0% found this document useful (0 votes)
12 views16 pages

The Influence of Price On Purchase Intentions Comp

Uploaded by

Kusuma Aji28
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
0% found this document useful (0 votes)
12 views16 pages

The Influence of Price On Purchase Intentions Comp

Uploaded by

Kusuma Aji28
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
You are on page 1/ 16

Article

The Influence of Price on Purchase Intentions: Comparative


Study between Cognitive, Sensory,
and Neurophysiological Experiments
Gabriel R. D. Levrini 1,* and Mirela Jeffman dos Santos 2

1 Marketing Department, Escola Superior Propaganda e Marketing, São Paulo 04018-010, Brazil
2 Programa de Pós-graduação Scricto Senso em Ensino de Ciências e Matemática,
Department of Administrative Sciences, Universidade de Caxias do Sul–UCS,
Caxias do Sul 95070-560, Brazil; mirelajs@gmail.com
* Correspondence: levrini@terra.com.br

Abstract: Price is considered one of the most important attributes in consumer’s choice. On the other
hand, consumer’s knowledge about price tends to be imprecise. This study aims at providing new
insights analyzing consumers’ perception of retail store brand (focused on Skin Care Products) com-
paring with two other skin care products, a premium, and a popular national brand. Second, to
analyze the association price versus quality variables in the purchasing decision process. Third, to
demonstrate the influence of both, unconscious and cognitive process during the purchase choice
intention. Through Neuromarketing tools and protocols (quantitative and qualitative), the study
exposes participants to a blind test of the three products and asks participants to talk about their
sensory impressions like scent, feelings, and products texture. Using facial electromyography
(EMG) and eye-tracker devices we measured consumers’ responses when we introduced price and
brand name variables, by this way comparing unconscious and cognitive responses. The findings
showed that an unconscious decision could be change when new variables were revealed. The study
showed how conscious price variable was the major influence in their purchase intention.
Citation: Levrini, G.R.D. Santos, M.
J. The Influence of Price on Purchase
Intentions: Comparative Study
Keywords: store brands; consumer perception; brand recognition; eye-tracker; facial reader
Between Cognitive, Sensory, and
Neurophysiological Experiments.
Behav. Sci. 2021, 11, 16.
https://doi.org/10.3390/bs11020016 1. Introduction
Traditionally, retail seeks differentiation by offering unique products and services or
Received: 15 November 2020
even new business formats. However, as suppliers could be the same for all retailers and
Accepted: 4 January 2021
business formats could be copied, it is necessary that retailers identify new ways of dif-
Published: 25 January 2021
ferentiating themselves from competitors. Consumers have impressions and images
about the brands and these impressions have a great impact on the choice of stores for
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neu-
tral with regard to jurisdictional
purchases and in the same way influence the purchasing behavior of these consumers [1]
claims in published maps and insti-
One of retail’s strategy differentiation is the development of store brands [2]. Literature
tutional affiliations. asserts that Store Brands (SBs) could be understood as assign the store name to the prod-
ucts [3]. In addition, Calvo-Porral and Levy-Mangin (2016) [4] indicated that SBs are re-
stricted to the store chain and linked to it in a single way, especially if the brand name is
the same.
Copyright: © 2021 by the authors. Several authors [5] pointed out that when customers have a favorable retailer’s brand
Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. image and trust it, they tend to transfer these attributes to SBs. In this way, SBs image can
This article is an open access article be understood as a set of features like store name, store services, prices, quality of mer-
distributed under the terms and con- chandise, and knowledgeable salespeople [5,6] that meet consumers evaluation of the
ditions of the Creative Commons At- store, and, consequently, its SBs.
tribution (CC BY) license (http://cre- In the 1970s, SBs were considered low-priced products, with inferior quality, non-
ativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/). specialized suppliers, and basic products priced at least 20% lower than the leading

Behav. Sci. 2021, 11, 16. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs11020016 www.mdpi.com/journal/behavsci


Behav. Sci. 2021, 11, 16 2 of 16

brands. Poor quality and price-based competition are characteristics of the first generation
of store brands [7]. From the second SBs generation on, the concern in terms of quality
levels began, although the focus remains on lower prices than those of competitors. Prod-
ucts started to have retail-related brands, and the price level remains below the category
leaders (between 10 and 20% lower). With the third SBs generation, both the quality and
the price level of SBs are close to the leading market national brand. In this phase, the
strategy for adopting own brands was to follow the leaders (me-too). The competition
starts to take place in terms of value, price, and quality. We are in the fourth SBs genera-
tion, where the retail SBs strategy is to offer value-added products, differentiated from
competitors, developed with innovative technology. In this generation, SBs have the same
(or even superior) image and quality as those of leading national brands but with a seg-
mented offering [8].
The retail landscape has changed considerably in recent decades. Globally, it is esti-
mated that totally SBs represent close to 15% of the total market. According to Nielsen
Report [9], SBs growth has been higher in recent years almost globally, from 2015 to 2019
was 3.7% (year ago) and CAGR 2.5% (4 years), against 1.9% (year ago) and CAGR (4 years)
1% from branded products. For that reason, SBs are being called future “brand killers” or
“Brand Disruption next phase” [10].
This research was delimited within SBs universe: we select to study Beauty and Per-
sonal Care category, in particular Skin Care products that had one of the highest growth
rates under this scenario.
The justification for this choice is the past and projected growth of this category. The
projected growth to Beauty and Personal Care products is 7.2% globally (CAGR 2019–
2024) specifically in Asian and emerging markets, like Brazil where Beauty and Personal
Care category increased 19.8% (from 2016 to 2018) and within these category, Skin Care
products grew 101% (from 2016 to 2018) [11].
We are seeing an emerging rise of premium SBs products and it is reshaping retail
strategies across the retail landscape. Instead of price, retailers’ current focus is on having
SBs with quality equal to or better than national brands [12]. As perceived quality is gen-
erally seen as opposed to the price function [13], the evolution of SBs in quality products
changed this relationship, varying perceptions of price and value.
This exploratory study proposes the following research questions:
- Do store brands (SBs) skin care products, meet the consumers expectations regarding
price and quality association compared with premiums brands and popular brands?
- Will neurophysiological responses vary when compared with conscious actions in
this context?
This research aims to analyze how prices influenced the purchase intentions compar-
ing cognitive process, sensorial, and neurophysiological results.
First, we investigate sensorial perceptions and later the brand image perceptions an
important process as consumers use images or previous experiences in their purchase
process. We explore the concept of SBs price image and price consciousness, which con-
siders the multidimensionality of perceptions about products [14].

2. Background
The gap between industry and retail is being reduced, either by new forms of con-
sumption and new customers (millennials generation) or new technologies, new trends,
and changes in markets dynamics. Loyalty had always been a treasured commodity for
companies but now consumers have endless choice and omni channel access, their disloy-
alty, or brand switching, should worry manufacturers and retailers [15].

2.1. Price
Price is undoubtedly one of the most influential factors in the perceptions of products
on the market. According to Beneke et al. (2015) [16], price is a real clue that consumers
Behav. Sci. 2021, 11, 16 3 of 16

use in their purchasing decisions. The price variable is always present in the daily pur-
chase and represents the value of the economic expenditure Zeithmal (1988) [17], refers as
the sacrifice value) that must be given up by consumers to carry out a certain purchase
transaction. Considering that the price represents “a sacrifice”, this variable has a contrary
position to the purchase intention where generally the higher prices, the lower purchase
possibilities. Although, in some cases, researchers like Nevin e Houston (1980) [18], Mitch-
ell (2010) [19], and Kara, et al. (2009) [20] explained that consumers do not always see price
in a negative way, which represents economic expenses, instead it is a complex variable
on purchase decision. They suggest that price could be a signal of product quality and
represents elegance and status. These price role perceptions, negative or positive, give rise
to consumer’s price image [21].
The price image comprises a subjective and multidimensional concept, which in-
volves emotional factors associated to products or services [14]. Thus, consumers percep-
tion about price is built through a complex process and does not necessarily reflect the
real brand or product price. Therefore, the gap between the true price and the consumers’
perceptions about price can be large. The price can have a greater impact on purchase
intentions when other extrinsic tips such as the brand-value (or product-value) or intrinsic
tips related to the physical properties of the object are not clearly defined. Well-known
brands, such as a strong extrinsic tip, have a symbolic level that positively affects their
perception of value. Thus, when the brand name gives evidence of the company's reputa-
tion, the consumer may prefer to use the brand to infer quality and value and conse-
quently the purchase instead of the price [21].
There are two vital concepts when we think about the price image formation:
- Value consciousness: means the consumer evaluation in a purchase decision in which
price paid is compared to the benefits received with the product or service [17].
- Price consciousness, which is the price perception for some consumers.
However, the term “price consciousness” has been broadly used to refer to consumer
perception about price [17]; we use the term to refer to how much interest consumers have
in saving money and, consequently, paying lower prices. Several researchers [22–24] pre-
sented the price awareness associated to the priority consumers attribute to pay low price,
instead of other aspects like quality, design, style, and so on.

2.2. Prices, Brands, and Perceived-Value


The brand, then, is the tangible representation of a set of meanings, values, attributes,
and experiences related to a product or service recognized and represented by a name,
logo, and visual support language that defines its identity in a complete way [12]. Accord-
ing to Aaker (2002) [7], the brand must be a symbol, from which consumers perceived-
value and particular attributes, which carries peculiar characteristics and makes the link
between the individual and the brand.
The perceived-value means the balance between perceived benefits from the product
compared to the sacrifices (financial, physical, time, and psychic) [17,20]. Such perception,
then, involves an exchange between what the customer takes (e.g., level of quality, joy,
well-being) and what he gives to buy, use, or consume the product (e.g., store services,
product price, waiting lines, and other emotional feelings). This value evaluation can be
understood as a continuum, which goes from a perception of the price as a simple mone-
tary value paid to a complex process of choice [14].
The product value awareness influences the consumer's purchase decision, since they
use price information as a unit of measurement of value, although partial, to make their
mental processing of choice [16].
In this context, the brand and the price level play a fundamental role in the quality
evaluation and serve as heuristic hints. According to Vahie and Paswan (2006) [25], heu-
ristic hints are generally used as a kind of mental shortcut to simplify the evaluation of an
Behav. Sci. 2021, 11, 16 4 of 16

object or an event (experience), when individuals are not motivated to process infor-
mation or when decisions are less important. In fact, research shows that when there are
no other quality tips or inferences, the consumer has used the price and the brand to infer
the quality of a product.
Previous studies which investigated the reasons store brands are successful (for ex-
ample, [26,27] explained that similar quality of national brands and SBs is a decisive factor
for consumers’ choice, especially if national brands had a higher price than SBs. In other
words, it implies that the success of SBs depends not only on price but also on product
quality [28]. The quality gap between national brands, premiums brands, and SBs has
been decreasing (reflected by the growing of market share), the more consumers see value
on a product, the greater the purchase intention [26].

2.3. Theory
The association between quality and price reflects the general belief of the consumer
that the price level positively influences the level of product quality. While some authors
argue that this association of quality and price varies between different categories [29],
other authors [30,31], following Attribution Theory, argue that this association is product
specific.
The Attribution Theory [31,32] explains that the low price of certain SBs products can
lead consumers to attribute it to some dubious characteristic of the product, perceiving
them as being of inferior quality. Thanasuta (2015) [33] argued that the more aware con-
sumers are about price, the more engaged they are in buying SBs products. Santos et al.
(2016) [34] pointed out that, considering the same product category, consumers are more
likely to buy low price SB products instead of national brands offering higher prices. So,
the combination of high quality and low-price tends to excite and attract consumers to
buy SBs [6,21].
In their pioneering studies, Monroe and Krishnan (1985) [35] investigated the rela-
tionships among objective price, perception of price, perceived quality, perceived sacri-
fice, perceived-value, and willingness to pay. They proposed that perception of price ra-
ther than objective price positively affects consumers’ perceived quality and perceived
sacrifice through the mediator of price perception (Figure 1).

Figure 1. Price-value model proposed by Monroe and Krishman (1985) [35].

However, Kwon (1990) [26] studied the influence of buying experience at the store
on consumers’ perception of price, quality, and value. The results showed that store-shop-
ping experience has a greater impact in consumers’ value perceptions of a retail store
when compared to price or quality perceptions. Their findings confirmed the existence of
nonproduct-related intrinsic attributes (e.g., consumers’ perceived store shopping experi-
ence), which may affect perceived-value (e.g., consumers' perceptions of store value).
Behav. Sci. 2021, 11, 16 5 of 16

2.4. Consumers' Perceptions of Price


Olson and Jacoby (1974) [36] and Jacoby, Olson and Haddock (1971) [37], in their first
studies, reported that external stimuli do not directly affect behavior exercising only indi-
rect effects. Stimuli must first be perceived and then decoded and interpreted before af-
fecting behavior and decision-making. The perceived discount is defined as the coded
discount through a subjective interpretation process that assigns meaning to the objective
price or be the discounted price [4,36].
Following Jacoby and Olson (1977) [38] and Jacoby, Olson and Haddock (1971) [37]
definitions of objective prices (which means the real price of a good) and perceived price
(that is, the price consumers encode in their minds), [17] defined the perceived price, as
the sacrifice to obtain a product. By this logic, the perceived price of consumers will be
affected by the perceived discounts and in the same way by the perceived quality and the
price image influenced by the brands power. The concept of perceived sacrifice, instead
of perceived price, directly enters the process of evaluating consumers because the per-
ceived-value is conceptualized as the compensation between perceived quality and per-
ceived sacrifice [17,39]. Lindquist (1974) [40] argues that price image perceptions are in-
fluenced by the retailer's store image and thus the store's image would affect SBs. Some
authors, however, do not agree with this explanation, arguing that price and store image
are different concepts [14].
The store's price image is defined as “a global representation of the relative price
level” [17,39]. In this way, SBs price-image can be associated to costume price practiced
by the retailer, according to its pricing strategies and positioning in the market. However,
some authors identified many dimensions to the concept—like Zielke (2010) [14] and
Lichtensteinetal (1993) [22]—considering that the price occupies a salient place in the con-
sumer buying behavior process, especially for SBs.
In this regard, SBs price characteristics (for example, price awareness and value
awareness) have been a discussion target of several researchers. SBs’ price image become
a reference to consumers when purchasing a SB product. Retailers usually offer many
types of SB products, such as luxury SBs, average SBs, economic SBs, and so on, varying
quality and price [41,42].
We can infer that relying on the price image is more convenient for consumers than
on any other price-related construct. Preceding studies showed that the SB image is a cen-
tral element in development of store’s image [16,25,43]. Therefore, based on this discus-
sion, we expect consumers to be influenced by the SB’s price image in their SB purchase
decision making, since customer perceived-value means the balance between benefits re-
ceived by the customer and costs paid by customer [42,44].

3. Material and Methods


The experimental sample was composed by three brands: store brand (S2), a pre-
mium brand (S3), and a national brand (S1). This choice aimed to compare consumers’
perceptions among different levels of quality, price, attributes, and perceived-value. S2
skin care was chosen as the SB, which was developed by the retailer and sought similar
sensory attributes of a premium brand (S3) mainly in texture and aroma. To be consistent,
we used S3 skin care as our premium sample and S1 skin care as the national brand sam-
ple. S1 is a very popular national brand in Brazil. All the experiments were done in Porto
Alegre city, Brazil between August and October 2019.

3.1. Research Design


We divided the field research into two experimental stages.
The first stage was conducted by an exploratory qualitative research, using think-
aloud protocols. At this time, participants were invited to try three skincare samples (Store
Brand, National Brand and Premium Brand) in a blind test. During the test, participants
were asked to say whatever comes into their mind as they complete their task. A total of
Behav. Sci. 2021, 11, 16 6 of 16

80 participants of our target group (women between 25 and 40 years old) participated in
the experiment. Participants, all women, were recruited in their homes, as they all live in
two populous condominiums in the same city and the neighborhood all middle-class so-
cial demographic segment. We invited them to participate and there was no payment; in
fact, they enjoyed testing skin care products. We explained to the participants a general
view of the experiment but not details in order not to jeopardize the experiment.
At the end of the final session (5th), we invited them to a closing cocktail to celebrate
the end of the experiment that took place when we reached our goal of 80 participants for
establishing us in the design of the experiment.
The blind test was done individually in 5 sessions, in a control environment. During
the individual experiment, we interviewed all the sample group recording their experi-
ence, utilizing the Talking Aloud protocols. Participants explained their experience and
sensorial feelings.
Seeing that consumer purchase decision process is grounded by quality and price
perceived, and that perceived-value is understood as the evaluation a consumer does
about the benefits received and the costs paid for the product or service [17], at the end of
each stage a basic question was asked: which product would you buy?
The second stage was conducted with neurophysiological devices. The sample group
were exposed to three products (premium, store brand, and national). We used eye tracker
and facial electromyography (EMG) devices aiming to measure consumers’ emotional re-
sponses and to compare perceptions. While participants were looking at products labels,
their eyes movements were registered at 60 Hz through the eye tracker equipment, which
was integrated with the screen on which products were presented. The duration of each
trial participant took on average 2 min as the three slides changed automatically every 20
s. Participants sat on a chair, which was at 65 cm from the screen and received the instruc-
tion to move as little as possible. Before starting each task, participants followed calibra-
tion procedure of Tobii Studio Professional version.
Finally, results included quantitative data, experimental setup, physiological and be-
havioral data collection, and statistical procedures.

3.2. Materials
For the experiment application, we used three skin care products that we maintain
the anonymity of the brands: sample 1 (S1) a national brand, medium quality, low price,
Sample 2 (S2) SBs of a large drugstore, with good quality, medium price, and sample 3
(S3) a premium brand with high quality and higher price among the samples.
For neurophysiological measurements, a biometric Imotions Platform was used,
which has eye-tracking equipment Tobii 4.0 60 Hz, which monitors and describes the oc-
ular movement of people, indicating which stimuli attract the most attention of an indi-
vidual and providing the path that the participant’s eyes take when facing these stimuli,
being therefore ideal for this study Eye tracking equipment is largely used in social science
researches, because it offers data about individuals’ eyes fixation and visualization, which
let researchers deeply analyze people’s behavior [45,46].
The Tobii device uses infrared light to illuminate the eyes of the study participants;
the infrared light hits the user’s eyes and several sensors on the monitor capture them,
allowing the software to interpolate the positioning of the eyes [47]. According to the au-
thors, Tobii allows identifying with excellent accuracy where users are looking. The mon-
itor is very similar to that of a common computer not necessarily being any type of addi-
tional apparatus coupled to the heads of the participants.
The facial EMG is an exact evaluation of facial muscle activity. This technique al-
lowed analyzing spontaneous facial expressions that are deep emotions indicators.
To operationalize this procedure, we fixed sensors over defined individuals’ facial
muscle, as shown in Figure 2, and observed muscle contraction through electrical current.
This technique was already used to test individuals’ perception of television advertise-
Behav. Sci. 2021, 11, 16 7 of 16

ments [48], radio advertisements [49], computer interfaces, new products [50], and inter-
net [51]. Sensory inputs, such as those from pictures, slides, or videos, can be conducted
straight to the amygdala (within limbic system) and to neocortex areas, where thought
actions occur. According to previous research in neuromarketing area, emotional reac-
tions are associated to unconscious brain zone when exposed to stimulus like advertise-
ments or products pictures or brands films [52].
For current experiment, we considered two facial muscles: smile muscle—whose
technical name is zygomatic major, representing a pleasant emotion—and frown—whose
technical name is corrugator supercilii, representing an unpleasant emotion. Following
Peacock, Purvis, and Hazlett (2011) [53] research, the activation was measured individu-
ally, because they are an evaluative indicative process, which had independent motivators
of consumer behavior [54]. Positive activations influenced positive feelings, while Nega-
tive activation may expose negative emotion of viewer experiences.

Figure 2. Placement of the electrodes. Source: Cacioppo, et al. (1999) [48].

Each participant sat in front of the equipment (Acer notebook with 32 Gg RAM, 2TB
HD). At this moment, aspects such as distance between participant and monitor, height
of the chair, and positioning of the person and environmental control variables was veri-
fied.
The visual stimuli chosen was a set of three slides (A, B, C) that was exposed for 20 s
each. Slide A (Figure 3) showed three transparent tubes containing the creams, only with
different caps colors. Slide B (Figure 4) was the same three previous tubes, however, at
this moment revealing market common prices (but without identifying the brand names);
here, we seek to verify the physiological reactions when revealing the variable price. On
slide C (Figure 5), we presented the three products with their respective attributes and
prices, seeking insights about the perceived risk in relation to SBs.

Figure 3. Slide A—Blind test transparent tubes. Source: the authors (2020).
Behav. Sci. 2021, 11, 16 8 of 16

Figure 4. Slide B—Transparent tubes with prices. Source: the authors (2020).

At the end, we showed the last slide (C) where we introduced our coding (attributes
specification) as S1 (pink cap), S2 (blue cap), S3 (white cap) (attributes) giving to the par-
ticipants all the information about the products (excluding the brand name)

4. Results
4.1. First Stage Blind Test: Think Aloud Protocols
On first stage, basically, we explored only sensorial responses like smell, texture, and
color of anonymous products (transparent tubes). In the blind test, we presented the three
transparent tubes and invited the participants to try the creams, feel its texture and aroma,
and say whatever comes into their minds during the experience. This procedure is named
“Think-aloud” protocols and it involves asking people to use some product and tell about
their experience, including feelings, sensations, impressions, and so on. Participants’
speeches were recorded for further analysis. This method is recommended because of the
relaxed atmosphere it provides, which allows people to talk openly and informally about
the products, being more sincere and faithful [55].
At the end, we asked the 80 participants which was the selected product and buying
option. Twenty-five participants chose S2, 40 selected S3 and 15 elected S1, as summarized
below (Box 1).

Box 1. Purchase intention.

S2 (25/80) S3 (40/80) S1 (15/80)

4.2. Second Stage: Neurophysiological Experiment


After the blind test, we submitted individually the participants to biometric devices.
We presented the Slide A (they already known the transparent tubes from the blind test),
and later we showed B and C slides, using eye-tracker and facial reader to understand
their responses. Eye tracking’s objective is to identify areas of interest (AOI) and to meas-
ure for how long people fix their attention on specific areas. In this research, this method-
ology was used to analyze how people pay attention to prices and brand names. Eye-
tracking devices help the researcher to determine what objects or parts of objects are being
perceived and how they influence evaluation [56]. In our study, we defined specific de-
tection parameters:
• Time to First Fixation (TTFF): It showed which areas were firstly looked by the par-
ticipants and for how long the eyes were fixed on each AOI.
• Fixation: It identified people’s interests on slide or environment.
• Refixations: It measured how many times the participant looked at the same AOI. It
is an important piece of data because it allows one to identify which areas call more
attention from the participants (for better or worse) [57].
Behav. Sci. 2021, 11, 16 9 of 16

• Area of Interest (AOI): Consists in dividing the display stimulus in selected areas and
analyzing data for the specific regions. In this study, AOIs were defined as shown in
Figure 5.

AOI 1 (transparent tube with white color cap corresponding to S3 skin care)
AOI2 (price of S3 skin care)
AOI 3 (transparent tube with blue color cap corresponding to S2 skin care)
AOI 4 (price of S2 skin care)
AOI 5 (transparent tube with pink color cap corresponding to S1 skin care)
AOI 6 (price of S1 skin care)
AOI 7 (S3 skin care picture)
AOI 8 (S3 skin care picture with price)
AOI 9 (S2 skin care picture)
AOI 10 (S2 skin care picture with price)
AOI 11 (S1 skin care picture)
AOI 12 (S1 skin care picture with price).

Figure 5. Areas of interest (AOIs) definitions. Source: the authors (2020).

In slide B price figures were shown to analyze possible price versus quality (influence
from the blind test) (AOIs 2, 4, and 6). We aimed in Tables 1–3 to measure mainly price
effect for S3, S2, and S1 as we showed only the transparent tubes and prices as the follow-
ing:

Table 1. AOI 2 ANOVA test S3 Price.

χ2 σ df F Sig.
TTFF
Within AOI 14,775 1.934 1 3.872 0.13
Between AOIs 390,625 0.878 79 100,875 0.01 **
FIXATION
Within AOI 133,112 0.899 1 1,767 0.08
Between AOIs 462,776 0.799 79 122,443 0.05 *
RE-FIXATION
Within AOI 189,332 1.221 1 3,224 0.05 *
Between AOIs 473,223 2.331 79 179,998 0.01 **
** significative at 99%, *significative at 95%.

Table 2. AOI 4 ANOVA test S2 Price.

χ2 σ df F Sig.
TTFF
Within AOI 13,891 0.891 1 2 0.11
Between AOIs 398,222 1.12 79 98,221 0.05 *
FIXATION
Within AOI 113,987 0.781 1 4332 0.01 **
Between AOIs 477,654 0.886 79 154,222 0.01 **
RE-FIXATION
Within AOI 197,334 0.88 1 5233 0.01 **
Between AOIs 489,011 0.79 79 165,112 0.01 **
** significative at 99%, *significative at 95%.
Behav. Sci. 2021, 11, 16 10 of 16

Table 3. AOI 6 ANOVA test S1 Price.

χ2 σ df F Sig.
TTFF
Within AOI 10,322 0.81 1 2 0.16
Between AOIs 287,009 0.98 79 121,223 0.05 *
FIXATION
Within AOI 100,233 1.12 1 3887 0.05 *
Between AOIs 336,887 1.05 79 158,998 0.01 **
RE-FIXATION
Within AOI 119,02 0.776 1 4112 0.01 **
Between AOIs 244,898 0.887 79 182,998 0.01 **
** significative at 99%, *significative at 95%. Source: the authors (2020).

We found significant difference in almost all AOIs where prices were displayed, in
fixation and refixation. In other words, it was no surprise that the premium brand, S3, was
selected by most of participants, when it was anonymously presented in the blind test;
however, when prices were revealed, participants purchase intention changed. When the
price was showed, 19 participants chose S2, 16 selected S3, and 45 elected S1, as summa-
rized below (Box 2).

Box 2. Purchase Intention (with Price).

S2 (19/80) S3 (16/80) S1 (45/80)

In Tables 4–6 we showed ANOVA test for Slide C when we revealed our coding (at-
tributes) and prices.

Table 4. AOI 8 ANOVA test S3 Price.

χ2 σ df F Sig.
TTFF
Within AOI 19,729 0.998 1 3 0.13
Between AOIs 338,983 0.789 79 121,223 0.05 *
FIXATION
Within AOI 34,509 0.99 1 4334 0.05 *
Between AOIs 499,221 1.03 79 182,443 0.01 **
RE-FIXATION
Within AOI 16,112 1.08 1 1223 0.12
Between AOIs 334,224 1.34 79 168,999 0.01 **
** significative at 99%, *significative at 95%.

Table 5. AOI 10 ANOVA test S2 Price.

χ2 σ df F Sig.
TTFF
Within AOI 98,781 1.02 1 1 0.15
Between AOIs 135,788 0.98 79 118,333 0.05 *
FIXATION
Within AOI 128,881 0.94 1 3887 0.05 *
Between AOIs 497,344 1.02 79 202,11 0.05 *
RE-FIXATION
Within AOI 212,371 0.96 1 3566 0.11
Between AOIs 398,221 0.97 79 164,22 0.05 *
*significative at 95%.
Behav. Sci. 2021, 11, 16 11 of 16

Table 6. AOI 12 ANOVA test S1 Price.

χ2 σ df F Sig.
TTFF
Within AOI 21,233 1.12 1 2 0.18
Between AOIs 366,221 1.05 79 176,334 0.05 *
FIXATION
Within AOI 98,887 1.02 1 3455 0.05 *
Between AOIs 287,478 0.95 79 188,322 0.01 **
RE-FIXATION
Within AOI 103,12 1.02 1 1443 0.17
Between AOIs 221,223 0.99 79 3556 0.05 *
** significative at 99%, *significative at 95%. Source: the authors (2020).

We found significant difference between AOIs in almost all parameters measured.


When coding was revealed, we expect some attributes’ influence reflecting in decision
process and perceived-value. However, only four participants changed in favor of S3 and
S1 national brand lost 14 participants. S2 increased the purchase intention in 10 partici-
pants, as summarized below (Box 3).

Box 3. Purchase intention (with attributes).

S2 (29/80) S3 (20/80) S1 (31/80)

4.3. EMG Results


Facial EMG data were analyzed by SPSS 20.0 software. Frown muscle registered
more activation on Slide B (M = 345.21%), which reveled products’ prices. In sequence,
Slide C (M = 268.01%), which showed products’ prices and attributes. The last one was the
Slide A (M = 98.32%), which presented the transparent tubes already known for the par-
ticipants. The differences among three slides were significative, in terms of their EMG
mean, F(2, 64) = 14.26, p < 0.001, n2 = 0.31.
Post hoc tests using the Bonferroni correction, demonstrated that EMG mean re-
sponses to Slide B were significantly higher, than those for Slide C, which in turn, were
significantly greater than for Slide A. Through ANOVA test, we found significant differ-
ence for the negative emotion measure between Slide A (without prices) and Slide B (with
prices) (F(4, 10) = 2,98, p=0.05) and also between Slide A and Slide C (F(1, 32) = 4.76, p =
0.03).
We did not find significant differences of positive emotion between Slide B (prices)
and Slide C (attributes) (F(1, 31) = 2.23, p = 0.13), neither for Slide A nor C (F(1, 29) = 2,01,
p = 0,18). Price variable reveled higher negative impression than attributes. These findings
suggest, as we expected, that Slide B, showing prices, evoked more unpleasant emotions,
followed by Slide C and Slide A.

5. Conclusions
In recent years, retail store brands (SBs) sales have grown faster than traditional
brands, and in some cases have become market leaders (some later called “the future
brand killers”). As an example, and focus of our research, we have Beauty and Personal
Care category that remains one of the fastest growing categories. The rapid evolution of
SBs happened mainly because retailers focused on product quality, without giving up the
lower price characteristics of SBs. To reinforce this paradigm, in our research, we used a
retail SB (S2 skin care) that was in its development, inspired in a premium brand (S3 skin
care) both compared to a national brand (S1 skin care).
This exploratory study aimed to analyze how prices influenced the purchase inten-
tions comparing cognitive process, sensorial, and neurophysiological results.
Behav. Sci. 2021, 11, 16 12 of 16

Our research had two stages: the first was a blind test (looking for perceptions and
sensorial feelings) and the second stage used neurophysiological tools and cognitive re-
sponses.
This exploratory study proposes the following research questions:
- Do store brands (SBs) skin care products meet the consumers expectations regarding
price and quality association compared with premium brands and national brands?
Consumer perception is an interaction of the characteristics (attributes) inherent to
the product, the socioeconomic conditions, and physiological and cognitive influences.
When the consumer is in the market, the purchase decision is influenced by the brand (or
attributes) and it influences the evaluation response sensory. Prior study focused on the
effect of the brand, by the price, product appearance, and sociocultural or general prefer-
ence that could affect the sensory evaluation [58].
In the experimental blind test procedure, consumers hedonically tested the products
and were invited to express their purchase intention. The consumers’ hedonic evaluation
changed significantly from the first slide A (Blind test without revealing brand names)
and the second slide B (Blind plus price). First, premium brand S3 was selected by 50% of
participants, followed by the good quality S2 (31.25%) and national brand S1 (18.75%).
This result is coherent with products quality. Sample S3 is considered the best skincare
cream from the three samples presented. However, S1 is the simplest product, while S2 is
a good quality product. So, the blind test revealed an expected consumers perception.
After revealing prices, in Slide B, the key question “which product would you buy?”
was asked again and participants changed their option and selected national brand S1
(56%), they showed an increase option towards S2 (24%) and a decrease option for S3
(20%). At this point, we can see price influence over the purchase decision overlapping
the previous sensorial selection. These results are aligned with Stefani, Romano, and
Cavicchi (2006)’s previous research [59]. Furthermore, this result showed that partici-
pants, although preferred S3, are not willing to pay for it. In addition, their perceptions
about the three creams on first stage were favorable, that is, participants enjoyed the three
products and they chose the cheapest one.
These results showed that the disclosure of the attributes allowed participants to an-
alyze the quality and the relevance of the products quickly and briefly [58]. When Slide C
was showed and, again, we asked the key question, purchase option again slightly
changed but at least showing that there exists an attributes’ influence and consciousness
on consumer reply. When attributes are acknowledged, it generates an influence on the
sensory acceptance of the product, that is, the consumer increases or decreases its valua-
tion in relation to the knowledge or price positioning in the market. The final selection,
after all the information revealed, showed the following purchase option: S1 decreased to
38.75%, S2 increased to 36.25% and S3 also increased to 25%. The results showed that con-
sumers have expectations for certain attributes and in them generates different effects. In
addition, this result suggests that consumers accept paying more for a skin care product
since they know their complete attributes.
Consumers perceive high-quality products, generally founded as more expensive
than medium or low-quality ones [60]. In our experiment it was possible to see purchase
intention change towards a cheaper product (and against the blind test sensory choices)
when prices were revealed, but later, when all the information was given, that is, the sam-
ple could be associated with price and product attributes, the purchase options changed
again. Product price, however, had the major factor influencing in selecting the purchase
intention. This distribution shows different consumers profiles: who values quality and
attributes, who values low price, and the ones who value a balance between quality and
price.
- Will neurophysiological responses vary when compared with conscious actions in
this context?
Behav. Sci. 2021, 11, 16 13 of 16

In our research, it was clear the differences between the sensorial responses and the
cognitive actions (especially when prices are disclosed). Sensorial responses suggested
consumers’ preference for premium brand, which indicated that smell, texture, and color
of this product were considered better than the others. In this way, participants had better
sensations when they tried S3 skin care cream. However, when prices were reveled, par-
ticipants made their purchase decision in a rational way and had cognitive actions, that
way many of them changed their minds. In this way, neurophysiological responses were
different from conscious actions.

5.1. Research Contributions


Our research has important academic contributions for consumer behavior studies
mainly regarding the conscious impact of the price factor in the purchase intention process
applied to skin care products. We take the opportunity to analyze three different products
with a deep interest in retail SBs, due to its emerging market position.
This exploratory study proposes to respond to the following research questions:
- Do store brand (SB) skin care products meet the consumers’ expectations regarding
price and quality association compared with premiums brands and popular brands?
In general terms 36% of the participants chose the store brand, comparatively only
the S3 (low price low quality) had a slightly higher level of choice of 38% which was
a significant difference. This suggests that in terms of quality, participants were sat-
isfied with the store brand; however, the price factor still had greater influence, pos-
sibly due to the current pandemic moment with high unemployment rates and finan-
cial difficulties of the population.
- Will neurophysiological responses vary when compared with conscious actions in
this context? During the blind test and experiment first stage, the sensory influence
was total, S2 (premium product) had a 50% choice, obviously the participants assim-
ilated the best texture and aroma of the product. However, in the final stage when
the price and attributes of all products were revealed, conscious choice had only 25%
of choices, which was a significant difference F(183, 223) p < 0.001. That is, for the
participants in their rational assessment in terms of the relationship between price
and quality, the quality of the premium product was only recognized and valued by
25% of the participants in the experiment.
Our research in practical terms goes deeper into the concepts of price image, price
consciousness, and price perception. The perceived-value represents the consumer eval-
uation of the benefits from the product compared to the sacrifices, which gives base of
price perception. It was no surprise that the premium brand S3 was selected by 50% of
participants, when it was anonymously presented in the blind test due to its intrinsic at-
tributes (texture and aroma). When prices were revealed, participants’ purchase intention
changed. This result suggests that people interviewed do not see value in a product whose
price is double another one, which they also enjoy, although they preferred it in their ini-
tial choice. This change clearly reflects the concept of price consciousness. However, when
all variables were revealed, a different understanding of price consciousness with the as-
sociation of real price versus perceived quality and attributes was observed. Well-known
attributes have strong extrinsic tip, a symbolic level that positively affects their perception
of value.
The good quality S2 also reflects these changes. When prices were revealed (Slide B)
its choice decreased; however, when attributes were revealed, its price versus quality as-
sociation caused an increase in its choice. From neurophysiological experiment, it was
clear that price variable reveled higher negative impression than attributes.
We understand that our finding could be interesting for retail managers.
Behav. Sci. 2021, 11, 16 14 of 16

In methodological terms, a differentiated way of capturing consumers' perceptions


was utilized, analyzing sensory responses, unconscious responses, and conscious re-
sponses applied at the same time. In practice, we saw in this experimental research how
different variables could affect consumer decision in consumer products.

5.2. Limitations and Future Research Directions


We understand this research has some limitations. Generally, in accordance with Or-
quin, Ashby, and Clarke (2016) [61], eye-tracking method considers fixations to identify
visual attention, focused on an individual’s foveal vision. However, this tool despises pe-
ripheral vision, which limits the analyses of real attention paid on each zone of the image.
In other words, the fact that they are not fixations in AOIs does not mean that they are not
aware that it was there. Still, it may be that some information presented in their visual
field was not focused on it. Although participants had fixed their eyes on specific AOI, we
cannot affirm that it was all they had observed. We considered a random sample of 80
individuals only, so it is a small sample for generalization.
That said, we suggest for future studies to continue this investigation to cover our
limitations. It would be important to expand participants to other regions and countries.
Sample enlargement will allow a deeper investigation, building hypothesis and testing
them by structural equation modeling.

Author Contributions: Conceptualization, G.R.D.L. and M.J.d.S.; methodology, G.R.D.L.; soft-


ware, M.J.d.S.; validation, G.R.D.L.; formal analysis, G.R.D.L. and M.J.d.S.; investigation, G.R.D.L.;
resources, G.R.D.L. and M.J.d.S.; data curation, M.J.d.S.; writing—G.R.D.L. and M.J.d.S.; writing—
M.J.d.S.; supervision, G.R.D.L.; project administration, G.R.D.L.; All authors have read and agreed
to the published version of the manuscript.
Funding: This manuscript received no external funding.
Institutional Review Board Statement: Approved by Federal University of Rio Grande do Sul Ethic
Committee under the number of 344656/2019.
Informed Consent Statement: All the participants agreed and signed a consent document.
Data Availability Statement: All the data presented in this paper is original, were not manipulated
or inappropriately selected, enhanced, fabricated and the analysis tools or methods support all con-
clusions obtained.
Conflicts of Interest: The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References
1. Aghekyan-Simonian, M.; Forsythe, S.; Suk Kwon, W.; Chattaraman, V. The role of product brand image on perceived risks and
online purchase intentions for apparel. J. Retail. Consum. Serv. 2012, 19, 325–331, doi:10.1016/j.jretconser.2012.03.00.
2. Aaker, D.A. Managing Brand Equity; Simon and Schuster: New York, NY, USA, 2009.
3. Beneke, J.; Carter, S.R. The development of a consumer value proposition of private label brands and the application thereof in
a South African retail context. J. Retail. Consum. Serv. 2015, 25, 22–35, doi:10.1016/j.jretconser.2015.03.002.
4. Beneke, J.; Zimmerman, N. Beyond private label panache: The effect of store image and perceived price on brand prestige. J.
Consum. Mark. 2014, 31, 301–311, doi:10.1108/jcm-12-2013-0801.
5. Beneke, J.; Brito, A.; Garvey, K.-A. Propensity to buy private label merchandise. Int. J. Retail. Distrib. Manag. 2015, 43, 43–62,
doi:10.1108/ijrdm-09-2013-0175.
6. Bialkova, S.; Van Trijp, H. What determines consumer attention to nutrition labels? Food Qual. Prefer. 2010, 21, 1042–1051,
doi:10.1016/j.foodqual.2010.07.001.
7. Cacioppo, J.T.; Bush, L.K.; Tassinary, L.G. Microexpressive Facial Actions as a Function of Affective Stimuli: Replication and
Extension. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 1992, 18, 515–526, doi:10.1177/0146167292185001.
8. Porral, C.C.; Lévy-Mangin, J.-P. Food private label brands: The role of consumer trust on loyalty and purchase intention. Br.
Food J. 2016, 118, 679–696, doi:10.1108/bfj-08-2015-0299.
9. Dawson, T. Private Label Brands: The Future Leaders of Retail? Branding Strategy Insider, 2012. Available online:
http://www.brandingstrategyinsider.com (accessed on 15 July 2019).
10. Della Lucia, S.M.; Minim, V.P.R.; Silva, C.H.O.; Minim, L.A. Análise Sensorial: Estudos com Consumidores, 2nd ed.; Editora UFV:
Viçosa, SP, Brazil, 2010; pp. 125–167.
Behav. Sci. 2021, 11, 16 15 of 16

11. Diallo, M.F. Effects of store image and store brand price-image on store brand purchase intention: Application to an emerging
market. J. Retail. Consum. Serv. 2012, 19, 360–367, doi:10.1016/j.jretconser.2012.03.010.
12. Diallo, M.F.; Burt, S.; Sparks, L. The influence of image and consumer factors on store brand choice in the Brazilian market. Eur.
Bus. Rev. 2015, 27, 495–512, doi:10.1108/ebr-03-2013-0048.
13. Diallo, M.; Cliquet, G. Store image perceptions and customer knowledge cues in emerging markets: A cross-country inves-
tigation in Brazil and Vietnam. International. J. Retail Distrib. Manag. 2016, 44, 1182–1193, doi:10.1016/j.jretconser.2012.03.010.
14. Djamasbi, S.; Siegel, M.; Tullis, T. Generation Y, web design, and eye tracking. Int. J. Hum. Comput. Stud. 2010, 68, 307–323,
doi:10.1016/j.ijhcs.2009.12.006.
15. Draper, S. Working Report, Department of Psychology. University of Glasgow, UK. 2008. Available online:
http://www.psy.gla.ac.uk/~steve/HCI/cscln/trail1/Lecture5.html (accessed on 26 October 2019).
16. Essamri, A.; McKechnie, S.; Winklhofer, H. Co-creating corporate brand identity with online brand communities: A managerial
perspective. J. Bus. Res. 2019, 96, 366–375, doi:10.1016/j.jbusres.2018.07.015.
17. Girard, T.; Trapp, P.; Pinar, M.; Gulsoy, T.; Boyt, T.E. Consumer-Based Brand Equity of a Private-Label Brand: Measuring and
Examining Determinants. J. Mark. Theory Pract. 2017, 25, 39–56, doi:10.1080/10696679.2016.1236662.
18. Grewal, D.; Krishnan, R.; Baker, J.; Borin, N. The effect of store name, brand name and price discounts on consumers’ evalua-
tions and purchase intentions. J. Retail. 1998, 74, 331–352, doi:10.1016/s0022-4359(99)80099-2.
19. Hamilton, R.; Chernev, A. Low Prices are Just the Beginning: Price Image in Retail Management. J. Mark. 2013, 77, 1–20,
doi:10.1509/jm.08.0204.
20. Hazlett, R.L. Measurement of User Frustration: A Biologic Approach. In Proceedings of the CHI 2003, Conference on Human
Factors in Computing Systems, Fort Lauderdale, FL, USA, April 2003.
21. Hazlett, R.L.; Benedek, J. Measuring emotional valence to understand the user’s experience of software. Int. J. Hum. Comput.
Stud. 2007, 65, 306–314, doi:10.1016/j.ijhcs.2006.11.005.
22. Hazlett, R.L.; Hazlett, S.Z. Emotional Response to Television Commercials: Facial EMG vs. Self-report. J. Advert. Res. 1999, 39,
7–23.
23. Helkkula, A.; Kelleher, C.; Pihlström, M. Characterizing Value as an Experience. J. Serv. Res. 2012, 15, 59–75,
doi:10.1177/1094670511426897.
24. Holbrook, M.B. Service Quality: New Directions in Theory and Practice; SAGE Publications: Thousand Oaks, CA, USA, 2012; pp.
21–71.
25. Kara, A.; Rojas-Méndez, J.I.; Kucukemiroglu, O.; Harcar, T. Consumer preferences of store brands: Role of prior experiences
and value consciousness. J. Target. Meas. Anal. Mark. 2009, 17, 127–137, doi:10.1057/jt.2009.6.
26. Krucien, N.; Ryan, M.; Hermens, F. Visual attention in multi-attributes choices: What can eye-tracking tell us? J. Econ. Behav.
Organ. 2017, 135, 251–267, doi:10.1016/j.jebo.2017.01.018.
27. Kumar, N.; Steenkamp, J.B.E.M. Private Label Strategy: How to Meet the Store Brand Challenge, 1st ed.; Harvard Business School
Press: Boston, MA, USA, 2007.
28. Kwon, Y.-H. Brand Name Awareness and Image Perception of Women’s Daytime Apparel. Percept. Mot. Ski. 1990, 71, 743–752,
doi:10.2466/pms.1990.71.3.743.
29. Kwon, E.; Ratneshwar, S.; Thorson, E. Consumers’ Social Media Advocacy Behaviors Regarding Luxury Brands: An Explana-
tory Framework. J. Interact. Advert. 2017, 17, 13–27, doi:10.1080/15252019.2017.1315321.
30. Lindquist, J.D. Meaning of image–A survey of empirical and hypothetical evidence. J. Retail. 1974, 50, 29–38.
31. Lichtenstein, D.R.; Ridgway, N.M.; Netemeyer, R.G. Price Perceptions and Consumer Shopping Behavior: A Field Study. J. Mark.
Res. 1993, 30, 234–245, doi:10.1177/002224379303000208.
32. Liljander, V.; Polsa, P.; Van Riel, A. Modelling consumer responses to an apparel store brand: Store image as a risk reducer. J.
Retail. Consum. Serv. 2009, 16, 281–290, doi:10.1016/j.jretconser.2009.02.005.
33. Maroco, J. Análise Estatística, 7th ed; Art Med, São Paulo, Brazil, 2015.
34. Doolittle, M.M. Digital Retail Enters the Next Phase of Disruption. Available online: https://www.mcmillandoolittle.com/digi-
tal-retail-enters-the-next-phase-of-disruption (accessed on 20th October 2019).
35. Miller, K.W.; Mills, M.K. Probing brand luxury: A multiple lens approach. J. Brand Manag. 2012, 20, 41–51,
doi:10.1057/bm.2011.64.
36. Minim, V.; Milagres, M.; Silva, R.; Vasconcelos, C.; Martins, E.; Sampaio, S. Análise de Risco na Avaliação da Influência da
Marca na Aceitabilidade não Sensorial de Requeijão Cremoso. Rev. Inst. Latic. Cândido Tostes 2012, 387, 79–85.
37. Mitchell, V.-W. Re-conceptualizing consumer store image processing using perceived risk. J. Bus. Res. 2001, 54, 167–172,
doi:10.1016/s0148-2963(99)00086-7.
38. Mordor Intelligence Beauty and Personal Care Products Market-Growth, Trends, Forecast (2019–2024). Available online:
https://www.mordorintelligence.com/industryreports/ global-beauty-and-personal-care-products-market-industry (accessed
on 4 November 2019.).
39. Monroe, K.B.; Krishnan, R. The Effects of Price on Subjective; SAGE Publications: Thousand Oaks, CA, USA, 1985.
40. Jacoby, J.; Chipman Olson, J.; Corrie Olson, J. Perceived Quality: How Consumers View Stores; Lexington Books: Lexington, MA,
USA, 1985; pp. 209–232.
41. Nevin, J.R.; Houston, M.J. Image as a component of attraction to intra-urban shopping area. J. Retail. 1980, 56, 77–93.
Behav. Sci. 2021, 11, 16 16 of 16

42. Niedrich, R.W.; Sharma, S.; Wedell, D.H. Reference Price and Price Perceptions: A Comparison of Alternative Models. J. Consum.
Res. 2001, 28, 339–354, doi:10.1086/323726.
43. Nielsen: 13th Private Label Study, Ed Nielsen. SP, Brazil, 2017.
44. Panorama of Retail Private Brands, 2019. Available online: https://www.nielsen.com/br/pt/insights/article/2019/produtos-de-
marcas-proprias-ganhamdestaque-no-canal-farma-e-crescem-dezenove-por-cento (accessed on 15 July 2019).
45. Orquin, J.L.; Loose, S.M. Attention and choice: A review on eye movements in decision making. Acta Psychol. 2013, 144, 190–
206, doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2013.06.003.
46. Orquin, J.L.; Ashby, N.J.S.; Clarke, A.D.F. Areas of Interest as a Signal Detection Problem in Behavioral Eye-Tracking Research.
J. Behav. Decis. Mak. 2015, 29, 103–115, doi:10.1002/bdm.1867.
47. Peacock, J.; Purvis, S.; Hazlett, R.L. Which Broadcast Medium Better Drives Engagement? J. Advert. Res. 2011, 51, 578,
doi:10.2501/jar-51-4-578-585.
48. Sanchez, M.; Beriain, M.J.; Carr, T. Socio-economic factors affecting consumer behavior for United States and Spanish beef under
different information scenarios. Food Qual. Prefer. 2012, 24, 30–39.
49. Dos Santos, J.P.M.; Martins, M.; Ferreira, H.A.; Ramalho, J.; Seixas, D. Neural imprints of national brands versus own-label
brands. J. Prod. Brand Manag. 2016, 25, 184–195, doi:10.1108/jpbm-12-2014-0756.
50. Sheau-Fen, Y.; Sun-May, L.; Yu-Ghee, W. Store brand proneness: Effects of perceived risks, quality and familiarity. Australas.
Mark. J. AMJ 2012, 20, 48–58, doi:10.1016/j.ausmj.2011.10.014.
51. Sethuraman, R.; Cole, C. Factors influencing the price premiums that consumers pay for national brands over store brands. J.
Prod. Brand Manag. 1999, 8, 340–351, doi:10.1108/10610429910284319.
52. Stefani, G.; Romano, D.; Cavicchi, A. Consumer expectations, liking and willingness to pay for specialty foods: Do sensory
characteristics tell the whole story? Food Qual. Prefer. 2006, 17, 53–62, doi:10.1016/j.foodqual.2005.07.010.
53. Sethuraman, R.; Gielens, K. Determinants of Store Brand Share. J. Retail. 2014, 90, 141–153, doi:10.1016/j.jretai.2014.04.002.
54. Slanzi, G.; Balazs, J.A.; Velásquez, J.D. Combining eye tracking, pupil dilation and EEG analysis for predicting web users click
intention. Inf. Fusion 2017, 35, 51–57, doi:10.1016/j.inffus.2016.09.003.
55. Teas, R.K. Expectations, Performance Evaluation, and Consumers’ Perceptions of Quality. J. Mark. 1993, 57, 18–34,
doi:10.1177/002224299305700402.
56. Thanasuta, K. Thai consumers’ purchase decisions and private label brands. Int. J. Emerg. Mark. 2015, 10, 102–121,
doi:10.1108/ijoem-02-2011-0016.
57. Vahie, A.; Paswan, A. Private label brand image: Its relationship with store image and national brand. Int. J. Retail. Distrib.
Manag. 2006, 34, 67–84, doi:10.1108/09590550610642828.
58. Venkatraman, V.; Clithero, J.A.; Fitzsimons, G.J.; Huettel, S.A. New scanner data for brand marketers: How neuroscience can
help better understand differences in brand preferences. J. Consum. Psychol. 2012, 22, 143–153, doi:10.1016/j.jcps.2011.11.008.
59. Vila, J.; Gomez, Y. Extracting business information from graphs: An eye tracking experiment. J. Bus. Res. 2016, 69, 1741–1746,
doi:10.1016/j.jbusres.2015.10.048.
60. Von Wallpach, S.; Hemetsberger, A.; Espersen, P. Performing identities: Processes of brand and stakeholder identity co-con-
struction. J. Bus. Res. 2017, 70, 443–452, doi:10.1016/j.jbusres.2016.06.021.
61. Weiner, B. Attributional Thoughts about Consumer Behavior. J. Consum. Res. 2000, 27, 382–387, doi:10.1086/317592.

You might also like

pFad - Phonifier reborn

Pfad - The Proxy pFad of © 2024 Garber Painting. All rights reserved.

Note: This service is not intended for secure transactions such as banking, social media, email, or purchasing. Use at your own risk. We assume no liability whatsoever for broken pages.


Alternative Proxies:

Alternative Proxy

pFad Proxy

pFad v3 Proxy

pFad v4 Proxy