Theory and Practice: Solving: Is It? How Can We Teach
Theory and Practice: Solving: Is It? How Can We Teach
derstand the problem, the problem lem when they encounter a situation in
solver may break down the problem, which they must respond but do not
activate old information (from mem- have the immediate solution. Regardless
ory), or seek new information. If the of the exact steps or methods for solving
solution is successful, the task ends. If the problem, the students need basic
it fails, the person backtracks, side- prerequisite information and relevant
tracks, or redefines the problem or the knowledge to assess the situation and to
method used to solve it. Problem solv- work out a strategy for solving the prob-
ing is not linear; the problem solver may lem.
jump around, skip, or combine steps. The strategies used are related to the
In this connection, Richard Cyert students’ age and the specific problem.
suggests that problem solving is heur- According to researchers, not all suc-
istic and includes 10 steps: cessful students will use the same
0 Keeping the big picture in mind with- strategies, and more than one strategy
out bogging down with details can often be used to solve the same
. Avoiding early commitment to a par- problem. A number of strategies can be
ticular hypothesis when several hypoth- used to solve even a simple problem
eses are possible (e.g., 6 + 6 ?).5 =
Downloaded from bul.sagepub.com at UCSF LIBRARY & CKM on April 25, 2015
115
Downloaded from bul.sagepub.com at UCSF LIBRARY & CKM on April 25, 2015
116
to use alternative approaches; knowing stages of cognitive growth, not until the
when to search for another strategy; child is 11 years old is he or she capable
using random approaches that are sen- of performing many of these meta-
sible and plausible when one’s original cognitive skills (corresponding with
approach has been unsuccessful.’ formal mental operations), and not until
Low-achieving and younger students age 15 is the child capable of fully per-
have less procedural knowledge to per- forming all metacognitive skills effi-
form these metacognitive skills com- ciently.9
pared to high-achieving and older stu- Study Skills, Subjects, Problem
dents.~ The implication for teaching is
that increased knowledge of content or Solving
subject matter does not necessarily pro- Every instructional program incor-
duce changes in metacognitive skills. porates several study skills, especially
These skills develop over years and in as the student passes from grade level to
general reflect high-order thinking pro- grade level. At the elementary school
cesses that cannot be learned or devel- level, study skills are often considered
oped overnight or in one subject. as a separate subject or content area. At
Moreover, age or human growth and the secondary level, study skills are
development is crucial in limiting poten- blended into curriculum as part of the
tial metacognitive skills among stu- subject matter. Every academic subject,
dents. An eight-year-old student is just at the junior high and senior high level,
capable of so much and cannot cogni- incorporates a number of specific study
tively be pushed more than one or two skills, suggested by Figure 1.
as
Downloaded from bul.sagepub.com at UCSF LIBRARY & CKM on April 25, 2015
118
porate it into their lessons. Others view standing previously learned skills and
problem solving as interfering with or concepts of the subject.
taking time away from curriculum cov- 2. Attending to relationshships. Sub-
erage, since many skills and processes jects comprise a large body of logical
are required. and closely related ideas; the teacher
More important, problem solving should emphasize meaning and interpre-
does not express itself in neatly orga- tation of ideas.
nized packages-that is, easily observed 3. Attending to representation. The
behaviors and outcomes-that can be more the student is able to represent a
Downloaded from bul.sagepub.com at UCSF LIBRARY & CKM on April 25, 2015
119
cability of the idea to students; skills and are no precise equivalents in other lan-
processes that apply to many settings guages or subjects and whose meanings
should be practiced. are therefore context specific.’2
5. Attending to language. Teachers In general, the improvement of stu-
should use precise terminology of their dents’ thinking-from what Lipman
subject, and students must learn basic calls &dquo;ordinary thinking&dquo; to &dquo;problem
terms and concepts of the subject.&dquo; solving&dquo;-depends on the students’
Although the authors describe the ability to identify good criteria for their
problem-solving process for math- opinions or solutions. The criteria
ematics, the five processes seem to should be based on logic and must be
apply subjects. The authors con-
to all &dquo;relevant,&dquo; &dquo;reliable,&dquo; &dquo;coherent,&dquo;
tend that teachers can learn to incor- and &dquo;consistent&dquo; with acceptable stan-
porate these steps into their own teach- dards in that particular subject or field. 13
ing processes, and students can also In this connection, the most charac-
learn to use these thinking processes. teristic feature of problem solving is that
Matthew Lipman adds five other it discovers its own weakness and recti-
processes that are more subtle and also fies what is at fault in its procedures;
cut across situational contexts and sub- thus, it is self-correcting.
ject matter. Problem-solving teaching
should take into account (1) exceptional Problem-Solving Strategies
or irregular circumstances: certain In general, teachers must provide stu-
things may be permissible under certain dents with good problem-solving strate-
conditions and not so under most condi- gies and practice using such strategies.
tions, or vice versa; (2) special limi- An extensive catalog of instructional
tations or constraints: certain theories, strategies is discussed below.
principles, or concepts may have limi- 1. The problem-solving process in most
tations or contingencies when they don’t cases is either schema driven, with little
apply or when they break down; (3) search for solution procedures because
overall configurations: something may appropriate procedures are activated by
be wrong or objectionable when taken recognizing the particular problem type,
out of context but valid or proper as a or based on search strategies, in which
whole, or vice versa; (4) evidence may the problem solver looks for or decides
be atypical-overgeneralizing from a on a particular strategy before solution
14
small sample or one experiment is procedures are tried or worked out.
somewhat risky; and (5) some meanings
do not translate well from one context to 12. Matthew Lipman, "Critical Thinking—
another-that is, there are terms, ex- What Can It Be?" Educational Leadership,
pressions, concepts, etc. for which there September 1988, pp. 38-43.
13. Ibid. Also see Joan B. Baron and Robert
J. Sternberg, eds., Teaching Thinking Skills
11. Thomas L. Good and Douglas A. (New York: Freeman, 1987).
Grouws, "Increasing Teachers’ Under- 14. Mary L. Glick "Problem-Solving
standing of Mathematical Ideas Through In Strategies," Educational Psychologist, Win-
Service Training," Phi Delta Kappan, June ter/Spring 1986, pp. 99-120; Penelope Pet-
1987, pp. 778-783. erson, "Teachers’ and Students’ Cognitional
Downloaded from bul.sagepub.com at UCSF LIBRARY & CKM on April 25, 2015
120
Downloaded from bul.sagepub.com at UCSF LIBRARY & CKM on April 25, 2015
121
10. In some advanced subjects, knowl- ence, generalize from findings, move
edge when to apply certain strategies
of from concrete to abstract and then from
cannot be taught to students. Students abstract to abstract. In all cases, there is
must induce the knowledge indirectly or need to stress objectivity of judgment in
intuitively and in other cases by exam- light of existing conditions.
ining problems already worked out, 13. Students can learn to use specific
means-ends analysis, etc.
16 strategies through consistent practice in
11. Perhaps the most fundamental a variety of settings so eventually their
Downloaded from bul.sagepub.com at UCSF LIBRARY & CKM on April 25, 2015