Jump to content

Q methodology

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia

Q methodology is a research method used in psychology and in social sciences to study people's "subjectivity"—that is, their viewpoint. Q was developed by psychologist William Stephenson. It has been used both in clinical settings for assessing a patient's progress over time (intra-rater comparison), as well as in research settings to examine how people think about a specific topic (inter-rater comparisons).

Technical overview

[edit]

The name "Q" comes from the form of factor analysis that is used to analyze the data. Normal factor analysis, called "R method," involves finding correlations between variables (say, height and age) across a sample of subjects. Q, on the other hand, looks for correlations between subjects across a sample of variables. Q factor analysis reduces the many individual viewpoints of the subjects down to a few "factors," which are claimed to represent shared ways of thinking. It is sometimes said that Q factor analysis is R factor analysis with the data table turned sideways. While helpful as a heuristic for understanding Q, this explanation may be misleading, as most Q methodologists[who?] argue that for mathematical reasons no one data matrix would be suitable for analysis with both Q and R.[citation needed]

The data for Q factor analysis come from a series of "Q sorts" performed by one or more subjects. A Q sort is a ranking of variables—typically presented as statements printed on small cards—according to some "condition of instruction." For example, in a Q study of people's views of a celebrity, a subject might be given statements like "He is a deeply religious man" and "He is a liar," and asked to sort them from "most like how I think about this celebrity" to "least like how I think about this celebrity." The use of ranking, rather than asking subjects to rate their agreement with statements individually, is meant to capture the idea that people think about ideas in relation to other ideas, rather than in isolation. Usually, this ranking is done on a score sheet with the most salient options at the extreme ends of the sheet, such as "most agree" and "most disagree".[1] This score sheet is usually in the form of a bell curve where a respondent can place most Q sorts at the middle and the fewest Q sorts at the far ends of the score sheets.

The sample of statements for a Q sort is drawn from and claimed by the researcher to be representative of a "concourse"—the sum of all things people say or think about the issue being investigated. Commonly, Q methodologists use a structured sampling approach in order to try and represent the full breadth of the concourse.

One salient difference between Q and other social science research methodologies, such as surveys, is that it typically uses many fewer subjects. This can be a strength, as Q is sometimes used with a single subject, and it makes research far less expensive. In such cases, a person will rank the same set of statements under different conditions of instruction. For example, someone might be given a set of statements about personality traits and then asked to rank them according to how well they describe herself, her ideal self, her father, her mother, etc. Working with a single individual is particularly relevant in the study of how an individual's rankings change over time and this was the first use of Q methodology. As Q methodology works with a small non-representative sample, conclusions are limited to those who participated in the study.

In studies of intelligence, Q factor analysis can generate consensus based assessment (CBA) scores as direct measures. Alternatively, the unit of measurement of a person in this context is his factor loading for a Q-sort he or she performs. Factors represent norms with respect to schemata. The individual who gains the highest factor loading on an Operant factor is the person most able to conceive the norm for the factor. What the norm means is a matter, always, for conjecture and refutation (Popper). It may be indicative of the wisest solution, or the most responsible, the most important, or an optimized-balanced solution. These are all untested hypotheses that require future study.

An alternative method that determines the similarity among subjects somewhat like Q methodology, as well as the cultural "truth" of the statements used in the test, is Cultural Consensus Theory.

The "Q sort" data collection procedure is traditionally done using a paper template and the sample of statements or other stimuli printed on individual cards. However, there are also computer software applications for conducting online Q sorts. For example, UC Riverside's Riverside Situational Q-sort (RSQ), claims to measure the psychological properties of situations. Their International Situations Project[2] is using the tool to explore the psychologically salient aspects of situations and how those aspects may differ across cultures with this university-developed web-based application. To date there has been no study of differences in sorts produced by use of computer based vs. physical sorting.

One Q-sort should produce two sets of data. The first is the physical distribution of sorted objects. The second is either an ongoing 'think-out-loud' narrative or a discussion that immediately follows the sorting exercise. The purpose of these narratives were, in the first instance, to elicit discussion of the reasons for particular placements. While the relevance of this qualitative data is often suppressed in current uses of Q-methodology, the modes of reasoning behind placement of an item can be more analytically relevant than the absolute placement of cards.

Application

[edit]

Q-methodology has been used as a research tool in a wide variety of disciplines including nursing, veterinary medicine, public health, transportation, education, rural sociology, hydrology, mobile communication, and even robotics.[3][4][5][6][7][8] The methodology is particularly useful when researchers wish to understand and describe the variety of subjective viewpoints on an issue.[1]

Validation

[edit]

Some information on validation of the method is available.[9] Furthermore, the issue of validity concerning the Q-method has been discussed variously. However, Lundberg et al. point out that "[s]ince participants’ Q sorts are neither right nor wrong, but constructed through respondents’ rank-ordering of self-referent items, validity in line with quantitative tenets of research is of no concern in Q".[10]

Criticism of Q methodology

[edit]

In 2013, an article was published under the title "Overly ambitious: contributions and current status of Q methodology" written by Jarl K. Kampen & Peter Tamás.[11] Kampen & Tamás state that "Q methodology neither delivers its promised insight into human subjectivity nor accounts adequately for threats to the validity of the claims it can legitimately make".[11] This in turn makes the method, according to the authors, "inappropriate for its declared purpose".[11]

In response to Kampen & Tamás's criticism, Steven R. Brown, Stentor Danielson & Job van Exel published the response article "Overly ambitious critics and the Medici Effect: a reply to Kampen and Tamás".[12] Brown et al. states that since its inception, Q methodology "has been a recurring target of hastily assembled critiques"[12] which has served no other purpose than to misinform other researchers and readers. Due to the amount of criticism towards the Q methodology, Brown et al. gather these criticisms under the term the Medici Effect, named after the famed family that denied Galileo Galilei's evidence while refusing to look through his telescope.

Brown et al. continue by responding to certain points of Kampen & Tamás's criticisms:

  1. On the nature of subjectivity
  2. Concourse and Q samples
  3. Factor analysis and interpretation
  4. The force Q-sort distribution
  5. Items:persons ratio
  6. Researcher bias
  7. Miscellany

One point which the authors point out in section 3 is that Kampen & Tamás seek to claim that "the limits in the number of factors that Q can produce"[12] defies logic because "Q can identify no more factors than there are Q statements".[12] This argument, however, Brown et al. "have never before encountered".[12] The authors continue on this argument by stating that "the data of Q methodology are not responses to individual statements alone, but more importantly in their relationships, as when they are rank-ordered".[12]

In their conclusion, Brown et al. point out that, much like Medici's refusal to look through Galileo's telescope, "these critics have failed to engage personally with Q in order to see if their abstract critiques hold up in practice".[12]

See also

[edit]

References

[edit]
  1. ^ a b Van Exel NJA, G de Graaf. 2005: "Q methodology: A sneak preview" (PDF). Archived from the original (PDF) on January 24, 2013. Retrieved January 2, 2013.
  2. ^ "International Situations Project". Archived from the original on 2 November 2017.
  3. ^ Q methodology net. "Q methodology database by subject". Archived from the original on 14 September 2009. Retrieved 20 September 2008.
  4. ^ Bracken, Stacey Storch; Janet E. Fischel (2006). "Assessment of preschool classroom practices: Application of Q-sort methodology". Early Childhood Research Quarterly. 21 (4): 417–430. doi:10.1016/j.ecresq.2006.09.006.
  5. ^ Previte, Josephine; Janet E. Fischel; et al. (2007). "Q Methodology and Rural Research" (PDF). Sociologia Ruralis. 47 (2): 135–147. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9523.2007.00433.x. hdl:20.500.11937/17462.
  6. ^ Raadgever, G. T.; Mostert, E.; van de Giesen, N. C.; et al. (2008). "Identification of stakeholder perspectives on future flood management in the Rhine basin using Q methodology". Hydrology and Earth System Sciences. 12 (4): 1097–1109. Bibcode:2008HESS...12.1097R. doi:10.5194/hess-12-1097-2008.
  7. ^ Liu, Chung-Chu. "Mobile phone user types by Q methodology: an exploratory research". Retrieved 20 September 2008.
  8. ^ Mettler, T.; Sprenger, M.; Winter, R. (2017). "Service robots in hospitals: new perspectives on niche evolution and technology affordances" (PDF). European Journal of Information Systems. 26 (5): 451–468. doi:10.1057/s41303-017-0046-1. S2CID 27579290.
  9. ^ Buckley, Maureen E.; Klein, Daniel; Durbin, C. Emily; Hayden, Elizabeth; Moerk, Kirstin; et al. (2002). "Development and Validation of a Q-Sort Procedure to Assess Temperament and Behavior in Preschool-Age Children". Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology. 31 (4): 525–539. doi:10.1207/S15374424JCCP3104_11. PMID 12402571. S2CID 11292218.
  10. ^ Lundberg, Adrian; de Leeuw, Renske; Aliani, Renata (November 2020). "Using Q methodology: Sorting out subjectivity in educational research". Educational Research Review. 31. doi:10.1016/j.edurev.2020.100361. ISSN 1747-938X – via Science Direct.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: date and year (link)
  11. ^ a b c Kampen, Jarl K.; Tamás, Peter (2014-11-01). "Overly ambitious: contributions and current status of Q methodology". Quality & Quantity. 48 (6): 3109–3126. doi:10.1007/s11135-013-9944-z. ISSN 1573-7845.
  12. ^ a b c d e f g Brown, Steven R.; Danielson, Stentor; van Exel, Job (March 2015). "Overly ambitious critics and the Medici Effect: a reply to Kampen and Tamás". Quality & Quantity. 49 (2): 523–537. doi:10.1007/s11135-014-0007-x. ISSN 0033-5177.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: date and year (link)
pFad - Phonifier reborn

Pfad - The Proxy pFad of © 2024 Garber Painting. All rights reserved.

Note: This service is not intended for secure transactions such as banking, social media, email, or purchasing. Use at your own risk. We assume no liability whatsoever for broken pages.


Alternative Proxies:

Alternative Proxy

pFad Proxy

pFad v3 Proxy

pFad v4 Proxy