0% found this document useful (0 votes)
20 views10 pages

Agbo-Egwu ABAH Anyagh DigitalAmnesia

Uploaded by

newdaleguy122
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
0% found this document useful (0 votes)
20 views10 pages

Agbo-Egwu ABAH Anyagh DigitalAmnesia

Uploaded by

newdaleguy122
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
You are on page 1/ 10

The Influence of Smartphone Over-dependence on the

Recall of Basic Mathematics among Mathematics


Education Students in a Nigerian University
Abel Okoh Agbo-Egwu, Joshua Abah Abah, Paul Igber Anyagh

To cite this version:


Abel Okoh Agbo-Egwu, Joshua Abah Abah, Paul Igber Anyagh. The Influence of Smartphone Over-
dependence on the Recall of Basic Mathematics among Mathematics Education Students in a Nigerian
University. Science & Technology, 2018, 4, pp.97-105. �hal-01819070�

HAL Id: hal-01819070


https://hal.science/hal-01819070v1
Submitted on 19 Jun 2018

HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est


archive for the deposit and dissemination of sci- destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents
entific research documents, whether they are pub- scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non,
lished or not. The documents may come from émanant des établissements d’enseignement et de
teaching and research institutions in France or recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires
abroad, or from public or private research centers. publics ou privés.

Distributed under a Creative Commons Attribution - NonCommercial - ShareAlike 4.0 International


License
REPORT
REPORT ARTICLE Vol. 4, 2018

Science & Technology


ISSN
2394–3750
EISSN
2394–3769

The influence of smart phone over-dependence


on the recall of basic mathematics among
mathematics education students in a Nigerian
university
Abel Okoh Agbo-Egwu, Joshua Abah Abah☼, Paul Igber Anyagh

Department of Science Education, University of Agriculture, Makurdi, Benue State, Nigeria


Corresponding author:
Department of Science Education
University of Agriculture, Makurdi
Benue State, Nigeria.
*Corresponding Author’s E-mail: abahjoshua.a@gmail.com
Tel: +2347039240534

Article History
Received: 29 April 2018
Accepted: 16 June 2018
Published: June 2018

Citation
Abel Okoh Agbo-Egwu, Joshua Abah Abah, Paul Igber Anyagh. The influence of smart phone over-dependence on the recall of basic
mathematics among mathematics education students in a Nigerian university. Science & Technology, 2018, 4, 97-105

Publication License

This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.

General Note
Article is recommended to print as color digital version in recycled paper.
97

ABSTRACT
Page

Recent decades have seen dramatic increase in the amount of technology available to and used by children, a fact that has clearly
shaped the way children learn mathematics. An unintended outcome of technology integration in mathematics education is the

© 2018 Discovery Publication. All Rights Reserved. www.discoveryjournals.org OPEN ACCESS


REPORT ARTICLE

growing dependence among students on the internet as a source of information previously committed to memory. This study
employs a simple survey to determine how the ubiquity of smart phones among mathematics education students influence their
ability to recall basic mathematics concepts. Structured Basic Mathematics Recall Task (BMRT) and Smart phones Influence on
Mathematics Recall Questionnaire (SIMRQ) were developed for the study and administered to a random sample of 180 mathematics
education students. Simple percentages and means were used to analyze the data the data obtained from the BMRT and SIMRQ.
The outcomes of the study indicate that 46.7% of mathematics education students could correctly recall formulas and values of basic
mathematical constants, 31.6% could correctly provide the statement and definition of basic math concepts, and 28.3% could
correctly tell when to use a particular formula under specific conditions. The results of the study reveal that processes of human
memory are adapting to the advent of digital technology, with students displaying low recall of foundational facts and high
impatience to quickly rush to the internet in quest of information previously memorized. The findings of this study points to the fact
that true mathematical prowess in the current age is only achievable through the conscious effort of the learner in internalizing basic
mathematical concepts and regular practice of learnt facts. Based on the findings, it was recommended that students should be
guided through measured technology integration in mathematics instructional delivery to avoid over-dependence on technological
tools.

Keywords: Mathematics, Memory, Memory augmentation, Digital amnesia, Mathematics prowess

1. INTRODUCTION
Mathematics plays a key role in shaping how individuals deal with the various spheres of private, social, and civil life. The knowledge
and application of mathematics made it fundamental to all facet of human endeavour. The relevance of the subject has position it as
a compulsory subject in Basic Education and a foundational course in virtually all professional fields of study. Thus, mathematics is a
part of children’s life experience.
Simply stated, the main goal of mathematics education is the mathematics child’s thought processes (National Council of
Educational Research and Training -NCERT, 2006). Clarity of thought and pursuing of problems in a systemic manner is central to the
mathematical enterprise. The target of educational efforts anywhere in the world is to develop life skills and link schooling to
livelihood. This explains why the school system deliver instruction to enable children use abstractions to perceive relationships, to
understand the basic structure of mathematics, and to appreciate the scope and power of mathematics in inculcating the right
habits of thought and communication.
The expectation, generally, is that as mathematics advances, learners incorporate it into their thinking. As their thinking becomes
more sophisticated, they generate new mathematical concepts and new mathematical structures (Thurston, 1994). Evidently, the
subject matter of mathematics changes to reflect how people think and eventually recall basic facts.
As children grow older and make progress up the educational ladder, mathematics learning will make no sense if it is a
remarkable mental process and a mental system which receives information from stimuli, retains it and makes it available on a future
occasion. Memory provides continuity in experiences across deferent time points. This continuity is a fact on which the entire
educational process hinges, with knowledge and skill taught at incrementally spiral levels. Mathematics in particular is structured in a
way such that present learning is built on the foundations of past learning experiences. An outcome of this spiral structure is the
persistent need to recall foundational facts, theorems, axioms, and associated formulas every now and then.
Memory and remembering in particular has been shown to be a constructive process, with recall being a combination of retrieval
and reconstruction. Information tends to evaporate with time unless it is actively maintained (Altmann & Gray, 2002). With respect
to the study of mathematics, fundamental prowess in recall and application of mathematical facts is more a function of practice than
of time. Forgetting of basic mathematical concepts is now being felled by several side attractions of the current age in which focus
and attention are in limited supply, especially among youngsters.
It is quite obvious that the invention of computers has turned human existence into an information-ruled one (Iji, Abah & Uka,
2013). Digital technologies are revolutionizing human thinking and opening up access to whole lot of possibilities in the education
sector. Schools all over the world are becoming an integral part of the broadband and technology transformation, harnessing the
potentials of technology to drive and empower more personalized horning. Despite the benefits accruing from the availability of
98

digital technologies, over-dependence on them turns out to be bad for children’s mathematical memory. This over-dependence,
Page

according to Felt and Robb (2016) can lead to technological addiction.

© 2018 Discovery Publication. All Rights Reserved. www.discoveryjournals.org OPEN ACCESS


REPORT ARTICLE

The tendency to rely heavily on information available online instead of tasking ones memory is currently on the increase. The
enormity of this perspective of technological dependence was brought to the light in a recent quantitative study commissioned by
the cyber-security firm, Kaspersky Lab. People, including students of mathematics, rely on the internet and internet-enabled devices
such as smartphones to connect them to a vast repository of knowledge, anywhere anytime (Kaspersky Lab, 2015). With the
unending reach of instant search engines, students tend to relinquish the duty of simple recall of core mathematical concepts to
digital technologies. This growing trend poses real danger to the preservation and transfer of the mathematical culture inherited by
the present generation.
Digital Amnesia is the experience of forgetting information that one trusts a digital device to store and remember for him
(Kaspersky Lab, 2015). This present study seeks to find out how the ubiquity of smart phones among mathematics education
students influences their ability to recall basic mathematical facts, theorems, axioms, and formula. What prospect does the observed
phenomenon hold for the quality of mathematics teachers produced from Nigerian higher educational institutions? What are the
expected long-term implications for mathematics education in general?

Literature review
Mathematics is seen as the study of patterns of structure, chance, and space. Mathematics is a language of symbols, technical
definitions, computation, and logic (Thurston, 1994). Simply put, mathematics is the study of figures and numbers. It is the
investigation of axiomatically defined abstract structures using logic and mathematical notation.
There is no doubt that mathematics is of immense practical value in life. We regularly use mathematics in our everyday life: from
measuring distances and weights to reading timetables and in estimation (Legner, 2013). Mathematics is essential for full
comprehension of technological and scientific issues.
As a subject, mathematics is full of automatic procedures that have to be mastered (Iji, Abah & Uka, 2013). As an outcome,
school mathematics attaches emphasis to factual knowledge, procedural fluency and conceptual understanding (NCERT, 2006). A
relevant curriculum and pedagogy of mathematics focus on a whole range of processes, including formal problem solving,
optimization, use of patterns, visualization, reasoning and proof, making connections, and mathematical communication. Generally,
the aim of teaching mathematics includes the development of good understanding of numbers and the number system, improving
speed in understanding relationships, achieving rapid mental recall of facts, developing the ability to solve problems, and improving
skills in decision making.
Learning mathematics entails building up a database of concepts, abstraction, generalization, and applications. This is why
mathematics is taught incrementally on basic foundations of rules, axioms, theorems, and formula. Burgin (2000) observes that these
foundations provide means, concepts, structures, and methods for the development of the whole mathematics, or at least, of its
major part. Most of these foundational rubrics as found in named sets theory require the students to imbibe fundamental
mathematical knowledge as an indispensable companion in life.
Mathematics is proud of its history, confident in its tradition, and certain that the truth it pronounced are real truths applying to
the real world (Wells, 2006). Such real world applications are made possible by years of accumulated and structured facts, taught in
school to children as the subject of mathematics. Evidently, mathematics can be seen as the act and science of proving theorems,
and a theorem is a statement that, given the premises laid down by the axioms and certain agreed upon rules of inference, is
apodictically true. The mathematics taught in school is the interconnections between several of these facts and one of the objectives
of teaching the subjects is to raise the aesthetic value of its culture, stimulate curiosity, create enjoyment of mathematics, and
develop children depth of understanding (Massachusetts Department of Elementary and Secondary Education, 2011).
The nature of mathematics requires students to articulate sound mathematics explanations and always justify their solutions
(Anthony & Walshaw, 2009). Years of schooling are expected to instill mathematics confidence in students and build in them the
capacity to persevere in the face of mathematical challenge. With the emergent paradigm of the pedagogy of mathematics, focus is
shifting from crude memorizing of algorithms and computations to mathematical intuition, mathematical thinking and real-world
problem solving (Legner, 2013). In this perspective, recall of simple mathematical facts should come in handy for students
irrespective of technological interference.
Memory is basically a store of things learned and retained from an organism activity or experience as evidenced by modification
of structure or behaviour or by recall and recognition (Merriam-Webster, 2016). Learning will make no sense if it is not retrained
since the capacity of the memory is the only means a person can relate to different events, experiences, conditions, people a nd
99

objects. Memory makes it possible to operate beyond the constraints of time and place.
Page

© 2018 Discovery Publication. All Rights Reserved. www.discoveryjournals.org OPEN ACCESS


REPORT ARTICLE

The human memory system receives, encodes, modifies, retains and retrieves information. Encoding refers to the translation of
incoming stimulus into a unique neural code that a person’s brain can process while storage is the retention of the material encoded
over a period of time. Retrieval is the recovery of the stored or retained information at a later occasion.
A general model of the human memory depicts three distinct types of memory, namely, sensory register, short term memory and
long-term memory. The sensory register is the first point of interaction of the memory system with information gathered through
the senses. It holds information for a short duration before passing the information onwards. It occurs within the sensory system
while the message is being transmitted to the brain.
The short-term memory is the working memory. It holds immediate visual images, sounds, words and sentences before
transmitting them to the long term memory. The sensory memory and short term memory are limited in terms of duration. The
long-term memory is a relatively enduring memory in which information is stored for use at a later time. Meaningful verbal materials
and life events that are acquired in the course of experiences persist in the long term memory and are often retrieved into the
working memory for usage.
Information retained in the memory is subject to change and modification. Forgetting is failure to recall information stored in
memory. Myers (2007) sees forgetting as the inability to retrieve information, due to poor encoding, storage or retrieval. Many
causes, and factors and theories of forgetting have being propounded by a wide range of psychologist over the years. The comm on
list of factors or causes comprises decay of memory traces interference, motivations, retrieval failure, misinformation and
imagination effects, and amnesia. However, Wixted (2004) observes that these common points of view have changed in recent times
from a theoretically coherent interference-based account to a theoretical laundry list of factors that may or may not play a role.
Arguably, high levels of practice and usage of stored fact enhance effective retrieval for future usage (Altmann & Gray, 2002).This
truth holds particularly in mathematics where performance is evidently related to practice.
One of the crucial lessons emanating from any casual review of literature memory is the role of interest and sound background
in the subject area. Learners who show interest in the particular subject area tends to recall information better. Also the more
background the learner has in a subject, the better he can form associations and discern relationships between the new and the old
information (May & Einstein, 2013). This is particularly emphatic for the recall of mathematical facts in later years by students as the
gradually advance in their studies. Poor recall of information considered simple and fundamental mathematics maybe a pointer to
something deeper than forgetting. It may indicate a new dimension of high- handedness on the part of students and may reveal
underlying altitudinal issues in mathematics education.
Memory is often defined as the application of learning over time (Mays & Einstein, 2013). It establishes links across different
learning experiences. Memory is needed in developing social relationships, mastering cognitive competencies and solving various
problems. The indispensability of a good memory makes forgetting appear to be a fatal failure (Connerton, 2008).
The need to sustain steady recall leads to augmentation. Basically augmentation is complimentary measure meant to support a
falling or an inadequate system. Augmentation ranges from simple memory enhancing strategies such as mnemonics and memory
aids to information on the internet. The wide range of available technological operations only underscores the assertion that
learning experiences today are defined by the pervasive influence of digital technology (Bavelier, Green & Dye, 2010).
People willingly accept technological augmentation to ease the demand of recalling vast amount of information. The Internet is
now being used as an extension of the brain (Kaspersky Lab, 2015). Presently, the available body of evidence has not being able to
establish whether Internet use is creating a generation with fundamentally different cognitive skills (Mills, 2014).But there are
suggestions that being a part of highly connected network can help individuals solve problems by facilitating the propagation of
correct information. Conclusively, the effect of internet use on cognition can likely nuanced, but could strengthen specific cognitive
strategies in young adult, if carefully articulated into existing framework.
The Internet, with its search engines such as Google and online databases has become an external memory source that people
can access at any time (Bavelier et al, 2010). Social network services like Facebook are increasingly turning to information repositions
forming a digital bridge for those afraid of swiftly loosing fond memories. A most recent upgrade to the service of face book, for
instance, has open up an educational front in which users can save interesting web pages and contents to their timeline on the
social networking giant by simply clicking a save-to-Facebook button to be made available on third party sites and services. These
saved materials will permanently be available for referencing by the users as long as their Facebook accounts exist.
Iji, Abah and Anyor (2014) rightly noted that cloud technology integration in mathematics education has resulted in the
100

transformation of young minds known for a culture of learning, leisure and social interaction. Documents and memories pushed into
the cloud on smartphones are easily accessible by students many years after. But a serial habit of relying on this cloud-sourced
Page

memory is resulting in a sense of over-dependence on augmentation at the detriment of personal cognitive efforts.

© 2018 Discovery Publication. All Rights Reserved. www.discoveryjournals.org OPEN ACCESS


REPORT ARTICLE

Memory and learning are two different processes that are closely related. Learning refers to the acquisition of new behaviour
through experience that has persisted in memory. Memory is a skill that can be improved upon the limits of one’s potential. The
formation of concrete memory is often hampered when the content to be retained is not correctly, clearly, and forcibly impressed on
the mind. If mathematical knowledge is approached with anything less, learning will be hardly achieved. The over-dependence on an
external memory source builds an attitude among learners that is devoid of necessary attention and interest. This attitude eventually
leads to disuse of learned mathematical concepts resulting in gradual effervescence of retained mathematical knowledge (Mills,
2014).
Based on the Kaspersky Lab (2015) research, it can be argued that the trend to look up information before even trying to recall it
prevents the build-up of long-term memories, and thus makes people process information merely on a shallow, moment-to-
moment basis. This outcome is a threat to mathematical posterity. The future will indeed be uncertain if mathematical creativity is
abandoned in favour of mathematical referencing. Having online access to search engines, databases, and the like, is fast becoming
a primary transitive memory source in itself (Sparrow et al, 2011), with students getting more interested in “where” information can
be obtained than “what” the content of such information is.
The resulting trend and outcome of technological interference in concrete mathematical learning have upset the expected order
of mathematical development in children. Through-out the early years of life, children notice and explore mathematical dimensions
of their world. They compare quantities, find patterns, navigate in space, and grapple with real problems (National Association for
the Education of Young Children –NAEYC, 2010). Mathematics helps children make sense of their world and helps them construct a
solid foundation for success in life. In life, students need mathematical proficiency and understanding to make informed decisions
and make progress. This is the natural pathway to mathematical prowess –a phenomenon that is currently being challenge by an
inadvertent relinquishing of effort to easily accessible internet connections.
Children’s confidence, competence and interests in mathematics flourish when new experiences are meaningful and connected
with their prior knowledge and experience. Lack of proper connection to explicit concepts learnt earlier sometimes prevents
students from making full use of retained knowledge (NAEYC, 2010). These connections and explicit formulation is the formalization
of mathematics as taught in schools. Hatcher (2008) maintains that everyone agrees that mathematical activity involves the following
processes: contemplation of abstractions, the generation of mental construction, and the explicit formulation of rules of symbolic
manipulation. The process of attaining mathematical prowess requires active participation on the part of the learner to internalize
mathematical knowledge.
A proficient mathematician is a master of critical thinking, of analysis, and of deductive reasoning (Krantz, 2007). These skills
travel well and should be bankable, particularly by prospective mathematicians who are being grilled to stand out with the use of
rigorous proofs and further the timelessness of the subject of mathematics. Sparks (2006) maintains that it is partly through
memorization that we eventually become “unconsciously competent”, a true master of our skill, practicing it in an almost effortless,
automatic sense. Knowing basic facts and understanding the reasoning behind them propels one towards the road to mathematical
fluency, so essential in our modern high-tech society.

Research Questions
The following research questions were raised to guide the study.
1.What percentage of mathematics education students correctly recalled basic mathematical facts?
2.How does the ubiquity of smartphones among mathematics education students influence their ability to recall basic
mathematical facts?

2. METHODOLOGY
Simple survey research design was employed in this study to investigate how the ubiquity of smartphones among mathematics
education students influences their ability to recall basic mathematical facts. Mathematics education students from the three
mathematics education programme options of B.Sc.(Ed) Mathematics/Computer Science, B.Sc.(Ed) Statistics/Computer Science and
B.Sc.(Ed) Mathematics/Statistics, offered at a federal University in North Central Nigeria, were used for the study. The population of
the study comprises the 1218 mathematics education students. Specifically, a random sample of 180 second-year (200 Level)
mathematics education students across the three options were used for the study. The choice of the class was along the line of
101

reasoning that second-year students are at the middle of their degree programmes and yet not very far from their secondary school
backgrounds where basic mathematical concepts were explicitly learnt.
Page

Mathematical concepts considered in this study are foundational axioms and formula such as Pythagoras theorem, sine and
cosine rules, basic axioms in set theory and a few definitions. These were built into the researcher-developed Basic Math Recall Task

© 2018 Discovery Publication. All Rights Reserved. www.discoveryjournals.org OPEN ACCESS


REPORT ARTICLE

(BMRT) and the Smartphones Influence on Math Recall Questionnaire (SIMRQ) instruments. The SIMRQ is structured on a four-point
scale with Strongly Agree (SA=4), Agree (A=3), Disagree (D=2), and Strongly Disagree (SD=1). It comprises nine (9) items to elicit
responses in relation to perceived performance in on the BMRT. As such, the BMRT was administered to respondents before the
SIMRQ.
The two instruments used for this study were validated by three (3) experts in mathematics education from the University of
Agriculture Makurdi, Nigeria. The experts’ corrections, opinions and suggestions were used to restructure the items in terms of
suitability and relevance, increasing an initial effort of 8 items to a final output of 9 items for the SIMRQ. The reliability analysis of
the SIMRQ yields a Cronbach alpha coefficient of 0.74, indicating a high level of internal consistency for the instrument.

3. RESULTS AND DISCUSSIONS


The result of analysis carried out on data obtained with the Basic Recall Task (BMRT) and Smartphones Influence on Math Recall
Questionnaire (SIMRQ) are presented here in respect to the research questions.
Research Question One: What percentage of mathematics education students correctly recalled basic mathematics facts?

Table 1 Recall on BMRT


S/N Basic Math Formulae/Value Statement/Definition When to Use
1 Pythagoras Theorem 73% 68% 45%
2 Sine Rule (Law of Sines) 43% 22% 10%
3 Cosine Rule (Law of Cosine) 36% 13% 7%
4 DeMorgan’s Law 27% 20% 18%
5 Quadratic Formula 47% 32% 30%
6 Distance from A to B on straight line 23% 19% Not Applicable
7 Mid-point of the Segment 31% 25% Not Applicable
8 Slope of a Straight Line 89% 52% 46%
9 Volume of a Cylinder 34% 29% Not Applicable
10 nth term of GP 62% 60% 55%
11 Quotient Rule (calculus) 48% 44% 40%
12 Product Rule (calculus) 51% 50% 43%
13 Heron’s Formula for Area of Triangle 41% 26% 15%
14 Golden Ratio 3% 3% Not Applicable
15 Simple Interest 87% 81% Not Applicable
16 Persistence (Multiplicative) Not Applicable 9% Not Applicable
17 DeMoivre’s Theorem 2% 2% 2%
(Complex Numbers)
18 Derivation of pi ( ) 97% 14% Not Applicable

Average Value 46.7% 31.6% 28.3%

The results in Table 1 shows the percentage of mathematics education students who correctly recall a set of randomly selected
basic mathematical fact by clearly stating the required formulae/value, statement/definition, and when the particular item is should
be used. The overall report indicates that 46.7% of mathematics education students could correctly recall formulas and values of
basic mathematical facts, while 31.6% of mathematics education students could correctly provide the statement and definition of
basic math concepts/facts. 28.3% could tell when to use a particular formula and could state specific conditions.
The overall averages from the items in Table 1 fall short of the expected 50%. There seem to be a downward trend when it
comes to providing details about basic mathematical concepts, pointing out that possibly students did not properly internalized the
concepts even at the time they were taught. This poses a great danger to the students’ growth and development when considered
along the perspective of Willingham (2010) who emphasized that automatic retrieval of basic mathematics concepts from memory is
102

critical to solving complex problems since complex problems have simpler problems imbedded in them.
Page

Research Question Two: How does the ubiquity of smartphones among mathematics education students influence their ability to
recall basic mathematical facts?

© 2018 Discovery Publication. All Rights Reserved. www.discoveryjournals.org OPEN ACCESS


REPORT ARTICLE

Table 2 Analysis of Responses on SIMRQ


S/N Items/Statements Mean Remark
1. I have been taught most of the basic maths concepts before. 3.12 Accepted
2. I used to easily recall mathematical facts better in the past than now 2.5 Accepted
3. I actually thought I would recall better if I had the chance to “ask Google”. 3.24 Accepted
4. It is impatience to quickly rush to the internet for information one has previously 3.37 Accepted
memorized.
5. Under a different situation I would have checked for the correct answers on my 3.65 Accepted
smartphone even before “stressing my head”.
6. Except when preparing for a test or an exam, I quickly check on the Internet when 2.71 Accepted
confronted with basic math problems
7. Students tend to remember less basic math facts knowing they can always depend on 3.16 Accepted
their smartphones.
8. I consider over-dependence on the Internet for simple recall a threat to the future of 3.18 Accepted
mathematics knowledge.
9. I believe a conscious effort to understand math is better for me than depending on 2.94 Accepted
technology all the time.
Grand Mean 3.10

Table 2 above shows an acceptance (grand mean = 3.10) among respondents that they often depend on technological
augmentation for simple recall. Respondents also accepted that the phenomenon poses a great danger to the future of
mathematical knowledge. They supported the belief that proper learning of mathematical content is more reliable than outright
dependence on technology.

4. CONCLUSIONS
This study has attempted to demonstrate the impact of over-dependence on digital communication gadgets on students’ memory
efficiency in mathematics. The pattern of students’ ability to recall basic mathematical facts, theorems, axioms, and formula indicates
a negative influence of smartphone over-dependence on simple recall. The participants of this study accepted that over-reliance on
the Internet for simple recall poses a great threat to the future of mathematical prowess.
The findings of this study clearly support the outcome of the Kaspersky Lab (2015) survey in affirming that mathematics
education students tend to rely heavily on the Internet and Internet-enabled devices such as smartphones to connect them to the
vast repository of knowledge, anywhere anytime. There is indeed a waning desire to properly commit learning to memory due to the
ubiquity of smartphones and their ever-present internet connectivity. This growing dependence on the internet as a source of
information we might previously have memorized or looked for elsewhere can reflect impatience or the need for speed in a fast-
moving world. It can be argued that if we preferably use our smartphones to off toad currently irrelevant information so we can
access it again at a later time when necessary, interference with the learning of new information can be greatly reduced. In this way,
dependence on memory augmentation will be justifiable without rendering the human memory redundant and passive. The act of
memorization in mathematics is a skill which must be developed and sustained for the very survival of mathematical prowess.

Recommendations
In view of the findings of this study, the following recommendations are made:
1.The teaching and learning of mathematics at the foundational level should be given adequate attention with focus on conceptual
understanding, procedural fluency, strategic competence, adaptive reasoning, and productive disposition. Children should be
made to practice simple routines such as “borrow and regroup” to foster the learning of the sequence of steps by which a
frequently encountered problem can be solved. To build the strands of mathematical proficiency, children can be guided to count
successfully before they understood all of counting’s properties, such as the irrelevance of order. A productive disposition could be
103

enhanced in students when mathematics teachers adopt methodologies that integrate the use of modern tools that exploit a
variety of self-regulation strategies and drive individualized learning.
Page

2.Children should be guided through measured technological integration in mathematics instructional delivery to avoid over-
dependence on technology. Technology deployment does not always help learning – it sometimes impedes it. This is most likely

© 2018 Discovery Publication. All Rights Reserved. www.discoveryjournals.org OPEN ACCESS


REPORT ARTICLE

when technological tools are so visually interesting that they distract from their purpose or when their relationship to the concept
to be represented is obscure.
3.Teachers of mathematics at the early childhood and basic education levels should stimulate in children an aesthetic value of the
subject to enhance sustained interest of the subject. This can be achieved by deploying instructional materials such as
manipulatives and concretizing abstract terms through vivid analogies. For instance, a teacher might teach students that algebraic
equations may be thought of like a balance scale. The two sides are equivalent and their equivalence can be maintained so long as
one perform the same operations on both sides,
4.In teaching procedural and factual knowledge to young adults, schools should ensure students get to automaticity. Current efforts
of the National Mathematics Centre Abuja to resuscitate Mental Arithmetic should be supported and transferred to all schools
within the Basic Education level for the full development of the Nigerian child.
5.At the university level, the teaching of mathematics should cultivate greater conceptual knowledge as well as emphasize
procedural and factual knowledge. Mathematics teachers at this level should never assume their students are already abreast with
some vital mathematical concepts. Even at this level, depth matters the more.

REFERENCE
1. Altmann, E.M. & Gray, W.D. (2002). Forgetting to remember: 12. Krantz, S.G. (2007). The history and concept of mathematical
The functional relationship of decay and interference. proof. California: Caltech. Pp1-5.
Psychological Science, 13(1), 27-33. 13. Legner, P. (2013). The value of teaching mathematics.
2. Anthony, G. & Walshaw, M. (2009). Characteristics of Retrieved on 5th May, 2016, from
effective teaching of mathematics: A view from the West. http://www.mathigon.org/resources/value-of-
Journal of Mathematics Education, 147-164. mathematics.pdf
3. Bavelier, D., Green, C.S.S. & Dye, M.W.G. (2010).Children, 14. Massachusetts Department of Elementary and Secondary
wired: For better and for worse. Neuron, 67, 692-701 Education (2011). Massachusetts Curriculum Framework for
4. Burgin, M. (2000).Unified foundations for mathematics. Mathematics. Malden, MA: Massachusetts Department of
Mathematics Subject Classification, 3, 1-39. Elementary and Secondary Education, pp 1-192
5. Connerton, P. (2008). Seven types of forgetting. Memory 15. May, C. P.& Einstein, G. O. (2013). Memory: A five-day unit
Studies, 1(1), 59-71. DoI:10.1177/1750698007083889. lesson plan for high school pyschology teachers.
6. Edwards, L. D. (2003). The nature of mathematics as viewed Washington, DC: Teachers of psychology in Secondary
from cognitive science. A Paper presented at the Third Schools (TOPSS) of the American Psychological Association.
Congress of European Society for Research in Mathematics 16. Merriam-Webster (2016). Merriam-Webster English
Education, Bellaria, Italy. Dictionary. Android Application by Merriam-Webster,
7. Felt, L. J. & Robb, M. B. (2016). Technology addiction: Incorporated.
Concern, controversy, and finding balance. San Francisco, 17. Mills, K. L. (2014). Effects of internet use on the adolescent
CA: Common Sense Media, Inc. pp 1-36 brain: Despite popular claims, experimental evidence
8. Hatcher, W.S.(2008). The foundations of mathematics: An remains scarce. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(8), 385-387
overview at the close of the second millennium. Retrieved 18. Myers, D. (2007) Psychology (8th edition). Henderson: Worth
on 15th April 2016 from http://william.hatcher.org. publishers.
9. Iji, C. O., Abah, J. A. & Uka, N. K. (2013). Attaining the 19. National Association for the Education of Young Children-
Millennium Development Goals (MDGs) through effective NAEYC (2010). Early childhood mathematics: Promoting
mathematics education. Proceeding of the 54th Annual good beginnings. A Joint Position Statement of the National
Conference of the Science Teachers Association Nigeria Association for the Education of Young Children (NAEYC )
(STAN) 2013. and the National Council of Teachers of Mathematics
10. Iji, C.O., Abah, J. A. & Anyor, J.W. (2014). Enhancing national (NCTM). Washinton DC: NAEYC, pp 1-2
competitiveness through the utilization of the cloud services 20. National Council of Educational Research and Training-
to improve the learning of school mathematics. Proceedings NCERT (2006). Position paper of the National Focus Group
of September 2014 Annual National Conference of the on Teaching of Mathematics. New Delhi: Publication
Mathematical Association of Nigeria. pp 230-243. Department-NCERT.
104

11. Kaspersky Lab (2015). The rise and impact of digital amnesia. 21. National Mathematics Centre, Abuja (2016). Latest News.
Retrieved on 12th March, 2016, from Retrieved on 11th March 2016 from http://www
Page

https://www.kasperskycontenthub.com/usa/files/2015/06/Di nmcabuja.org/latest-news.html.
gtal-Amnesia-Report.pdf.

© 2018 Discovery Publication. All Rights Reserved. www.discoveryjournals.org OPEN ACCESS


REPORT ARTICLE

22. Sparks, J. C. (2006). The handbook of essential mathematics.


In D.G. Gregory and V.R. Miller (Eds.) The handbook of
Essential Mathematics: Formulas, Processes and Tables, plus
Application in Personal Finance. The Airforce Research
Laboratory, pp 1-100.
23. Sparrow, B., Liu, J. & Wegner, D. M. (2011) Google effects on
memory: cognitive consequences of having informational
our fingertips. Science, 333, 776-778. DOI:
10.1126/science.1207745
24. Thurston, W.P. (1994). On proof and progress in
mathematics. Bulletin of the American Mathematics Society,
30(2), 161-177.
25. Wells, R.B. (2006). Mathematics and mathematical axioms.
Retrieved on 31st July, 2016, from
http://www.mrc.uidaho.edu/~rwells/Critical%20Philosophy%
20and%Mind/Chapter%2023.pdf
26. Willingham, D. T. (2010). Is it true that some people just can’t
do math? American Educator, Winter 2009-2010, 14-39.
27. Wixted, J.T. (2004). The psychology and neuroscience of
forgetting. Annual Review of Psychology, 55, 235-269. DOI:
10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.141555.

105
Page

© 2018 Discovery Publication. All Rights Reserved. www.discoveryjournals.org OPEN ACCESS

You might also like

pFad - Phonifier reborn

Pfad - The Proxy pFad of © 2024 Garber Painting. All rights reserved.

Note: This service is not intended for secure transactions such as banking, social media, email, or purchasing. Use at your own risk. We assume no liability whatsoever for broken pages.


Alternative Proxies:

Alternative Proxy

pFad Proxy

pFad v3 Proxy

pFad v4 Proxy