Vocabulary in Textbooks For Young Learners
Vocabulary in Textbooks For Young Learners
net/publication/375304669
CITATIONS READS
5 128
1 author:
Marie Nordlund
Luleå University of Technology
14 PUBLICATIONS 174 CITATIONS
SEE PROFILE
All content following this page was uploaded by Marie Nordlund on 08 November 2023.
Marie Nordlund
EDUCARE 2015:1 73
MARIE NORDLUND
Introduction
Knowledge of vocabulary is imperative to communication in a foreign lan-
guage. Research on vocabulary learning has demonstrated, among other
things, the importance of vocabulary in making oneself understood, the size
of vocabulary needed and how vocabulary is best taught and learned (see,
e.g., Milton, 2009; Nation, 1990, 2001, 2008; Read, 2000; Schmitt, 2000). In
many foreign-language classrooms, the textbook is often the main, and
sometimes the only, teaching material used; despite this centrality, few stud-
ies have focussed on the correlation between the structure of teaching mate-
rials and vocabulary content (see, however, Meara & Suárez García, 2010;
Rixon, 1999). In a study of seven international textbooks in English for be-
ginners, Rixon (1999) found that the books were lacking in several respects,
one of them being inadequate reuse of words. Recurrent meetings with
words enable the automatization of word knowledge and recognition (Tyler,
2012, p. 53) and the development of a sight vocabulary (Coady, 1997, p.
235; Laufer, 1997, p. 23)—the direct and unconscious decoding of the mean-
ing of words read is as important as learning to directly decode words heard.
Hence, vocabulary learning should be in focus in the beginners’ classroom
(Coady, 1997, p. 229). To build up a vocabulary in a new language is not an
easy task, and learners need all the help they can get—from teachers as well
as from the structure and content of the teaching material used. Since
Rixon’s (1999) results indicate that textbooks often conform badly to ad-
vances within our understanding of the role of vocabulary acquisition in
second language learning, they beg the question, Do other textbooks in Eng-
lish have the same flaws? In the Swedish context, studies have focussed on
the high school level (Ljung, 1990, 1991), but so far, no comprehensive
study has focussed on how vocabulary is presented in textbooks for young
learners. The results of a survey carried out by the Swedish National Agency
for Education on the use of teaching materials (Skolverket, 2006) indicate
that it is far from evident that the contents of textbooks used actually corre-
spond to the learning objectives set out in the national curriculum. There is
thus more than one reason to study this area further.
Learning vocabulary
The relation between the development of a sight vocabulary and the auto-
matic decoding of words may seem self-evident, but determining what con-
stitutes ‘knowing’ a word is not as straightforward. Nation (1990, p. 31), for
instance, lists eight different aspects of knowing a word:
• its meaning(s)
• its written form
• its spoken form
68
74 EDUCARE 2015:1
Vocabulary in textbooks for young learners
Clearly, not all words in one’s vocabulary are ‘known’ to the same extent;
some are recognised and understood in context only (receptive vocabulary),
while others are readily available for productive use. However, there are also
degrees in between, as Melka (1997) puts it:
69
EDUCARE 2015:1 75
MARIE NORDLUND
Core vocabulary
When children with English as their first language start school around the
age of five, they generally have a productive vocabulary of approximately
3,200 words (Biemiller, 2003, p. 327)—a vocabulary that grows with around
500 words each year (Nation & Waring, 1997, p. 7; Schmitt, 2000, p. 3).
Since a second language learner seldom has access to daily input of authen-
tic language, the same development of vocabulary cannot be expected. The
question is then what words to learn.
There have been attempts to define a core vocabulary for English. One is
Ogden’s Basic English, the development of which began in the 1930s. Basic
English contains 850 words and, it is claimed (Ogden, 1944, p. 10), is suffi-
cient for everyday conversations. The problem with Basic English is that it is
not as basic as its name would suggest. The words included are highly
70
76 EDUCARE 2015:1
Vocabulary in textbooks for young learners
polysemous and estimations indicate that, added together, its 850 words
actually have 12,425 different meanings that the learner has to acquire
(Carter & McCarthy, 1988, p. 3).
A later attempt is West’s (1953) A general service list of English words
(GSL), which comprises 2,000 words. The list was compiled from corpus
data and contains information about the different meanings of the words as
well as how frequent these meanings are. Just as Basic English, the words in
the GSL are said to make participation in everyday conversations possible;1
additionally, they also enable the learner to understand approximately 80%
of a text on a general topic (Carter & McCarthy, 1988, p. 7). In more special-
ized texts, much of the semantic burden lies on low-frequency words. For a
learner to able to guess the meaning of unfamiliar words with the help of
context, that kind of text is more likely to require a coverage of 95–98%
(Laufer, 1997, p. 127; Nation & Waring, 1997, p. 10; Schmitt, 2008, p. 330),
which would call for a vocabulary containing the 3,000 most frequent words
(Nation & Waring, 1997, p. 11).
Within the European context, work on establishing lexical thresholds for
different usage levels is longstanding and has resulted in the development of
the Common European framework of reference for languages (CEFR; Coun-
cil of Europe, 2001). The original work preceding the framework (e.g., van
Ek & Trim, 1998a, b) contained word lists indicating a vocabulary of ap-
proximately 1,000 words for basic users (A2 level) and 2,000 words for in-
dependent users (B1 level). Primarily, beginning learners should acquire
highly frequent but neutral words since these can be used in more than one
context (Schmitt, 2000, p. 37). A realistic goal for basic language use could
thus be the 2,000–3,000 most frequent words (ibid., p. 84; see also Stæhr,
2008). An aspect worth mentioning in this context is that there is not much
discussion in the literature about when this goal should be achieved, even
though Cameron (2001, p. 75) maintains that acquiring 500 new words per
year is a reasonable goal for young language learners. From an educational
point of view, it is relevant to consider how much time learners have at their
disposal to acquire a vocabulary of this size since this will have implications
for the creation of national curricula and the planning of teaching in both a
short-term and a long-term perspective as well as for progression in the de-
sign of teaching materials. In the Swedish national curriculum for the com-
pulsory school (Skolverket, 2011), no vocabulary size is mentioned among
the goals to achieve, but “it is reasonable to expect that students should reach
at least the B1 level in English at the end of compulsory school [school year
9] in order to obtain a pass” (Skolverket, 2012, p. 28, my translation). In
theory, then, after 480 hours of English teaching (Skolverket, 2011), students
1
Read (2004, p. 150) is, however, of the opinion that a more realistic figure for everyday communication
is a vocabulary of 3,000 words.
71
EDUCARE 2015:1 77
MARIE NORDLUND
72
78 EDUCARE 2015:1
Vocabulary in textbooks for young learners
Material
The material analysed comprises three textbooks aimed at school years 4–6
in Sweden (pupils aged 10–12 years) from the series Good Stuff, published
by Almqvist & Wiksell/Liber. Together with two other series, it is one of the
most commonly used textbooks in Sweden. The books analysed—Good Stuff
4 (school year 4), Good Stuff 5 (school year 5) and Good Stuff A 2 (school
year 6)—will henceforth be referred to as GS4, GS5 and GSA. The texts in
the books are arranged in thematic units (see Table 1). Both GS4 and GS5
contain eight units, each covering more than one theme (for example,
Sports/School/Friends) and containing a varied number of different texts. In
GSA, each of its thirteen units is devoted to one theme only (for instance,
Sports).
Analysis
To analyse and compare the vocabulary presented in the three books, a cor-
pus of all the texts was compiled. The corpus consists of three sub-corpora,
one for each book. The size of the corpus in number of tokens (running
words) and types (individual words) is shown in Table 2.
2
Good Stuff A is followed by Good Stuff B–D aimed at school years 7–9.
73
EDUCARE 2015:1 79
MARIE NORDLUND
To facilitate analyses and comparison, the corpus has been tagged with in-
formation about word class, number, tense, etc. Tagging has been conducted
according to the principles used for the construction of the British National
Corpus (BNC). To search the corpus the concordancing software tool
MonoConc Pro has been used.
MonoConc Pro counts singular and plural forms of nouns, comparative
and superlative forms of adjectives, and verb inflections as different types.
This has been adjusted manually in Table 2 above. In other words, in this
study, for example, cat-cats, old-older-oldest and ask-asks-asking-asked are
treated as one type each. An additional adjustment has also been made for
the category of nouns. According to the tag key used, names for days,
months, holidays and countries are tagged as proper nouns and, as such,
would not show in the results generated by the software program for a search
of nouns. In this study, they have been treated as common nouns and have
been added to the data analysed.
Even though the whole corpus has been tagged, the present study is lim-
ited to an analysis of the open word classes adjectives, nouns and verbs.
These constitute the three largest categories of content words as well as be-
ing the most important word classes. They also account for a major part of
the material analysed as a whole. The books vary slightly in this respect: in
GS4 the three word classes comprise 46% of the total number of tokens, and
in GS5 and GSA the corresponding figures are 44% and 45%, respectively.
Expressed in types, the dominance of these three word classes is even more
pronounced, with figures ranging between 72 and 78%. The data, after man-
ual adjustments, are presented in Table 3.
Table 3. Number of types per word class and % of total number of types per
book.
Word class GS4 GS5 GSA
Adjectives 155 / 12.0 219 / 13.2 288 / 13.5
Nouns 575 / 44.5 778 / 46.9 932 / 43.8
Verbs 201 / 15.5 292 / 17.6 372 / 17.5
Total 931 / 72.0 1,289 / 77.7 1,592 / 74.8
74
80 EDUCARE 2015:1
Vocabulary in textbooks for young learners
75
EDUCARE 2015:1 81
MARIE NORDLUND
ances and ways of expressing themselves, would be desirable for the de-
velopment of both breadth and depth of vocabulary.
Another difference between the books is the presence of colour terms in
GS4. Only red entered the top list, but added together, colours account for
almost one-fifth of the total number of adjective tokens. The corresponding
figures for colour terms in GS5 and GSA are merely four and seven per cent,
respectively. In English textbooks written for young learners in Sweden,
colour terms are generally of high frequency. A quick and by no means sci-
entific search on Google (5 April 2013) for the terms ‘young learners’ and
‘colours’ also resulted in more than 1.5 million hits, many of them contain-
ing exercises to use in the classroom. GS4 could, thus, be said to follow what
seems to be a set pattern: at an early stage, young learners should learn how
to describe the colour of an object.
If there is little variation among high-frequency adjectives, the opposite
holds for nouns (as shown in Table 5).
As is clear from Table 5, the number of nouns with a frequency of at least
15 tokens is quite similar in GS4 and GS5: 14 and 15 types, respectively.
There is a marked increase in GSA, however, with 40 different types. The
variation between the books as regards high-frequency nouns is consider-
able: only six types are common to all the books (in alphabetical order: dad,
day, friend, mum/mom, school and time).
A closer analysis reveals that the themes, and hence the texts, chosen for
inclusion in the books have a major impact on what words are highest in
frequency. Words like dad and mum/mom are a natural part of texts about
children and their families, but they are also frequent because they are ex-
plicitly named as participants in dialogues. The high number of tokens for
man (GS5 and GSA), boy (GSA) and assistant (GSA) can also be explained in
the same way. This will sometimes skew the vocabulary count. In the 100
million word corpus BNC, assistant has a total frequency of 27 tokens, al-
most the same as in the 41,000 word corpus compiled for the present study.
When the concordances for the GS4 words bed, doctor and monkey are ana-
lysed in more detail, it is revealed that most of the tokens stem from the song
Five little monkeys. If these tokens were not counted, none of the words
would reach the 15-token level. Indeed, monkeys would not have any tokens
at all. In similar ways, it is possible to explain the high frequency of other
nouns in GS4 as well as in GS5 and GSA.
76
82 EDUCARE 2015:1
Vocabulary in textbooks for young learners
In GSA, many of the high-frequency nouns stand out and do not have an
equal position in general corpora. Many of them would not reach a fre-
quency of 15 tokens had it not been for the themes chosen. This is the case
for words such as dragon, snake, pirate, witch, cane toad, narrator and sol-
dier. In the material accessed for the study, it is not stated what rationale is
behind the choice of themes. The decidedly skewed results, favouring words
such as cane toad which have little use in common contexts, lead to the con-
clusion that vocabulary was not incorporated into the planning of the text-
77
EDUCARE 2015:1 83
MARIE NORDLUND
The number of verb types reaching the 40-token level is relatively similar
when the three books are compared: nine, eleven and fourteen types, respec-
tively. As can be clearly seen, be is the most frequent verb in all three books
and represents a large number of tokens: 21% in GS4, 20% in GS5 and 22%
78
84 EDUCARE 2015:1
Vocabulary in textbooks for young learners
Recurrence of words
A higher frequency for a word means that learners are more exposed to it,
something which contributes to learning. For the exposure to have any ef-
fect, however, it should also occur over time. Learners must be given the
opportunity to meet and process the word on several separate occasions in
order to acquire a thorough knowledge of it (Nation, 1993; Webb, 2007),
with the first repetition relatively shortly after the first encounter and then
with prolonged intervals (Klingberg, 2011; Nation, 2003).
The high-frequency words discussed in the previous section constitute
only part of the total number of words in the books. Hence, there are many
words to which the learners are not exposed to the same degree. The cut-off
point for low frequency has been set to a maximum of four tokens because
although most researchers agree that at least ten encounters are needed for a
word to be learned (Matsuoka, 2012, p. 158), a minimum of five encounters
79
EDUCARE 2015:1 85
MARIE NORDLUND
50
40
30 GS4
20 GS5
GSA
10
0
1 time2 times3 times4 times > 4
times
50
40
30 GS4
20 GS5
GSA
10
0
1 time 2 3 4 >4
times times times times
80
86 EDUCARE 2015:1
Vocabulary in textbooks for young learners
The distribution of one-time nouns differs slightly between the books, with a
range of 46%–50%. In GS4 and GS5, low-frequency nouns account for ap-
proximately 83% of the total number of noun types; the corresponding figure
in GSA is about 75%.
Even though the verbs conform to the pattern set by adjectives and nouns,
the low-frequency figures are generally lower. This is illustrated in Figure 3.
About 70% of all verb types in the three books have a frequency of maxi-
mum four tokens.
50
40
30 GS
4
20
10
0
1 time 2 times 3 times 4 times> 4 times
However, the figures presented in Figures 1–3 above are somewhat mislead-
ing. Many of the words with a frequency of two, three or four tokens—and
this applies to all three word classes—occur in the same text or song. Conse-
quently, they have no exposure over time, which makes the learning oppor-
tunities provided by the books even more infrequent. Modifying the results
accordingly yields new figures for one-time words: on average, 54% of ad-
jectives, 59% of nouns and 47% of verbs occur only once.
When the three books are compared to see whether vocabulary is reused
over the series as a whole, the result is similarly discouraging (as shown in
Table 7).
81
EDUCARE 2015:1 87
MARIE NORDLUND
An analysis of the words occurring in two books (one-fifth of the total) re-
veals that the combination GS5+GSA is the most common one. It is also
shown that GS4 has more adjectives and nouns in common with GSA than
with GS5. Whether this is pure coincidence or a conscious form of repetition
on the part of the authors—that is, that words are introduced in school year 4
in order to be reinforced in school year 6—cannot be concluded from the
material at hand. An additional 20% of the words occur in all the three
books, and many of these are also low-frequency words: 27% of common
adjectives, 30% of the nouns and 22% of the shared verbs. With as much as
60% of the words occurring in only one book, the writers seem to have fa-
voured (consciously or not) to opt for vocabulary breadth rather than depth.
However, considering the high number of one-time words, the likelihood of
learners achieving either breadth or depth must be questioned.
Since researchers on vocabulary acquisition have emphasised the impor-
tance of frequency and repetition more than once (Ellis, 2002; Nation, 1990;
Schmitt, 2000), it is alarming to see how many of the words in the data ana-
lysed are of such low frequency. Without proper reuse, efforts spent on
learning new vocabulary will be wasted. Unfortunately, the results of the
present study are not unique. Nation (1990) reports similar figures: “In some
courses, half of the different words occur only once and most of the words
occur less than five times” (p. 44; cf. Matsuoka & Hirsh, 2010, p. 65). Of
course, many of the words can be seen as ‘fillers’, words which only func-
tion as a background, to fill out the text and make it more interesting to read.
As such, they can be seen as less important for classroom treatment. Yet,
many of the low-frequency words are listed in the text glossaries, which, at
least in Sweden, quite often means that they will be part of the vocabulary
homework assigned to learners. Admittedly, this means that learners have to
practice the low-frequency words, which should make them easier to learn
and remember. On the other hand, despite this effort, they are unlikely to
reach the level of exposure needed, thus wasting the time spent on learning
them (Schmitt, 2000, p. 137). In practice, it would of course be impossible to
reuse every word in a textbook to the degree necessary to achieve long-term
retention. Moreover, students are not left on their own with the textbook as
82
88 EDUCARE 2015:1
Vocabulary in textbooks for young learners
Conclusion
There are many teachers in Sweden who, for different reasons, follow the
textbook rigidly and plan all of their teaching in accordance with its structure
and content (Skolverket, 2006). In the teaching material analysed here,
aimed for young learners of English, variation in vocabulary is considerable.
Many low-frequency words means that learners are not sufficiently exposed
to new vocabulary, which in turn is not beneficial for learning, thereby mak-
ing it more difficult for learners to reach the objectives set in the national
curriculum and in the course plans. If such teaching materials are used in the
classroom, teachers will need to go outside the textbook in order to provide
learners with opportunities to engage with new vocabulary. In Swedish
schools today, there are many excellent teachers who enable their pupils to
develop their linguistic knowledge, but their less skilled colleagues need
more support. The importance of having adequate teaching materials, whose
structure and content are based on results from research within the field of
second language acquisition, cannot be emphasised enough.
83
EDUCARE 2015:1 89
MARIE NORDLUND
References
Abello-Contesse, Christián & López-Jiménez, M. Dolores (2010). The
treatment of lexical collocations in EFL textbooks. In María Moreno
Jaén, Fernando Serrano Valverde & María Calzada Pérez (Eds.),
Exploring new paths in language pedagogy: lexis and corpus-based
language teaching (pp. 95–109). London: Equinox.
Aitchison, Jean (2012). Words in the mind: an introduction to the mental
lexicon (4th ed.). Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell.
Biemiller, Andrew (2003). Vocabulary: needed if more children are to read
well. Reading Psychology, 24(3–4), 323–335.
Burton, Graham (2012). Corpora and coursebooks: destined to be strangers
forever? Corpora, 7(1), 91–108.
Cameron, Lynne (2001). Teaching languages to young learners. Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press.
Carter, Ronald & McCarthy, Michael (1988). Vocabulary and language
teaching. London: Longman.
Coady, James (1997). L2 vocabulary acquisition through extensive reading.
In James Coady & Thomas Huckin (Eds.), Second language vocabulary
acquisition: a rationale for pedagogy (pp. 225–237). Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press.
Council of Europe (2001). Common European framework of reference for
languages: learning, teaching, assessment. Strasbourg: Council of
Europe, Modern Languages Division.
Ellis, Nick (2002). Frequency effects in language processing: a review with
implications for theories of implicit and explicit language acquisition.
Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 24(2), 143–188.
Kirk, Steven & Carter, Ronald (2010). Fluency and spoken English. In María
Moreno Jaén, Fernando Serrano Valverde & María Calzada Pérez (Eds.),
Exploring new paths in language pedagogy: lexis and corpus-based
language teaching (pp. 25–38). London: Equinox.
Klingberg, Torkel (2011). Den lärande hjärnan. Om barns minne och
utveckling [The learning brain: on children’s memory and development].
Stockholm: Natur & Kultur.
Laufer, Batia (1997). The lexical plight in second language reading: words
you don’t know, words you think you know, and words you can’t guess.
In James Coady & Thomas Huckin (Eds.), Second language vocabulary
acquisition: a rationale for pedagogy (pp. 20–34). Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press.
Ljung, Magnus (1990). A study of TEFL vocabulary. Stockholm: Almqvist
& Wiksell.
84
90 EDUCARE 2015:1
Vocabulary in textbooks for young learners
Ljung, Magnus (1991). Swedish TEFL meets reality. In Stig Johansson &
Anna-Brita Stenström (Eds.), English computer corpora: topics in
English linguistics (pp. 245–256). Berlin/New York: Mouton de Gruyter.
Matsuoka, Werner (2012). Searching for the right words: creating word lists
to inform EFL learning. In David Hirsh (Ed.), Current perspectives in
second language vocabulary research (pp. 151–177). Bern: Peter Lang.
Matsuoka, Werner & Hirsh, David (2010). Vocabulary learning through
reading: does an ELT course book provide good opportunities? Reading
in a Foreign Language, 22(1), 56–70.
Meara, Paul & Suárez García, Jesús (2010). Missing words: the vocabulary
of BBC Spanish courses for adults. In María Moreno Jaén, Fernando
Serrano Valverde & María Calzada Pérez (Eds.), Exploring new paths in
language pedagogy: lexis and corpus-based language teaching (pp. 77–
84). London: Equinox.
Melka, Francine (1997). Receptive vs. productive aspects of vocabulary. In
Norbert Schmitt & Michael McCarthy (Eds.), Vocabulary: description,
acquisition and pedagogy (pp. 84–102). Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press.
Milton, James (2009). Measuring second language vocabulary acquisition.
Bristol: Multilingual Matters.
Nation, Paul (1990). Teaching and learning vocabulary. New York:
Newbury House.
Nation, Paul (1993). Sixteen principles of language teaching. In Laurie
Bauer & Christine Franzen (Eds.), Of Pavlova, poetry and paradigms:
essays in honour of Harry Orsman (pp. 209–224). Wellington: Victoria
University Press.
Nation, Paul (2001). Learning vocabulary in another language. Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press.
Nation, Paul (2003). Effective ways of building vocabulary knowledge. ESL
Magazine, 6(4), 14–15.
Nation, Paul (2008). Teaching vocabulary: strategies and techniques.
Boston: Heinle Cengage Learning.
Nation, Paul & Waring, Robert (1997). Vocabulary size, text coverage and
word lists. In Norbert Schmitt & Michael McCarthy (Eds.), Vocabulary:
description, acquisition and pedagogy (pp. 6–19). Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press.
Ogden, Charles K. (1944). Basic English: a general introduction with rules
and grammar (9th ed.). London: Kegan Paul, Trench, Trubner & Co.
Phillips, Sarah (1993). Young learners. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Read, John (2000). Assessing vocabulary. Cambridge: Cambridge University
Press.
85
EDUCARE 2015:1 91
MARIE NORDLUND
86
92 EDUCARE 2015:1