0% found this document useful (0 votes)
19 views9 pages

Computerized Progressive Attentional Training - CPAT

The article discusses the efficacy of the Computerized Progressive Attentional Training (CPAT) program as a direct intervention for children with ADHD. The study involved 20 children receiving CPAT and a control group of 16 children, with results showing significant improvements in reading comprehension and attentiveness for the experimental group. The findings suggest that CPAT effectively enhances attentional functions and academic performance in children with ADHD.

Uploaded by

bhasi
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
0% found this document useful (0 votes)
19 views9 pages

Computerized Progressive Attentional Training - CPAT

The article discusses the efficacy of the Computerized Progressive Attentional Training (CPAT) program as a direct intervention for children with ADHD. The study involved 20 children receiving CPAT and a control group of 16 children, with results showing significant improvements in reading comprehension and attentiveness for the experimental group. The findings suggest that CPAT effectively enhances attentional functions and academic performance in children with ADHD.

Uploaded by

bhasi
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
You are on page 1/ 9

This article was downloaded by: [Erciyes University]

On: 23 December 2014, At: 12:31


Publisher: Routledge
Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered
office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK

Child Neuropsychology: A Journal on


Normal and Abnormal Development in
Childhood and Adolescence
Publication details, including instructions for authors and
subscription information:
http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/ncny20

Computerized Progressive Attentional


Training (CPAT) Program: Effective Direct
Intervention for Children with ADHD
a b c
Lilach Shalev , Yehoshua Tsal & Carmel Mevorach
a
Department of Education and Psychology , Open University of
Israel
b
Department of Psychology , Tel Aviv University
c
School of Psychology , University of Birmingham
Published online: 12 Jun 2007.

To cite this article: Lilach Shalev , Yehoshua Tsal & Carmel Mevorach (2007) Computerized Progressive
Attentional Training (CPAT) Program: Effective Direct Intervention for Children with ADHD, Child
Neuropsychology: A Journal on Normal and Abnormal Development in Childhood and Adolescence,
13:4, 382-388, DOI: 10.1080/09297040600770787

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09297040600770787

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the
“Content”) contained in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis,
our agents, and our licensors make no representations or warranties whatsoever as to
the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the Content. Any opinions
and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors,
and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content
should not be relied upon and should be independently verified with primary sources
of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any losses, actions, claims,
proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever or
howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising
out of the use of the Content.

This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any
substantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing,
systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms &
Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-
and-conditions
Downloaded by [Erciyes University] at 12:31 23 December 2014
Child Neuropsychology, 13: 382–388, 2007
http://www.psypress.com/childneuropsych
ISSN: 0929-7049 print / 1744-4136 online
DOI: 10.1080/09297040600770787

COMPUTERIZED PROGRESSIVE ATTENTIONAL


TRAINING (CPAT) PROGRAM: EFFECTIVE DIRECT
INTERVENTION FOR CHILDREN WITH ADHD

Lilach Shalev,1 Yehoshua Tsal,2 and Carmel Mevorach3


1
Department of Education and Psychology, Open University of Israel, 2Department of
Psychology, Tel Aviv University, and 3School of Psychology, University of Birmingham
Downloaded by [Erciyes University] at 12:31 23 December 2014

We tested the efficacy of a pioneering intervention program grounded in a contemporary


theoretical framework of attention and designed to directly improve the various attentional
functions of children with ADHD. The computerized progressive attentional training
(CPAT) program is composed of four sets of structured tasks that uniquely activate
sustained attention, selective attention, orienting of attention, and executive attention.
Performance was driven by tight schedules of feedback and participants automatically
advanced in ordered levels of difficulty contingent upon performance. Twenty 6- to 13-year-
old children with ADHD were assigned to the experimental group and received the CPAT
sessions twice a week over an 8-week period. Sixteen age-matched control children with
ADHD were assigned to the control group and participated in sessions of the same fre-
quency, length, and format except that instead of performing the training tasks they played
various computer games during the session. The experimental participants showed a signif-
icant improvement in nontrained measures of reading comprehension, and passage copy-
ing as well as a significant reduction of parents’ reports of inattentiveness. No significant
improvements were observed in the control group. We thus concluded that the above
academic and attentional improvements were primarily due to the CPAT.

INTRODUCTION
Attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is a chronic mental health disorder of
childhood onset characterized by inattention, impulsiveness, and hyperactivity (Diagnostic and
Statistical Manual of mental disorders, 4th edition [DSM-IV]; APA, 1994). Given the high
prevalence of ADHD and its impact on many aspects of development and every day function-
ing, extensive effort has been focused on various treatments to lessen symptoms. Since this
study reports a novel cognitive nonpharmacological treatment, we will briefly review the
recent developments in the field of nonpharmacological treatments for children with ADHD.

Cognitive Treatment
Many different types of cognitive-behavioral treatments have been applied to
children with ADHD, including verbal self-instructions, problem-solving strategies, cognitive

Address correspondence to Lilach Shalev, Behavioral Brain Sciences Centre, School of Psychology,
University of Birmingham, Edgbaston, B15 2TT, Birmingham, UK. E-mail: 1.shalev.1@bham.ac.uk

© 2007 Psychology Press, an imprint of the Taylor & Francis Group, an Informa business
ATTENTIONAL TRAINING FOR ADHD 383

modeling, self-monitoring, self-evaluation, and self-reinforcement among others. These


interventions are focused on improvement of self-controlled behavior through the enrich-
ment of problem-solving strategies. Unfortunately, many studies (Abikoff & Gittelman,
1985; Abikoff, 1991; Bloomquist, August, & Ostrander, 1991) showed that cognitive-
behavioral treatments do not provide significant improvements in the behavior and
academic performance of children with ADHD.
More recently, Kerns, Eso, and Thomson (1999) argued that repeated activation and
stimulation of attentional systems can facilitate changes in cognitive capacity that presumably
reflects underlying changes in neuronal activity. They tested the efficacy of a new set of
child-oriented direct intervention materials and found that children with ADHD receiving
this direct intervention did significantly better than a control group on a number of
untrained measures of attention and academic performance. No significant improvement
in parent and teacher rating scales was obtained.
Downloaded by [Erciyes University] at 12:31 23 December 2014

A recently developed training method (Klingberg et al., 2005; Klingberg, Forssberg, &
Westerberg, 2002) demonstrated that intensive computerized training of working memory
(WM) gradually increased the amount of information that children with ADHD can keep
in WM. Posner and Rothbart (2005) demonstrated that normal 4-year-old children bene-
fited from five days attentional training. Their EEG data suggested that the training altered
activity in the anterior cingulate. They concluded that attentional training could change the
underlying network in ways that might lead to extensive generalization.

Theoretical Framework of the Present Study


The present research is based on Posner and Petersen’s (1990) influential theory of
attention networks and its recent application to ADHD research (Tsal, Shalev, &
Mevorach, 2005). Posner and Petersen (1990) proposed that the attentional system can be
broken down into three networks represented in distinct anatomical areas: (a) The
vigilance network, responsible for maintaining a state of alertness (right frontal and pari-
etal regions); (b) the visual orienting network, which controls the selection of information
from sensory input (superior parietal lobe, temporal parietal junction); and (c) the
executive attention network, responsible for resolving conflicts among responses (midline
frontal areas and lateral prefrontal cortex).
The present study was designed to test the hypothesis that attention deficits of
children with ADHD can be ameliorated by a systematic individualized training program
involving a direct intensive exercising of the attentional networks. Studies examining the
academic achievements of children with ADHD indicate that many of them receive low
grades in academic subjects and low scores on standard measures of reading, math, spelling,
or written language (Anderson, Williams, McGee, & Silva, 1987; Barkley, Fischer,
Edelbrock, & Smallish, 1990; Zentall, 1993). Thus, our aim was to demonstrate that
attentional training in children with ADHD can primarily improve academic performance.
A computerized progressive attentional training (CPAT) program was developed on the
basis of leading theories and commonly used methodologies in the visual attention
literature. The program was specifically designed for use with children (aged 6 years and
above) who cope with attentional difficulties. The program includes four sets of tasks each
designed to extensively train one of the four attentional networks: sustained attention,
selective attention, orienting of attention, and executive attention. The rationale for
improvement in training was based on the gradual progress across difficulty levels driven
by tight schedules of feedback and accumulation of points.
384 L. SHALEV ET AL.

METHOD
Participants. Participants were diagnosed by a qualified psychiatrist, neurologist
or psychologist according to DSM-IV criteria. The participants were randomly assigned to
either the experimental group or the control group, and group identity was known neither
to participants nor to their parents. Written parental consent was a prerequisite for partici-
pation in the study. The experimental group included 20 children (17 males and 3 females)
and the control group included 16 children (13 males and 3 females) all with ADHD. The
experimental group had a mean age of 9.1 years (range 6–13 years); the mean age of the
control group was 9.2 years (range 6–13 years). There was no significant difference
between ages (p = .88) and intelligence (p = .41) between the two groups. The majority of
the participants met the DSM-IV criteria for ADHD combined subtype (15/20 in
the experimental group and 13/16 in the control group). The other participants met the
DSM-IV criteria for ADHD inattentive subtype (DuPaul et al., 1998).
Downloaded by [Erciyes University] at 12:31 23 December 2014

The training tasks. The four comprehensive training tasks are based on expan-
sions and modifications of various tasks that have extensively been investigated in the
attention literature and are known to uniquely reflect the various attentional functions. The
four training tasks included the Computerized Continunuous Performance Task (CCPT;
based on Rosvold, Mirsky, Sarason, Bransome, & Beck, 1956), which was designed to
improve the function of Sustained Attention, the Conjunctive Search Task (based on
Treisman & Gelade, 1980), which was designed to improve the function of Selective
Attention, the Combined Orinenting and Flanker Task (based on Eriksen & Eriksen, 1974
and Posner, Snyder, & Davidson, 1980), which was designed to improve the function of
Orienting Attention and the Shift Stroop-like Task (based on Navon, 1977), which was
designed to improve the function of Executive Attention.

Procedure. On the average each session included eight blocks from four different
tasks. Number of blocks varied across participants primarily due to age differences. Every
block contained 40 trials, except for blocks in the CCPT that consisted of either 80 trials or
160 trials depending on the level of difficulty. Participants advanced in levels of difficulty
according to prespecified criteria based on fixed-accuracy and individually adjusted reac-
tion time (RT). Participants received an auditory feedback (beep) when an error was com-
mitted and visual feedbacks on RT performance. These feedbacks were translated into
points that were presented on the screen at the end of each block.

The training program. The training program was carried out over a period of
eight weeks consisting of two one-hour sessions per week. Each participant was supervised
by a research assistant during the entire session. During each session participants performed
a selection of tasks and within each task participants advanced in the levels of difficulty
according to their gradual progress, expressed in speed and accuracy performance.
In order to evaluate the success of our program participants were tested on several
criteria before and after training. Improvement on these tasks (relative to natural progress
of control participants served as an indication for the success of the CPAT. The control
group consisted of children with ADHD who participated in sessions of the same
frequency, length, and format except that instead of performing the training tasks they
played various computer games and were involved in various paper and pencil activities
during the session. These computer games contained inherent scoring and feedback mech-
anisms. These games also included multiple levels of difficulty.
ATTENTIONAL TRAINING FOR ADHD 385

The training evaluation tests. Pre- and posttreatment measures were adminis-
tered prior to beginning the intervention and within 2 weeks of completing the treatment.
The corresponding pretests and posttests were different but of equal difficulty levels.
Since there are no standard academic tests in Hebrew, the academic tests for each participant
were taken from school-books of the relevant grade. The research assistants who adminis-
tered both the pre- and posttests were randomly assigned to avoid any consistent bias.
Participants were presented with the following tests: passage copying, math exercises,
reading comprehension, and parents’ rating scale (DuPaul et al., 1998).

RESULTS AND DISCUSSION


Figure 1 presents the pre- and posttraining scores of the three academic measures for
experimental and control participants. The figure suggests a clear distinction between the
Downloaded by [Erciyes University] at 12:31 23 December 2014

experimental and control groups in reading comprehension and passage copying. That is,
on these two measures training produced a substantial improvement and no effect was
obtained for the control participants. Statistical analyses indeed confirmed these observa-
tions.These data were subjected to ANCOVA (Analysis of covariance) in order to remove
the extraneous variability that derives from preexisting individual differences. These analy-
ses revealed that in both reading comprehension, F(1, 31) = 4.89, p < .05; η2 = .12, and
speed of copying, F(1, 31) = 6.11, p < .05; η2 = .17, the significant improvements that
were obtained in the experimental group are attributed to the type of treatment and are not
due to preexisting individual differences. However, the analysis also revealed that the
improvement of experimental participants relative to control participants in mathematical
performance could not be fully attributed to training, F(1, 32) = 1.2, p < .30.
Figure 2 presents the pre- and posttraining scores of parents’ ratings (DuPaul et al.,
1998) of both inattention and hyperactive symptoms. The figure shows a substantial
reduction in inattention symptoms for the experimental participants and no change in the
control participants. The figure also shows a reduction in hyperactive symptoms for
children in both groups. An ANCOVA carried out on these data indicated that while the

a b c
1.00
Math Excercises Passage Copying 1.00
Reading Comprehension
0.18
0.95 0.16 0.95
0.90 0.90
0.14
Evaluation scores

Evaluation scores
Words per second

0.85 0.85
0.12
0.80 0.80
0.10
0.75 0.75
0.08
0.70 0.70
0.65 0.06 0.65
0.60 0.04 0.60
0.55 0.02 0.55
0.50 0.00 0.50
Pre-test Post-test Pre-test Post-test Pre-test Post-test

Experimental Control Experimental Control Experimental Control

Figure 1 Mean pre- and posttraining measures (± s.e.m.) of academic performance for experimental and control
participants. a. Proportion of correct answers in math exercises. b. Number of copied words per second. c. Proportion
of correct answers in reading comprehension.
386 L. SHALEV ET AL.

a b
Inattention Hyperactivity-Impulsivity
18 18

16 16

14 14

12 12
Scores

Scores
10 10

8 8

6 6

4 4

2 2
Downloaded by [Erciyes University] at 12:31 23 December 2014

0 0

Pre-test Post-test Pre-test Post-test

Experimental Control Experimental Control

Figure 2 Mean pre- and posttraining scores (± s.e.m.) on the behavioral rating scale for experimental and control
participants. The Y-axis represents the cumulative score of symptoms on each factor: a. Inattentive symptoms.
b. Hyperactive-impulsive symptoms.

reduction in inattention symptoms could be attributed to training, F(1, 24) = 8.37, p < .01;
η2 = .29, the reduction in hyperactive symptoms could possibly be attributed to extraneous
influence, F(1, 24) = 2.34, p = .14.
The results clearly show that a relatively short-term computerized progressive atten-
tional training (CPAT) that operates on attentional networks produced significant
improvements in both academic tests and parents’ behavioral ratings. It should be noted
that in both groups children were treated very positively and were semantically rewarded
for being consistent and cooperating throughout the 16-session intervention. Alternative
explanations such as spontaneous development or a general supportive atmosphere can be
ruled out since no systematic changes on these measures were observed for the control
group. While the program also significantly improved mathematical performance and
reduced behavioral symptoms, the present analyses could not rule out the possibility that
these effects were due to extraneous variability deriving from preexisting individual
differences. The present findings lend support to the proposition (Kerns et al., 1999) that
process specific interventions that directly stimulate and exercise attentional systems improve
cognitive capacity as they presumably reflect underlying changes in neuronal activity.
In considering the manner in which the CPAT could affect academic performance it
is important to keep in mind that every academic task requires the efficient functioning of
attentional systems and that attention deficit is the primary cause for poor academic
performance in children with ADHD (Zentall, 1993). Thus, for example, effective copy-
ing performance requires orienting to the location of the momentarily relevant word
(orienting of attention) while ignoring other adjacent words (selective attention), avoiding
lapses of attention during this prolonged process (sustained attention), and regulating the
entire task in the presence of potential interference (executive attention). Hence, it is
possible that systematic improvement in these four attentional functions may generalize to
academic tasks by virtue of improving the mediating attention processes.
ATTENTIONAL TRAINING FOR ADHD 387

While it is generally hard to identify the specific cause of changes in treatment-


oriented investigations (Kerns et al., 1999), it is worth noting two unique characteristics of
feedback in CPAT that distinguishes this program from other activities and are likely to
contribute to its success. First, feedbacks provided in other forms of treatment and daily
activities are rather general and vague in the sense that they are not reinforcing the specific
desired response. In the present training the attentional components of the tasks were iso-
lated and, thus, those processes mediating attentional control were immediately and
directly linked to the feedback. The second distinctive characteristic of CPAT was the
advancement in difficulty levels in a systematic fashion under highly structured condi-
tions. Structured training procedures with defined “working rules” have been shown to
facilitate self-regulation and control. For example, Reid and Borkowski (1987) found that
attention of impulsive children could be brought under some degree of voluntary control
by self-instructional procedures. In addition to its positive cognitive aspects, this gradual
Downloaded by [Erciyes University] at 12:31 23 December 2014

learning process also had a significant motivational influence. Given that within each dif-
ficulty level effective responses were within reach, it is likely that children experience a
gain in self-esteem and sense of control as they improve on treatment tasks. It was clear
that the number of points presented at the end of each block served as a tremendous source
of fulfillment and satisfaction.
There are three major limitations in the present study. The first limitation of the
present study is that we did not include in our evaluation battery any objective attentional
measure assessing attentional difficulties before and after training. Future studies evaluat-
ing the mechanisms that underlie the effects of the CPAT should include attentional mea-
sures in order to investigate the extent to which improvements in academic performance
are associated with improvements in the various attentional measures. The second limita-
tion is that without any long-term follow-up study, it is not possible to determine whether
the improvement observed in the present study is time limited or whether it represents an
enduring change in underlying ability. A longer term investigation is warranted to more
adequately address this question. The third limitation is the absence of teachers’ evalua-
tions regarding the children’s classroom functioning and academic performance. Future
research should enlarge the tools that assess the effectiveness and generalization effects of
the attentional training, including teachers’ evaluations.

Original manuscript received November 9, 2005


Revised manuscript accepted April 21, 2006
First published online October 20, 2006

REFERENCES
Abikoff, H. (1991). Cognitive training in ADHD children: Less to it than meets the eye. Journal of
Learning Disabilities, 24, 205–209.
Abikoff H, & Gittelman R. (1985). Hyperactive children treated with stimulants. Is cognitive train-
ing a useful adjunct? Archives of General Psychiatry, 42, 953–961.
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of mental disorders
(4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
Anderson, J. C., Williams, S., McGee, R., & Silva, P. A. (1987). DSM-III disorders in preadolescent
children. Prevalence in a large sample from the general population. Archives of General Psychiatry,
44, 69–76.
Barkley, R. A., Fischer, M., Edelbrock, C. S., & Smallish, L. (1990). The adolescent outcome of
hyperactive children diagnosed by research criteria: I. An 8-year prospective follow-up study.
Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 29, 546–557.
388 L. SHALEV ET AL.

Bloomquist, M. L., August, G. J., & Ostrander, R. (1991). Effects of a school-based cognitive-
behavioral intervention for ADHD children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 19,
591–605.
DuPaul, G., Anastapoulos, A., Power, T., Reid, R., Ikeda, M., & McGoey, K. (1998). Parents’ rating
of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms: Factor structure and normative data.
Journal of Psychopathology and Behavior, 2, 83–102.
Eriksen, B. A., & Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target
letter in a nonsearch task. Perception and Psychophysics, 16, 143–149.
Kerns, K. A., Eso, K., & Thomson, J. (1999). Investigation of a direct intervention for improving
attention in young children with ADHD. Developmental Neuropsychology, 16, 273–295.
Klingberg, T., Forssberg, H., & Westerberg, H. (2002). Training of working memory in children
with ADHD. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 24, 781–791.
Klingberg, T., Olesen, P. J., Johnson, M., Gustafsson, P., Dahlstrom, K., et al. (2005). Computerized
training of working memory in children with ADHD: A randomized, controlled trial. Journal of
the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 44, 177–186.
Downloaded by [Erciyes University] at 12:31 23 December 2014

Navon, D. (1977). Forest before trees: The precedence of global features in visual perception.
Cognitive Psychology, 9, 353–383.
Posner, M. I., & Petersen, S. E. (1990). The attention system of the human brain. Annual Review of
Neurosience, 13, 25–42.
Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (2005). Influencing brain networks: Implications for education.
Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 99–103.
Posner, M. I., Synder, C. R., & Davidson, B. J. (1980). Attention and the detection of signals. Journal
of Experimental Psychology: General, 109, 160–174.
Reid, M. K., & Borkowski, J. G. (1987). Causal attributions of hyperactive children: Implications
for teaching strategies and self-control. Journal of Educational Psychology, 79, 296–307.
Rosvold, H. E., Mirsky, A. F., Sarason, I., Bransome, E. D., & Beck, L. H. (1956). A continuous
performance test of brain damage. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 20, 343–352.
Treisman, A. M., & Gelade, G. A. (1980). Feature-integration theory of attention. Cognitive
Psychology, 12, 97–136.
Tsal, Y., Shalev, L., & Mevorach, C. (2005). The diversity of attention deficits in attention deficit
hyperactivity disorder: The prevalence of four cognitive factors in ADHD vs. controls. Journal
of Learning Disabilities, 38, 142–157.
Zentall, S. S. (1993).Research on the educational implications of ADHD. Exceptional Children, 60,
143–153.

You might also like

pFad - Phonifier reborn

Pfad - The Proxy pFad of © 2024 Garber Painting. All rights reserved.

Note: This service is not intended for secure transactions such as banking, social media, email, or purchasing. Use at your own risk. We assume no liability whatsoever for broken pages.


Alternative Proxies:

Alternative Proxy

pFad Proxy

pFad v3 Proxy

pFad v4 Proxy