Okcu Selen 201105 PHD
Okcu Selen 201105 PHD
A Dissertation
Presented to
The Academic Faculty
by
Selen Okcu
In Partial Fulfillment
of the Requirements for the Degree
Doctor of Philosophy in the
School of Architecture
May, 2011
DEVELOPING EVIDENCE-BASED DESIGN METRICS AND
METHODS FOR IMPROVING HEALTHCARE SOUNDSCAPES
Approved by:
dissertation work. Dr. Craig Zimring was an open minded supporter at all times and a
guide. Dr. Ryherd introduced me to the interesting world of acoustics. Her willingness
and openness to address multi-disciplinary research was always motivating. Dr. Bafna`s
thoughtful comments provided some of the very interesting and key directions in this
study. I highly appreciate Dr. Shpuza`s collaboration that helped facilitate some of this
work. Our early brainstorming sessions helped me start critically thinking about the
relationships between design and acoustics. He was also very generous to share with me
his computerized floor-plate shape analysis tool. I am also grateful to Mr. Pelton for
providing his expert knowledge. It has been very much mind opening to work with
I also would like to thank to numerous people outside the Georgia Institute of
Technology who contributed to this study directly and indirectly. I appreciate Dr. Owen
Ann Huntley and Mary Still, registered nurses Tim Rice and Anya Freeman and to all
Neuro-ICU and MedSurg ICU nurses, patients and family members for their continuous
I cannot say enough about my dearest parents (Ayse and Hikmet Okcu) for their role in
getting me where I am. There is no greater comfort than having a supportive and loving
iii
family who believes in you deeply. They were always there during the good and bad
(Ahmet Okcu) for taking care of my parents when I was not physically there. I am also
very thankful to my dearest Tim and Mr. and Mrs. Frank for their support and generosity.
I can never thank enough to my dearest Tim for his kindness and warmness, essentially
iv
TABLE OF CONTENTS
Page
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS iii
LIST OF TABLES ix
LIST OF FIGURES xi
SUMMARY xix
CHAPTERS
1.1 BACKGROUND 1
1.2 PROBLEM STATEMENT 2
1.3 SCOPE AND OBJECTIVES 2
1.4 RESEARCH QUESTIONS 4
1.5 SIGNIFICANCE 8
v
2.4 EMPIRICAL STUDY 3:
EFFECTS OF PARTICULAR HOSPITAL NOISE SOURCES
2.4.1 Scope 61
2.4.2 Methodology 61
2.4.3 Findings 61
2.4.4 Discussion 64
vi
CHAPTER 5. MEASUREMENTS AND SIMULATIONS OF THE
ACOUSTICS OF LONG ENCLOSURES
vii
CHAPTER 7. VALIDATION STUDY
CHAPTER 8. CONCLUSION
APPENDICES
REFERENCES 299
VITA 312
viii
LIST OF TABLES
Page
Table 2.1: Mean perception ratings of noise-induced nurse outcomes in the two ICUs 38
Table 2.2: Mean perception ratings of loudness and annoyance in the two ICUs 39
Table 2.3: Correlations between objective noise and subjective noise levels 41
Table 2.4: Speech interference levels at different locations in the two ICUs 46
Table 2.5: Annoyance and loudness perceptions for weekdays vs. weekend 51
Table 2.6: Annoyance and loudness perceptions during different times of the day 53
Table 5.2: JND values for the room averaged RT30 131
Table 5.3: JND values for RT30 measured at 150ft away from the source 132
Table 5.4: Material absorption coefficients of the race track design corridor 134
Table 5.6: JND values for RT30 at the selected receiver locations 137
Table 5.8: JND values for room averaged predicted and measured RT30 140
Table 5.9: JND values for RT30 at the selected receiver locations 142
Table 6.1: Partial correlation analysis results linking corridor design and RT30 167
Table 6.2: Partial correlation analysis results linking radial distance and RT30 169
ix
Table 6.5: Correlation analysis results (absorption coefficient = 0.1) 183
Table 6.6: Partial correlation analysis results linking RT30, and RGD + VF 190
Table 6.9 Summary of the empirical study findings conducted in Chapter 6 196
Table 7.1: Absorption coefficients of materials applied to surfaces of the two wards 211
Table 7.2: JND results of measured RT30 in the two wards 212
Table 7.3: JND results of simulated RT30 at the “T” and “+”shaped intersections 214
Table 7.4: JND results of RT30 at the corridors w/ different number of branches 215
Table 7.5: JND results of RT30 at the corridors with different number of turns 218
Table 7.6: JND results of RT30 at the corridors with different corridor length 220
Table 7.7: JND results of RT30 at the corridors with different total corridor length 222
Table 7.8: JND results of RT30 at the corridors with similar total corridor length 224
x
LIST OF FIGURES
Page
Figure 2.1: Overall LAeq, LMax, LMin and LPeak noise levels in the two ICU`s 32
Figure 2.2: Occurrence rate of different LMax noise levels in the two ICU`s 33
Figure 2.4: Occurrence rate of LMax noise levels at the nurse stations 36
Figure 2.9: Non-weighted noise fluctuation levels, annoyance and loudness levels 44
Figure 2.10: Floor plans of the two ICUs showing the distribution of nurse stations 50
Figure 2.14: Occurrence rate of Lpeak noise levels during the day times 56
Figure 2.15: Occurrence rate of Lpeak noise levels during the shift changes 57
Figure 2.16: Occurrence rate of Lpeak noise levels during the night times 57
Figure 2.17: Overall Leq, Lmax, Lpeak and Lmin levels in the two ICU`s 58
Figure 2.18: Overall occurrence rates of Lpeak noise levels in the two ICU`s 59
Figure 2.19: Percentage of nurses who agreed that they sometime tune out alarms 63
xi
Figure 3.3: Perceived importance of auditory monitoring vs. visual monitoring 87
Figure 3.4: Perceived importance of different medical alarms for patient safety 88
Figure 3.5: Frequency of auditory monitoring at different locations in the two ICU`s 90
Figure 3.6: Perceived sound task performance of nurses in the two ICUs 91
Figure 4.1: Commonly applied concert hall floor plate shapes 101
Figure 4.2: Royal Albert Hall with elliptical floor-plate shape 102
Figure 4.3: Boston Symphony Hall with rectangular floor-plate shape 103
Figure 4.4: Berlin Philharmonie Concert Hall with vineyard floor-plate shape 104
Figure 4.8: RT30 and EDT due to absorptive qualities of boundaries 113
Figure 4.9: RT30 and EDT due to reflective qualities of boundaries 114
Figure 4.10: Floor-plans of two long enclosures with different length 115
Figure 4.14: Spatial distribution of RT30 along a “+” street junction 120
Figure 4.15: Floor-plans of theoretical long enclosures with a single branch 121
Figure 4.16: Distribution of RT30 in long enclosures with a single branch 122
Figure 4.17: Floor-plans of theoretical long enclosures with multiple branches 123
Figure 4.18: Distribution of RT30 in long enclosures with multiple branches 124
Figure 5.3: Measured vs. predicted RT30 at a selected receiver location 132
xii
Figure 5.4: 3D-CATT model and floor-plan of a race track corridor 133
Figure 5.5: Room averaged RT30 in a race track design corridor 135
Figure 5.6: RT30 at the selected receiver locations in a race track design corridor 136
Figure 5.9: Measured vs. predicted RT30 at the selected locations (T-shaped) 141
Figure 6.1: Spatial distribution of sound and noise sources in the two ICUs 151
Figure 6.2: A speech sound that is masked differently in two reverberant rooms 152
Figure 6.3: Total length of nursing unit corridors in different hospitals 154
Figure 6.4: Floor plans of nursing units located in different hospitals 155
Figure 6.8: Sound propagation in race track and tree-like design corridors 161
Figure 6.10: Plans of the corridors located in the educational buildings 165
Figure 6.13: 3D-CATT models of theoretical race track design hallways 178
Figure 6.15: The relationship between RGD + VF and RT30 at 500Hz 191
Figure 6.16: The relationship between RGD + VF and RT30 at 1kHz 192
xiii
Figure 6.17: Scatter plots showing the relationship b/w RGD, VF and RT30 194
Figure 6.18: Scatter plots showing the relationship b/w RGD, VF and RT30 195
Figure 7.1 Impulse response measurements in the two ICU corridors 200
Figure 7.2: Distribution of source and receiver locations in the Neuro-ICU 200
Figure 7.3: Distribution of source and receiver locations in the MedSurg-ICU 201
Figure 7.4: Source and receiver locations at the “T” and “cross” intersections 202
Figure 7.5: Source and receiver locations along the corridors with multiple branches 203
Figure 7.6: Source and receiver locations along the corridors without any branches 204
Figure 7.7: Distribution of source and receivers (2 turns away from the source) 205
Figure 7.8: Distribution of source and receivers that is 1 turn away from the source 205
Figure 7.9: Distribution of source and receivers along a long corridor 206
Figure 7.10: Distribution of source and receivers along a shorter corridor 207
Figure 7.11: Distribution of source and receivers in the Neuro-ICU corridors 208
Figure 7.12: Distribution of source and receivers in the MedSurg-ICU corridors 208
Figure 7.13: Distribution of source and receivers in the idealized Neuro-ICU 209
Figure 7.14: Distribution of source and receivers in the MedSurg-ICU corridors 210
Figure 7.15: Averaged RT30 in the MedSurg-ICU and Neuro-ICU corridors 212
Figure 7.16: RT30 at the “T” and “+”shaped intersections ( 0.3) 213
Figure 7.17: RT30 at the “T” and “+”shaped intersections ( 0.1) 214
Figure 7.18: RT30 at the corridors with different number of branches ( 0.3) 215
Figure 7.19: RT30 at the corridors with different number of braches ( 0.1) 215
Figure 7.20: RT30 at the corridors 1-2 turns away from the source ( 0.3) 217
Figure 7.21: RT30 at the corridors 1-2 turns away from the source ( 0.1) 217
Figure 7.22: RT30 in the corridors with different length ( 0.3) 219
Figure 7.23: RT30 in the corridors with different length ( 0.1) 219
xiv
Figure 7.24: RT30 in the corridors with different total length ( 0.3) 221
Figure 7.25: RT30 in the corridors with different total length ( 0.1) 221
Figure 7.26: RT30 in the corridors with similar total length ( 0.3) 223
Figure 7.27: RT30 in the corridors with similar total length ( 0.1) 223
xv
LIST OF ABBREVIATIONS
Acoustics
C50/80: clarity
D50/80: definition
dB: Decibel
G: Sound strength
xvi
MLS: Maximum length sequence
xvii
WHO: World health organization
xviii
SUMMARY
settings, medical staff members conduct vital tasks that may have life-and-death
implications. Patients visit hospitals to heal. Their expectations include fast recovery,
restful sleep, and privacy (e.g., speech privacy). However, sound environment qualities of
care settings often fall far from supporting the mission of hospitals. There is strong and
growing evidence showing that soundscapes in healthcare settings impact healing, errors
and stress for patients, families and staff but it is still not clear what measures of the
sound environment best predict key healthcare outcomes and what design strategies best
impact those measures. By using a multi-method approach, this study aims to develop a
between three types of variables: (1) architectural floor-plate design metrics, (2) acoustic
In Chapter 2, the study explores the effects of hospital sound environments on caregiver
critical care setting (ICU) with an award winning design ICU. For the objective and
subjective assessment of sound environments of the two ICUs, continuous noise level
statistical analysis results, critical care sound environments with different designs can
vary drastically and impact caregivers` perceived wellbeing and overall task
xix
performance. Differences between healthcare sound environments and nurses` annoyance
and loudness perceptions can also vary during different times of the day and days of the
week. Moreover, particular noise sources such as impulsive noises are likely to dominate
the ICU sound environments and interfere with perceived caregiver health and
performance. Despite their extensive use, traditional noise metrics sometimes may not be
study validates the effectiveness of a new more detailed noise metric, “occurrence rate”,
environments.
In Chapter 3, the study analyzes the association between healthcare sound environments
and specific type caregiver tasks: “critical sound tasks” (i.e., patient auditory monitoring).
In order to gain detailed information about critical sound tasks conducted by caregivers
and assess caregiver`s ability to conduct these tasks in different care settings, an online
survey is utilized. Interesting differences are found between critical sound task
the human auditory system. Analysis of the survey data also provides important baseline
information (e.g., types of critical sounds and listening locations) for the following
phases of the research. For example, caregivers reported that they frequently monitor
sounds in the corridors of their units as well as other locations such as patient rooms and
xx
nurse stations. However, the association between architectural floor-plate design and
in the following phases of this research more controlled studies are conducted via
sound behavior in proportional and non-proportional spaces and the association between
floor-plate design and acoustics are provided by reviewing literature. In Chapter 5, the
acoustic outcomes. In Chapter 6, the statistical analysis results clarifies the association
between floor-plate design characteristics (e.g., corridor length, number of turns and
Various theoretical models are generated based on the heuristic design analysis of various
nursing units. Acoustic qualities of these theoretical models are analyzed by the validated
real-world wards and field measurements are utilized in Chapter 6 to verify the proposed
xxi
Figure 1.1 Phases of the research (See Appendix A for the definition of terms and
descriptions of different analysis methods included in the graph above)
xxii
CHAPTER 1
RESEARCH PROBLEM
1.1 BACKGROUND
Hospitals are getting noisier each year (Busch-Vishniac et al., 2005). There is strong and
growing evidence of the negative impacts of a poor hospital soundscape such as staff
et al., 2005; Flynn et al., 1999; Morrison et al., 2003; Ryherd, Persson Waye, &
Ljungkvist, 2008; Topf, 1988; Topf, & Dillon, 1988). Patients may suffer from sleep
disturbance, anxiety, cardiovascular arousal, and decreased wound healing (Baker et al.,
1993; Freedman et al., 2001; Gabor et al., 2003; Hagerman et al. 2005; Hsu, Ryherd &
Persson Waye, 2009; McCarthy, Quimet, & Daun, 1992; Parthasarathy, & Tobin, 2004;
Wysocki, 1996). In critical care units, poor soundscapes might even have life-and-death
implications as caregivers rely on auditory cues such as help calls, and alarms. To date, a
remain limited because of the complex nature of hospital soundscapes and the limited
(Busch-Vishniac et al., 2005; Ryherd, Persson Waye, & Ljungkvist, 2008). Additionally,
while many hospitals are clearly noisy there is limited documentation of their actual
known that there are significant impacts of architectural design features on the
soundscapes of music halls, offices, and other spaces (Long, 2006). The few studies that
1
do exist show conflicting results; for example, studies conducted before and after
the soundscape (Blomkvist et al., 1996). This limits the ability of architects to effectively
A healing healthcare environment is quiet, orderly, and conducive to patient recovery and
safety, family comfort, and employee health and productivity. Too often, healthcare
facilities are noisy and stressful; the complex auditory environment, or “soundscape,” has
long been a key source of complaints. While there is an urgent need for effective
the architects` contribution in addressing this problem from the early design phase
experimental research to relate specific architectural layout metrics, acoustic metrics, and
2
o Test the effectiveness of traditional and more detailed acoustic metrics in
healthcare settings
outcomes
3
o Provide basis for the theoretical design analysis
Validation study: Acoustic analysis (via simulation and impulse response) of real
connected corridors
Chapter 2
Chapter 2 introduces the findings of three empirical studies. The main goal of these
study 1 assesses the objective and subjective noise levels at different locations in the two
ICUs. Empirical study 2 documents the objective and subjective noise levels during
different times at each unit nurse stations by conducting the occurrence rate analysis and
assessing nurse perceptions. Empirical study 3 compares the level of nurse disturbance
due one of the dominant impulsive sounds, clinical alarms, in the two ICUs. Specific
4
1. Do acoustic qualities (objective and subjective) of healthcare sound environments
3. Which noise measures are effective in capturing the differences between critical
Chapter3
and introduces the finding of a case study comparing nurses` auditory monitoring
performance in two ICUs with different architectural designs. Specific research questions
critical sounds?
monitoring?
3. What are the critical sounds that caregivers monitor for patient safety?
5
5. Do patient auditory monitoring performances of caregivers differ in healthcare
Chapter 4
proportional and non-proportional spaces, and the association between floor-plate design
are:
2. What are the design factors that affect the behavior of sound in proportional and
non-proportional spaces?
Chapter 5
Chapter 5 introduces the findings of various impulse response measurements and acoustic
1. Are the differences between predicted (via CATT acoustic modeling program)
perceptible?
6
Chapter6
Chapter 6 presents the results of a heuristic design analysis and statistically explores the
association between acoustics and design variables of interconnected corridors via three
educational settings. To test the findings of the pilot study, a more controlled follow up
Another theoretical design analysis (Empirical Study 2) is also conducted to assess the
are:
acoustic qualities?
Chapter 7
Chapter 7 assesses the validity of the previous study findings (in Chapter 6) by analyzing
the acoustics of real-world hospital ward corridors via impulse response measurements
and acoustic simulation analysis. Specific research questions addressed in Chapter 7 are:
7
1. Do field measurements and acoustic modeling predictions of the real-world wards
Chapter 8
Chapter 8 summarizes the findings of each chapter and defines the study contributions,
1.5 SIGNIFICANCE
This study addresses the issue of wellbeing and task supportive healthcare soundscapes
8
billion a year by 2011(FMI, 2006; Zimring, & Bosh, 2008). Potentially, the
results will have resonating effects on the health of society, as 4.9 million
Americans work in hospitals and many more are treated in them (U.S. Dept of
Labor, 2008).
Verify the necessity of using effective acoustic metrics and methods for the
outcomes.
providers.
Expand principles of statistical acoustics that have been widely used to explore
metrics.
Allow architects to design hospital layouts that are more conductive to occupant
9
addressed using engineering-oriented solutions such as sound-absorbing
materials.
10
CHAPTER 2
Growing research on hospital acoustics indicate that hospitals have noisy and multi-
source sound environments. Background noise levels in hospitals have been consistently
increasing (Bush-Vishniac et al. 2005). The study conducted a timeline analysis of noise
levels reported in thirty-five studies published over the last forty-five years. It was found
that since 1960, day time and night time hospital noise levels have risen from 57dBA to
(WHO) guidelines, background noise levels particularly in the patient rooms should not
exceed 30dBA for day time and 35dBA for night time (Berglund et al., 1999).
Previous research has used different metrics to measure and describe the sound
environment. Most research to-date has focused on characterizing overall noise levels.
Equivalent (Leq), minimum (Lmin), maximum (Lmax), and peak (Lpeak) sound pressure
levels have been most commonly reported. This may be based on the practicality and
convenience of these measures, and because they are incorporated into various guidelines
11
such as WHO. Leq, Lmin, Lmax and Lpeak sound levels provide a good general
overview of the sound environment, but are limited in usefulness. More detailed acoustic
measures such as the exceedance level (Ln), reverberation time (RT), speech
intelligibility (SI), and frequency analysis or noise criteria indicators of spectral content
have been less commonly reported. In the ICU, there are a diversity of noise sources such
complex, varying sound environment. More detailed acoustic measures are important to
more fully understand the spectral character of the sound environment, its behavior over
There is a good deal of variance in the methods applied during the sound sample
collections in the previous literature on ICUs. Compared to other spaces, noise levels in
the ICU patient rooms have been commonly documented. Measurements typically took
place either in a representative patient room (Christensen, 2005; McLaughlin et al., 1996;
Ryherd, Persson Waye, & Ljungvist, 2008) or in more than one patient room with
patient beds (Aaron et al., 1996; Aitken, 1982; Blomkvist et al., 2005; Bush-Vishniac et
al., 2005; Hilton, 1985; MacKenzie, & Galbrun, 2007; Meyer et al., 1994; Morrison et
al., 2003; Moore et al., 1998; Williams, Drongelen, & Lasky, 2007). There is general
consistency in the location of the sound meter, with most studies locating the microphone
as close as possible to patient head to capture what the patient hears while avoiding any
interference with nurse work flow. Hanging the microphone from the top of the medical
12
tower in the patient room has been introduced as a practical solution (Ryherd, Waye, &
Ljungvist, 2008).
Different durations have been used in the data collection, ranging from a few minutes to
168h, with 24h recordings most widely used. A few ICU noise studies have also
conducted measurements at the nurse stations using 24h or 168h durations (Blomkvist et
al., 2005; Bush-Vishniac et al., 2005; Moore et al., 1998). Busch-Vishniac et al. (2005)
also described the sound environment of hallways. There are also differences in the sound
level meter response times used, with many using a slow response time (1s) as suggested
by the Occupational Safety and Health Administration (OSHA) for typical occupational
noise measurements (ANSI 1996; Bush-Vishniac et al. 2005; Williams, Drongelen, &
Lasky, 2007). Some studies used fast response time (0.125s) as suggested by WHO to
better capture fluctuations (MacKenzie, & Galbrun, 2007; Ryherd, Waye, & Ljungvist,
2008). Averaging intervals also ranged from 5s to 24h (e.g., 30s, 1min, 5min). Among
ICU-noise studies the use of 1min averaging interval was most common. Measurements
were often analyzed as a function of time, with day time and night time average sound
levels commonly reported. Weekdays were most commonly measured, with limited data
on weekends. Morrison et al. (2003) and Ryherd, Waye, & Ljungvist (2008) considered
day and night time based on twelve hour nurse shifts (day time:7am-7pm; night time:
7pm-7am). MacKenzie, and Galbrun (2007) considered the day and night time periods
13
2.1.4 Hospital noise and its effects on staff members
The acoustic environment in hospitals can affect all occupants, including staff, patients,
and visitors. Related with the focus of this study, the following discussion is mostly
limited to its effects on staff members: stress and annoyance; work performance; health
outcomes and work overload. Further information about how hospital acoustics may
impact patients and visitors can be found in sources such as Busch-Vishniac et al. (2005)
and Ryherd et al. (2008). The staff’s wellbeing, efficiency and effectiveness in delivering
care and performing critical tasks is critical to maximize patient safety, satisfaction, and
ICUs are stressful care settings and the stress can be exacerbated by the sensory overload
caused by environmental factors, including the high noise levels. Stress is the individual’s
cope (Clarke, 1984). Depending on the severity and duration, stress may lead to illness
(e.g., elevated blood pressure, indigestion) and behavioral changes (e.g., sadness,
stimuli or activity producing arousal (Gast, & Baker, 1989). Excessive anxiety can be
detrimental and lead to different health effects such as sleep deprivation and confusion.
Like anxiety, annoyance is one of the early psychological responses which reflect the
negative quality of the environment stimuli (Baker, 1984), and it relates to the intrusion
14
In one study, higher average sound pressure levels predicted higher nurse heart rate,
2003). The sound pressure level was measured using the A-weighted equivalent sound
pressure level (LAeq). In this study, the sound levels were averaged every 30min.
Regression analysis was used to correlate the LAeq with continuously recorded heart rate
and saliva samples and stress/annoyance ratings that were also collected in every 30min.
During 3h periods, samples were collected from 11 nurses. Simultaneously noise levels
were also recorded. Subjective staff responses were gathered via the U.S. Army Research
Lab-Specific Rating of Event Scale. A total of 33h of sound measurements showed that
in multiple-bed patient rooms overall average sounds levels (Leq) were approximately
61dBA, and average sound levels during day (7am-7pm) and night (7pm-7am) were
6dBA and 59dBA, respectively. Three other noise measures considered in this study were
In another study, nurses working 8h evening shifts were significantly distressed by noise
in two ICUs surveyed (Topf, 1989). It was also found that less sensitivity to noise and
greater personality hardiness (such as commitment, control, and challenge) were linked
with less noise-induced stress. This research did not collect any objective sound data but
explored noise-induced subjective staff stress via personality hardiness measures and
sensitivity to noise with the use of regression. Their sample size was 100 ICU nurses.
Noise-induced stress data was collected via Disturbance Due to Hospital Noise Scale
(DDHNS). Other results were collected via the Weinstein noise sensitivity scale, locus of
15
control, work scale of alienation test, and security scale of California life goals evaluation
schedule.
There is a very limited number of noise-induced nurse stress studies conducted in the
ICUs. The following stress and annoyance studies are therefore included even though
they are not conducted in the ICUs but in other sections of the hospital. In one study,
enhanced reverberation time (RT) and speech intelligibility (SI) via acoustical
as low work demand, less strain, less pressure) (Blomkvist et al., 2005). The acoustical
8-bed coronary care unit that originally had a sound reflective ceiling (base condition).
Perceived psychosocial work environment data was collected repetitively before and after
every 8h shift from 36 nurses in both the base and renovated conditions. Noise levels
were also recorded during the two study periods, each of which lasted for 1 week. The
staff perception was evaluated with multiple psychological factors (such as strain,
variance (ANOVA) was used for the comparison of the data collected before and after the
renovation. The SI and RT levels were measured in 3 private patient rooms and at the
nurse station before and after the renovation. The RT levels improved from 0.8 s to
0.4sec at the nurse station and from 0.9s to 0.4s in the patient rooms after the design
intervention. Overall average sound levels (Leq) were 57dBA and 56dBA at the nurse
station and 56dBA and 50dBA in the patient room during the base and renovated
conditions, respectively.
16
In another study, noise-induced annoyance was reported as a problem by the hospital
staff (Bayo et al., 1995). In this study, descriptive statistics were used to describe noise-
induced perceived staff outcomes more than causal relationships between objective sound
levels and outcomes. Five- and 10-point scale surveys were used to measure perceived
staff outcomes and loudness, respectively. Among the 300 staff members surveyed, 70%
reported “very much” as the level of their annoyance due to noise. The other noise-
induced outcomes were: work performance (13%), quality of work (13%), personal
satisfaction (17%), and health (16%). Average perceived loudness was reported as 6.5 out
of 10, with 10 being “very loud”. Leq, Ln percentiles, Lmin, and Lmax at different
locations were measured based on 10min, 5min, and 1min sound measurements at 232
different locations inside the hospital on a regular floor (e.g., non-medical areas,
corridors, clinics, operating theatre, wards, intensive care units) and at 121 locations
outside the hospital (e.g., external premises and on the street). The Leq inside the hospital
ranged between 52-75dBA, and outside the hospital between 52-82dBA. The
To summarize, there is some evidence that high noise levels in care settings contribute to
staff stress and annoyance. However, the number of the studies specifically exploring the
relationship between ICU noise levels on staff outcomes remains very limited.
17
Work Performance
Hospital sound environments that are supportive of hospital tasks could potentially
improve staff effectiveness in delivering care. Improved nurse work performance in ICUs
can prevent adverse events, improve healthcare quality, and optimize resource utilization.
Studies of noise and hospital staff performance are quite limited, and some conflicting
results are seen. Work performance in hospital noise studies has previously been assessed
both quantitatively (i.e., mental efficacy, memory tests, and motion analysis systems)
(Murthy et al., 1995) and qualitatively (i.e., blind observation) (Moorthy et al., 2004).
variety of different ways (e.g., Leq, background noise levels, preferred speech
interference levels, noise criteria, sound pattern, Ln percentiles, Lmax, Lmin, Lpeak,
spectral qualities) (Hodge, & Thompson, 1990; Moorthy et al., 2004; Murthy et al., 1995;
One study found that noise in the workplace was perceived to have a negative impact on
staff work performance (91%) and concentration (43%) (Ryherd et al., 2008). Perceived
noise-induced outcomes were gathered from 47 nurses via 5-point scale questionnaire.
during 5 week days. Different noise criteria methods (i.e., RC , RC Mark II, NC, NCB)
were used to describe the sound quality of ICU noises. It was found that most ICU noises
were hissy (excessive energy from 1-8 kHz). The study also considered the mean length
of times when specific noise level conditions were met during the day and night times
18
such as Lpeak <75dBA (6min, 8min respectively); Lmax<55dBA (10min, 8min
overall average sound levels (ranged between Leq 53dBA-58dBA). Statistical level
exceedances were also reported. It was found that 90% of the time the maximum and
A Neonatal-ICU study showed that sound that exceeds 55dBA most of the time can
potentially interfere with work (Thomas, & Martin, 2000). This multidisciplinary
literature review study highlighted that tasks requiring rapid reaction time and vigilance
Noise-induced work performance research has been more commonly conducted in the
operating theatres. For example, it was found that noise conditions with Leq of 77dBA
were related to short term memory and decreased mental efficiency among twenty
anesthesia residents (Murthy et al., 1995). The Trail Making Test, Digital Symbol Test,
and Benton Visual Retentive Test were used for the assessment of cognitive tasks, mental
efficacy, and short term memory. The performance tests were conducted in an
acoustically treated room in which previously recorded operating room noise was played
back. Overall Leq levels were recorded during 5 different types of operations in the
operating room.
Another operating room study found that unpredictable and uncontrollable noise such as
intermittent, intrusive noise (i.e., alarms >75-85dBA) can negatively affect performance
19
and concentration during complex tasks (Hodge, & Thompson, 1990). This study pointed
out that background conversation and peak sound levels (70-80dBA) can reduce the
reliability of oral communication. In this study, only objective sound measurements were
conducted and these results were interpreted with the literature reviewed. Based on the
noise measurements conducted in the operating theatre, 50% of the time it was louder
than 46dBA. The overall average Leq was 51dBA. Lpeak was 108dBA. Background
noise level was 13dBA. Moreover to evaluate the reliability of the verbal communication,
the Preferred Speech Interference (PSI) level (the arithmetic average of three octave
bands centered at 500Hz, 1kHz and 2kHz) was compared with a commonly accepted
value (noise levels between 47-52dB can be acceptable if the distance between speaker
surgeons under various noise/music conditions. This was potentially related with
However their ability to concentrate in noisy conditions can require too much effort. This
study hypothesized that the complexity of the surgical tasks that require high
concentration and skills moderate the effect of noise on a surgeon’s performance. Three
different acoustical conditions (music, noise and quiet) applied during 12 surgeries
measured and recorded during 500-700min surgeries in the operating theatre. While
surgeon performance was assessed by 2 blinded observers and validated motion analysis
20
system, the noise recorded in the operating theatre played back continuously. Statistical
To summarize, the impact of noise on staff performance (especially in ICUs) has not
been widely examined and the findings of the some of the previous studies are
conflicting. Caregivers and patients can highly benefit from additional research to
determine how the sound environment effects staff`s work performance in ICUs.
Health
The acoustic environment throughout the hospital may contribute to negative acute or
chronic symptoms in staff. Critical care nursing is a very demanding job and it requires
continuous alertness, vigilance, and well-being to conduct critical tasks effectively. One
study found that of the 47 nurses surveyed in an ICU, 66% reported irritation and fatigue,
while 40% reported headaches due to workplace noise (Ryherd et al., 2008). However,
the impact of noise on surgeons` health in the operating theatres has been the primary
focus of the previous literature examining noise and staff health. Due to the use of power
instruments in operating rooms, medical staff members are commonly exposed to high
noise levels. One review study suggested an association between surgeons hearing loss
and 3 sound qualities of intensity, spectral frequency and pattern (i.e., intermittent,
Another study further examined the relationship between noise exposure levels and
surgeon’s hearing loss in the operating theatre during five surgeries (Love, 2003). Noise
21
dosimeters were used to measure noise exposure levels during 2 knee replacement and 3
hip replacement surgeries conducted by 3 operating room surgeons. Leq levels varied
between 75-82dBA. High Lpeak and Lmax levels were recorded at 140dB and 108dBA,
respectively.
The association between high noise levels in the operating theatre and surgeons` noise-
induced hearing loss was also suggested by Kracht et al. (2007). They recorded Leq noise
levels as high as 66dBA in the operating theatre. They documented the noise levels
procedures. During the neuro-surgery, Lpeak exceeded 100dB over 40% of the time and
In addition to noise-induced hearing loss, the general health of staff is also a concern.
However, very limited data in this area exists. One study showed that an increase in
average sound levels was significantly related to an increase in heart rate, in addition to
finding relationships between noise and perceived stress and annoyance as discussed
To summarize, there are agreed findings in the literature suggesting that surgeons’
hearing health can be affected by high noise levels during surgery. Noise-induced hearing
loss has particularly been a concern for surgeons in operating theatres. However, noise-
induced health outcomes (including hearing loss) of ICU nurses` have not yet been the
22
Work overload
When noise levels exceed a nurse’s coping abilities it can lead to sensory overload
(Baker, 1984). Sensory overload can cause emotional exhaustion, dissatisfaction, and
2001). Laschinger and Leiter (2006) found that medication error and other adverse events
Noise-induced stress due to ICU noise was positively related to nurse emotional
exhaustion and burnout (Topf , &Dillon, 1988). It was found that ICU nurses who rotated
shifts underwent more emotional exhaustion. Moreover during different shifts, nurses
analyze the impact of covariates (i.e., sensitivity to noise, age, experience) and noise
levels on nurse burnout, emotional exhaustion, and shift. Nurse responses were gathered
via different surveys: the DDHNS for noise induced disturbance, Jones’s Staff Burnout
Scale Health Profession (SBS-HP) for burnout, Maslach`s Burnout Inventory for
emotional exhaustion, Weinstein’s noise sensitivity, and a life event stress and nursing
stress survey. The study subjects were 100 critical care nurses from two hospitals that
interventions in an intensive coronary unit was found to improve perceived staff attitude
(Hagerman et al., 2005). Perceived staff attitude was collected via a 10-point scale
23
questionnaire. After the application of sound absorbent materials, noise levels in the
patient rooms dropped 5-6dB, but not at the nurse station. Reverberation time reduced
from 0.9s to 0.4s in the patient rooms and from 0.8s to 0.4s at the nurse station.
Measurements were conducted during the weekdays in relation to the changes in staffing
during weekends. Speech intelligibility was measured in two different ways: RASTI and
staff self-report. Non-parametric significance tests were used to compare the changes
patient safety, and the overall well-being of staff. Poor acoustical conditions in
workplaces can aggravate staff attitude and perceived work overload. The limited
determine appropriate acoustic conditions that will minimize negative work overload
effects.
Caregivers continuously monitor auditory cues and respond to them immediately to keep
patients safe. Clinical alarms are one of the key auditory cues in care settings. They are
intentionally designed to sound highly dangerous, frightening, unpleasant, loud, and tense
(Kuwano et al., 2001) because they have to be clearly distinguishable and alerting.
Regardless their importance for patient safety, caregivers may find these sounds highly
interrupting, annoying and overloading. The following section overviews the previous
24
Work performance
Frequently occurring excessive number of loud alarms can be problematic for nurse work
physiological parameters (Chambrin, 2001). Each medical device emits multiple alarms
with different acoustic qualities. However there is a limit to the amount of information
that human auditory systems can process. Cropp et al. (1994) conducted listening tests in
a critical care setting and found that excessive numbers of alarms confuse nurses
(especially less experienced ones) and potentially affect their auditory monitoring
performance. When nurses were presented previously recorded clinical alarms, they were
able to correctly identify only half of the critical alarms. Wallace et al. (1994) also found
the presence of similar clinical alarm detection difficulties in the operating rooms.
Another study highlighted the waste of valuable staff time and delayed response time to a
highly urgent condition related with difficulties during the detection of alarms -
particularly high pitch tones (Meredith, & Edworthy, 1995). In particular caregivers with
unilateral hearing loss can experience more difficulty in localization and discrimination
of clinical alarms (Newby, 1979). Non-hospital research also indicated the difficulties of
learning and remembering the significance of more than 8 different sounds even in
Alarms providing false information (a.k.a. false alarms) can also be problematic for nurse
work performance. Nurses continuously asses all audible signals occurring in care
settings as any auditory cue that might be clinically significant. However, false alarms
and/or improper alarm settings cause unnecessary interruptions in the nurse work flow.
25
They also potentially lead to misconceptions and create a “cry wolf” environment where
nurses ignore or inappropriately inactivate alarm signals (ECRI, 2007). A study found
that only 10 of the 33 alarms were critical suggesting a serious problem with either
patient or equipment (Cropp et al., 1994). As a result, false alarms are the indicators of
Health
Excessively loud alarms can potentially cause health problems among patients and
caregivers. Kahn (1998) reported that noise sources in critical care settings with peak
levels higher than 80dBA are amenable to behavior modification among staff members.
Another study indicated that loud alarms can aggravate the rest of the staff members
nurses potentially experience sleep problems after an intensive work day with many
noise levels can lead to hearing disorders and often caregivers might not be aware of such
significant health problems. For example, a study reported 37% of the physicians
working in critical care settings were unaware of their hearing disorder (Wallace et al.,
1994).
Disturbance by false alarms is commonly reported by nurses. Alarms that are not
signaling a medical emergency can cause staff irritation and annoyance (Meredith, &
Edworthy, 1995). A false alarm indicates a violated parameter of a vital sign that has no
26
clinical consequence/ significance (Kerr, & Hayes, 1983). Lawless (1994) surveyed 2176
alarms and found 68% of them were false. Chambrin (2001) found only 5.9% of 3166
Not only false alarms but noxious and repetitive signals are perceived as bothersome by
caregivers (Chambrin, 2001; Schmidth, & Baysinger, 1986). Parallel with staff
frightening (Gast, & Baker, 1989; Kerr, &Hayes, 1983). According to SCCM, less
noxious alarms can be provided by the modulation of critical alarms without reducing the
Clinical alarms can also increase the anxiety levels of the occupants. One of the key
that potentially poses a risk to patient safety. Difficulties in determining the location of
the alarms delay caregiver response time to an alarm which in turn leads to increased
experience increased anxiety levels when they have difficulties in distinguishing between
the alarms of alerting equipment as any alarming signal might pose a risk to their health
27
2.2 EMPIRICAL STUDY 1
2.2.1 Scope
Intensive care units (ICUs) have important but challenging sound environments. Alarms
and equipment generate high levels of noise and ICUs are typically designed with hard
surfaces that reflect sound. A poor sound environment can add to stress and make
auditory tasks more difficult for clinicians. In particular, the Intensive Care Unit (ICU)
sound environment is aurally demanding. Nurses often execute complex tasks in caring
for critically ill patients and it is important to understand the subjective and objective
qualities of the ICU sound environments from the nurses` point of view. However, few
studies have linked detailed analyses of the ICU acoustical environment to the
In this study, subjective perception evaluations are coupled with objective sound level
measurements to gain a more thorough grasp of how perceptual and physical acoustic
parameters interact in the ICU setting. The subjective and objective qualities of two ICU
environments with different design features are compared at multiple locations within
each ICU. Specifically, the study aims to determine whether objective noise levels and
(c) when comparing overall (average) levels in the two ICU’s to each other;
28
Additionally, relationship between objective and subjective noise levels was examined,
2.2.2 Methodology
The research is conducted in two intensive care units at Emory University Hospital:
MedSurg-ICU and Neuro-ICU. Detailed information about the physical design qualities
Both ICUs apply similar staffing models using intensivists and nurse practitioners and
accommodate critical care patients with similar acuity levels. In both units, 10 to 12
registered nurses are typically working during each shift. The Neuro-ICU nurses mostly
work 12h shifts (7am-7pm, 7pm-7am); the MedSurg-ICU nurses work both 12h and 8h
shifts (7am-3pm, 3pm-11pm, and 11pm-7am). In both units, nurses can work weekend,
Objective noise level measurements are conducted at 4 different locations in each unit:
(a) centralized nurse station, (b) unoccupied patient room with the door closed, (c)
occupied patient rooms with and without a respiratory ventilator, with the door
continually opening and closing to accommodate patient care, and (d) multiple data
points in the corridors. A total of 96h of samples are collected at the nurse stations in
each unit from Thursday to Monday. Saturday and Sunday is intentionally included as
29
In both units, patients with respiratory failure are connected to respiratory ventilator and
most of those patients are critically ill which restricts the entries into the patient rooms.
Due to this limited access, 45min samples are collected from the occupied patient rooms
room without respiratory ventilator. In each unit, 24h continuous stationary sound level
patient room while patient room doors are closed. At the corridors, multiple 15min sound
samples are collected at randomly selected times during day and night. In total,
approximately 246h of sound data are collected from both units. For the calculation of
overall noise levels in each unit, all sound data collected at different locations are taken
into consideration. Noise levels in the two ICUs are generated by different types of
sources. Medical equipment alarms occurring in the patient rooms, patient monitor
alarms occurring both in the patient rooms and at the nurse stations, sound of the ice
machine, phone ringing, staff conversation, and rolling medical carts in the corridors are
some of the common noises in two units. In the MedSurg-ICU, nurses are paged via
overhead pagers. In the Neuro-ICU 3G-phones or regular phones at the alcoves are used
instead.
At the corridors, the microphone is located at a height of approximately 1.4 m (4.5 ft)
slightly off the room center and stabilized on a tripod. In the patient rooms, the
microphone is hung from the ceiling at a height of approximately 1.8 m (6 ft) above
finished floor. Similarly, the microphone is hung from the ceiling at the nurse stations at
30
a height of approximately 1.8 m (6 ft) above finished floor. In the Neuro-ICU, the sound
level meter is set up at the nurse station in the 14-bed cluster. In the MedSurg-ICU, the
sound level meter is located at slightly off the center of the central nurse work zone in the
center of the unit. All sound data is collected using a fast response time for equivalent,
maximum and minimum levels (0.125 s) and one-minute averaging intervals. Three
Larson Davis type 824 sound level meters and Larson Davis 824 Utility software is used.
MedSurg-ICU. The survey consists of 4 sections: nurse profile and working conditions,
perceived sound environment in the workplace, perceived impact of noise levels on nurse
outcomes, general hearing health and noise sensitivity. The survey is completed by 35
35%, respectively. In the Neuro-ICU, 85% of the respondents are full-time and 15% part-
time. In the MedSurg-ICU, 70% were full-time, 26% part–time, and 4% PRN (on-call
nurse). In both units more than 80% of the respondents are female and more than 80% of
31
2.2.3 Findings
The noise levels measured at 4 different locations in each unit (nurse station, occupied
and unoccupied patient rooms, and corridors) are averaged to find single number overall
levels in each ICU. As shown in Figure 2.1, the overall averaged levels in the two units
range from 57-58 dBA Leq, 97-105 dB LMax, 54-58 dB LMin, and 113-120 dBC LPeak.
Given these values, the difference between overall averaged LAeq levels in the Neuro-
120
110
Sound Pressure Level
100
(dB re 20 μPa)
90
80
70
60
50
40
30
20
Leq-dBA Lmax-dB Lmin-dB Lpeak-dBC
MedSurg-ICU Neuro-ICU
Figure 2.1 Overall LAeq, LMax, LMin and LPeak noise levels in the two ICU`s
Further analysis is shown in Figure 2.2, which presents the percentage of time that the
maximum noise levels (LMax) exceeded values ranging from 70 to 90 dB. This type of
analysis is referred as the “occurrence rate” in this study, and has been used successfully
in previous hospital studies by the authors to analyze differences between day and night
(Ryherd, Persson Waye, & Ljungkvist, 2008), in addition to related analyses by others
32
(Kracht, Busch-Vishniac, &West, 2007; MacKenzie, & Galbrun, 2007; Williams,
Drongelen, & Lasky, 2007). In both units, LMax exceeds 70 dB more than 98% of the
time and LPeak exceeds 80 dBC more than 96% of the time. Thus the difference in the
occurrence rate of lower level transient sounds (<70 dB LMax and <80 dBC LPeak) and
very high level transient sounds (>90 dB LMax and >100 dBC LPeak) in the two ICU’s
is negligible. However, a difference between the two wards emerges for mid-level
transient sounds, as Figure 2.2 indicates. LMax exceeds 80 dB more of the time in the
MedSurg-ICU than in the Neuro-ICU. Similarly, LPeak exceeds 90 dBC more of the time
in the MedSurg-ICU than in the Neuro-ICU. This indicates that the MedSurg-ICU is a
100%
90%
Percentage (%) of time
80%
70%
60%
50%
40%
30%
20%
10%
0%
LMax>70dB LMax>80dB Lmax>90dB
MedSurg-ICU Neuro-ICU
Figure 2.2 Occurrence rate of different LMax noise levels in the two ICU`s
To summarize, the sound environments of two ICU’s are similar based on traditional
measures of overall Leq, LMax, LMin, and LPeak, but are different based on the
33
occurrence rate of mid-level transient sounds. Note that the terminology “mid-level
transient sounds” does not refer to the perceived loudness of the transient level, but
simply serves to distinguish the level ranges analyzed in this study into lower, mid, and
higher regions.
The average noise levels measured at each of the four different locations (nurse station,
occupied and unoccupied patient rooms, and corridors) are shown in Figure 2.3 for each
ICU. The occupied patient rooms without respiratory ventilator are chosen for this
analysis as there has been better access for longer measurements as compared to the
patient rooms with respiratory ventilators as previously discussed. The overall averaged
levels in the two ICU’s range between 45-60 dBA Leq depending on the location. The
unoccupied patient rooms are the quietest (45-52 dBA) and the nurse station, occupied
patient rooms, and corridors have similar noise levels (56-60 dBA). It is observed that the
differences between overall averaged LAeq levels for similar occupied locations in the
Neuro-ICU and MedSurg ICU are imperceptible (e.g. the nurse stations in both units
have similar Leq levels). However, the overall averaged LAeq is higher in the
trends are found for traditional measures of overall LMax, and LPeak in the two ICU’s.
34
Sound Pressure Level
70
A-weighted Equivalent
Sound Pressure Level
60
(dB re 20 μPa)
50
40
30
20
(dBA
10
0
Nurse Occupied Unoccupied
Empty Corridor
Station Patient Patient
Patient
Room w/out Room
Room
Resp. Vent.
MedSurg-ICU Neuro-ICU
Figure 2.3 Overall A-weighted equivalent sound pressure levels (Leq in dBA) at different
locations in the two ICUs
The occurrence rates of LMax and LPeak at each of the 4 locations in each ICU are also
analyzed. At all 4 locations in both units, LMax exceeds 70dB more than 98% of the time
and LPeak exceeds 80 dBC more than 94% of time. As before, a difference between the 4
spaces emerges for mid-level transient sounds, (Figure 2.4-Figure 2.6). For example,
Figure 2.4 shows the central nurse station results, where LMax exceeds 80 dB more often
in the MedSurg-ICU (43%) than in the Neuro-ICU (15%). Figure 2.5 and Figure 2.6
show similar analysis for all spaces in the two ICU’s. The occupied spaces (nurse station,
occupied patient rooms, and corridors) are more impulsive in the MedSurg-ICU (Figure
2.5) compared to the Neuro-ICU (Figure 2.6). The occurrence rates for the unoccupied
patient rooms are similar in the two ICU’s. LPeak occurrence rate analysis for the four
spaces in both ICU’s shows very similar results to LMax occurrence rate results.
35
100%
90%
Figure 2.4 Occurrence rate of different LMax noise levels at the nurse stations
MedSurg-ICU
100%
90%
Percentage (%) of time
80%
70%
60%
50%
40%
30%
20%
10%
0%
Lmax >70dB Lmax >80dB Lmax >90dB
Nurse Station
Lmax-dB Nurse Station Occupied
Lmax-dB Patient R. P. R.
Occupied
Unoccupied Patient R. P.R.
Lmax-dB Unoccupied Corridor
Lmax-dB Corridor
Figure 2.5 Occurrence rate of different LMax noise levels in the MedSurg-ICU
36
Neuro-ICU
100%
90%
Figure 2.6 Occurrence rate of different LMax noise levels in the Neuro-ICU
Does subjective perception differ when comparing two ICU’s to each other?
environment. Results for 5 nurse outcomes are shown in Table 2.1: perceived loudness,
annoyance, work performance, general health outcomes, and anxiety. A discrete 5-point
rating scale is used in for all 5 outcomes shown. Workplace noise is perceived as louder,
more annoying, and having a greater negative impact on work performance, health
outcomes, and anxiety by the MedSurg-ICU nurses compared to the Neuro-ICU nurses.
The nurses` sensitivity to noise and tolerance to high noise levels in the workplace does
not differ significantly between ICU’s (p<0.05). Overall, the nurses in both units have
rated themselves as not very sensitive to noise and thought they can somewhat tolerate
37
high noise levels. Perceived hearing ability of two unit nurses also does not differ
Table 2.1 (a) Mean perception ratings of noise-induced nurse outcomes in the two ICUs
(b) distribution of noise-induced nurse outcomes in the two ICUs
Neuro-ICU MedSurg-ICU
(1: completely disagree; (1: completely disagree; Nurse outcomes
5: completely agree) 5: completely agree)
38
Does subjective perception differ when comparing similar locations in the two ICU’s to
Though not all results are statistically significant, the trend is that the noise is consistently
perceived as louder and more annoying at all 4 locations (nurse station, occupied and
unoccupied patient rooms, and corridors) in the MedSurg-ICU as compared to the Neuro-
ICU. Results are shown in Table 2.2. A discrete 5-point rating scale is used in for
that the differences for the nurse stations and in the empty patient rooms are statistically
Table 2.2 Mean perception ratings of loudness and annoyance at different locations in the
two ICUs
Unoccupied
2.25* 1.6 2.7** 1.8
Patient Room
Occupied
3.0 2.6 3.4 3.1
Patient Room
39
Differences in perception between 4 locations within each individual ICU are also found
based on the values in Table 2.2. In the MedSurg-ICU, the nurse station is perceived as
significantly louder than other three locations (p<0.05). In the Neuro-ICU, the empty
patient room is perceived as significantly quieter and less annoying than the other three
locations (p<0.05).
Spearman nonparametric correlation tests are used to analyze the relationships between
objective noise levels and subjective loudness and annoyance perception. As shown in
Table 2.3, subjective annoyance and loudness perception is significantly and positively
correlated with Leq (dBA) in both ICU’s. The interpretation is that the nurses perceive
the noise as more annoying and louder as the Leq increases. Additionally, the mid-level
annoyance and loudness perception (Table 2.3). Nurses perceive noise as more annoying
and louder the more often (higher % of time) LMax exceeds 80 dB or LPeak exceeds 90
dBC.
40
Table 2.3 Correlations between objective noise levels (i.e., Leq, LMax and LPeak
occurrence rates) and subjective perception of annoyance and loudness
Spectral content
The frequency distribution of noise levels for the two ICU’s are shown in Figure 2.7 and
Figure 2.8. Similar trends in frequency content are seen for the 4 locations within each
individual ICU, with the unoccupied patient room generally quieter than the occupied
spaces.
41
MedSurg-ICU
80
(dB re 20 μPa)
50
40
30
20
10
0
16
31.5
63
125
250
500
1000
2000
4000
8000
Nurse Station
Occupied Paitent Room w/out resp. vent
Empty Patient
Unoccupied RoomR.
Patient
Corridor
Figure 2.7 Average sound pressure levels across frequency in the MedSurg-ICU
Neuro-ICU
80
Sound Pressure Level
70
(dB re 20 μPa)
60
50
40
30
20
10
0
16
31.5
63
125
250
500
1000
2000
4000
8000
Nurse Station
Occupied Paitent Room w/out resp. vent
Unoccupied
Empty Patient
Patient
RoomR.
Corridor
Figure 2.8 Average sound pressure levels across frequency in the Neuro-ICU
42
When comparing the two ICU’s to each other, the sound pressure levels are generally
somewhat higher in MedSurg-ICU, particularly from 250 Hz-8 kHz. At 8 kHz, clearly
noticeable noise level differences have occurred between two ICU’s nurse stations and
nurse station=49dB). At 250 Hz and 500 Hz, clearly noticeable noise level differences
have occurred between the empty patient rooms in the two wards (e.g., at 500Hz Neuro-
ICU= 40dBA; MedSurg-ICU=51dB). The differences between ICU’s above 250 Hz are
likely related to increased noise from occupants, alarms, and airflow from ventilation
systems in the MedSurg-ICU. Below 250 Hz, sound levels are generally somewhat higher
in the Neuro-ICU. In the empty and occupied patient room, noise level differences
difference between the ICU’s at low frequencies may be related to noise from HVAC
equipment located near the center of the Neuro-ICU. The equipment is located in an
outdoor space that is not accessible by the occupants but included in the design to provide
Noise Fluctuations
The traditional fluctuation metric of (L10 - L90) does not have sufficient range to capture
the noise fluctuation differences in two ICU`s and at different locations in each ICU. At
all measurement locations, L33 results are consistently within 1 dB of non-weighted Leq
maximum and averaged sound pressure levels (LMax minus Leq); and peak and averaged
sound pressure levels (LPeak minus Leq) are effective in capturing the noise fluctuation
43
differences. Overall, noise fluctuations are higher in the MedSurg-ICU compared to the
Neuro-ICU as shown in Figure 2.9. The interpretation is that the MedSurg-ICU nurses
are exposed to more dramatic noise level changes compared to the Neuro-ICU nurses.
This result is consistent with the significant differences between nurse outcomes and
subjective loudness and annoyance levels in the two ICUs. Figure 2.9 also shows the
consistency between the noise fluctuation trends and the trend generated by annoyance
100 5
90
Sound Pressure Level
4.5
70
60 3.5
50 3
40 2.5
30
2
20
10 1.5
0 Corridor 1
Occupied
P.R.
Corridor
Unoccup.
P.R.
Nurse
NSNurse
Station
Occupied
Station
P.R.
Unoccup.
P.R.
OPR
COR
OPR
COR
Neuro-ICU
EPR
EPR
MedSurg-ICU
NS
Neuro-ICU MedSurg-ICU
LMax-Leq (dB)
Lmax-Leq-dB LPeak-Leq (dB)
Lpeak-Leq-dB
Annoyance Loudness
Figure 2.9 Non-weighted noise fluctuation levels (calculated based on noise level
differences between LMax -Leq and LPeak -Leq), annoyance and loudness levels
44
Speech Interference Levels
The potential for the background noise to interfere with speech communication, or
“speech interference level (SIL)” is also evaluated, as shown in Table 2.4. The SIL is
calculated as the arithmetic average of octave band sound pressure levels from 500Hz to
4kHz, with a higher SIL indicating more potential for interference (Mehta, Johnson, &
Rocafort, 1999). In general, the SIL’s in the MedSurg-ICU are slightly higher at all 4
locations compared to the Neuro-ICU. The SIL is highest at the nurse stations, ranging
from 50-53 dB. At this level, two female nurses will be able to (barely) communicate
with each other in normal voice up to a distance of nearly 0.9m-1.2 m (3ft-4 ft). The
distance would increase to 1.6m-2.3 m (5.5ft-7.5 ft) if the nurses raise their voices
(Mehta, Johnson, & Rocafort, 1999). Lower SIL levels can enable safer
45
Table 2.4 Speech interference levels at different locations in two ICUs (also includes
corresponding distances between speaker and listener for reliable communication during
normal voice and raised voice usage)
MedSurg-ICU Neuro-ICU
Speaker- Speaker-
SIL listener distance (ft) SIL listener distance (ft)
(dB) Normal Raised (dB) Normal Raised
voice voice voice voice
Nurse
53 3.0 5.5 50 4.0 7.5
Station
Occupied
51 3.5 6.5 49 4.5 9.0
Patient Room
Unoccupied
40 12.0 23.5 35 21.5 43.0
Patient Room
2.2.4 Discussion
more annoying, and having a greater negative impact of noise on work performance,
health outcomes, and anxiety as compared to the Neuro-ICU. Additionally, the noise is
consistently perceived as louder and more annoying at the all four locations (nurse
station, occupied and unoccupied patient rooms, and corridors) in the MedSurg-ICU as
compared to the Neuro-ICU. However, the nurses` loudness and annoyance perceptions
significantly differed only at the nurse station and in the unoccupied patient room.
Surprisingly, there is little difference between the sound environments of the two ICU’s
46
based on traditional overall measures of Leq, LMin, and LMax, and LPeak, particularly
for occupied spaces. The objective differences between the occupied sound environments
in the two wards only emerges through a more comprehensive analysis of the occurrence
rate, frequency content, and the speech interference level. Further, perceived annoyance
and loudness levels are significantly and positively correlated to mid-level transient
sound occurrence rates. This indicates that the traditional overall noise measures may be
Interesting differences are also found when comparing similar locations (nurse station,
occupied and unoccupied patient rooms and corridors) within each individual ICU.
other locations within that ICU and the Neuro-ICU unoccupied patient room is perceived
as significantly quieter and less annoying than other locations within that ICU. Nurse
stations are highly transient sound environments where sound sources include medical
alarms, telephone ringing, staff talking and laughing, footfall noise, etc. Unlike nurse
stations, empty patient rooms with the doors closed have more stationary sound
environments where the main noise source is the HVAC system. In highly transient
sound environments such as nurse stations, differences in perception are likely more
related to differences in the occurrence rate of maximum and peak levels. On the other
hand, in more stationary sound environments such as unoccupied patient rooms the
differences in subjective perception are likely more related to differences between overall
47
By comparing the objective noise measures and subjective perception between the two
ICU’s and at four locations within each ICU, an interesting conclusion regarding noise
measures emerges. One must consider the overall equivalent level (Leq) or some related
measure of loudness in addition to the occurrence rate, or some related measure of the
transient nature of the sound. The perception of unoccupied spaces (such as empty patient
rooms) will likely be more related to the overall level, while the perception of occupied
spaces (such as nurse stations) will likely be more related to the transient nature of the
sound. The frequency content and SIL (or related speech measures) should also be
considered.
2.3.1 Scope
The study discussed in the previous section compared two critical care sound
environments objectively and subjectively. One of the ICUs is perceived as louder, more
annoying, and having a greater negative impact of noise on work performance, health
outcomes, and anxiety. Surprisingly, there is little difference between two ICU sound
between the occupied sound environments in the two units only emerges through a more
comprehensive analysis of the “occurrence rate” of peak and maximum levels, frequency
content, and the speech interference level. To assess the effectiveness of the new acoustic
metric, “occurrence rate”, a follow up empirical study is conducted. The second study is
also important to assess the behavior of sound over time in the two critical care settings.
48
2.3.2 Methodology
Two additional sets of subjective noise level analyses are conducted by using the data
collected in the previous empirical study: a) weekday vs. weekend, and b) day vs. night
vs. shift changes. In addition to the components already discussed, the online survey also
includes questions about noise-induced annoyance and loudness levels during different
times of the day and different days of the week. Details of the methodology are shown in
section 2.2.2.
Two additional sets of objective noise level analyses are conducted by using the data
from the previous empirical study: a) weekday vs. weekend, and b) day vs. night vs. shift
changes. As previously described in section 2.2.2, 96 h of sound data has been collected
at the central nurse stations of the two ICUs (see Figure 2.10 and Figure 2.11 for nurse
station locations and layouts). In addition to weekdays, noise level measurements are
intentionally conducted during the weekend as these days have not been typically
included in the previous work. Noise level measurements have taken place at the nurse
stations from Thursday to Monday. Sound data collected during the three weekdays
(Thursday-Friday and Monday), and at the weekend (Saturday and Sunday) are analyzed
separately. For the calculation of overall noise levels in each ward, all sound data
collected from Thursday to Monday are averaged. In the second analysis, noise levels
during the day, night and shift times are analyzed separately. Day and night times are
defined based on 12h nurse shift in the Neuro-ICU (7am-7pm and 7pm-7am) and 8h
49
nurse shift in the MedSurg-ICU (7am-3pm, 3pm-10pm and 10pm-7am). In two wards,
nurse shift changes continue about 45-60min after and start 20-25min before the shift.
Figure 2.10 Floor plans of two ICU`s showing the distribution of nurse stations
(left) Neuro-ICU; (right) MedSurg-ICU
50
2.3.3 Findings
The first set of subjective noise level analysis is conducted to assess MedSurg-ICU and
Neuro-ICU nurses` annoyance and loudness perceptions for weekdays vs. weekends.
Results are shown in Table 2.5. A discrete 5-point rating scale was used for noise-
induced annoyance and loudness perception. Nonparametric Mann Whitney U test is used
to analyze the significant differences between the perceptions of nurses in the two units.
both during the weekdays and the weekend. However, statistically significant differences
between loudness and annoyance perceptions of the nurses in the two units have occurred
only during the weekdays. Overall, Neuro-ICU nurses` loudness and annoyance
perceptions do not vary much when comparing weekday to weekends. However, the
trend is that MedSurg-ICU nurses found weekends quieter and less annoying compared to
weekdays.
Table 2.5 (a) Annoyance and loudness perceptions for weekdays vs. weekend
(b) distribution of nurse annoyance and loudness perceptions in the two ICUs
Annoyance Loudness
Weekday Weekend Weekday Weekend
51
Table 2.5 continued
Annoyance Loudness
5-point scale
Weekday Weekend Weekday Weekend
Completely disagree-1 25.3% 8.6% 15.0% 10.3%
Somewhat disagree-2 18.4% 8.6% 20.5% 10.3%
Neuro-ICU Neither agree nor disagree-3 17.1% 15.4% 14.3% 18.0%
Somewhat agree-4 22.0% 21.0% 24.0% 25.0%
Completely agree-5 24.0% 28.0% 25.0% 26.0%
Completely disagree-1 4.0% 17.4% 0.0% 10.0%
Somewhat disagree-2 4.3% 4.3% 4.3% 8.0%
MedSurg-ICU Neither agree nor disagree-3 8.7% 26.1% 8.7% 26.1%
Somewhat agree-4 43.0% 24.0% 39.0% 30.0%
Completely agree-5 39.0% 25.0% 40.0% 22.0%
Subjective noise level during the day and night times and shift change
The second set of subjective noise level analysis is conducted to assess Neuro-ICU and
MedSurg-ICU nurses` noise level perceptions during different times of the day. Two unit
nurses are asked to rate their annoyance and loudness perceptions during the day times,
night times, and shift changes. According to Mann Whitney nonparametric significance
test results, the MedSurg-ICU is consistently perceived to be more annoying and louder
during certain times of the day compared to Neuro-ICU (Table 2.6). Although the trend is
that the MedSurg-ICU is perceived worse during all times, statistically significant
differences occurred only during the day times and shift changes. Additionally, the trend
is that in both units nurses generally has found noise levels slightly more annoying during
the shift changes compared to day and night times. However, their loudness perceptions
during the day times and shift changes are very similar. The trend is that night times were
52
Table 2.6 Annoyance and loudness perceptions during different times of the day
Loudness Annoyance
Day Night Shift Day Night Shift
Annoyance Loudness
5‐point scale
day time night time shift time day time night time shift time
Completely disagree‐1 18.6% 10.0% 8.6% 8.6% 11.4% 25.0%
Somewhat disagree‐2 17.1% 11.4% 17.1% 12.9% 11.4% 17.6%
Neuro‐ICU Neither agree nor disagree‐3 20.0% 12.9% 14.3% 25.0% 13.0% 25.4%
Somewhat agree‐4 16.0% 20.0% 26.0% 27.0% 21.0% 23.2%
Completely agree‐5 22.0% 24.0% 27.0% 23.0% 25.0% 21.6%
Completely disagree‐1 4.3% 13.0% 4.3% 0.0% 16.0% 0.0%
Somewhat disagree‐2 0.0% 15.0% 4.3% 0.0% 18.0% 4.3%
MedSurg‐ICU Neither agree nor disagree‐3 19.0% 18.0% 13.0% 21.7% 13.0% 17.4%
Somewhat agree‐4 28.0% 25.0% 34.0% 36.0% 25.0% 33.0%
Completely agree‐5 44.0% 27.0% 47.0% 45.0% 30.0% 48.0%
To summarize, subjective noise levels in each unit varied during different times of the
day and different days of the week. The trend is that the MedSurg-ICU is consistently
perceived worse. However statistically significant differences occurred only during the
weekdays. Significant differences between nurse perceptions is also evident only during
The first set of objective noise level analysis is conducted to assess the differences
between the two ICU sound environments and occurring in each unit during the
53
weekdays and the weekend. Related with the scope of this study, noise levels in the two
The overall averaged sound pressure levels (Leq) at the nurse stations of two units range
between 57-60dBA and 56-58dBA during the weekdays and at the weekend respectively.
Given these results, the difference between two wards during different days of the week
As shown in Figure 2.12-Figure 2.16, occurrence rate values present the percentage of
time that the peak noise levels exceeded values ranging from 80dBC to 100dBC. In both
units, Lpeak noise levels exceed 80dBC more than 95% of the time during the weekdays
and at the weekend. The occurrence rate of Lpeak noise levels exceeding 100dBC is
negligible in both units during the weekdays and at the weekend. On the other hand, as
shown in Figure 2.12, substantial differences between the two wards emerge for mid-
level Lpeak noise levels during the weekdays. As noted in earlier sections, the
terminology for “mid-level” for 90dBC does not refer to the perceived loudness of the
transient level, but simply serves to distinguish the level ranges analyzed in this study
into lower, mid and higher regions. In the MedSurg-ICU, Lpeak noise levels exceed
90dBC more of the time than in the Neuro-ICU during the weekdays. The difference
between the two units is much more evident during the weekdays as compared to the
weekend. Similar results are also found for Lmax levels. To summarize, although there is
not a difference in overall levels (LAeq), the occurrence rate analysis indicate that the
54
MedSurg-ICU is a more impulsive sounding environment compared to Neuro-ICU,
Figure 2.12 Occurrence rate of Lpeak noise levels during the weekdays in the two units
Figure 2.13 Occurrence rate of different Lpeak noise levels during at the weekend in the
two units
55
Noise levels during the day times, night times and shift change
The second set of objective noise level measurements is conducted to assess noise level
differences during different times of the day. The overall averaged Leq levels at the
Neuro-ICU and MedSurg-ICU nurse stations range between 58-61dBA, 55-58dBA and
59-61dBA during the day, night and shift change time, respectively. Given these results,
the difference between two wards in comparing different times of the day is either
However, the occurrence rate of mid-level transient sounds is higher in the MedSurg-ICU
at all times of the day as shown in Figure 2.14, Figure 2.15 and Figure 2.16. The
difference between the two units is much more evident during the day times and shift
changes as compared to night times. In the MedSurg-ICU, more percentage of the time
Lpeak noise levels exceed 90dBC during the day times and shift changes compared to
night times. The occurrence rate of the mid-level transient sounds at the Neuro-ICU nurse
station does not vary much during the different times of the day.
Figure 2.14 Occurrence rate of different Lpeak noise levels during the day times in the
two units
56
Figure 2.15 Occurrence rate of different Lpeak noise levels during the shift changes in
the two units
Figure 2.16 Occurrence rate of different Lpeak noise levels during the night times in the
two units
For the third set objective noise level analyses, entire 96h data collected at the nurse
station of each unit are considered. Sound samples collected from Thursday-Monday at
each nurse station are averaged to calculate overall noise levels. Overall averaged noise
57
levels at the nurse stations of two units range from 56-60 dBA Leq, 97-105 dB Lmax, 54-
57dB Lmin, 113-120dBC Lpeak (Figure 2.17). As before, based on Leq the difference
between two unit nurse station sound environments is just perceptible to human ear. The
difference between two unit sound environments emerges for mid-level transient sounds.
Figure 2.18 shows the overall levels at the two unit nurse stations where Lpeak exceed
90dBC more often in the MedSurg-ICU (53%) than in the Neuro-ICU (24%).
Figure 2.17 Overall Leq, Lmax, Lpeak and Lmin levels in the two ICU`s
58
Figure 2.18 Overall occurrence rates of Lpeak noise levels in the two ICUs (Please note
that Figure 2.18 and Figure 2.4 are not the same. Figure 2.4 represents the occurrence
rate of different LMax noise levels and Figure 2.18 represents the occurrence rate of
different LPeak noise levels)
2.3.4 Discussion
Empirical study 2 conducts objective and subjective noise level analyses to assess
changing sound environment characteristics of each unit over time and assesses whether
occurrence frequency of transient sounds track with the nurse loudness and annoyance
perceptions during different times of the day and days of the week.
The first set of occurrence rate analysis indicates that MedSurg-ICU is consistently more
rate of mid-level transient sounds in the Neuro-ICU only during the day time and shift
change. These results are consistent with the perceptions of nurses in the two units.
Nurses have found MedSurg-ICU more annoying and louder at all times. However
59
significant differences between perceptions of the nurses in the two units have occurred
The second set of occurrence rate analysis indicates that MedSurg-ICU is consistently
more impulsive compared to Neuro-ICU during the weekdays and the weekend.
Neuro-ICU only during the weekdays. These results are also consistent with the
perceptions of nurses in the two units. Nurses have found MedSurg-ICU more annoying
and louder during the weekdays and the weekend. However significant differences
between perceptions of nurses in the two units only have occurred during the weekdays.
In the third set of occurrence rate analysis, the entire 96h data collected at the nurse
station of each unit is considered. The results indicate that the occurrence rate of mid-
level transient sounds is substantially higher at the nurse station in the MedSurg-ICU
compared to the occurrence rate of mid-level transient sounds at the nurse station in the
Neuro-ICU. This result is also consistent with the perceptions of the nurses in the two
units. As presented in Table 2.2, MedSurg-ICU nurses have found the nurse station in
their unit significantly more annoying and louder compared to the Neuro-ICU nurses.
Overall, occurrence rate analysis findings track with the loudness and annoyance
perceptions of the nurses in the two units. This suggests the potential effectiveness of
occurrence rate in predicting the nurse annoyance and loudness perceptions in ICUs.
60
2.4 EMPIRICAL STUDY 3
2.4.1 Scope
Clinical alarms are impulsive and frequently reoccurring sound events in ICUs. The
previous empirical studies (1 and 2) discussed in the last two sections have highlighted
presented below that assesses the effects of a particular highly impulsive sound event
(i.e., clinical alarms) by comparing the nurse outcomes in the two ICUs.
2.4.2 Methodology
Nurse perceptions
Subjective noise level analysis is conducted by using the data collected in empirical study
1. In addition to the components previously discussed, the survey also includes questions
about alarm-induced nurse outcomes. For methodology details, please refer to section
2.2.2.
2.4.3 Findings
A subjective noise level analysis is conducted to compare nurses` disturbance levels due
to alarms in the two units. Mean perception ratings of the alarm-induced nurse outcomes
in the MedSurg-ICU and Neuro-ICU are shown in Table 2.7. The effects of alarms on the
MedSurg-ICU nurse outcomes are consistently significantly higher than the effects of
61
alarms on the Neuro-ICU nurse outcomes. To restate, the MedSurg-ICU nurses have
perceived alarms as louder, more annoying, and having higher negative impacts on their
health, work performance, and anxiety levels. Moreover, the difference between the two
units is the highest for anxiety compared to the other outcomes. In the MedSurg-ICU, the
trend is that alarms have a greater impact on particular nurse outcomes of anxiety levels
and loudness perceptions as compared to the other outcomes. In the Neuro-ICU, the trend
is that alarms have the highest impact on nurses` loudness perceptions as compared to the
other outcomes. All of the other outcomes in the Neuro-ICU are on average less than 3.0,
which would correspond to “disagreement” ratings on the 5-point scale. This indicates
that on average the Neuro-ICU nurses have not perceived alarms as contributing to
Table 2.7 Perceived medical alarm-induced outcomes (i.e., loudness, annoyance, work
performance, health and anxiety) of the nurses in the two ICUs. Higher numbers
represent more negative perception.
62
Table 2.8 Distribution of medical-alarm induced outcomes (i.e., loudness, annoyance,
work performance, health and anxiety) of the nurses in the two ICUs
The nurses in the two units have been also asked to rate their level of agreement with the
following statement: “I sometimes tune out the alarms.” The majority of the nurses
“somewhat agree” with this statement (Figure 2.19). Only 20% of the nurses completely
Figure 2.19 Percentage of nurses who agreed that they sometime tune out alarms
averaged across the two units
63
2.4.4 Discussion
This study compares the effects of clinical alarms on nurse outcomes in the two ICUs.
Alarms are one of the highly impulsive sound events that occur frequently in the ICUs.
Regardless of their good intent, alarms can be highly disruptive of nurse outcomes.
Furthermore, effects of clinical alarms on nurse outcomes can vary significantly between
different care settings. For example, in the MedSurg-ICU nurses have found clinical
alarms more disruptive of their outcomes (i.e., health and performance outcomes, anxiety,
annoyance, and loudness perceptions) compared to nurses in the Neuro-ICU. This finding
also agrees with occurrence rate analysis findings introduced in Section 2.2.3 and Section
2.3.3. According to two empirical study findings, the occurrence frequency of mid-level
MedSurg-ICU nurses have reported that they sometimes tune out clinical alarms.
64
CHAPTER 3
In complex hospital sound environments, caregivers conduct vital tasks including patient
perception and objective sound level measurements to gain a more thorough grasp of how
perceptual and physical acoustic parameters interact in the ICU setting. Further, the
Chapter 2 research suggested that the Acoustic qualities of hospital sound environments
overview of the factors related to auditory monitoring including previous research related
to the topic; and presents the results from a case study comparing nurses` auditory
Today’s ICUs are the outgrowth of respiratory care units established to provide
continuous patient monitoring, and life support treatments for patients suffering from
polio or tetanus in the early 20th century (Berenson, 1984). The establishment of these
units was initiated by the invention of a mechanical ventilator called the “iron lung” in
the late 1920s. In the 1960s, ICUs started providing electronic monitoring. Originally,
65
electronic monitoring was developed for cardiac rhythm monitoring to preserve the
In today`s ICUs, continuous monitoring is provided for critically ill patients by a wide
vital signs (Safar, & Grenvik, 1971) . Critical care units differ based on their specialty
(e.g., multispecialty, specialty); however, patient monitoring is key for all types. In
multispecialty ICUs (e.g., Medical Surgical-ICU), seriously ill medical surgical patients
with a wide spectrum of illnesses are provided care. In specialty ICUs, patients with
similar problems or specific diseases are provided care such as care of premature or
critically ill newborn (Neonatal-ICU), care of critically ill and injured children (Pediatric-
ICU), adult cardiac disease (Cardiac-ICU), trauma care, care of multiple organ
patients (Neuro-ICU).
Patients in ICUs generally suffer from different health problems including failing bodily
functions, multiple and complex sets of medical problems requiring support for two or
more organ systems (Bennett, & Bion, 1999). Patients are accepted to ICUs mainly
because they need monitoring for potential disturbances to those who are critically ill,
and receive life-supporting treatment or continuous intensive nursing and physician care
(Berenson, 1984). ICUs typically differ based on the level of care provided such as
Level-I, Level-II and Step-down critical care units (Miller, & Swensson, 2002). Typically
Level-I critical care units house patients with multisystem failure, and complicated
66
medical needs requiring continuous availability of sophisticated equipment and
specialized medical staff. Level-II critical care settings typically offer care to patients
with single organ failure. Step-down units or intermediate care patients typically require
There is a growing demand for critical care in the U.S. There are approximately 6,000
ICUs caring for 55,000 critically ill patients each day (SCCM, 2006). About 16% of the
annual admissions to U.S. hospitals are admitted to ICUs, which corresponds to about six
million patients and 2% of the U.S. population (Kersten et al., 2003). ICU patients` health
ICU diseases can develop rapidly in seconds as multiple underlying medical problems
can interact and produce severe unpredictable physiologic complications (Hillman, &
Bishop, 2004). Berenson (1984) indicated that 20% to 40% of the ICU patients died in
the hospital after being transferred from the ICU to the regular medical floor. Providing
particularly in ICUs.
Critical care nurses provide care to patients who are very sick and critical care nursing
can be a very demanding job. Critical care nurses are educated to provide highly
technical expert care and trained to deal with end-of-life cases (Gross, 2006). They
continuously provide vigilant efforts to maintain patient safety while working under time
pressure.
67
Unfortunately, their workplaces are not always equipped with support work
environments. Demanding work conditions and poorly designed work environments can
lead to high levels of cognitive, physical, and emotional workload, negative health
outcomes, and job dissatisfaction among ICU nurses (Aiken et al., 2002; Carayon et al.,
1999; Ulrich et al., 2009). Findings indicate that emotional stress and working under time
pressure can also lead to increased risk for work-related musculoskeletal disorders,
Critical-Care Nurses (AACN) national survey, a significant portion of the nurses are not
satisfied with their jobs: more than 16% of the nurses indicated that their intention is to
quit their job in the following 12 months and about 27% in the next 3 years (Ulrich et al.,
2009). Similarly, Aiken (2002) found that 1 in 4 bedside nurses is considering leaving
their jobs in a year and half of the nurses reported high burnout range. Given these
estimated that the shortfall of nurses could approach 800,000 by 2020 (DHHS, 2002).
On the other hand, there is some evidence showing positive impacts of improved
empirical studies in Chapter 2 compared the sound environments of two different ICUs
and found significant differences between nurse outcomes in the different units due to the
acoustic qualities of these sound environments. Another non-hospital study found that
68
task supportive environmental conditions such as effective sound environments in the
workplaces might help to buffer the effects of job stressors (Leather et al., 2003).
ICU nurses conduct different critical and non-critical tasks. Highly critical nurse tasks
mainly relate to the care of the sick and maintenance of patient safety (Diers, 2004).
Some of the tasks related with the care of the sick include skin wound care, managing
pain, providing comfort, teaching patients and families how to manage their care after
prevention of hazards. Nurse tasks that critically relate to patient safety include providing
continuous patient monitoring for early detection of adverse events and complications,
medication errors, and mobilization of resources for timely interventions and rescue
(Aiken, 2005). Nurse time is also allocated with non-critical tasks such as waiting on
other systems (e.g., delivery of medication, lab results), retrieving patient supplies, and
looking for equipment (Hendrich et al., 2008). However, the same study found that
majority of the nurse time (78%) is spent on clinical nursing practice functions including
Critical care nurses conduct highly routine patient monitoring tasks. These tasks require
vigilant attendance to multitude of cues and continuously alert minds, vigorous body
monitor the patients` health status and maintain their normal bodily conditions by
69
assessing visual and auditory cues. To recognize any irregularities, nurses remain
attentive to the patients’ physiologic status and treatment devices, and they are
continually “tuned in” to the immediate recognition of any disruption in the patients`
condition. They must remain tuned in even when performing routine maintenance
activities (e.g., medication preparation, blood procurement). In the event of any abnormal
changes, nurses instantly evaluate the significance of the event to patient safety, and
Since the late 19th century, visual patient monitoring has been a significant element of
nursing practice. Visual cues have been used for the early detection of some health
complications. Examples include changes in the color or texture of the skin (e.g., rashes,
patient on the bed, invasive equipment around the patient bed, and any other condition
that might be related with distress and patient comfort (Downes, 2009). Effective visual
monitoring can be enabled by the absence of any barrier between observer and target. It is
identified as inadequate when nurses are assigned to patients whose rooms are physically
distant from each other (Kalish, 2005). To maximize the patient visibility, the design of
today’s critical care units aim to provide a direct line of vision between every patient
room and nurse work areas by adopting different design strategies such as radial design
70
3.1.1.6 Auditory Patient Monitoring
Like visual monitoring, assessment of auditory cues has become a significant component
of nursing practice (Downes, 2009). As a part of the auditory monitoring process, nurses
attentively listen to the auditory cues, detect the ones that might present risks to patient
safety, and localize them to provide proper and immediate response to abnormal sounds.
ICU nurses develop auditory skills that enable them to differentiate auditory cues from
each other and immediately recognize the meaning of each while considering the
response to triggered alarms in ICUs includes physically assessing the patient and
alarm in a timely manner can be highly critical. During personal interviews with ICU
nurses, appropriate initial response time to a highly urgent alarm was found to be 30
seconds or less. Caregiver response time to a moderate risk alarm is expected to be 1-3
minutes, and to a low risk alarm 3-5minutes for patient safety (Phillips, &Barnsteiner,
2005). Lack of proper monitoring can delay the response time and even disable the early
detection of complications which can result in serious patient injury and death due to
ventilator failure, breathing circuit disconnections, etc. (ECRI, 1986). According to ECRI,
ventilator dependent patients need special attention and they should never be left
unattended. Otherwise, caregivers should limit their visits in the unit to only locations
where they can hear and rapidly respond to alarms. This is mainly because ventilator
dependent patients cannot breathe spontaneously for substantial periods of time and
71
cannot mutually ventilate themselves. Impaired gas exchange (i.e., supply of adequate
oxygen to the body and elimination of carbon dioxide) can lead to poor tissue perfusion
which leads to a reduction in oxygen delivery to cells as well as a retention of CO2 in the
caregivers vigilance level (Clarke, &Aiken, 2003). Growing evidence from hospital and
non-hospital research also indicate the effect of environmental factors on nurse work
performance utilizing auditory cues. These qualities are discussed details in the following
sections.
With the adoption of computer based patient monitoring systems, nurses started
monitoring the clinical alarms as well as non-alarm sounds. Today, electronic monitors
are extensively used in critical care units and medical alarms are accepted as one of the
A variety of different medical equipment is used in ICUs. Each type of equipment can
produce multiple alarms to communicate about the urgency level of the alarming
(cautionary alarms) and low-level urgency (alerting alarms) (CEN, 1995; Chambrin et al.,
1999; Meredith, & Edworthy, 1995). High-level urgency alarms (e.g., 3-star cardiac
alarms) indicate an urgent situation that can lead immediately to a vital problem and
requires immediate attention of the registered nurses. Medium-level urgency alarms (e.g.,
72
2-star cardiac alarms) indicate a dangerous situation and require rapid attention of the
registered nurses. Low-level urgency alarms (e.g., equipment failure that would pose a
minimum adverse effect) indicate an alert situation that still requires the attention of staff
Most ICU patients are attached to some combination of medical equipment which can be
equipment (Hirose et al., 2005). Some examples of patient monitoring systems include
oximeters, and capnometers (Kerr, &Hayes, 1983). Some of the infusion devices include
hemodialysis units, and anesthesia machines are some of the commonly used life support
equipment in ICUs. Some of the most commonly used medical equipment in ICUs is
Given the plethora of medical devices, a high density of critical medical alarms is not
uncommon in modern ICUs. One study reported that there are at least 33 different
medical alarms that nurses continuously monitor in ICUs (Cropp et al., 1994). The same
study also found that during an average hour on the day shift at least 50 audible medical
very important for the early detection of health complications. These sounds can be
include patient bodily sounds such as gagging (the sound similar to choaking), strider
73
(constriction in breathing pattern), and sleep apnea (irregular breathing patterns). Safety
threatening sounds might include patient falls, the sounds of leaking air from the oxygen
tube, and sounds of falling objects. ICU nurses typically monitor breathing rates,
respiratory efforts, pattern of respiration (i.e., rhythm and depth), presence of audible
wheeze, and dry cough to assess patient respiratory health (Owen, 1982). Examples of
critical speech sounds include help calls by patients and caregivers, announcements from
overhead pagers, medical conversations either conducted on the phone (e.g., between
Monitoring of both alarm and non-alarm auditory cues can be significantly affected by
environmental factors. One study indicated that improved hospital sound environments
can have a significant impact on speech intelligibly outcomes (Blomkvist et al., 2005). In
addition to environmental factors, strategic design of auditory cues such as alarms and
drug names can also reduce the risks to patient safety. Similarities in orthographic (i.e.,
spelling) and phonological (i.e., sound) qualities of drug names increase the probability
of making false recognitions (Lambert et al., 2001). According to national statistics, 1 out
of 4 medical errors in the U.S. involves drug name confusion (U.S. Pharmacopeia, 1997).
based on vowel sounds can be less irritating and less susceptible to masking by the
74
There are also studies focusing on three of the individual components of the auditory
monitoring process: sound detection, sound recognition, and sound localization. The
findings of hospital and non-hospital research on these three components are discussed in
the following sections. Overall, sound detection, recognition, and localization can be very
in controlled lab environments as described below provide some insight to the attributes
Sound detection involves the ability to hear an auditory cue, or “target.” In settings like
The human auditory system is capable of focusing attention on one source/speaker, even
when there are multiple competing sound sources in the background (Cherry, 1953).
This phenomenon is known as the “cocktail party effect”. However, Stifelman (1994)
indicated that listening to the target speech signal while simultaneously exposed to two
other competing background signals may require too much effort. There is some evidence
indicating that such demanding sound tasks can be supported by specific acoustic
qualities of sound targets and competing signals. For example, Treisman (1964) found
background signals had similar acoustic features to each other. This phenomenon is
two competing sound signals with similar acoustic features (e.g., pitch, spatial location)
form one single auditory stream. This enables the perception of multiple competing
75
signals as one single interfering signal. Bess and Humes (1990) found that human hearing
is most accurate for frequencies between 1kHz-4kHz, the frequency range also
the same frequency range as speech signals (1kHz-4kHz) thus can potentially mask a
speech target. Other research indicated that a frequency difference of 110Hz between
target and competing signals can improve the intelligibility of the target speech signal
(Brokx, & Nooteboom, 1982). Blauert (1997) indicates that intelligibility of target sounds
with and without meaning might have a significant impact on signal detection.
There is also some evidence showing that at certain sound pressure levels speech
detection can be more effective. According to Kobayaski (2007), the minimum listening
difficulty occurred when the target speech level was 50-55dBA. At higher or lower
speech levels, listening difficulty increased. The same study indicated that humans have a
known as the “Lombard Effect.” Speech levels generally stayed constant when the
background level was less than 40dBA. However, when background level was higher
than 40dBA, the talker adjusted his/her speech level to maintain approximately 15dBA of
signal-to-noise ratio.
conducted listening tests with nurses to assess their ability to identify critical alarms
occurring in ICUs (Cropp et al., 1994). It was found that only 50% of critical alarms
76
presented to nurses were identified correctly. Moreover, caregivers with more than one
year experience scored higher than those with less than one year. Wallace et al. (1994)
rooms. These findings might be associated with limited capabilities of human auditory
system. Patterson and Mikoy (1980) suggested that humans are not able to easily learn
and remember the significance of more than 8 different sounds even in unstressed
conditions. Similarly, another group suggested that 6-10 auditory warning signals would
auditory cues. According to the study findings, orthographic (i.e., spelling), and
Recognizing a specific medical alarm is difficult in part due to the similar acoustic
content of the many different alarms in an ICU. Some studies have documented the
acoustic qualities of medical alarms. Wallace (1994) measured the intensity and
majority of the alarms consisted of mid to high range frequencies ranged between 250Hz
to 8000Hz. Almost all of the alarms were multi-frequency signals. Different from sine
tones (a.k.a. pure tones), a multi-frequency tone consists of multiple frequencies. Nine of
the alarms consisted of 4-5 different frequencies including 500, 1000, 2000, 4000, and/or
8000Hz. Other two alarms also had 250Hz content. Five of the multi-frequency alarms
were limited to frequencies higher than 1000Hz and seven of them were limited to
77
frequencies higher than 2000Hz. Only three of the twenty-six alarms were pure tones and
had a frequency of 4000Hz. Loudness levels of these alarms ranged between 45dBA to
medical equipment (Hirose et al., 2005). Different from the findings of the previous
study, the study found that 90% of the alarms generated by the equipment included only a
single frequency and the majority had discontinuous patterns. The spectral content of the
monitor alarms, NIBP and pulse oximeter ranged between 500 and 4000Hz. The
frequency level of the life support equipment including infusion pumps and ventilators
ranged between 1000-4000Hz. Most of the time, the maximum (Lmax) and minimum
(Lmin) sound pressure levels generated by the alarms exceeded 70dB and 50dB,
respectively. The repeating module of the warning signal composed of alarm and silence
frequencies so that they are less likely to be masked by the background noise. For
example, ASTM Standard F29.03.04 for anesthesia and respiratory care medical
equipment alarm signals requires that medical alarms should have a fundamental
Sound localization relates to auditory system’s ability to make decisions about the
direction and distance of the sound event (Moore, 1997). Auditory information that is
78
critical for sound localization is known as a “spatial” auditory cue. Various studies have
been conducted in controlled lab environments to explore the attributes necessary for the
Developments in binaural hearing research explain how the human auditory system uses
binaural cues obtained by two ears during sound localization. The auditory system
essentially assesses time and sound level differences between the sound waves arriving at
the left and the right ear (Blauert, 2005). Due to the different path lengths to the two ears,
the arrival times of the sound waves emitted from a single sound source are not always
the same at the left and the right ear (Thompson, 1877). This acoustic principle is known
as “inter-aural time difference” (ITD). On the other hand, existence of the head between
the two ears creates an acoustic shadow and causes sound attenuation which leads to an
“inter-aural (sound) level difference” (ILD) between the left and the right ears
binaural cues (Rayleigh, 1907). According to Rayleigh’s “duplex theory”, ITDs are most
critical for the localization of low-frequency sounds and ILDs are most critical for high
frequencies.
The sound level of sources, a listener`s familiarity with sound events, and the acoustic
quality of sources are the major factors for distance and direction judgments. For
example, in a sound field free of reflecting surfaces, every doubling in distance produces
6dB reduction in the sound level at the ears. Distances longer than 15m (49ft) lead to an
79
additional sound attenuation and slight change in the spectral balance of the sound
reaching the ears (Ingard, 1953; Plack, 2005). This is mainly because air absorbs more
high frequency energy than low frequency in relation to the length of the air path.
Study findings indicate that a listener’s ability to make accurate directional estimations is
associated with their familiarity with signals. Makous and Middlebrooks (1990) indicated
them made errors of less than 5 degrees both in horizontal and vertical directions. Plenge
and Bruschen (1971) found that subjects` directional judgment performance improved
when short speech signals were pronounced by people that they are familiar with. The
authors also reported listeners` tendency to report the unfamiliar signals as if they were
The acoustic quality of signals, such as duration and spectral content, also influence
localization estimations. For example, Aschoff (1963) explored the effects of duration on
direction estimations of the auditory system (cross ref. Blauert 1997). According to study
findings, listeners were able to hear the noise circling around their heads when signals
from circular array of loudspeakers were played with a slow switching speed. When
switching speed was increased, the noise was heard to move between left and right sides.
When the switching speed further increased, the auditory event was heard approximately
in the middle of the head. As another example, Blauert (1997) explored the effect of
spectral content on sound localization and conducted an experiment with 140 subjects by
using pulses of white and pink noise. Approximately 90% of the subjects succeeded in
80
their direction judgments. Sandel et al. (1955) found that the localization performance
was worst around 1.5 kHz-3 kHz. According to another study, localization precision was
best for frequencies between 200Hz-1 kHz and the error magnitude peaked around 3 kHz
(Mills 1958) .
Listeners` ability to localize sounds varies depending on the acoustic environment. For
This is mainly because later-arriving reflections can reduce the impact of direct sound
and early-arriving reflections necessary for sound localization. The human ear is capable
of integrating early reflections (typically up to 50-80ms after the arrival of the primary
signal) with the direct sound signal. However, strong and later-arriving reflections
(typically those greater than 80ms after the arrival of the primary signal) can cause
changes in the perception of the auditory event such as changes in the direction of the
primary sound signal (Blauert, 1997). Hartman (1983) conducted listening tests in a
controlled room with human subjects to assess the impact of signal qualities and room
when design interventions were adopted to reduce reverberation time such as including
low ceiling configuration the reverberation time was about half of the high ceiling
condition. The study also concluded that the localization of the sine tones is independent
of the room reverberation time. In other words, when localizing sine tones the human
auditory system is limited to benefit from the useful reflections in the rooms. Different
81
from complex nature of the sounds in hospitals, sine tones are composed of a single
frequency. As with sound detection, studies also show that sound level differences
between target signal and competing signals can affect sound localization. Houtgast and
Plomp (1968) suggested sound localization can be more effective if the level of
competing noise is 15dB or more below than the target signal. Lorenzi et al. (1999)
indicated sound localization accuracy remains unaffected by competing noise until a 0–6
3.2.1 Scope
Intensive Care Unit (ICU) patients are one of the most sensitive patient populations in a
hospital. Mortality rates among ICU patients range between 10% to 20% in most
hospitals (SCCM 2006). To promote patient safety, ICU nurses continuously monitor the
health status of their patients, and strive to maintain appropriate bodily conditions by
assessing visual and auditory cues, and responding immediately to abnormal changes.
The ability to conduct uninterrupted patient monitoring is a challenging task for ICU
requires the absence of a barrier between observer and target. Study findings indicate that
increased visibility of patients in the ICUs through design strategies can help improve
82
Different from visual monitoring, auditory monitoring is an “eyes-free” cue assessment
technique. When caregivers` hands and eyes are busy, auditory monitoring can enable
Therefore, it is one of the early requirements for novice nurses to familiarize themselves
to auditory cues that might pose a risk to patient safety. Nurses` ability to conduct
hospital sound environments. Even though the significance of visual monitoring in ICUs
for patient outcomes including patient falls has been well documented, there is limited
information about auditory monitoring. This study aims to provide (1) a comprehensive
overview of previous research related to auditory monitoring; and (2) to describe a case
3.2.2 Methodology
A case study is conducted in the two ICUs described in Chapter 2 in order to assess the
discussed, the online survey contains questions about nurse tasks highly important for
patient monitoring, ICU sounds necessary for patient safety, and key listening locations
in the unit. Please refer to section 2.2.2 for additional details on the subjects.
The units are similar in staffing and patient acuity as discussed in section 2.2.2: similar
number of private patient rooms and staffing models (intensivists and nurse
practitioners), acuity levels of patients, nurse working hours. On the other hand, the
designs of these two units are different with the following specific differences that might
83
impact auditory monitoring. As shown in Figure 3.1, different from the design principles
distances that results in smaller volumes and shorter corridors. For example, total length
of the staff corridors in MedSurg-ICU is 73 m (240 ft) while it is 183 m (600 ft) in
Neuro-ICU. Moreover, spaces in the Neuro-ICU are installed with high performance
acoustic ceiling tiles (with higher sound absorption qualities), while it is only regular
ceiling tiles (with less sound absorption qualities) from 1980`s in the MedSurg-ICU.
Figure 3.1 Floor-plans of two ICUs showing circulation patterns and impulse response
measurement locations
qualities of the corridors in the two wards. The impulse response measurements have
taken place at 6 different receiver locations in two hallways and in an unoccupied patient
84
room in each ward as shown in Figure 3.1. The measurement system consists of a GSR
Gateway data acquisition system (DAQ), laptop equipped with EASERA software v.1.1,
and Larson Davis 824 sound level meter microphones. A maximum length sequence
(MLS) excitation signal is used. The sound source is located slightly off the center of the
rooms in each unit. The receivers are located about 7.6m-10.6m (25ft-35ft) away from
the source in each hallway. In the unoccupied patient rooms of the two wards, the source
is also located about 1.3m (4.5ft) away from the receivers. In both the corridors and
patient rooms of the two wards, receivers are placed at distances outside the critical
distance to avoid significant impact of direct sound on the reverberation time. In the
patient rooms of the two wards, the critical distances range between 0.76m- 0.9m (2.5ft-
3ft). In the corridors the critical distances are 4.5m (15ft) in the Neuro-ICU and 1.8m
3.2.3 Findings
The Neuro-ICU and MedSurg-ICU nurses have been asked to rate the perceived
critical care nursing. The distribution of the nurse responses with the distribution normal
curve are shown in Figure 3.2. More than 85% of the Neuro-ICU and MedSurg-ICU
nurses “completely agreed” that visual and auditory patient monitoring are important
tasks in critical care nursing, while the majority of the rest “somewhat” agreed that these
two tasks were important. Mean levels for perceived importance of visual and auditory
85
monitoring are 4.7 and 4.8 out of 5, respectively. Based on significance test results, there
are no statistically significant differences between the perceptions of nurses in the two
units (p>0.05).
“Visual patient monitoring is a crucial task in critical care nursing.” “Overall auditory monitoring (listen, recognize and respond to auditory cues)
is a crucial task in critical care nursing.”
Figure 3.2 Perceived importance of visual vs. auditory monitoring averaged across the
two units
More detailed analyses are shown in Figure 3.3 which represents the perceived
majority of the nurses from two units believe that individual sound tasks conducted
during auditory monitoring (i.e., ability to hear, differentiate and localize auditory cues)
are also highly important nurse tasks. Based on analysis of variance test results, there are
86
Figure 3.3 Perceived importance of three auditory monitoring tasks vs. visual monitoring
averaged across the two units
Nurses from both units have been also asked to rate the importance of different medical
alarms for patient safety. Results are shown in Figure 3.4. Almost all nurses believe that
ventilator alarms are “very” critical for patient safety. More than 70% of the nurses
reported hissing sounds of the respiratory ventilators are also very critical for patient
safety. These two auditory cues are monitored to ensure patient’s adequate and proper
functioning of pressure relief valves. The alarms generated by the medical ventilator
might be associated with different events such as secretion in endotracheal tube (ETT), a
kink in the vent tubing, patient biting on ETT, increased airway pressure, a disconnect in
the vent tubing, displaced ETT, patient anxiety or pain, hypoxia, hypercapnia, and lack of
87
As shown in Figure 3.4, the perceived importance of other non-ventilator alarms (patient
monitor, IV-pump, nurse call and feeding-pump alarms) varied. About 85% of the nurses
believed that patient monitor alarms are “very” critical for patient safety. Patient monitor
alarms inform any abnormal changes in the physiologic parameters such as heart rate,
temperature, ECG, SpO2, blood pressure, and CO2. A specific type of patient monitor
alarm, known as the “code” indicates highly critical occurrences that might have life-and-
death implications such as heart failure, cardiac arrest. The percentage of nurses who
believe that the IV-pump alarms and nurse call alarms are “very” critical for patient
safety was 63% and 55%, respectively. The majority of the rest believe that these tasks
are “somewhat” critical. Not many nurses (20%) have rated the feeding pump alarms as
“very” critical for patient safety, but about 60% of the nurses have reported that they are
Figure 3.4 Perceived importance of different medical alarms for patient safety averaged
across the two units
88
In addition, nurses have been also asked to report any other auditory cues including alarm
and non-alarm sounds necessary for patient safety. Some of those reported alarms include
bed rail alarms, bed exit alarm, CRRT alarms (from the dialysis machine), PCA-pump
parameters (from patient monitors). The latter three as well as the code alarms might lead
to rapid heart failure. Examples of non-alarm auditory cues mentioned include staff help
calls which might indicate the need for additional resources and assistance or a
significant problem with the patient, patient help calls which generally occur when a
patient is not capable of reaching the call light, unusual or distress noise from patient
rooms (e.g. patient getting out of the bed, patient fall), family help calls, and patient
bodily sounds.
Nurses have been also asked how often they use auditory monitoring in various locations
in their ICUs. Specifically, they have been asked about corridors, patient rooms, central
nurse stations, medication prep zones, and supply rooms. Note that both ICUs contained
these various spaces. Additionally, nurses in the Neuro-ICU have been asked about
distributed nurse stations, as this is a design feature unique to this unit and distributed
nurse stations are not contained in the MedSurg-ICU. As Figure 3.5 shows, in both the
Neuro-ICU and MedSurg-ICU, the locations where the most auditory monitoring
occurred are the corridors, patient rooms, and central nurse stations. The Neuro-ICU
nurses additionally have reported that they also frequently monitor auditory cues at
medication preparation zones and at the distributed nurse stations. On the other hand,
89
49% of the MedSurg-ICU nurses have reported that they rarely monitor auditory cues at
the medication preparation zones. In both wards, they rarely monitor auditory cues at the
supply rooms. In the MedSurg-ICU, the medication preparation and supply room spaces
are separated by walls that extend from floor to ceiling. In the Neuro-ICU, the medication
Figure 3.5 Frequency of auditory monitoring at different locations in the two ICUs
90
Auditory monitoring performance of nurses in MedSurg-ICU and Neuro-ICU
Nurses have been also questioned about their ability to recognize, hear, and localize
auditory cues. As shown in Figure 3.6, the results vary. As shown in Figure 3.6a, the
that they can hear the critical sounds in the unit. This percentage is somewhat higher in
Figure 3.6 Perceived sound task performance of nurses in the two ICU`s: (a) ability to
hear critical sounds (b) ability to differentiate critical sounds (c) ability to localize critical
sounds
91
Figure 3.6 continued
Also as shown in Figure 3.6b, the majority of nurses “completely” agree that they can
differentiate the critical sounds in the unit. This percentage is slightly higher in the
agree that they can guess the location of the critical sounds in the unit. In the MedSurg-
differences between the perceptions of the nurses in the two units are not significant.
92
To summarize, hearing and sound localization are perceived to be somewhat higher in the
noise levels and reverberation time. As discussed earlier, the overall noise levels are the
same in each unit, although the MedSurg-ICU has been found to be more “peaky.”
However, the MedSurg-ICU is a more compact unit and the reverberation time was
measured to be lower. It is possible that the more compact setting and lower
reverberation time of the MedSurg-ICU led to the improved ability of nurses hear and
localize sounds, despite unit being more “peaky”. The reason for the difference in
differentiation perception between the two wards is less clear; one hypothesis is that the
more “peaky” sounds in the MedSurg-ICU cause confusion when differentiating one
To compare the length of the reverberation time (RT30) in the two wards, impulse
response measurements are conducted. Averaged RT30 in the corridors of the MedSurg-
ICU ranges between 0.44 and 0.56s across different frequencies (Table 3.1). In one of the
corridors, the RT30 is as low as 0.3s. In the patient room of the MedSurg-ICU, averaged
93
Table 3.1 Distribution of averaged RT30 across frequency measured in the corridors and
in the unoccupied patient rooms in the two ICUs
In the Neuro-ICU corridors and unoccupied patient rooms reverberation times are higher
Neuro-ICU ranges between 0.70 and 0.87s across different frequencies. In one of the
corridors the RT30 is as high as 0.92s and the lowest RT30 measured in different
corridors is 0.65s. In the patient room of the Neuro-ICU, averaged RT30 (250 Hz-4 kHz)
is 0.8s. Given these values, the difference between reverberation times in the Neuro-ICU
and MedSurg-ICU are perceptible based on ISO/DIS 3382-1 standards (Bork, 2000).
3.2.4 Discussion
particularly in ICUs have not been investigated as much as visual patient monitoring.
monitoring, including previous research related to the topic. Additionally, results from a
case study examining nurses` perception of auditory monitoring in two ICUs are
presented.
94
In the case study, ICU nurses strongly agree that auditory monitoring is highly important
for patient safety in addition to visual monitoring. In addition, nurses ability to hear,
differentiate, and localize auditory cues are also perceived to be critically important for
patient safety. However, previous research shows that these sound tasks are hard to
complex as they are multi-source, noisy, and most times reverberant in nature. Previous
research indicates that particularly in highly reverberant spaces, sound localization can be
very challenging. High background noise levels can also potentially negatively affect the
detection of target sounds. Furthermore, the human auditory system has limited
monitoring capabilities. Pushing the limits of the auditory system to monitor high number
of sound events can require excessive efforts, in particular for novice nurses.
In the case study, interesting differences are found when comparing the importance of
different ICU sounds for patient safety. Some of the alarm sounds are perceived to be
more important than others. Specifically, the majority of nurses believe that medical
ventilator alarms and patient monitor alarms are very important for keeping patients safe.
A considerable percentage of nurses also believed that nurse call, feeding pump, and IV-
Additionally, it is found in the case study that nurses listen to the auditory cues from
different key locations in the critical care units. Some of those key locations include:
patient rooms, nurse stations, and corridors. It needs to be noted that all these spaces are
all connected through corridors. In the Neuro-ICU where the medication room is also
95
connected to the corridors, it was also reported as a listening location. On the other hand,
since in both units` supply rooms are separated from the corridors with walls from floor
to ceiling, they are not reported as listening locations. Considering these results, the
locations.
By comparing nurses` ability to conduct sound tasks in the case study MedSurg-ICU and
performance is higher in the MedSurg-ICU compared to the Neuro-ICU even though the
Neuro-ICU is installed with high performance absorptive acoustic ceiling tiles and the
MedSurg-ICU was found to be more “peaky”. Parallel to nurse perceptions, RT30 levels
(based on impulse response measurements) in the corridors and in the patient room are
environments such as spatial design features associated with RT30 levels in addition to
between different design features and reverberation time, more controlled follow up
studies are also conducted. The study results are introduced in the following sections.
Overall, lack of proper monitoring of auditory cues can potentially delay the nurses`
rescue efforts by disabling the early detection of complications, and could result in
serious patient injury and even death. For effective auditory monitoring in hospitals, both
96
environmental acoustic (e.g., reverberation time, overall noise levels) and sound source
specific (e.g. acoustic qualities of sounds, number) solutions should be researched and
adopted.
97
CHAPTER 4
Chapter 3 demonstrated that acoustic qualities of hospital sound environments can have
caregivers. Previous research indicates that design features of architectural settings such
as floor-plate shape qualities can impact acoustic qualities of the built environment. This
chapter provides an overview of the previous literature liking design and acoustics,
The human auditory system is capable of differentiating the acoustic qualities of the
sound produced inside and that produced outside an enclosed space (Mehta et al., 1997).
Sound produced inside a room bounces back and forth from boundary surfaces. On the
other hand, sound generally travels freely away from the source outdoors, unless major
significantly affect the sound qualities and sound levels. This field of acoustic research
exploring the sound behavior in enclosed spaces with different design implications is
98
4.2 ROOM ACOUSTICS PARAMETERS
Acoustic measures used in the analysis of rooms are called room-acoustics parameters.
reverberation time (RT) and other room-acoustic metrics including sound strength (G),
clarity (C), definition (D), early decay time (EDT), lateral fraction (LF/LFC) and centre
widely used in architectural acoustics for the assessment of various types of architectural
spaces including music halls, hospitals, worship places and more recently in long
motivation was to identify and quantify different aspects of the overall acoustic quality of
the music halls that are important for listeners' perception. Detailed information about the
room acoustics parameters relevant to this chapter can be found in the “Definition of
This section reviews sound propagation theories and empirical data presented in the
previous research.
The classic sound propagation theory (a.k.a diffuse field theory) was developed to predict
the behavior of sound in concert halls. According to this theory, direct sound becomes
99
negligible approximately after 10m (19.6ft) (a.k.a. “reverberation radius” or “critical
distance”) (Galindo et al., 2005). At the critical distance, sound energy of the direct and
reverberant sound fields are equal. After this point acoustic qualities are mainly defined
by volume and RT. Additionally, empirical data shows that reverberation time in rooms
does not vary much by distance or the position of the receiver (Abdou, 2003; Lundeby &
acoustic qualities of sound environments. Previous studies linking floor-plate design and
acoustics took place mainly in the concert halls as discussed in the following section.
Since the 18th century, concert hall floor-plates took a variety of different shapes. Typical
concert hall floor-plate shapes include shoebox (rectangular), fan-shape, vineyard and
100
Shoe-box Fan
Vineyard Horseshoe
Figure 4.1 Commonly applied concert hall floor-plate shapes (from Barron, 1993)
There are also concert halls with elliptical floor-plate shapes but not many. One of the
reasons is that elliptical floor-plates focuses sound at the centers of the geometry. This
unwanted focal effect (Cox & D'Antonio, 2009). A rare successful example of this type is
Royal Albert Hall which required many efforts from acousticians (Figure 4.2) (Zhang,
2005).
101
Figure 4.2 Royal Albert Hall with elliptical floor-plate shape (from Zhang, 2005)
Concert hall floor-plate shapes can also influence qualities of lateral sound (i.e.,
reflections coming from the side walls). Early lateral reflections contribute to the feeling
in concert halls. Findings of recent studies indicate that parallel and narrow walls of shoe-
box concert halls with rectangular floor-plate shapes improve the early lateral reflections
(Mehta et al., 1999). This is a primary factor explaining the successful acoustic
performance of many shoe-box shaped concert halls. Beranek and Hann surveyed
acoustic qualities of various concert halls (Hann, & Fricke, 1995). According to their
findings, two thirds of the concert halls rated as “excellent” were in the shape of shoebox.
102
Figure 4.3 Boston Symphony Hall with rectangular floor-plate shape (from Hann, &
Fricke, 1995)
more complex floor-plate shapes such as vineyard since the beginning of the 20th century.
surrounded by side walls. This type of design improves sound quality by providing lateral
reflections. The Berlin Philharmonic is one of the most successful modern concert halls
103
Figure 4.4 Berlin Philharmonie Concert Hall with vineyard floor-plate shape (from Mehta
et al., 1999)
The previous section introduced typical floor-plate shapes applied in concert halls.
Several studies statistically and systematically analyzed the impact of floor-plate design
qualities on acoustic outcomes in the concert halls. This section reviews the findings of
Early studies indicated the significant impact of hall width in predicting sound qualities
in concert halls (Gade, 1990; Klosak & Gade, 2008; Schroeder, Gottlob, & Siebrasse,
1974). Gade (1990) conducted impulse response measurements in 32 European halls and
statistically analyzed the relationship between room-averaged acoustic data and design
variables. By comparing the expected (based on classic sound propagation theory) and
104
measured results, the study concluded that volume and reverberation time are the two
main factors affecting new room-acoustic parameters such as reverberance and clarity.
This finding supports how well the classic theory predicts acoustic conditions (e.g., EDT,
C, L) of the concert hall as a function of RT and volume. The study also indicated the
significant impact of floor-plate shape on clarity. Based on regression analysis results, the
study found that clarity is positively correlated with hall width and angle between side
walls. In other words, clarity is expected to be higher in wide/ fan-shaped walls compared
to narrow/rectangular halls. The interpretation is that wider rooms tend to minimize the
distance between source and receiver and increase the seating capacity of the halls and
splayed side walls directs the early sound energy to the rear of the room (Mehta et al.,
1999). Gade (1990) also found a fairly high correlation between hall width and LEF
Schroeder, Gottlob, and Siebrasse (1974) correlated subjective perception with objective
design and acoustic parameters (i.e., volume, width, time delay, reverberation time,
statistical analysis. For the subjective evaluations, a pre-recorded signal was played in
each hall and re-recorded at the receiver locations with the use of an artificial head.
Acoustic parameters were obtained from measured impulse response at the artificial
head`s ears. Subjective evaluation of the recorded signals took place in an anechoic
105
aural coherence (negatively), volume (negatively) and width of the halls (negatively)
width was negatively correlated with reverberation time. These findings indicate the
A recent study used acoustic simulations and generated various theoretical design models
shoebox-shaped concert halls (Klosak, & Gade, 2008). In total 24 theoretical models
were generated, as shown in Figure 4.5. Only two design variables were changed among
those theoretical models: a) volume and b) length-to-width ratio. The floor-plate shapes
of the theoretical models ranged from square to elongated rectangle as shown in Figure
4.5.
Figure 4.5 Floor-plate shapes of theoretical design models (from Klosak, & Gade, 2008)
106
Figure 4.5 continued
Acoustic simulation results calculated for 24 theoretical models indicated that uniformity
of the distribution for sound quality metrics such as clarity (C80) and strength (G)
decreases as the shape gets more elongated. This was particularly true in the rooms with
smallest volume (8000m3). Similar results were observed for increasing volume. The
study also highlighted the impact of both width and length in predicting lateral fraction
(LF). Among theoretical models with similar L/W ratio, as the width increased LF values
(i.e., the amount of the useful reflections arriving from the side walls) decreased. The
study suggested the following regression equation showing the relationship between
qualities and acoustic outcomes and found statistically significant relationships. Even
though these studies were particularly conducted in concert halls and correlated the
107
design features and acoustic parameters more specific to concert halls, their findings in
Long enclosures are considered as non-proportional spaces related with their extreme
spatial proportions. Examples of long enclosures are corridors and underground train
stations. Acoustics of these non-proportional spaces differ from the acoustics of more
relationships between room design features and acoustics do not necessarily apply to non-
proportional spaces. The following section defines the differences between sound
Classic theory assumes the existence of diffuse field and uniform distribution of the
sound rays in all directions (Kang, 2002b). In proportional spaces where a sufficiently
diffuse sound field exist (e.g., concert halls), beyond the reverberation radius, the
formulas is considered to be the same at any point in the room beyond the reverberation
radius. On the other hand, sound field in long enclosures is not uniformly diffuse related
with the non-proportional/extreme dimensions. It is one of the key reasons why sound
108
behavior in long enclosures is quite different compared to sound behavior in the
Various studies have been conducted to analyze the behavior of sound in long enclosures
with rectangular floor-plate shapes. The main goal of these studies was to explain how
different qualities of sound vary by distance and long enclosure design features (e.g.,
cross section size). The majority of the studies systematically documented acoustic
qualities of long enclosure sound environments (e.g., sound attenuation, early decay time
distances from the sound source. In the following sections, the findings of these studies
are introduced.
corridor and multiple underground stations with rectangular floor-plate shapes. These
field studies primarily explored the changing sound behavior with distance. The length,
width and height of the corridor were 42.5m (139ft), 1.56m (5.2ft) and 2.83m (9.3ft)
respectively. The boundaries of the corridor were geometrically reflective and average
absorption was low (approximately 0.1 at 500Hz and 1 kHz). The results of the field
measurements conducted at different distances from the sound source are shown in
Figure 4.6 below. Overall, reverberation time results measured at different receiver
109
Figure 4.6 Spatial distribution of reverberation times in a single corridor (from Kang,
2002c)
The underground stations studied by Kang (2002c) were deep tube stations with circular
cross-sectional shape (named Old Street, Warren Street and St John`s Wood). The results
of the field measurements conducted at different distances from the sound source are
shown in Figure 4.7 below. As the source and receiver distance increases, the
reverberation time and early decay time increase along the length until about 40m (131ft)
and then become approximately stable or decrease slightly. In general reverberation time
values were greater than early decay time values, indicating that sound decay curves of
110
Figure 4.7 Spatial distributions of RT30 and EDT levels in tunnels (from Kang ,2002c)
The number of studies exploring the association between design and acoustics of long
studies to systematically assess the impact of different design factors on acoustics of long
enclosures (Kang, 2002c; Kang, 2002d). In these theoretical studies, mainly the design
and acoustics of long enclosures with rectangular floor-plate shapes (e.g., single
corridors) were analyzed. Related with the focus of this thesis, the following section is
limited to studies exploring the impact of long enclosure designs on reverberant qualities.
More detailed information about the relationship between long enclosure design and
111
Design factors affecting reverberant qualities of long enclosure sound environments
Different design factors can affect length of reverberation time in long enclosures such as
corridor length, absorptive qualities of the surface materials, sound source type and cross
systematically assess the impact of design features on length of the reverberation time in
A set of theoretical models were generated to assess how reverberation time varies along
the corridor length in long enclosures with different absorption coefficients (Figure 4.8).
Long enclosures with 6mx4m (20ftx13ft) cross section were assigned two absorption
coefficients: 0.1 and 0.2. The boundaries of the long enclosures were geometrically
reflective. For the less absorptive case ( = 0.1), reverberation time values increased
rapidly until a maximum and then decreased very slightly in the long enclosures. For the
more absorptive case ( = 0.2), reverberation time values increased less rapidly until a
maximum and then decreased with a steeper slope as compared to the ( = 0.1) case.
112
Figure 4.8 RT30 and EDT due to absorptive qualities of boundaries in long enclosures
(from Kang, 2002c)
A second set of theoretical models were generated to assess the impact of diffusely and
interesting differences between behaviors of sound in these two long enclosures. In the
geometrically reflective long enclosure, reverberation time increased slightly and then
time consistently increased. Moreover, reverberation time values were generally 30-60%
113
Figure 4.9 RT30 and EDT due to reflective qualities of boundaries (from Kang, 2002c)
A third set of theoretical models was generated to assess the impact of corridor length on
reverberation time values (Figure 4.10). One of theoretical models was 60m (196ft) long
and the other was 120m (394ft) long. The boundaries of both long enclosures were
diffusely reflective and the end walls were open. Reverberation time values along the
corridor length in the shorter case were about 20-30% less compared to the longer case.
114
Figure 4.10 Floor-plans of two long enclosures with different length (from Kang, 2002c)
A fourth set of theoretical models was generated to assess the shape and size qualities of
long enclosure cross sections on length of reverberation time in long enclosures with
diffusely reflective boundaries. Theoretical models with 5 different cross sections (and
thus different aspect ratios) were generated including 12mx8m (39ftx26ft), 16mx6m
of all models were 120m (393ft) and all surfaces were assigned an absorption coefficient
of 0.2. As shown in Figure 4.11, for a given cross-sectional area, the reverberation time
values could vary significantly with the aspect ratio. Reverberation time values become
greater as the cross-section tends towards square. In addition, in the long enclosures with
larger cross section, reverberation time values were longer. Similar results were observed
115
Figure 4.11 RT30 and EDT due to cross-sectional size (from Kang, 2002c)
A fifth set of theoretical models were generated to assess the distribution of boundary
had constant amounts of absorption with five different distributions ranged from evenly
distributed absorption in cross section (D1) and one boundary strongly absorbent and the
continuously and the variation was about 30%. Reverberation time was the longest with
absorption that is evenly distributed in cross section and the shortest when one boundary
116
Kang also systematically analyzed the impact of street design on length of the
reverberation time and other acoustic outcomes such as sound attenuation by conducting
computer simulations (2002d). Similar to other long enclosure types, in the urban streets
increasing distance between source and receiver. Similarly, reverberation time values also
The previous section reviewed the findings of theoretical acoustic studies conducted in
long enclosures with rectangular floor–plate shapes. There are very few studies that have
interconnected long enclosures with complex floor-plate shapes (e.g., long enclosures
with branches). The following section reviews the findings of the studies conducted in
Abel et al. (2008) analyzed the association between design and acoustics of historical
off narrow corridors (Figure 4.12, Figure 4.13). Some of the other features of the galleries
were rectilinear shapes, narrow width, short height and stone walls.
117
Figure 4.12 Site-plan of historical underground galleries (from Abel et al., 2008)
118
Impulse response measurements were conducted at multiple different locations. As
shown in Figure 4.13, location K is a representative receiver location located at one of the
the underground galleries reverberation time was short (generally less than 0.5s) to
enable the rhythmic sound of shell trumpets. Some other sound qualities include dense
and energetic early reflections and low-inter-aural cross correlation. The study also found
that number of turns (in the path between source and receiver) have significant impact on
reverberation time. Increasing the number of turns between source and receiver led to
higher reverberation time values. Reverberation time was shortest along straight lines
Kang (2002d) systematically compared the sound fields in street canyons with different
designs that incorporated a main street / side street configuration. A set of computer
simulations was generated to assess how source location affects reverberation time. As
shown in Figure 4.14, when the sound source moved to different locations in the main
street with two side streets, a slight increase (10%) in average reverberation time levels
was observed. Moreover, related with lack of direct sound, in the side streets
reverberation time was systematically longer compared to the values in the main street.
119
Figure 4.14 Spatial distribution of reverberation time (in second) along a “+” street
junction (from Kang, 2002d)
Liu and Lu (2009a) compared the sound fields of straight long enclosures without any
branch and the sound fields of the long enclosures with one branch (vertical, left inclined
and right inclined) via 1:10 scale physical models (Figure 4.15). The dimensions of the
full scale long enclosure were 94m-length (308ft), 7m-width (23ft) and 5m –height (16ft).
However the characteristics of the branches such as length, width and angle were not the
same.
120
Figure 4.15 Floor-plans of theoretical long enclosures with a single branch (from Liu, and
Lu, 2009a)
Reverberation time varied at different frequencies (Figure 4.16). For example at the low
frequencies, RT30 was longest for the straight long enclosure, second longest for the long
enclosure with a widened left inclined branch, third longest for the long enclosure with a
left inclined branch and the shortest for the long enclosure with an extended left inclined
branch.
121
Figure 4.16 Distribution of RT30 in long enclosures with a single branch
(top): 500Hz; (bottom):1000Hz (from Liu, and Lu, 2009a)
Another study by Liu and Lu (2009b) analyzed the sound fields of long enclosures with
multiple vertical or inclines branches via 1:10 scale physical models. The dimensions of
the full scale long enclosure were 94m –length (308ft), 7m –width (26ft) and 5m-height
(16ft). Sound source and receivers were arranged along the central line of the main
122
Figure 4.17 Floor-plans of theoretical long enclosures with multiple branches (from Liu,
and Lu, 2009b)
RT30 values of long enclosures with multiple branches were frequency dependent
(Figure 4.18) and frequency has little impact on RT30 levels in the enclosures with one
branch. Long enclosures with multiple branches had lower RT30 levels compared to long
enclosures with one branch and without any branch. In the low frequency range,
orientation of the branches did not affect the results. However, in the high frequency
range, orientation of the braches had a significant impact on RT30 values. Overall, the
123
study findings indicated that sound field of a long enclosure with multiple branches is
more complex and inhomogeneous compared to long enclosures without branches or with
one branch.
124
4.6 CONCLUSION
Reverberation time is a universal acoustic metric that has been used to assess the acoustic
characteristics of various spaces including long spaces and rooms. Newer room-acoustic
metrics were developed such as early decay time, clarity, sound strength to particularly
assess the acoustic qualities of music halls that relate to audience experience. Even
though some studies have documented the RT and EDT levels in long enclosures, there
are still discussions whether the newer acoustic metrics are applicable for the acoustic
Related with the design qualities of spaces such as spatial proportions, the acoustic
theories applicable in one type of space (e.g., concert halls) might not be applicable in
other types of spaces (e.g., corridors). For example, the principle of classic room acoustic
theory that is generally valid in music halls is not necessarily applicable in long
enclosures. Various studies have been conducted in the music halls and statistically
such as width, length and acoustic outcomes. However the findings of these studies are
limited to more proportional shaped rooms and do not necessarily apply to long
enclosures.
The number of studies conducted in long enclosures exploring the association between
design and acoustics is growing. The acoustic conditions in long enclosures were studied
125
systematic analysis of the acoustic conditions in long enclosures. However most of those
studies mainly considered straight long enclosures such as urban street or underground
train stations. A few studies explored the reverberant qualities of long enclosures with
urban streets. However, very limited information exists on the acoustic conditions of
corridors differ from other long enclosures (e.g., underground stations, urban streets)
Overall, findings of long enclosure studies agreed that as the shape of the long enclosures
gets more complicated, sound behavior in these settings also becomes highly
plate shapes.
126
CHAPTER 5
LONG ENCLOSURES
The majority of the previous research has assessed the acoustic qualities of hospital
tools in healthcare acoustics is still not very common. It is probably because healthcare
well as proportional spaces such as patient rooms. Various studies have tested the
spaces such as concert halls and long enclosures with simple geometries (Kang 2002; Li,
and Lu, 2004; Li, and Lu, 2005; Yang, and Sheild, 2004). However, the number of
limited. This study tested the effectiveness of an acoustic modeling program that uses
Accurate simulation of sound behavior is highly complex. Two key factors necessary for
accuracy of acoustic predictions are: a) the overall prediction method adopted by the
acoustic modeling developers, and b) modeling settings selected by the user. Appendix I
modeling settings and the findings of studies assessing the validity of computerized
127
5.1 EMPIRICAL STUDY 1
connected spaces. Even though the use of an acoustic simulation program with hybrid
prediction program has been validated for proportional spaces, its use for complex non-
proportional spaces has not been validated. To assess the effectiveness of CATT acoustic
studies in the actual inter-connected corridors. Previous study findings agree that ISM
(Li, and Lu, 2005) and ray tracing methods (Yang, and Sheild, 2004) perform well in
predicting the acoustics of long spaces. Hybrid methods incorporate the best features of
ISM and ray tracing methods. However, the use of hybrid methods for acoustic analysis
of long spaces has not been validated yet. In addition, particularly in complex settings,
diffraction is one of the important factors that contribute to sound quality as well as
reflection and absorption. However, not many acoustic simulation programs are capable
diffraction from edge diffusion. The effectiveness of this feature and some other useful
CATT features has not been tested yet in this context. CATT also offers an alternative
prediction method other than corrected tail ray tracing method (RTC) to handle the late
measured (in situ impulse response) results, this study assessed the effectiveness of
128
5.1.1 L-shaped corridors
The first set of impulse response measurements has taken place in an L-shaped corridor.
This corridor is located in an educational building from the 1960s on the Georgia Institute
of Technology campus. The total volume of the L-shaped corridor is 397 m3 (14,019ft3).
Its dimensional properties are as follows: height: 2.7m (8.8ft); width: 2.4m (7.8ft) and
length: 60m (197ft). In total, the L-shaped corridor includes 31 doors and 18 windows.
Figure 5.1 shows the location of the sound source and the receivers. Related with the
focus of this study, the measurements have taken place only in the non-visual sound
fields of the corridors. This study examines the association between design and acoustics.
However, in the visual sound field, direct sound significantly affects the acoustic
outcomes. To isolate the interfering effects of direct sound, this study only focuses on the
The same space is modeled with the use of CATT-Acoustics simulation program. Similar
materials are assigned to enclosure surfaces and receivers are located at identical
129
locations. Table5.1 shows the absorption qualities of materials assigned to the enclosure
surfaces. Two types of ceiling tiles are used. The one represented as “medium
coefficients are also used for the ceiling tiles represented as “low absorption” in Table5.1.
The reason is that majority of the educational settings visited in this pilot study are more
than 40 years old, damaged in some places, and more than likely they are not performing
Room-averaged predicted and measured reverberation time results across frequency are
shown in Figure 5.2. When idealized low absorption coefficients are considered, the
difference between measured and predicted results is lower. The “just noticeable
difference”-(JND) for the room averaged reverberation time levels range between 0% and
20% as shown in Tables 5.2 and 5.3. According to ISO 3382 the differences as low as 5%
are perceptible, but other sources show JNDs as high as 39% are perceptible (Meng,
Zhao, & He, 2006). Appendix J contains additional information about JND for
130
reverberation time. Regardless, the study findings are parallel overall to the findings of
previous research. For example, the JND results are quite similar to those reported by
Kang (2002) where he later used the validated acoustic simulation program for several
theoretical studies conducted in long enclosures. Therefore, the findings of this section
reverberation time outcomes for L-shaped corridors. However, it needs to be noted that
this validation analysis only took into account non-visual sound field.
Figure 5.2 Room averaged measured vs. predicted reverberation time in an L-shaped
corridor
Table 5.2 Just noticeable difference values for room averaged reverberation time (non-
visual sound field)
131
Figure 5.3 Measured vs. predicted reverberation times at the receiver location 164ft
(50m) away from the sound source
Table 5.3 Just noticeable difference values for reverberation times measured at 164ft
(50m) away from the source
The second set of impulse response measurements has taken place in a complex inter-
connected corridor system with a race track layout design. This corridor is located in a
different educational building from the 1960s era on the Georgia Institute of Technology
campus. The total volume of the racetrack design corridor system is 895 m3 (31,606ft3).
Its dimensional properties are as follows: height: 2.5m (8.2ft); width: 1.8-2.5 m (5.9ft-
8.2ft) and total length: 173m (567ft). In total, this race track corridor includes 48 doors
132
Figure 5.4 3D-CATT model and floor-plan of a race track corridor
The same space is modeled with the use of CATT-Acoustics simulation program. Similar
materials are assigned to enclosure surfaces and receivers are located at identical
locations. Table 5.4 shows the absorption coefficients of the materials assigned to
133
Table 5.4 Material absorption coefficients of the race track design corridor
The room-averaged predicted and measured reverberation time values across frequencies
are shown in Figure 5.5. Similar to previous validation analysis, only sound behavior in
the non-visual sound field is considered. JND for the room averaged reverberation times
mostly range between 2% and 20% with a few exceptions (Table 5.5). Again based on
the previous study findings, the difference between predicted and measured results is
134
Figure 5.5 Room averaged measured vs. predicted reverberation times in a race track
design corridor
135
Table 5.5 Just noticeable difference values for room averaged reverberation time (non-
visual field)
Figure 5.6 Measured vs. predicted reverberation times at the selected receiver locations in
a race track design corridor
136
Figure 5.6 continued
Table 5.6 Just noticeable difference values for reverberation times at the selected receiver
locations (non-visual field)
137
5.1.3 T-Shaped Corridor
Third set of impulse response measurements took place in another complex inter-
connected corridor system with a T-shaped layout design. This corridor is located in a
different educational building from 1960s era on the Georgia Institute of Technology
campus. The total volume of the corridor system with T-shaped floor-plated shape is
900m3 (31,783ft3). Its dimensional properties are as following: height: 2.4m (7.8ft);
width: 2.3m (7.5ft) and total length: 111m (366ft). In total, the T-shaped corridor
includes 33 doors and 19 small and 3 big windows from as shown in Figure 5.7.
The same space is modeled with the use of CATT-Acoustics simulation program.
Similar materials are assigned to enclosure surfaces and receivers are located at identical
locations. Table 5.7 shows the absorption coefficients of the materials assigned to
Ceiling Acoustic ceiling tile 0.39 0.39 0.39 0.39 0.39 20%
(low absorption)
Acoustic ceiling tile- 0.33 0.54 0.69 0.69 0.56 20%
(medium absorption)
Floor Vinyl on concrete 0.03 0.03 0.03 0.03 0.02 10%
Room averaged predicted and measured acoustic outcomes across frequencies are shown
in the graphs below (Figure 5.8). JND values for room averaged RT30 levels range
between 6% and 23% (Table 5.8). Figure 5.9 shows the measured and predicted RT30
levels at the selected receiver locations. JND values for RT30 levels at the selected
receiver locations ranged between 3% and 24% (Table 5.9). Similar to the previous
analysis, based on the previous study findings, the difference between predicted and
measured results is small enough to suggest the potential effectiveness of hybrid method
139
Figure 5.8 Room averaged measured vs. predicted reverberation times in a T-shaped
corridor
Table 5.8 Just noticeable difference values for room averaged predicted and measured
reverberation times (non-visual field).
140
Figure 5.9 Measured vs. predicted reverberation times at the selected receiver locations in
a T-shaped corridor
141
Figure 5.9 continued
Table 5.9 Just noticeable difference values for reverberation times at the selected receiver
locations (non-visual field)
142
5.1.4 Conclusion
This part of the study has tested the effectiveness of CATT in estimating the acoustics of
interconnected corridors. Related with the goals of this study, the measurements only
have taken place in the non-visual sound field. Based on the jnd thresholds suggested by
ISO 3382 standards, the differences between predicted and measured results are
perceptible (i.e., 5% or greater). However, this is not a surprise when the findings of
previous validation studies are considered. Typically the differences between measured
and predicted RT30 results reported by other computer modeling studies are above the
jnd thresholds suggested by ISO 3382. As the findings from this empirical study are
parallel overall to the findings of previous research, this study suggests the potential
qualities of long enclosures via Empirical Study 1, the other goal of this chapter is to
understand the behavior of sound in long enclosures. Empirical Study 2 analyzes the
differences between the diffuse sound fields of single and interconnected corridors via
measurements.
143
5.2.1 Methodology
Impulse response measurements have taken place in two different settings: a single
corridor and a T-shaped corridor. Both settings are located in educational buildings on the
Georgia Tech campus and had similar surface materials such as vinyl flooring, ceiling
tiles, wood doors and concrete walls. Figure 5.10 shows the interior of the single corridor
144
5.2.2 Findings
Figure 5.12 below shows the sound behavior in three corridor segments located in the
two different corridor settings. The first corridor setting has a rectangular shape floor-
shown in the graphs included in Figure 5.12, reverberation times are not always the same
at different locations of the two long enclosures. In the rectangular corridor, reverberation
time values followed similar trends across frequency, where there is a trend for them to
linearly increase with increasing distance from the sound source. However the sound
behavior is more complex in the inter-connected hallways of the T-shape corridor. Thus,
the behavior of sound in the non-visual-sound field (a.k.a. absence of direct sound) of the
T-shaped corridor is more complex compared to the sound behavior in the visual-sound
145
Measured RT30 Levels along the Corridor Length
Non-visual sound field Measured RT30 Levels along the Corridor Length (Non‐Visual)
Figure 5.12 Measured reverberation times for the receivers located in the visual and non-
visual fields of two different corridor settings with rectangle and T shaped-floor-plates
146
5.2.3 Conclusion
This study has conducted field measurements and acoustic simulations (1) to test the
analyze the behavior of the sound in the single and inter-connected corridors.
In Empirical Study 1, usually the CATT- predicted results are within 5-22% accuracy,
with a few exceptions. Even though predicted results are likely perceptibly different from
the measured results, the agreement between them can still be considered acceptable
Overall, the findings of the validation studies conducted in this part of the study suggest
the potential positive impact of CATT features such as hybrid prediction method,
automatic edge function and diffuse reflections on the accuracy of the predicted RT30
outcomes particularly for the non-visual sound field of the long enclosures. Different
from the visual sound field, in the non-visual sound field there is no direct visual
In Empirical Study 2, the spatial distribution of reverberation times in the single and
single corridor, the trend of the data set is approximately linear for all frequencies.
Similar to previous study findings, RT30 levels increases with increasing distance. This
single corridors. Different from the single corridor case, the trend of the data sets
recorded in the T-shaped corridor is not linear. In particular, the shape of the data set
147
trend lines in the non-visual sound field of the T-shaped corridor is more complex
compared to that in the visual sound field. This can be explained by the dominant effects
of the reflections in the non-visual sound field. To further assess the impact of different
design factors on reverberation time values in addition to distance, this study also
conducts more controlled follow up studies The results are discussed in the following
Chapters.
148
CHAPTER 6
Field study findings conducted in the previous chapters indicate the potential significant
impact of design features on the acoustic qualities of healthcare settings and highlight the
necessity of conducting more controlled studies. Controlled studies can enable the
analysis of the impact of particular design features on the acoustic qualities of healthcare
settings while controlling for other interfering factors. Chapter 6 systematically explores
the association between design and acoustics of interconnected nursing unit corridors
6.1 BACKGROUND
Auditory patient monitoring is one of the key nurse tasks which might have life-death
consequences. Preliminary study findings discussed in section 3.2.3 indicate that nurses
monitor auditory cues at different locations including patient rooms, corridors, central
and de-central nurse stations in the critical care units. In the patient rooms, assessment of
auditory cues is supported with visual cues. Moreover, the nurses are very close to the
sound source and receive direct sound which is one of the critical factors for effective
auditory monitoring. However when they are mobile walking in the corridors, they most
times do not receive direct sound. Therefore, conducting auditory monitoring in the
149
Figure 6.1 shows the distribution of critical sounds, interfering noise sources, active (i.e.,
physician and nurse) and passive listeners (i.e., patient and visitors) in two ICUs. In these
units one nurse is typically responsible for two patients in the unit. In these sound maps,
lines are used to connect active listeners and the critical sound events that they monitor.
Particularly in the ICUs and nursing units, caregivers are usually mobile in the corridors
either dealing with other tasks or exchanging information with other caregivers.
Moreover, in most hospitals, the local sound environments of nurse stations (central and
de-central) are most times directly connected to the sound environments of corridors.
Therefore, acoustic qualities of corridors also potentially affect the nurses` auditory
150
Red: noise sources
Yellow: Passive listener (i.e. patient and visitors)
Blue: Active listeners (i.e. caregivers)
Green: Critical sounds (i.e. medical alarms)
Figure 6.1 Spatial distribution of sound and noise source in the Neuro-ICU and MedSurg-
ICU
of auditory cues. As described before, reverberation time represents the rate of the sound
decay. Extended sound decay increases the length of reverberation time and masks the
typical speech stream as the sound of a syllable decays, it tends to mask the sound of the
subsequent syllable. The masking effects of two rooms with different reverberant
qualities are shown in Figure 6.2. In the more reverberant room (RT30=2s) where the
decay is slower, more sound is masked therefore speech intelligibility is lower. A hospital
study also indicated the positive impact of reduced reverberation time values on speech
151
intelligibility levels and improved psychosocial work environment reported by nurses
Figure 6.2 A speech sound that is masked differently in two rooms with different
reverberation (from Mehta et al., 1997)
described in Section 3.1.3, the human auditory system uses different cues when localizing
sounds such as inter-aural differences in arrival time and intensity and spectral cues.
However, late reflections caused by the boundary surfaces of rooms can result in
dramatic physical changes to the sound waves such as sound energy and direction. “Late”
reflections arrive later than the direct sound and early reflections. When late reflections
dominate the direct sound and useful early reflections, and they negatively affect the
sound localization (Litovsky, Colburn, Yost, & Guzman, 1999; Blauert, 1997).
Therefore, the human auditory system mainly benefits from the first arriving energy
al., 1949). In cases where there is no direct sound such as in non-visual sound fields, the
human auditory system depends only on the early arriving energy. Therefore, particularly
152
in the non-visual sound field the impact of late reflections on sound localization can be
highly detrimental.
Overall, high reverberation times can negatively affect sound tasks conducted by
caregivers in the care settings. To assess task supportive design features of care settings,
this research conducted more controlled studies and examined the association between
different design features and reverberation time. The results of these studies are
6.1.3.1 Scope
such as corridors, patient rooms, public and private visitor areas, staff work areas and
lounges and medical care support areas. As discussed in Section 2, this study focuses on
considerable amount of their time in the corridors while navigating between spaces. They
also conduct critical sound tasks in the corridors including conducting critical medical
conversations (e.g., patient care management, medication dosage and medical procedure)
and localization and assessment of critical sounds. Poorly designed corridor settings can
potentially aggravate the difficulty that caregivers experience while conducting critical
sound tasks. In this part of the study, design characteristics of corridor settings located in
17 actual nursing units from hospitals located around the United States are analyzed.
153
6.1.3.2 Overall Corridor Design
Corridors in nursing units connect different types of spaces such as patient rooms, nurse
stations and care support areas. As shown in Figure 6.3, the total length of the corridors
connecting different spaces in the entire nursing unit range between 87m (285ft) and
connected hallways forming complex floor-plate shapes as shown in Figure 6.4. The
nursing unit in Paimio Hospital and Bellevue Hospital consist of two intersecting
hallways. In the rest of the nursing units, the number of hallways ranged between 3 and
17.
Figure 6.3 Total length of the nursing unit corridors in different hospitals, expressed as
number of hallways
154
Figure 6.4 Floor plans of nursing units located in different hospitals (Floor-plans of the
hospitals were collected by a group of PhD students including myself as a part of the
Hospital Typology Class led by Dr John Peponis, Dr Sonit Bafna and Dr Craig Zimring)-
All drawings are in the same scale
155
Figure 6.4 (continued)
156
Except for the nursing units of Paimio Hospital and Bellevue Hospital, all nursing units
shown in Figure 6.4 consist of multiple patient-care clusters. Related with care
management and infection control purposes, typically patient-care clusters and their
corridors are segregated from other patient-care clusters with operable doors. The result
is that each patient-care cluster operates as a unique sound environment. This study
unit patient-care cluster corridors segregated with doors. A total of 43 different types of
157
Figure 6.5 (continued)
The number of segregated patient-care corridors with different floor-plate shapes varies
among different nursing units and ranges between 1 and 6 as shown in Figure 6.6.
158
Figure 6.6 Number of clustered corridors in the nursing units
Spatial organization of spaces on the nursing unit layouts has been classified in different
categories by James and Tatton-Brown (1986) based on the examination of more than 60
hospital designs (Figure 6.7). These layout design categories are nightingale, corridor or
cluster and radial type. The logic behind the formation of these layout configurations is
outside the scope of this study. However it needs to be noted that each of these layout
of nurse stations around clusters of patient rooms. Detailed information about these
layouts can be obtained from the following sources: James and Tatton-Brown (1986);
Rashid (2006).
159
Figure 6.7 Nursing unit layout types (from James, & Tatton-Brown, 1986)
Based on their influence on sound propagation, the floor-plate shapes of the 44 different
clustered corridors shown in Figure 6.5 can be grouped in two categories: race track
design and tree-like design. In a race track design, corridors surround the centralized care
160
support areas and make a loop around those care support areas. On the other hand a tree-
like design includes straight, segmented or intersecting corridors. As shown in Figure 6.8,
in a race track design corridor setting sound propagates in two major directions away
from a source. On the other hand in a tree-like design corridor, sound propagates in one
major direction.
Figure 6.8 Sound propagation in race track and tree-like design corridors (a) Race track
design hallways (b) tree-like design corridor (Blue dot represents the sound source and
arrows represent the sound rays)
Tree-like design corridors can have a variety of different floor-plate shapes including I,
T, L, cross and different combinations of I, L and T. Race track design corridors can also
have various floor-plate shapes including rectangular, triangular and circular floor-plate
shapes. However, race track design with rectangular floor-plate shapes is the most
common. Moreover, in many cases parallel hallways in these rectangular floor-plates are
Overall, the width of the corridors (including both tree-like design and race track design)
varies between 2m (6.5ft) to 4.7m (15.4ft) while usually it is approximately 2.7m (8.8ft).
The total length of the corridors composed of one hallway ranges between 8m (26ft) and
161
34m (111ft) as shown in Figure 6.9. However, usually the corridor clusters are composed
of multiple hallways. The total length of the corridor clusters with multiple hallways
ranges between 30m (98ft) and 210m (689ft). Except Bellevue and Paimio, the length of
each individual hallway composing these corridor clusters mostly ranges between 15m
162
6.2 PILOT STUDY
EDUCATIONAL CORRIDORS
6.2.1 Scope
In sufficiently diffuse sound fields, reverberation time is primarily a function of two room
parameters: room volume and absorption. In non-diffuse sound fields such as long
enclosures, there are different design factors that have been found to have impact on
reverberation time values such as size and shape of the cross section, distribution of
reverberation time has not yet been systematically investigated. To statistically assess the
actual educational settings. The other goal of this pilot study is to provide input for the
more controlled acoustic simulation studies that follow. In this pilot study, design
6.2.2 Methodology
shapes of these interconnected corridors are shown in Figure 6.10. Each inter-connected
corridor is composed of several hallways and the length of the hallways ranged between
163
15m (49ft) to 45m (148ft). The red dot placed in the corridor drawings is used to
represent sound source locations and lines are used to illustrate the paths where receivers
are systematically distributed 5m (16ft) apart from each other. Color code is applied to
represent the receivers located at different hallways that are 1 and 2 turns away from the
sound source.
6.10 are correlated with mean and standard deviation of reverberation time values
measured in the same hallway. Some of the design variables considered in this analysis
corridor), number of turns and radial distance from the source. The effects of these design
variables on reverberation time values are assessed while controlling for other design
factors such as material and volume. The acoustic measure considered in the analysis is
mean reverberation time values. Mean values (represented as “RT-Mean” in Table 6.1)
represent the overall/averaged reverberation time values in each hallway. The mid
frequency range (500Hz and 1 kHz) is considered in particular because of its significant
164
Figure 6.10 Plans of the corridors located in the educational buildings where impulse
response measurements were conducted
In this study, the design variable “number of turns” represents the order of the visibility
zones from the sound source location. For example the hallway/zone visible from the
sound source location is considered as “0-order visibility zone” and the hallway/zone
visibility zone”. Similarly, the hallway/zone perpendicularly connected to this 1st order
visibility zone is considered as “2nd order visibility zone”. The zone visible from the
sound source is referred as “visual field” in the following sections of this work. Receivers
located in the hallways where they do not have any visual connection with the sound
source (a.k.a. lack of direct sound energy) referred as the “non-visual sound field” are
165
found to experience longer reverberation times. The “critical distance” in all educational
corridor settings ranges between 5m (16.4ft) and 15m (49ft). Critical distance is the
distance where both the energy of the direct and reflected sound is equal. Conducting
acoustic measurements outside the critical distance is important to avoid the effects of
direct sound on acoustic outcomes. In this research, all impulse response measurements
6.2.3 Findings
Partial correlation analysis results presented in Table 6.1 indicate that when other
interfering design variables are controlled (total volume, average distance, corridor
length, number of branches and material), the number of turns design variable is
significantly and positively correlated with mean reverberation time values. In other
words, receivers located at the hallways where they have a visual connection with the
sound source (a.k.a. presence of direct sound energy) are found to experience lower
reverberation times. Compared to receivers located in the non-visual zones with lower
visibility order, receivers located in the non-visual zones with higher visibility order are
also found to experience longer reverberation time. It needs to be noted that these
interpretations are limited to 1 and 2 turns because of the characteristics of the hallways
166
Table 6.1 Partial correlation analysis results linking corridor design variables, mean of
reverberation times (RT-Mean) and standard deviation of reverberation times (RT-SD)
RT-Mean
Independent corridor Control Frequency Partial
design variable Variables correlation
coefficient
Number of branches
500Hz 0.60**
Number of turns
Corridor length Material
Avg. distance
Volume 1kHz 0.50**
Corridor length
500Hz - 0.35
Number of turns
Number of branches Material
Avg. distance
Volume 1kHz - 0.30
Corridor length
500Hz 0.60**
Number of branches
Number of turns Material
Avg. distance
Volume 1kHz 0.70**
* When controlled for the other variables, the correlation b/w variables is statistically significant at 0.1 level (p<0.1)
** When controlled for the other variables, the correlation b/w variables is statistically significant at 0.05 level (p<0.05)
The design variable number of branches is used to represent the number of hallways
interfering design variables are controlled (total volume, average distance, corridor
length, number of turns and material), the number of branches design variable is
negatively but not significantly correlated with mean reverberation time. In other words,
this finding suggests that in the hallways with higher number of branches, reverberation
time values are expected to be lower. The impact of corridor length on reverberation
time values is also investigated with partial correlation analysis. The length of the
167
time values when other interfering design variables were controlled (total volume,
average distance, number of turns, number of branches and material). This finding
reverberation time value in the particular hallway are also expected to increase. The
results of this analysis also agreed with the earlier sound behavior analysis results. As
shown in Figure 5.12, reverberation time tends to increase with increasing hallway
length.
Another partial correlation analysis is conducted to assess the impact of radial distance on
reverberation time values. This analysis is partially different from the others because the
mean reverberation time values are not used this time. Instead, reverberation time values
measured at each receiver location are directly correlated with radial distance (direct
distance instead of walking distance) between the sound source and the receiver location.
As shown in Table 6.2, when volume and material are controlled for, radial distance is
distances. This finding suggests that receivers located at positions with longer radial
distance from the sound source are expected to experience longer reverberation time
168
Table 6.2 Partial correlation analysis results between corridor design variables of radial
distance and reverberation time at discrete distances.
500Hz 0.56**
Radial distance Material
Volume 1kHz 0.50*
* When controlled for the other variables, the correlation b/w variables is statistically significant at 0.1 level (p<0.1)
** When controlled for the other variables, the correlation b/w variables is statistically significant at 0.05 level (p<0.05)
Overall, the findings of the pilot study analysis indicate the potential effects of floor-plate
design features on the acoustic qualities of corridor sound environments. In the following
sections, these findings are followed up with more detailed and controlled simulation
studies.
CORRIDOR MODELS
6.3.1 Scope
The pilot study introduced in previous section has statistically investigated the
relationship between reverberation time and design variables of real life corridors with
complex floor-plate shapes. Some of these design features such as number of turns and
corridor length are found to be significantly associated with mean reverberation time
values. These interpretations are based on field measurements conducted in the actual
settings. In this part of the study, a more controlled follow up study is conducted to
enable the systematic analysis of the relationship between design and acoustic
169
characteristics of hallways located only in the non-visual sound fields of corridors with
complex floor-plate shapes. The motivation for focusing on non-visual sound fields is to
control for the interfering effects of direct sound present in the visual-sound field on
acoustic outcomes.
6.3.2 Methodology
theoretical models are generated as shown in Figure 6.11. The total lengths of the
corridors composed of multiple hallways range between 30m (98ft) to 215m (705ft). The
length of each individual hallway included in these theoretical models range between
15m (49ft) and 40m (131ft). Among all theoretical models, corridor width (3m/9.8ft) and
Several layers of grouping are used to group the 60 theoretical models to control for the
effects of interfering design factors, as shown in Figure 6.11. The theoretical models are
first grouped based on their layout design types: tree-like design and race track design.
For each layout design type, 30 different theoretical models are generated. Theoretical
models in each group are further grouped based on number of turns. In addition to layout
design type, controlling for number of turns is necessary because Phase 1 pilot study
findings have indicated the significant association between this corridor design variable
and reverberation time values. The red dots in Figure 6.11 represent a theoretical sound
source. The hallways of the theoretical models that are 1 turn and 2 turns away from the
sound source are highlighted with blue and red lines respectively. In total, 15 different
170
theoretical models are generated for each number of turns category. Those models are
further grouped under three categories to allow systematic study of different design
Acoustic qualities of these models are analyzed with CATT acoustic simulation program.
The sound source is located 5m (16ft) away from one of the end walls. Multiple receivers
are located 5m (16ft) apart from each other along the hallways highlighted with red or
blue lines. Two different acoustic analyses are conducted to assess the impact of two
levels of absorption on the association between the design and acoustic variables. At first,
all surfaces are assigned a material with 0.1 absorption coefficient (). Second, all
surfaces are assigned a material with 0.3. These particular absorption coefficients are
chosen because they represent room averaged absorption coefficients of hospital settings
installed with lower and higher performance acoustic ceiling tiles. In total, 120 different
171
Test cases #1: tree-like design | 1 turn
Figure 6.11 Plans of theoretical models grouped based layout type, number of turns,
number of branches, branch distance and corridor length criteria.
172
Figure 6.11 (continued)
173
Figure 6.12 3D-CATT models of theoretical tree-like design hallways
174
Figure 6.12 (continued)
175
Figure 6.12 (continued)
176
Figure 6.13 3D-CATT models of theoretical race track design hallways
177
Figure 6.13 (continued)
178
Figure 6.13 (continued)
179
6.3.3 Findings
First, 8 different partial correlation analyses are conducted. For each analysis, 15
theoretical models are considered. Design characteristics of each sample group are
Number of Number of
Layout design Surface material
turns theoretical models
Tree-like design Absorption coefficient=0.3 1 turn 15 cases
Tree-like design Absorption coefficient=0.3 2 turns 15 cases
Tree-like design Absorption coefficient=0.1 1 turn 15 cases
Tree-like design Absorption coefficient=0.1 2 turns 15 cases
Race track design Absorption coefficient=0.3 1 turn 15 cases
Race track design Absorption coefficient=0.3 2 turns 15 cases
Race track design Absorption coefficient=0.1 1 turn 15 cases
Race track design Absorption coefficient=0.1 2 turns 15 cases
Overall, findings of this follow up study agree with the earlier findings of the pilot study
introduced in Section 6.2. In the first analysis, all theoretical models are applied with
absorption coefficient = 0.1 and in the second analysis all theoretical models are applied
with absorption coefficient = 0.3. As shown in Table 6.4-Table 6.5, corridor length is
positively and significantly correlated with mean (averaged) reverberation time values.
This finding suggests that overall receivers in the longer hallways are likely to experience
correlated with the mean reverberation time values. This finding suggest that as the
180
number of hallways (a.k.a. branch) connected to a particular core hallway increase,
overall reverberation time values in that particular core hallway are expected to decrease.
Another analysis is conducted to assess whether the way hallways are connected to a
particular hallway affect reverberation time values though the variable branch distance.
The findings indicate that the distance between the branch and the closest hallway
parallel to it is positively correlated with mean reverberation time values. However these
correlations are not statistically significant. Consistent with the pilot study findings,
number of turns is also positively and significantly correlated with mean reverberation
time values when controlled for interfering design factors. This finding suggests that as
the receivers navigate to corridors with higher number turns from the sound source, they
Based on beta values included in Table 6.4 and Table 6.5, the effect size of the number of
turns variable is the highest compared to other design variables including corridor length,
number of branches and branch distance. The interpretation is that one unit change in
number of turns will potentially lead to more increase/decrease in RT30 compared to one
unit change in corridor length, number of branches and branch distance. The corridor
length variable has the second highest effects size. The interpretation is that one unit
compared to one unit change in number of branches and branch distance. Number of
branches variable has the third highest effect size on the mean RT30.The interpretation is
181
that one unit change in corridor length will potentially lead to more increase/decrease in
Based on beta values represented in Table 6.4 and Table 6.5, changes in RT30 associated
with one unit change in the number of turns, corridor length, number of branches and
changes in RT30.
Table 6.4 Partial correlation analysis results (showing the strength of the relationship
between different corridor design variables and reverberation time values in hallways
with absorption coefficient = 0.3 surface materials. The sample group consisted of 60
theoretical models)
182
Table 6.5 Partial correlation analysis results (showing the strength of the relationship
between different design variables and reverberation time values in hallways with
absorption coefficient = 0.1 surface materials. The sample group consisted of 60
theoretical models)
183
6.4 EMPIRICAL STUDY 2
6.4.1 Scope
etc. Instead of design qualities of particular hallways, this part of the study focuses on the
effects of overall floor-plate shape qualities of nursing unit corridors by using two new
6.4.2 Methodology
The two new design metrics introduced are used to quantify shape characteristics of
corridor floor-plates: visual fragmentation (VF) and relative grid distance (RGD)
(Shpuza, & Peponis, 2008). Floor-plate design metrics introduced in Chapter 6.3 are used
to quantify design qualities of particular hallways in a corridor system. In this section, the
RGD and VF are used to quantify shape qualities of the entire corridor floor-plates.
Relative grid distance (RGD) values represent the degree to which a floor-plate shape
diverges from square with regard to total distances between pairs of locations in the
shape. Square is assigned the smallest RGD value of 1. Higher RGD values indicate
more elongated floor-plate shapes. Visual fragmentation (VF) values reflect the
184
convexity of a floor-plate shape as the amount of directional changes needed to connect
any two locations on the floor-plate. Visual fragmentation values range between 0 and 1.
values indicate more fragmented/jagged floor-plate shapes and those with holes.
Studies exploring the relationship between of floor-plate design and acoustics have
mostly considered simple floor-plate shape metrics like width-to-length ratio. Even
though these metrics have been successfully used for the shape analysis of simple floor-
plate shape geometries, they do not provide a comprehensive picture of the complex
floor-plate shape qualities such as fragmentation and compactness. RGD and VF are
chosen because these two metrics have been successfully used for the quantification of
such as the layout of work stations (Shpuza, & Peponis, 2008). Moreover, the underlying
logic of RGD and VF involves computing distance between grid cells and computing the
number of turns. Whereas these were initially developed because of their potential social
and programmatic impacts, there are in fact the same considerations that impact the
sound environment. One of the correspondences is related with similarities between path
system. RGD calculations take into account every possible distance needed to connect
each cell to all other cells located in the grid system. Interestingly, when sound rays are
emitted by the sound source, they travel different distances till they get to the receiver`s
185
directional change considerations. VF calculations take into account all directional
changes needed to connect any two locations on the grid system. It is also well known
that directional changes (a.k.a. turns) leads to some changes in the behavior of sound.
To explore the relationship between RGD, VF and RT30, 20 different theoretical design
models are developed based on the heuristic design analysis of nursing unit corridor
floor-plates (Figure 6.14). As shown in Figure 6.9, the total corridor length of nursing
unit corridor clusters mainly range between 30m to 120m. Mid range total corridor length
value of 66m/216ft is assigned to all theoretical models. The width of the nursing unit
corridors range between 2m to 4.7m but most times it is about 3m. Therefore, the width
As shown in Figure 6.5, about 2/3rd of the nursing unit corridor clusters are tree-like
design corridors and the rest of them are race track design corridors. In this analysis, all
of the theoretical models are examples of tree-like corridor design. Theoretical models
with more complex floor-plate shapes are purposefully excluded from the sample group;
particularly because RGD and VF values have not varied sufficiently for corridors with
more complex floor-plate shapes. The complexity of floor-plate shapes of the nursing
unit corridor clusters varied. For the same reason, floor-plate shapes of the theoretical
models are also designed to have different levels of geometric complexity. Ten of them
are composed of a single hallway with different floor-plate shapes. The rest of them are
composed of two main hallways connected with another hallway as shown in Figure
6.14. The number of intersecting corridor parts range from 2 to 6. This number is quite
186
similar to the number of the intersecting corridor parts of the nursing unit corridor
RGD and VF shape metrics have been used for the quantification of floor-plate shape
characteristics of office spaces; however, they have not been used for the floor-plate
shape analysis of long enclosures. This study tests the applicability of these two design
metrics for floor-plate shape analysis of long enclosures and analyzes the association
1 2 3 4 5
8 9 10
7
6
11 12 13 14 15
16 17 18 19 20
Figure 6.14 Floor plans of theoretical corridor models with different floor-plate shape
qualities
187
Acoustic characteristics of the theoretical models are analyzed with CATT acoustic
modeling program. A sound source (represented with a red dot) is located at the center of
the connecting corridor as shown in Figure 6.14. In this analysis two considerations are
taken into account to control for the effects of direct sound on reverberation time values.
First, the length of the hallway where the sound source is located is kept the same
(10m/33ft) in all theoretical models. Second, only non-visual sound field characteristics
are reported in the acoustic analysis of these theoretical models. Multiple receivers are
systematically distributed 5m (16.4ft) apart from each other along the highlighted paths
6.4.3 Findings
Two partial correlation analyses are conducted to assess the individual effects of VF and
RGD on mean reverberation time values. The reverberation time of each theoretical
model is represented with a single number. This single number is calculated by averaging
the reverberation time values calculated for each receiver located in the non-visual sound
field. The floor plate design variables of VF and RGD of each theoretical model are also
Qelzie is a Java applet designed for calculating the shape measures of RGD and VF.
According to analysis results as shown in Table 6.6, relative grid distance is positively
and significantly correlated with mean reverberation time values. This finding suggested
that as the floor-plate shape gets more elongated, averaged reverberation time values are
expected to increase. Interestingly, this finding is consistent with earlier findings of this
188
study. Elongated corridors potentially have longer corridors and according to previous
study findings reverberation time values are likely to be higher in the corridors with
reverberation time values. This finding suggests that as the shape gets more fragmented/
jagged, reverberation time values are likely to decrease. This finding can also be expected
based on the earlier study findings. Given the same total corridor length, more
fragmented long enclosures potentially have shorter individual corridors and in corridors
with shorter corridor length, reverberation time values are likely to be lower. Similarly, a
higher number of branches potentially results in more fragmented long enclosures. In the
Linear regression analysis is also conducted to assess the amount of variance caused by
the RGD and VF characteristics of the floor-plate shapes. As shown in Table 6.7, 75% of
the variance in reverberation time values (specifically for 500Hz) is explained with VF
and RGD. The variance accounted for is 65% at 1kHz. For visual assessment of the
correlation between floor-plate metrics and mean reverberation time values, scatter-plots
189
Table 6.6 Partial correlation coefficients showing the strength of the relationship between
floor-plate shape characteristics (RGD + VF) and reverberation time.
190
Figure 6.15 Scatter plots showing the relationship between RGD + VF and RT30 at
500Hz.
191
Figure 6.16 Scatter plots showing the relationship between RGD + VF and RT30 at 1kHz
192
A follow up analysis is conducted to explore the relationship between visual
fragmentation, relative grid distance and RT30. As shown in Table 6.8, bivariate
correlation analysis results indicate that VF and RGD values are not significantly related.
For visual assessment of the correlation between floor-plate shape metrics and mean
reverberation time values, scatter-plots are also generated as shown in Figure 6.17 and
Figure 6.18. In these scatter-plot graphs, two real world hospitals are also included in
addition to 20 theoretical models. Like theoretical models, acoustic qualities of these two
real world hospitals are analyzed with CATT acoustic modeling program. Mean RT30 is
consistently and significantly higher in the two real world hospitals compared to the
theoretical models. This is probably related with significant differences between the total
corridor lengths of the two real world hospitals and theoretical models.
193
Figure 6.17 Scatter plots showing the relationship between RGD, VF and RT30 at 500Hz.
(MedSurg-ICU (VF=0.8, RGD=2.2, RT30= 1.75); Neuro-ICU (VF=1; RGD=2.2,
RT30=1.80)).
194
Figure 6.18 Scatter plots showing the relationship between RGD, VF and RT30 at 1kHz.
(MedSurg-ICU (VF=0.8, RGD=2.2, RT30=1.80); Neuro-ICU (VF=1; RGD=2.2,
RT30=1.90)).
6.5 DISCUSSION
acoustic simulations and statistical analyses. The summary of these empirical study
findings can be found in Table 6.9. More detailed discussion about these findings can be
195
Corridor Design Variable Impact on Reverberation
Increased Number of Turns Increased Mean RT
Increased Corridor Length Increased Mean RT & SD
Increased Number of Branches Decreased Mean RT
Increased Branch Distance Decreased SD
Increased Relative Grid Distance Increased Mean RT
Increased Visual Fragmentation Decreased Mean RT
The main motivation of these empirical studies is to assess how different aspects of
time is one of the critical acoustic measures predicting the effectiveness of critical sound
critical care units, caregivers continuously assess and localize auditory cues. Previous
study findings indicate that the capability of the human auditory system to monitor and
these corridor settings. The pilot study findings suggest the potential significant impact of
design features of particular hallways (e.g., number of turns, corridor length, and number
study, in the hallways that are 2 turns away from the sound source, averaged
reverberation time values are likely to be higher compared to hallways 1 turn away from
the sound source. Moreover, in the longer corridors averaged reverberation time values
196
are likely to be higher compared to shorter corridors. On the other hand, in the hallways
with higher number branches, the reverberation time values are expected be lower
To test the findings of the pilot study, Empirical Study 1 is designed to be a more
controlled follow up study using CATT acoustic simulation program. Based on heuristic
analysis of the existing nursing unit corridors, 80 different theoretical models with
different design implementations are generated and in total 140 acoustic simulations are
conducted. Findings of Empirical Study 1 are quite similar to the pilot study findings. For
example, corridor length of the hallways located in the non-visual sound fields is
According to this analysis, RGD is positively correlated with mean reverberation time
values and VF is negatively correlated with mean reverberation time values. Moreover,
197
CHAPTER 7
VALIDATION STUDY
7.1 SCOPE
The previous section has discussed the findings of the theoretical studies linking design
Chapter 6 track with the study findings based on real-world hospital corridor geometries,
7.2 METHODOLOGY
The design features of the two units are reviewed in detail in Section 2.2.2. Shortly, the
Neurological ICU (Neuro-ICU) is a fairly new 20-bed unit. The Neuro-ICU has a tree-
like design corridor system with long corridors. The total length of the hallways is 200m
(656ft).
The Medical Surgical ICU (MedSurg-ICU) is a 1980s era 20-bed unit. The MedSurg-ICU
has a triangular shape race track design corridor system with shorter corridors. The total
198
In-situ impulse response measurements
Impulse response measurements are conducted to assess the reverberant qualities of the
two ward corridors objectively (Figure 7.1). The impulse response measurements have
taken place at 4 different receiver locations in two hallways in each ward as shown in
Figure 7.2 and Figure 7.3.The preliminary measurements first have taken place in the
signal to noise ratio (SNR) could not be achieved. In the other non-hospital settings, the
SNR can be improved by simply increasing the level of the driving sound signal.
However, ICUs are noise sensitive settings and occupied 24/7. This limits the level of the
driving sound signal for the impulse response measurement. In the MedSurg-ICU and
Neuro-ICU, the critical distances are 3m (9.8ft) and 5m (16.4ft), respectively. At all
times, the receivers are located beyond the critical distance. The distance between source
and receivers are 8m (26ft) and 12m (39ft) in both wards. Detailed information about the
During the measurements, all patient room doors are closed in both wards. All staff
members including the directors of the both wards and the visitors are informed about the
procedure.
199
Figure 7.1 Impulse response measurements in the hospital corridors
200
Figure 7.3 Distribution of source and receiver locations in the MedSurg-ICU
Idealized 3D acoustic models of the two real world ward corridors are also generated.
The models allowed for control of the interfering effects of the differently distributed
materials on the boundary surfaces. Original corridor lengths are assigned to 3D acoustic
models of the two ward corridors. Acoustic simulation analysis of these corridors is
conducted in 6 phases.
In phase 1, the impacts of T and cross shaped corridor intersections on reverberation time
levels are tested (Figure 7.4). The analysis has taken place only in the corridors of the
(S1) is activated and reverberation times at the R1 and R2 locations are recorded. Second,
source 2 (S2) is activated and reverberation times at the R3 and R4 locations are
recorded. The distances between source and receivers are kept the same for both test
201
cases. To represent the acoustic conditions associated with the T shaped intersection,
reverberation times predicted for receivers R1-R2 are averaged. Similarly, predicted
results for receivers R3 and R4 are averaged to represent the acoustic conditions
Figure 7.4 Source and receiver locations considered for the acoustic analysis of “T” and
“cross” shaped intersections in the phase 1 Neuro-ICU simulations
In phase 2, the impact of number of branches on reverberation time is tested. The analysis
has taken place in the corridors of both the Neuro-ICU and MedSurg-ICU (Figure 7.5-
7.6). The receivers R1, R2 and R3 are located in one of the Neuro-ICU corridors with
multiple branches (Figure 7.5); and receivers R3, R4 and R5 are located in one of the
MedSurg-ICU corridors without any branches (Figure 7.6). Predicted RT30 levels for
202
R1-R2-R3 and R4-R5-R6 are averaged to represent the overall acoustic conditions along
Figure 7.5 Distribution of source and receivers along the corridor with multiple branches
in the phase 2 Neuro-ICU simulation
203
Figure 7.6 Distribution of source and receivers along the corridor without any branches in
the phase 2 MedSurg-ICU simulation
In phase 3, the impact of number of turns on reverberation time is tested (Figure 7.7-7.8).
In the Neuro-ICU, the receivers R1 and R2 are located along the corridor that was two
turns away from the sound source (Figure 7.7). In the MedSurg-ICU, the receivers R3
and R4 are located along the corridor that is one turn away from the sound source (Figure
7.8). The distances between source and receivers are kept the same in the Neuro-ICU and
MedSurg-ICU. Predicted results for R1-R2 and R3-R4 are averaged to represent the
204
Figure 7.7 Distribution of source and receivers that are 2 turns away from the source in
the phase 3 Neuro-ICU simulation
Figure 7.8 Distribution of source and receivers that are 1 turn away from the source in the
phase 3 MedSurg-ICU simulation
205
In phase 4, the impact of corridor length on reverberation time is tested (Figure 7.9-7.10).
In the Neuro-ICU, receivers R1 and R2 are placed along the longest corridor (Figure 7.9).
In MedSurg-ICU, receivers R3 and R4 are placed along a shorter corridor (Figure 7.10).
Receivers R1-R2 and R3-R4 are averaged to represent the overall acoustic conditions
Figure 7.9 Distribution of source and receivers along a long corridor in the phase 4
Neuro-ICU simulation
206
Figure 7.10 Distribution of source and receivers along a shorter corridor in the phase 4
MedSurg-ICU simulation
placing the receivers in different corridors of each unit (Figure 7.11-7.12). Visual
fragmentation values for Neuro-ICU and MedSurg-ICU are 1.0 and 0.8, respectively. The
floor-plate shape of the MedSurg-ICU. Relative grid distance value for Neuro-ICU and
MedSurg-ICU is the same (2.2). In other words, floor-plate shapes of the Neuro-ICU and
R4-R5 are averaged to represent the acoustic conditions in the idealized corridors of the
207
Figure 7.11 Distribution of source and receivers in the phase 5 Neuro-ICU corridor
simulation
Figure 7.12 Distribution of source and receivers in the phase 5 MedSurg-ICU corridor
simulation
Finally in phase 6, another analysis is conducted to test the impact of overall corridor
208
the Neuro-ICU floor-plate design is considered instead of the original floor-plate design
used in the previous phase 5 analysis (Figure 7.13). The original design of the MedSurg-
ICU corridors remained unchanged (Figure 7.14). The motivation is to test the impact of
two different corridor designs with similar total corridor length. Visual fragmentation
values for the idealized design of the Neuro-ICU and MedSurg-ICU is 1.0 and 0.8,
respectively. The interpretation is that the idealized floor-plate shape of the Neuro-ICU is
more fragmented compared to the floor-plate shape of the MedSurg-ICU. The relative
grid distance values for the idealized floor-plate shape of the Neuro-ICU and MedSurg-
ICU are 1.9 and 2.2. In other words, the floor-plate shape of the MedSurg-ICU is more
represent the acoustic conditions in the idealized corridors of the Neuro-ICU. Predicted
reverberation time values for R6-R7-R8-R9-R10 are averaged to represent the acoustic
Figure 7.13 Distribution of source and receivers in the phase 6 idealized Neuro-ICU
corridor simulation
209
Figure 7.14 Distribution of source and receivers in the phase 6 MedSurg-ICU corridor
simulation
In all phases, similar materials are applied to the boundary surfaces of the two wards. The
absorption and scattering coefficients of the surface materials are shown in Table 7.1. In
each phase, the same analysis is repeated for two different types of ceiling tiles:
absorption coefficient = 0.1 and absorption coefficient = 0.3. This is necessary to test
how different material applications affect the impact of floor-plate design on reverberant
210
Table 7.1 Absorption coefficients of materials applied to the surfaces of the two wards
7.3 FINDINGS
The averaged measured reverberation times in different corridors of each unit are shown
in Figure 7.15. In the MedSurg-ICU, averaged reverberation times range between 0.44s
and 0.56s across frequency. In one of the corridors reverberation times are as low as
0.30s and the highest was 0.76 s. In the Neuro-ICU corridors, averaged reverberation
times are consistently higher compared to averaged reverberation times in the MedSurg-
ICU (Figure 7.15), even though the Neuro-ICU is installed with absorptive acoustic
ceiling tiles. The reverberation times in the Neuro-ICU range between 0.70 s and 0.87 s.
The lowest reverberation time recorded in the Neuro-ICU corridors is 0.67 s and the
highest was 0.92 s. The difference between the reverberation times measured in two ward
corridors range between 33% and 68%. This result indicates the potential significant
reverberation time. For example, the floor-plate design of the MedSurg-ICU corridor is
more compact with shorter corridors. According to the previous Chapter 6 findings, in the
211
corridor settings with more compact and shorter corridors, reverberation times are likely
to be less.
Figure 7.15 Averaged measured reverberation times in the MedSurg-ICU and Neuro-ICU
corridors
Table 7.2 Just noticeable difference results of measured reverberation times in the two
wards
JND
(impulse 33% 50% 68% 55% 62%
response
measurement)
212
Acoustic Simulation Analysis
the T-shaped corridor intersection compared to the “+” shaped corridor intersection
particularly at 250Hz, 500Hz, 1kHz and 2kHz (Figure 7.16 and Figure 7.17). In the light
of the Chapter 6 findings, this result is expected because compared to a T-shaped corridor
a cross shaped corridor has more branching corridors. In Chapter 6, increased number of
reverberation time.
Figure 7.16 Averaged simulated reverberation times at the phase 1 “T” and “+”shaped
intersections (ceiling tile 0.3)
213
Figure 7.17 Averaged simulated reverberation times at the phase 1 “T” and “+”shaped
intersections (ceiling tile 0.1)
Table 7.3 JND results of simulated reverberation times at the phase 1 “T” and “+”shaped
intersections
JND
ceiling tile 12% 13% 10% 10% 8%
0.3
JND
ceiling tile 14% 15% 9% 8% 9%
0.1
In the Neuro-ICU corridor with multiple branches, averaged reverberation times are
lower compared to the reverberation times in the MedSurg-ICU corridor without any
branches. Similar to the phase 1 previous analysis, the difference is not as significant at
214
500Hz and 1kHz (Figure 7.18 and Figure 7.19). This result is expected because in
Figure 7.18 Averaged simulated reverberation times at the phase 2 corridors with
different number of branches (ceiling tile 0.3)
Figure 7.19 Averaged simulated reverberation times at the corridors with different
number of branches (ceiling tile 0.1)
215
Table 7.4 Just noticeable difference results of simulated reverberation times at the
corridors with different number of branches
JND
ceiling tile 10% 12% 11% 4% 7%
( 0.3)
JND
ceiling tile 11% 13% 13% 9% 7%
( 0.1)
The averaged reverberation times are higher in the corridor that is 2 turns away from the
sound source (Neuro-ICU) compared to the reverberation times in the corridor that is 1
turn away from the source (MedSurg-ICU). The difference is higher at lower frequencies
including 250Hz, 500Hz and 1kHz (Figure 7.20 and Figure 7.21). This result is expected
positively correlated with reverberation time. However it is also possible that slightly
216
Figure 7.20 Averaged simulated reverberation times at the phase 3corridors that are 1
turn and 2 turns away from the source (ceiling tile 0.3)
Figure 7.21 Averaged simulated reverberation times at the phase 3 corridors that are 1
turn and 2 turns away from the source (ceiling tile 0.1)
217
Table 7.5 Just noticeable difference results of simulated reverberation times at the phase
3 corridors that are 1 and 2 turns away from the sound source
JND
ceiling tile 11% 13% 10% 6% 7%
0.3)
JND
ceiling tile 18% 16% 12% 5% 6%
( 0.1)
The averaged reverberation time is longer in the longer corridor of the Neuro-ICU
compared to the averaged reverberation time in the shorter corridor of the MedSurg-ICU.
In particular, the difference is higher at lower frequencies including 250Hz, 500Hz and 1
kHz (Figure 7.22 and Figure 7.23). This result is expected because in Chapter 6,
increased corridor length is found to be significantly and positively correlated with the
reverberation time. However it is also possible that slightly larger volume of Neuro-ICU
218
Figure 7.22 Averaged reverberation times in the phase 4 corridors with different corridor
length (ceiling tile 0.3)
Figure 7.23 Averaged reverberation times in the phase 4 corridors with different corridor
length (ceiling tile 0.1)
219
Table 7.6 Just noticeable difference results of simulated reverberation times at the phase
4corridors with different corridor length
JND
ceiling tile 12% 11% 13% 9% 6%
0.3
JND
ceiling tile 13% 16% 15% 9% 10%
0.1
Even though, the Neuro-ICU has a more fragmented floor plate shape, the mean of the
reverberation times both measured and predicted in different corridors of the Neuro-ICU
are higher compared to the MedSurg-ICU. The relative grid distance values of the two
unit floor-plate shapes are similar. On the other hand, the total corridor length of the
significant impact of total corridor length on the mean RT30 of the two unit corridors.
Consistent with previous analysis, the difference is higher at lower frequencies including
220
Figure 7.24 Averaged simulated reverberation times in the phase 5 corridors with
different total corridor length (ceiling tile 0.3)
Figure 7.25 Averaged simulated reverberation times in the phase 5 corridors with
different total corridor length (ceiling tile 0.1)
221
Table 7.7 Just noticeable difference results of simulated reverberation times at the phase
5 corridors of two wards with different total corridor length
JND
ceiling tile 11% 13% 14% 7% 9%
0.3)
JND
ceiling tile 15% 18% 17% 8% 6%
0.1)
Different from previous analysis, the mean of the predicted reverberation time in the
idealized corridors of the Neuro-ICU is shorter compared to the mean reverberation times
frequencies including 250Hz, 500Hz and 1 kHz (Figure 7.26 and Figure 7.27). These
Chapter 6) between floor-plate shape qualities and mean RT30. Idealized floor-plate
shape of the Neuro-ICU is more fragmented and less elongated compared to the original
222
Figure 7.26 Averaged simulated reverberation times in the phase 6 corridors with similar
length and different shape (ceiling tile 0.3)
Figure 7.27 Averaged simulated reverberation times in the phase 6 corridors with similar
length and different shape (ceiling tile 0.1)
223
Table 7.8 Just noticeable difference results of simulated reverberation times at the phase
6 corridors of two wards with similar total corridor length
JND
ceiling tile 12% 14% 10% 9% 8%
0.3)
JND
ceiling tile 14% 17% 13% 12% 6%
0.1)
7.4 DISCUSSION
qualities of two real world hospital ward corridors are analyzed in Chapter 7 by
studies.
Interestingly, even though the Neuro-ICU is installed with absorptive ceiling tiles,
MedSurg-ICU. This suggests the potential significant impact of design features on RT30
has a more compact layout with shorter corridors and thus a shorter reverberation time.
corridor design features on RT30 levels is tested including corridor length, number of
224
branches, number of turns and overall design (race track vs. tree-like design) by
Overall, the differences between the simulated averaged reverberation times measured in
different corridors of the two wards are higher at lower frequencies including 250Hz,
500Hz and 1 kHz. At all times, the simulation study findings agree with the findings of
Chapter 6. For example, the means of the predicted reverberation times in the corridors
with longer corridor length, lower number of branches and higher number of turns from
the sound source are higher. However, it is possible that slightly larger volume of Neuro-
ICU might also be contributing to the larger RT30. In addition, (given two simulated unit
corridors have similar total corridor length, width and height, and equal absorption
treatment were applied), averaged reverberation times are higher in the more race track
design unit (MedSurg-ICU) compared to the more fragmented and more compact
idealized Neuro-ICU corridors. On the other hand, (when original designs of the two unit
corridors were considered), increased corridor length in the Neuro-ICU led to significant
To summarize in the units with shorter, more compact, fragmented corridors with
multiple number of branching hallways, reverberation time levels are likely to be less.
225
CHAPTER 8
CONCLUSION
Chapter 1
In Chapter 1, introductory information about the general structure of the thesis and the
Chapter 2
perceptions, and evaluated the impact of particular noise sources on caregiver outcomes.
In total, three different empirical studies have been conducted. These studies took place
Empirical study 1 assessed the objective and subjective noise levels at different locations
in the two ICUs. The MedSurg-ICU is perceived as louder, more annoying, and having a
greater negative impact of noise on work performance, health outcomes, and anxiety as
compared to the Neuro-ICU. Surprisingly, there is little difference between two ICU
sound environments using traditional overall noise measures. The objective differences
between the occupied sound environments in the two units emerged more clearly through
a more comprehensive analysis of the “occurrence rate” of peak and maximum levels,
226
frequency content, and the speech interference level. Furthermore, mid-level transient
sound occurrence rates are significantly and positively correlated to perceived annoyance
Empirical study 2 documented the objective and subjective noise levels during different
times at each unit nurse stations by conducting the occurrence rate analysis and assessing
nurse perceptions. Sound environments of the two unit nurse stations are significantly
different based on the occurrence rate analysis. This is consistent with nurse perceptions.
different times of the day and days of the week. Similar to the previous empirical study
findings, these two sound environments are similar based on traditional Leq noise level
analysis.
Empirical study 3 compared the level of nurse disturbance due to overall-noise and
alarms in the two ICUs. MedSurg-ICU nurses have perceived medical alarms as more
detrimental to their work performance and health outcomes, anxiety and annoyance levels
performance outcomes among caregivers. Moreover the use of more detailed acoustic
metrics such as occurrence rate (a newer metric used in this study) can provide a better
227
picture than traditional measures of the aspects of the hospital sound environments as
they impact user experience and well being. Moreover, objective and subjective qualities
of hospital sound environments can vary between settings with different designs.
However conducting more controlled studies are necessary to identify the impact of
Chapter 3
from a case study comparing nurses` auditory monitoring performance in two ICUs with
The literature review findings indicate that different factors can affect the monitoring of
auditory cues including environmental factors and acoustic qualities of signals. For
example, in highly reverberant and noisy spaces, localization and detection of auditory
cues can be very challenging. In reverberant spaces, late arriving reflections can diminish
the positive effects of early reflections. In spaces with high background noise levels, the
signal-to-noise ratio decreases and therefore the signal becomes less distinguishable.
As a part of the empirical study, nurses are asked to rate the importance of different nurse
tasks and different types of including auditory cues for patient safety. The ICU nurses in
both units have indicated that auditory monitoring is as highly important and similar in
importance to visual monitoring for patient safety. Moreover, a majority of the nurses
228
have found various medical alarms highly important for patient safety such as ventilator
alarms, patient monitor alarms, IV pump alarm, and nurse call as well as ventilator
hissing sound. The nurses also indicated that they monitor these critical sounds at
multiple different locations in their units. These key listening locations include: patient
rooms, nurse stations, corridors in both wards and the medication preparation zone in the
Neuro-ICU.
Nurses in the two units are also asked to rate their ability to conduct sound tasks in their
units. In the MedSurg-ICU, nurse hearing and localization performance are perceived to
be higher compared to the Neuro-ICU even though the Neuro-ICU included a high
performance absorptive acoustic ceiling. Acoustic ceiling tiles are commonly used to
indicates the potential significant impact of other design features such as volume and
Overall, chapter 3 findings suggest that like visual monitoring, auditory monitoring is a
highly critical nurse task for patient safety. Nurses monitor a variety of different critical
sounds including alarm and non-alarm sounds at different locations in the nursing units.
Nurses` auditory monitoring performance can vary in healthcare settings with different
reverberant qualities. The design of hospitals is complex and different design features
229
conducting more controlled studies is necessary to identify design features impacting
Chapter 4
parameters, the sound behavior in proportional and non-proportional spaces, and the
Reverberation time has been commonly used for the acoustic analysis of different types
of spaces including long spaces and rooms. Newer room acoustic-parameters including
early decay time, clarity and sound strength have been developed particularly for the
newer acoustic parameters to other spaces has not yet been resolved.
Related with their design qualities such as spatial proportions, the acoustic characteristics
one type of space might not be applicable to another space. Various studies have been
conducted in concert halls and statistically analyzed the relationship between specific
design characteristics of floor-plate shapes such as width, length and acoustic outcomes.
However, the number of studies that systematically and statistically analyzed the
relationship between design and acoustics of long enclosures still remains limited. Some
studies explored the acoustics of long enclosures with complex floor-plate geometries
such as underground stations with branches or staggered urban streets. However, not
230
many studies investigated the acoustic conditions of inter-connected corridors located in
the buildings.
Overall, the study findings indicate that as the shape of the long enclosures gets more
complicated, sound behavior in these settings also becomes highly complicated. This
research conducted more controlled follow-up studies to clarify the impact of specific
shapes.
Chapter 5
Chapter 5 has conducted various impulse response measurements and acoustic simulation
Impulse response measurements (in situ measurements) have been commonly used for
non-proportional spaces. Acoustic simulation programs have been particularly used for
estimating the acoustic qualities of proportional spaces (rooms) such as concert halls.
Various validation studies have tested the reliability of these results predicted by the
has been validated. However, not many studies have tested the effectiveness of prediction
methods in estimating the acoustics of long enclosures. This study has conducted various
impulse response measurements and acoustic simulation analyses in long enclosures with
231
different floor-plate shapes to compare the difference between field measured and
predicted results. Most times predicted results are within 5-22% accuracy with a few
exceptions. This is parallel with the finding of previous studies. For example, in Kang`s
(2002b) study the difference between predicted and measured results range between 10-
25%. He has also used the same simulation program to conduct theoretical studies and
Another analysis has been conducted to compare the behavior of sound in a T-shaped
corridor and a single corridor. Different from single corridor, the trend of the data sets
recorded in the T-shaped corridor is not linear. In particular, the shape of the data set
trend lines in the non-visual sound field of the T-shaped corridor is more complex
compared to that in the visual sound field. This can be explained with the dominant
simulation tool with hybrid prediction (i.e., CATT) in studying the acoustic qualities of
Chapter 6
Chapter 6 presents the results of a heuristic design analysis, and statistically explores the
232
First, a pilot study is conducted in the inter-connected corridors of educational settings.
The pilot study findings suggest the potential significant impact of design features of
particular hallways (e.g., number of turns, corridor length, and number of branches) on
reverberation time values. For example, according to findings of the study, in the
hallways that are 2 turns away from the sound source, averaged reverberation time values
are likely to be higher compared to hallways 1 turn away from the sound source.
Moreover in the longer corridors, averaged reverberation time values are likely to be
higher compared to shorter corridors. On the other hand, in the hallways with higher
number branches, the reverberation time values are expected be lower compared to
To test the findings of the pilot study, a more controlled follow up study (Empirical Study
design models are generated based on the findings of the heuristic design analysis.
According to the findings, the corridor length of the hallways located in the non-visual
sound fields is significantly and positively correlated with averaged reverberation time
Another theoretical design analysis (Empirical Study 2) is conducted to assess the effects
correlated with averaged reverberation time values and VF is negatively correlated with
233
mean reverberation time values. Overall these findings agree with the Empirical Study 1
findings as higher RGD values indicate more elongated floor-plates with potentially
longer corridors. On the other hand, more fragmented floor-plates with higher VF values
Overall, the findings of Chapter 6 suggest that in addition of design features of particular
hallways such as corridor length, number of turns and number of branching hallways,
overall design of nursing unit floor-plate shapes can have significant impact on the
environments, designers should consider more compact and more fragmented nursing
unit floor-plate shapes.. All else equal, nursing units with shorter corridors and with more
branching hallways have shorter reverberation times. Also, the more turns that care
providers are from the patient rooms, the poorer their ability to monitor critical sounds
Chapter 7
In Chapter 7, the validity of the previous study findings (in Chapter 6) has been assessed
by analyzing the acoustics of real-world hospital ward corridors via impulse response
The impulse response measurements are conducted in different corridors of the two ICUs
already mentioned. In the Neuro-ICU averaged RT30 levels are consistently and
234
suggested the significant impact of different design features in addition to absorptive
qualities of the surface materials. For example, the MedSurg-ICU has a more compact
corridor layout with shorter corridors and thus a shorter reverberation time.
corridor design feature on RT30 is assessed. At all times, the simulation study findings
have agreed with the findings of Chapter 6. For example, the mean of the reverberation
times measured/predicted in the corridors with longer corridor length, lower number of
branches and higher numbers of turns from the sound source are higher. Averaged
reverberation times are also lower in the more fragmented and more compact unit
(Neuro-ICU) compared to less fragmented and less compact (MedSurg-ICU) when both
the simulated units has similar total corridor length, width and height and equal
absorption treatments. On the other hand, increased total corridor length in the Neuro-
Overall, Chapter 7 findings confirm the findings of Chapter 6. In other words, when
designing real world hospital floor-plate geometries, designers can refer to the findings of
8.2 CONTRIBUTION
The research findings contribute to ongoing efforts to improve the currently problematic
235
Provides a detailed understanding of the hospital sound environments by studying
Tests and validates the effectiveness of more detailed acoustic metrics (compared
to traditional metrics) that have not been commonly used in noise propagation
analysis
tasks
Suggests the use of hybrid acoustic simulation programs for the analysis of
corridors
236
Provides an opportunity for designers, engineers and care providers to think and
discuss hospital sound environments starting from the early design phases by
8.3 LIMITATIONS
Limitations
External validity
The studies introduced in Chapter 1 and 2 are conducted in the ICUs and the
study participants are ICU nurses. Related with the sample group characteristics,
generated based on the heuristic design analysis of the nursing units. Related with
the limited size of the sample group, the generalizability of the Chapter 6 findings
analysis program and floor-plate shape metrics used in Chapter 6 are only
effective in quantifying the shapes of the tree-like design nursing unit corridors
with limited complexity. Due to the limited size of the sample group, the findings
237
8.4 FUTURE WORK
Hospitals have complex and problematic sound environments. This research was able to
the relationship between architectural design features, acoustics and caregiver outcomes.
Future work should also consider patient and visitor outcomes. In addition, this study
measurement future work might explore other safety, quality and well-being outcomes
Moreover, this research focused particularly on reverberant and sound level qualities of
hospital sound environments. Even though reverberation time has significant impact on
speech intelligibility levels, this research did not focus on the direct relationship between
speech intelligibility and design features. In fact, speech intelligibility was one of
acoustic metrics that was not reliably predicted by CATT Acoustics V8 for the complex
long enclosures modeled. Therefore, future research should also explore reliable acoustic
the design and acoustics of nursing units was the focus of this study. Future research
should also analyze acoustics and design features of different healthcare settings such as
emergency departments.
238
APPENDIX A: DEFINITION OF TERMS
The following definitions of the acoustics terms are gathered from the following sources:
specifically, absorption coefficient is defined as the fraction of the incident sound energy
absorbed by a surface.
microphones built into the ears of an artificial human head and torso.
Occurrence rate: Percentage of the time that specified peak and maximum sound levels
Binaural hearing: A recording and playback configuration emulating hearing with two
ears.
Centre time (Ts): Center time is a measure used to describe where the sound energy is
concentrated in the echogram (energy distribution map of the sound). Low values of Ts
indicate that the arriving sound is concentrated in the early part while high values indicate
C-weighting: An attempt to electronically reduce the unequal sensitivity of the human ear
239
Clarity (C): Clarity is a measure used to quantify the ability to distinguish individual
sounds (e.g., music, warning, whirring machine) from the general audible stream, or the
degree to which rapidly occurring individual sounds are distinguishable. Clarity values
depend on the sound level difference between early and late arriving reverberant energy.
Direct sound and early reflections are usually considered to be useful sounds that
improve clarity. The cutoff value that represents the transition from early to late sound is
Cocktail party effect: Humans ability to focus one`s listening attention on a single talker
Critical distance: The distance at which the sound pressure level of the direct and
Decibel: The decibel is a logarithmic unit that indicates the ratio of a physical quantity
depend on the percentage ratio of the early arriving sound energy to total sound energy.
Direct sound and early reflections are considered to be useful sounds that improve
definition. The cutoff value that represents the transition from early to late sound is
240
Diffuse field: A sound field in which the sound comes in equal intensity from all
directions. For example, a diffuse sound field can be obtained in a large room with highly
reflective surfaces.
Diffuse reflection: Reflection of sound from a rough surface in such a manner that the
Direct sound: The sound that arrives at a receiver along a direct line from the source
Early decay time (EDT): Early decay time is another acoustic measure used to quantify
sound decay. EDT is the time required for the initial part the sound to decay 10dB. This
value is multiplied by 6 to extrapolate the results to RT60. Main difference between two
multitude reflections. This is mainly because EDT is calculated from the initial slope of
Exceedance level: Level of sound exceeded during specified percent of the time. For
example, L33 = 50 would mean that 33%of the run time, the decibel level was greater than
or equal to 50dB.
Equivalent sound pressure level: The sound pressure level of a steady sound which, in a
specific time period, has the same energy as the time varying sound.
Frequency: The number of cycles per second measured. The unit of frequency is cycles
Hybrid prediction method: A prediction method that optimizes the best features of two
different acoustic prediction models: image source model (ISM) and ray tracing (RT).
241
Image source modeling: A prediction method that determines the images of the source to
calculate the direction of the reflected sound rays off the enclosure surfaces.
Inter-aural level difference: Sound level difference between the sound signals reaching
Inter-aural time difference: Difference between the arrival times of the sound signals
Lateral energy fraction (LF/LFC): Lateral fraction is used to assess the relative
contribution of strong early lateral reflections arriving at the listener from either side at
the angles of 20-90degree relative to the front of the listener. Statistically, lateral fraction
is the linear ratio of lateral energy or the energy arriving from the sides within (5ms-
Lateral fraction: Ratio of sound energy arriving laterally (from the side walls) over
Long enclosures: Enclosed settings with higher L/H; L/W ratio compared to normal
242
Loudness: An auditory sensation. Loudness depends on factors such as sound pressure
Maximum sound pressure level: Maximum sound level is the highest sound pressure
Minimum sound pressure level: Minimum sound level is the lowest sound pressure level
between two variables depends on the value of one or more other variables.
as level.
Non-visual sound field: Sound field/zone that is not visible from the sound source.
Objective noise level: Noise levels quantified with the use of a sound level meter.
Octave band: Frequency interval between two sounds whose frequencies are related to
One-third octave band: An octave band divided into three one-third octave bands. The
upper frequency limit of a one-third octave band is 21/3 times its lower frequency limit.
Peak sound pressure level: Peak sound pressure level is different from maximum sound
pressure level. It represents the true peak of sound pressure wave. Therefore the signal
does not pass through a pressure circuit or calculator. For a pure tone the Peak is 3dB
243
above the Maximum sound level. However for varying signals this difference can be
much higher.
Pink noise: A noise whose level decreases with increasing frequency in such a way that
Precedence effect: Early reflections arriving between 2ms and 50 ms after the arrival of
the direct sound are typically considered to be useful reflections because they can be
combined with the direct sound. The human auditory system localizes sound sources by
effect.
Ray tracing: Prediction of numerical acoustic measures by tracing acoustic rays based on
Relative grid distance: Relative grid distance (RGD) values represent the degree to
Reverberant sound field: A sound field created by repeated reflections of sound from the
Reverberation: The continuation of sound in an enclosed space after the initial source has
terminated.
decay. In scientific terms, reverberation time (RT60) is the time it takes sound to decay
60dB or to one millionth of its initial energy. When the background noise in a space is
loud enough that a full 60dB decrease cannot be feasibly measured, reverberation time is
also measured over a range of 30dB (RT30) and 15dB (RT15). In more general terms,
reverberation time refers to the time it takes for sound to fade away after a source has
244
stopped. Strong reflections provided by the enclosure extend the reverberation time.
Excessive values of reverberation time can interfere with the activities conducted in the
Room acoustics: Field of acoustics that defines the sound decay qualities in enclosed
settings.
Room gain or Sound strength (G): Sound strength is used to assess sound level gained
location in a room relative to the total energy of the same source measured in an anechoic
Signal to noise ratio (SNR): A measure of signal strength relative to background noise.
Sound lateralization: A type of sound localization but where the subjects localize the
Sound level meter: An instrument for measuring sound pressure levels and other acoustic
parameters.
245
Sound localization: A hearing system`s ability to make accurate judgments about the
Sound pressure level: Sound pressure level is a logarithmic measure of the effective
sound pressure of a sound relative to a reference value. The commonly used reference
sound pressure in air is pref = 20 µPa (rms), which is usually considered as the threshold
of human hearing.
frequencies.
Specular reflection: Reflection of a sound from a smooth surface in such a manner that
Speech interference levels: Arithmetic average of the sound pressure levels at 500Hz,
1kHz and 2kHz center frequencies and is a measure of the degree to which background
The center time (Ts): A measure used to describe where the sound energy is concentrated
in the echogram.
Visual fragmentation: Visual fragmentation (VF) values reflect the convexity of a floor-
plate shape defined as the amount of directional changes needed to connect any two
246
Visual sound field: Sound field/zone that is visible from the sound source.
White noise: A noise whose energy is uniform over a wide range of frequencies.
247
APPENDIX B: DESIGN QUALITIES
The Neurological ICU (Neuro-ICU) is a recently opened 20-bed unit (A.B1). This unit
received the “ICU Design Citation” award in 2008, co-sponsored by the Society of
Critical Care Medicine (SCCM), the American Association of Critical Care Nurses
(AACN), and the American Institute of Architects Academy on Architecture for Health
(AIA). The award was for the Neuro-ICU’s design intent to enhance the critical care
environment for patients, families and clinicians. Some unit design features include large
private patient rooms with family studios and distributed nurse work areas and care
support areas. High performance absorptive acoustic ceiling tiles and drop ceiling
applications reside mainly along the two parallel sides of the corridors and at the nurse
stations, painted dry wall, acoustic rubber flooring, and 1.8 m (6 ft) wide (two-wing)
glass patient room doors are some of the surface applications in the unit. The patient care
248
Figure A.B1 Floor-plans of two critical care units (Key=a: patient room with family
studio, b: family area around the periphery of the unit, c: central nurse station, d: open
space, e: MED. area (i.e., medication preparation, pixes machine), f: other service/support
areas; grey highlighted zones: de-central nurse stations in the Neuro-ICU or patient
observation cores in the MedSurg-ICU).
The Neuro-ICU has a cluster-type architectural layout, composed of a 6-bed and 14-bed
clusters. Each cluster has a central nurse station with its own care support areas (e.g.,
medication room, supply room), and computerized patient monitoring system. In total,
the unit has two central nurse stations and seventeen distributed nurse work areas. Each
36 m2 (390 sqft) patient room is segregated into two portions: a patient care area
(approximately two thirds of the total area) and a family lounge area. The two areas are
separated by a semi-opaque glass wall. Approximately 1/3rd of the patient care core floor
area is occupied by the corridors, which are 183 m (600 ft) total length. The Neuro-ICU
includes additional spaces such as public family areas, CT scan lab, and a healing garden.
249
The Medical Surgical ICU (MedSurg-ICU) is a 1980s era 20-bed unit (Figure A.B1).
Compared to the Neuro-ICU, the MedSurg-ICU has more traditional surface applications
including standard ceiling tiles, vinyl flooring, approximately 1.5 m (5 ft) wide (two-
wing) glass patient doors, and painted dry wall. The patient care core of this unit is
The MedSurg-ICU has a triangular shape race track layout design. In this layout, medical
and support areas are located in the center and patient rooms are located on the perimeter,
with a corridor separating the two space types. Twenty private patient rooms are
organized around one large triangular-shaped service core that contains a centralized
nurse station and other care support areas such as medication room, equipment and
supply room. The centralized nurse station contains two patient monitoring cores (each
serves up to ten patients) at the corners and has a separate computerized patient
monitoring system. Patient rooms in this unit are approximately 18 m2 (190 sqft). This
particular layout type requires the use of segregated corridors for staff and family
members. Approximately 1/4th of the patient care core floor area is occupied by the staff
corridor, with a total length of 73 m (240 ft). The total MedSurg-ICU sits on
ICU (VF=1, VF=0.8, respectively). On the other hand, floor-plate shapes of the two units
250
APPENDIX C: NOISE LEVEL FIELD MEASUREMENTS
Monaural recordings
Although the human auditory system gathers information with two ears (a.k.a. binaural
hearing) some auditory information such as loudness can be obtained by listening through
one ear (a.k.a. monaural hearing). Based on their practicality and convenience, usually
sound level meters (with a single channel microphone such as a condenser microphone)
are used for the assessment of background noise levels via monaural recordings. The
condenser microphone samples the sounds as shown in Figure A.C1. The preamplifier
prepares an electronic signal for further amplifications and processing. It also keeps the
microphone away from the body of the instrument to avoid strong reflections. The body
of the instrument includes electronic circuits to process the sound detected by the
microphone. The digital signal processor filters the sound in various ways (e.g., A, C and
Flat broadband filters; Real-time FFT and 1/1 and 1/3 octave band filters; Slow, Fast
RMS detectors; Impulse and Peak detectors) and prepares the readouts to be displayed on
251
Figure A.C1 Larson Davis sound level meter
necessary if the sound level meter is going to be left unattended in the field (Figure
A.C2). The body of the instrument is located in the outdoor measurement case. Through
the holes located on the sides of the case, the microphone extension cable can be run
252
Figure A.C2 Set of equipment necessary for continuous noise level measurement
Placement of sound level meter is critical to capture accurate sound samples. When
appropriate, a sound level meter can be place on a tripod as shown in Figure A.C3.
253
Figure A.C3 Placement of sound level meter in an unoccupied patient room
In the clustered occupied patient rooms, the use of tripod might not be practical. A
practical solution can be hanging the microphone from the ceiling while leaving the
254
Figure A.C4 Placement of a single channel microphone in an occupied patient room
When applicable the microphone can also be placed on one of the medical equipment
255
Figure A.C5 Placement of a single channel microphone in a ward
256
APPENDIX D: NOISE LEVEL MEASUREMENT GUIDE
257
258
259
260
APPENDIX E: HOSPITAL NOISE SURVEY QUESTIONS
261
262
263
264
265
266
APPENDIX F: OCCURRENCE RATE
This appendix aims to provide more information about occurrence rate analysis.
Occurrence rate represents the % of the time during which background noise levels
exceeded specified peak and maximum sound pressure levels. This study considered
maximum sound pressure values of 70dB, 80dB and 90dB and peak sound pressure levels
of 80dBC, 90dBC and 100dBC as the threshold values. It was mainly because the
occurrence rates of maximum sound pressure levels less than 70dB and higher than 90dB,
and the occurrence rates of peak sound pressure levels less than 80dBC and more and
Figure A.F1represents the sound pressure levels associated with different sound events to
enable cross comparisons. However it needs to be noted that not only sound level but also
other qualities of sound events such as frequency content and duration can also
significantly affect annoyance and loudness perceptions of the human auditory system.
267
Figure A.F1. Sound pressure levels associated with different sound events
There are not many detailed guidelines providing allowable exposure times to maximum
and peak sound levels. According to OSHA, exposure to an impulsive noise event should
not exceed 140dB peak sound pressure level (OSHA, Occupational Safety and Health
Standards). According to WHO, Lmax sound pressure levels should not exceed 40dB in
the hospital settings particularly in the patient rooms (Berglund et al., 1999).
To calculate occurrence rates, peak and maximum sound pressure levels are documented
every minute (a.k.a. 1-minute interval) during the study period. Then peak and maximum
sound pressure levels are compared with the specified levels as shown in Figure A.F2.
268
Figure A. F2. Example acoustic data set used for the calculation of occurrence rate of
maximum and peak sound pressure levels.
Different from occurrence rate analysis, traditional metrics used to assess impulsive
characteristics of sound environments only consider the highest LMax and LPeak values
that occurred during the study period. In other words, they represent the characteristics of
occurrence rate analysis of LMax and LPeak sound pressure levels provide a better
traditional metrics, the use occurrence rate analysis can be highly important.
269
APPENDIX G: IMPULSE RESPONSE FIELD MEASUREMENTS
conducting field measurements. The use of different equipment is necessary for this type
of field measurement (Figure A.G1). The microphone is located at the receiver position.
It captures the room response to a sound burst produced by the sound source located at
another location. In particular, the ISO 3382-1 standard suggests the use of an omni-
reverberation time (ISO 3382-1). Some other components of the system that were used in
this study included an Outline amplifier, ProSonus EASERA Gateway data acquisition
system (DAQ) and lap-top equipped with EASERA software v1.1 (Figure A.G2). The
DAQ system enables the data transfer between different system components including
270
Speakon cable Connector
Amplifier
Audio cable
271
Maximum Length Sequence (MLS) Method vs. Others
Different sound signals can be used during for impulse response measurements. A
commonly used signal is the “sine sweep” which consists of a swept sine wave with a
frequency sweeping over the desired frequency range (Figure A.G3). This is not a
random signal and the extracted impulse response is free from noise contributed from the
weighting factors (Acoustics Engineering, 2007). By deconvolving the sweep signal with
In this study, a maximum length sequence (MLS) signal was used. It is a periodic,
pseudorandom white noise signal. Its frequency spectrum over one period is flat (Figure
A.G4). It is considered as the weighted sum of delayed impulses with weighting factors
the MLS and the signal at the receiver location, the room impulse response is obtained.
is rejected. Therefore, the extracted impulse response is therefore not “polluted” by any
noise due to the excitation signal. It is one of the reasons why this method enables more
reliable impulse response results in noisy environments such as HVAC background noise.
272
For reliable results, ISO 140 Standard recommends 6dB as the minimum acceptable
EASERA
such as reverberation time from room impulse response. EASERA`s post processing
engine calculates all acoustic functions and measures according to ISO Standard 3382
based on octave or 1/3 octave filters (EASERA Tutorial). The impulse response of a
room can be captured via EASERA by using a variety of excitation signals such as sine
sweep, white and pink noise and MLS signal as shown in Figure A.G5.
273
Figure A.G5 EASERA interface
Critical Distance
The energy of the sound source decreases by distance. The distance where both energy
densities of the direct and reflected sound are equal is called “critical distance” (Kutruff,
2000). The total energy of the sound is the sum of direct sound and reflected sound.
Inside the reverberation radius (a.k.a. critical distance) direct sound is dominant, however
beyond the reverberation radius the effect of direct sound is negligible as shown in Figure
A.G6.
274
Figure A.G6 Critical distance graph (from Kutruff, 2000)
as most times the main interest of acousticians is the assessment of reflections caused by
the enclosure boundaries. The following formula is used for the calculation of the critical
distance. The critical distance (rc) is in meter. “A” is the total absorption area and equal to
Sxa. “S” is the total surface area and “a” is the average absorption coefficient of surfaces
in the room.
275
APPENDIX H: IMPULSE RESPONSE MEASUREMENT GUIDE
276
277
APPENDIX I: COMPUTER MODELING
Different theories have been developed to describe sound behavior in enclosed spaces
such as wave-based acoustics and geometric acoustics. Wave based acoustics considers
sound as pressure waves. Some of the computer simulation methods developed based on
wave-based acoustics principles include Finite Element Method (FEM) or the Boundary
Element Method (BEM). However, these methods are often not practical for architectural
increases. Therefore, the use of these methods is typically limited to predict the acoustics
of small rooms and low frequencies (Smith, 2004). Geometric acoustics consider sound
as rays, similar to light rays used in the field of optics that propagate in straight lines.
This consideration assumes that the dimensions of a rooms are large compared to the
wavelength of the sound. In small rooms, this assumption does not always hold for the
low frequency sounds because wavelengths of low frequency sound is larger compared to
wavelengths of high frequency sounds. Related with this, the use of geometric acoustics
acoustic modeling programs have been developed based on geometric acoustics theory
that are capable of simulating different behaviors of sound in the enclosed spaces such as
specular reflection, diffuse reflection and absorption. These programs most times do not
take into account diffraction. However, recent advances in computer simulation programs
enabled the development of algorithms that emulate diffraction from edges. This topic is
278
Some of the computer simulation methods developed based on geometric acoustics
principles include ray tracing, image source method, and hybrid method. In the following
section, the qualities of each method are introduced based on the information gathered
from following sources: Smith 2004, Long 2006, CATT manual, and Odeon Manual.
Some information about the Radiosity method is also included below. Kang (2002a) used
Ray Tracing
In acoustic modeling applications that use a ray tracing method, a source emits a finite
number of rays representing the sound waves in either an omni-directional pattern (i.e.,
spread of sound rays in every direction) or in a specific pattern based on the directivity of
the sound source (Figure A.I1). The ray tracing method predicts numerical acoustic
measures by tracing acoustic rays based on the rules of the geometric acoustics (e.g.,
generating specular and/or diffuse reflections) and using statistical methods to calculate
energy loss via absorption. The sound rays are traced until they reach a virtual listener
represented with a sphere. Detection of the sound ray is significantly influenced by the
size of the detector. For example, a large spherical detector will record a large number of
hits from the rays compared to a receiver represented with a smaller sphere. By recoding
the energy, direction and arrival time of sound rays, the program generates an echogram
displaying the strength of the reflections at the receiver location over time. One of the
main advantages of the ray tracing method is the shorter computation time because the
sound source emits sound rays randomly. The computational time is proportional to the
number of rays and the order of the reflections used. Another advantage of this method is
279
that multiple receivers at different locations can be investigated because sound is emitted
in every direction.
Figure A.I1 Distribution of sound rays in an enclosed space (only rays crossing the circle
contribute to the impulse response calculations)
ISM method determines specular reflections between source and receiver. It generates the
images of the sound source known as virtual sources or image of the source (Figure
A.I2).
280
These virtual sources replace the boundaries of the room as shown in Figure A.I3
(Cheenne, 2002). ISM first calculates the first order image sources of the main sources in
all reflecting planes. From each of these first order image sources, second order sources
are created by calculating new images sources in all reflecting planes. This procedure is
repeated until the order of reflection and arrival time defined by the user. Once all image
sources are found, the program calculates arrival times by calculating the length of each
image source to the receiver. The amplitude of each reflection is calculated from the
output power and directivity of the source and is adjusted by absorption and diffusion
This method concerns only with the sound reflections reaching the receiver and ignores
reflections that do not reach the receiver. It collects the amplitude, arrival time and the
each image source with corresponding delays and attenuation (Tsakostas, 2004). It needs
to be noted that, each virtual source contributes only a pure impulse of known strength
In particular, this method has been mainly preferred because of its effectiveness in
handling the early arriving energy or low-order reflections (i.e., direct sound, first and
281
Figure A.I3 Distribution of sound source images (from Cheenne, 2002)
Hybrid Method
Hybrid algorithms have been developed to optimize the best features of two different
acoustic prediction models: image source model (ISM) and ray tracing (RT). The hybrid
method is the most up-to-date prediction method. Hybrid algorithms uses ray tracing
method to determine the valid image sources. Each ray detected by a receiver is
associated with an image source. These image sources are found by tracing it back from
the receiver location. To keep track of the valid image source, “image tree” is created.
Hybrid methods handle early and late reflections differently by incorporating the best
features of ISM and ray tracing methods. Often the image source method is used to
predict the early part of the impulse response and mostly ray tracing methods is used to
282
Acoustic modeling programs such as CATT and ODEON use a hybrid algorithm. These
hybrid methods may vary based on the type of receiver used such as cone, beam and
pyramid tracing vs. ray tracing. In this study CATT Acoustics V8 is used. In CATT V8,
Early Part Detailed ISM prediction method uses the image source method to calculate the
early part of the echogram which is an approximation to the squared impulse response of
a room (Smith, 2004; CATT manual). The Full Detailed Calculation prediction method
uses Randomized Tail-Corrected Cone tracing (RTC) or Late-Part Ray Tracing. The use
of latter option is suggested for coupled rooms, and rooms with complex floor-plate
shapes.
Radiosity Method
The radiosity method has been predominantly used to calculate light energy. It is
considered to be applicable in acoustics when used for high frequency band (Kang,
2002a). The radiosity method divides boundaries into a number of patches and distributes
the sound energy of an impulse source to patches (Figure A.I4). The patches are then
determined as sound sources which redistribute the sound energy to other patches. In
summary, this method calculates the energy response at the receiver locations by
Figure A.I4 Patches used in the Radiosity method (from Kang, 2002a)
283
Acoustic Modeling User Settings
diffraction can also be predicted by some programs. For the accuracy of the predicted
acoustic outcomes and simulation of sound behaviors in enclosed spaces, user selected
In accordance with “law of reflection”, a light/sound ray is reflected from the boundary
surfaces with a predictable manner. Reflection off smooth surfaces leads to a reflection
known as “specular reflection”. As a rule of thumb, the angle of the incidence is equal to
the angle of the reflection for specular reflections (Figure A.I5). Specularly reflected
sound rays remain concentrated in a bundle upon leaving the surface. Reflection off
reflected sound rays diffuse in many different directions. Diffusive qualities of boundary
surfaces are significant for acoustic outcomes such as reverberation time (RPG Diffuser
Systems). For example, when the surfaces of a room are diffusely reflective, there is
more chance that the sound rays will hit various surfaces including walls and ceiling
(where most time absorbers are located). Diffusely reflective surfaces also provide more
284
Figure A.I5 Sound reflections off diffusely and geometrically reflective surfaces (from
Cheenne, 2002)
Scattering coefficient
diffusely reflective qualities of the boundary surfaces. It is a measure of the total amount
of sound scattered randomly from the boundary surfaces in relation to total reflected
The energy that is not scattered is absorbed or specularly reflected by the boundary
surfaces. In more detail, if the incident energy is normalized to1, the total reflected sound
energy (that is not absorbed by the surface) will be (1-α) where α is the absorption
coefficient. This energy is used for specular and diffuse reflections. The component of
the sound energy that is specularly reflected is considered as (1- α)(1-δ) and the
A.I6).
285
Figure A.I6 Diffusely and specularly reflected sound rays off rough surfaces (from
Cheenne, 2002)
Rough surfaces are assigned higher values of scattering coefficients to indicate more
provides some guidelines for approximating the scattering coefficients of the surfaces. A
minimum of 20% scattering is recommended for an average size, flat, smooth surfaces
and 10% for larger flat and smooth surfaces. Some of the other surfaces that CATT
range between 30% and 70%) and rough surfaces with 0.3m surface roughness
higher values of scattering coefficient rather than assigning lower values is suggested.
Absorption coefficient
All surface materials absorb sound to some degree and the rest is reflected or transmitted
(Figure A.I7). Materials that are specifically employed for the purpose of absorbing
sound are called “sound absorbing materials”. A standard method of rating the
286
absorption coefficient varies with the frequency of sound. In other words, a surface
material does not absorb the sound similarly across frequencies. To enable
vary greatly across frequencies. However, it is likely that a reflective material has low
values of absorption coefficient in six octaves ranging from 125Hz to 8kHz is typically
Figure A.I7 Reflected, absorbed and transmitted sound (Mehta et al., 1999)
described as pressure waves. Bending of pressure waves around obstacles such as corner
of buildings and walls of interior spaces is known as “diffraction”. Reflection alone does
not account for all the indirect/reflected sounds at the receiver location. Diffraction also
contributes to qualities of the sound at the receiver location, particularly at the receiver
287
locations where the direct sound is not present (a.k.a. non-visual sound field). For
example, outdoors a person around the corner of a building can hear us even if there is no
reflection and direct sound. When sound diffracted in air at standard conditions, its speed,
frequency and wavelength generally remain unchanged, since the wave remains in the
same medium.
Recent developments in computer simulation research also suggest algorithms that can
predict diffraction caused by the object edges such as doors and windows or boundary
function which takes into account the impact of diffraction on sound quality at the
quarter of a wavelength from an edge are not specularly reflected but diffusely reflected
288
Number of rays
A user defined “number of rays” option is necessary to describe the amount of rays to be
randomly emitted by the sound source. It is one of the significant factors that affect
accuracy of predicted results. However, there are not commonly agreed solutions for
calculating number of rays. CATT Acoustics V8 has an option where the program
calculates the minimum number of rays needed for each particular acoustic model called
Truncation time
Truncation time represents the length of time that the sound rays will be traced. There is
not an agreed solution for calculating truncation time but it is suggested that truncation
time should be set to at least 2/3 of the reverberation time. Similar to the “number of
rays” option, CATT Acoustics V8 has an option where the program calculates the
Geometry modeling
Geometry files (also known as “GEO” files) include necessary information to describe
the hall geometry in a text format. It also includes information about x, y, z coordinates
of the planes corners, surface absorption and diffusion properties. The hall geometry can
have any shape and can be composed of various planes (a max of 99,999 according to
CATT). Entry of the geometry data can be defined either by using a text editor (Figure
A.I9) or exporting the drawing from the AutoCAD interface (Figure A.I10). Once a set of
Auto-LISP procedures are introduced to AutoCAD, the entire acoustic model (including
289
all surface planes, source, receiver positions, plane names and absorption names) can be
separate layer that can be exported as a GEO file compatible with CATT. This method
has been applied widely as well as the text editor method. Martin and Arana (2006)
indicated that they did not find any problem with this procedure.
290
It also needs to be noted that high levels of detail in the models do not necessarily
improve the accuracy of the results. In fact, Bork (2000) argues that very detailed
geometries could reduce the accuracy of the predicted results. Parallel with Bork`s (2002)
argument, Naylor and Rindel (1992) suggest that an acoustic model should replicate the
general forms of the room while avoiding unnecessary small surfaces and details.
However, according to Bradley and Wang (2002) the level of model detail on predicted
results does not affect the accuracy of predictions. In this study, related with these
Acoustic modeling programs have been widely used by acoustic consultants and
researchers, particularly for the acoustic analysis of rooms such as concert halls. The
validity of these acoustic modeling programs, in particular the ones using the hybrid
Martin and Arana (2006) compared the results of the impulse response measurements
conducted in the new Symphony Hall in Spain with the predicted results by two acoustic
modeling programs (P1 and P2) that use hybrid prediction method (Figure A.I11). In
general the results provided by the two programs were similar but some differences
291
Figure A.I11 Three dimensional geometry of the Symphony Hall in Spain showing
source and receiver locations (from Martin, & Arana, 2006)
In general across different frequencies, measured reverberation time results were higher
measured results were generally perceptible for reverberation time and other measures
such as Ts, C80, D50 and EDT based on the jnd thresholds suggested by ISO 3382.
Compared to P2, the reverberation time results provided by P1 were generally higher and
more similar to the measured results. Among the two programs, results provided by P1
followed a similar trend to the trend of the measured results. The key reason for such
differences is potentially related with different algorithms used in the programs such as
differently handled statistical corrections for late reflections and diffuse reflections. It
was not explicitly mentioned in the study but based on the description of the program
mentioned earlier CATT and ODEON are two of the major acoustic modeling programs
292
Overall, the study concluded that commercial acoustic modeling programs largely
coincide with field measured results despite perceptible differences. This conclusion is
only valid when we consider the similar trends followed by predicted and measured
results. A similar trend line analysis is also used in this study to compare the predicted
Figure 5.A.I12 Predicted vs. measured RT30 results at 500Hz and 1kHz (from Marin and
Arana 2006)
293
Various Round Robin studies were also conducted to assess the effectiveness of different
programs for room acoustics computer simulations. Vorlender (1995) compared the
predicted results and measured results for a speech auditorium. In total, predicted results
Figure A.I13 represents the predicted results by different modeling programs and the
thick line represents the field measured results. The simulations were carried out by
different participants independently and most of them were software developers. The
results provided by only three programs using hybrid prediction methods were considered
as reliable. The differences between measured and predicted results provided by these
three programs were still perceptible based on ISO 3382 standards. Another important
finding of the study was the significant impact of absorption coefficients on accuracy of
the results.
Figure A.I13 Reverberation time levels predicted for the same hall by different
participants (from Vorlender, 1995)
294
Acoustic modeling of long enclosures
the acoustics of long enclosures. These studies tested the applicability of different
prediction methods such as ray tracing (Yang, and Sheild, 2001) and ISM (Li & Lu,
2004; Li & Lu, 2005). The applicability of different prediction methods including ISM
and ray tracing methods has been generally validated for the acoustic analysis of long
of hybrid prediction programs that combines the best features of ISM and ray tracing
method has not been verified for the acoustic analysis of long enclosures. Additionally,
there are very few studies that document the behavior of sound in more complex long
enclosures, such as interconnected long enclosures with complex floor-plate shapes (e.g.,
long enclosures with branches). This study tested the effectiveness of a modeling
program that uses hybrid prediction method (CATT-Acoustics V8) in acoustic analysis of
295
APPENDIX J: JUST NOTICEABLE DIFFERENCE (JND)
Various studies have assessed the reliability of the predicted results via acoustic modeling
programs by comparing them with field measured results. The lower the difference
between predicted and field measure results, the better the accuracy/precision of the
predicted result is. This difference is desired to be as low as possible so that the human
auditory system will not perceive the difference. Various studies have been conducted to
assess the human auditory system`s ability to perceive the difference between different
limen” or “just noticeable difference (jnd)”. If the difference between two levels is less
than one subjective limen then the two levels are not perceptibly different. When
comparing the measured and predicted results, the most desirable condition is that this
difference is less than 1 subjective limen. According to draft international ISO/DIS 3382-
1 standard, subjective limen for reverberation time is 5% as shown in Table A.J1 (Bork,
2000).
296
Table A.J1 Just noticeable difference thresholds for different room acoustic parameters
(from ISO 3382).
However this allowable difference is very small therefore many studies could not obtain
results at this precision (Martin, Arana 2005; Shiokawa, & Rindel, 2007). Similarly,
program developers also concluded that it is highly difficult or even impossible to obtain
results that are within 1 limen range of the measured results (Odeon Tutorial).
The allowable error for reverberation time found by recent studies varied between 4.5%
and 10% (Karjalainen, 2001; Niaounakis, 2002). Even higher results were found by a
more recent study by Meng, Zhao and He (2006). Measured reverberation JND for
different subjects (e.g., audio technicians, common students and audio engineering
students) ranged between 21% and 39% (Table A.J2). However, this study used music
motifs to approximate the experience of the audiences in the concert halls. The
297
Table A.J2 Just noticeable difference thresholds for different room acoustic parameters
(from Meng et al., 2006).
Overall, there is not a consensus for the reverberation JND. However, the majority of the
validation studies used jnd values suggested by ISO 3382-1 when assessing the difference
298
REFERENCES
Aaron, J. N., Carlisle, M. A., Carskadon, C. C., et al. (1996). Environmental noise as a
cause of sleep disruption in an intermediate respiratory care unit. Sleep, 19, 707-
710.
Abel, J.S., Rick, J.W., Huang, P.P., Kolar, M.A., et al. (2008). On the acoustics of the
underground galleries of the ancient Chavin de Huantar, Peru. Presented at the
Acoustics` 08 Conference, Paris.
Acoustics Engineering. (2007). Measuring impulse response using dirac. Technical Note
(TN001). Available at www.acoustics-engineering.com (Accessed November
2010)
Aiken, L. H., Clarke, S. P., Sloane, D. M., et al. (2002). Hospital nurse staffing and
patient mortality, nurse burnout, and job dissatisfaction. Journal of the American
Medical Association, 288, 1987-1993.
Baker, C. F. (1984). Sensory overload and noise in the ICU: sources of environmental
stress. Critical Care Nursing Quarterly, 6, 66-80.
Baker, C. , Garvin, B., Kennedy, C., et al. (1993). The effect of environmental sound and
communication on CCU patients’ heart rate and blood pressure. Research in
Nursing Health, 16, 415–421.
Balogh, D., Kittinger, E., Benzer, et al. (1993). Noise in the ICU. Intensive Care
Medicine, 19 (1), 343-346.
299
Barling, J. (2001). Drowning not waving: Burnout and mental health nursing.
Contemporary Nurse, 11, 247-259.
Bennett, D., & Bion, J. (1999). Abc of intensive care: organisation of intensive care.
British Medical Journal, 318, 1468-1470.
Berenson, R. A. (1984). Intensive Care Units (ICUs): Clinical outcomes, costs and
decision making (Health Technology Case Study 28). Office of Technology
Assesment, U.S. Congress OTA-HCS-28.
Berglund, B., Lindvall, T. and , Schwela,D (ed). 1999. Guidelines for community noise.
Tech. Report, World Health Organization.
Bess, F. H., & Humes, L. E. (1990). Audiology: The Fundamentals. Baltimore: Williams
& Wilkins.
Bindl, L., Dresbach, K., & Lentze, M. J. (2005). Incidence of acute respiratory distress
syndrome in German children and adolescents: a population-based study. Critical
Care Medicine, 33, 209-212.
Blomkvist, V., Eriksen, C. A., Theorell, T., et al. (2005). Acoustics and psychosocial
environment in intensive coronary care. Journal of Occupational Environmental
Medicine, 62 (3), 1-8.
Bork, I. (2000). A comparison of room simulation software- The 2nd Round robin on
room acoustical computer simulation. Acustica, 86, 943-956.
Bradley, J. S., Sato, H., & Picard, M. (2003). The importance of early reflections for
speech intelligibility in rooms. Journal of Acoustical Society of Japan,113, 3233-
3244.
Bradley, D., & Wang, L. (2002). Effect of model detail level on room acoustic computer
simulations, Journal of Acoustical Society of America, 111, 2389(A).
300
Bregman, A. S. (1990). Auditory Scene Analysis: The Perceptual Organization of Sound.
Cambridge: The MIT Press.
Brokx, J. P. L., & Nooteboom, S. G. (1982). Intonation and the perceptual separation of
simultaneous voices. Journal of Phonetics, 10, 23-36.
Busch-Vishniac, I. J., West, J. E., Barnhill, C., et al. (2005). Noise levels in Johns
Hopkins Hospital. Journal of Acoustical Society of America, 118, 3629-3645.
Carayon, P., Smith, M. J., & Haims, M. C. (1999). Work organization, jobstress and work
related musculoskeletal disorders. Hum Factors, 41, 644-663.
Carnevale, F. A. (2009). Nursing care in the pediatric intensive care unit. In H. R.,
Wheeler, and T. P. Shanley, Science and Practice of Pediatric Critical Care
Medicine. London: Springer-Verlag.
CATT Tutorial.
Chambrin, M. C., Ravaux, I., Calvelo-Aros, D., et al. (1999). Multicentric study of
monitoring alarms in the adult intensive care unit: a descriptive analysis. Intensive
Care Medicine, 25, 1360-1366.
Chambrin, M. C. (2001). Alarm in the intensive care unit: how can the number of false
alarms be reduced? Critical Care, 5, 184-188.
Cheene, D. J. (2002). Modeling and auralization. In G. Ballou (Ed.), Handbook for Sound
Engineers. New York:Elsevier.
Cherry, E. C. (1953). Some experiments on the recognition of speech with one and with
two ears. Journal of Acoustical Society of America, 25, 975-979.
Christensen, M. (2005). What knowledge do ICU nurses have with regard to the effects
of noise exposure in the Intensive Care Unit? Intensive Critical Care Nursing,
21, 199-207.
Clarke, M. (1984).The constructs stress and coping as a rationale for nursing activities.
Journal of Advance Nursing, 9, 267-275.
301
Cox, T. J., & D'Antonio, P. (2009). Acoustic absorbers and diffusers: theory, design and
application. New York: Spon Press.
Cropp, A., Woods, L., Raney, D., et al. (1994). Name that tone: The proliferation of
alarms in the intensive care unit. Chest, 105, 1217-1220.
Department of Health and Human Services (DHHS), National Center for Health
Workforce Analysis, Health Resources and Services Administration. (2002).
Supply, Demand, and Shortages of Registered Nurses, 2000-2020.
Diers, D. (2004). Speaking of Nursing: Narratives of Practice, Research, Policy, and the
Profession. Sudbury, MA: Jones and Bartlett.
Downes, J. J. (2009). Development of pediatric critical care medicine: how we get here
and why? In D. S. Wheeler, H. R. Wong, & T. P. Shanley (Ed), Science and
Practice of Pediatric Critical Care Medicine. London: Springer-Verlag.
ECRI Institute. (2006). Patient safety reviews of medical technology: intensive care
environments. Available at
https://www.ecri.org/Products/Pages/Patient_Safety_Reviews.aspx (Accessed
November 2009).
ECRI Institute. 2007. Too many alarms can be hazardous to patients. Available at
https://www.ecri.org/Press/Pages/AlarmOverload.aspx (accessed November
2009).
Fife, D., & Rappaport, E. (1976). Noise and hospital stay. Am. J. Public Health 66: 680–
681.
Galindo, M., Zamarreno, T., and Giron, S. (2005). Acoustic analysis in Mudejar-Gothic
churches: experimental results. Journal of Acoustical Society of America, 117(5),
2873-2888.
302
Gardner, M. B., & Gardner, R. S. (1973). Problem of localziation in the median plane:
effect of pinnae cavity occlusion. Journal of Acoustical Society of America, 45,
47-53.
Gast, P, & Baker, C. (1989). The CCU patient: Anxiety and annoyance to noise. Critical
Care Nursing Quarterly, 12, 39–54.
Glucksberg, S., & Cowen, J. G. N. (1970). Memory for non-attended auditory material.
Cognitive Psychology , 1, 149-156.
Gross, A. G. (2006). End-of-life care obstacles and facilitators in the critical care units of
a community hospital. Journal of Hospice and Palliative Nursing, 8, 92-1102.
Haan, C. H. & Fricke, F. R. (1995). Musician and music critic responses to concert hall
acoustics, Vol. Trondheim, Norway.
Hagerman, J., Rasmanis, G., Blomkvist, V., et al. (2005). Influence of intensive coronary
care acoustics on the quality of care and physiological state of patients.
International Journal of Cardiology, 98, 267–270.
Hendrich, A., Chow, M. P., Skierczynski, B. A., et al. (2008). A 36-hospital time and
motion study: how do medical-surgical nurses spend their time?. The Permanente
Journal, 12, 23-34.
Hillman, K., & Bishop, G. (2004). Routine care of seriously ill. In Clinical Intensive
Care and Acute Medicine. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Hilton, B. A. (1985). Noise in acute patient care areas. Research in Nursing & Health, 8,
283-291.
Hinchliff, S. M., Montague, S. E., & Watson, R. (1988). Physiology for Nursing Practice.
London: Bailliere Tindall.
Hirose, M., Sato, E., Taguchi, M., et al. (2005). Characteristics of auditory alarms for
medical equipment and future issues. Journal of Clinical Engineering, 30(4), 208-
213.
303
Hodge, B., & Thompson, J.F. (1990). Noise pollution in the operating theatre. Lancet,
335 (8694), 891-894.
Houtgast, T., & Plomp, R. (1968). Lateralization threshold of a signal in noise. Journal of
Acoustical Society of America, 44, 807-812.
Imaizumi, H., Kunimatsu, S., & Isei, T. (2000). Sound propogation and speech
transmiision in a branching underground tunnel. Journal of Acoustical Society of
America, 108 (2), 632-42.
James, W.P., & Tatton-Brown, W. (1986). Hospitals: Design and Development. London:
The Architectural Press.
Joseph, A., & Rashid, M. (2007). The architecture of safety: hospital design. Current
Opinion on Critical Care, 13,714-719.
Kahn, D. M., Cook, T. E., Carlisle, C. C., et al. (1998). Identification and modification of
environmental noise in an ICU setting. Chest, 114, 535-540.
Kalish, B. J. (2005). Missed nursing care: a qualitative study. Journal of Nursing Care
Quality, 21, 306-313.
Kang, J. (2002a). Basic acoustic theories and modeling techniques. In Acoustics of Long
Enclosures. Thomas Telford:London.
Kang, J. (2002c). Parametric study: design guidance afor long enclosures. In Acoustics of
Long Enclosures. Thomas Telford:London.
Kang, J. (2002d). Parametric study: design guidance for urban streets. In Acoustics of
Long Enclosures. Thomas Telford:London.
304
Karjalainen, M., & Jarvelainen, H. (2001). More about this reverberation science:
perceptually good late reverberation. 111th AES Conference, New York.
Kerr, J. H., & Hayes, B. (1983). An alarming situation in the intensive therapy unit.
Intensive Care Medicine, 9, 103-104.
Kersten, A., Milbrandt, E. B., Rahim, M. T., et al. (2003). How big is critical care in the
US? . Critical Care Medicine, 31, A8.
Klosak, A. K., & Gade, A. C. (2008). Relationship between room shape and acoustics of
rectangular concert halls. Paper presented at the Acoustics`08, Paris.
Kracht, J. M., Busch-Vishniac, I. J. & West, J. E. (2007). Noise in the operating rooms
of Johns Hopkins hospital. Journal of Acoustical Society of America, 121, 2673-
2680.
Kuwano, S., Namba, S., & Mizunami, T. (2001). Desirable properties for identification of
auditory warning signals. Acoustical Science and Technology, 22, 311-314.
Lambert, B., Chang, K., & Lin, S. (2001). Effect of orthographic and phonological
similarity on false recognition of drug names. Social Science & Medicine, 52(12),
1843–1857.
Laschinger, H. K., & Leiter, M. P. (2006). The impact of nursing work environments on
patient safety outcomes: The mediating role of burnout/ engagement. Journal of
Nursing Administration, 36, 259-267.
Laws, P. (1972). On the problem of distance hearing and the localization of auditory
events inside the head. Technishe Hochschule, Aachen.
Lawless, S. T. (1994). Crying Wolf: False alarms in pediatric intensive care unit. Critical
Care Medicine, 22, 981-985.
Lerche, E., & Plath, P. (1961). On the localization of sound sources with headphones.
Pflugers Archiv, 274, 91.
Li, K.M., & Lu K.K. (2004). Propagation of sound in long enclosures. Journal of
Acoustical Society of America,116 (5), 2759-2770.
305
Li, K.M., & Lu K.K. (2005). Prediction of reverberation time and speech transmission
index in long enclosures. Journal of Acoustical Society of America, 117 (6), 3716-
3726.
Litovsky, R.Y., Colburn, H.S., Yost, W.A., & Guzman, S.J. (1999). The precedence
effect. Journal of American Society of America. 106, 1633-1654.
Lorenzi, C., Gatehouse, S., & Lever, C. (1999). Sound localization in noise in normal-
hearing listeners. Journal of Acoustical Society of America, 105, 1810-1820.
Love, H. (2003). Noise exposure in the orthopaedic operating theatre: a significant health
Hazard. ANZ Journal of Surgery, 73 (10), 836-838.
Lui, J.C., & Lu P. (2009a). Study on sound propogation in long enclosures with a vertical
or inclined branch of different dimensions. Applied Acoustics, 70, 522-29.
Lui, J.C., & Lu P. (2009b). The effect of multiple branches on sound propogation in long
enclosures. Applied Acoustics, 70, 894-901.
Martin, R.S., & Arana, M. (2006). Predicted and experimental results of acoustic
parameters in the new Symphony Hall Pamplona, Spain. Applied Acoustics, 67, 1-
14.
McCance, K. L., & Huether, S. E. (2002). Pathophysiology: the biological basis for
disease in adults and chidlren. St Louis: Mosby.
MacKenzie, D. J., & Galbrun, L. (2007). Noise levels and noise sources in acute care
hospital wards. Building Services Engineering Research and Technology, 28, 117-
131.
McLaughlin, A., McLaughlin, B., Elliott, J., et al. (1996). Noise levels in a cardiac
surgical intensive care unit: A preliminary study conducted in secret. Intensive
Critical Care Nursing, 12, 226-230.
Mehta, M., Johnson, J., & Rocafort, J. (1999). Architectural Acoustics. New Jersey:
Prentice-Hall, Inc.
306
Meng, Z., Fengjie, Z., & He M. (2006). The just noticeable difference of noise length and
reverberation perception. IEEE: T2A-3.
Meredith, C., & Edworthy, J. (1995). Are there too many alarms in the intensive care
unit?: An overview of problems. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 21, 15-20.
Meyer, T. J., Eveloff , S. E., Bauer, M. S., et al. (1994). Adverse environmental
conditions in the respiratory and medical ICU settings. Chest, 105, 1211-1216.
Miller, R. L., & Swensson, E. S. (2002). Hospital and Healthcare Facility Design. New
York, NY: W.W Norton.
Minkley, B. (1986). A study of noise and its relationship to patient discomfort in the
recovery room. Nursing Research, 17, 247–250.
Monks, M., Oh, M., & Dorsey, J. (1996). Acoustic Simulation and Visualization using a
New Unified Beam Tracing and Image Source Approach. Paper presented at the
Proceedings of the Convention of the Audio Engineering Society
Moore, M. M., Nguyen, D., Nolan, S. P., et al. (1998). Interventions to reduce decibel
levels on patient care units. The American Journal of Surgery, 64, 894-899.
Moray, N. (1970). Attention: Selective Processes in Vision and Hearing. New York:
Academic Press.
Moorthy, K.. Munz, Y., Undre, S., et al. (2004). Objective evaluation of the effect of
noise on the performance of a complex laparoscopic task. Surgery, 136, 25-30.
Morrison, W., Haas, E., Shaffner, D., et al. (2003). Noise stress and annoyance in a
pediatric intensive care unit. Critical Care Medicine, 31, 113–119.
Murthy, V. S., Malhotra, S. K., Bala, I., & Raghunathan, M. (1995). Auditory functions
in anaesthesia residents during exposure to operating room noise. Indian Journal
of Medical Research, 101, 213-216.
307
National League for Nursing Accrediting Commission (NLNAC). (1933). A concept of
nursing. American Journal of Nursing, 33, 6.
Naylor, G., & Rindel, J. (1992). Predicting room acoustical behavior with the ODEON
computer model. Journal of Acoustical Society of America, 92, 2346(A).
Niaounakis, T.I., & Davies, W.J. (2002). Reverberation time perception in small listening
rooms. Journal of the Audio Engineering Society. 50, 343-50.
ODEON Tutorial.
Occupational Safety and Health Administration (OSHA). Occupational Safety and Health
Standards. Available at
http://www.osha.gov/pls/oshaweb/owasrch.search_form?p_doc_type=STANDAR
DS&p_toc_level=1&p_keyvalue=1910. (Accessed March 2011).
Paolo, L., Athanase, B., & Stevo, J. (2006). The impact of increased heart rate on clinical
outcomes in hypertension: implications for antihypertensive drug therapy. Drugs,
66, 133-144.
Park, S., Song, H., Han, J., et al. (1994). Effect of noise on the detection of rib fractures
by residents. Investigative Radiology, 29, 54-58.
Parthasarathy, S., & Tobin, M. (2004). Sleep in the intensive care unit. Intensive Care
Medicine, 30, 197–206.
Patterson, R. D., & Mikoy, R. (1980). Auditory warnings on civil aircraft the leaming
and retention of warnings. Prepared for Civil Aviation Authonty 7D/S/0142.
Pelorson, X., Vian, J.-P., & Polack, J.-D. (1992). On the variability of room acoustical
parameters: reproducibility and statistical validity. Applied Acoustics, 37, 175-
198.
Phillips, J. A., & Barnsteiner, J. H. (2005). Clinical alarms: improving efficiency and
effectiveness. Critical Care Nursing Quarterly, 28, 317-323.
Plenge, G., & Brunschen, G. (1971). A priori knowledge of the signal when determining
the direction of speech in the median plane. Presented at the 7th Int Congr on
Acoustics, Budapest.
Rashid, M. (2006). A decade of adult intensive care unit design. Critical Care Nursing
Quarterly, 29 (4) , 282-311.
308
Rayleigh, L. (1907). On our perception of sound direction. Philosophical Magazine, 13,
214-232.
Rindel, J., Shiokawa, H., Christensen, C., et al. (1999). Comparisons between computer
simulations of room acoustical parameters and those measured in concert halls.
Presented at the Joint Meeting of the Acoustical Society of America and the
European Acoustical Association.
Ryherd, E. E., Persson Waye, K., & Ljungkvist, L. (2008). Characterizing noise and
perceived work environment in a neurological intensive care unit. Journal of
Acoustical Society of America, 123, 747-756.
Safar, P., & Grenvik, A. (1971). Critical care medicine: organizing and staffing intensive
care units. Chest , 59, 535.
Sandel, T. T., Teas, D. C., Feddersen, W. E., et al. . (1955). Localization of sound from
single and paired sources. Journal of Acoustical Society of America, 27, 842-852.
Schroeder, M. R., Gottlob, D., & Siebrasse, K. F. (1974). Comparative study of European
concert halls: correlation of subjective preference with geometric and acoustic
parameters. Journal of Acoustical Society of America, 56(4), 1195-1201.
Shpuza, E., Peponis, J. (2008). The effect of floor plate shape upon office layout
integration. Environment and Planning B: Planning and Design, 35, 318-336.
Society of Critical Care Medicine (SCCM). 2006. Critical care statistics in the United
States. Available at http://sccmcms.sccm.org/NR/rdonlyres/1FCD55C6-71B0-
48CA-B652-31EBB61DD202/504/WebStatisticsPamphletFinalJune07.pdf
(accessed November 2009).
Smith, H. (2004). Geometric acoustic modeling of the LDS Conference Center. Master
Thesis. Department of Physics and Astronomy, Brigham Young University
309
Soni, N., & Chamberlain, S. K. (1999). Monitoring. Best Practice & Research Clinical
Anaesthesiology, 13, 307-319.
Spieth, W., Curtis, J. F., & Webster, J. C. (1954). Responding to one of two simultaneous
messages. Journal of Acoustical Society of America, 26, 391-396.
Stanford, L. M., Mclntyre, J. W. R., & Hogan, J. T. (1985). Audible alarm signals for
anaesthesia monitoring equipment. International Joumal of Clinical Monitoring
and Computing 1: 251-256.
Tsakostas, C. (2004). Image Receiver Model: An efficient variation of the 'Image Source
Model' for the case of multiple sound sources and a single receiver. Paper
presented at the Conference of Hellenic Institute Of Acoustics in Thessaloniki.
Thomas, K.A., & Martin. P.A. (2000). NICU sound environment and the potential
problems for caregivers. Journal of Perinatal Medicine, 20(8), 94-99.
Topf, M. (1988). Noise-induced occupational stress and health in critical care nurses.
Hospital Topics, 66, 30–34.
Topf, M., & Dillon, E. (1988). Noise induced stress as a predictor of burnout in critical
care nurses. Heart and Lung, 17, 567-574.
Ulrich, B. T., Lavandero, R., Hart, K. A., et al. (2009). Critical care nurses’ work
environments. Critical Care Nurse, 29, 93-102.
310
Yang, L.N., & Shield, B.M. (2001). The prediction of speech intelligibility in
underground stations of rectangular cross section. Journal ofAmerican Society of
America,109, 266-73.
World Health Organization. (1999). Guidelines for community noise. B. Berglund and T.
Lindvall (Ed.), Technical Report. Geneva, Switzerland.
311
VITA
Selen Okcu was born in Turkey. She received both her Master’s degree in Design
University, College of Architecture. In her professional life she has been deeply involved
Design Research Group, Healthcare Acoustics Research Group and Health Systems
sponsored research projects. Her research experience ranges from qualitative and
employee`s job satisfaction, job commitment and health outcomes. In 2009, she was
awarded a Newman Medal from the Acoustical Society of America for excellence in the
study of architectural acoustics. She has received more awards from professional
organizations for her acoustics research including the Martin Hirshorn IAC Prize,
ASHRAE Graduate Student Award. Recently, her research that relates layout design to
acoustic outcomes has been awarded by Boston Society of Architects. She has been also
awarded for her research on high performance school buildings and user outcomes by
312
of Heating, Refrigerating and Ventilation Engineering, Society of Women Engineers, and
313