Nussbaum (2008) Self Esteem
Nussbaum (2008) Self Esteem
net/publication/5574967
CITATIONS READS
364 5,831
2 authors, including:
David Nussbaum
University of Chicago
5 PUBLICATIONS 1,051 CITATIONS
SEE PROFILE
All content following this page was uploaded by David Nussbaum on 06 June 2014.
Published by:
http://www.sagepublications.com
On behalf of:
Additional services and information for Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin can be found at:
Subscriptions: http://psp.sagepub.com/subscriptions
Reprints: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.nav
Permissions: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
Citations http://psp.sagepub.com/cgi/content/refs/34/5/599
How people maintain and repair their self-esteem has Much attention has been paid to defensive processes
been a topic of widespread interest. In this article, the that can be used to safeguard and repair self-esteem.
authors ask, What determines whether people will use For example, people can dismiss the validity of nega-
direct, remedial actions, or defensive actions? In three tive feedback by rejecting it as inaccurate or biased
studies, they tested the hypothesis that a belief in fixed (Baumeister, 1998; Kernis, 2003; Kunda, 1990). If
intelligence (entity theory) would produce defensive- undermining the feedback itself is impractical, people
ness, whereas a belief in improvable intelligence (incre- can compare their performance with relatively worse off
mental theory) would foster remediation. In each study, others (Taylor & Lobel, 1989; Wills, 1981) or compen-
participants assigned to the entity condition opted for sate for their shortcomings symbolically (Wicklund &
defensive self-esteem repair (downward comparison in Gollwitzer, 1982). Indeed, Tesser (2000) has docu-
Studies 1 and 3; a tutorial on already mastered material mented such a wide range of mechanisms for self-esteem
in Study 2), but those in the incremental condition repair that he has labeled them a “self-zoo” of inter-
opted for self-improvement (upward comparison in changeable processes. As noted, these processes also
Studies 1 and 3; a tutorial on unmastered material in share an important shortcoming: They are defensive in
Study 2). Experiment 3 also linked these strategies to nature (see Sherman & Cohen, 2002). Each of these
self-esteem repair; remedial strategies were the most processes is defensive because it restores lost self-esteem
effective in recovering lost self-esteem for those in the without addressing the underlying cause of the negative
incremental condition, whereas defensive strategies feedback. Instead, by employing these processes, people
were most effective for those in the entity condition. adjust to the problem psychologically rather than con-
fronting it head on (Folkman, Lazarus, Dunkel-Schetter,
Keywords: defensiveness; remediation; self-esteem; theories DeLongis, & Gruen, 1986; Heckhausen & Schultz,
of intelligence 1995; Rothbaum, Weisz, & Snyder, 1982). Research on
599
defensive mechanisms, however, may paint an incom- different. Negative feedback, while perhaps disappoint-
plete picture of self-esteem management processes, since ing, is not an indictment of a permanent ability but is
in most defensiveness paradigms participants are not part of a learning process (Blackwell, Trzesniewski, &
given the option of choosing a direct remedial response. Dweck, 2007; Hong, Chiu, Dweck, Lin, & Wan, 1999).
In one study that did offer the possibility of directly In a framework in which intelligence can be acquired,
responding to a threat, people did prefer a direct reso- exerting additional effort is typically the means of doing
lution to a defensive one (Stone, Wiegand, Cooper, & so. Moreover, because additional failed efforts do not
Aronson, 1997). reflect on a permanent ability, attempts at remediation
The aim of the current research is to explore what appear less risky. Therefore, although the self-esteem of
determines when people will respond to setbacks defen- both entity and incremental theorists might suffer as a
sively and when they will actively pursue remedial result of important negative feedback, the relative attrac-
strategies. We propose that one of the critical factors tiveness of direct remediation as a means of repairing
in determining this response is people’s perception of self-esteem might be substantially different.
the choices they have in responding to the situation There is already evidence of important differences in
(Heckhausen & Schulz, 1995; Lazarus & Folkman, entity and incremental theorists’ willingness to pursue
1984) and particularly whether they believe that direct remedial strategies, but it has not been linked to self-
improvement is possible (Heine et al., 2001, Study 3; esteem concerns. In a field study at the University of
Stone et al., 1997; Testa & Major, 1990). In keeping Hong Kong (Hong et al., 1999, Study 2), students
with Heckhausen and Schulz’s (1995) contention that who had performed poorly on their English proficiency
people generally resort to indirect psychological adjust- examinations—a subject that is essential to success at the
ment (secondary control) only when they perceive a university because all classes are taught in English—were
direct option (primary control) to be impractical, we less interested in taking remedial English classes when
propose that people tend to respond defensively to neg- they held an entity theory of intelligence than when they
ative feedback primarily when they perceive that an held an incremental theory. In a follow-up study, a sim-
attempt to improve on a poor performance may be ilar response was found among participants who were
futile or will present a further risk to their self-esteem. experimentally induced to hold either an entity or an
However, when improvement seems possible and not incremental theory of intelligence and given negative
unduly risky, people should opt to repair their self- performance feedback (Hong et al., 1999, Study 3).
esteem by directly confronting their poor performance. A consistent pattern was also apparent in a recent
Specifically, when receiving negative academic feedback, longitudinal study of students making the transition
people’s lay theories about the nature of intelligence— into middle school (Blackwell et al., 2007). In this
their beliefs about whether intelligence is fixed or study, incremental theorists were more likely than entity
improvable—should play an important role in their theorists to endorse working harder and spending more
response to this feedback (Dweck, 1999). Building on time studying next time in response to an academic set-
this framework, in the current research we seek to exper- back, whereas entity theorists were more likely than
imentally assess whether indirect, defensive processes incremental theorists to report that they would spend
(secondary control) are prevalent when people hold an less time on the subject and would attempt to avoid it
entity theory of intelligence (believing that intelligence altogether in the future (Blackwell et al., 2007, Study 1).
cannot be changed) and whether more direct, remedial Taken together, these studies provide convergent evi-
responses (primary control) will be more prevalent when dence that incremental theorists are more inclined to
people hold an incremental theory of intelligence (believ- pursue remedial strategies following a poor performance
ing that intelligence can be improved). than are entity theorists.
The link between persistence after failure and the
belief that skills can be developed is also supported by
Lay Theories of Intelligence and Remediation
cross-cultural work (Heine et al., 2001; White &
Entity theorists have good reason to be wary of Lehman, 2005). In one study by Heine and his col-
making efforts to remediate their shortcomings. From leagues (Heine et al., 2001, Study 3), persistence on a
their perspective of fixed intelligence, exerting additional difficult task was found to be a function of differences
effort in order to master a task may not seem to be par- in cultural beliefs about the efficacy of effort. These
ticularly effective. Moreover, in trying to improve they results are consistent with the hypothesis that people’s
risk repeating the failure, which could confirm their beliefs about the possibility of improvement play a role
inability and could further compromise their self-esteem. in determining whether or not they are willing to con-
For incremental theorists the situation appears quite front failure directly.
ostensibly scoring the test, all participants were told of intelligence. This debriefing procedure was followed
that they had scored in the 37th percentile of students in all subsequent studies. When probed, no participants
at their university. This predetermined score was reported suspicion that the theory induction article was
selected to be low enough to be a threat to participants’ intended to influence either their beliefs or actions.
self-esteem and yet leave open the possibility of both
upward and downward comparison. In order to ensure
that participants in both conditions had similar experi- RESULTS
ences, before proceeding to the comparisons, partici-
pants were asked whether they thought speed reading Preliminary Analyses
was useful and important, how well they thought they Participants in both conditions scored equally well
had done, and how much they would want to be a (Minc = 7.67, Ment = 7.79) on the eight-question reading
better speed reader, all on 7-point Likert-type scales. comprehension test that followed the article used for the
theory of intelligence induction, t(27) = 0.60, ns. These
Selection of Social Comparison Targets scores also ensured that participants had understood the
article well.
Finally, participants were given the opportunity to
There were no gender differences for any of the
engage in upward or downward comparisons in the
dependent variables, and all subsequent analyses are
context of reviewing strategies for success used by past
collapsed across gender.
participants who had achieved varying levels of perfor-
There were also no differences between the two con-
mance. They were presented with a table listing eight
ditions in their reports of how well they felt they had
past participants identified only by a participant identi-
done on the test (on 7-point scales, with higher values
fication number and their percentile score. There were
reflecting more positive responses), Mincremental = 3.73,
three strategies associated with each past participant,
SD = 0.93; Mentity = 3.64, SD = 1.34, t(27) = –0.21, ns;
and in order to review a strategy, participants simply
how helpful, Minc = 4.40, SD = 1.35; Ment = 4.07, SD =
had to click the link provided in the table and the strat-
1.07, t(27) = –0.72, ns, or important they believed speed
egy appeared on the right-hand side of the screen. The
reading to be, Minc = 4.53, SD = 1.25; Ment = 4.36, SD =
scores of the eight prior participants ranged from the
1.39; t(27) = –0.36, ns; or how much they would like to
14th to the 98th percentile, spaced at roughly even
be a better speed reader, Minc = 4.13, SD = 1.64; Ment =
intervals. Lower scoring participants were associated
3.57, SD = 1.70, t(27) = -0.91, ns. Thus, it was not dif-
with less useful strategies, such as “I try to skim the text
ferences in any of these beliefs about speed reading that
as quickly as possible while trying to understand as
led to any subsequent differences in behavior by partic-
much as I can.” In contrast, strategies associated with
ipants in the two conditions.1
higher scoring participants were selected to be more
instructive, for instance, “I read the first and last sen-
Social Comparison Choices
tences of each paragraph the most carefully. Those usu-
ally have the most information, and I can skim the rest Before proceeding to the analysis of which strategies
more quickly.” Participants were told that they were participants chose to examine, we should note that on
free to review as many strategies as they liked and could average, participants chose to examine approximately
move on to the next part of the study whenever they felt one third of the 24 available strategies (M = 8.76, SD =
ready. They were not explicitly told whether or not 4.82). Participants in the incremental condition (incre-
there would be any further speed-reading tasks, but we mental participants) reviewed somewhat more strategies
ensured that there was sufficient time left in the session (M = 10.0, SD = 4.26) than those in the entity condition
that this remained a distinct possibility. (entity participants; M = 7.4, SD = 5.12), but this differ-
When participants were finished reviewing strategies, ence was not significant, t(27) = –1.46, p = .16.
they were told that the study was over, and they were Recall that our central hypothesis was that partici-
fully debriefed about the goals and procedures of the pants would prefer to examine different strategies
study. Particular care was taken to explain that the depending on whether they had been induced to have an
theory induction article was fictitious and that there was entity or an incremental theory of intelligence. In order
no final consensus in the scientific community about the to test this hypothesis, we analyzed participants’ choice
nature of intelligence but that intelligence was generally of strategies in terms of the average level of strategy
understood to have both stable and changeable qualities. chosen, the first three strategies chosen, and the pro-
Participants were also provided with references for fur- portion of strategies chosen that were true downward
ther reading both about the nature of intelligence and comparisons. We began by comparing the average level
research on the psychological effects of people’s theories of strategy that participants chose to examine. Because
DISCUSSION
the social comparison targets’ percentile scores were
roughly evenly spaced, we coded the strategies associ- Consistent with our hypotheses, participants induced
ated with the lowest score a 1 and the one associated to hold entity and incremental theories of intelligence
with the highest score an 8. By this measure, incremen- displayed different patterns of social comparison fol-
tal participants chose to examine significantly “better” lowing negative feedback. Entity participants preferred
strategies (M = 5.41, SD = 0.92) than did entity partic- to examine the strategies of relatively lower performers,
ipants (M = 3.30, SD = 2.33), t(27) = –3.25, p < .01, with over 40% of their comparison targets being down-
d = 1.2 (see Figure 1). ward comparisons. As most research on defensiveness
Simply comparing the average strategy, however, suggests, entity participants responded to negative feed-
may obscure an important aspect of participants’ social back with a defensive self-esteem-restoring process.
comparison choice. Participants choosing to examine Incremental participants, however, responded in a dif-
the best (or worst) strategies first would have nowhere to ferent manner. Having been induced to believe in the
go but down (or up) after exhausting the top (or bot- malleability of intelligence, they engaged in very little
tom) choices, thus artificially moving the means closer downward comparison, choosing instead to examine
together. Also, it is important to determine which the strategies of high performers and providing them-
strategies participants chose to examine first, since these selves with the opportunity to improve. These results
should provide the best indication of their initial complement the findings of Testa and Major (1990),
response to the opportunity to engage in social compar- who report that following an initial failure, participants
ison following their poor performance. Therefore, we were more interested in downward comparison infor-
also compared the first three strategies that participants mation when they believed that they were unlikely to
opted to examine. Comparing participants’ first three improve on a subsequent test than when they thought
choices, we found again that incremental participants improvement was possible.
opted to examine significantly better strategies (M = It is true, however, that it may not be particularly
7.11, SD = 1.40) than participants in the entity condi- damaging to respond defensively to a setback on a task,
tion (M = 3.31, SD = 3.12), t(27) = –4.29, p < .001. such as speed reading, that may not figure prominently
Notably, incremental participants showed interest in in one’s life. It is considerably more costly to do so
even higher comparisons in their first three choices than when one is performing poorly on a task that is vital to
in their overall choices, t(14) = –4.49, p = .001, but success in a domain central to one’s identity. Therefore,
entity participants made equally low comparison in Experiment 2, participants were given feedback on
choices throughout, t(13) = –0.023, ns. just such an identity-relevant task and were then given
Finally, it is important to note that because partici- the opportunity to do a tutorial that would help them
pants believed they had scored in the 37th percentile, not improve on their shortcomings.
have behaved similarly regardless of the valence of feed- self-esteem and is more situationally responsive than a
back. The distracter-task condition was included to con- typical trait self-esteem scale, allowing us to observe
firm that it was in fact the social comparison process changes in self-esteem throughout the course of the study.
that helped repair participants’ self-esteem and not The “performance” component scale solicits participants’
merely the distraction of doing another task or the agreement with seven statements such as “I feel as smart
simple passage of time. as others,” “I feel confident that I understand things,” and
In replicating Study 1 with these additional measures “I feel like I'm not doing well” (reverse scored), on a 7-
and conditions, we were able to more carefully test our point scale. The full version of the scale also includes sub-
hypotheses in a 3 (positive feedback vs. negative feed- scales for social and appearance self-esteem; these scales
back vs. negative feedback without social comparison) × 2 were not included in the present research.
(entity theory vs. incremental theory) design. We antici-
pated that the more that entity participants lost self- Theory Induction
esteem, the more they would engage in downward
As in Studies 1 and 2, participants were randomly
comparison. We also expected that the more they
assigned to do a “baseline” reading exercise that was a sci-
employed this defensive response, the more they would
entific article supporting either an entity or an incremental
recover lost self-esteem. For incremental participants, we
theory of intelligence. After reading the article, they were
anticipated that the more their self-esteem dropped in
given a five-question reading comprehension test that
response to negative feedback, the more they should
allowed us to ensure that they read the article carefully.
engage in upward comparison, and the more they
engaged in upward comparison (with its skill-building Self-Esteem Threat
information), the more they would recover self-esteem.
Moreover, if our hypotheses are correct, then we Recall that we had three conditions for entity and incre-
should not see this divergent pattern of social compari- mental participants: one in which participants received neg-
son from entity and incremental participants when they ative feedback (a replication of Study 1; nent = 13, ninc = 15),
receive positive feedback, because in this condition their one in which participants received positive feedback (nent =
self-esteem is not under threat. Finally, if it is actually 13, ninc = 13), and one in which participants received nega-
the opportunity to be defensive or to seek improvement tive feedback but completed a distracter task instead of the
that allows participants to recover self-esteem, then self- social comparison task (nent = 13, ninc = 13). Accordingly, par-
esteem should not be recovered when instead of social ticipants in the first and third condition received feedback that
comparison, participants do a distracter task. they had scored in the 37th percentile on the speed-reading
test. Participants in the positive feedback condition were told
that they had scored in the 91st percentile.
METHOD
Selection of Social Comparison Targets
Participants and Design The strategy-review portion of the study was slightly
Eighty undergraduates at a West Coast university adjusted in Study 3. Instead of allowing participants an
(38 females and 42 males) were recruited to participate unlimited number of social comparisons, we sought to
in exchange for course credit. The basic design of the equate the number of strategies participants examined.
study was identical to Study 1 but included two new Thus, the computer program automatically moved on
conditions (positive feedback and distraction) and the after participants had reviewed five strategies. In addi-
tracking of participants’ self-esteem. tion, there were now nine levels of strategy to choose
from, rather than eight as in Study 1, providing an addi-
tional downward comparison option.
Self-Esteem Tracking
Participants in the distracter-task condition were not
Participants’ state self-esteem was measured using the given the opportunity to review any strategies. Instead,
performance subscale of Heatherton and Polivy’s (1991) they completed a task that required them to concentrate
State Self-Esteem Scale at three different points: first, at on words that appeared on the screen and to click as
the very outset of the study (Time 1), then immediately soon as a target word appeared. The task was designed
following feedback on the speed-reading task (Time 2), to take roughly the same amount of time as the strategy
and finally at the end of the study (Time 3). review. It was also selected to require concentration in
The performance subscale of Heatherton and Polivy’s order to minimize participants’ opportunity to engage
(1991) State Self-Esteem Scale measures participants’ current in alternate means of self-esteem repair.
Feedback Condition
n 13 13 15 13 13 13
Baseline self-esteem (Time 1) 43.08 45.46 45.67 43.38 45.08 44.46
Self-esteem after feedback (Time 2) 42.38 42.62 36.73 34.69 36.54 36.92
Final self-esteem (Time 3) 43.00 43.08 43.60 41.85 39.85 37.46
Level of social comparison (1-9) 5.34a 5.11a 6.63b 3.06c
NOTE: Means in the same row with different subscripts are significantly different from one another at the p < .05 level.
Figure 3 Feedback Condition. Level of State Self-Esteem (out of 49) at Baseline (Time 1), Following Feedback (Time 2), and Following Social
Comparison or Distracter Task (Time 3) Among Participants in Each of the Three Feedback Conditions: Positive Feedback and Social
Comparison, Negative Feedback and Social Comparison, and Negative Feedback With No Social Comparison (Study 3)
of feedback condition but independent of theory condi- Having established that self-esteem levels followed
tion (see Figure 3). Additional t tests confirmed that the predicted patterns according to feedback condition,
there were no differences in self-esteem as a function of we next focused specifically on the negative feedback
theory condition in any of the feedback conditions at condition, which replicated Study 1, and examined the
any time, all ts < 1, ps > .40 (see Table 1). relationship between social comparison and self-esteem
Follow-up analyses revealed that at Time 1 (baseline), change in that condition as a function of our theory of
there were no self-esteem differences among the condi- intelligence manipulation. Accordingly, we tested the
tions, F(2, 77) = .02, ns. At Time 2 (immediately fol- hypotheses that (a) participants’ drop in self-esteem
lowing feedback), a planned contrast indicated that would predict their social comparison choices and that
participants in the two conditions that received negative this would be moderated by condition and (b) that
feedback had lower self-esteem than those who received participants’ social comparison choices would in turn
positive feedback t(77) = −2.44, p = .02. This was, in predict the rebound in their self-esteem levels and that
essence, a manipulation check confirming that partici- this, too, would be moderated by condition. In other
pants’ self-esteem dropped following negative feedback. words, we predicted that participants would restore
Finally, at Time 3 (following social comparison), a planned their self-esteem following negative feedback using
contrast revealed that as hypothesized, participants who social comparison, but that entity participants would do
had the opportunity to engage in social comparison had this using downward comparison, while incremental
significantly higher self-esteem than those who were given theorists would use upward comparison.
a distracter task instead t(77) = 2.13, p = .04. As can be First, we tested the hypothesis that participants’
seen from Figure 3, all participants receiving negative social comparison choices would be a function of the
feedback experienced a drop in self-esteem, but only those degree to which their self-esteem dropped (operational-
who had the opportunity to engage in social comparison ized as self-esteem at Time 2 – Time 1), but that this
recovered their self-esteem by Time 3. effect would be moderated by our manipulation of their
= 0.49
***
= 0.77*** = 0.33* = 0.86*
r = .64** r = .58** r = .57 ** Upward r = .50 *
Downward
Drop in Recovery of Drop in Social Recovery of
Social
Self-Esteem Self-Esteem Self-Esteem Comparison Self-Esteem
Comparison
Figure 4 The Relationship Between Drop in Self-Esteem and Social Comparison and the Relationship Between Social Comparison and the
Recovery of Self-Esteem for Participants Receiving Negative Feedback in the Entity Theory (4a) and Incremental Theory (4b)
Conditions (Study 3)
* p < .10. **p < .05. ***p < .01.
theory of intelligence. We conducted an ordinary least well as the interaction between these two variables, as
squares (OLS) regression analysis that included partici- predictors of the improvement in their state self-esteem
pants’ theory of intelligence (dummy coded 0 for the following social comparison. There was a marginally
incremental condition and 1 for the entity condition) significant main effect of social comparison choices, β =
and the drop in participants’ self-esteem (mean cen- 0.86, t(24) = 1.81, p = .08, and no main effect of theory,
tered, M = –8.82), as well as the interaction between β = 0.01, t(24) = 0.04, ns. These effects were qualified
these two variables, as predictors of their social com- by a significant interaction between theory of intelli-
parison choices. There was a marginally significant gence and social comparison choices, β = –1.16, t(24) =
main effect of self-esteem drop, β = –0.33, t(24) = –1.82, –2.94, p = .01. The simple slopes analyses revealed that
p = .08, and a significant main effect of theory, for participants in the entity condition, downward
β = –0.72, t(24) = –6.38, p = .001, in predicting social social comparison was a significant predictor of self-esteem
comparison choices. However, these main effects were recovery, β = –0.77, t(24) = –2.72, p = .01; the lower
qualified by a significant interaction between theory of these participants’ social comparison targets, the more
intelligence and self-esteem drop, β = 0.64, t(24) = 3.53, their self-esteem rose (see Figure 4a). The opposite was
p = .002. Following Aiken and West (1991), we con- true for participants in the incremental condition, for whom
ducted simple slopes analyses to determine how partici- upward social comparison choices were a marginally sig-
pants’ theory of intelligence moderated the effect of nificant predictor of self-esteem recovery, β = 0.86, t(24) =
self-esteem drop on their social comparison choices. As 1.81, p = .08; the higher their social comparison targets,
hypothesized, for participants in the entity condition, the more self-esteem they recovered (see Figure 4b).
self-esteem drop was a significant predictor of social
comparison, β = 0.49, t(24) = 3.41, p = .002; the more
their self-esteem dropped, the more downward compar- DISCUSSION
isons they made (see Figure 4a). For participants in the
incremental condition, the effect of self-esteem drop had Studies 1 and 2 revealed that following negative feed-
a marginally significant, but opposite, effect on social back, entity participants tended to respond defensively
comparison choices, β = –0.33, t(24) = –1.82, p = .08; while incremental participants preferred to directly
the more their self-esteem dropped, the more upward address the cause of their poor performance. In Study 3,
comparisons they made (see Figure 4b). we found support for the hypothesis that these different
responses are a reaction to threatened self-esteem and
Self-Esteem Repair and Social Comparison
that both are effective means of restoring self-esteem for
Choices
those who employ them.
Although entity and incremental participants responded Among entity participants who received negative feed-
to self-esteem threat by making different social compar- back, the more their self-esteem dropped, the lower were
ison choices, we wanted to know whether or not these their social comparison targets; they responded to the
choices were effective in actually repairing self-esteem threat by defensively comparing themselves to others
(operationalized as self-esteem at Time 3 – Time 2). We who had performed relatively poorly. As it turns out, this
again conducted an OLS regression analysis that included was an effective response for entity participants, as the
participants’ theory (dummy coded 0 for the incremen- lower their social comparison targets, the more their self-
tal condition and 1 for the entity condition) and their esteem was restored (i.e., the more they once again agreed
social comparison choices (mean centered, M = 4.97), as with statements such as “I feel confident I understand
threatened, different responses are required. In the entity true profits, the results could be costly. If, fearing the dis-
self-system, with no clear way to change one’s level of approval of their mentors, surgical interns defensively
underlying ability, the most direct and effective thing dismiss criticism of their techniques or diagnoses, the
people can do is to re-adjust their thinking about their abil- results could be even worse. We contend that the evalu-
ity, and defensive processes fit the bill. In an incremental ative context can play an important role in determining
self-system, the most direct and effective thing people can the way negative feedback is interpreted and whether
do is to work to bring their skills into line with the level of one tries to learn from it or responds to it defensively
competence they desire. instead. While an evaluative context focused on nurtur-
ing development may not be ideal for every setting, it
The Limitations of Self-Improvement may be effective in reducing defensiveness in settings
where learning is valued and the goal is to minimize lost
As noted, because all three of the current studies were
opportunities to improve.
designed to compel participants to choose between an
In conclusion, defensiveness can stand in the way of
available remedial response and a defensive one, striving
learning and thus have serious consequences in class-
for self-improvement could be considered the more adap-
rooms and workplaces. This research shows that an
tive response in each case. However, there are certainly
incremental theory has a role to play in leading people
instances in which disengagement may be a better response
away from defensiveness and toward confronting and
than prolonged persistence in a futile effort to improve
addressing their shortcomings.
(Wrosch, Scheier, Carver, & Schulz, 2003; Wrosch,
Scheier, Miller, Schulz, & Carver, 2003; Wrosch, Schulz,
& Heckhausen, 2004), or instances in which dealing with
the psychological effects of failure may be more important NOTES
than dealing with the failure itself. At times, dealing with 1. In reporting their attitudes toward speed reading, participants
the psychological effects of failure may be a prelude to cop- had an apparent opportunity to restore self-esteem by disparaging the
ing with the actual failure. Indeed, the stress and coping lit- importance of speed reading. However, their willingness and ability to
pursue this route of self-esteem repair was constrained by the fact that
erature (e.g., Lazarus & Folkman, 1984) maintains that they had been told, immediately prior to the test, that it was a valid and
dealing with stress—of which negative feedback can be one important indicator of competence and a predictor of future success.
instance—usually requires a mix of problem-focused cop- 2. Three participants in the entity condition made no social com-
parisons and were therefore excluded from this analysis.
ing (directly addressing the source of the stress) and 3. Females reported having taken marginally fewer engineering-
emotion-focused coping (dealing with the emotions result- related classes (M = 5.1) than males (M = 10.5), t(24) = 2.02, p = .06.
ing from the stress). Thus, employing defensive processes Females also reported greater interest in doing a generic tutorial (M =
7.5) than did males (M = 5.6), t(24) = –2.57, p < .05, and were mar-
to repair one’s self-esteem may be adaptive when a direct ginally more interested in the tutorial for the module they had done
response is either impossible or when protecting self- poorly on (M = 7.5) than were males (M = 6.2), t(24) = 0.08. However,
esteem sustains motivation and facilitates later attempts at females made up a similar percentage of participants in the entity con-
dition (53%) as in the incremental condition (45%), and gender does
engaging one’s problems (Nussbaum & Steele, 2007; not interact with any of the dependent variables of interest.
Sherman & Cohen, 2006; Wayment & Taylor, 1995;
Wood, Taylor, & Lichtman, 1985; see also Heckhausen &
Schulz, 1995; Rothbaum et al., 1982).
REFERENCES
The Evaluative Context Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and
interpreting interactions. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
In the current studies, we manipulated participants’ Baumeister, R. F. (1998). The self. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, &
theories of intelligence by directly targeting their beliefs. G. Lindzey (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (4th ed.;
We suggest, however, that similar effects could result pp. 680-740). New York: McGraw-Hill.
Bergen, R. (1991). Beliefs about intelligence and achievement-related
from a manipulation of participants’ evaluative context, behaviors. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of
either by creating an environment that primes one theory Illinois, Urbana-Champaign.
or the other (Ross, Nussbaum, & Dweck, n.d.) or by Berglas, S., & Jones, E. E. (1978). Drug choice as a self-handicapping
strategy in response to noncontingent success. Journal of
creating an environment in which people feel their fixed Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 405-417.
intelligence is being judged or their malleable intelligence Blackwell, L. S., Trzesniewski, K. H., & Dweck, C. S. (2007). Implicit
is being cultivated (Murphy & Dweck, 2006). Whether theories of intelligence predict achievement across an adolescent
transition: A longitudinal study and an intervention. Child
in a classroom, a hospital, or a boardroom, the theory of Development, 78, 246-263.
intelligence conveyed in a setting may function to reduce Crocker, J., Brook, A. T., Niiya, Y., & Villacorta, M. (2006). The
or exacerbate defensive behaviors. If, when facing the pursuit of self-esteem: Contingencies of self-worth and self-regulation.
Journal of Personality, 74, 1749-1772.
critical gaze of a board of directors, CEOs are unwilling Crocker, J., & Park, L. E. (2004). The costly pursuit of self-esteem.
to admit to past mistakes and distort their company’s Psychological Bulletin, 130, 392-414.
Dweck, C. S. (1999). Self-theories: Their role in motivation, person- Rothbaum, F., Weisz, J. R., & Snyder, S. S. (1982). Changing the
ality, and development. New York: Psychology Press. world and changing the self: A two-process model of perceived
Dweck, C. S., Higgins, E. T., & Grant, H. (2003). Self-systems give unique control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42, 5-37.
meaning to self variables. In M. R. Leary & J. P. Tangney (Eds.), Sherman, D. K., & Cohen, G. L. (2002). Accepting threatening infor-
Handbook of self and identity (pp. 239-252). New York: Guilford. mation: Self-affirmation and the reduction of defensive biases.
Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social comparison processes. Human Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11, 119-123.
Relations, 7, 117-140. Sherman, D. K., & Cohen, G. L. (2006). The psychology of self-
Folkman, S., Lazarus, R. S., Dunkel-Schetter, C., DeLongis, A., & defense: Self-affirmation theory. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances
Gruen, A. R. (1986). Dynamics of a stressful encounter: Cognitive in experimental social psychology (Vol. 38, pp. 183-242). San
appraisal, coping, and encounter outcomes. Journal of Personality Diego, CA: Academic Press.
and Social Psychology, 50, 992-1003. Stone, J., Wiegand, A. W., Cooper, J., & Aronson, E. (1997). When
Heatherton, T. F., & Polivy, J. (1991). Development and validation of exemplification fails: Hypocrisy and the motive for self-integrity.
a scale for measuring state self-esteem. Journal of Personality and Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 54-65.
Social Psychology, 60, 895-910. Taylor, S. E., & Lobel, M. (1989). Social comparison activity under
Heckhausen, J., & Schulz, R. (1995). A life-span theory of control. threat: Downward evaluation and upward contacts. Psychological
Psychological Review, 102, 284-304. Review, 96, 569-575.
Heine, S. J., Kitayama, S., Lehman, D. R., Takata, T., Ide, E., Leung, Tesser, A. (1988). Toward a self-evaluation maintenance model of social
C., et al. (2001). Divergent consequences of success and failure in behavior. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psy-
North America and Japan: An investigation of self-improving chology (Vol. 21, pp. 181-227). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
motivations and malleable selves. Journal of Personality and Tesser, A. (2000). On the confluence of self-esteem maintenance mech-
Social Psychology, 81, 599-615. anisms. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 4, 290-299.
Hong, Y., Chiu, C., Dweck, C. S., Lin, D. M., & Wan, W. (1999). Testa, M., & Major, B. (1990). The impact of social comparison after
Implicit theories, attributions, and coping: A meaning system failure: The moderating effects of perceived control. Basic and
approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 588-599. Applied Social Psychology, 44, 672-682.
Kernis, M. H. (2003). Toward a conceptualization of optimal self- Wayment, H. A., & Taylor, S. E. (1995). Self-evaluation processes:
esteem. Psychological Inquiry, 14, 126. Motives, information use, and self-esteem. Journal of Personality,
Kunda, Z. (1990). The case for motivated reasoning. Psychological 63, 729-757.
Bulletin, 108, 480-498. White, K., & Lehman, D. R. (2005). Culture and social comparison
Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and coping. seeking: The role of self-motives. Personality and Social Psychology
New York: Springer. Bulletin, 31, 232-242.
Leary, M. R. (2005). Interpersonal cognition and the pursuit of social Wicklund, R. A., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (1982). Symbolic self-completion.
acceptance: Inside the sociometer. In M. Baldwin (Ed.), Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Interpersonal cognition (pp. 85-102). New York: Guilford. Wills, T. A. (1981). Downward comparison principles in social psy-
Molden, D. C., & Dweck, C. S. (2006). Finding “meaning” in chology. Psychological Bulletin, 90, 245-271.
psychology: A lay theories approach to self-regulation, social Wood, J. V., Taylor, S. E., & Lichtman, R. L. (1985). Social compar-
perception, and social development. American Psychologist, 61, ison in adjustment to breast cancer. Journal of Personality and
192-302. Social Psychology, 49, 1169-1183.
Morse, S., & Gergen, K. J. (1970). Social comparison, self-consistency, Wrosch, C., Scheier, M. F., Carver, C. S., & Schulz, R. (2003). The
and the concept of the self. Journal of Personality and Social importance of goal disengagement in adaptive self-regulation:
Psychology, 16, 148-156. When giving up is beneficial. Self and Identity, 2, 1-20.
Murphy, M. C., & Dweck, C. S. (2006). So that’s what smart is: Wrosch, C., Scheier, M. F., Miller, G. E., Schulz, R., & Carver, C. S.
Environment theories of intelligence influence self-presentation (2003). Adaptive self-regulation of unattainable goals: Goal disen-
and core values. Manuscript submitted for publication. gagement, reengagement, and subjective well-being. Personality
Niiya, Y., Crocker, J., & Bartmess, E. N. (2004). From vulnerability and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 1494-1508.
to resilience: Learning orientations buffer contingent self-esteem Wrosch, C., Schulz, R., & Heckhausen, J. (2004). Health stresses and
from failure. Psychological Science, 15, 801-805. depressive symptomatology in the elderly: A control-process view.
Nussbaum, A. D., & Steele, C. M. (2007). Situational disengagement Current Direction in Psychological Science, 13, 17-20.
and persistence in the face of adversity. Journal of Experimental
Social Psychology, 43, 127-134.
Ross, J. S., Nussbaum, A. D., & Dweck, C. S. (n.d.). [The effects of Received January 17, 2007
contextual cues on theories of intelligence]. Unpublished raw data. Revision accepted October 13, 2007