0% found this document useful (0 votes)
3 views16 pages

Psych Core Studies

The document discusses various psychological studies focusing on social behavior, including Milgram's obedience study, Perry et al.'s research on personal space, and Piliavin et al.'s investigation into bystander apathy. It highlights the influence of social contexts, authority, and empathy on behavior, as well as the implications of these findings for understanding human interactions. Additionally, it covers Bandura's work on aggression and social learning, emphasizing the impact of observational learning on children's behavior.
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
0% found this document useful (0 votes)
3 views16 pages

Psych Core Studies

The document discusses various psychological studies focusing on social behavior, including Milgram's obedience study, Perry et al.'s research on personal space, and Piliavin et al.'s investigation into bystander apathy. It highlights the influence of social contexts, authority, and empathy on behavior, as well as the implications of these findings for understanding human interactions. Additionally, it covers Bandura's work on aggression and social learning, emphasizing the impact of observational learning on children's behavior.
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
You are on page 1/ 16

SOCIAL APPROACH

assumptions –
-​ our behaviour, cognitions and emotions can be influenced by actual,
implied or imagined presence of others
-​ all of our behaviour, cognitions and emotions can be influenced by
social contexts, social environments and groups

1.​ Milgram (obedience)


●​ psychology being investigated: destructive obedience, i.e, obedience
that has the potential to cause psychological or physical harm or injury
to another; to prove that the ‘germans are different’ hypothesis that
arose post-holocaust was wrong.

●​ aim: to investigate how obedient individuals would be to orders received


from a person in authority

●​ research method & design: controlled observation; p’s levels of


obedience measured through observation using operationalised
definitions for maximum voltage given; p’s body language and verbal
comments were noted

●​ sample: 40 men between 20 and 50 years; volunteer sample; recruited


from New Haven area, USA from different backgrounds and
occupations

●​ procedure:
-​ participants were promised $4.50 for participating, not conditional on
their completing the study
-​ study took place in lab at yale uni
-​ p’s were told they’d be allocated roles of either ‘teacher’ or ‘learner’, in
reality all p’s were allocated teacher
-​ p was taken to another room where stooge was strapped to a chair with
attached electrodes
-​ p presented w/ a shock generator with voltage readings from 15V to
450V in ascending order with words like ‘moderate’ or ‘danger’, final two
switches labelled ‘XXX’
-​ p was given 45V shock as demonstration
-​ p seated behind a wall - could hear but not see stooge
-​ experimenter remained w/ p whole time - 31-year old dressed in grey
technician’s coat and serious behaviour
-​ p’s instructed in memory task - read pairs of words aloud to learner and
then test on recognition, if learner made mistakes, p’s had to administer
shock, increase level by 15V for each error
-​ stooge followed pre-set plan of mistakes
-​ learner began to bang wall in protest from 300V onwards
-​ p’s told that no answer was equivalent to wrong and required shock
-​ when p’s protested, exp gave verbal prods such as ‘please go on’,
‘please continue’, ‘you must go on’
-​ procedure considered complete if p refused to give more shocks or if
450V shock had been given
-​ p was finally interviewed and deception was explained, given chance to
meet learner to ensure they weren’t injured

●​ controls: same experimenter in same clothes, same pre-set mistakes


by learner, same verbal prods in same order

●​ results:
-​ p’s estimated pain level of 450V shock on a scale of 0-14, mean
estimate was 13.42 → p’s believed they was causing serious pain
-​ some p’s protested against orders but verbal prods convinced them to
go on
-​ after procedure ended, p’s showed signs of relief
-​ minority of p’s didn’t show elevated levels of stress and appeared calm
throughout
-​ 5/40 stopped at intense shock (255-300V)
-​ 8/40 stopped at extreme intensity shock (315-360V)
-​ 1/40 stopped at danger: severe shock (375-420V)
-​ 26/40 gave maximum XXX shock (435-450V)

●​ conclusions:
1.​ individuals far more obedient to authority than expected
2.​ despite high levels of obedience, people find experience of carrying out
destructive acts under orders stressful due to conflict b/w two social
phenomena: need to obey those in authority, need to avoid harming
other people.

●​ strengths and weaknesses


-​ strict controls
-​ standardised procedure: reliability ↑
-​ realistic design of generator and example shock: validity ↑
-​ quantitative measurement of voltage: objectivity ↑
-​ qualitative measurement of p’s behaviour: rich data, subjectivity ↑
-​ sample w/ various backgrounds and occupations: external validity ↑
-​ low generalisability due to small sample

●​ ethical issues: deception, lack of informed consent, physical harm


(distress, guilt, etc)

●​ issues, approaches, debates


-​ raised awareness of the power of authority in the workplace
-​ introduction of whistle-blowing policies to encourage reporting of
mistakes
-​ highlighted importance of situational factors in influencing obedience
-​ role of individual factors as well

2. Perry et al. (personal space)


●​ psychology being investigated:
-​ interpersonal distance: relative distance between people, area of
space around a person in which they prefer not to have others enter
-​ oxytocin (OT): social hormone influencing personal space behaviour;
heightens importance of social cues and is linked to positive social
behaviour
-​ empathy: how people respond to the observed experiences of others,
feeling concerned or upset for them; plays a role in determining
personal space preferences
-​ social salience: importance or attention given to cues from other
people: body language, expressions, etc

●​ aim: to test the differential effect of the social hormone OT on personal


space preference in relation to a person’s empathy ability

●​ research method & design: lab experiment, uni of haifa, mixed exp
design to allocate p’s, 2 experiments, 3 IVs: empathy (natural, not
manipulated), treatment, condition, DV: personal space preferences
●​ sample: 54 male undergraduates, 19-32 years, participated for course
credit or payment, normal vision and no history of disorders; high IRI
group ( 40≥ ) had 20 p’s, low IRI group ( below 33 ) had 20 p’s

●​ procedure: experiments 1 and 2

precursors: (order counterbalanced)


1.​ OT administration: p’s randomly given OT solution or placebo;
double-blind technique: p’s and researchers didn’t know who got what
2.​ assessment of empathy: p’s completed IRI questionnaire

experiment 1: CID paradigm


-​ modified version of older interpersonal space test
-​ circle on computer screen, p’s asked to imagine themselves in the
centre and imagine a person approaching them along a radius
-​ p had to press keyboard space bar when they wanted person to stop
-​ four options for imagined other: close friend, stranger, authority figure,
rolling ball

experiment 2: choosing rooms


-​ p’s were shown pairs of similar rooms and had to choose their preferred
one, told info would be used to design room of their choice
-​ computerised stimuli were coloured pictures w/ two identical chairs,
table, cupboard, plant, lamp, clock
-​ E.G: preferred distance between chairs, angle of chairs positions
-​ C.G: distance between table and plant, angle of table and plant
positions

●​ controls: same rooms shown, same distances and angles, images


shown for 2s duration

●​ results:

Experiment 1
interaction effect: treatment x empathy
-​ OT decreased mean pref. dist. in high empathy group, increased mean
pref. dist. in low empathy group
-​ administration of OT had differential effect on high and low empathisers
-​ high empathy group: 23.29% (OT group) and 26.11% (placebo group)
-​ low empathy group: 30.20% (OT group) and 26.98% (placebo group)
interaction effect: treatment x condition x empathy
-​ high empathisers w/ placebo showed significant differences b/w friend
and authority, and friend and stranger
-​ high empathisers w/ OT showed same differences but w/ differences
b/w ball and stranger, and ball and authority

Experiment 2
-​ differences in preferences for chair distance but not angles
-​ high empathisers chose closer chair distances following OT admin than
placebo admin
-​ low empathisers chose farther chair distances following OT admin than
placebo admin
-​ interaction b/w treatment and empathy only seen in E.G; no effect of
empathy on distances b/w table and plant

●​ conclusions
-​ administration of OT enhances social cues in opposite ways for
individuals with different empathetic abilities, supporting idea of social
salience
-​ people need less distance between them and close friends than they
need with strangers

●​ strengths and weaknesses


-​ low ecological validity bcs lab exp
-​ tasks lacked mundane realism cause in everyday life our personal
space choices are influenced by social cues such as expressions, tone,
etc.
-​ ↑ internal validity - order of exp counterbalanced to prevent order effects
-​ extraneous variables controlled
-​ double-blind procedure - ↓ demand characteristics ↓ researcher bias
-​ ↑ standardisation - ↑ reliability

●​ ethical issues
-​ informed consent obtained
-​ no side effects from OT or placebo
-​ deception in exp 2 - fully debriefed

●​ issues, approaches, debates


-​ important implications for those with social difficulties
-​ shows effect of OT to be more complex due to its differential effect
-​ administering OT could actually cause further social difficulty
-​ situational factors matter in personal space preferences (e.g-
relationship w person), but so do individual factors (e.g.- empathy)

3. Piliavin et al (subway samaritans)


●​ psychology being investigated:
1.​ bystander apathy: a bystander not showing concern for a person in
need
2.​ diffusion of responsibility: an individual being less likely to take action
during an emergency in a group setting because of a reduced sense of
personal responsibility
3.​ modelling: when we watch a person perform the desired behaviour

●​ aim: to study bystander apathy and diffusion of responsibility in a


natural setting

●​ research method & design: field exp, took place in new york city
subway, IMD, four IVs: type and race of victim, behaviour of model, size
of bystander group; DV: level of bystander helping, operationalised as
total number of passengers who helped

●​ sample: people travelling on NYC subway between harlem and bronx


on weekdays between 11 am and 3 pm (opportunity sample), total
estimation: 4450 (44% black 55% white), mean number per carriage: 43

●​ procedure:
-​ four teams of student researchers carried it out
-​ two males and two females - female confederates sat adjacent to critical
area and observed and recorded data, male confederates took role of
victim and model
-​ victim stood at the pole in centre of critical area, model remained
standing
-​ each trial in same route with 7.5 min gap b/w two stations; 70 seconds
into journey, victim staggered and collapsed, remained lying on the floor
for a while till help received
-​ victim played by different males, aged 26-35 years, 3 white 1 black,
dressed in identical, casual clothing
-​ 38/103 trials: alcoholic victim; remaining 65: sober with black cane
-​ models: white males aged 24-29 years dressed informally
-​ trials split into five conditions:
●​ critical/early: critical area, 70 seconds to help
●​ critical/late: critical area, 150 seconds to help
●​ adjacent/early: adjacent area, 70 seconds to help
●​ adjacent/late: adjacent area, 150 seconds to help
●​ no model: model didn’t help till end of trial

●​ controls: same route, between same stops, same time intervals, similar
clothing, same action

●​ results:
-​ high frequency of helping recorded
-​ 60% of cases: more than one p helped
-​ p’s more likely to help victim w/cane than drunk victim
-​ cane victim: white victim (received help in 100% model and no model
trials), black victim (received help in 100% no model trials)
-​ drunk victim: white victim (100% in no model and 77% in model trials)
black victim (73% in no model and 67% in model trials)
-​ both black and white cane victims equally likely to receive help; black
victims received less help overall in drunk condition
-​ 90% of helpers were males
-​ early model intervention were more likely to result in helping behaviour
than waiting until 150 seconds had passed
-​ study found no evidence to support the diffusion of responsibility
-​ evidence suggested that when more passengers were present, rates of
helping were higher
-​ researchers proposed cost-benefit model wherein people weigh up the
pros and cons of helping and then decide whether to do so or not

●​ conclusion:
-​ someone using a cane will be helped more than a drunk person
-​ men more likely to help than women
-​ people more likely to help members of their own race, especially if
victim is drunk
-​ the longer an emergency continues, less likely it is that anyone will help

●​ strengths and weaknesses


-​ ↑ ecological validity due to field setting
-​ less control over extraneous variables such as weather, train delays
-​ p’s could’ve taken part in the exp twice, making them more likely to
help, creating demand characteristics
-​ sample may be unrepresentative as they’re all passengers travelling in
NYC subway, however large sample w/ various races makes it more
inclusive and thus, generalisable
-​ quantitative measure: ↑ objectivity
-​ qualitative data collected: more rich data

●​ ethical issues
-​ no consent taken
-​ no debriefing
-​ deception
-​ risk of psychological harm or guilt

●​ issues, approaches, debates


-​ tells us about specific situational factors that make bystanders more
likely to help
LEARNING APPROACH

assumptions –
-​ each life begins as a ‘blank slate’: observable changes to our behaviour
can result from interactions with our environment
-​ the process of social learning, operant conditioning, and classical
conditioning are the ways in which humans and animals learn. these
processes involve stimulus-response.

1.​ Bandura et al. (aggression)

●​ psychology being investigated: social learning, aggression, models

●​ aim: to investigate whether a child would learn aggression by observing


a model and would reproduce this behaviour in the absence of the
model, and whether the sex of the model was important.

4 hypotheses –
-​ obs. agg. beh. will be imitated; children seeing an agg. model will be
more agg. than those seeing a non-agg or no model
-​ obs. non-agg. beh. will be imitated so children seeing non-agg. models
will be less agg. than children seeing no model
-​ children more likely to copy same-sex model
-​ boys more likely to copy agg. than girls

●​ research method & design: lab exp, controlled environment, IMD, 3


IVs: model type, model sex, learner sex; DV: behaviour displayed by
child, measured using controlled observation

●​ sample: 72 children aged 3-6 (36b, 36g) from stanford uni nursery
school

●​ procedure:
-​ children were first observed in school and rated on four measures of
aggression - physical, verbal, agg. to inanimate objects, and aggression
inhibition (anxiety) on a five-point scale
-​ assigned to three groups, ensuring aggression levels were matched
-​ 51 p’s rated by two observers, rest rated by only 1 (inter-rater reliability)
-​ 12b and 12g assigned to no-model group (same assignment to
remaining two groups to same and opposite sex models)
-​ exp and child entered obs room, play area shown
-​ activities were taught to children
-​ opposite corner of room had toys: table, chair, Tinkertoy set, mallet,
bobo doll, inflatable clown-like doll – model sat here
-​ exp remained in room to ensure child wouldn’t refuse to be alone or try
to leave early
-​ non-agg condition: model assembled tinker toys for 10 mins
-​ agg condition: 1 minute, model attacked bobo doll (laid, sat on,
punched, hit with mallet, kicked, agg and non-agg verbal comments)
-​ exp procedure continued when all p’s were deliberately mildly annoyed
-​ test of child’s agg followed wherein they were observed for 20 mins from
a one-way mirror, i.e, p didn’t know they were being observed
-​ agg model group: test of delayed imitation in experimental room
containing bobo doll, mallet, peg board, dart gun, and non-agg toys like
tea set, crayons, ball, farm animals, etc – toys presented in same order
-​ children’s behaviours observed in 5-sec intervals using response
measures: imitative phy agg, imitative verbal agg, imitative non-agg
verbal responses
-​ partially imitative agg categories: mallet agg, sits on bobo doll
-​ further categories: agg gun play, non-imitative phy and verbal agg
-​ male model scored children’s behaviours and to test reliability, second
scorer independently rated half the children

●​ controls: deliberate annoyance, toys in same order

●​ results:
-​ children exposed to agg models were more agg physically and verbally
-​ imitative phy agg for boys seeing male model was 25.8
-​ imitative phy agg for girls seeing female model was 5.5
-​ girls imitated more phy agg when seeing male model and more verbal
agg when seeing same-sex model
-​ boys more likely to imitate same-sex models than girls
-​ children seeing non-agg model much less likely to exhibit aggression
-​ differences seen in non-agg play: girls played more with tea set and
colouring, boys more explorative and gun play
-​ records of remarks about agg models showed that some comments
seemed to be based on previous knowledge of sex-typed behaviour, i.e,
actions typically performed by one particular sex and seen more
appropriate for a sex – disapproving comments about female agg,
approving comments about male agg
●​ conclusion:
-​ obs agg behaviours are imitated: children seeing agg models more
likely to be more agg than those seeing non-agg or no model
-​ obs non-agg behaviours are imitated: children seeing non-agg models
less agg than those seeing no model
-​ children more likely to copy same-sex model, may depend on extent to
which behaviour is sex-typed
-​ boys more likely to copy agg than girls

●​ strengths and weaknesses:


-​ lab exp - ↑ controls
-​ standardised behaviours - ↑ validity ↑ reliability
-​ ↑ inter-observer reliability
-​ use of one-way mirror - ↓ demand characteristics
-​ clearly defined categories - ↑ validity ↑ reliability
-​ children matched to reduce risk of p variables
-​ only six children in each E.G from same background - ↓ generalisability
↓ validity, risk of sample bias
-​ quantitative and qualitative data collected

●​ ethical issues:
-​ children might have been harmed by becoming more agg
-​ seeing aggression could’ve been distressful for the children
-​ deliberate annoyance could’ve also been distressing

●​ issues, approaches, debates:


-​ clear implications: children exposed to violence at home or on TV and
are influenced by it
-​ many countries now have restrictions on television programs and films
-​ situational influence of models on acquisition of agg behaviours are
examples of nurture
-​ differences between boys and girls explained by nature AND nurture
-​ nature factors: hormones, brain development
-​ nurture factors: acquired stereotypes, sex-type behaviours rewarded
-​ shows individual and situational factors influence imitation
-​ unclear if informed consent from kids and parents was obtained
-​ use of children in study was more ideal as they may have much less
extraneous factors affecting their agg levels + less likely to suspect
2.​ Fagen et al. (elephant learning)

●​ psychology being investigated:


-​ operant conditioning: learning through consequences: reward/
punishment
-​ positive reinforcement: rewarding desirable behaviour to encourage
repetition
-​ positive reinforcer: reward for behaviour that fulfills a biological need
-​ secondary reinforcer: stimulus associated with primary reinforcers can
also be learned
-​ secondary positive reinforcement: training wherein a secondary
reinforcer is used followed by administration of a primary positive
reinforcer

●​ aim: to see whether free-contact, traditionally trained elephants can be


trained to participate in a trunk wash (to detect tuberculosis) by using
positive reinforcement

●​ research method & design: controlled and structured observation

●​ sample: 5 female elephants from nepal: 4 juvenile, 1 adult, all docile


and traditionally trained, no previous experience of SPR; spent most of
the day grazing under mahout’s control, diet of fresh grasses, grain and
nutritional supplements and given access to river during grazing hours

●​ procedure:
-​ chopped banana (primary reinforcer), short whistle blow (secondary
reinforcer)
-​ training sessions conducted with mahout present for safety but to the
side and no speaking or signalling
-​ purpose was to teach elephants to perform voluntary trunk wash in
several behavioural steps
-​ marker-reward relationship established, three methods taught: capture,
lure, shaping
-​ behavioural tasks: trunk here (lure), trunk up (lure and shaping), bucket
(lure), blow (capture and shaping), steady (shaping)
-​ task paired with verbal cue neither in english or nepali
-​ behavioural chaining followed individual behavioural tasks, wherein the
separately trained behaviours are performed in sequence
-​ tasks were first paired, then put in longer sequences
-​ trainer introduced syringe to trunk-here position using desensitisation
method, i.e, reading and extinguishing fear response to syringe stimulus
-​ to avoid elephants drinking saline or water, they were offered drinks
prior to sessions
-​ no time limit on stages of process, depended on success of elephants
-​ after 10 sessions, elephants tested every five sessions, passing score
was 80% (8/10 correct)

●​ results:
-​ 4 juvenile elephants successful, not adult
-​ reasons for adult’s failure: older, likely had some visual impairment or
trunk weakness, distracted, impatient, foot abscess
-​ mean success rate went from 39% after 10 sessions to 89.3% after 35
sessions

●​ conclusion: juvenile, free-contact elephants can be trained to


participate in a trunk wash using only SPR techniques; can be carried
out with voluntary participation of elephants avoiding punishment

●​ strengths and weaknesses:


-​ small sample - ↓ generalisability, not representative
-​ practical challenges: control for distractions and other extraneous
variables
-​ controlled observation - ↑ replicability
-​ attempts to maintain validity: mahouts asked to not speak, verbal cues
with no meaning in nepali or english - ↓ demand characteristics
-​ quantitative data - ↑ objectivity, easier to analyse

●​ ethical issues:
-​ elephants well-treated, no physical harm, freely grazed, fed adequately,
allowed to socialise, no punishment so minimised harm

●​ issues, approaches, debates


-​ study shows that using positive reinforcement learning techniques can
enhance captive animal welfare and help increase the well-being and
safety of those working with animals
-​ nurture-based approach as human training shaped the elephants’
behaviour
-​ however, as all the elephants didn’t acquire the learner behaviour, it
suggests the influence of some individual differences that may affect the
elephant’s ability to learn
-​ animals were ethically fed, housed, and treated

3.​ Saavedra and Silverman. (button phobia)

●​ psychology being investigated:


-​ classical conditioning: learning through association
-​ stages of classical conditioning: UCS triggers an UCR, and a NS
triggers no response before conditioning; UCS + NS triggers an UCR
during conditioning; CS triggers a CR after conditioning
-​ phobias: irrational, persistent fear of an object or event that poses little
real danger but creates anxiety and avoidance
-​ evaluative learning: form of classical conditioning wherein attitudes
towards stimuli are considered to be the product of complex thought
processes and emotions, leading to the individual’s negative perception

●​ aim: to examine the role of classical conditioning in relation to fear and


avoidance of a particular stimulus; to see if using a type of exposure
therapy could reduce the disgust and distress associated w/ buttons

●​ research method & design: clinical case study; data collected using
self-report measures

●​ sample: 9-year old hispanic american boy who had sought support from
child anxiety and phobia program at miami with his mom, met criteria for
having a specific phobia

●​ procedure
-​ boy and mother provided informed consent
-​ interviewed to understand background – phobia began at age of 5 when
boy knocked over bowl of buttons in front of class and teacher
-​ aversion to buttons steadily increased and was now interfering with
daily functioning
-​ researchers created hierarchy of feared stimuli with boy using feelings
thermometer
-​ boy treated with two interventions: contingency management and
imagery exposure therapy
1.​ contingency management
-​ form of positive reinforcement
-​ boy rewarded for showing less fear and actually handling buttons
-​ reward given to boy by mother after completion of gradual exposure to
buttons
-​ researchers observed how boy approached buttons and measured
subjective ratings of distress on feelings thermometer

2.​ imagery exposure therapy


-​ person asked to vividly imagine their feared object/stimulus, uses
visualisation techniques
-​ interviews revealed boy found buttons touching him disgusting and that
they smelled unpleasant
-​ disgust-related imagery exposures were incorporated with self-control
strategies
-​ boy asked to imagine buttons falling on him and describe how it made
him feel
-​ exposures progressed in line with boy’s fear hierarchy
-​ self-report measures on feelings thermometer taken every session

●​ results

1.​ positive reinforcement therapy


-​ successful completion of all exposure tasks
-​ boy approached buttons more positively
-​ subjective ratings increased significantly between sessions two and
three, and continued to rise
-​ despite behaviour towards buttons improving, feelings of disgust and
fear actually increased

2.​ imagery exposure therapy


-​ successful in reducing boy’s distress
-​ boy’s subjective ratings of disgust and fear reduced from 8 before
imagery therapy to 5 midway through IET and 3 at the end
-​ 6-month and 12-month follow-ups conducted following treatment
wherein boy reported feeling minimal distress about buttons, no longer
met diagnostic criteria for specific phobia, and feelings towards buttons
no longer affected normal functioning
●​ conclusion
-​ positive reinforcement therapy was successful in changing observable
behaviour, and IET was successful in reducing feelings of disgust and
fear
-​ emotions and cognitions relating to disgust are important when learning
new responses to phobic stimuli
-​ imagery exposure can have a long-term effect in reducing the distress
associated with specific phobias as it tackles negative evaluations

●​ strengths and weaknesses:


-​ small sample - not representative, ↓ generalisability
-​ different methods of data collection used - ↑ validity
-​ standardised measures (feelings thermometer)
-​ quantitative AND qualitative data collected
-​ study pretty subjective because self-reports used
-​ high risk of researcher bias - ↓ validity
-​ ↑ demand characteristics - may have affected ratings

●​ ethical issues:
-​ informed consent
-​ overall experience was distressing but the overall aim of improving
boy’s quality of life and minimise distress justified it
-​ boy’s anonymity preserved

●​ issues, approaches, debates:


-​ showed that methods such as disgust imagery exposure can be used in
clinical practice to help overcome feelings of fear and disgust
-​ demonstrates long-term improvement that can result from exposure
therapies
-​ relies on nurture-based explanation of learning
-​ phobias are not considered innate or genetically inherited but products
of negative experiences
-​ treatment also based on nurture, subsequent neutral or positive
experiences with stimulus can reduce fear

You might also like

pFad - Phonifier reborn

Pfad - The Proxy pFad of © 2024 Garber Painting. All rights reserved.

Note: This service is not intended for secure transactions such as banking, social media, email, or purchasing. Use at your own risk. We assume no liability whatsoever for broken pages.


Alternative Proxies:

Alternative Proxy

pFad Proxy

pFad v3 Proxy

pFad v4 Proxy