shona verb like ndi-cha-chi-tora (I will not take) and (nda-ka-chi-
tora) (I then now take). The first rule BLOCK encompasses exclusive prefix chi-
, the second block comprises of the future cha- and a remote ka-. The final
BLOCK is made up of the concord prefixes ndi- and nda-, which expresses the
Stump (2001) provides a Swahili verbal positional analysis of the verb ona (see).
prefixational rules. The rules are organized into three coherent blocks with
first plural subject agreement is realized through the prefix tu- while second
person is marked by the affix m (w)-. The study on Swahili position class
analysis provided rich information from which the morpheme slots and
On the other hand BLOCK B is occupied by the prefix ta- and li- to express the
future and past tense respectively as BLOCK C takes up the prefix ku- for 2nd
singular object agreement and m(w) for 3rd class 1 object agreement (Stump,
2001, p.20). The studies carried out by Aranovich and Stump are relevant to this
study the inflectional morphology was approached through the use of Inferential
Realization theory which guided our study. Based on the studies by (Stump, 2001
and Aranovich 2015) the current study set out to give analysis of selected verbs
in Lubukusu and subject them to Stumps theory for analysis. The rules of
dialect.
25
gives an example of the English word sailed and asserts that the past tense and
participle of the verb are realized through inflecting the verb sail with the suffix-
aspect among others. In a good number of languages, the choice of a stem plays a
1998).
Spencer and Zwicky (1998) argue that in Latin verbal paradigm there is a very
present tense stem (this includes concomitant lengthening of the final vowel of
the stem).The stem in Latin forms imperfect of verbs in conjugations and even
the future verbs for the first and the second conjugation. In line with this
argument the -b suffix in laudab (the b stem of the conjugation verb laudare
exponent of imperfect in the verb laudam which means I praised and therefore
The current study looks at the stems that are associated with particular
morphosyntactic properties and the specific affixes attached to the given stems.
and the past tense of the verb. The pieces that combine to realize the form of a
exponent and the property are related using realization rules. Exponence is
affixes that appear after the stem. Corbett (2006, p. 73) gives the following
1 Nov-aja Knig-a
The current study sought to discuss the use of suffixes and prefixes in the
realization of tense, aspect and negation in the language. Additionally, the present
study provided a rule based approach in the analysis of morphemes with their
Corbett (2006) argues that in non-canonical inflection stems may show minor
2006). In languages such as French the verb „savoir’ (know) in the present tense
has several stems in both singular and plural Corbett (2006, pp. 74).
SINGULAR PLURAL
1. Sais Savons
2. Sais Savez
3. Sait Savent
The inflectional information within a word form is within a feature value notation
NUMBER PLURAL
Secondly, the inflectional categories are presented using subscripts of the word
indicating singular it is introduced by a rule whereby /X/ is any verb stem that
carries the third person, singular, present tense and indicative mood features. The
V PER 3
NUM sg
TNS pres
MOOD: Indic
X X- Y
28
The type of approach has inflectional rules grouped into BLOCKS in that the
rules that are within similar BLOCKS are excluded in their application. Our study
determines the properties that are associated with an entire word. Therefore, a
word is inflected. For example, a word such as loves arises because of a certain
rule appending the suffix –s to a given verb stem that is associated with such
properties like 3sg subject agreement, present tense and even indicative mood
(Stump, 2001). In our study of Lubukusu the paradigm „ba-loma‟ (they say)
arises through the morphosyntactic rule that appends the prefix ba- to any given
Lubukusu verb stem that bears the following set of properties; 3rd plural subject
inflectional paradigms. The property distinguishes phrases that are of the same
as agreement.
29
values. The inflectional morphology study takes this form of pairing as key to the
properties
NUM sg, pl
related with a set of morphosyntactic properties. The only factor that mediates
The verbal paradigms are shared by a language‟s system of rules that include
morphosyntactic properties like plural, number and tense. The theory states that
Stump, (2001) argues that paradigms play a key role in defining the inflectional
morphology of any given language. The order in which inflectional rules apply to
Such rules associate a given affix with the property set for example 1st person
plural subject agreement or 2nd person subject agreement among others. Stump
expressed by more than one marking in the same word.” A study of Swahili
concerning multiple exponence by (Stump, 2001) indicates that the negative past
tense verb negation is expressed by the use of the negative past tense prefix ku-
and the prefix ha- (haku-taka) “he/she did not want.” In the negative plural, it is
study generates a discussion on the prefixes se- and sa- that are attached on a verb
such as kula (buy) to realize paradigms like seengula (I don‟t buy) and saakula
featurally coherent if only each member communicates one or any other given
further notes that deletion involves a given consonant or vowel being omitted
can be deductible from the one that is similar to the cell in a different paradigm.
rules are organized in BLOCKS. The rules belonging to the same block compete
for the same position in the sequence of rule applications defining a word‟s
2.3 Summary
The chapter has objectively reviewed the literature related to the study. The
literature from the Bantu verbal morphology studies was reviewed systematically
as per the objectives of the study. The focus of the literature delved more on the
Gregory Stump in the year 2001. The next chapter evaluates the research
CHAPTER THREE
RESEARCH METHODOLOGY
3.0 Introduction
This chapter gives a detailed description of the research methodology. The sub-
headings discussed in this section include research design, study site and target
considerations.
argues that the design allows the researcher to gather information, summarize and
interpret the data. The design enabled the researcher to identify the
Further, the design provided a detailed summary and analysis of elicited, corpora
and introspected data. The design suits our study since it is qualitative and hence
it does not involve numerical data. It is also reliable since it helps the researcher
to explain the scenario as it is based on the paradigms collected from the field.
Specifically, the villages for study were Kimatuni, Lumboka and Mateka. Makila
(1978) asserts that the purest form of the Lubukusu is spoken in the former
Bumula constituency. The site for study was accessible and had a large
Kenya.
The study used convenience sampling to arrive at the sample size. Dornyei
(2003) argues that in this kind of sampling, members of the target group are
proximity and availability at a given time. Aiken (1997) argues that “convenience
samples are mostly purposive since besides accessibility; subjects also possess
subjects that are rich in the information required for the study. For purposes of
random sampling was utilized to select the villages to be visited for the study. In
this study, subjects were selected based on the linguistic competence. The three
villages were randomly sampled. The three villages are Kimatuni, Lumboka and
Chomsky (1981, 1982) argues that the central goal of linguistic inquiry is to
entails the speaker‟s ability to identify verbal paradigms that are acceptable and
from the three villages mentioned above. According to (Marlo 2013) small
samples tend not to be necessary for linguistic studies. Therefore, out of the three
elicitation while the other speaker from Lumboka took part in sentence
elicitation. The third speaker from Mateka took part in the checking of the
The next sample size entailed the verbal paradigms gathered from the field,
Lubukusu Bible, elicitation and through self- generation. Therefore, the verbs
elicited from the respondents in the field were fifty while forty sentences were
elicited. Fifty verbs were sampled from the Bible and one hundred were self
generated based on the objectives of the study. The verbs from the Bible were
sampled purposively depending on the goals of the study. The verbs sampled
from the Bible were the infinitives. After this, the researcher used his intuitions as
aspect, tense, negation and agreement on the stated verbs. For purposes of
analysis and documentation of Lubukusu smaller sample sizes were created from
the larger ones above. The paradigms that were used for analysis were scaled
down to sixty.
35
Bible. After that, the researcher used an interview schedule (see Appendix iii).
This was used to elicit data from two native speakers who hail from Kimatuni
and Lumboka village. In order to get acceptable and the right structures of the
verbal paradigms the field researcher asked the respondents the same question at
least twice to get the most accepted form of the paradigms. The researcher used
recorders during the interview sessions while in the field. The purpose of this tool
was to help the researcher to get the actual pronunciation for later analysis on
aspects of tone marking on some tensed verbs. Specific questions concerning the
research study were asked to generate the right data for study. The researcher
intended for study. Later the researcher used word lists to create categories for
The researcher used a direct form of elicitation with three proficient native
speakers of the language. The researcher elicited data and prompted the
respondents to repeat answers for each question at least two times (see Appendix
vii and ix). The researcher started with simple verbs and continued to elicit more
verbs that are complex. The complexity of the verb is realized though the length
The field researcher carried out substitutions on aspects of mood marking, tense,
aspect, negation, number, and agreement. As the list of elicited words enlarged,
concerning different sub categories of tense and aspect in Lubukusu. Further, the
Moreover, the researcher got corpora data by collecting, examining and analyzing
a sample of fifty paradigms from Lubukusu Bible (see appendix vi). Within the
Bible, thirty verbal paradigms were sampled from different books of the Old
Testament while the rest came from the New Testament. The verbs were sampled
morphosyntactic features that arose from the verbs. Consequently, the researcher
collected 240 paradigms from the Bible, elicitation and using an interview
confirm the sampled data for purposes of grammaticality, structure and meaning
generated from the forms. Later on the researcher engaged two speakers of the
dialect from Mateka village for purposes of grammatical checking of the elicited
verbs and sentences concerning the structure, morphological marking and their
meanings.
37
The verb elicitation process generated one hundred paradigms while sentence
elicitation gave rise to forty sentences, then the Bible and interview schedule
provided fifty paradigms each. Additionally, the researcher used the native
speaker‟s intuitions to inflect the verbs for tense and aspect. These comprised 100
verbs each for both tense and aspect. The groups were listed as group A (tense)
and intransitive verbs, finite and non-finite verbs, active and passive verbs among
disyllabic and trisyllabic verbs. These samples represented all forms of tense,
aspect, person, mood, agreement and negation used in the analysis and
presentation of data.
The researcher started by organizing and preparing data by typing the notes
gathered from the field. The coding process helped the researcher to give a vivid
transcriptions of the paradigms gathered from the three sources. The phonological
the researcher to assign tone patterns on various tense and aspect categories. The
Furthermore, the analysis also entailed sub-dividing the paradigms into smallest
made concerning the type and function of the morphemes identified. The study
identified morphemes in a given paradigm and the rules that explain the entire
markings that carry tense, subject, object, person, number, negation, agreement
and mood were identified once the selected infinitives verbs were inflected.
After isolating the morphemes, the researcher checked the form, meaning and
function of the same morphemes, and then explained alternations that existed.
Further, the morphemes were placed in specific slots in tables. The tables
The first step in corpus analysis entailed extracting lexemes from the Bible with
diverse conjugations. Secondly, the researcher identified the forms that encode
tense, agreement, negation, person and aspect within the verbal forms that were
purposively sampled from Lubukusu Bible. The items were later arranged and
labeled from far (past) to far (future). The morphological property person was
presented in a table from first person to third person along with their
corresponding singular and plural forms. Lastly, the category aspect was arranged
The study also looked at the rules of insertion and deletion of selected verbal
applied some aspects of the theoretical framework such as realizational rules and
rule BLOCKS to organize the selected morphemes that mark tense, agreement,
person and number into featurally coherent blocks. The realization rules are
derived from the coherently organized BLOCKS, which form an integral aspect
position BLOCKS.
The researcher applied for the research permit from National Commission for
Science, Technology and Innovation (NACOSTI) before collecting data from the
by the use of the field notebook and a laptop. The daily log file helped the
fieldwork researcher to summarize what was learnt during the day hence making
a review of data into a most elaborate and clear way. Data was organized in data
base files which included paradigms that were studied. The organization of data
procedures. Vaux and Justin (2003) argue that the field notebook should be
organized based on the sentences the researcher was working with during the
study. The lexemes were typed on separate pages to cater for neatness and logical
arrangement of data.
40
The respondents signed a consent form before they got involved in the interview
process. Data was used for the purposes stated to the respondents and made clear
at the start of data collection process. The researcher further promised the
respondents the rights to privacy and confidentiality of the data provided. The
researcher used pseudonyms to conceal the identity of those taking part in the
research.
3.9 Summary
This chapter discussed the research methodology. Our discussion in this chapter
has focused on the research design, study site and target population, sampling
technique and sample size, data collection tools, data collection procedure, data
analysis and lastly data management and ethical considerations. The following
CHAPTER FOUR
4.0 Introduction
The chapter deals with presentation and analysis of the data collected from the
field. The section presents data in line with the objectives and research questions
for the study. Firstly, it delves into inflectional markings that realize subject,
object, negation, tense and aspect among other concerns of verbal inflectional
which forms the key aspect of the interplay between morphology and syntax.
Lastly, prefixational rules and realization rules are discussed as the last concern
of the chapter.
According to the data that was collected through self generation, elicitation and
secondary source, this section provides a formal analysis of the the order of
nine slots for attachment of morphemes though the ninth slot is not evident in
Lubukusu. The presentation below gives the slots, class and specific morphemes
9 Post final -
Based on the data above, Lubukusu being a highly agglutinative language allows
several slots to be filled using specific inflectional morphemes. The slots, class of
form and this gives a second way of analyzing a verbal template structure that
In table 4.1, the slots on the verbal template indicate that the stated morphemes
possess a fixed order that starts from the left to the right side of the template.
verbal template. The above is a modified version of the Bantu verbal template
template helps to analyze the order of morphemes found within Lubukusu verbal
morphology.
43
This comprises a set of affixes that occupy slot 1 on the template structure. They
occupy the first position at the left side of the root. Lubukusu realizes three
negative morphemes, which pattern with the particle [ta] to form negative verbal
paradigms. Within the language, affixes that mark negation include [se-], [so-]
and [sa-]. These morphemes are accompanied with high-toned particle [tá] to
2nd so 2nd se
3rd sa 3rd se
Immediate, future 1st se 1st se
2nd so 2nd so
3rd sa 3rd sa
Table 4.2 shows that the choice of the negative prefixes is dependent on
tense/aspect and number features. Such groups of affixes are specific with certain
verb roots, tense, aspect and person. It is with this regard that the table presented
The table also shows the patterning of negation with tense, aspect and person.
The subject marker precedes the negative morpheme. The morphemes are used
categories. In the data presented negative morphemes occur pre-verbally and that
is why they are always called pre-initial negative. The present tense utilizes the
negative morphemes se-, so-, and sa- in singular and plural forms. These
morphemes are attached before the root. The intermediate past and remote past
are negated by the negative morpheme se-, which is attached before the subject
markers, marking the 1st, 2nd, and 3rd persons. The morphemes are used both
with the singular and plural forms of verbs. The immediate past behaves
differently from the two forms because it uses the negative morphemes [se-], [so-
] and [sa-] with the 1st person, 2nd person and 3rd person respectively.
The intermediate future, immediate future and remote future are negated using
the negative morphemes se-, so- and sa-. The prefix se- is used with the 1st
person, while so- is attached before the 2nd person subject markers [xa-], [nda-]
and [la-] to mark negation in the three categories of future tense. Lastly, the
negative morpheme sa- is attached before the 3rd person to realize negation in the
three forms of future tense. The morphemes [se-], [so-] and [sa-] are applicable to
The negative morphemes are attached before the auxiliary. The auxiliaries
3rd se 3rd se
2nd so 2nd se
3rd sa 3rd se
Completive aspect 1st se 1st se
2nd se 2nd se
3rd se 3rd se
Persistive aspect 1st se 1st se
2nd so 2nd se
3rd sa 3rd se
46
In table 4.3, the progressive aspect is negated in the 1st, 2nd, and 3rd person
using the negative morphemes [se-], [so-] and [sa-] respectively. The habitual
aspect is negated through the morpheme that is placed before the specific subject
markers na-, wa-, ka, and xa-. This form of aspect behaves uniquely from the rest
because it only utilizes the prefix se- for negation of 1st, 2nd and 3rd persons in
both singular and plural. The negation of the perfective aspect singular within 1st,
2nd and 3rd persons is accomplished through the negative morphemes [se-], [so-]
and [sa-] respectively while the plural persons is marked using the prefix se-.
Lubukusu utilizes two pre final markers on the right of the verb. They occupy slot
Paradigm gloss
SM root pref fv
SM TM root pref fv
From the data above the suffixes [––aŋɡ] and [ –enŋɡ-] are used to mark habitual
aspect and perfective aspect. They are always used with habitual aspect.
47
The pre-final marker [–aŋɡ-] is used with on going habitual while [-enŋɡ-] is
The final vowel forms which come after the pre-final markers form a set of
suffixes that appear at the end of the verb in the FV slot on a verbal template.
Examples of paradigms
Sm root fv
The data above confirms that the final vowel changes from [–a], to [–e] and [–il]
depending on the tense, aspect and mood. Lubukusu utilizes three final vowels
The vowels are not presented in isolation but instead they pattern with tense,
aspectual and mood markers within the language. They come after the tense and
aspect markers as presented in the data above. The morphemes help in the
Lubukusu verbs have markers for person/class through subject. The language is
marked for person and number for both singular and plural.
In table 4.4, the 1st, 2nd and 3rd person paradigms have both singular and plural
forms. These markers agree with the class of the nouns in question. Lubukusu has
23 noun classes and only class1, 2 and 3 are inflected for person and number
The subject prefixes are obligatory markers in finite verb forms within Lubukusu.
Subject marking is realized through subject concords that appear in the initial
position. The subject concord is a necessary element of inflected finite verb thus,
morphology.
Sm root fv Sm root fv
Sm root fv root fv
According to the data above, the infinitive comprises the prefix [xu-] and the
final vowel [-a] while the imperatives have the final vowel [-a] alone. These
forms do not have the subject markers/concords like other forms of verbal
paradigms.
50
template and the class of noun it attaches to. An object marker is a type of
within Lubukusu. The object prefixes are placed on OM slot on a verb within
next/close to the verb immediately before the verb root. The prefixes mark the
verb for person/class of the object. Lubukusu allows a single object to be marked
on the verb and is preceded by the subject marker. The object markers are
Lubukusu, object markers for person are only two and these include; [mu-] and
[βa-]. The morpheme mu- marks the singular person while [βa-] is evident with
the plural forms. Lubukusu has a variety of object markers for both animate and
Lubukusu tense and aspect system is quite complex. The complexity is realized
FUTURE TENSE
Immediate future -la Xu-la-tim-a We will run
Intermediate future -xa xu-xa-tim-e We will run
Remote future -li xu-li-tim-a We will run
HABITUAL ASPECT
Ongoing habitual -aŋɡ, -a xu-tim-aŋɡ-a We usually run
Past habitual -aŋɡ -a xwa-tim-aŋɡ-a We usually run
Future habitual -eŋɡ-e xu-xa-tim-eŋɡ-e We usually run
PERFECTIVEASPECT
immediate future -a -il, - xu-laβa-xwa-tim-il- We will have run
intermediate future e e We will have run
remote future -e -il - xu-xa-b-e-xwa-tim- We will have run
e il-e
-a -il -e xu-li-b-a-xwa-tim-
il-e
Persistive aspect -sii- -a xu-sii-tim-a We still run
Subjunctive aspect -e xu-tim-e I want you to run
Imperative -a, -e tim-a tim-e You run
Completive aspect -a xwa-a-tim-a We already ran
Progressive aspect -lixo - xu-li-xo-xu-tim-a We are still
xee running
In table 4.6, the tense morphemes are placed immediately before the verbal root
while the aspectual markers are placed either before or after the verbal root. In
the table presented above, there are instances when the auxiliaries are used to
mark tense and aspect. This is evident with the future tense and perfective aspect.
53
In some rare cases the adverb is used to mark the habitual, persistive, completive
and progressive aspect. The tense and aspect markers are discussed in detail in
the later section of the project. These will involve subcategories of past tense,
verb accompanied with a system of high tones on some tense categories. The data
presented and analysed within the following subsections of 4.2 confirms to us the
The present tense is also called the unmarked present or the zero present because
Singular
Surface Gloss
SM root fv
1st
/e- ŋɡon- a/ [eŋɡóna] I sleep
Sm root fv
1st
/xuu- kon- a/ [xukóna] we sleep
The paradigms in present tense in Lubukusu are not prefixed but there is always
the final vowel [–e] or [–a] at the end of the verbal root. The stem‟s initial vowel
gets a high tone in both the singular and plural paradigms making it high toned.
The verb is used with both the 1st, 2nd and 3rd persons. The order of morphemes is
the subject marker, verbal root and the final vowel represented in a few
paradigms below. The data below gives the underlying and the surface level of
This is the location in time prior to the present period. It locates the situation in
the past time and does not continue to or extend to the present period. Lubukusu
past tense is categorized into three periods in the past: intermediate past, remote
The intermediate past is formed by the prefix a- before the root and the suffix –
SM root TM fv
SM root TM fv
The data above indicates the realization of the intermediate past tense in
Lubukusu. In a tensed verb, the subject markers are obligatorily attached before
the root. In the first person singular and plural, the subject prefixes na- and xwa-
are attached immediately before the root. The second person singular involves the
subject prefix wa- attached immediately before the root while the plural takes the
prefix mu- before the root. The stem‟s initial vowel receives a high tone. Lastly,
the prefixes ka- and βa- are attached to the root to mark third person singular and
plural respectively.
The structure of the verbal paradigm entails the prefix a- attached before the root
and the suffix [–ile] that comes immediately before the root. The subject markers
[na-], [xwa-], [wa-], [mu-], [ka-] and [βa-] are placed next to the prefix a- as
obligatory markers. The underlying forms are not transcribed phonologically but
the surface levels, which are the actual realization of paradigms during
The remote past refers to events that took place long ago. It is realized through
SM root FV
SM root FV
In the data above the remote past is formed by the final vowel [-a] which is
always placed at the end of the verb root. The subject markers [na-], [wa-] and
[ka-] receive high tones on the vowel [a]. The surface level in this case has a
high tone on the subject-marker vowel [a] making the entire subject marker be
high toned.
57
The immediate past is used with events that have happened in the last few
minutes.
SM root fv
SM root fv
The data above on immediate past confirms that the stem‟s initial vowel is
marked with a high tone within the surface level of each paradigm in singular and
plural. This is followed up with the suffix [ –il-] and [-e] that attaches at the end
of the paradigm. The person markers are attached at the initial position just
before the root because the entire verbs formed are tensed.
The future reference is derived diachronically from the modal expression for
example desirability. It signals that a given situation is located on the time axis
Therefore the future is more speculative because the future prediction that is
made can change based on the intervening event and even our conscious
The intermediate future is also called the near future. It refers to the events that
will happen in the next few days after the time of speaking.
SM TM root fv
The intermediate future paradigms are toneless in both the singular and plural
forms. This takes the affix [-xa-] meaning that an action will take place some
hours after now during the same day. This is followed up by the final vowel [–e]
which attaches at the end of the paradigm. The subject markers are always
The immediate future talks about events that will take place very soon (mostly
SM TM Root FV
SM TM Root FV
The data above which was collected through verb elicitation indicates that the
immediate future marked using the tense marker [–la-] although the 1st person
singular takes the affix -nd-. The final vowel [–a] is attached at the end of the
The remote future informs us that the events are not expected to take place soon.
This means that the events will happen in several months or years to come.
SM TM root fv
SM TM root fv
The reaization of remote future in the data above is through the tense marker –li-
that is attached after the subject marker. After attaching the tense marker the final
vowel [–a] is placed immediately after the verbal root. The template for the
remote future takes the subject marker, tense marker, the verbal root and the final
vowel. The remote future bears a slight resemblance with the present tense
because in this case the stem‟s initial vowel is marked with a high tone making it
4.3 Aspect
categorized into progressive, habitual and perfective with the last two forms
This aspect describes the situation in progress and the listener need not to have
witnessed the activities from the start to the end but middle part of the activity
SM aux OM root fv
SM Aux OM root fv
3rd / βa- li-xo- ba- tim- a/ [βalíxó batíma] they are running
2nd / mu- li-xo- mu- kon- a/ [mulíxó mukóna] you are sleeping
3rd / βa- li-xo- ba- kon- a/ [ βalíxó bakóna] they are sleeping
The data above indicates how the progressive aspect is realized. It is formed by
attaching the auxiliary [–lixo-] in the middle of the subject marker and the object
marker. In all the paradigms in the progressive the vowels present are always
high toned which are accompanied with a high tone in the stem‟s initial vowel.
with paradigms in first person singular. The more standard and accepted form is
the auxiliary [–lixo-] and not [–xee-]. The template for the paradigm in
progressive aspect comprises the subject marker, auxiliary, object marker, the
feature of the entire period. It is divided into ongoing habitual, past habitual and
future habitual. The ongoing habitual and the past habitual are formed using the
final vowel-aŋɡ and [–a] but the future habitual utilizes [–e] instead of [–a]
although it retains [–aŋɡ]. The structure of the three forms of habitual aspect is
similar apart from the subject markers in the initial position that vary.
SM root pref fv
SM root pref fv
The on-going habitual refers to an event that is going on during the present
moment at regular intervals. The data above indicates that it is realized through
the pre final vowel [–nɡ] and the final vowel –a in all paradigms in 1st, 2nd and 3rd
person paradigms. The pre final vowel is placed before the root as the final vowel
comes last after the pre final vowel. In this case, high tone marking is always
within the stem‟s initial vowel in both the singular and the plural forms. The
subject markers form an important aspect and are always attached before the
verb.
Another form of habitual aspect is called the past habitual indicating an event that
used to occur in the past at regular intervals or always as a habit. The time when
SM root pref fv
SM root pref fv
It is evident from the data above that the past habitual is formed using the pre
final –aŋɡ and the final vowel a in the 1st, 2nd and 3rd person paradigms. The
concept of tone marking behaves a little bit different from the present, past and
ongoing habitual because while the three mark tone on the stem‟s initial vowel
The last form of habitual aspect is the future habitual which points at an event
that will take place in the future at regular intervals or it is a habit. The exact time
2nd /o- xa- kon- enŋɡ- e/ [oxakoneenŋɡe] you will usually sleep
3rd /a- xa- kon- enŋɡ- e/ [axakoneenŋɡe] he/she will usually sleep
2nd /mu- xa- kon- aŋɡ- e/ [muxakoneenŋɡe] you will usually sleep
The future habitual is formed using the pre final [–enŋɡ] and the final vowel [–e].
The pre final vowel is placed immediately after the verbal root on the right side.
The final vowel comes after the pre final vowel. The entire verbs in both the
singular and the plural are toneless as indicated in the data above.
65
SM root perf fv
SM root perf fv
The data above indicates that the perfective is formed by attaching the affix [–a-]
after the subject marker then the perfective-il is attached after the root. The final
On the other hand, the immediate future perfective indicates activities that will
1st /e- ndaβa- na- kon- il- e/ [endaβanakonile] I will have slept
2nd /o- laβa- wa- kon- il- e/ [olaβawakonile] you will have slept
3rd /a- laβa- ka- kon- il- e/ [alaβakakonile] he/she will have slept
66
2nd /mu- laβa- mu-a- kon- il- e/ [mulaβamuakonile] you will have slept
3rd /βa- laβa- βa- kon- il- e/ [βalaβaβakonile] they will have slept
From the data above, the immediate future perfective is formed using the
auxiliary verbs [ndaβa] (will have) or [laβa] (will have). The auxiliary [ndaβa] or
[laba] is attached before the subject prefix. The auxiliary [laβa] becomes ndaβa
through prefixing the pronoun I. Therefore, ndaβa is used with the 1st person
speaker in singular while laba is mostly used with the remaining verbal
paradigms. The final vowels [–il] and [-e] is attached at the end of the paradigms.
Secondly, the intermediate future perfective is also called near future perfective.
2nd /o- xa-b-e wa- kon- il- e/ [oxabe wakonile] you will have slept
3rd /a- xa-β-e ka- kon- il- e/ [axaβe kakonile] he/she will have slept
2nd /mu- xa-β-e mwa-a- kon- il- e/ [muxaβe mwaakonile] you will have
slept
3rd /βa- xa-β-e ba- kon- il- e/ [βaxaβe βakonile] they will have slept
67
(will be) placed before the subject marker. The final vowel [–il-] and [-e] are
The final sub-category of perfective aspect is called the remote future perfective.
Singular Surface Gloss
SM aux OM root perf fv
1st /n di- β-a na- kon- il- e/ [ndiβa nakonile] I will have slept
2nd /o- li- β-a wa- kon -il- e/ [oliβa wakonile] you will have slept
3rd /a- li- β-a ka- kon- il- e/ [aliβa kakonile] he/she will have
slept
slept
2nd /mu- li-βa- mu-a- kon- il- e/ [muliβa mwaakonile] you will have
slept
3rd /ba- li-βa- ba- kon- il- e/ [baliβa bakonile] they will have
slept
The remote future perfective refers to events, which will happen in the future
(mostly a few months or years). The remote future perfective is formed using the
auxiliary [liβa] which changes to [ndiβa] when used with the 1st person singular
and[ li-β-a] (will have) for the other forms. The final vowel is placed at the end of
4.4 Agreement
This section addresses objective three which is the interplay between morphology
lexical category, which includes classes 1-11. These classes mark number. The
inherent inflectional categories present within nouns are number and class. It is
with the pre-prefix and prefix that the two morphological categories are realized
The starting point for studying agreement is a partial listing of noun class markers
In light of this, a partial analysis of this concept is presented in the table above
The subject markers form a salient feature of Lubukusu language using subject
markers alongside the agreeing prefixes that appear within the verb. They are
discussed with reference to the noun class markers. This classification is realized
verb stem. The subject marker is placed earlier in the word on the left of some
tense and aspect markers. Therefore, a tensed verb has an obligatory subject
16 a- Amoni a-
tʃun-a
17 xu- Xumusala On the
xu-funix-a tree ki-
breaks
18 Mu- Mumaindi mu- Mumaindi
muxaxisia
20 ku- Kuxana ku- The big ki- Kimixana The big
kend-a girl walks ki-kend-a. girls
walk
23 e- emakanda e- Emakanda
luy-a e-luuy-a
71
In table 4.8 we present a summary of noun class preprefixes and prefixes which
agree in number with the subject prefix attached on the verb. To start with, class
1-2 nouns are human beings where by class1 is marked by the prefix o-mu as
class 2 is realised by the prefix structure ba-ba- but realized as baa- before the
consonantal initial stem. On the other hand, class 3-4 has the singular forms
The data also indicates that class 5-6 are realized by the prefix /li-li/ in the
singular while the plural is markerd by /ka-ma/. The class 7-8 are use /si-/ in the
singular while the plural class 8 is realized through the prefix /bi-/.\
From the table above class 9-10 contains nouns marked by the prefix /N-/ in the
singular and plural but sometimes differentiated by the preprefixes [e] in the
The singular forms of the nouns in class 11 are morphologically formed in the
singular by the use of the syllable [lu-] and most of them are pluralized as class
10 with the prefix [či]. The class 15 is the infinitives, which are mostly verbs.
The Bukusu infinitives share characteristics with nouns, which include doubling
The table above also indicates that the class 16, which is also referred to us as the
locative [a], is formed by replacing the preprefix of a noun with a prefix that
On the other hand, class 17 which also called locative [xu] follows the order of
formation just like class 16 nouns although in former case the prefix [xu] replaces
The class 20 nouns are formed by replacing the prefix of a noun in class1-11 and
14 with the argumentative ku-ku. The argumentative marker ku-ku means huge,
large, enormous or gigantic. Last category of the nouns in the table above is class
23 which is realized by the locative prefix [e--]. The formation of class 23 entails
thr locative prefix replacing the preprefix of nouns from other classes as indicated
Agreement is also evident within modifiers. The modifiers in this case are
adjectives whose main function is to describe the nouns. The affix modifier must
agree in number with the one attached on the noun class and verb in terms of
number and person. This means that the singular modifier affix agrees with the
singular marker attached on the verb and the noun. The plural agreement is
explained alongside the plural markers attached on the noun, verb and adjective.
The table below gives a summary of noun, modifier and verb agreement.
73
Table 4.9 demonstrates that the subject prefix resembles the subject prefix
attached on the verb. It is with this similarity of the two markers that the subject-
agreement to be achieved, the subject prefix on the noun, modifier and the verb
must agree in number. The singular and plural subject markers agree in number
sentences below.
sikono
The small child gave Wekesa a basket the small children gave Wekesa the
baskets
The clever girl went home the clever girls went home
The pre-prefix o- in a singular subject agrees with the pre-prefix [o] marked on
the modifier. On the other hand, the pre-prefix [βa] marked on the subject must
The object concord functions like an object or the noun phrase and resemble the
O-xa-ci--kul-e you will buy them (sg) mu-xa-ci-kul-e you will buy them
(pl)
a-xa-ci-kul-e he/she will buy them βa-xa-ci-kul-e they will buy them
The object marker in the above set of verbs is the morpheme [-ci-] which agrees
with the subject markers [e-], [xu-], [o-], [mu-],[a-] and [βa-]. This means that the
class 10 object marker [-ci-] agrees with the person markers in class 1 as
presented above.
On the other hand, agreement in negative predicates involves the third person
The tree did not break The trees did not break
In the above set of data the class 1 nouns agree with the verbal paradigms in
Additionally, the negative prefix [sa-] agrees in number with the singular third
person paradigm while the negative prefix [se-] agrees with the plural third
person verbal paradigm. In inanimate objects the negative prefix does not change
the form in marking agreement while in animate the prefix shifts from [sa-] in
In Lubukusu, most nouns that refer to human objects are found in class 1 and 2.
The nouns that refer to animals are placed in classes 9 and 10.
Cl2
Cl6 cl6
The data presented above on animante and inanimate noun phrases, If the class 1
nouns are conjoined to form a subject then the agreeing verb should have a plural
marker. This is because the two nouns that are conjoined show invariable plural,
which is class two. The conjoined nouns agree in number with the subject prefix
It is evident from the data below that the subject and verb agreement that
prefixes.
The data above indicates that the number and class prefixes include [e-], [ci-],
[ku-], [ki-], [si-] and [βa-]. The singular subject marker makes it clear that
agreement is based on the class and number of the agreeing prefixes. The prefixes
should belong in the same number and class to complete the concept of
agreement. The singular and plural marking that indicates agreement on the verb
The object markers are co-referential to the noun phrase within the sentence. The
object marker always matches the person and number or class features of the co-
This focuses on number of the object marker and the noun phrase being referred
the child will tear the book the children will tear the books
the parent will wash the child the parents will wash the children
the student has read the book the students have read the books
In the examples above, the object marker agrees in number with the noun phrase
that takes the position of the object hence leading to well formedness of the
sentence. The singular object marker agrees with a singular object while the
plural object marker agrees in number with the plural object. The two elements
On the other hand, the ditransitive verbs are verbs that take two objects and this
Agreement within the above sentences is marked through subject markers evident
on the subject and the verb to indicate either singular or plural agreement.
We now turn to the last concern of the study which is the presentation and
description of the rules that play a key role in the realization of future tense,
negation, object, agreement, person and number and formation of both simple
The study of prefixational rules starts with a clear understanding of slot analysis,
morphemes allows for a derivation of rules and rule blocks from the slots.
In position class analysis a set of affixes within each slot are only featurally
coherent if each affix in the stated slots communicates the same morphosyntactic
feature/s
him. (singular)
80
In the data presented above the affixes within the given slot, communicate a
specific feature. These features are later presented in specific feature blocks. The
subject markers are presented in slot A while slot B carries tense markers as slot
The prefixational rules presented below are derived from the inflectional slots
BLOCK B
BLOCKC
In the data above each affixal slot corresponds to a separate functional head. The
affixal slot in this are three and they include subject agreement node, tense node
The morphological markers for tense, aspect, person and agreement are
applicable to the verbal paradigms. This leads to the realization of the property
sets such as singular subject agreement, plural subject agreement, future tense
and object agreement (singular and plural). The BLOCKS are featurally coherent
communicates tense marker. The last BLOCK C is filled with object markers. In
communicate subject agreement prefixes for 1st, 2nd and 3rd person. These affixes
occupy slot A within the verbal slots. They include e-, xu-for 1st person singular
and plural respectively. The agreement between the subject and a given paradigm
On the other hand, the 2nd person plural prefixes include o- for singular while mu-
indicates plurality in BLOCK A. Lastly, the prefix and [ba-] mark singular and
The future tense is occupied within BLOCK B and is marked using the prefix xa-.
The last BLOCK indicates object agreement. The prefixes that occupy this
BLOCK include mu- which mark 1st person (singular), 2nd person (plural) and 3rd
Remote
Remote 1st e-ndi-mu-rum-a I will send him/her
Plural gloss
The paradigms above are used to form blocks, which later generate the realization
rules. The paradigms have subject, object, tense markers and other affixal
markers for negation. The starting point for the reconstruction of Lubukusu
realization rules is to organize paradigms into immediate and remote future with
their corresponding singular and plural forms. The singular and plural negative
forms for both immediate and remote future are formed to derive the realization
rules. The morpheme marking are presented as singular subject markers, object
The singular subject markers include [e-], [o-] and [a-] while the singular object
marker is [mu-]. On the other hand, the plural subject markers are [xu-], [mu-]
and [ba-] the immediate and remote future tenses are marked by the prefix [la-]
and [li-] although this are accompanied by a high tone on the subject marker.
The rules governing the realization of immediate, remote future, subject and
object marking paradigms are derived from the verbal paradigms in remote and
The data above indicates that the rules are ordered in four rule blocks. There are
thirteen realization rules, which are ordered in BLOCKS (A-D). Each block
houses a specific affix class. In this case, if a rule applies to one block the other
rule does not apply. The affixal markers for tense are placed in BLOCKA,
Block A contains rules for tense inflection. The BLOCK A (a) introduces
immediate future while (b) marks the remote future. The last sub-category of
As can be observed from the data above, BLOCK B contains rules that introduce
prefixes, which are exponents of subject agreement. In this case (d) realizes the
first person singular through the prefix e- while (e) realizes 2nd person singular
using the affixal marker o-. The 3rd person singular is realized through the prefix
a- within (f). The plural agreement markers are realized through (g) to (i).
86
The prefix xu- indicated within the category (g) is an exponent of the 1st person
agreement. Secondly, (h) contains the prefix mu-, which marks the 2nd person
plural agreement. The last affix in BLOCK B is the 3rd person plural agreement
marker βa-.
An examination of the data above indicates that BLOCK C introduces the rules
that carry the negation markers. As stated earlier, negation is realized through the
prefixes [se-], [sa-] and [so-]. These prefixes are specific with the person and
number of the paradigm. The prefix so- is used with 2nd person singular paradigm
which is placed in BLOCK C (j). The prefix sa is attached to the 3 rd person plural
The last BLOCK (D) introduces rules that mark object agreement. Object
marking is realized through the singular marker mu- and plural marker βa-. The
former mark 1st, 2nd and 3rd person singular paradigms while the later are used to
mark plural.
The rule block interaction entails a combination of two or more blocks to realize
a paradigm.
As can be noted from the data above, the negative paradigm is defined by the
negation. Therefore, the formation of the negative paradigms utilizes the three
On the other hand, the formation of negative imperatives involves two blocks.
The BLOCKS involved in the formation of the negative imperatives are B and C.
It is noted that BLOCK B presents rules for subject agreement while BLOCK C
The imperatives are the present forms and therefore they are tonally realized.
singular plural
(plural)
3rd [saarúmá] he/she does not send [sebarúmá] they don‟t send
In the examples above, the rules presented in Block B and C realizes the
following paradigms for the negative imperatives. The particle [ta-] which is high
We now turn to the realization rules for immediate and future paradigms.
Based on the realization rules above the paradigms formed for the immediate
future and remote future are indicated below. In this case, the morphemes that
mark subjects are drawn from BLOCK B while those that realize tense are
The realization of object marking in a tensed verb entails the interaction of rules
The rules in BLOCK A entail person, while BLOCK B comprise rules that carry
tense and the last BLOCK D involve object marking rules. These morphemes
2nd olamuruma You will send him/her olaβaruma you will send him/her
3rd alimuruma he/she will send him/her βalaβaruma they will send him/her
The data above provides realization rules for a complex paradigm. The longest
paradigm that utilizes all the blocks is inclusive of the rules from all the
which mark negation, number, tense and object are always at play in the
forms the longest verb structure as presented using the rule blocks indicated
within Lubukusu.
The following is a list of paradigms that are drawn from the rules presented from
Singular gloss
Plural gloss
It is evident from the rules that the negative prefixes se-, so- and sa- are realized
through the rules in BLOCKS A (c) and BLOCK C. On the other hand, the tense
markers are presented in BLOCK (B) while the subject markers are evident in
indicated by a final particle ta which is not presented in the rule blocks and
paradigm schema.
The chapter discussed the verbal template structure of Lubukusu verbs, which
formed the starting core aspect of morphological analysis. The template provides
inflectional slots within which the inflectional morphemes are placed. The
template structure. There arrangement and slot numbers begins from the left to
the right of the template. Tense and aspect are complex categories in Lubukusu
The past and future tenses are sub-classified into intermediate, immediate and
remote past or future. The present is the only tense that does not have the sub-
categories. A tensed verb has an obligatory subject marker and non- obligatory
object marker.
comes into three sub categories, which include ongoing, past and future habitual.
On the other hand, the progressive also takes up the sub classes like immediate
It is the tone patterns and the subject markers that differentiate the ongoing and
the past habitual. The former takes a high tone on the stem‟s initial vowel while
the latter is high toned on the subject‟s vowel. Agreement as an interface between
morphology and syntax is realized through the inflectional features such as the
subject and object markers. The subject and object markers have the singular and
The starting point for discussing agreement was the presentation of tables with
subject marking, object marking and noun class system. For agreement to be
realized then the agreeing markers should belong in the same noun class. In this
case, the class one subject markers must bear agreement features of
number/person with subject markers attached on either the verb or the object.
94
prefixational rules. It is noted that the rules in the systematically organized blocks
and agreement. Additionally, the realization rules are formed by looking at the
paradigm schema of the verb „ruma‟ (send). The next chapter handles summary,
CHAPTER FIVE
5.0 Introduction
The aim of this chapter is to provide a summary from the findings, conclusion,
recommendations, and areas for further study. The study examined the order of
inflectional morphemes and lastly the rules that govern the inflectional
morphology of Lubukusu. From the objectives stated and the analysis of the data
done the summary of the findings is presented and followed up with the
conclusion, recommendations and probable areas for further study within the
The study analyzed the order of verbal morphemes on a verbal template and
morphemes are attached to the left and right of the root to realize the
morphemes are placed in specific slots on a verbal template structure. The left
side of the root takes the morphemes called prefix markers while the suffixes are
always attached to the right side of the template. The total number of inflectional
slots from the template structure is eight although not all of them can be occupied
summarized table and explanation was done on how the morphemes are attached
on selected verbs. The negation morphemes [se-], [so-] and [sa-] are specific with
person, tense or aspect but the particle ta which is high toned is applicable to all
theory. It was established that the language is agglutinating just like most Bantu
languages. Within the language, a single word may contain a series of prefixes
The tense system within the language is quite complex and provided a step
forward for study. The complexity of tense is realized through tense sub
categorization and tone patterns attached to some paradigms. The past tense and
future tense are classified into immediate, intermediate, remote tenses. On the
other hand, the future tense is also categorized similar to the past. The tense and
The study established that Lubukusu verbs distinguishes six forms of aspect with
a few having subcategories. The study noted that perfective aspect is classified
into immediate future perfective, intermediate future perfective and remote future
perfective.
97
On the other hand, habitual aspect is classified into ongoing habitual, past
habitual and future habitual. The forms of aspect that are not sub categorized are
persistive, completive and progressive aspect. Just like most Bantu languages, the
study, verbs agree with objects and subject markers in terms of nominal class,
person, number and inanimate objects. In regard to this, the study looked at
agreement in simple and negative predicates, animate and inanimate NPs and
The language is rule based as pointed out through the findings of the study. The
only rules that govern the study within the Inferential Realization theory are
inflectional category. On the other hand, the realization rules are reconstructed
based on the organization of immediate and remote future paradigms into 1st, 2nd
and 3rd person forms. It is with this arrangement of paradigms that the rules are
ordered in designated BLOCKS with each BLOCK carrying specific rules for the
5.2 Conclusion
From the findings of this study, it was realized that the verbal template of
Lubukusu is the same cross linguistically with most Bantu languages. Although
the similarity of the verbal structure exists, there are modifications that were done
to suit the structure that is evident in the language hence serving as a blue print
for other researchers. The study reaffirmed that the basic verb can play the role of
a simple sentence. It is also worth noting that the verb that has a series of
inflectional morphemes can stand out as a complex sentence. In light of this, the
longest verb takes at most seven inflectional morphemes attached to the left and
The study confirmed that the basis of a verbal inflectional study is the
slots on the template structure are numbered from the left to the right. It is with
this knowledge that the template structure was studied to serve as a basis for
morphology of the language. It was evident that the paradigms derive their
structure from the template structure and this provided a base for studying the
Bantu languages. The language is agglutinating despite making use of high tones
It thus utilizes tone patterns on some tense, aspect and person morphemes. It is
agglutinating because of the inflectional morphemes that are attached on the right
and the left of the root. The study concluded that any study on verbal morphology
markers and noun class markers indicated on verbs and verbs within sentences.
This interaction is somewhat partly skewed to inflectional morphemes and not the
entire word or sentence. It is with this biaseness towards the morphemes that a
It is evident that the rule based study within Inferential Realization is governed
by prefixational rules and realization rules. The former are reconstructed based
on the slot analysis of the inflectional morphemes while the latter are generated
from the paradigm schema of a given verb. These two sets of rules are core in the
5.3 Recommendations
From the findings of this study, it was discovered that the findings on Lubukusu
verbal morphology has closer connections with most Bantu languages. Since
studies on Bantu languages are interrelated, the researcher recommends that close
a blue print for building a scholarly research especially for those who intend to
During the study, it was realized that literature on Lubukusu especially on verbal
morphology was little and the ones available was unpublished. I further
This study has only focused on inflectional structure of Lubukusu within the
conducted within Minimalist Program, which handles tense and aspect and the
patterns with tense and aspect. This calls for a study of negation in tensed verbs
and aspect.
101
language to test its efficacy and whether the dialects share the same verbal
Future studies can also focus on the interaction and realization of tense through
5.5 Summary
REFERENCES
Arnoff, M. & Fuderman, K. (2011). What is morphology? (2nd ed). West Sussex:
Wiley Blackwell.
Bowern, C. (2008). Linguistic field work. A practical guide. New York, NY:
Palgrave Macmillan.
Bryman, A. (2008). Social research methods. (3rd ed) Oxford: Oxford University
Press.
Chomsky, N. (1986). Knowledge of language and its nature, origin and use. New
York, NY: Praeger.
Ebarb, K. (2007). Tone and variation in Idakho and other Luhya varieties, PhD
dissertation, Indiana University.
Heine, B. &. Wilhem, M. (1980). Language and Dialects Atlas of Kenya. Vol 1.
Berlin: Diertrich Reimer.
Kenya National Bureau of Statistics. (2010). 2009 Kenya population and housing
census Volume 1.
Lewis, M. (Ed.) (2009). Ethnologue: language of the world, 16th ed. SIL
International Dallas. Online version: http://www.ethnologue.com/.
Nida, E. (1946). Morphology. The descriptive analysis of words. (2nd ed). New
York, NY: The University of Michigan Press.
Nikki, A. (2010). The Zulu ditranstive verb phrase. PhD thesis, University of
Chicago.
Nurse, D. (2008). Tense and aspect in Bantu. New York: Oxford University
Press.
Odden, D. (2013). Introducing phonology. (2nd ed). New York, NY: Cambridge
University Press.
APPENDICES
Na-pil-e orio e-ŋga-li xu-mi-lim-o kino . kamaxuwa niko xut∫-a xu-ka-t∫ul-a ano
I appreciate the support you have given me concerning this work. I promise you
that whatever we are going to discuss here will be used purposely for my research
only.
Xu-xw-a-lix-a/xu-bey-i-sya(married)……………………………………………...
ki-mi-limo (Profession)…………………………………………………………….
We-sis-a-ni/we-six-a-si male/female………………………………………………
A. Data on tense
nom-a
e-ndien-a?
how if I don‟t want to cut the tree a few hours to come, what do I say?
How if I had cut the tree five days ago, what do I say?
110
If you think I did not cut the tree today what do I say?
How if it happens that I did not cut the tree a few minutes ago , what do I say?
F. Data on subjunctive
22. Omwana omukekhe awele Nekesa sikono (the small child gave Nekesa a
basket)
23. Omukhana omukesi achile engo (the clever child went home)
24. Omusoleli omuleyi abukule sitabu (the big boy picked the book)
30. Engokho nende epusi chitibile (the hen and the cat ran)
31. Omwana akhasitabule sitabu (the child will tear the book)
34. Makasi awele lukorito liembe (Makasi gave Lukorito the Mango)
35. Omusoleli akulile nanjala kumukoye (the boy bought for Nanjala the rop)
36. Maumo akhalile embwa kumukhinga (Maumo cut the dog‟s tail)
37. Nandutu awele Nanyama kamabele (Nandutu gave Nanyama the milk)