The document discusses threats to internal and external validity in experimental research. It defines internal validity as the ability to determine if a causal relationship exists between variables. It also defines external validity and threats that can limit generalizing results to the overall population. It provides examples of 6 common threats to internal validity and 3 threats to external validity.
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PPT, PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
0 ratings0% found this document useful (0 votes)
25 views23 pages
Experimental Validity
The document discusses threats to internal and external validity in experimental research. It defines internal validity as the ability to determine if a causal relationship exists between variables. It also defines external validity and threats that can limit generalizing results to the overall population. It provides examples of 6 common threats to internal validity and 3 threats to external validity.
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PPT, PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
You are on page 1/ 23
CHERRY ROSE L.
SORROSA MA in Science Education (Biology) EXPERIMENTAL VALIDITY
Experimental validity refers to the
manner in which variables influences both the results of the research and the generalizability to the population at large. THREATS TO INTERNAL AND EXTERNAL VALIDITY: Is the researcher’s conclusion correct?
Are the changes in the independent variable
indeed responsible for the observed variation in the dependent variable? Is it possible that the variation in the dependent variable can be attributed to other factors? INTERNAL VALIDITY Internal validity refers to a study’s ability to determine if a causal relationship exists between one or more independent variables and one or more dependent variables.
In other words, can we be reasonably sure that
the change (or lack of change) was caused by the treatment? . WHY IS INTERNAL VALIDITY IMPORTANT? If the study shows a high degree of internal validity, then we can conclude that we have strong evidence of causality.
If a study has low internal validity, then we
can conclude that the study has little or no evidence of causality. 1. HISTORY History refers to any event outside of the research study that can alter or effect subjects’ performance.
The events occurring between the first and
second measurements in addition to the experimental variable which might affect the measurement. Using randomization procedures can often minimize this risk, assuring that outside events that occur in one group are also likely to occur in the other.
Example: Researcher collects gross sales data
before and after a 5 day 50% off sale. During the sale a hurricane occurs and results of the study may be affected because of the hurricane, not the sale. 2. MATURATION Maturation refers to the natural physiological or psychological changes that take place as we age.
The process of maturing which takes place in the
individual during the duration of the experiment which is not a result of specific events but of simply growing older, growing more tired, or similar changes. Example: Subjects become tired after completing a training session, and their responses on the Posttest are affected.
For instance, an episode of major depression typically
decreases significantly within a six-month period even without treatment. Imagine we tested a new medication designed to treat depression. If our results showed that subjects who took this medication showed a significant decrease in depressive symptoms within six months, could we truly say that the medication caused the decrease in symptoms? Probably not, especially since maturation alone would have shown similar results. 3. PRE-TESTING The effect created on the second measurement by having a measurement before the experiment.
Example: Subjects take a Pretest and think about
some of the items. On the Posttest they change to answers they feel are more acceptable. Experimental group learns from the pretest. 4. MEASURING INSTRUMENTS Changes in instruments, calibration of instruments, observers, or scorers may cause changes in the measurements.
If the measurement device(s) used in your study
changes during the course of the study, changes in scores may be related to the instrument rather than the independent variable. Example: Interviewers are very careful with their first two or three interviews but on the 4th, 5th, 6th become fatigued and are less careful and make errors.
For instance, if your pretest and posttest are
different, the change in scores may be a result of the second test being easier than the first rather than the teaching method employed. For this reason, it is recommended that pre- and posttests be identical or at least highly correlated. 5. STATISTICAL REGRESSION It refers to the tendency for subjects who score very high or very low to score more toward the mean on subsequent testing.
Groups are chosen because of extreme scores of
measurements; those scores or measurements tend to move toward the mean with repeated measurements even without an experimental variable. Example: Managers who are performing poorly are selected for training. Their average Posttest scores will be higher than their Pretest scores because of statistical regression, even if no training were given.
If you get a 99% on a test, for instance, the odds
that your score will be lower the second time are much greater than the odds of increasing your score. 6. EXPERIMENTAL MORTALITY The loss of subjects from comparison groups could greatly affect the comparisons because of unique characteristics of those subjects. Groups to be compared need to be the same after as before the experiment.
Example: Over a 6 month experiment aimed to change
accounting practices, 12 accountants drop out of the experimental group and none drop out of the control group. Not only is there differential loss in the two groups, but the 12 dropouts may be very different from those who remained in the experimental group. EXTERNAL VALIDITY External validity refers to the generalizability of a study.
In other words, can we be reasonably sure that
the results of our study consisting of a sample of the population truly represents the entire population? Threats to external validity can result in significant results within a sample group but an inability for this to be generalized to the population at large.
External Validity asks to what populations,
settings, treatment variables, and measurement variables can this observed effect be generalized.
Threats to external validity compromise our
confidence in stating whether the study’s result are applicable to other groups. 1. PRE-TESTING Individuals who were pretested might be less or more sensitive to the experimental variable or might have "learned" from the pre-test making them unrepresentative of the population who had not been pre-tested.
Example: Prior to viewing a film on Environmental
Effects of Chemical, a group of subjects is given a 60 item antichemical test. Taking the Pretest may increase the effect of the film. The film may not be effective for a nonpretested group. 2. DIFFERENTIAL SELECTION The selection of the subjects determines how the findings can be generalized. Subjects selected from a small group or one with particular characteristics would limit generalizability. Randomly chosen subjects from the entire population could be generalized to the entire population.
Example: Researcher, requesting permission to conduct
experiment, is turned down by 11 corporations, but the 12th corporation grant permission. The 12th corporation is obviously different then the others because they accepted. Thus subjects in the 12th corporation may be more accepting or sensitive to the treatment. 3. EXPERIMENTAL PROCEDURES The experimental procedures and arrangements have a certain amount of effect on the subjects in the experimental settings. Generalization to persons not in the experimental setting may be precluded.
Example: Department heads realize they are
being studied, try to guess what the experimenter wants and respond accordingly rather than respond to the treatment.