MITHST 725S09 Lec16 Rhy PDF
MITHST 725S09 Lec16 Rhy PDF
Rhythm: patterns
of events in time
Thursday, May 14, 2009 Courtesy of John Hart (http://nanobliss.com). Used with permission.
1. Rhythmic pattern induction & expectation
chunking of repeating patterns
2. Meter -
the inferred metrical grid
Tempo
Ranges of events; intervals from 50 ms to 2 sec
Too short: events fuse
Too long: successive events don't cohere, interact
Pitch (> 30 Hz); infra-pitch (10-30 Hz); rhythm (<
10 Hz)
For a brisk tempo of 120 bpm, 2 Hz,
a quarter note is 500 msec (2 Hz)
an eighth note is 250 msec (4 Hz)
a sixteenth note is 125 ms (8 Hz)
a 32nd note is 62 ms (16 Hz)
10
Repetition
of a
rhythmic
pattern
establishes
the pattern Image removed due to copyright restrictions.
a) Two measure rhythmic pattern.
b) Complete 2-bar pattern, followed by a repetition of the complete pattern.
c) Complete 2-bar pattern, followed by two repetitions of the 2nd measure.
d) Complete 2-bar pattern, followed by two repetitions of the 2nd measure in reverse.
e) Complete 2-bar pattern; unique 3rd measure, and then a repetition of the 2nd measure.
Music Theory,
Fig. 2.4 in Sethares, W. A. Rhythm and Transforms. Springer, 2007. ISBN: 9781846286391. Preview in Google Books.
see Sethares, 2007. Necklace notation: Safi al-Din al-Urmawi 13th c. Bagdad Book of Cycles
Thursday, May 14, 2009
Fig. 2.5 in Sethares, W. A. Rhythm and Transforms. Springer, 2007. ISBN: 9781846286391.
Preview in Google Books .
Sethares, 2007
Thursday, May 14, 2009
Hierarchy & time order (Snyder, Music & Memory, MIT Press, 2000)
0
1
2
3
1 = 11 ms = recurrence time
of input pattern10101100101
Input pattern
1010110010110101100101101011001011010...
Thursday, May 14, 2009
Coincidence ∆ -mbre
units
Direct inputs
Figure by MIT OpenCourseWare.
Input time sequence
∆ dura-on
2:
3:
4:
6:
Pulse
http://www.music.indiana.edu/som/courses/rhythm/illustrations
Thursday, May 14, 2009
Visual grouping
Dember & Bagwell, 1985, A history of perception, Topics in the History of Psychology, Kimble & Schlesinger, eds.
Accent causes
grouping which
determines
perceived rhythmic
pattern
Rhythm is a
perceptual
attribute
http://www.music.indiana.edu/som/courses/rhythm/illustrations
Thursday, May 14, 2009
http://www.music.indiana.edu/som/courses/rhythm/illustrations
Thursday, May 14, 2009
Fig. 1. Pure-tone (262 Hz, 50 ms duration) stimulus patterns are shown with inter-onset intervals of 390 ms (above) and schematized metrical accent
representations (below). The periodic control condition consisted of isochronous tones designed to elicit a simple pulse perception (A). The binary control
condition consisted of alternating loud and soft tones, designed to elicit a duple meter perception (B). The omit-loud condition consisted of the binary control
pattern with missing loud tones on 30% of two-tone cycles (C). The omit-soft condition consisted of the binary control pattern with missing soft tones on 30%
of two-tone cycles (D).
35
Source: Snyder, J. S., and E. W. Large. "Gamma-band Activity Reflects the Metric
Structure of Rhythmic Tone Sequences." Cog Brain Res 24 (2005): 117-126.
Thursday, May 14, 2009 Courtesy Elsevier, Inc., http://www.sciencedirect.com. Used with permission.
120 J.S. Snyder, E.W. Large / Cognitive Brain Research 24 (2005) 117–126
Figure 4. Courtesy of University of Finance and Management, Warsaw. Used with permission.
(a) Time-frequency representation of the evoked and induced GBA results, averaged over all subjects. Tone onset occurs at
zero and 390 ms. (b) Comparison of induced/evoked peak activity in the presence and absence of loud and soft tones.
Source: Snyder, J. S., and E. W. Large. "Gamma-band Activity Reflects the Metric Structure of
Rhythmic Tone Sequences." Cog Brain Res 24 (2005): 117-126.
Courtesy Elsevier, Inc., http://www.sciencedirect.com. Used with permission.
Figure 7. Tone omissions: induced and evoked GBA.
Thursday, May 14, 2009
Figure 5.
Perturbed stimuli; ‘x’ represents tone onset.
Courtesy of University of Finance and Management,
Warsaw. Used with permission.
Thursday, May 14, 2009
Evoked GBA
Figure 6. Courtesy of University of Finance and Management, Warsaw. Used with permission.
Time-frequency representation of the evoked and induced GBA in response to early, late, or on-time tones averaged over all
subjects. The white dashed line represents where a tone was expected. (a) Evoked activity is predicted by the presence of
tones. The white box highlights an exception, activity where the tone was expected in the case of an early tone. (b) The white
box indicates a peak in the induced activity where the tone was expected for the case of late tones.
Figure 6. Courtesy of University of Finance and Management, Warsaw. Used with permission.
Time-frequency representation of the evoked and induced GBA in response to early, late, or on-time tones averaged over all
subjects. The white dashed line represents where a tone was expected. (a) Evoked activity is predicted by the presence of
tones. The white box highlights an exception, activity where the tone was expected in the case of an early tone. (b) The white
box indicates a peak in the induced activity where the tone was expected for the case of late tones.
SUMMARY
Evoked GBA appears to represent sensory processing
as predicted by the presence of tones, much like the
MLR. Induced GBA may reflect temporally precise ex
pectancies for strongly and weakly accented events in
sound patterns. Moreover, induced GBA behaves in a
manner consistent with perception-action coordination
studies using perturbed temporal sequences. Taken
together, the characteristics of induced GBA provide
evidence for an active, dynamic system capable of
making predictions (i.e., anticipation), encoding metri
cal patterns and recovering from unexpected stimuli.
GBA appears to be a useful neuroelectric correlate
of rhythmic expectation and may therefore reflect pulse
perception. Due to the anticipatory nature of GBA, it
may be supposed there is an attentional dependence.
Future research should aim to manipulate attentional
state, localize neural sources and further probe the
role of induced GBA in meter perception.
Thursday, May 14, 2009 Courtesy of University of Finance and Management, Warsaw. Used with permission.
• Timbre effects
• Pitch difference
47
Smulevitch & Povel (2000) in Rhythm: Perception & Production, Desain & Windsor eds
Rhythmic
Hierarchy
Handel
Thursday, May 14, 2009
Rhythmic Hierarchy
Handel
Thursday, May 14, 2009
Polyrhythms
Polyrhythms (polyrhythms:rhythm::polyphony:melody)
Source: Handel, S. Listening: An Introduction to
the Perception of Auditory Events. Cambridge,
MA: MIT Press, 1989.
Courtesy of MIT Press. Used with permission.
Conlon Nancarrow
Handel
Thursday, May 14, 2009
Rhythm & Grouping
Timescales
& memory�
Memory
& grouping �
(Snyder, Music & Memory)
Time
"time…does not exist without changes." Aristotle, Physics, IV
"A man in sound sleep, or strongly occupy'd with one thought, is insensible of time… Whenever we
have no successive peceptions, we have no notion of time, even tho' there be a real succession
in the objects…time cannot make its appearance to the mind, either alone, or attended with a
steady unchangeable object, but is always discovered by some perceivable succession of
changeable objects." Hume as quoted in Fraisse, pp. 3-4
Measurement of time
How is time measured, psychologically, by the neural mechanisms and informational
organizations that constitute our minds?
Duration 60 BPM
Our sense of the length of time (Fraisse, 1962, The Psychology of Time)
152 BPM
Constant Weber fractions for interval estimation
Errors are proportional to the interval estimated
Weber's law for timing; jnd's on the order of 8-12%
depending on modality (hearing, touch, vision)
Temporal prediction of reward in conditioning
(Scalar timing intimately related to the response latency in conditioning when interval
between stimulus and reward are varied, see R. Church, A Concise Introduction to Scalar
Timing Theory, 2003. See also Fraisse's (1963) discussion of Pavlov and Popov
cyclochronism model)
Some general observations (Fraisse via Snyder, Music & Memory):
Filled time durations appear shorter than empty ones
Rate of novel events makes durations appear shorter
(monotonous durations are experienced as longer, but remembered as shorter)
Aging: young children overestimate durations; older adults underestimate durations
(A systematic change in internal timing mechanisms with age? cf �absolute pitch)
Implications for music: pieces with high event densities go faster; those
with low ones seem to take forever; duration is in the mind of the
beholder and his/her expectations
Thursday, May 14, 2009
Beat
induction
and
duration
discrimination
Weber's
Law Image removed due to copyright restrictions.
Graph illustrating Weber's Law. See Fig. 4.13 in Jones and Yee,
Succession
Time order:
before and after (Fraisse, Snyder)
Warren:
Holistic &
analytic
sequence
recognition
holistic:
temporal
compounds
Image removed due to copyright restrictions.
analytic:
explicit ID
of elements
and orders
• On all timescales:
– mechanisms for internalizing timecourses of
"transparency", non-interference
Relative delay
Vowel [ae]
F0 = 100 Hz
Vowel [er]
F0 = 125 Hz
Double vowel
[ae]+[er]
0 10 20 30 40 50
Time (msec)
Thursday, May 14, 2009
Auditory "pop-out"
phenomena suggest
Last 2 periods - first 2
a period-by-period
transient
disparity
ongoing
disparity
0
1
2
3
1 = 11 ms = recurrence time
of input pattern10101100101
Input pattern
1010110010110101100101101011001011010...
Thursday, May 14, 2009
Coincidence units
Direct inputs
Input waveform
Recurrent
timing net
with single
Error-adjustment rule:
Btau = tau/33 ms
Two vowels with different fundamental frequencies (F0s) are added together
and passed through the simple recurrent timing net. The two patterns build up
In the delay loops that have recurrence times that correspond to their periods.
0 10 20 30 40 50
Time (msec)
Vowel [ae]
F0 = 100 Hz
Period = 10 ms
Vowel [er]
F0 = 125 Hz
Period = 8 ms
Time (ms)
Thursday, May 14, 2009
Fragment from G. Ligeti's Musica Ricercata
-1
0.5
0
0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 sec
This image is from the article Cariani, P. "Temporal Codes, Timing Nets, and Music Perception."
Journal of New Music Research 30, no. 2 (2001): 107-135. DOI: 10.1076/jnmr.30.2.107.7115.
This journal is available online at http://www.ingentaconnect.com/content/routledg/jnmr/
-1
0.5
0
0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 sec
Autocorrelogram
200
150
100
50
150
100
50
This image is from the article Cariani, P. "Temporal Codes, Timing Nets, and Music Perception."
Journal of New Music Research 30, no. 2 (2001): 107-135. DOI: 10.1076/jnmr.30.2.107.7115.
This journal is available online at http://www.ingentaconnect.com/content/routledg/jnmr/
Thursday, May 14, 2009
Autocorrelogram
200
150
Delay (samples)
100
50
Profile of mean signal values in delay channels Profile of std. deviations in delay channels
44 40
42 35
40
30
38
25
36
20
34
32 15
0 50 100 150 200 0 50 100 150 200
Delay (samples) Delay (samples)
This image is from the article Cariani, P. "Temporal Codes, Timing Nets, and Music Perception."
Journal of New Music Research 30, no. 2 (2001): 107-135. DOI: 10.1076/jnmr.30.2.107.7115.
This journal is available online at http://www.ingentaconnect.com/content/routledg/jnmr/
42
40
38
36
6
Loop delay = 88
34
4
32
0 50 100 150 200
Delay (samples) 2
0
Recurrent timing net
6
Loop delay = 134
1.5
4
0
1
8 Loop delay = 178
Time (samples)
6
2
0.5
0
0 50 100 150 200 700 750 800 850
Delay channel (samples) Time (samples)
This image is from the article Cariani, P. "Temporal Codes, Timing Nets, and Music Perception."
Journal of New Music Research 30, no. 2 (2001): 107-135. DOI: 10.1076/jnmr.30.2.107.7115.
This journal is available online at http://www.ingentaconnect.com/content/routledg/jnmr/
Thursday, May 14, 2009
MIT OpenCourseWare
http://ocw.mit.edu
For information about citing these materials or our Terms of Use, visit: http://ocw.mit.edu/terms.