Discourse Analysis
Discourse Analysis
Reading list
Discourse analysis
• Brown, Gillian and George Yule.1983. Discourse Analysis. Cambridge:
CUP.
• Chafe, Wallace, 1994. Discourse, Consciousness, and Time. Chicago: The
University of Chicago Press.
• Johnstone, Barbara. 2008. Discourse Analysis. Malden,MA/London:
Blackwell Publishing.
• Liddicoat, Anthony J. 2007. An Introduction to Conversation Analysis.
London/ New York: Continuum.
• Schiffrin, Deborah. 1994. Approaches to Discourse. Cambridge, MA:
Blackwell Publishers Inc.
• Schiffrin, D., Tannen, D. and Hamilton, H.E. (eds.). 2001. The Handbook of
Discourse Analysis. Malden, MA and Oxford: Blackwell.
• Tsui, A.M.B. 1994. English Conversation. Oxford: OUP.
• van Dijk, T. (ed.). 1997. Discourse as Structure and Process. London and
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, Inc.
• van Dijk, T. (ed.). 1997. Discourse as Social Interaction. London and
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, Inc.
• Woods, Nicola. 2006. Describing Discourse: A Practical Guide to
Discourse Analysis. London: Hodder Arnold.
Corpus linguistics
• Kennedy, Graeme. 1998. An Introduction to Corpus Linguistics.
London: Addison Wesley Longman Ltd.
• McEnery, Tony et al. 2006. Corpus-Based Language Studies: An advanced
resource book. London & New York: Routledge.
• Meyer, Charles F. 2002. English Corpus Linguistics: an introduction.
Cambridge: CUP.
• Teubert, Wolfgang & Krishnamurthy (eds.), R. 2007. Corpus Linguistics (6
volume set). London & New York: Routledge.
Research methodology
• Wray, Alison & Bloomer, Aileen. 2006. Projects in Linguistics:
A Practical Guide to Researching Language. London: Hodder
Arnold.
Suggested projects
level of analysis
4) Style: stylistic analysis
5) Rhetoric: rhetorical analysis
6) Schemata: schematic structures or superstructures
• Discourse as Action and Interaction
Speech acts
Conversation as interaction
• Cognition
Mind, memory
Mental processes and representations
• Discourse and society
Social power
Gender
Ethnicity
Culture
CDA
Types of discourse analysis
1) those which focus on discourse “itself”, that is , on structures
of text or talk;
2) those which study discourse and communication as
cognition
3) those which focus on social structure and culture
• Point of possible
turn-transfer
next speaker
self-select
Conversation Analysis
Sacks et al. point out that this system operates at the end
of every turn or what they call “locally”, rather than on an
overall or “global” basis. In other words, turn allocation
cannot be agreed in advance at the beginning of the
conversation, but must be continually renegotiated at each
TCU boundary. The system has one aim: to ensure that
when the current speaker finishes his turn at talk, some
other speaker will start talking. The conversation analysts
thus modeled conversation as an infinitely generative turn-
taking machine, whose design suggests that the major
concern of interactants is to avoid lapse: the possibility that
no one is speaking.
Conversation Analysis
Given that conversation is driven by a turn-taking
machinery expressly designed to keep going, it
becomes quite problematic to determine how a
conversation could ever stop. It was in exploring an
answer to this issue of conversational closure that CA
made what many see as their most significant
contribution to the analysis of interaction: the
identification of the adjacency pair.
Conversation Analysis
Sacks et al. noticed that the occurrence of the
second turn can be explained by the first: a question in
some ways implies that the next turn will be an answer.
Adjacency pairs were identified as typically having
three characteristics:
a. two utterance length
b. adjacent positioning of component utterances
c. different speakers producing each utterance
Conversation Analysis
Adjacency pairs were identified as functioning as a turn-
transfer technique, i.e. they function both to allocate the
next turn, and to exit from the current turn. It is important to
understand that the system is not one of determination,
but of expectation. Given that a first pair part has been
produced, there is a very strong likelihood that the
addressed participant will be the next speaker, and will
produce a relevant second pair part.
e.g.
A: Who was the first president of the United States?
B: George Washington.
Conversation Analysis
The conversation analysts recognized that in fact there
are two types of second pair parts. There is first a
preferred second pair part. e.g. question/answer,
demand/compliance, offer/accept, request/grant etc.
However, it is also possible for the addressee to
produce some kind of discretionary alternative--- what
in CA terms is referred to as a dispreferred second pair
part.
Conversation Analysis
e.g.
(1) A: Who was the first president of the United States?
B: You could ask my five-year-old kid.
(2) A: Are you still having some sort of contact with your
former husband?
B: I’m afraid that’s a rather personal question.
Conversation Analysis
• The identification of the adjacency pair
was the basis for two further developments
in CA. The first was the recognition of
sequences longer than two units, and the
second was the formulation of the theoretical
concept of sequential relevance/sequential
implicativeness.
Conversation Analysis
• e.g.
1. C: And have you got…the… first day covers 首日封 of…?
2. S: Yes.
3. C: (Anzac)
4. S: How many would you like?
5. C: Four please.
6. S: Two of each?
7. C: What have you got?
8. S: There’s two different designs on the-
[5 secs---S shows C the covers]
9. C: I’ll take two of each.
10.S: uhum.
Conversation Analysis
Yours …
• A: Where is Bill?
• B: There is a yellow VW outside Sue’s house.
flouting the maxim of quality
Everybody knows A betrays B.
• A is a fine friend. (irony)
• Queen Victoria was made of iron.
(metaphor)
Someone broke all the furniture.
• He is a little intoxicated. (meiosis)
• Every nice girl loves a sailor. (hyperbole)
flouting the maxim of relevance
• A: The hostess is an awful bore,
don’t you think?
B: The roses are lovely, aren’t they?
flouting the maxim of manner
cancellability
Dear Sir,
Mr. X’s command of English is excellent and his
attendance at tutorials has been regular. However, his
ability at and enthusiasm for
philosophy are quite adequate for the job.
Yours …
non-detachability
The conversational implicature is attached to the semantic content of
what is said, not to the linguistic form used.
Therefore it is possible to use a synonym and keep the
implicature intact. In other words, the implicature will not be
detached, separated from the utterance as a whole,
even though the specific words may be changed.
e.g.
a-d said ironically will all implicate e
a. John is a genius.
b. John is a mental prodigy.
c. John is an enormous intellect.
d. John is a big brain.
e. John is an idiot.
• Calculability
• 4. off record
(18c) I’m sorry I have to ask, but could you lend me a hundred dollars?
(negative politeness)
(18d) Oh no, I’m out of cash! I forgot to go to the bank today. (off record)
The strategies are numbered according to their degree of politeness.
(Strategy 5 is, from this point on, left out of consideration.) If the W of a
FTA is high, the speaker will choose a strategy with a higher number.
This explains why grave accusations or inconvenient requests are often
formulated indirectly (strategy 4).
Pragmatics
The Goffman approach and Brown and Levinson’s theory provide an adequate
research framework for determining gradations of politeness and for analyzing
indirect language. The following question is an example of an indirect request.
(19) Are you doing anything special tonight?
The form of this utterance makes it clear that this is an inquiry about an
individual’s planned activities. This question can, however, also be an invitation
on the part of the speaker to the hearer to go out together.
How can a question in this form have an entirely different function? According
to Levinson (1983), the answer is that in some cases speakers first make a pre-
request in order to find out whether they will get a positive response to their
request. Levinson describes this in an underlying structure consisting of four
positions. Below is an example and the underlying structure.
(20) A: (1) Are you doing anything special tonight?
B: (2) No, not really. Why?
A: (3) Well, I wanted to ask if you would like to go out to
dinner with me.
B: (4) I’d love to.
Pragmatics
The underlying structure of (20):
(1) Pre-request
(2) ‘Go ahead’ reaction
(3) Request
(4) Consent
Goffman’s work on ‘face’ offers an explanation for the pre-
request phenomenon. If B had given an evasive answer to the
pre-request, then that would have eliminated the necessity of
making the main request, preventing the loss of face of either
participant. A does not have to deal with a refusal and B does
not have to refuse the request in a direct manner; after the pre-
request, B can claim to be extremely busy which will soften
the blow of the refusal.
Pragmatics
Indirect requests have certain similarities with pre-requests in that both are attempts to
ascertain whether or not there are grounds for refusing a direct request. Consider the
following example. A customer walks into a shoestore and asks:
(21) Do you sell jogging shoes?
This question is actually a preliminary check to see if the sales clerk will be able to
give an affirmative response to a request to see an assortment of jogging shoes. In
Levinson’s opinion, indirect requests can be viewed as pre-requests in an underlying
structure consisting of four positions.
(22) A: (1) Do you sell jogging shoes?
B: (2) Yes.
A: (3) Would you show me some, please?
B: (4) I’ll go get them for you.
In many cases the reaction to a pre-request is the same as to the direct request.
(23) A: (1) Do you sell jogging shoes?
B: (2) Yes, I’ll show you some.
A: (3) Thank you.
This reduction can be explained with the politeness strategy. It ensures that the
customer does not lose face; the customer is no longer obliged to formulate a direct
request.
Pragmatics
Presupposition
• A presupposition is something the speaker assumes
to be the case prior to making an utterance.
• Presuppositions are inferences about what is assumed to be true in
the utterance rather than directly asserted to be true (Peccei, J.
1999).
X: Your car
Y: You have a car.
• Your car >> You have a car.
a. John didn’t manage to pass his exams.>> John tried to pass his
exams.
b. John didn’t manage to pass his exams, in fact he didn’t even try
to.
c. John didn’t manage to pass his exams, if indeed he even tried.
d. Either John never tried to pass his exams, or he tried but he
never managed to pass them.
e. John didn’t manage to pass his exams; he got through without
even trying.
In b, c, d, e, the presupposition of the first clause “John tried to pass
his exams” has dropped out just because of the propositions
expressed by the rest of the clauses of the complex whole.
Small differences, big difference:
Interactional Sociolinguistics
• 1. A: excuse me David//
• 2. B: yeah
• 3. A: could I have those sheets back please//
• 4. B: those sheets?/ I put on your desk yesterday//
• 5. A: haven’t seen on my↑desk//
• 6. B: ah/ I’m sure I did/ I’m sure I did.
• 7. A: no/no/ I’ve not seen on my desk/you’re/are you sure?
• 8. B: absolutely certain/ it said on the top/ checklist for April// those
9. are the ones aren’t they//
10. A: no/no/ not checklists/ I want the timesheets//
11. B: oh the timesheets//sorry/yes/I’ve got them here//
• Trainees who hear a tape of this exchange---and also a
tape in which the same scenario was role-played by
two Anglo-British speakers--- commented that the two
speakers lacked rapport and that the Indian English
speaker sometimes sounded ‘extremely angry’ or
‘slightly offensive’.
Interactional Sociolinguistics
• A: What have you been doing since you were made redundant?
• B: Nothing.
• The question is actually a veiled invitation to the
candidate to reassure the selectors that s/he has
not simply been doing nothing since losing a
previous job. But since this is not made explicit
in the wording of the question, understanding it
depends on sharing, or at least being aware of,
certain culturally specific ideas about work and
the lack of it.
Interactional Sociolinguistics
• In the culture the interviewer belongs to, unemployment is often
regarded less as a misfortune than as a moral failing--- a sign
that the unemployed person does not have the necessary ‘work
ethic’( 职 业 道 德 ). Where a job applicant is not currently
employed, s/he will often be expected to provide evidence that
s/he is not merely idle and apathetic. But speaker B in the above
exchange fails to understand this; he takes it that the interviewer
is concerned simply to elicit facts about his present situation,
and answers accordingly. The example shows that ‘crosstalk’
between people of differing cultural backgrounds is not just
a matter of surface linguistic features but also of the
assumptions language-users make about the kind of speech
event they are participating in and what is appropriate or
‘normal’ in that context.
5. Ideology: critical discourse analysis ( CDA)
Racist discourse