Kimball 1986
Kimball 1986
Kimball (1986)
The social cognitive theory argues that we may be
enculturated by observing the behavior of others.
This includes the development of our gender roles
and/or gender identity.
Background information
The sociocultural approach argues that media - whether it be digital or paper - can have a significant effect
on our behavior. The social cognitive theory argues that the modeling of behaviors by characters that
children identify with, or want to identify with, will affect their own behavior. One behavior that has been
explored with regard to the role of modeling in the media is the development of gender roles and gender
stereotyping.
Social cognitive theory is based on two basic mechanisms of learning. The first is modeling. The theory
argues that children learn gendered behavior by watching others, including parents, peers, teachers, and
the media. Children learn best from same-gender models. In addition, children learn to imitate behavior if
the model is rewarded for his/her behavior. Finally, attractive role models are particularly influential.
The second mechanism of social cognitive theory is direct tuition, in which children are encouraged and
rewarded for engaging in behaviors considered appropriate for their sex, and are discouraged from gender-
inappropriate activities. Parents play an important role in this way of learning as they are the ones that
provide toys for their children and choose what to read them before going to bed at night. However, once a
child enters preschool, peers also engage in direct tuition by reinforcing gender-typed toy selection and
gendered behavior.
The following classic study looked at the role of television in the development of gender stereotypes in
elementary school children.
Kimball (1986) carried out a natural experiment, taking advantage of the introduction of television in a
remote area of Canada. She wanted to see if exposure to "normal television viewing" would lead to a
change in the level of gender stereotyping in a Northern Canadian community.
The participants were 536 children in four different communities. The sample consisted of 130 children
from Notel (that is, No Television), 135 from Unitel (one station), 166 from Multitel (more than one station),
and 105 from Vancouver. Both Unitel and Multitel were from the same geographic region. Vancouver was
used as a control.
The children's level of gender stereotyping was measured using the Sex Role Differentiation (SRD) scale.
The SRD asks children to rate how appropriate or frequent certain behaviors are for boys and girls their
own age, as well as how often their mothers or fathers perform certain tasks.
The SRD was administered to all students in grades 6 and 9 in each of the three towns both before and 2
years after television was introduced to Notel. The control group's data was obtained from a previous study
that was carried out 8 months before this study began.
about:blank 1/3
05/12/2024, 11:57 Page printer
The children filled in the questionnaire during normal
class time. Since information about the children's parents
was requested, the responses were anonymous.
* If you would like to read a graphic "novel" about this study, please see Stuart McMillan's fantastic site.
Evaluation
The study is a natural experiment, so ecological validity is high. However, since the researcher did not
manipulate the independent variable, participants were not randomly allocated to conditions, and there was
no ability to control for extraneous variables, the study has low internal validity and causality cannot be
determined.
The study is quite dated, so it is difficult to determine the study's temporal validity - that is, whether the
findings can be generalized to modern society. However, there is modern research that shows similar
findings (see the box below).
Since the data were anonymous, this means that the researchers did not compare data from Phase 1 and
Phase 2 for specific children. Instead, the study is cross-sectional. The researchers could not see
changes in the gender stereotypes of individual children over time.
Going deeper
Coyne et al (2016) examined children's level of engagement with Disney Princess media and gender-
stereotypical behavior.
Participants consisted of 198 children (mean age of 4.8 years old). The children were tested twice,
approximately one year apart. Longitudinal findings showed that Disney Princess engagement was
associated with more female gender-stereotypical behavior 1 year later.
The researchers argue that being "feminine" is not a problem. However, stereotypical female behavior may
potentially be problematic if girls believe that their opportunities in life are limited because of preconceived
notions regarding gender or if they avoid the types of exploration and activities that are important to
children learning about the world in order to conform to stereotypical notions about femininity (Coyne, p
1921).
Dinella (2013) found that grown women who self-identified as “princesses” gave up more easily on a
challenging task, were less likely to want to work, and were more focused on superficial qualities.
Casey, Novick, and Lourenco (2021) recently published a study to investigate how gender-stereotyped
roles in children's books have changed since 1960. They analyzed 3280 children's books published
between 1960 and 2020 to determine the proportion of male and female protagonists. The following graph
shows their findings.
about:blank 2/3
05/12/2024, 11:57 Page printer
The researchers found that although
the proportion of female
protagonists has increased over this
60-year period, male protagonists
remain overrepresented even in
recent years.
The researchers noted that their study shares a common limitation of such research - their analyses do not
reflect actual reading rates. In other words, they analyzed children’s books to estimate general trends in
gender roles, but they did not measure the popularity of the books and the frequency that they are read by
various age groups.
about:blank 3/3