Personality Psychology: The Basics (Gaines, 2019)
Personality Psychology: The Basics (Gaines, 2019)
SEMIOTICS
DANIEL CHANDLER
SPORT MANAGEMENT
ROBERT WILSON AND MARK PIEKARZ
SPORTS COACHING
LAURA PURDY
TRANSLATION
JULIANE HOUSE
TOWN PLANNING
TONY HALL
Typeset in Bembo
by Swales & Willis, Exeter, Devon, UK
To my son, Luther Gaines-White: You exist;
therefore, I am.
CONTENTS
List of boxes ix
Preface x
Introduction 1
1 An introduction to personality psychology 3
Conclusion 153
8 Concluding thoughts on personality psychology 155
References 167
Index 215
BOXES
AN INTRODUCTION TO
PERSONALITY PSYCHOLOGY
One of the most important lessons that one learns from the his-
tory of social psychology (which is covered in Social Psychology:
The Basics; Frings, in preparation) is that interpersonal situations
can exert considerable influence on individuals’ thoughts, feel-
ings, and behaviour (e.g., G. W. Allport, 1968/1985). By the
same token, the history of social psychology acknowledges that
not all individuals necessarily think, feel, or behave the same
way in a particular interpersonal situation, at a particular point
in time (e.g., E. E. Jones, 1985/1998). Indeed, throughout the
history of social psychology, individual differences in attitudes
(i.e., individuals’ positive versus negative thoughts and feelings
toward various persons, places, things, and other entities; Blair,
Dasgupta, & Glaser, 2015) have been examined empirically as
predictors of interpersonal behaviour (Ross, Lepper, & Ward,
2010).
Gordon W. Allport was a pioneer in the field of social psych-
ology, especially regarding the conceptualisation and measurement
4 INTRODUCTION
worlds that they inhabit (see Swann & Bosson, 2010). Having
been influenced by Charles Darwin’s (1859) theory of natural selec-
tion, James (1890/2010) emphasised the biological origins of the
self (e.g., the self is a product of the mind – which, in turn, is
a product of the brain). However, James (1902) subsequently
encouraged readers to decide for themselves whether they believe
that the self ultimately is a product of biology or a product of div-
inity. (Within philosophy and theology, the older term of soul his-
torically was used to describe the self as a divinely ordained entity;
see Calkins, 1917, for a critique of the pre-psychology literature
on the soul.)
In turn, according to James (1890/2010), two major compo-
nents of the self can be identified – namely, (1) the pure Ego, or
self-as-knower; and (2) the empirical Me, or self-as-known
(G. W. Allport, 1955). G. W. Allport contended that the empir-
ical Me – which G. W. Allport preferred to label as the proprium –
is the aspect of the self that is directly accessible to individuals’
consciousness. Having sidestepped the problems that plague
James’s conceptualisation of the pure Ego (e.g., if the pure Ego
reflects upon the self, then what is the entity that presumably
reflects upon the pure Ego, and so on; C. S. Hall & Lindzey,
1970), in Personality: A Psychological Interpretation (1937/1951),
G. W. Allport promoted the empirical Me or proprium in Pattern
and Grown in Personality (1961/1963) as the component of the self
that encompasses and gives order to the wide array of traits,
values, and other constructs that are part and parcel of individuals’
personalities (Ewen, 1998).
Within the empirical Me or proprium, G. W. Allport (1955)
accepted James’s (1890/2010) further division into the material self
(i.e., individuals’ physical possessions, including their own bodies),
social self (i.e., the roles and relationships within which individuals
are embedded), and spiritual self (i.e., individuals’ intelligence and
personality characteristics). (The term “spiritual self”, which James
chose over potentially less soul-evoking terms such as “psychic
self”, does not appear to have been problematic for G. W. Allport,
who shared James’s (1902) interest in religion and spirituality; e.g.,
G. W. Allport, 1950.) G. W. Allport believed that, in everyday life,
individuals do not experience components of the proprium as dis-
tinct from each other (C. S. Hall & Lindzey, 1970). Nevertheless,
AN INTRODUCTION TO PERSONALITY PSYCHOLOGY 7
G. W. Allport’s own programme of research – which included the
development of surveys to measure traits (e.g., G. W. Allport,
1928) and values (G. W. Allport, Vernon, & Lindzey, 1960; see also
Vernon & G. W. Allport, 1931) – tended to prioritise aspects of the
spiritual self (see Ewen, 1998).
With regard to James’s (1890/2010) spiritual self, G. W. Allport
(1937/1951, 1955, 1961/1963) drew a distinction between indi-
vidual differences in intelligence (i.e., presumed cognitive ability)
and individual differences in personality (i.e., a variety of psycho-
logical attributes that lie outside the domain of presumed cognitive
ability; see C. S. Hall & Lindzey, 1970). As it turns out,
G. W. Allport’s psychology of the individual largely predates the
emergence of cognitive psychology, which currently addresses the-
ories and research on intelligence (see Gobet, in preparation). In
any event, G. W. Allport viewed intelligence as a construct that
should be considered separate from the subject matter of personal-
ity psychology (a view that is shared by many, but not all, of
G. W. Allport’s followers; see Ewen, 1998).
Unlike James (1890/2010), G. W. Allport (1937/1951, 1955,
1961/1963) wrote systematically about the relevance of several
modern-day personality constructs to the spiritual self (see
C. S. Hall & Lindzey, 1970). For example, G. W. Allport’s psych-
ology of the individual includes traits, values, attitudes, and motives
(the latter of which can be defined as internal forces that direct
individuals’ behaviour; Sheldon & Schuler, 2015). In principle,
one could add affect (i.e., individual differences in feelings at
a particular point in time; Augustine & Larsen, 2015) – including
emotions (i.e., feelings that tend to be directed toward particular
entities) and moods (i.e., feelings that are not necessarily directed
toward any particular entity; R. Brown, 1965) – to the list of
major personality constructs. Nevertheless, G. W. Allport devoted
the bulk of his scholarly efforts toward understanding traits in all
of their complexity (Ewen, 1998).
Regarding traits, G. W. Allport (1937/1951, 1955, 1961/
1963) made a distinction between common traits (which can be
found in varying degrees among large numbers of individuals
and are especially amenable to quantitative research methods)
and personal traits (which, in principle, might be found only
among one individual and are especially amenable to qualitative
8 INTRODUCTION
PRELUDE TO CHAPTER 2
Perhaps no other school of thought has stimulated as much popular
curiosity outside personality psychology, or generated as much schol-
arly controversy within personality psychology, as has the psycho-
dynamic school (which we defined earlier in the present chapter).
AN INTRODUCTION TO PERSONALITY PSYCHOLOGY 13
Representative theories include (in order of appearance) the psycho-
analytic theory of Sigmund Freud (1900/1965), which evolved from
S. Freud’s earlier work with Josef Breuer (e.g., Breuer & S. Freud,
[1895/1995]; the analytical psychology of Carl Jung [1912/1916];
the individual psychology of Alfred Adler [1927/1957]); the “social-
psychological” personality theories of Karen Horney (1922–37/
1966), Harry Stack Sullivan (1953), Erich Fromm (1941), and Wil-
helm Reich (1933/1980); the ego psychology theories of Anna
Freud (1936/1966), Heinz Hartmann (1939), David Rapaport
(1960), and Erik Erikson (1950); the object relations theories of
Melanie Klein (1927), Donald Winnicott (1931), Ronald Fairbairn
(1952), and Harry Guntrip (1969), as well as the attachment theory
of John Bowlby (1969/1997); the “return-to-Freud” psychoanalytic
theory of Jacques Lacan (1966/1977); the self-psychology theories of
Heinz Kohut (1971) and Otto Kernberg (1967), neither of which
should be confused with the aforementioned self-theory of James
(1890/2010); and the personology of Henry Murray (1938). In
Chapter 2, we shall examine psychodynamic theories, as well as
research that addresses those theories.
CLASSIC SCHOOLS OF
THOUGHT I
2
PSYCHODYNAMIC PERSPECTIVES
ON PERSONALITY
years of age), the primary erotogenetic zone is the anus (with toilet
training as the source of conflict); personality characteristics reflect-
ing the enduring influence of the anal stage in adulthood include
orderliness, parsimoniousness, and obstinacy (and their opposites).
Third, during the urethral stage (also from approximately 1 to 3
years of age), the primary erotogenetic zone is the urethra (with
bedwetting as the source of conflict); personality characteristics
reflecting the enduring influence of the urethral stage in adulthood
include ambition (and its opposite). Fourth, during the phallic stage
(from approximately 2 to 5 years of age), the primary erotogenetic
zones are the penis and clitoris (with the Oedipus complex signifying
opposite-sex sexual desires and same-sex aggressive desires in boys,
and the Electra complex signifying opposite-sex sexual desires and
same-sex aggressive desires in girls, as sources of conflict); personal-
ity characteristics reflecting the enduring influence of the phallic
stage in adulthood include vanity and recklessness (and their oppos-
ites). Finally (and following a latency phase from approximately 6 to
12 years of age, when no major milestones are experienced in terms
of personality development), during the genital stage (from adoles-
cence to early adulthood), the primary erotogenetic zones are the
penis and vagina (with the inevitable difficulties of life as the sources
of conflict); personality characteristics reflecting the enduring influ-
ence of the genital stage (the goal of normal development in
S. Freud’s psychoanalytic theory) include a sincere interest in
others, effective sublimations, and realistic enjoyments.
The numerous controversies that surround Sigmund Freud’s
(1905/1962) psychoanalytic theory are the stuff of legend (Hall &
Lindzey, 1957). Consider S. Freud’s claim that gay men suffer
from “arrested development” (i.e., never progress as far as the
genital stage), ostensibly because they experienced such high levels
of love and affection from their mothers early in life that they
ended up rejecting women in general as potential romantic part-
ners, upon reaching adulthood (see McClelland, 1985/1987). One
might think that, by the time that the American Psychiatric Asso-
ciation dropped homosexuality from its list of personality disorders
in 1980, followers of S. Freud’s psychoanalytic theory would have
disavowed the claim about gay men’s “arrested development” (see
Millon, 1996). However, in a study of 90 gay men (most of
whom were recruited from gay bars) and 109 heterosexual men
PSYCHODYNAMIC PERSPECTIVES ON PERSONALITY 23
(most of whom were recruited from university classes), Rubinstein
(2010a) concluded that gay men scored significantly lower on self-
esteem (i.e., individuals’ realistic evaluation of themselves, whether
positive or negative, which we covered in Chapter 1 of the pre-
sent book) and significantly higher on narcissism (i.e., individuals’
unrealistically positive evaluation of themselves; Swann & Bosson,
2010) than did heterosexual men – results that seemingly support
S. Freud’s (1905/1962) claim about gay men’s problematic devel-
opment. Rubinstein’s results are tainted by the fact that most of
the gay men, but none of the heterosexual men, were recruited
from bars (e.g., it is not clear whether bar-frequenting heterosex-
ual men would have scored differently from bar-frequenting gay
men on measures of self-esteem or narcissism; Drescher, 2010).
Interestingly, Rubinstein (2010b) responded to a separate critique
(Hartmann, 2010) by proclaiming that he is a “very gay-friendly
investigator and therapist” (p. 48) who was expecting nonsignifi-
cant group differences and who would welcome disconfirming
evidence regarding those differences.
Putting aside the issue of “arrested development” as a function of
individuals’ sexual orientation, for a theory that supposedly is rooted in
the biological sciences, Sigmund Freud’s (1900/1965) version of psy-
choanalytic theory is notably deficient (e.g., libido, or psychic energy
that ostensibly surges through individuals and is popularly associated
with id-directed sexual behaviour, currently lacks any basis in empirical
fact; Ewen, 1998). However, the contributions of S. Freud’s psycho-
analytic theory to the social sciences are numerous (Schellenberg, 1978).
For example, the authoritarian or prejudiced personality is a construct that
Theodor Adorno and colleagues (Adorno, Frenkel-Brunswik, Levin-
son, & Sanford, 1950) developed on the basis of S. Freud’s earlier con-
cept of identification with the aggressor (i.e., in response to aggressive
urges toward same-sex parents [as distinct from sexual urges toward
opposite-sex parents], individuals are presumed to redirect their aggres-
sive urges toward members of psychological outgroups within a given
society; Snyder & Ickes, 1985); in Box 2.1, we will encounter Bob
Altemeyer’s (1981) Right-Wing Authoritarianism (RWA) Scale,
which was influenced by Adorno et al.’s research. Furthermore, later in
the present chapter, we will elaborate upon narcissism (a construct that
also looms as important within the self-psychology theories of Kohut,
1971 and Kernberg, 1967). To put it simply, S. Freud’s version of
24 CLASSIC SCHOOLS OF THOUGHT I
At one time, Sigmund Freud viewed Carl Jung as the natural succes-
sor to lead psychoanalytic theory beyond the early part of the twenti-
eth century (Ewen, 1998). Instead, Jung became the first major
defector from psychoanalytic theory, strongly rejecting S. Freud’s
(1900/1965) emphasis on infant sexuality (Hall & Lindzey, 1957).
Afterward, Jung (1912/1916) developed his own analytic psychology as
a competing psychodynamic theory of personality (Millon, 1996).
Jung placed less emphasis on S. Freud’s “master motives” in general
and hinted at a different, largely unexplored need to fulfil one’s
potential; this latter need eventually became known as the self-
actualisation motive among proponents of humanistic psychology (see
Chapter 4 of the present book).
In terms of personality structure, Jung’s (1912/1916) analytical
psychology retained the constructs of id, ego, and superego from
S. Freud’s (1900/1965) psychoanalytic theory, although Jung
renamed the id as the personal unconscious; renamed the ego as the per-
sonal conscious, and renamed the superego as the persona or collective con-
scious (Ewen, 1998). Jung’s most original contribution to
understanding personality structure was the addition of the collective
unconscious, which is a repository of unconsciously experienced
images that all humans share (and, presumably inherit from pre-
human as well as human ancestors; Hall & Lindzey, 1957). The pri-
mary individual-difference construct in Jung’s analytical psychology is
a de-sexualised version of the libido (e.g., Jung, 1921/1971), which
26 CLASSIC SCHOOLS OF THOUGHT I
“SOCIAL-PSYCHOLOGICAL” PSYCHODYNAMIC
THEORIES
sublimation, (11) identification with the aggressor, and (12) altruism (the
latter three of which S. Freud had not explored in detail,
P. F. Kernberg, 1994). Westen and Chang (2000) pointed out that
A. Freud (1958) was especially concerned with the defence mech-
anisms that individuals use during adolescence, which is a time of
sexual awakening.
Notwithstanding Anna Freud’s (1936/1966) efforts toward docu-
menting all of the ways that the ego might reduce (and, perhaps, elim-
inate) individuals’ intrapersonal anxiety, no single, agreed-upon survey
exists that could measure most (if not all) of the defence mechanisms
(although a small number of A. Freud’s followers have attempted to do
so; e.g., Joffe & Naditch, 1977). However, to the extent that the rela-
tively new field of neuropsychoanalysis has revived academicians’ and
practitioners’ interest in defence mechanisms (including, but not limited
to, repression; Boag, 2006), the measurement of A. Freud’s complete
list of defence mechanisms remains a worthy conceptual and empirical
goal within the psychodynamic school of personality psychology (see
Davison & MacGregor, 1998). In summary, A. Freud’s ego psychology
functions as a viable alternative to “id psychology” (i.e., the psychoana-
lytic theory of S. Freud, 1900/1965) without requiring a full-scale
rejection of S. Freud’s core constructs (Millon, 1996).
PRELUDE TO CHAPTER 3
Despite the differences that we have observed between William
James’s (1890/2010) self-theory and Sigmund Freud’s (1900/1965)
version of psychoanalytic theory, both theories can be classified as
mentalist, due to their emphasis on conscious and unconscious
within-person influences on individuals’ behaviour (Kihlstrom,
2004). Conversely, the behaviourist school – which alternatively has
been depicted as part of personality psychology (e.g., Hall & Lind-
zey, 1978) or as an alternative to personality psychology (e.g.,
Ewen, 1998) – emphasises outside-person influences on individuals’
behaviour (Moore, 2013). Representative theories include (in
order of appearance) the reflexology of Ivan Pavlov (1926/1928),
the connectionism of Edward Thorndike (1911), the classical con-
ditioning theory of John B. Watson (1916), Edward Tolman’s
(1932) stimulus-stimulus theory, the drive reduction theory of
Clark Hull (1943), the operant reinforcement theory of
B. F. Skinner (1938), the stimulus-response theory of Neal Miller
and John Dollard (1941), the social learning theory of Julian
Rotter (1954), and the social learning theory of Albert Bandura
and Richard Walters (1963). In Chapter 3, we shall examine
behaviourist theories and relevant research in detail.
CLASSIC SCHOOLS OF
THOUGHT II
3
BEHAVIOURIST PERSPECTIVES ON
PERSONALITY
Many (if not most) historians of science have concluded that Wilhelm
Wundt was the founder of modern-day psychology, during the late
1800s (J. F. Brennan, 2003). After all, Wundt published the ground-
breaking Principles of Physiological Psychology in 1874 (reprinted in
1910) and established the world’s first psychological laboratory in
1879 (Wade, Sakurai, & Gyoba, 2007). However, some historians of
science would assert that the real founder of modern-day psychology
was Ivan Pavlov, during the early 1900s (Walsh, Teo, & Baydala,
2014). In particular, Pavlov’s landmark publication of Lectures on Con-
ditioned Reflexes: Twenty-Five Years of Objective Study of the Higher Ner-
vous System in 1926 (reprinted in 1928) documented the evolution of
Pavlov’s own programme of research from a focus on digestive physi-
ology (for which he won the Nobel Prize in 1904) to an emphasis on
classical conditioning (a cornerstone of early behaviourism in psych-
ology; Todes, 1997).
Pavlov (1926/1928) initially viewed his physiologically orien-
tated “reflexology” (which dealt with dogs’ conditioned responses to
environmental cues regarding the imminent availability of food, as
built upon dogs’ unconditioned responses to the food itself) as dis-
tinct from psychology (J. F. Brennan, 2003) but eventually aligned
his reflexology with psychology (Walsh, Teo, & Baydala, 2014).
62 CLASSIC SCHOOLS OF THOUGHT II
(starting with a white rat and adding various white, furry objects
thereafter; LeUnes, 1983). As it turns out, J. B. Watson and
Rayner were not the first behaviourists to apply classical condi-
tioning techniques to a child (Windholz & Lamal, 1986). How-
ever, the ethical lapses in J. B. Watson and Rayner’s study (not
the least of which was the researchers’ apparent use of prior
knowledge that “Little Albert” reacted quite negatively to loud
noises, which would explain the researchers’ subsequent choice of
the repeated banging of a gong as their unconditioned stimulus;
see Sheehy, 2004) were so egregious – even by pre-World War II
standards – that this particular study is a standout because of its
precise application of behaviourist principles and its problematic
methodology (Beck, Levinson, & Lyons, 2009).
Perhaps the most regrettable aspect of J. B. Watson and Rayner’s
(1920) classical conditioning study was the fact that – having induced
anxiety in “Little Albert” regarding several harmless objects – the
researchers never attempted to de-condition the infant (who already
was suffering from a cognitive impairment at the time of their experi-
ment, as the researchers may have been aware; Fridlund, Beck,
Goldie, & Irons, 2012). One of J. B. Watson’s students, Mary Cover
Jones (1924a), eventually developed a behaviour modification proto-
col for use with another young child (Kornfeld, 1989); M. C. Jones’
pioneering work is profiled in Box 3.1. Perhaps the most neutral
evaluation that one can offer concerning J. B. Watson’s classical con-
ditioning theory (e.g., J. B. Watson, 1916) and related research (e.g.,
J. B. Watson & Raynor, 1920) is that J. B. Watson clearly imple-
mented a “behaviourist manifesto” (as originally presented by
J. B. Watson, 1913) but offered a version of behaviourism that is
questionable in terms of its ultimate benefit to science, especially
when one considers the physical and psychological stress that Watson
and Raynor inflicted upon “Little Albert” (Walsh, Teo, & Baydala,
2014).
During the interval between the First and Second World Wars,
the behaviourist school succeeded in prompting psychologists en
masse to re-define their discipline as the science of behaviour
(rather than the science of the mind; Hebb, 1960). However, by
the time that the Second World War had begun to wreak large-
scale havoc within and beyond academia, some behaviourists
already were seeking to re-incorporate certain “mentalistic” con-
structs within their theories (Mandler, 2002). One of the earliest
examples of the resulting neo-behaviourist perspective on person-
ality was Neil Miller and John Dollard’s (1941) stimulus-response
theory, which drew jointly upon Sigmund Freud’s (1900/1965)
psychoanalytic theory and Ivan Pavlov’s (1927) reflexology (Ewen,
1998). According to stimulus-response theory, behaviour can be
74 CLASSIC SCHOOLS OF THOUGHT II
PRELUDE TO CHAPTER 4
In Societal Structures of the Mind, Uriel G. Foa and Edna B. Foa
(1974) contended that psychodynamic therapists (in the tradition
of S. Freud, 1900/1965) tend to deny respect (if not affection) to
their clients; whereas it is not clear whether behaviourist therapists
(in the tradition of Skinner, 1938) give or deny the interpersonal
resources of affection and respect to their clients. In stark contrast,
according to U. G. Foa and E. B. Foa, humanistic therapists (in the
tradition of Rogers, 1961) tend to give affection as well as respect
to their clients – a testament to the “Third Force” that Abraham
Maslow (1954) envisioned for humanism as a counterpoint to the
psychodynamic and behaviourist schools in personality psychology
(Sappington, 1989). Within the school of humanism include, rep-
resentative theories include (in order of appearance) the love-
oriented aspects of Erich Fromm’s (1957) didactic humanism (as
distinct from the hate-orientated aspects; e.g., Fromm, 1941); Kurt
Goldstein’s (1939) organismic psychology, Charlotte Buhler’s
(1968) version of humanistic psychology, Abraham Maslow’s
(1968) version of self-actualisation theory, and Carl Rogers’s
BEHAVIOURIST PERSPECTIVES ON PERSONALITY 81
(1961) version of self-actualisation theory. [In addition, within the
related school of existentialism, representative theories include (in order
of appearance) Jean-Paul Sartre’s (1943/1956) version of existential
philosophy, Simone de Beauvoir’s (1947) version of existential philoso-
phy, Frantz Fanon’s (1952/1967) version of existential psychology, and
Rollo May’s (1953) version of existential psychology.] In Chapter 4,
we will examine these theories and associated research – alongside
alternative perspectives from the emergent positive psychology movement
(i.e., Diener, Emmons, Larsen, & Griffin, 1985; Ryan & Deci, 2000b;
Ryff, 1989) – in greater detail.
4
Ed Diener and colleagues (e.g., Diener, Oishi, & Lucas, 2003) define
subjective well-being as individuals’ cognitive (i.e., thought-based) and
affective (i.e., feeling-based) evaluation of their lives – basically, indi-
viduals’ positive versus negative attitude toward their lives (see
S. C. White, Gaines, & Jha, 2012). Diener and colleagues contend
HUMANISTIC AND EXISTENTIAL PERSPECTIVES 99
that the cognitive component of subjective well-being is life satisfac-
tion (measured via the Satisfaction With Life Scale, or SWLS; Diener,
Emmons, Larsen, & Griffin, 1985); whereas the affective components
of subjective well-being are positive moods and negative moods (meas-
ured via the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule, or PANAS;
D. Watson, Clark, & Tellegen, 1988). Thus, according to Diener
and colleagues, high subjective well-being represents a combination
of high levels of life satisfaction, high levels of positive moods, and
low levels of negative moods (e.g., D. G. Myers & Diener, 1995).
One of the most noteworthy sets of findings from Diener and
colleagues’ programme of research on subjective well-being (e.g.,
D. G. Myers & Diener, 1995) is that (1) on average, individuals in
economically rich nations report significantly higher levels of sub-
jective well-being than do individuals in economically not-rich
nations; yet (2) within the economically richest nation on Earth
(i.e., the United States), the positive effect of income on individ-
uals’ subjective well-being is statistically significant but practically
negligible (see Peterson & Seligman, 2004). Actually, within the
US, it appears that the impact of income on subjective well-being
is best understood as curvilinear (i.e., highest levels of subjective
well-being among individuals within the $100,000 to $250,000
bracket, lower scores of subjective well-being among individuals
whose incomes either fall below or fall above that range;
D. G. Myers & Diener, 2018), rather than linear. All things con-
sidered, Diener’s conceptualisation and measurement of subjective
well-being have largely defined the literature on well-being within
the positive psychology movement (see S. C. White, Gaines, &
Jha, 2012).
PRELUDE TO CHAPTER 5
During the latter half of the twentieth century, the trait school
(which, unlike the humanistic/existential school, emphasises indi-
viduals’ striving for accuracy – rather than positivity – in gathering
information about themselves; see Robins & John, 1997) gradually
104 CLASSIC SCHOOLS OF THOUGHT II
TRAIT PERSPECTIVES ON
PERSONALITY
Nunnally & Bernstein, 1994) of the “Big Five” traits (for a broader
critique of predictive ability within personality psychology, see
Funder, 2001).
slowness and lacking in energy; (2) restraint and seriousness vs. rhathymia
and impulsiveness; (3) ascendance and social boldness vs. submissiveness
and timidity; (4) sociability and social interest vs. seclusiveness and shy-
ness; (5) emotional stability and optimism vs. instability and depression;
(6) objectivity vs. subjectivity and hypersensitivity; (7) friendliness and
agreeableness vs. hostility and belligerence; (8) thoughtfulness or reflective-
ness vs. unreflectiveness; (9) good personal relations and cooperativeness vs.
criticalness and intolerance; and (10) masculinity of interests and emotions
vs. femininity. Amelang and Borkenau (1982) suggested re-
interpreting and re-analysing J. P. Guilford’s data in such a way
that the equivalents of conscientiousness, extraversion, agreeable-
ness, and neuroticism could be extracted as higher-level dimen-
sions via orthogonal factor analyses (cf. Digman, 1990).
J. P. Guilford and Wayne S. Zimmerman (1949) developed the
Guilford-Zimmerman Temperament Survey to measure the ten afore-
mentioned, lower-order traits that eventually would become the core
dimensions in Guilford’s (1975) version of factor-analytic trait theory
(Digman, 1990). The term “temperament” in the Guilford-
Zimmerman Temperament Survey reflects the belief that certain bio-
logically hardwired precursors to traits are present at birth, manifested
in individual differences in behaviour from infancy onward (see Shiner,
2015) – a belief that also is reflected in the term “temperamental traits”,
as promoted by Cattell (1965); see also Rutter and Silberg (2002). Put-
ting aside the question of temperamental precursors to traits, Goldberg
(1981) – who apparently coined the term “Big Five” prior to Costa
and McCrae’s (1985, 1989, 1992) popularisation of the term (Digman,
1990) – concluded that J. P. Guilford and Zimmerman had not meas-
ured more than five traits (and, as we have seen, four factors were
extracted by Amelang & Borkenau, 1982). Thus, as a measure of ten
distinct traits, the Guilford-Zimmerman Temperament Survey is ques-
tionable in terms of construct validity (for a discussion of the extent to
which acceptance of J. P. Guilford’s ten factors is based upon the per-
suasiveness of theoretical arguments rather than scientific data, see
TRAIT PERSPECTIVES ON PERSONALITY 117
C. S. Hall & Lindzey, 1978). As for internal consistency, independent
reviewers rarely (if ever) comment upon the reliability of the scales that
underlie the Guilford-Zimmerman Temperament Survey (as assessed
via Cronbach’s alpha; e.g., Schuerger, Zarrella, & Hotz, 1989).
Unlike the other factor-analytic trait theories and relevant surveys
that we have covered so far, one will not find any acknowledgement
of J. P. Guilford’s (1975) version of factor-analytic trait theory or the
Guilford-Zimmerman Temperament Survey (J. P. Guilford & Zim-
merman, 1949) in Ozer and Reise’s (1994) review of the literature on
personality assessment (which, in turn was designed to complement
the emphasis on personality structure in an earlier review by Wiggins
& Pincus, 1992). Similarly, one will not find any mention of
J. P. Guilford’s factor theory and trait survey within the fifth edition of
Ewen’s An Introduction to Theories of Personality (1998). Nevertheless,
in the third edition of Theories of Personality, C. S. Hall and Lindzey
(1978) regard the influence of J. P. Guilford’s theory and survey upon
the field of personality psychology as comparable to the influence of
H. J. Eysenck’s factor theory (e.g., H. J. Eysenck, 1947) and trait
survey (e.g., H. J. Eysenck & S. B. G. Eysenck, 1968). Perhaps
J. P. Guilford’s contributions to personality psychology are best sum-
marised as (1) pioneering the use of factor analysis to identify
a manageable set of major traits, (2) developing a coherent theory to
account for those theories, and (3) creating a trait survey that has stood
the test of time – a set of accomplishments that, at a minimum, support
the inclusion of J. P. Guilford’s theory and survey in historical over-
views of trait perspectives in personality (Digman, 1990).
During the same year that S. Bem (1974) published the aforemen-
tioned BSRI, Janet Spence and colleagues (Spence, Helmreich, &
Stapp, 1974) released their own survey – namely, the 24-item
TRAIT PERSPECTIVES ON PERSONALITY 121
Personal Attributes Questionnaire (PAQ) – that was designed to
measure “masculinity” and “femininity” as separate dimensions
(Lenney, 1991). Like S. Bem, Spence et al. concluded that some
men and some women are psychologically “androgynous” (i.e.,
score relatively high in “masculine” and “feminine” traits). How-
ever, unlike S. Bem, Spence and colleagues never assumed that
the PAQ (or the BSRI, for that matter) measured individuals’
sexual orientation (R. Brown, 1986). Indeed (as noted in Chapter
1 of the present book), Spence et al. eventually developed surveys
that not only measured gender-related traits (e.g., Spence, Helm-
reich, & Holahan, 1979) but also gender-related attitudes (e.g.,
Spence, Helmreich, & Stapp, 1973) and achievement-related
motives that have been linked empirically to gender (e.g., Helm-
reich & Spence, 1978) – an eclectic approach that led to the
development of Spence’s (1993) multifactorial gender identity theory
(Frable, 1997).
Having reflected upon the content of S. Bem’s (1974) BSRI
and their own PAQ (Spence, Helmreich, & Stapp, 1974), Spence
and Helmreich (1978) concluded that they (and S. Bem) had pri-
marily (and, in the case of the PAQ, exclusively) measured socially
desirable or positive aspects of “masculinity” and “femininity”.
Afterward, Spence and colleagues (Spence, Helmreich, & Hola-
han, 1979) developed the 40-item Extended Personal Attributes
Questionnaire (EPAQ) to measure negative as well as positive
aspects of “masculinity” and “femininity”. Finally, Spence and col-
leagues (Helmreich, Spence, & Wilhelm, 1981) published results
regarding the psychometric properties of the original PAQ and the
EPAQ. Not only did oblique factor analyses identify positive and
negative aspects of “masculinity” and “femininity” as distinct
dimensions; but – with the notable exception of negative “femin-
inity” in the EPAQ (i.e., Cronbach’s alphas below .50) – the
internal consistency of the PAQ and EPAQ scales proved to be
satisfactory (i.e., Cronbach’s alphas near or above .70; Nunnally &
Bernstein, 1994).
Spence’s (1993) multifactorial gender identity theory and two
versions of the PAQ (Spence, Helmreich, & Holahan, 1979;
Spence, Helmreich, & Stapp, 1974) have not enjoyed quite the
same popularity that S. Bem’s (1981) gender schema theory and
BSRI (Bem, 1974) have enjoyed within the literature on gender
122 CURRENT AND EMERGING SCHOOLS OF THOUGHT
Wiggins and colleagues have relied too heavily upon obscure syn-
onyms and antonyms (e.g., “unbold”, “ironhearted”) in order to
generate nearly-perfect circumplex patterns of lower-order trait
correlations (e.g., Benjamin, 2011). In an effort to resolve the lin-
guistic problems that underlie the IAS and IAS-R,
Patrick M. Markey and Charlotte N. Markey (2009) have devel-
oped the everyday-language International Personality Item Pool-
Interpersonal Circumplex (IPIP-IPC) survey (which is profiled in
Box 5.2). Notwithstanding empirical issues concerning Wiggins
et al.’s measurement of interpersonal traits, Wiggins’s (1991) inter-
personal circumplex theory has gained prominence due to the ease
with which it allows researchers to incorporate the gender-related
traits of positive “masculinity” (i.e., high end of the dominance
axis), positive “femininity” (high end of the nurturance axis),
negative “femininity” (low end of the dominance axis), and nega-
tive “masculinity” (low end of the nurturance axis) – as well as
the “Big Five” traits of extraversion (combination of high domin-
ance and high nurturance) and agreeableness (combination of low
dominance and high nurturance) – within one circular model of
interpersonal traits (Helgeson, 2015).
PRELUDE TO CHAPTER 6
By the end of the twentieth century, a fifth major school of
thought had emerged within personality psychology – namely, the
cognitive school (which emphasises individuals’ information-
processing and other higher-order thought processes as influences
on behaviour; Butt, 2004). Initially, Funder (2001) referred to this
school of thought as social-cognitive – an overt nod to the legacy of
the social learning theories of Rotter (1954) and the team of
TRAIT PERSPECTIVES ON PERSONALITY 127
Bandura and Walters (1963) that we reviewed in Chapter 3 of the
present book (see also Gaines, 2016/2018). However, Funder and
Fast (2010) labelled the school simply as “cognitive” – a term that
is sufficiently inclusive to accommodate contributions from cogni-
tive psychology (see Gobet, in preparation), as distinct from social
psychology (Frings, in preparation). Representative theories within
the cognitive school include (in order of appearance) Kelly’s
(1955) personal construct theory, Mischel’s (1973) version of
social-cognitive theory, Bandura’s (1986) recasting of his earlier social
learning theory (Bandura & Walters, 1963) as a separate version of
social-cognitive theory, Higgins’s (1987) self-discrepancy theory, and
Baldwin’s (1992) relational schema theory. In Chapter 6, we shall
examine cognitive theories of personality in greater detail.
6
COGNITIVE PERSPECTIVES ON
PERSONALITY
MINI-THEORIES OF COGNITION
PRELUDE TO CHAPTER 7
The end of the 1990s marked the emergence of a sixth major
school of thought within personality psychology – namely, the bio-
logical school (which emphasises “organic” influences on individuals’
behaviour; Funder & Fast, 2010). To some extent, one can trace
the origins of the biological school to the trait perspective in per-
sonality (Funder, 2001). Moreover, we have already encountered
one prominent personality theory that is largely biological in orien-
tation (see Chapter 5 of the present book) – namely,
H. J. Eysenck’s (1947) version of factor-analytic trait theory (which,
at its core, is a physiological theory of arousability; Geen, 1997).
However, given that psychophysiologists have noted a lack of
empirical support for (and, to some extent, significant results in the
opposite direction to) key predictions from H. J. Eysenck’s theory
(Haslam, Smillie, & Song, 2017), we will not highlight that theory
in Chapter 7 of the present book. Instead, for our purposes, repre-
sentative theories within the biological school include (in order of
appearance) Darwin’s (1859) theory of natural selection; Galton’s
(1869) theory of eugenics; J. A. Gray’s (1970) reinforcement sensi-
tivity theory; Cloninger’s (1987) version of neurotransmitter
theory; Depue’s (1995) version of neurotransmitter theory;
A. H. Buss and Plomin (1975) temperament theory of personality
development; Trivers’s (1972) theory of parental investment; and
D. M. Buss and Schmitt (1993) sexual strategy theory.
7
BIOLOGICAL PERSPECTIVES ON
PERSONALITY
nations (i.e., the Big Five model is less “universal” than many biologic-
ally orientated psychologists had assumed; Kwan & Herrmann, 2015).
Finally, we will learn about attempts by evolutionary psychologists to
reconcile evolutionary and cultural perspectives on personality
(D. M. Buss & Kenrick, 1998).
PRELUDE TO CHAPTER 8
Having surveyed each of the major schools of thought regarding
personality, we are struck by many personality psychologists’ lack
of attention to individuals’ intentionality – a concept that is
152 CURRENT AND EMERGING SCHOOLS OF THOUGHT
CONCLUDING THOUGHTS ON
PERSONALITY PSYCHOLOGY
INTENTIONALITY: A QUINTESSENTIALLY
EXISTENTIALIST CONSTRUCT
In Love and Will, May (1969) devoted two chapters to the construct
of intentionality. First, May offered an academic conceptualisation of
intentionality, transforming Rene Descartes’ famous dictum of “I
think, therefore I am” into “I conceive – I can – I will – I am” (p.
CONCLUDING THOUGHTS ON PERSONALITY PSYCHOLOGY 157
243). Second, May commented on the clinical implications of inten-
tionality, noting that Descartes’s dictum prioritises human cognition
over human affect – an ironic prioritisation on Descartes’s part, given
that Descartes developed an entire theory regarding emotions as “pas-
sions of the soul” that ostensibly direct the “rational soul” or mind
toward particular objects, energising individuals to approach or avoid
those objects (Greenberg, 2007). In any event, we shall cover May’s
academic and clinical observations concerning intentionality, in that
order.
According to Damasio (1994), Descartes introduced the mind-
body problem (i.e., the question of how a supposedly immortal
“rational soul” or mind manages to co-exist, let alone interact,
with a mortal body) into natural philosophy and, consequently,
into modern psychology. One might question the assumption
(articulated by Damasio, 2001, among others) that Descartes had
incorrectly depicted thinking as an activity that was separate from
the body (e.g., see Kirkeboen, 2001). Nevertheless, from the
standpoint of the existentialist perspective (e.g., May, Angel, &
Ellenberger, 1958), Descartes’s mind-body dualism does not
adequately capture humans’ lived experience (Buxton, 2005). May
(1969) in particular challenged the premise of Descartes’s mind-
body problem by emphasising core aspects of humans’ inten-
tionality (e.g., consciousness as experienced toward something or
someone, not as occurring in a social-psychological vacuum;
action as reflecting individuals’ active decision-making processes,
not as passively responding to emotional states; see Eliason,
Grafton, Samide, G. Williams, & Lepore, 2010).
One need not embrace May’s (1969) conceptualisation of
intentionality (or, more generally, May’s theory of existential
psychology) in order to appreciate May’s practical contributions to
psychotherapy (e.g., see A. H. Craig, 1995). However, Eliason
and colleagues (2010) hinted that May’s view of therapists as co-
creators of genuine relationships with clients (rather than dispas-
sionate dispensers of advice to clients) reflects May’s belief in
intentionality as a human capacity that transcends social roles (a
belief that also is reflected in the neo-Freudian interpersonal
theory of Sullivan, 1953; see Shahar, 2011). Interestingly, Milton
(1993) argued that May’s advocacy of authenticity among therap-
ists and clients alike (within the context of therapist-client
158 CONCLUSION
A FINAL WORD
In their chapter on the future of personality psychology, Benet-
Martinez et al. (2015) offered an eclectic set of recommendations
and aspirations concerning conceptual and empirical trends in the
field. For the sake of brevity, we shall limit our own “wish list” for
the future of personality psychology to a single (albeit multifaceted)
request: Building upon the promise of Wiggins’s (1991) interpersonal
circumplex theory (which we reviewed in Chapter 5 of the present
book), apply a laser-like focus to those dimensions of personality that
are especially likely to be reflected in individuals’ behaviour toward
significant others (following Gaines, 2016/2018). For example,
within the context of heterosexual romantic relationships, the inter-
personal trait of nurturance (see Wiggins, 1979) generally emerges as
a significant positive predictor of individuals’ giving of interpersonal
resources (i.e., affection and respect; U. G. Foa & E. B. Foa, 1974)
toward their partners (see Gaines, 1996); but will future researchers
obtain similar results for the interpersonal value of altruism (equivalent
to the communal value; Trapnell & Paulhus, 2012), the interpersonal
attitude of positivity toward significant others in relation to self (frequently
labelled as positive working model of other; Griffin & Bartholomew,
1994), or the interpersonal motive of intimacy (also known as the need
for tenderness; Sullivan, 1953)? Such research could simultaneously
assess the construct validity (i.e., the extent to which scales measure the
dimensions that they were designed to measure) and predictive validity
(i.e., the extent to which scores on psychological dimensions are
reflected in scores on outcome measures, such as behaviour; Car-
mines & Zeller, 1979).
166 CONCLUSION
Buss, A. H., Plomin, R., & Willerman, L. (1973). The inheritance of personal-
ity traits. Journal of Personality, 41, 513–524.
Buss, D. M. (1989). Sex differences in human mate preferences: Evolutionary
hypotheses tested in 37 cultures. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 12, 1–14.
Buss, D. M. (1997). Evolutionary foundations of personality. In R. Hogan,
J. Johnson, & S. Briggs (Eds.), Handbook of personality psychology (pp. 317–
344). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Buss, D. M., & Kenrick, D. T. (1998). Evolutionary social psychology. In
D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), The handbook of social psych-
ology (4th ed., Vol. 2, pp. 982–1026). New York: McGraw-Hill.
Buss, D. M., & Penke, L. (2015). Evolutionary personality psychology. In
M. Mikulincer & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), APA handbook of personality and social
psychology (Vol. 4: Personality Processes and Individual Differences, pp. 3–
29). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Buss, D. M., & Schmitt, D. P. (1993). Sexual strategies theory: An evolutionary
perspective on human mating. Psychological Review, 100, 204–232.
Buss, D. M., & Schmitt, D. P. (2011). Evolutionary psychology and feminism.
Sex Roles, 64, 768–787.
Butt, T. (2004). Understanding people. London: Palgrave Macmillan.
Buxton, A. (2005). Conceptualisation and existential therapy. Existential Ana-
lysis, 16, 131–143.
Calkins, M. W. (1917). The case of self against soul. Psychological Review, 24,
278–300.
Campbell, W. K., & Campbell, S. M. (2009). On the self-regulatory
dynamics created by the particular benefits and costs of narcissism:
A contextual reinforcement model and examination of leadership. Self
and Identity, 8, 214–232.
Campbell, W. K., & Foster, C. A. (2002). Narcissism and commitment in
romantic relationships: An investment model analysis. Personality and Social
Psychology Bulletin, 28, 484–495.
Campbell, L., & Simpson, J. A. (2013). The blossoming of relationship science.
In J. A. Simpson & L. Campbell (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of close relation-
ships (pp. 3–10). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Capps, D. (2012). Erikson’s schedule of human strengths and the childhood
origins of the resourceful self. Pastoral Psychology, 61, 269–283.
Carlin, N. (2010). The paranoia of everyday life: Some personal, psychological,
and pastoral thoughts. Pastoral Psychology, 59, 679–695.
Carmines, E. G., & Zeller, R. A. (1979). Reliability and validity assessment. New-
bury Park, CA: Sage.
Carroll, E. N., Zuckerman, M., & Vogel, W. H. (1982). A test of the optimal
level of arousal theory of sensation seeking. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 42, 572–575.
REFERENCES 175
Carton, J. S., & Nowicki, S. (1994). Antecedents of individual differences in
locus of control of reinforcement: A critical review. Genetic, Social, and Gen-
eral Psychology Monographs, 120, 31–81.
Carver, C. S. (2015). Behavioral approach, behavioural avoidance, and
behavioural inhibition. In M. Mikulincer & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), APA
handbook of personality and social psychology (Vol. 4: Personality Processes
and Individual Differences, pp. 307–327). Washington, DC: American
Psychological Association.
Caspi, A., Roberts, B. W., & Shiner, R. L. (2005). Personality development:
Stability and change. Annual Review of Psychology, 56, 453–484.
Castagna, P. J., Davis, T. E., III, & Lilly, M. E. (2017). The behavioral avoid-
ance task with anxious youth: A review of procedures, properties, and
criticisms. Clinical Child & Family Psychology Review, 20, 162–184.
Cattell, R. B. (1946). Development and measurement of personality. New York:
World Book Co.
Cattell, R. B. (1950). Personality: A systematic, theoretical, and factual study.
New York: McGraw-Hill.
Cattell, R. B. (1965). Factor analysis: An introduction to essentials I: The pur-
pose and underlying models. Biometrics, 21, 190–215.
Cattell, R. B. (1970). Personality and social psychology: Collected papers of Raymond
B. Cattell. San Diego, CA: Knapp.
Cattell, R. B. (1973). Personality and mood by questionnaire. San Francisco, CA:
Jossey-Bass.
Celani, D. P. (2001). Working with Fairbairn’s ego structures. Contemporary
Psychoanalysis, 37, 381–416.
Cervone, D. (1999). Bottom-up explanation in personality psychology: The
case of cross-situational coherence. In D. Cervone & Y. Shoda (Eds.), The
coherence of personality: Social-cognitive bases of consistency, variability, and organ-
ization (pp. 303–341). New York: Guilford Press.
Cervone, D. (2004). The architecture of personality. Psychological Review, 111,
183–204.
Cervone, D. (2005). Personality architecture: Within-person structures and
processes. Annual Review of Psychology, 56, 423–452.
Cervone, D., & Shoda, Y. (1999a). Social-cognitive theories and the coherence
of personality. In D. Cervone & Y. Shoda (Eds.), The coherence of personality:
Social-cognitive bases of consistency, variability, and organization (pp. 3–33).
New York: Guilford Press.
Cervone, D., & Shoda, Y., Eds. (1999b). The coherence of personality: Social-cognitive
bases of consistency, variability, and organization. New York: Guilford Press.
Chase, C. I. (1998). A chat room – Educational psychologists but no curricu-
lum methodologists? Educational Psychology Review, 10, 239–248.
176 REFERENCES
Damasio, A. R. (1994). Descartes’ error: Emotion, reason, and the human brain.
New York: Putman.
Damasio, A. R. (2001). Descartes’ error revisited. Journal of the History of the
Neurosciences, 10, 192–194.
Danzer, G. (2012). Integrating ego psychology and strengths-based social work.
Journal of Theory Construction & Testing, 16, 9–15.
Darwin, C. R. (1859). On the origin of species by means of natural selection, or the
preservation of favoured races in the struggle for life. London: John Murray.
Darwin, C. R. (1871). The descent of man, and selection in relation to sex. London:
John Murray.
Davis, M., Barad, M., Otto, M., & Southwick, S. (2006). Combining pharma-
cotherapy with cognitive behavioral therapy: Traditional and new
approaches. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 19, 571–581.
Davis, T. (2007). The relevance of the Freudian concept of ‘transference’ to
existential psychotherapy. Existential Analysis, 18, 348–357.
Davison, K., & MacGregor, M. W. (1998). A critical appraisal of self-report
defense mechanism measures. Journal of Personality, 66, 965–992.
de Beauvoir, S. (1947). The ethics of ambiguity. New York: Philosophical Library.
de Beauvoir, S. (2009). The second sex (Vol.’s 1 & 2). New York: Knopf. (Ori-
ginal work published 1949).
Deary, I. J. (2009). The trait approach to personality. In P. J. Corr &
G. Matthews (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of personality psychology (pp. 89–
109). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Deaux, K., & LaFrance, M. (1998). Gender. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, &
G. Lindzey (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology (4th ed., Vol. 1, pp. 788–
827). Boston, MA: McGraw-Hill.
Debbane, E. (2011). Envy and its relation to destructiveness. Canadian Journal of
Psychoanalysis, 19, 108–124.
DeCarvalho, R. J. (1990). The growth hypothesis and self-actualization: An
existential alternative. The Humanistic Psychologist, 18, 252–258.
DeCarvalho, R. J. (1991). Gordon Allport and humanistic psychology. Journal
of Humanistic Psychology, 31, 8–13.
DeCarvalho, R. J. (1992). The humanistic ethics of Rollo May. Journal of
Humanistic Psychology, 32, 7–18.
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). The “what” and “why” of goal pursuits:
Human needs and the self-determination of behavior. Psychological Inquiry,
11, 227–268.
Depue, R. A. (1995). Neurobiological factors in personality and depression.
European Journal of Personality, 9, 413–439.
Depue, R. A., & Collins, P. F. (1999). Neurobiology of the structure of per-
sonality: Dopamine, facilitation of incentive motivation, and extraversion.
Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 491–569.
REFERENCES 179
DeRobertis, E. M. (2006). Charlotte Buhler’s existential-humanistic contribu-
tions to child and adolescent psychology. Journal of Humanistic Psychology, 46,
48–76.
DeRobertis, E. M. (2015). Toward a humanistic-multicultural model of devel-
opment. In K. J. Schneider, J. F. Pierson, & J. F. T. Bugental (Eds.), The
handbook of humanistic psychology: Theory, research, and practice (2nd ed., pp.
227–242). Los Angeles, CA: Sage.
Dewey, J. (1909). Moral principles in education. New York: Houghton Mifflin.
Dewsbury, D. A. (2000). Comparative cognition in the 1930s. Psychonomic Bul-
letin & Review, 7, 267–283.
DeYoung, C. G. (2015). Openness/intellect: A dimension of personality
reflecting cognitive exploration. In M. Mikulincer & P. R. Shaver (Eds.),
APA handbook of personality and social psychology (Vol. 4: Personality Processes
and Individual Differences, pp. 369–399). Washington, DC: American Psy-
chological Association.
DeYoung, C. G., & Gray, J. R. (2009). Personality neuroscience: Explaining
individual differences in affect, behavior, and cognition. In P. J. Corr &
G. Matthews (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of personality psychology (pp.
323–346). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
DeYoung, C. G., Weisberg, Y. J., Quilty, L. C., & Peterson, J. B. (2013). Uni-
fying the aspects of the Big Five, the interpersonal circumplex, and trait
affiliation. Journal of Personality, 81, 465–475.
Diener, E., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., & Griffin, S. (1985). The satisfaction
with life scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 49, 71–75.
Diener, E., Oishi, S., & Lucas, R. E. (2003). Personality, culture, and subject-
ive well-being: Emotional and cognitive evaluations of life. Annual Review of
Psychology, 54, 403–425.
Diener, E., Sapyta, J. J., & Suh, E. (1998). Subjective well-being is essential to
well-being. Psychological Inquiry, 9, 33–37.
Diener, E., Wirtz, D., Tov, W., Kim-Prieto, C., Choi, D.-W., Oishi, S., &
Biswas-Diener, R. (2010). New well-being measures: Short scales to assess
flourishing and positive and negative feelings. Social Indicators Research, 97,
143–156.
Digman, J. M. (1990). Personality structure: Emergence of the Five-Factor
model. Annual Review of Psychology, 41, 417–440.
Dijkic, M., & Oatley, K. (2004). Love and personal relationships: Navigating
on the border between the ideal and the real. Journal for the Theory of Social
Behaviour, 34, 199–209.
Dixon, J., & Levine, M. (2012). Introduction. In J. Dixon & M. Levine (Eds.),
Beyond prejudice: Extending the social psychology of conflict, inequality and social
change (pp. 1–22). Cambridge: Cambridge University press.
180 REFERENCES
Field, A. P., & Nightingale, Z. C. (2009). Test of time: What if Little Albert
had escaped? Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 14, 311–319.
Figueredo, A. J., Gladden, P., Vasquez, G., Wolf, P. A. A., & Jones, D. N.
(2009). Evolutionary theories of personality. In P. J. Corr & G. Matthews
(Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of personality psychology (pp. 265274–265376).
Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Fincham, F. D., & Beach, S. R. (1999). Marital conflict: Implications for work-
ing with couples. Annual Review of Psychology, 50, 47–77.
Finkel, E. J., Cheung, E. O., Emery, L. F., Carswell, K. L., & Larson, G. M.
(2015). The suffocation model: Why marriage in America is becoming an
all-or-nothing institution. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 24, 238–
244.
Finkel, E. J., Hui, C. M., Carswell, K. L., & Larson, G. M. (2014). The suffo-
cation of marriage: Climbing Mount Maslow without enough oxygen. Psy-
chological Inquiry, 25, 1–41.
Finkel, E. J., Larson, G. M., Carswell, K. L., & Hui, C. M. (2014). Marriage at
the summit: Response to the commentaries. Psychological Inquiry, 25, 120–145.
Finkel, E. J., Simpson, J. A., & Eastwick, P. W. (2017). The psychology of close
relationships: Fourteen core principles. Annual Review of Psychology, 68, 383–411.
Fisher, S. (1949). An overview of trends in research dealing with personality
rigidity. Journal of Personality, 17, 342–351.
Fiske, A. P., Kitayama, S., Markus, H. R., & Nisbett, R. E. (1998). The cul-
tural matrix of social psychology. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey
(Eds.), The handbook of social psychology (4th ed., Vol. 2, pp. 915–981).
New York: McGraw-Hill.
Fiske, D. W. (1949). Consistency of the factorial structures of personality
ratings from different sources. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 44,
329–344.
Fiske, S. T. (1998). Prejudice, stereotyping, and discrimination. In
D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology
(4th ed., pp. 357–411). New York: McGraw-Hill.
Fleeson, W. (2012). Perspectives on the person: Rapid growth and oppor-
tunities for integration. In K. Deaux & M. Snyder (Eds.), Oxford hand-
book of personality and social psychology (pp. 33–63). Oxford: Oxford
University Press.
Foa, U. G., & Foa, E. B. (1974). Societal structures of the mind. Springfield, IL:
Charles C. Thomas.
Frable, D. E. S. (1997). Gender, racial, sexual, and class identities. Annual
Review of Psychology, 48, 139–163.
Freeman, T. (2008). Psychoanalytic concepts of fatherhood: Patriarchal para-
doxes and the presence of an absent authority. Studies in Gender and Sexuality,
9, 113–139.
REFERENCES 183
Freidan, B. (1963). The feminine mystique. New York: Norton.
Freud, A. (1958). Adolescence. Psychoanalytic Study of the Child, 13, 255–278.
Freud, A. (1966). The ego and the mechanisms of defense. New York: International
Universities Press. (Original work published 1936).
Freud, A. (1975). Introduction to the technique of child analysis. Stratford, NH:
Ayer Company Publishers. (Original work published 1927).
Freud, S. (1922). Group psychology and the analysis of the ego. New York: Bond &
Liveright.
Freud, S. (1925). Character and anal eroticism. In S. Freud, Collected papers
(Vol. 2, pp. 167–175). London: Hogarth Press. (Original work published
1908).
Freud, S. (1927). The ego and the id. London: Hogarth Press. (Original work
published 1923).
Freud, S. (1950). Libidinal types. In S. Freud, Collected papers (Vol. 5). London:
Hogarth Press. (Original work published 1931).
Freud, S. (1953). On narcissism: An introduction. In J. Strachey (Ed. & Trans.),
The standard edition of the complete psychological works of Sigmund Freud (Vol.14,
pp. 69–102). London: Hogarth Press. (Original work published 1914).
Freud, S. (1961). Beyond the pleasure principle. New York: Norton. (Original
work published 1920).
Freud, S. (1962). Three essays on the theory of sexuality (James Strachey, transla-
tor). New York: Basic Books. (Original work published 1905).
Freud, S. (1965). The interpretation of dreams. New York: Avon. (Original work
published 1900).
Freud, S. (1990). New introductory lectures on psychoanalysis. New York: Norton.
(Original work published 1933).
Fridlund, A. J., Beck, H. P., Goldie, W. D., & Irons, G. (2012). Little Albert:
A neurolologically impaired child. History of Psychology, 15, 302–327.
Friedman, H. (2008). Humanistic and positive psychology: The methodological
and epistemological divide. Humanistic Psychologist, 36, 113–126.
Frings, D. (2019). Social psychology: The basics. London: Routledge.
Fromm, E. (1941). Escape from freedom. New York: Farrar & Rinehart.
Fromm, E. (1957). The art of loving. London: George Allen & Unwin.
Funder, D. C. (1991). Global traits: A neo-Allportian approach to personality.
Psychological Science, 2, 31–39.
Funder, D. C. (2001). Personality. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 197–221.
Funder, D. C., & Fast, L. A. (2010). Personality in social psychology. In
S. T. Fiske, D. T. Gilbert, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology
(5th ed., Vol. 1, pp. 668–697). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
Gaines, S. O., Jr. (2007a). Personality and personal relationship processes: An
introduction to the special issue. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 24,
475–478.
184 REFERENCES
Gaines, S. O., Jr. (2007b). Personality and personal relationship processes: Con-
cluding thoughts. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 24, 613–617.
Gaines, S. O., Jr. (2012). Stereotyping, prejudice and discrimination revisited:
From William James to W. E. B. Du Bois. In J. Dixon & M. Levine (Eds.),
Beyond prejudice: Extending the social psychology of conflict, inequality and social
change (pp. 105–119). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Gaines, S. O., Jr. (2017). Identity and interethnic marriage in the United States.
New York: Routledge.
Gaines, S. O., Jr. (2018). Personality and close relationship processes. Cambridge,
UK: Cambridge University Press. (Original work published 2016).
Gaines, S. O., Jr., Marelich, W. D., Bledsoe, K. L., Steers, W. N.,
Henderson, M. C., Granrose, C. S., Barajas, L., Hicks, D., Lyde, M.,
Takahashi, Y., Yum, N., Rios, D. I., Garcia, B. F., Farris, K., & Page, M. S.
(1997a). links between race/ethnicity and cultural values as mediated by
racial/ethnic identity and moderated by gender. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 72, 1460–1476.
Gaines, S. O., Jr. (1996). Impact of interpersonal traits and gender-role compli-
ance on interpersonal resource exchange among dating and engaged/married
couples. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 13, 241–261.
Gaines, S. O., Jr., Marelich, W. D., Bunce, D., Robertson, T., &
Wright, B. C. (2013). MEIM expansion: Racial, religious, and national
aspects of sense of ethnic identity within the United Kingdom. Identity: an
International Journal of Theory and Research, 13, 289–317.
Gaines, S. O., Jr., Panter, A. T., Lyde, M. D., Steers, W. N., Rusbult, C. E.,
Cox, C. L., & Wexler, M. O. (1997b). Evaluating the circumplexity of
interpersonal traits and the manifestation of interpersonal traits in interper-
sonal trust. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 610–623.
Gaines, S. O., Jr., & Reed, E. S. (1994). Two social psychologies of prejudice:
Gordon W. Allport, W. E. B. Du Bois, and the legacy of Booker T.
Washington. Journal of Black Psychology, 20, 8–28.
Gaines, S. O., Jr., & Reed, E. S. (1995). Prejudice: From Allport to Du Bois.
American Psychologist, 50, 96–103.
Galton, F. (1869). Hereditary genius. London: Macmillan.
Gammelgaard, J. (2003). Ego, self and otherness. Scandinavian Psychoanalytic
Review, 26, 96–108.
Gangestad, S. W. (2012). Evolutionary perspectives. In K. Deaux & M. Snyder
(Eds.), The Oxford handbook of personality and social psychology (pp. 151–181).
Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Garcia-Valdecases, M. (2005). Psychology and mind in Aquinas. History of
Psychiatry, 16, 291–310.
Gaughan, E. T., Miller, J. D., & Lynam, D. R. (2012). Examining the utility of
general models of personality in the study of psychopathy: A comparison of
REFERENCES 185
the HEXACO-PI-R and NEO PI-R. Journal of Personality Disorders, 26,
513–523.
Geen, R. G. (1997). Psychophysiological approaches to personality. In
R. Hogan, J. A. Johnson, & S. R. Briggs (Eds.), Handbook of personality psych-
ology (pp. 387–414). New York: Academic Press.
Gieser, M. T. (1993). The first behavior therapist as I knew her. Journal of
Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 24, 321–324.
Gilbert, D. T. (1998). Ordinary personology. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, &
G. Lindzey (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology (4th ed., Vol. 2, pp. 89–
150). Boston, MA: McGraw-Hill.
Gildersleeve, M. (2015). Beauvoir and demystifying paradoxical characteristics of
Narcissistic Personality Disorder. Indian Journal of Psychological Medicine, 37,
251–253.
Gillett, G. (2001). Signification and the unconscious. Philosophical Psychology,
14, 477–498.
Gilliam, C. (2017). The existential unconscious: Sartre and the dialectic of
freedom. Existential Analysis, 28, 351–360.
Gobet, F. (in preparation). Intelligence: The basics. London: Routledge.
Goddard, M. J. (2012). On certain similarities between mainstream psychology
and the writings of B. F. Skinner. Psychological Record, 62, 563–576.
Goffman, E. (1959). The presentation of self in everyday life. Garden City, NY:
Doubleday.
Goffman, E. (1963). Stigma: Notes on the management of spoiled identity. Engle-
wood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Goldberg, L. (1981). Language and individual differences: The search for uni-
versals in personality lexicons. In L. Wheeler (Ed.), Review of personality and
social psychology (pp. 141–165). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Goldberg, L. R. (1999). A broad-bandwideth, public domain, personality
inventory measuring the lower-level facets of several five-factor models. In
I. Mervielde, I. Deary, F. DeFruyt, & F. Ostendorf (Eds.), Personality psych-
ology in Europe (Vol. 7, pp. 7–28). Tilburg, The Netherlands: Tilburg Uni-
versity Press.
Goldsmith, H. H., Buss, A. H., Plomin, R., Rothbart, M. K., Thomas, A.,
Chess, S., Hinde, R. A., & McCall, R. B. (1987). What is temperament?
Four approaches. Child Development, 58, 505–529.
Goldstein, K. (1939). The organism: A holistic approach to biology derived from patho-
logical data in man. New York: American Book Company.
Goldstein, K. (1940). Human nature in the light of psychopathology. Cambridge,
MA: Harvard University Press.
Gore, P. M., & Rotter, J. B. (1963). A personality correlate of social action.
Journal of Personality, 31, 58–64.
186 REFERENCES
McAdams, D. P., Albaugh, M., Farber, E., Daniels, J., Logan, R. L., &
Olson, B. (2008). Family metaphors and moral intuitions: How conservatives
and liberals narrate their lives. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95,
978–990.
McAdams, D. P., & Bryant, F. B. (1987). Intimacy motivation and subjective
mental health in a nationwide sample. Journal of Personality, 55, 395–413.
McAdams, D. P., Diamond, A., de St. Aubin, E., & Mansfield, E. D. (1997).
Stories of commitment: The psychosocial construction of generative lives.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 678–694.
McAdams, D. P., & Manczak, E. (2015). Personality and the life story. In
M. Mikulincer & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), APA handbook of personality and social
psychology (Vol. 4: Personality processes and individual differences, pp. 425–
446). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
McAdams, D. P., & Powers, J. (1981). Themes of intimacy in behavior and
thought. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40, 573–587.
McAdams, D. P., Reynolds, J., Lewis, M., Patten, A., & Bowman, P. J. (2001).
When bad things turn good and good things turn bad: Sequences of redemp-
tion and contamination in life narrative, and their relation to psychosocial
adaptation in midlife adults and in students. Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin, 27, 472–483.
McClelland, D. C. (1966). Longitudinal trends in the relation of thought to
action. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 30, 479–483.
McClelland, D. C. (1987). Human motivation. New York: Cambridge Univer-
sity Press. (Original work published 1985).
McClelland, D. C., Atkinson, J. W., Clark, R. A., & Lowell, E. L. (1953). The
achievement motive. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
McCrae, R. (2009). The Five-Factor Model of personality traits: Consensus and
controversy. In P. J. Corr & G. Matthews (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of
personality psychology (pp. 148–161). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. C., Jr. (1985). Using Norman’s “adequacy tax-
onomy”: Intelligence and personality dimensions in natural language and in
questionnaires. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 710–721.
McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T., Jr. (1986). Clinical assessment can benefit from
recent advances in personality psychology. American Psychologist, 41, 1001–
1003.
McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T., Jr. (1989). More reasons to adopt the
Five-Factor Model. American Psychologist, 44, 451–452.
McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T., Jr. (1997). Personality trait structure as
a human universal. American Psychologist, 52, 509–516.
McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T., Jr. (2008). The five factor theory of personal-
ity. In O. P. John, R. W. Robins, & L. A. Pervin (Eds.), Handbook of person-
ality: Theory and research (pp. 159–181). New York: Guilford Press.
REFERENCES 197
McDonald, M., & Wearing, S. (2013). A reconceptualisation of the self in
humanistic psychology: Heidegger, Foucault and the sociocultural turn. Jour-
nal of Phenomenological Psychology, 44, 37–59.
McLaughlin, N. (1998a). How to become a forgotten intellectual: Intellec-
tual movements and the rise and fall of Erich Fromm. Sociological Forum,
13, 215–246.
McLaughlin, N. (1999). Origin myths in the social sciences: Fromm, the
Frankfurt School and the emergence of critical theory. Canadian Journal of
Sociology, 24, 109–139.
McLaughlin, N. G. (1998b). Why do schools of thought fail? Neo-Freudianism
as a case study in the sociology of knowledge. Journal of the History of the
Behavioral Sciences, 34, 113–134.
McNulty, J. K. (2013). Personality and relationships. In L. Campbell &
J. A. Simpson (Eds.), Oxford handbook of close relationships (pp. 535–552).
Oxford: Oxford University Press.
McSherry, A. (2013). Jacques Lacan’s theory of the subject as real, sym-
bolic and imaginary: How can Lacanian theory be of help to mental
health nursing practice? Journal of Psychiatric and Mental Health Nursing,
20, 776–781.
Mead, G. H. (1967). Mind, self and society from the standpoint of a social behaviorist.
Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. (Original work published 1934).
Medina, M. (2008). Everyday courage: Living courageously without being a
hero. Existential Analysis, 19, 280–299.
Merkur, D. (2010). Explorations of the psychoanalytic mystics (Contemporary Psycho-
analytic Studies, vol. 11). Tijinmuiden, The Netherlands: Rodopi Press.
Meronen, P. (1999). The return of narcissism: Heinz Kohut in the context of
the history of ideas. International Forum of Psychoanalysis, 8, 211–220.
Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2016). Attachment in adulthood: Structure,
dynamics, and change (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford Press.
Milavec, A. (2006). How acts of discovery transform our tacit knowing powers
in both scientific and religious inquiry. Zygon, 41, 464–486.
Miller, N. E., & Dollard, J. (1941). Social learning and imitation. New Haven,
CT: Yale University Press.
Miller, N. E., & Dollard, J. (1950). Personality and psychotherapy. New York:
McGraw-Hill.
Millon, T. (1996). Disorders of personality: DSM-IV and beyond. New York:
Wiley.
Milton, M. J. (1993). Existential thought and client centered therapy. Counsel-
ling Psychology Quarterly, 6, 239–248.
Mischel, W. (1968). Personality and assessment. New York: Wiley.
Mischel, W. (1973). Toward a cognitive social learning reconceptualization of
personality. Psychological Review, 80, 252–283.
198 REFERENCES
Robins, R. W., & John, O. P. (1997). The quest for self-insight: Theory and
research on accuracy and bias in self-perception. In R. Hogan, J. A. Johnson,
& S. R. Briggs (Eds.), Handbook of personality psychology (pp. 649–679).
New York: Academic Press.
Robinson, M. D., & Wilkowski, B. M. (2015). Personality processes and pro-
cesses as personality: A cognitive perspective. In M. Mikulincer &
P. R. Shaver (Eds.), APA handbook of personality and social psychology (Vol. 4,
pp. 129–145). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Robinson, T. N., Jr., & Zahn, T. P. (1983). Sensation seeking, state anxiety
and cardiac and EDR orienting reactions. Psychophysiology, 20, 465.
Rocha, G. M. (2012). The unconscious: Ideal worker? International Forum of
Psychoanalysis, 21, 17–21.
Rogers, C. R. (1951). Client-centered therapy: Its current practice, implications and
theory. London: Constable.
Rogers, C. R. (1959). A Theory of therapy, personality, and interpersonal rela-
tionships as developed in the client-centered framework. In S. Koch (Ed.),
Psychology: A study of a science (Vol. 3, pp. 184–256). New York: McGraw-Hill.
Rogers, C. R. (1961). On becoming a person: A therapist’s view of psychotherapy.
London: Constable.
Rorschach, H. (1924). Manual for Rorschach Ink-Blot test. Chicago, IL: Stoelting.
Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton, NJ:
Princeton University Press.
Rosenberg, M. (1989). Society and the adolescent self-image (rev. ed.). Princeton,
NJ: Princeton University Press.
Ross, L., Lepper, M., & Ward, A. (2010). History of social psychology:
Insights, challenges, and contributions to theory and applications. In
S. T. Fiske, D. T. Gilbert, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology
(5th ed., Vol. 1, pp. 3–50). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
Rotter, J. B. (1954). Social learning and clinical psychology. Englewood Cliffs, NJ:
Prentice-Hall.
Rotter, J. B. (1966). Generalized expectancies for internal versus external control
of reinforcement. Psychological Monographs, 80 (1, Whole No. 609), 1–28.
Rotter, J. B. (1967). A new scale for the measurement of interpersonal trust.
Journal of Personality, 35, 651–665.
Rotter, J. B. (1975). Some problems and misconceptions related to the con-
struct of internal versus external control of reinforcement. Journal of Consult-
ing and Clinical Psychology, 48, 56–67.
Rowan, J. (2000). The self, the field and the either-or. International Journal of
Psychotherapy, 5, 219–226.
Rowe, D. C. (1997). Genetics, temperament, and personality. In R. Hogan,
J. Johnson, & S. Briggs (Eds.), Handbook of personality psychology (pp. 367–
386). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
REFERENCES 203
Rubinstein, G. (2010a). Narcissism and self-esteem among homosexual and
heterosexual male students. Journal of Sex & Marital Therapy, 36, 24–34.
Rubinstein, G. (2010b). Response to Hartmann. Journal of Sex and Marital Ther-
apy, 36, 48.
Rusbridger, R. (2012). Affects in Melanie Klein. International Journal of Psycho-
analysis, 93, 139–150.
Rusbult, C. E., & Van Lange, P. A. M. (2003). Interdependence, interaction,
and relationships. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 351–375.
Rusbult, C., Verette, J., Whitney, G., Slovik, L., & Lipkus, I. (1991). Accom-
modation processes in close relationships: Theory and preliminary evidence.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 53–78.
Rutter, M., & Silberg, J. (2002). Gene-environment interplay in relation to emo-
tional and behavioral disturbance. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 463–490.
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000a). Self-determination theory and the facilita-
tion of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being. American
Psychologist, 55, 68–78.
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000b). The darker and brighter sides of human
existence: Basic psychological needs as a unifying concept. Psychological
Inquiry, 11, 319–338.
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2001). On happiness and human potentials:
A review of research on hedonic and eudaimonic well-being. Annual Review
of Psychology, 52, 141–166.
Ryan, R. M., & Frederick, C. (1997). On energy, personality, and health: Sub-
jective vitality as a dynamic reflection of well-being. Journal of Personality, 65,
529–565.
Ryff, C. D. (1989). Happiness is everything, or is it? Explorations on the mean-
ing of psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57,
1069–1081.
Ryff, C. D. (1995). Psychological well-being in adult life. Current Directions in
Psychological Science, 4, 99–104.
Ryff, C. D., & Keyes, C. L. M. (1995). The structure of psychological
well-being revisited. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69,
719–727.
Ryff, C. D., & Singer, B. (1998a). Human health: New directions for the next
millennium. Psychological Inquiry, 9, 69–85.
Ryff, C. D., & Singer, B. (2008). Know thyself and become what you are:
A eudaimonic approach to psychological well-being. Journal of Happiness
Studies, 9, 13–39.
Ryff, C. D., & Singer, B. (1998b). The contours of positive mental health. Psy-
chological Inquiry, 9, 1–28.
Salzinger, K. (2008). Skinner’s verbal behavior. International Journal of Psychology
and Psychological Therapy, 8, 287–294.
204 REFERENCES
SWLS see Satisfaction with Life Scale 122–124; see also “Big Five”
systematic desensitisation 67 personality traits
transference 65
Tajfel, Henri 10, 39 transitional objects 42
“talking cure” 17–18 Trapnell, P. 123, 125
Taormina, Robert 89 trauma 45
Tate, C. C. 150 Trivers, Robert L. 139, 149–150
Taylor, S. E. 130, 138 true self 42
temperament 116–117, 139, 143, trust 159–160
148–149 Turner, B. J. 35
Temperament and Character
Inventory (TCI) 147 Umana-Taylor, Adriana 38, 39–40
Teo, T. 52 unconditional positive regard 91, 92
Terry, H. 52 the unconscious 20, 48–50, 93, 158
tests 36 unconscious processes 17
thanatos 21, 34–35, 40, 46 urethral stage 22
Thematic Apperception Test (TAT)
55–56 values 4, 7, 12, 130, 164
Thibaut, John W. 133 Van der Veer, R. 41
Thompson, Clara 56 Van Dijken, S. 41
Thorndike, Edward L. 57, 63–65, Van Ijzendoorn, M. 41
66, 69 Van Lange, P. A. M. 133
thought processes 129–130 virtues 37
Titchener, Edward B. 63
Tolman, Edward C. 57, 63, 66–69
Tosi, D. J. 88 Walkey, F. H. 77
traits 5, 103–104, 107–126; Allport 4, Wall, S. 46–47, 49
7–9, 19, 107–108, 109, 110, Wallace, Alfred Russell 140, 142
111–112, 124, 126, 164; Ashton Walsh, R. 33, 52
and Lee 117–119; basic Walters, Richard 57, 63, 78–79,
assumptions 108–109; Bem’s 126–127, 132
gender schema theory 119–120; Waterman, A. S. 37–38
Cattell 110–111; cognitive theories Waters, E. 46–47, 49
130; Costa and McCrae 113–114; Watson, John B. 57, 63, 65–66, 67,
critique of trait perspectives 68, 70, 72
124–126; cultural psychology Wegner, D. M. 155–156
141–142; definition of 109; Weiner, B. 75
Eysenck 111–113, 139, 144; Weisberg, Y. J. 125
gender-related 11; Guilford well-being: eudaimonic 101, 102;
114–117; Myers-Briggs Type hedonic 101, 102; psychological
Indicator 26; Spence’s 99–100, 101; subjective 98–99,
multifactorial gender identity 100, 101
theory 120–122, 160; Sullivan’s West, S. G. 74–75
interpersonal theory 30; Westen, D. 34
temperament 148; Wiggins’s Whitbourne, S. K. 37–38
interpersonal circumplex theory White, W. F. 78, 79
INDEX 229
Wiggins, Jerry S. 104, 109, 122–124, achievement 55; parental
125, 165 investment 149–150; psychosocial
Wilhelm, J. A. 121 development 37–38; sexual strategy
will 160–163 theory 150
Williams, Kevin 161 Women’s Rights Movement 11, 18,
Winnicott, Donald 13, 20, 41–43, 44, 28, 56
45, 48 Wundt, Wilhelm 61, 63, 163
Winston, A. 35
womb envy 28 Yazedijan, A. 38, 39
women: Buhler 87; critiques of Freud
56–57; “Dark Triad” 161–162; de
Beauvoir’s existential philosophy Zajonc, R. B. 78–79
94–95; Fanon’s existential Zimmerman, Wayne S. 116
psychology 96; Horney’s feminine Zuckerman, Marvin 145,
psychology 27–29; need for 146–147