Ideal Problem Solver
Ideal Problem Solver
Second Edition
John D. Bransford
Barry S. Stein
rn
W. H. Freeman and Company
New York
Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data
Bransford, John.
The ideal problem solver : a guide for improving thinking,
learning, and creativity I John D. Bransford, Barry S. Stein.- 2nd
ed.
p. em.
Includes bibliographical references and indexes.
ISBN 0-7167-2204-6 (cloth).- ISBN 0-7167-2205-4 (pbk.)
1. Problem solving. 2. Thought and thinking. 3. Creative
ability. 4. Learning, Psychology of. I. Stein, Barry S.
II. Title.
BF449.73 1993
153.4'3-dc20 92-36163
CIP
Copyright 1984, 1993 by W. H. Freeman and Company
No part of this book may be reproduced by any mechanical, photographic, or
electronic process, or in the form of a phonographic recording, nor may it be
stored in a retrieval system, transmitted, or otherwise copied for public or
private use, without written permission form the publisher.
Printed in the United States of America
l 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 0 VB 9 9 8 7 6 5 4 3
To
J. Rshle~ Bransford
and her outstanding namesakes:
Rnn Bransford and Jimmie Brown.
nnd to
Michael, Norma. and Eli Stein
CONTENTS
PREFACE xiii
CHAPTER I
THE IMPORTANCE OF PROBLEM SOLVING
Mental Escapes I0
PART I
A fRAMEWORK FOR USING KNOWLEDGE
MORE EFFECTIVELY I7
CHAPTER 2
A MODEL FOR IMPROVING PROBLEM-SOLVING SKILLS 19
The IDEAL Approach to Problem Solving 19
CHAPTER 3
CREATIVITY AND THE IDEAL FRAMEWORK 51
Identify Problems and Opportunities 52
CHAPTER 4
INTELLIGENT CRITICISM 83
The Importance of Evaluating Arguments 83
CHAPTER 5
EFFECTIVE COMMUNICATION I I 3
The Importance of Identifying Communication Problems I 13
Message 115
Contents-
Audience I 16
PART2
A MODEL FOR ACQUIRING NEW KNOWLEDGE 131
CHAPTER 6
IMPROVING MEMORY SKILLS 133
Remembering as Problem Solving 134
CHAPTER 7
LEARNING WITH UNDERSTANDING 163
Comprehension as Problem Solving 164
CHAPTER 8
INSTRUCTION THAT FACILITATES PROBLEM SOLVING 195
Why Problem Solving Needs to Be an Integral Part of Education 196
CHAPTER 9
CONCLUDING REMARKS 219
The Development of Problem-Solving Skills 220
Attitudes 220
Notes 222
APPENDIX A
ANSWERS TO PROBLEMS IN TEXT 223
APPENDIX B
ANSWERS TO ExERCISES 22 9
APPENDIX C
IDEAL PROBLEM NAVIGATION GUIDE 249
AUTHOR INDEX 255
SUBJECT INDEX 259
PREFACE
This book is not simply for people who love the intellectual challenge of
solving problems. It is also for people who dislike problem solving or who
feel intimidated by problems. And it is for people who want to help
others solve problems. For example, many parents may find this book
useful for helping their children perform better in school. Business lead-
ers who recognize the importance of skills that enable their employees to
identify and solve potential problems may also find this book useful. In
addition, we believe that this book can provide a helpful tool for edu-
cators and researchers. For example, we have tried to illustrate how
knowledge acquisition and knowledge production activities such as class-
room teaching and scientific research are instances of problem solving.
By becoming more aware of the processes used to solve problems, people
can improve their learning and their abilities to use knowledge to solve
new problems that they face.
We do not claim that this book will dramatically increase your IQ,
make you wealthy, or free you from all personal problems. However,
based on our work with high school. college, and even graduate students,
and with teachers, administrators, and business leaders, we are confident
that you will find something of value that you can use for the rest of your
life . It has been our experience that different people find different parts of
the book valuable, probably because people have different strengths and
weaknesses with respect to problem solving. Nevertheless, we have not
yet met anyone who felt that they learned nothing from this book.
We believe that this second edition of The IDEAL Problem Solver
represents a significant advance over the first edition. In talking and
working with people who have used the first edition, we have had the
opportunity to focus on the final component of IDEAL: looking at the
effects of our earlier efforts and learning from them.
We thank the hundreds of people who have given us feedback.
Some have shown us how they have creatively applied the IDEAL frame-
work to problems such as enhancing their professional growth, improv-
- Preface
ing their grades, and interacting with their children. They have used
IDEAL in creative ways that we never imagined and that have taught us
a great deal. Others have helped us see the need to clarify the IDEAL
framework.
In this edition we have modified the framework to make it easier to
understand and apply. For example, we have modified both the "define
problems" and the "act on strategies" components of the original IDEAL
framework. The changes take into account the fact that the way we
define our problem -solving goals affects our perception of problems and
constrains the types of strategies we explore. They also reflect the impor-
tance of anticipating the outcome of particular strategies before they are
fully implemented. The new edition includes many new examples and
exercises that will make it easier to understand and apply the IDEAL
framework. Finally, we have added a new chapter that explores strategies
for improving how schools and organizations prepare people to think and
solve problems.
This edition has benefited from the efforts of hundreds of dedicated
researchers in human thinking and problem solving. Thanks to them, we
now have a better understanding of the nature of problem solving than
we had before. We have tried to keep this book as "user oriented" as
possible, and hence we do not spend a great deal of time discussing the
scholarly literature on problem solving. Nevertheless, this literature has
had a profound effect on our thinking, and we are grateful to all of the
people who contribute to it.
It is impossible to thank everyone for the contributions they have
made to this book. However, some people have had such a long-term,
fundamental impact on our thinking that they deserve special mention.
These include Jeff Franks, Jim Jenkins, Bob Shaw, and Walter Weimer.
Rick Barclay, Marcia Johnson, and Nancy McCarrell have also made
significant contributions to our development. Others who have made
major contributions include Ruth Arbitman-Smith, Ann Brown, Joe
Campione, Keith Clayton, Reuven Feuerstein, Carl Haywood, and Mil-
dred Hoffman. Nevertheless, none of these people should be held respon-
sible for any of our mistakes.
Some of the people who have played especially strong roles in
guiding our thinking for this edition are Linda Barron, Bill Corbin, Jim
Dickinson, Linda Giesbrecht-Bettoli, Laura Goin, Elizabeth Goldman, Su-
san Goldman, Bob Harwood, Ted Hasselbring, Richard Johnson, Charles
Kinzer, Alison Moore, Jim Pellegrino, John Pigg, Victoria Risko, Deborah
Rowe, Diana Miller Sharp, Robert Sherwood, Richard Troelstrup, Jim
Preface . .
Van Haneghan, Nancy Vye, Susan Warren, Susan Williams, and Mike
Young.
We are also grateful for the opportunity to work closely with many
people who completed their graduate training at Vanderbilt-people who
taught us a great deal. These include Lea Adams, Pam Auble, Sue Burns,
David Chattin, Vic Delclos, Jon Doner, Joan Littlefield, Karen Mezynski,
Don Morris, Greg Perfetto, and Nancy Vye. Reviews of this book by
Bunny Bransford, Wesley Henry, Joy Henshall, Richard Mayer, Thomas
Mosley, and Robert Sternberg have also been very helpful. In addition,
we are grateful to the many students at Tennessee Technological Univer-
sity and Vanderbilt University who have participated in our courses on
problem solving and provided us with feedback.
We are especially grateful to members of our familes who have been
instrumental in helping us prepare this second edition. We thank Bunny
Bransford, Jason Bransford, Ashley Bransford, Camille Bransford, Ada
Haynes, and Michael Stein for all their help. Without them, we could not
have improved and completed this edition.
Finally, we thank Faapio Poe for her excellent assistance in helping
us prepare the second edition manuscript and Diana Siemens and Gina
Goldstein for their helpful editorial suggestions.
A number of people have asked whether we, as authors of The
IDEAL Problem Solver, are ourselves ideal problem solvers. If you think of
an ideal problem solver as someone who always knows or can immedi-
ately derive the answer to every problem, then the answer is no, we are
not ideal problem solvers. As you read this book, however, you will see
that our ideal problem solver is someone who continually attempts to
improve by paying attention to his or her processes and by learning from
any mistakes that are made. It is this commitment toward becoming an
ideal problem solver-toward continuing to learn each day-that we
endorse and try to live by. We find that it is an exciting commitment
because we learn something new almost every day.
It is our hope that you too will find it valuable to think about your
own problem-solving processes and that, by analyzing them, you will
enjoy the continual challenge of discovering ways that they can be im-
proved. And as you make discoveries and observations, we would love to
hear from you. Your ideas will help us continue to learn.
John D. Bransford
Barry S. Stein
January 1993
THE IDEAL PROBLEM SOLVER
THE IMPORTANCE
OF PROBLEM SOLVING
The date was October 16, 1962. The president and spedal members of
the Executive Committee of the National Security Coundl met in an
emergency session. Information had just been obtained that the Soviet
Union was in the process of building missile-launching bases in Cuba.
The missiles were capable of carrying nuclear warheads that could
destroy most major dties in the Western Hemisphere. During six days of
secret meetings, these men had to develop a plan that would remove or
destroy the missile bases before they became operational.
Several different plans were considered and evaluated. One plan
called for an air strike that would quickly destroy all the missile in-
stallations. Another called for a naval blockade that might then be
followed by an air strike or invasion if the missile bases were not volun-
tarily removed. After the blockade plan was selected, the president
made a televised speech in which he conveyed details of the crisis to the
nation and explained the immediate course of action that would be
taken.
Many historians consider the Cuban missile crisis the closest the
world has come to an all-out nuclear war. Had these men selected a dif-
ferent course of action for the problem they confronted, the world as we
know it might have changed drastically. In any case, we, the people of
the world, would have had to live with the solution they formulated,
for better or for worse. Such historical events dramatically illustrate the
significance of problem-solving skills. 1
- The Ideal Problem Solver
It is instructive to stop and think about the many ways in which our lives
are influenced by attempts to solve problems. We studied problem solv-
ing for some time before we began to appreciate the following point,
which now seems obvious: Our lives are influenced tremendously by the
solutions to problems that were proposed and implemented by people
who preceded us in history. Solutions to problems such as the Cuban
missile crisis and polio stand out as prime examples. However, many
more solutions proposed by others have had an even greater impact on
our daily lives. For example, you may not think of such things as stop
signs, traffic signals, or rules about which side of the road to drive· on as
solutions to problems, but they are. Many of them were originated by
William Eno, often called the father of traffic safety. Eno, who was born
in New York City in 1858, became concerned about the problem of
massive traffic jams (involving horse-drawn vehicles) caused by the ab-
sence of traffic regulations. Eno published an article entitled "Reform
in Our Street Traffic Urgently Needed" and thereby focused people's at-
tention on an important problem. He also proposed solutions for this
problem, such as stop signs, one-way streets, and pedestrian safety is-
lands-ideas we take for granted today.
Laws and rules represent only a small part of our everyday lives that
are affected by other people's solutions to problems. Such artifacts as
furniture, clothing, tools, and appliances are also the result of attempts to
solve problems. Even our language and number systems are inventions
that allow people to solve problems. Imagine the difficulties we would
experience if we did not have a language for expressing our ideas, or if
we had only a spoken language but no system for producing and inter-
preting written language. Number systems are equally important. These
inventions make it possible to solve a variety of problems that otherwise
might be impossible or at least very difficult to solve.
Many of our solutions create new problems of their own. Cars
increase the probability of pollution; advances in physics can increase the
potential for menacing weapons; medical breakthroughs often raise new
ethical dilemmas about life and death; new inventions like computers
place new demands on people's ability to learn. It seems that we will
always need effective thinkers and problem solvers. Indeed, researchers
such as Lauren Resnick2 and Ray Nickerson 3 argue that now more than
ever, increasingly rapid changes in society require citizens who can think,
reason, and solve problems on their own. In modern factories, for ex-
ample, managers want employees who can spot problems that have gone
unrecognized and do something about them. Many companies are at-
The Importance of Problem Solving
Specialized
knowledge and skills
Problem domain
Goal
State
Present
State
Rgure 2 A problem exists when an obstacle separates the present state from
the goal state.
to your own approach to problem solving, especially when you are deal-
ing with nonroutine problems where answers do not come to mind
immediately. When people begin to analyze their approaches to prob-
lems, many discover that they often employ a "let me out of here" ap-
proach when a problem seems difficult. At times like these there is a
natural tendency to attempt to get out of the situation and do something
else that is associated with a higher probability of success.
Over time, the let-me-out-of-here approach can result in self-fulfill-
ing prophecies. For example, people who initially have difficulty solving
math problems may come to believe that they have no math ability; they
may then avoid situations in which they must deal with math problems.
Since these people receive little practice with math because they avoid it,
their initial hypothesis about not being able to solve math problems is
quite likely to come true. In general, it seems clear that people who avoid
The Importance of Problem Solving -
dealing with problems place limitations on themselves that are not neces-
sarily there to begin with.
In our classes on problem solving we often present the following
problem. Imagine that the right front turn signal on your car stops work-
ing. Try to figure out why it won't work. For many people, the reaction
to this problem is "I don't know anything about cars." Therefore, they
assume that they cannot solve the problem. Once they are encouraged to
think about the problem, however, they discover that they know more
than they realized. For example, a turn signal presumably requires a
bulb, just as a lamp does. Similarly, both a lamp and a turn signal un-
doubtedly require some power supply. The first step in diagnosing trouble
with a turn signal, therefore, might be to ask whether the problem is
caused by the bulb or the power supply. As a test, we could replace the
bulb on the right (the broken side) with the one from the left turn signal.
If the good bulb still does not work, we could then consider possible
problems with the power supply-for example, a fuse may have blown.
The important point is that most people can do a relatively good job of
figuring out why something like a turn signal is not working, if they think
about the problem.
Negative reactions not only keep us from solving problems that we
could solve but also can keep us from exploring new areas. For example,
while participating in a weekend workshop on sports psychology, a friend
of ours became aware of ways he was limiting himself unnecessarily. Part
of the workshop was spent competing with others in such events as
Indian wrestling, leg wrestling, and arm wrestling. A major goal of these
activities was to help people explore their feelings about winning and
losing. Since our friend was quite muscular, he won most of these events.
Nevertheless, he felt sure that he was a humble winner; that is, he was
confident that his ego would not have been shattered had he lost.
Later in the workshop the activities turned from more muscle-
oriented events (such as arm wrestling) to more coordination-oriented
events. In particular, participants in the workshop were asked to learn a
new type of intricate dance. As the dance instruction proceeded, our
friend, who had always believed that he couldn't dance, found that he
was making many more errors than were others in the group (for ex-
ample, he kept turning the wrong way).
Our friend's most important discovery was that he had a strong
reaction to his dilemma: "This workshop has gotten ridiculous; I'm going
to leave." He almost did leave but was persuaded by the instructor to stay
(the instructor was watching for such reactions and knew how to deal
with them). The instructor then recruited some volunteers to work with
. . The Ideal Problem Solver
I Mental Escapes
Actually walking away from important problems is a relatively extreme
negative approach to problem solving. In other cases, people may men-
tally walk away without physically removing themselves. For example,
people often think that they are diligently trying to complete tasks, yet
when prompted to stop and think about it, they realize that they have not
been attending to the problem to be solved.
Consider the activity of studying a text or one's notes to prepare for
a test. Most people have had the experience of going through the mo-
tions of reading and suddenly realizing that nothing has registered; their
attention was directed somewhere other than toward the material to be
learned. Similar difficulties can arise when listening to lectures. We begin
to think about something else and only later realize we missed what was
said.
John Holt 14 notes that attention can be an important factor in class-
room learning:
During many of the recitation classes, when the class supposedly is work-
ing as a unit, most of the children paid very little attention to what was
going on. Those who most needed to pay attention, usually paid the least.
The kids who knew the answer to whatever question you were asking
wanted to make sure that you knew they knew, so their hands were
always waving . ... But most of the time, when explaining, questioning,
or discussing what was going on, the majority of children paid very little
attention or none at all.
Watching older kids study, or try to study, I saw after a while that they
were not sufficiently self-aware to know when their minds had wandered
The Importance of Problem Solving . .
off the subjed. .. . Most of us have very imperfect control over our atten-
tion . Our minds slip away from duty before we realize that they are gone.
Lack of attention to a task is not simply a result of laziness or lack of
interest. Attention can also be affected by fear and anxiety. For example,
it can be very difficult to focus attentively on a problem while we are
concerned with competing thoughts about personal problems or about
fears that we may fail.
Difficulties caused by competing thoughts can be illustrated by con-
sidering a problem we presented to a number of college students. We
asked them to note their thoughts and feelings from the moment they
read the following problem. You might try this, too.
Two train stations are 50 miles apart. At I P.M. on Sunday a train pulls
out from each of the stations, and the trains start toward one another.
Just as the trains pull out from the stations a hawk flies into the air in
front of the first train and flies ahead to the front of the second train.
When the hawk reaches the second train, it turns around and flies
toward the first train. The hawk continues in this way until the trains
meet. Assume that both trains travel at a speed of 25 miles per hour
and that the hawk flies at a constant speed of I00 miles per hour. How
many miles will the hawk have flown when the trains meet?
it is usually much more efficient to simply accept the fact that life is not
always a bowl of cherries, stop whining, and get on with the task.
I Notes
1. For more information on the Cuban missile crisis, see R. A. Divine,
The Cuban Missile Crisis. Chicago: Quadrangle Books, 1 971 .
2. L. Resnick, Education and Learning to Think. Washington, D.C .: Na-
tional Academy Press, 1987.
3. R. S. Nickerson, On improving thinking through instruction.
Review of Research in Education, 15 ( 1988):3-57.
4. U. Neisser, General, academic, and artificial intelligence. In L. Res-
. . . The Ideal Problem Solver
I Suggested Readings
Practical!~ Oriented Readings
Tobias, S. 1978. Overcoming Math Anxiety. New York: W. W. Norton.
Williams, R. L., and J.D. Long. 1975 . Toward a Self-Managed Life Style. New
York: Houghton Mifflin.
The Importance of Problem Solving . .
Figure 3
MEDICINE CLOCK. Set this little pill-box alarm to ring DON'T LOCK YOURSELF OUT
every 1/2, l, 2, 4, 8, or 12 hours, and it plays a little song OF HOUSE, CAR! Magnetic cases
to remind you to take your medicine . hide spare keys safely.
3 A.M. and a man asks, "Is this Home Pac Pizza?" What should you do? If
you simply say, "No, you have the wrong number," you will have failed
to anticipate the problem the caller will face as soon as he hangs up:
knowing whether the number itself was wrong or he had merely mis-
dialed.
Several years ago, after moving and receiving a new phone number,
one of us began to receive calls at all hours of the night. The callers all
asked about pizza deliveries, so there seemed to be some explanation for
. . RFramework tor Using Hnowledge More Effective I~
the calls. As it turned out, the new number provided by the phone
company had previously been the number of an all-night pizza place.
When the calls first started coming in, we would say, "No, you have
the wrong number" and try to go back to sleep as quickly as possible.
Without fail, the phone would ring again 30 seconds later and the same
caller would be on the line. Since the number of the pizza place was in
the current phone book, the callers assumed they had misdialed the first
time around.
Once we identified the problem with the way we answered the
phone, we changed our strategy, saying, "The number has been
changed." The moral of the story is that it pays to look for potential
problems. If problems are not identified, solutions to them are unlikely to
be proposed.
D=Define Goals
The second aspect of the IDEAL approach to problem solving is to care-
fully define your goals in the problem situation. This is different from
identifying the problem. For example, a group of people could identify
the existence of a general problem and agree that it represents an oppor-
tunity but still disagree about what their goals should be. Different goals
often reflect differences in how people understand a problem.
Consider the problem of disposing of waste generated by individuals
and businesses. Although there is general agreement that we are running
out of space in our landfills, there are many different ways to define our
goals in this situation. One possible goal would be to find alternative ways
of disposing of trash other than landfills. Another would be to reduce the
amount of trash generated in the first place. Different goals can lead
people to explore very different strategies for solving a problem. If our goal
is to find alternative ways of disposing of trash, we might consider such
ideas as incineration or sending the trash to some remote location. On the
other hand, if our goal is to reduce the trash being generated, we might
consider recycling or encouraging companies to package goods in reusable
containers. Of course, you often may want to pursue multiple goals.
The difference between identifying problems that represent oppor-
tunities and defining one's goals can be clarified further by considering
the example of the frying bacon. A number of people might identify the
splattering grease as a problem representing an opportunity. Neverthe-
less, the way they define their goals could differ. One possible goal would
HModel tor Improving Problem-Solving Skills IIlii
be to reduce the heat that causes grease to splatter. Here the emphasis is
on the importance of heat, and the solution to the problem would focus
on ways to reduce this factor. One solution is to turn down the heat on
the stove (assuming that one is cooking over a stove). If the heat source
is not variable (if one is cooking over a campfire, for example), the
solution could be to increase the distance between the frying pan and the
fire.
Another way to define your goal could be to make people less
susceptible to burns from bacon grease. This emphasizes people's vul-
nerability to burns, and the solutions would focus on ways to make them
less vulnerable. A long-sleeved, heat-resistant glove would be a good
solution given this definition of the problem.
A third goal would be to reduce the distance the hot grease travels.
This goal leads one to consider that hot grease splatters and can travel
relatively long distances; the solution would therefore focus on ways to
decrease the grease's journey. The invention illustrated in Figure 4 is one
solution that might be expected to stem from such a problem definition.
James Adams provides an excellent example of how our problem-
solving goals can affect the strategies we considerJ He describes the
experiences of a group of engineers who were attempting to design a
mechanical tomato picker that could harvest tomatoes in large quantities
without bruising them.
The engineers initially defined their goal as optimizing the design of
mechanical pickers so that tomatoes do not get bruised and damaged. As you can
imagine, this led them to consider various strategies for modifying the
machine, such as slowing it down to reduce the impact or padding its
arms. Although all of these solution strategies were plausible, none was
considered a breakthrough.
It wasn't until people considered alternative ways of defining the
goal-such as creating a tomato that is less likely to be bruised-that they
were able to explore a more productive solution like breeding tomatoes
that would be less likely to be damaged by mechanical pickers. By explor-
ing strategies to modify the tomato, the problem was eventually solved.
Growers and packers can now supply grocery stories with large quantities
of inexpensive, unbruised tomatoes.
We have found that people often fail to consider alternative goals
when they attempt to solve a problem. Instead, they jump immediately
to the exploration of strategies. For example, when one of us was work-
ing with a team of engineers who were exploring strategies to protect
people in automobile crashes, most of those involved immediately started
. . RFramework for Using Hnowledge More [ffectivelq
What day follows the day before yesterday if two days from now
will be Sunday?
Arthur Whimbey and Jack Lochhead have asked people who were
good at problems like these to think aloud as they solved them.8 In no
case did people simply read the problem and then have the answer pop
into their minds. Instead, these experienced problem solvers took a very
careful, systematic approach. In particular, they usually broke complex
problems into simpler ones that could each be solved more easily. Given
the problem just presented, for example, effective problem solvers might
ask themselves:
The problem becomes quite simple when broken into its component
parts. Furthermore, human beings seem to need to break complex prob-
lems into component parts in order to succeed.
Proceeding systematically is also necessary for effective reading
comprehension. Arthur and Linda Whimbey provide information rele-
vant to this point. 9 They presented the following passage to college stu-
dents wh o had comprehension difficulties.
The Whimbeys note that the college students who were poor compre-
henders failed to take a systematic approach to the problem. They state:
This was the first sentence of a reading comprehension article, and I had
to stop for a moment and reread a portion of it in order to understand its
meaning completely. Not so for the low-aptitude student I was testing.
He was halfway down the page by the time I had the details of the first
sentence sorted out. I asked him if he understood the sentence, and he
answered "No, not really." So I suggested he give it another try.
RModel tor Improving Problem-Solving Skills IIDI
The Whimbeys go on to note that good readers are much more systematic
in their efforts to comprehend information.
To the poor reader, however, the pattern ofgradual, sequential construc-
tion of exact meaning is totally foreign. One-shot thinking (Bereiter and
Englemann 's term) is the basis on which the poor reader makes inter-
pretations and draws conclusions.
There are three separate, equal-sized boxes. Inside each box are two
separate, small boxes, and inside each of the small boxes are four even
smaller boxes. How many boxes are there altogether?
Many people quickly realize the need to represent the problem by draw-
ing, but many others do not. We know college students who attempted
to do this problem in their heads and had difficulty with it. This was
especially true of younger students with whom we have worked (fourth
and fifth graders). Performance improved considerably when the stu-
dents were prompted to draw a representation of the problem (although
there is still one aspect of the problem that is easy to overlook, even after
it is drawn out; the answer to this problem appears in Appendix A).
. . RFramework tor Using Hnowledge More Effectivelij
A man had four chains, each three links long. He wanted to join the four
chains into a single, closed chain. Having a link opened cost 2 cents and
having a link closed cost 3 cents. The man had his chains joined into a
closed chain for I 5 cents. How did he do it?
By actually drawing the four chains, you don't have to use your memory to
keep imagining them (the answer to the problem appears in Appendix A).
There are many other ways of externalizing memory to keep track
of information. Additional examples, such as the use of graphs and Venn
diagrams, are discussed later. For the moment, the important point is that
problems can often be represented in a variety of ways, yet some repre-
sentations are more likely to lead to efficient solutions than others. For
example, if you are asked, "What is two-thirds of one-half?" you might
represent it on paper as ~3 x l/2. A simpler way to think of the problem is
"What is one-half of two-thirds?"
It is important to recognize that the most effective way to represent
information will depend on the nature of the problem and how we define
our goals. John Hayes 11 and Diane Halpern 12 note that some problems
are better solved if one uses a verbal representation, whereas others are
better represented visually or perhaps mathematically. In some situations
the most effective methods for representing information may differ from
those that we typically use. For example, Ruth Day 13 has found that
people often fail to use representations that would make it easier to
achieve their goals. Effective problem solvers explore a variety of repre-
sentations. We will explore alternative ways of representing information
later when we consider strategies that can facilitate learning.
It is 4 P.M. and you have just learned that you are expected for an impor-
tant company meeting in Chicago at 8 the next morning. There are two
flights open. One is a dinner flight that leaves at 6 P.M. and arrives in
Chicago at 6 A.M . the next day. The other flight departs at 7:30 P.M. and
arrives in Chicago at 7:30 A.M. the next day. When you arrive in Chicago
you will need to wait 20 minutes for your luggage and it will take 20
minutes by taxi to get to your meeting. Which flight should you take, and
will you need to buy dinner?
In the movie Raiders of the Lost Ark, Indiana Jones successfully removed a
golden idol from a booby-trapped platform by substituting something
that weighed approximately the same-a bag of sand. Assuming the gold-
en idol was solid gold and that it and the bag of sand were approximately
the same size, is it reasonable to assume that the idol and the bag of sand
weighed approximately the same?
To solve this problem, you clearly need to do more than break the prob-
lem into parts. You also need some way to estimate the weight of a bag of
sand and a golden idol of approximately the same volume. Since the
objects are not available, you cannot weigh them directly. How else might
you proceed?
People who have learned and understood the concept of density are
able to generate a strategy for solving this problem. They know that
different types of materials have different densities and that density
tables can be found in some science texts . By consulting these tables, it is
easy to determine that the density of a solid-gold object is approximately
19.4 grams per cubic centimeter, whereas the density of sand is ap-
proximately 2 grams per cubic centimeter. This means that a golden idol
of the size depicted in the movie would weigh approximately 60 pounds.
In contrast, the bag of sand would weigh only about 6 pounds.
Note that the strategy of consulting a density table to solve the
Indiana Jones problem requires knowing about the concept of density.
Without it, it is very difficult to know how to proceed. There is often no
substitute for specialized knowledge when solving certain types of prob-
lems. For example, by learning concepts in algebra and geometry (con-
cepts that have been refined over the centuries), it becomes possible to
solve a variety of problems that otherwise would be extremely difficult if
not impossible to solve. Similarly, concepts in physics enable people to
solve such problems as how to put a satellite into orbit at a particular
distance from the earth, and concepts in biology help us to alleviate
various infectious diseases. All areas of study, including biology, psychol-
ogy, economics, physics, and chemistry, among many others, involve
core concepts and theories that people have found helpful in defining
and solving important problems. These concepts actually simplify the
process of problem solving.
For a simple illustration of the power of concepts, consider the
drawings in Figure 6. By prompting people to make use of concepts they
HModel for Improving Problem-Solving Skills IIJII
12?<\:1
Figure 6 Some perceptual patterns.
have already learned, one can help them see these drawings in a new
way. The first can be viewed as a bear climbing up the opposite side of a
tree and the second as the Eiffel Tower viewed from the inside of an
armored car. Note how one understands the drawings differently when
they are viewed from these perspectives. The philosopher N. R. Hanson
argues that the creation of new scientific theories fulfills an analogous
function: It enables people to conceptualize events in new and previously
unappreciated ways. 17
That concepts provide tools that have powerful effects on problem
solving has important implications. The most important is that people who
want to develop effective problem-solving skills must become effective at learning
about relevant conceptual tools. Our discussion in Part 2 focuses on the
problem of developing more effective learning skills.
egy. For example, it could have asked people how they would feel if the
drink they had been purchasing all their lives were no longer available.
An article in the Wall Street Journa/ 18 discusses the importance of
anticipating the consequences of one's strategies. Entitled "Building a
Better Mousetrap Doesn't Ensure Success," the article discusses the plight
of several companies that have attempted to deal with a problem faced by
thousands of stores across the country: the theft of shopping carts. Since
the carts cost around $90 each, a store can lose a considerable amount of
money in a year.
Different companies have come up with different solutions for the
shopping cart problem. Several of them involve locking up carts when
they are not in use. For example, customers may have to pay to unlock a
cart and get their money back when they return the cart to its original
location.
Imagine that you are a grocery store owner trying to decide whether
to invest in an antitheft shopping cart system. There are a number of
steps you might take to anticipate the effects of implementing such as
system. First you might estimate the number of carts that you lose per
year, calculate the cost, and decide that the locked-cart system would pay
for itself within a year. Then you might test the invention that locks up
the carts and find that it works very reliably. Finally, you might recall
having seen similar types of devices in airports and conclude that since
the invention worked there it should work for your store as well.
Ideally, there are additional steps that you would take before pur-
chasing a locked-cart system. One concern should be customer percep-
tion-you need to anticipate their reactions to locked carts. A negative
reaction could be caused by inconvenience. If the cart system requires
customers to have correct change, what will they do when they don't
have it? If the store is busy, many customers will not want to spend time
standing in line at a change machine.
Another, potentially more serious, reaction to a locked-cart system
is that many customers will react negatively if a store suddenly appears
not to trust them. The consequences could be severe. Note that people
are probably much less likely to feel the same way about locked-cart
systems in airports. Airports are visited by masses of people, and many
other types of security measures have already been put in place to protect
the public. Also, airports usually have a monopoly on air travel so that
customers cannot always seek other alternatives. Therefore, the fact that
a system works in an airport may not mean that it will work well some-
place else.
HModel for Improving Problem-Solving Skills 11111
you read. Will that strategy be sufficient to achieve your goal of doing
well on the test?
Most students know that they have to anticipate the consequences
of using a particular study strategy in light of the test requirements. If the
test will involve a presentation of one or two paragraphs from the text
with the requirement that students explain the meaning of the para-
graphs in their own words, the study strategy noted above (to read each
chapter twice) may be adequate. On the other hand, if the test requires
students to recall key concepts from the text and discuss them in order of
importance, that same study strategy will probably be insufficient. (See
Chapter 7 for more discussion of this issue.) Unless students anticipate
the effects of adopting particular study strategies by acting on them, they
cannot make adjustments that will ensure success. In our example, the
best way to anticipate probable effects is to actually perform various tasks
such as recalling key concepts from the book chapters. If you are not
successful, you will know ahead of time that you need to change your
approach.
The previous discussion shows that it is often necessary to act on
our strategies before we can anticipate possible outcomes. We have ex-
perienced case after case in which people (including ourselves) believe
that an initial idea makes great sense and then are unpleasantly surprised
by obvious flaws that become apparent after the idea is put into practice.
Ideally, many of these flaws can be discovered during the act and antici-
pate outcomes phase of problem solving rather than later on, when real
damage can be done.
There are 12 cannonballs. They all look alike, but one is the oddball. The
oddball is either heavier or lighter than the other balls. You are supplied
with a balance scale that can hold as many cannonballs as you like on
each side. The problem is, in four weighings (four uses of the scale), find
the oddball.
Try only the first step in this problem-that is, the first weighing-and then
consider what you have found out. We have given this problem to more than
300 college students. The first strategy considered by over 90 percent of
them is to place six cannonballs on each side of the scale to determine the
side that contains the oddball. Once students actively attempt this strat-
egy and look at the possible effects, they discover a dilemma: They still do
not know which side the oddball is on.
After the initial weighing, most students understand the problem in
a different way. They now understand that the difficulty of the problem
lies in the fact that the oddball can be either lighter or heavier; hence, the
six-six weighing does not tell them which side the oddball is on. Note that
without actively trying out their initial strategy and looking at the effects,
the definition of oddball would not be adequately understood. (You may
now want to continue to try to solve the oddball problem. The answer is
in Appendix A.)
In addition to looking at the effects of one's actions or decisions, it is
important to learn from the experience. Ideally, one learns something
general so that subsequent attempts at problem solving can be improved.
For example, imagine that you have created a new advertisement for a
product and had attempted to anticipate its effects by using a focus group
of businessmen and women. When the ad actually airs on television, you
discover a very negative reaction to it by various groups of people, espe-
cially those who are out of work and unable to find a job. As a result, you
will have learned something about how better to select people to par-
ticipate in any future focus groups that you decide to use.
. . RFramework tor Using Hnowledge More Effecti ve!~
IDEAL cycle and will remain there until the problem is solved or he or
she gives up.
Different ways of reentering the IDEAL cycle can result in strategies
that are more or less creative. The creative person will often redefine his
or her goals and try to use strategies that differ considerably from those
used previously. For example, the creative problem solver may reex-
perience the failure to comprehend the haystack sentence and redefine
the goal as attempting to use his or her own knowledge to make sense of the
sentence. This redefinition of goals suggests a new strategy. Rather than
rereading, the optimal strategy is to generate a context that allows the
sentence to make sense (if you find yourself trying to discover such a
context, the word "parachute" should help). Strategies for enhancing
creativity are discussed in more detail in Chapter 3.
His first reaction is often of violent disturbance, as views that are very
dear are questioned or thrown to the ground. Nevertheless, if he will
"stay with it" rather than escape into anger and unjustified rejection of
contrary ideas, he will discover that this disturbance is very beneficial.
For now he becomes aware of the assumptive character of a great many
previously unquestioned features of his own thinking. This does not mean
that he will reject these assumptions in favor of those of other people.
Rather, what is needed is the conscious criticism of one's own meta-
physics, leading to changes where appropriate and ultimately, to the
continual creation of new and different kinds.
Of course, scientists are not the only people who have difficulty
redefining their goals because of emotional attachment to a set of as-
sumptions. Nearly all people experience such difficulties at one point or
another. When this happens, we fail to critically evaluate our assump-
tions. The IDEAL approach provides an important reminder of the value
of questioning assumptions and defining our goals in new ways.
I Summar~
Our goal in this chapter was to provide a model that can be used to
improve problem solving. The modeL represented by the acronym
IDEAL, emphasizes five components of the problem-solving process:
identify, define, explore, anticipate/act, and look/learn. Each of these com-
ponents is involved in any attempt to solve a problem; furthermore,
people often need to move through the IDEAL cycle flexibly and repeat
the cycle several times to arrive at a satisfactory solution. Creative prob-
lem solvers are especially likely to view a problem from a variety of
perspectives; that is, to define their goals in a number of different ways.
In the remaining chapters we will apply the IDEAL model to situa-
tions in which people must solve such problems as spotting flaws in
arguments and communicating effectively. In Part 2 we will examine
problems such as remembering sets of facts, comprehending new infor-
mation, and learning in ways that facilitate future problem-solving tasks.
Since the IDEAL model provides the organizing structure for the rest of
this book, it is important that you understand it and are able to use it in
a variety of ways. To help you reach this goaL we include at the end of
this and other chapters a number of exercises designed to provide prac-
tice in thinking within the IDEAL framework. The exercises are meant to
HModel tor Improving Problem-Solving Skills . .
I Exercises
Try to solve the two following problems as quickly as possible.
PARIS
IN THE
THE SPRING
RFramework tor Using Hnowledge More Effecti ve!~
GD
•
HModel for Improving Problem-Solving Skills lltJI
Three actual inventions are shown below. Try to define the problems they
were designed to solve.
6. Unusual shovel:
IIIII HFramework tor Using Hnowledge More Effectivelq
7.
8.
I Notes
l. M. Wertheimer, Productive Thinking. New York: Harper & Row,
1959.
2. G. Polya, How to Solve It. Garden City, N.Y.: Doubleday Anchor,
1957.
3. A. Newell and H. A. Simon, Human Problem Solving. Englewood
Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall, 1972.
4. E. C. Bliss, Getting Things Done. New York: Bantam Books, 1976.
5. This example is from E. de Bono (ed.), Eureka, An Illustrated History
of Inventions from the Wheel to the Computer; A London Sunday Times
Encyclopedia. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston, 1974.
6. Additional examples of inventions can be found in de Bono (see
note 5).
7. J. L. Adams, Conceptual Blockbusting: A Guide to Better Ideas (3d ed.).
Reading, Mass.: Addison-Wesley, 1986.
8. A. Whimbey and J. Lochhead, Problem Solving and Comprehension
(3d ed.) Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1985.
9. A. Whimbey, with L. S. Whimbey, Intelligence Can Be Taught. New
York: Dutton, 1975.
10. From Whimbey and Lockhead (see note 8).
11. J. R. Hayes, The Complete Problem Solver (2d ed.). Hillsdale, N.J.:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1989.
12. D. Halpern, Thought and Knowledge: An Introduction to Critical Think-
ing (2d ed.). Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1989.
13. R. S. Day, Alternative representations. In G. H. Bower (ed.), The
Psychology of Learning and Motivation, Vol. 22. New York: Academic
Press, 1988, pp. 261-305.
14. Additional examples of general problem-solving strategies can be
found in J. R. Hayes, The Complete Problem Solver (2d edition).
Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1989; B. F. Ander-
son, The Complete Thinker. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall,
1980; G. Polya, How to Solve It. Garden City, N.Y.: Doubleday
Anchor, 1957; W. A. Wickelgren, How to Solve Problems. San Fran-
cisco: W. H. Freeman, 1974.
15. For an excellent discussion of general versus specific strategies and
skills, see A. Newell, One final word. In D. T. Tuma and F. Reif
(eds.), Problem Solving and Education: Issues in Teaching and Research.
Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall, 1980.
RModel tor Improving Problem-Solving Skills . .
I Suggested Readings
Practical I~ Oriented Readings
Anderson, B. F. 1980. The Complete Thinker. Englewood Cliffs, N.J. : Pren-
tice-Hall.
Halpern, D. 1989. Thought and Knowledge: An Introduction to Critical Think-
ing (2d ed.) . Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Hayes, J. R. 1989. The Complete Problem Solver (2d ed.). Hillsdale, N.J.:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Polya, G. 1957. How to Solve It. Garden City, N.Y.: Doubleday Anchor.
Rubinstein, M. F., and K. Pfeiffer. 1980. Concepts in Problem Solving.
Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall.
Sternberg, R. J. 1986. Intelligence Applied. San Diego, Calif.: Harcourt
Brace Jovanovich.
Wickelgren, W. A. 197 4. How to Solve Problems. San Francisco: W. H.
Freeman.
Ell HFramework tor Using Hnowledge More Effectivelq
The uncreative mind can spot wrong answers, but it takes a creative
mind to spot wrong questions.
Antony Jay 1
One of the most common excuses we hear for why people are unable to
solve problems is that they are just not creative. It is therefore important
to ask, "Where is creativity in the IDEAL framework?" It may seem
surprising, but processes for enhancing creativity are everywhere in the
IDEAL model; they are involved in identifying problems and opportun-
ities, defining goals, exploring possible strategies, and so forth. Our goal in
this chapter is to explore ways to enhance creativity in the context of the
IDEAL model.
Before we can explore ways to enhance creativity, it is important to
define what we mean by creativity. Some people assume that only people
in the performing or other arts are creative. In fact, creativity can be
found in any problem-solving domain. For example, scientists can take
creative approaches to constructing and testing theories, business leaders
IIBI RFramework for Using Hnornledge More Effectivelq
can be creative in identifying new ideas for products and services and in
running their companies, and students can take creative approaches to
their choice of topics for term papers and projects. Furthermore, not all
artistic activities are creative. For example, drawing, sculpture, painting,
dance, and music can be approached noncreatively as well as creatively.
The important point is that any task can be approached from a creative or
a less creative point of view.
David Perkins 2 reminds us that there are degrees of creativity; it is
not something that you either have or don't have. It is also important to
take a realistic view of creativity. Many people erroneously assume that
someone is creative only if he or she is like Einstein or Bach or Picasso.
But if you look for creativity only at that level of achievement you will
miss the hundreds of opportunities to be creative each day. Children are
naturally creative, and all adults continue to have this potential.
In this book we use the term creative to refer to the ability to solve
problems in ways that are novel for the individual. An idea that you
generate can be creative even if you were not the first person in history
to think of it. What counts is your ability to approach and solve problems
in ways that are not routine for you. A creative idea is also one that is
appropriate to the situation, rather than simply novel but off the wall. As
we discuss creativity within the IDEAL model, we will focus on ways to
generate ideas that are both novel and appropriate to your goals.
important problem when he read a report that choking was the sixth
leading cause of accidental death. Although many other doctors had
probably noticed this problem, Heimlich recognized it as an opportunity
for a creative solution. He soon realized that two of the recommended
treatments for choking victims-removing the object with a finger and
slapping the victim on the back-would probably push the object deeper
into the victim's throat. Heimlich's experience as a chest surgeon helped
him realize that there would probably be enough air in the lungs to clear
the victim's airway if someone pushed on the diaphragm. His invention,
the Heimlich maneuver, is now recognized as the best lifesaving techni-
que for choking victims.
Sometimes creative ideas stem from identifying problems in other
people's solutions. For example, Cal Garland noticed that the styrofoam
nuggets used to package delicate objects sent through the mail are bad for
the environment. He realized that he could take advantage of material
available at his lumberyard to provide a solution to the packaging prob-
lem that was better for the environment. Garland patented a machine
that creates curls of paper-thin wood shavings that can provide packaging
protection and then be used as garden mulch. 3
These are all potentially good solutions. Nevertheless, they address only
one of a number of possible general goals. That goal is to speed up the
process of getting customers through the checkout line. An alternative
goal is to find ways to keep people from getting annoyed at slow checkout
lines. This suggests a host of potential strategies, such as installing tele-
IBII RFramework for Using Hnowledge More Effective I~
viswns that customers can watch while they wait in line, giving the
customers free samples while they wait in line (the store can conduct
market research at the same time that it helps with the checkout prob-
lem), and having live entertainment to amuse the customers.
Still another goal is to keep the store from having too many cus-
tomers at any particular time of day. This suggests strategies that are
different from the others. One is to get more of the store's regular cus-
tomers to come at nonpeak hours. This might be accomplished by dou-
bling the value of coupons during certain hours of the day, having special
sales at certain hours, or holding special events such as services for the
elderly at particular hours or a singles night.
In our experience with the grocery store problem, it is rare for
people to step back and try to define alternative goals. Instead, most read
the problem and almost immediately begin generating strategies. One
way that we try to help them become more creative is by asking them to
explicitly define a minimum of two or three different goals for each
problem situation.
making your goals explicit and then redefining them in order to be more
creative. Effective problem solving requires a delicate balance between
going with the routine (because it is efficient) and taking the extra time
to treat a situation as nonroutine and attempting to develop a more
creative approach.
Studies in cognitive science suggest that in many situations, routine
thinking and creative thinking may be complementary rather than an-
tithetical. The reason is that people have limited attention spans, and
real-world problems can easily overwhelm our processing capabilities. It
is difficult to be creative when one is overwhelmed.
Remember the experience of fir~t learning to drive a car? You had to
consciously attend to particular aspects of driving such as putting on the
turn signal, hitting the brake and clutch, looking in the rear-view mirror
before changing lanes, and so forth. These tasks demanded so much of
your attention that it was difficult to do something else simultaneously,
such as carry on a conversation with a passenger. But as these com-
ponents of driving became routine they demanded less and less attention,
until finally you could drive and carry on a conversation at the same
time. In general, routine tasks support creativity because they allow us to
shift our attention to new aspects of a situation.
As an additional example of how routine and nonroutine thinking
can be complementary, imagine that you are asked to coordinate a fund-
raising banquet and the organizer wants you to make it an especially
memorable occasion. This is a complex problem that can be broken down
into a number of subgoals that are routine for people who do this type of
thing often. These subgoals include finding a suitable location, arranging
decorations, notifying and inviting guests, arranging entertainment, se-
lecting a menu, preparing and serving food, and cleaning up. Attention to
each of these subgoals does not guarantee that your banquet will be
memorable, but if you overlook one of them the banquet will be a flop.
A task such as running a fund-raising banquet requires a combina-
tion of routine and nonroutine thinking. The more experience you have
organizing banquets the more likely you will be to think of all the impor-
tant subgoals. You can then decide where to focus your creative energies.
Perhaps you will need special invitations, an extravagant dessert, enter-
tainment, or an unusual theme and decorations. By first considering the
routine subgoals, you will get a better idea of the creative options avail-
able. Furthermore, each of the subgoals has a number of routine com-
ponents. All invitations have to meet certain prerequisites such as being
sent out on time and being clear about the place and time; similarly, all
meals require some sort of preparation and cleanup. You might proceed
Creativitq and the IDERL Framework IIDI
Rgure 8
Ell RFramework for Using Hnowledge More Effectivelq
ill\
Ill\
II I I
I I 1 I
I I I I
I I I I
I I I I
/ I I I
I I 1 I
I I I I
I I I I
I I I I
1 I 1 I
I J ~ ·~
Figure 9
reach them more easily, you have mastered another strategy that is often
overlooked because of functional fixedness. 8
Routine thinking can also prevent us from developing effective so-
lutions when we falsely assume that a new problem can be solved using
a routine approach. This phenomenon is often referred to as learning set.
Although learning sets can facilitate problem solving when dealing with
problems that require similar solution strategies, they can limit our ability
to explore other strategies. For example, many of our students have
falsely assumed that the study techniques they used in many previous
classes would work in a new course. It was only when they performed
poorly on a test that they recognized the need to break out of this implicit
set and treat the situation as nonroutine . We will address the impor-
tance of identifying assumptions that limit our creativity in more detail
when we discuss the importance of looking back at the effects of using a
strategy.
Fractionation
One strategy for enhancing the range of experiences we consider during
problem solving involves what Edward deBono calls fractionation. 9 The
goal of fractionation is to break a concept or idea into its component parts
so that new thoughts are more likely to come to mind. For example,
consider the problem of generating ideas for possible uses of bricks.
Rather than focusing only on bricks per se, it is helpful to fractionate
bricks into more basic properties and consider uses for each one. Thus, a
brick has a certain color and weight, is rectangular and porous, holds heat
but does not conduct electricity, and is rough and small enough to be
picked up in one hand.
Most people find that fractionating the concept of a brick into
component parts increases the number of potential uses they can
generate. Furthermore, when people try to generate uses but do not
employ the strategy of thinking about individual properties of bricks,
their responses usually reflect assumptions about those properties. For
example, a person who thinks of doorstops, paperweights, and bookends
as uses for bricks is probably, without necessarily realizing it, thinking
about the property of weight.
Here is another problem.
Breaking this problem into its components can also help you generate
novel ideas. For example, you might first think of the factors involved in
the problem; that is, roads, cars and trucks, people (presumably the ones
bothered by noise), and some medium (such as air) that carries sound.
You could then think about the individual properties of each. For ex-
ample, people have ears that might be covered to reduce the perception
of noise, cars and trucks have tires that might affect the level of noise, and
so forth. Such thinking can facilitate our access to ideas that may lead to
creative solutions.
campus housing. 10 Someone said, "We have BYOB (bring your own
booze) parties; why not a BYOH (bring your own housing) university?"
Hayes notes that, although this suggestion was partly meant as a joke, it
was in fact quite beneficial, leading to a discussion of mobile housing
units. In the past, discussion had focused only on conventional housing
such as dorms, which are expensive to build and impossible to move
around.
Analogies have played a key role in discoveries in science and tech-
nology. For example, Benjamin Franklin noticed that a pointed object
would draw a much stronger spark than a blunt object in the vicinity of
an electrified body.l 1 At first he thought this was an unimportant obser-
vation. Until he recognized the analogy between clouds and electrified
bodies, he did not realize that pointed rods of iron could be used to
protect buildings and ships from damage by lightning. Similarly, Gut en-
berg's invention of the printing press was developed in part out of anal-
ogies he saw with the wine press and the punches used for making coins.
Another illustration of the key role analogies can play in great dis-
coveries is provided in the reflections of August Wilhelm Kekule written
25 years after he published his model for the structure of the benzene
molecule. 12
I turned my chair to the fire and dozed . . .. Again the atoms were gamboll-
ing before my eyes. This time the smaller groups kept modestly in the back-
ground. My mental eye, rendered more acute by repeated visions of this
kind, could now distinguish larger structures, of manifold conformation;
long rows, sometimes more closely fitted together; all twining and twisting
in snakelike motion. But look! What was that? One of the snakes had
seized hold of its own tail, and the form whirled mockingly before my eyes.
Using the analogy of a snake biting its own tail, Kekule developed a
model for the ringlike structure of the benzene molecule that profoundly
affected the science of organic chemistry.
In addition to facilitating the discovery of new ideas and the crea-
tion of new inventions, analogies provide alternative frameworks for
interpreting familiar facts. For example, in a recent period of stock-
market decline, one analyst criticized the gloomy predictions of most of
his colleagues and argued instead that the market would experience a
substantial gain. His reasoning was based on an analogy with a diving
board-the more downward pressure placed on a diving board, the
greater the tendency for it to push back upward. We question the ap-
Creat i vit~ and the IDERL Framework . .
Brainstorming
In 1957, Alexander Osborn described the concept of brainstorming. 14 His
goal was to use a group setting to increase the production of creative
ideas. One of the most important characteristics of a brainstorming ses-
sion is that participants adopt an attitude of complete friendliness and an
openness to suggestions. At first, wild ideas are encouraged-the wilder
the better. Further, members of the brainstorming group must agree to
withhold their evaluation of ideas until later in the session; criticism is
therefore ruled out. This rule is extremely important. Since novel ideas
often differ from conventional wisdom, premature evaluations can
prevent one from appreciating their value. Premature evaluations in
brainstorming sessions are often referred to as "idea killers" because they
hinder creative thought.
A variation on brainstorming is to ask group members to write
down their ideas on sheets of paper and then exchange them so that
others can make modifications and suggestions. This procedure, called
brainwriting, has sometimes been found to be even more effective than
brainstorming. 15
Incubation
In the preceding discussion we assumed that one continues to work on a
problem until it is solved. However, if you have ever worked on a difficult
problem for some length of time (on ideas for a speech or paper, for
example) you have probably found that, at some point, you had to stop
thinking about it for a while. This is not merely because of mental fatigue;
often one is able to think about other things. Instead, the reason for
stopping is that the same old answers keep coming up. People get locked
into a particular way of thinking and need to break out.
The term incubation refers to something that happens during the
period when one has stopped working on a problem. For example, some
people purposely think about a problem before going to bed so they can
sleep on it. They hope that ideas will incubate (hatch) during the night
and fresh insights will be available when they wake up. And indeed, this
sometimes seems to occur. Many scientists have written about having
flashes of insight after they stopped thinking about a problem and did
something else (took a trip, read a novel, or slept). 16 The following quo-
tation is from the French mathematician Henri Poincare. 17
One evening contrary to my custom, I drank black coffee and could not
sleep. Ideas rose in crowds; I felt them collide until pairs interlocked, so to
speak, making a stable combination. By the next morning, I had estab-
lished the existence of a class of Fuchsian functions, those which come from
the hypergeometric series; I had only to write out the results, which took
but a few hours.
Creativit~ and the IDEAL Framework . .
Chopsticks
Trumpet
Roof
Tree
. . RFramework tor Using Hnowledge More Effectivelij
These probably don't suggest anything special; they probably don't pro-
duce an insight or the feeling of "aha!"
Now assume that you have been trying to comprehend the follow-
ing statement: "The food was uneaten because the wood was warped."
Assume also that after working on it for a while you stop and do other
things, one of which is to read the list of words above . This time, items on
the list (such as chopsticks) may provide a clue for problem solution.l 9 If
you had kept working on the problem you probably would not have read
the list. Furthermore, if the problem had not been in the back of your
mind, the insight afforded by the word chopsticks would not have oc-
curred to you.
Charles Darwin notes that while trying to develop a theory that
would account for the data he had collected on similarities and dis-
similarities among different species, he happened to read an essay by
Thomas Malthus on population. 20 According to Darwin, the essay helped
him formulate the theory of evolution despite the fact that it was about a
different topic. Had Darwin simply sat in his room pondering the data, he
might not have read Malthus's article . And if Darwin had not kept the
general problem of accounting for his data in the back of his mind,
Malthus's work would not have had the clue value it did.
This perspective on incubation suggests that we can take an active
role, rather than merely sleeping or not thinking about a problem. We
can talk with others, read books, look at magazines, view scenery, and so
forth. If we keep the problem in the back of our mind, we may find a host
of clues that can help us generate creative ideas.
Creativit~ Checklists
Another way to increase our ability to access knowledge and experiences
that might be relevant to a problem is by using special lists designed to
help people get around the constraints of routine thinking. Like frac-
tionation, creativity checklists are meant to expand the range of experi-
ences and strategies that we consider during problem solving. Alexander
Osborn 21 created one of the more famous creativity lists.
Creativity checklists can be useful in helping us get beyond routine
thinking. Although checklists such as Osborn's are limited to certain
types of problems, you can construct your own checklists to increase the
number of strategies to consider during problem solving. You can experi-
ence the effects of using a creativity checklist by thinking about ways to
Creativit~ and the ID£RL Framework 11f11
McCloskey asked students to predict the path a ball will travel when it
rolls off the edge of a cliff. 23 The answers students gave illustrate some
common misconceptions or faulty assumptions about the laws of physics.
Two of these are shown in Figure l 0. In reality, the ball will move forward
as it descends, following a path that closely approximates a parabola.
Scientists often explore the adequacy of their theoretical assump-
tions by performing thought experiments that involve anticipating out-
comes. A good example of how such thought experiments can be used to
explore and evaluate assumptions was provided by Albert Einstein. 24
Einstein imagined a person riding on a streetcar that was headed away
from a large clock. He then tried to predict how the clock would appear
to the traveler if the car were to travel at the speed of light. He reasoned
that since the light reflecting off the clock would be traveling at the same
speed as the observer, the observer would always see the same image of
the clock and therefore time or the clock would appear L have stopped.
The results of this thought experiment helped Einstein to understand the
Creativitq and the IDEAL Framework 11m
Most people have a difficult time solving these verbal puzzles, especially
the first and third. The reason is that they are making assumptions that
block the solutions.
Consider the first problem. You probably assumed that the super-
psychic could predict the final score of any game. But the question really
asks about the score of any game before it begins. Once you make this
assumption about the score to be predicted the answer is easy: zero to
zero.
In the second problem, you probably assumed that the two men
played each other. Once you realize that this assumption is unnecessary,
the problem is easy to solve.
The third problem is difficult for people who assume that they must
restrict themselves to working with roman numerals and that a line is
necessarily straight line. The problem becomes much simpler when you
break out of these assumptions. You can add a curved line (an S) to IX to
make SIX.
The idea of examining routine assumptions that are taken for
granted is important in all areas of inquiry. As an illustration, consider the
Copernican revolution. Copernicus was a careful and creative scientist
who eventually solved a problem that others before him had failed to
solve: how to account for the irregular movements of the planets in the
heavens. Astronomers had collected data indicating where various plan-
ets were at particular points in time (for example, during different
months), but no one had been able to come up with a satisfying theory
that explained why planetary movement appeared to deviate from the
Creativit~ and the IDEHL Framework . .
I left work at 4:30 P.M. on Friday and got caught in the usual rush-hour
traffic. When I arrived home I decided to take a shower before preparing
for my trip that weekend. After packing my clothes and putting food out
for the cat I decided to make a sandwich. To my horror the refrigerator
was not working. Since I would be gone all weekend it was important to
get it fixed before leaving early next morning. I immediately looked in the
phone book and called the only refrigerator repair service. Unfortunate-
ly, no one answered the phone. This was very aggravating since the adver-
tisement said it was open until 5 P.M. Monday through Friday and the
clock on my kitchen wall showed it to be 20 minutes before 5. In
desperation, I went out and bought a new refrigerator at the all-night
department store.
Did you notice any facts that appeared inconsistent with the
problem's being specific to the refrigerator? That the clock in the kitchen
read 4:40, which is only 10 minutes from the time the person left work
(and took a shower, packed, and fed the cat), could indicate that the
electrical power in the kitchen was not operating properly. To evaluate
further the assumption that the problem was localized in the refrigerator,
it would have been helpful to check other appliances in the kitchen and
the circuit-breaker panel. By searching for inconsistencies between what
we observe and what we assume to be true, we can discover assumptions
Creativitq and the IDEA~ Framework -
that limit our ability to respond creatively to novel problems and reenter
the IDEAL cycle.
For example, he defined his goal first as "How can I catch the bird?" and
next as "How can I catch the bird without hurting either it or myself?"
Neither of these definitions led to a solution. Approximately two minutes
after the bird entered the room, the teacher redefined the goal as "How
can I get the bird to leave the room?" This led to a strategy that worked
on the first try (see Appendix A).
I Summarq
Creativity is the ability to generate ideas and that are both novel and
appropriate. Everyone is creative to some degree, and we can all improve
our ability in this area. From the perspective of the IDEAL framework,
efforts to enhance creativity involve each component of IDEAL.
A hallmark of creative individuals is their ability to identify impor-
tant problems and opportunities that others have missed or taken for
granted. Tests of divergent thinking do not tap this ability and hence miss
a very important aspect of creative behavior. By actively attempting to
identify important problems and opportunities, people can enhance their
creativity.
Creativity can also be enhanced by learning to define a minimum of
two different goals for any problem, rather than jumping right to the
generation of possible solution strategies, as most people do. Attention to
goals helps us uncover a host of unexamined assumptions that can block
our ability to think in nonroutine ways.
Once goals are defined, there are a variety of strategies for thinking
more creatively about them. Examples include the use of the PMI (plus,
minus, interesting) strategy, the fractionation strategy, analogies, creativ-
ity checklists, brainstorming, and incubation (sleeping on the problem).
It is important not to evaluate ideas prematurely, because evaluations can
act as idea killers that hamper nonroutine thought.
Since the goal of being creative is to generate ideas and behave in
ways that are both novel and appropriate, it is very important to an-
ticipate the effects of implementing novel strategies. There are many
strategies that enhance the probability of anticipating both negative and
positive outcomes that might otherwise be unforeseen.
The ability to be creative depends in part on our ability to make
aspects of complex problems routine so that our attention is not over-
whelmed. If you look at a situation and decide that your responses were
not as creative as you desired, it can be helpful to ask whether part of the
problem is that you were overwhelmed by the complexity of the task.
Creativit~ and the IDER~ Framework . .
I Exercises
Use the IDEAL Problem Navigation Guide (in Appendix C) to help you
work through a real-world problem and explore creative alternatives.
Follow the suggestions given in this chapter and remember to define at
least three different goals for solving your problem.
tion? Note: Bouncing the ball is not permitted, nor may you
put a spin on the ball and roll it (this is really a form of bounc-
ing it) or tie anything to the ball.
5. Look at the nine dots below. Connect all of them using only
four straight lines, never retracing a line or removing your
pen or pencil from the paper as you draw.
• • •
• • •
• • •
6. Try solving this mystery. A county sheriff arrived at the scene
of an apparent homicide and found the victim lying on the
side of the road, dead. The only clue to the crime was a pair
of tire tracks left on the little-traveled dirt road. The sheriff
followed the tracks to a country farmhouse less than a mile
away. Although there were three men sitting on the front
porch, the sheriff was certain that the man he wanted for
questioning was sitting in the middle, even though he knew
that none of the men had a car and none had mud on his
boots. How did the sheriff know he should question the man
sitting in the middle?
7. Do you see any inconsistencies in the following passage? If so,
are there ways they might be resolved? The man was wor-
ried. His car came to a halt, and he was all alone. It was ex-
tremely dark and cold. The man took off his overcoat, rolled
down the window, and got out of the car as quickly as pos-
sible. Then he used all his strength to move as fast as he
could. He was relieved when he finally saw the lights of the
city, even though they were far away.
8. A farmer ate two eggs every morning for breakfast. He had no
chickens; nobody ever gave him any eggs; and he never
bought, borrowed, begged, or stole any eggs. Where did he
get the eggs? 32
9. A goat is tied to a rope. The rope is only 5 feet long, yet the
Creativitq and the IDEAL Framework . .
goat can reach a pile of hay that is 10 feet away. How is this
possible?
10. In 1969 it was reported that a young woman gymnast in New
York City fell from a window sill on the 25th floor and was
not badly hurt. There was nothing special to cushion the fall
or reduce the impact. How was this possible?
11. Jim and Tom find a long piece of pipe in a vacant lot. It's big
enough that each boy can just manage to squeeze into it and
crawl from one end to the other. If Jim and Tom go into the
pipe from opposite ends, is it possible for each to crawl the en-
tire length of the pipe and come out the other end? 33
12 . How can a boy stand behind his mother and at the same time
have his mother stand behind him? No stepmothers or
grandmothers are involved.
13. A man walked through a pouring rain for 20 minutes without
getting a single hair on his head wet. He didn't wear a hat,
carry an umbrella, or hold anything over his head. His clothes
got soaked. How could this happen?b
14. Several years ago a new baby was born to Bill's family. This
new baby wasn't Bill's son or daughter, his brother or sister,
his nephew or niece. In fact, it was not his relative. How is
this possible? 34
I Notes
1. A. Jay, Management and Machiavelli; An Inquiry into the Politics of Cor-
porate Life. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston, 1967.
2. D. N. Perkins, The Mind's Best Work. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard
University Press, 1981 .
3. Styrofoam blitz. Time (July 6, 1992):20.
4. W. T. Brown, Creativity: What are we to measure? In J. A. Glover,
R. R. Ronning, and C. R. Reynolds (eds.), Handbook of Creativity.
New York: Plenum Press, 1989.
5. M. Scardamalia and C. Bereiter, Higher levels of agency for
children in knowledge building: A challenge for the design of new
knowledge media. Journal ofthe Learning Sciences 1 (1991):37-68 .
6. L. G. Bolman and T. E. Deal, Reframing Organizations: Artistry,
Choice, and Leadership. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass, 1991.
7. N. R. F. Maier, Reasoning in humans II: The solution of a problem
1EJ1 RFrameworkfor Using Hnowledge Hore Effective!~
I Suggested Readings
Practical!~ Oriented Readings
Adams, J. L. 1986. Conceptual Blockbusting: A Guide to Better Ideas (3d ed.).
Reading, Mass.: Addison-Wesley.
Anderson, B. F. 1980. Chap. 4, "Creative Thinking," in The Complete
Thinker. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall.
Anderson, H. H. (ed.). 1959. Creativity and Its Cultivation. New York: Har-
per & Row.
de Bono, E. 1970. Lateral Thinking. New York: Harper & Row.
Gordon, W. J. 1961. Synectics: The Development of Creative Capacity. New
York: Harper & Row.
McKim, R. H. 1980. Experiences in Visual Thinking (2d. ed). Monterey,
Calif.: Brooks/Cole.
Osborn, A. F. 1963. Applied Imagination: Principles and Procedures of Creative
Problem Solving (3d rev. ed.). New York: Scribners.
. . HFramework for Using Hnowledge More Effective I~
The most important factor in the training ofgood mental habits consists
in acquiring the attitude of suspended conclusion, and in mastering the
various methods of searching for new materials to corroborate or to
refute the first suggestions that occur.
John Dewey 1
Factual
Assumptions Reasoning
evidence
Figure 11
negative-that is, if you are unable to show that the argument is factually
inaccurate-you may reenter the IDEAL cycle and redefine your goals
(for example, to determine whether the argument's conclusions follow
logically or whether the argument is based on inappropriate assump-
tions).
knows someone who is willing to part with an andent coin for a fraction
of its worth. You examine the coin and observe that it indeed looks
authentic. The counselor emphasizes that since the coin is very old (the
date stamped on it is 42 B.c.), it should be worth at least what you are
being asked to pay (which is $9100) . He also explains that the seller is
willing to part with the coin at this low price only because he needs the
money immediately and it usually takes about a month to sell an antique
coin at its fair value . Would you buy the coin? Why or why not?
If you dedded to buy the coin you also bought a faulty argument.
One of the facts used to support the claim that the coin is ancient cannot
be true. To stamp a coin 42 B.c., the coin maker would have had to know
in advance not only the exact year of the birth of Christ, but also that his
birth would be the basis for dating in the future. Since this is extremely
doubtful, the coin clearly is a fake.
A claim that a coin was minted in 42 B.c. is not necessarily false. It
is possible to find a coin this old. The problem with the argument is not
the claim itself but the evidence for the claim (that the date was printed on
the coin). Geologists and archaeologists make claims about the age of
various entities (fossils and skeletons), but they do so by providing
evidence that seems reasonable given current scientific knowledge (they
often use radioactive isotopes to date items). Effective critics pay careful
attention to the nature of the evidence on which claims are based. 5
How do you interpret this statement? Many people assume that brand A
has been shown to be superior to other brands. This is only an invited
inference, however. Legally, "unsurpassed" means that the researchers
have found no differences in the effectiveness of brand A versus other
brands.
Here is another example of a possible claim.
. . AFramework tor Using Hnowledge More Effective!~
Time~
What is missing here is any explicit mention of what was used for com-
parison. The advertised brand could have been compared to uncooked
cabbage, for example. Such information is often omitted in the hope that
consumers will make the invited inference that a reasonable comparison
group was used.
Look at the graph in Figure 12, which appeared in an advertisement
for a new weight-reducing product. Many people who see this graph
conclude that, by using the product advertised, they could lose a lot of
weight in a short period of time. However, if you examine the graph
more carefully you will notice that the units of weight loss and time are
not specified. The conclusion that the product is highly effective is only
an invited inference. It is possible, for example, that in actual tests the
new product produced a reduction of only an ounce in six months. When
analyzing graphs and charts it is always important to check the units of
measurement to evaluate the claims being made.
Scientific "Facts"
It would be unfair to imply that the purpose of all advertisements is to
mislead. Many ads are designed to inform us about products that are
quite good. Furthermore, misleading arguments are by no means con-
fined to advertising. Arguments found in newspapers, textbooks, conver-
sations, and even scientific articles may be based on questionable facts.
Intelligent Criticism . .
The following data show the number of deaths per million boat
passengers in 1973: 6
When asked to draw conclusions on the basis of these data, many people
state that there are fewer deaths in boats with motors, and they go on to
explain why these are safer. For example, a motor provides an alternative
means of transportation in case of an accident like a broken arm or a torn
sail; also, a motor allows one to travel faster and hence avoid such
problems as storms and high winds. In general, people think the data
show that boats with motors are safer than boats without motors. This state-
ment, therefore, appears to be a fact.
If you think about it, however, you will recognize a problem with
this "factual" statement, which is that there are many differences be-
tween boats besides the presence or absence of motors. For example,
white-water canoes would never have a motor, whereas canoes used on
calm lakes (which are generally safer than fast-moving streams) would
be much more likely to have one. And only the larger sailboats (those less
likely to capsize) are likely to have motors. Finally, inexperienced sailors
are more likely to have smaller, less stable boats. Given these considera-
tions, it is questionable that motors alone account for differences in
safety. Indeed, in many situations (such as canoeing on a fast river or
sailing a small boat on a windy lake) it seems that the presence of a motor
could make a boat less safe.
Many types of claims are based on similar types of data. For ex-
ample:
Do the data support this factual claim?It is quite possible that exposure
to computers increases people's liking for them. Nevertheless, the data
presented by the committee chairman do not really support this claim.
The chairman is suggesting that experience with computers causes people
to like them. But the opposite is also possible; namely, that people who
like computers are apt to seek to learn more about them, and such people
therefore have more experience with them. It is the degree of liking that
causes the contact with computers.
The data do not allow us to conclude that liking causes contact with
computers, that contact causes liking, or that each affects the other. The
reason is that the data involve only correlations between two variables.
Evidence for a correlation between variables (liking and contact with
computers or motors on boats and safety) does not necessarily include
evidence that either variable causes the other. Indeed, both might be
caused by a third variable. For example, it is not difficult to find a positive
correlation between increases in teacher salaries and increases in alcohol
consumption. This doesn't necessarily mean that teachers use the addi-
tional money to buy liquor. Instead, the correlation is probably due to
some third variable that affects raises as well as consumption. A likely
candidate would be growth in the general economy which would allow
schools to pay teachers more and increase the disposable income of other
people as well.
College professors are asked to indicate the degree to which they agree
with various statements. One is "Students who turn in papers that are
typed tend to get higher grades than students who do not type their
papers." Most of the professors surveyed indicate that they agree with
this statement. On the basis of these data, a typewriter company recom-
mends that students learn to type so that they can get better grades. Do
the data support this conclusion? Why or why not?
Sid Slye wants to sell you a special gas-saving device for your car for only
$1 00. "How do I know it works?" you ask. "That's easy," says Sid. "I have
one on my car and it gets 45 miles per gallon. My other car doesn't have
one, and it gets only 25 miles per gallon." Is this enough information to
convince you that the device works?
Can you conclude from these data that people who make more money
are less likely to worry about collecting rebates? Why or why not?
Argument 1
Some theories that have been criticized have turned out to be
valid (major premise).
My theory is being criticized (minor premise).
Therefore, it could turn out to be valid (conclusion) .
Argument 2
Some theories that have been criticized have turned out to be
valid (major premise).
My theory is being criticized (minor premise).
Therefore, it is valid (conclusion).
Rgure13 Note that "my theory" could fall into either of the two places in
the diagram marked by asterisks. If it falls into the space designated by the
asterisk on the left, the theory would not be currently accepted as true.
Rgure14 Note that "my theory" could fall into either of the two places in
the diagram marked by asterisks. If it falls into the space designated by the
asterisk on the left, the theory would not be currently accepted as true.
could also have been criticized initially. This is illustrated by the Venn
diagrams shown in Figure 14.
Consider one more hypothetical historical fact, that all new theories
initially criticized turned out to be valid. Given this premise, you would
be in a much stronger position to make the argument that the existence
of criticism proves your theory is valid (see Figure 15) . However, the
historical fact on which this argument is based is not true.
Intelligent Criticism . .
Agure lS Note that "my theory" must be accepted because, in this instance,
all theories that were initially criticized are currently accepted as true .
If-Then Hrguments
Many arguments we make follow an "if-then" structure. You are un-
doubtedly familiar with a very simple if-then statement known as a
guarantee: "If you are not satisfied with our product, [then] we will
refund your money." Another example is insurance policies: "If you have
a car accident, we will pay for damages." The if-then statement is used to
describe a relationship between two events; it tells us what should hap-
pen (we will be able to get our money back) if certain events (we are not
satisfied with the product) occur. Each if-then argument includes what is
called an antecedent (if you are not satisfied with our product) and a
consequent (we will refund your money) . When we are given a clear if-
then statement and find the antecedent condition to be true (we are not
satisfied with the product), we can rightfully expect the consequent con-
dition also to be true (we will to get our money back). This type of
argument illustrates a type of reasoning known as conditional syllogistic
reasoning.
If-then statements occur in a variety of settings. For example, sup-
pose again that you are a lawyer in a trial. This time you are a defense
lawyer. You have established that the defendant has an ironclad alibi for
9 to 12 A.M. on the day of the murder. You might therefore want to argue,
"If the murder occurred between 9 and 12 A.M., my client is innocent."
The form of this argument is "If A, then B."
Assume that the time of the murder is established at 10 A.M . Your
argument would then be "If the murder occurred between 9 and 12 A.M.,
. . RFramework tor Using Hnowledge More Effectivelq
my client is innocent (because of the airtight alibi). The murder did occur
during this time; therefore, my client is innocent." The general form of
this argument is "If A, then B; A, therefore B." This is a valid argument.
Suppose we try to reverse the argument. That is, if the client is truly
innocent, can we conclude that the murder therefore took place at the
time of his alibi; that is, between 9 and 12 A.M.? The answer is no; the
murder could have taken place at any number of times. The form of this
argument is "If A, then B; B, therefore A." Philosophers refer to this form
of argument as the fallacy of affirming the consequent (where A is the
antecedent and B is the consequent). This is an invalid form of argument.
It is instructive to note that many tests of scientific theories take the
form, "If A (this theory is correct), then B (the following results should be
obtained)." For example, a theory of the effects of imagery on memory
might predict that people who form images of information will remem-
ber more than people who do not form images (see Chapter 6 for a
discussion of imagery) . Assume that an experiment is conducted, and the
prediction holds. Can we conclude that the imagery theory is correct?
Note that the form of this argument would be "If A (the imagery theory
is correct), then B (the group told to form images should remember
more); B (the imagery group did remember more), therefore A (the
imagery theory is correct)." As noted above, this is an invalid form of
argument because it rests on the fallacy of affirming the consequent. For
this reason logicians caution that one cannot prove a theory correct.
Instead, one can only say, "These results are consistent with the theory"
or "These results provide support for the theory."
There are two more if-then argument forms; one is "If A, then B;
not A, therefore not B." Returning to the trial example, this translates as
"If the murder occurred between 9 and 12 A.M., my client is innocent.
The murder did not occur during this time; therefore, my client is not
innocent." This is not an argument you would want to make as a defense
lawyer. Fortunately, it also involves what philosophers call the fallacy of
denying the antecedent and is an invalid form of argument. In this case,
it is possible that the client was fishing alone at a remote cabin, but no
one else may have known it; hence, he could not prove that he was not
at the scene of the murder when the victim was killed. Nevertheless, the
absence of an alibi does not prove the presence of guilt.
The fourth form of if-then argument is "If the murder occurred
between 9 and 12 A.M., my client is innocent. My client is not innocent,
therefore the murder did not occur between 9 and 12 A. M. " Although as
a defense lawyer you might be reluctant to admit that your client is not
innocent, the form of this argument is valid. The form is "If A, then B; not
Intelligent Criticism Eil
Mmnntl
ILLUSTRATIONS OFVALID AND INVALID ARGUMENT FORMS
rules, a simple example may help you generate the correct rules on your
own. Start with an antecedent such as "If it snows six feet tonight" and
add the consequent "then public schools will be closed tomorrow." As-
sume that this relationship will always be true. To determine if any
particular form of argument is valid (B is true; therefore A is true) simply
examine how those arguments would be represented in terms of the
familiar case. For example, to remember if situation 2 (in Table l) is valid,
consider the following: Schools are closed today (B is true); can we
therefore conclude that it snowed six feet the night before (A is true)?
Obviously this is not a valid conclusion, since school may be closed for
other reasons (such as a holiday, a fire, or summer vacation). If school is
not closed (B is not true), can we conclude that it did not snow six feet
the night before (A is not true)? This is a valid conclusion. Try generating
the other situations in Table l using this model and then try to solve a
different set of arguments without referring to the table. For example,
consider the premise "If it is a triangle, then it is red." Now try to answer
the following questions: If the shape is not a triangle, is it not red? If it is
not red, is it not a triangle? (See Appendix A for answers.)
sponsibility for the same task (such as baking a cake) and each ap-
proaches it in a different way.
It is instructive to define the contexts in which each of the other two
proverbs are and are not appropriate . As you do so, you will begin to
appreciate how looking for inconsistencies and then attempting to
resolve them can lead to new insights that previously were taken for
granted, such as the need to define the conditions of applicability of an
idea. Harold Kushner 11 provides a fascinating look at one person's at-
tempt to create a logically consistent set of beliefs to explain people's
relationship to God.
The search for inconsistencies is very important in science. For ex-
ample, in discussing the search for black holes in space, Nigel Calder
describes the work of scientists who studied a galaxy of stars orbiting the
galaxy's center. 12 The scientists knew that the speed of rotation of stars
around a center or core depends on the mass of the core. The speed of the
stars being studied was calculated and found to be extremely fast, ap-
proximately 250 miles per second. Based on this information it was pos-
sible to calculate the mass of the core necessary to sustain such
movement. The calculations revealed that the mass must be enormous,
roughly 5000 million times greater than the sun's.
Calder notes that the scientists also had access to an electronic light
detector that allowed them to measure the amount of light (brightness)
generated by the core being studied. He states, "If there was an enormous
number of stars at the core, corresponding to the mass, the heart of the
galaxy would be very bright indeed." (Note the if-then reasoning used.)
When the scientists measured the light they found only a relatively faint
glow, not a dazzling display; the appearance of the core was therefore not
what one would expect if it were composed of stars. Calder goes on to
observe that the information about the appearance of the core was inconsistent
with calculations of the mass of the core, unless the core contained a black hole.
The detection of a possible inconsistency was therefore very important; it
showed the need to postulate something-a black hole-that would
allow the apparent inconsistency between the appearance and the mass
of the core to be resolved.
and (2) look for logical flaws and inconsistencies. The third strategy is to
evaluate the assumptions on which an argument is based.
The evaluation of assumptions is especially important. For example,
the fact that most arguments are based on sets of assumptions can make
it difficult to apply some of the idealized rules of logic discussed earlier.
Consider the premise "If John invents a better mousetrap, he will become
rich." From our previous discussion we can conclude that it is not valid to
argue that John must have invented a better mousetrap because he is
now rich (that is, it is invalid to reason "If A, then B; B is true, therefore
A is true"). Similarly, we know that it is not valid to say that John will
never become rich because he did not invent a better mousetrap (it is
invalid to reason "If A, then B; not A, therefore not B"). However, im-
agine that at some time in the future we meet John and find that he has
never become very wealthy. Can we conclude that John never invented
a better mousetrap? Logically, the form of the argument is "If A, then B;
not B, therefore not A." As discussed earlier, this is a valid form of
argument.
However, the logical forms discussed earlier represent ideal situa-
tions. In reality, John may have invented a better mousetrap but never
have marketed it successfully, or people may have stopped buying
mousetraps. What appears to be a valid argument logically (If A, then B;
not B, therefore not A) has become an ambiguous situation in the real
world. One reason for this ambiguity is that the truth of the relationship
expressed in our premise ("If John invents a better mousetrap, then he
will become rich") depends on the existence of other conditions or assumptions
(that mousetraps continue to be purchased, that John employs a good
marketing strategy, or that John finds a financial backer). Generally,
whenever we include events and observations from the real world in our
relationships,, we import additional assumptions or conditions that may
not be obvious immediately. However, these assumptions may have an
important effect on the reasonableness of the arguments we make. 13
John is able to come to the party tonight because his car broke down.
A woman called the police and stated that she had just murdered her
husband by shooting him. The police went to the house and found that,
sure enough, the man had just been shot. Despite the woman's finger-
prints on the gun and her testimony (she also passed a lie detector test),
the courts were unable to punish her by putting her in jail or sentencing
her to death. Why?
ticular analogy is appropriate, and if so, what its exact implications are
supposed to be .
Consider a controversial topic, such as whether couples should have
sex before marriage. One response to this question is to say, "You
wouldn't buy a car without taking it out for a test drive, would you?"
Although some might agree with this argument, others may be offended
by the inappropriateness of the analogy. For example, unlike a car, a
spouse does not become one's property, and unlike problems with a car,
those between couples are usually a two-way street. Furthermore, al-
though people usually don't feel guilty after taking a test drive, many feel
guilty after premarital sex.
You can undoubtedly find additional problems with the test-drive
analogy, but the basic point is clear: The analogy carries with it a host of
assumptions that are inappropriate and misleading. Assumptions have
powerful effects on our reasoning and it is therefore important to analyze
those we make as well as those we are invited to make by others.
At the beginning of this chapter, we noted that many ideologies are
closed systems that are difficult to crack because they are based on as-
sumptions considered to be immutable. For example, imagine a person
whose beliefs are based on the assumption that you are the enemy and
that one's enemy can never be trusted. Unless you can show that those
assumptions are faulty, it is doubtful that any logical argument you make
will be convincing. Thus, in addition to critically evaluating ideas, we also
need to be able to communicate our arguments effectively. In Chapter 5
we will examine how the IDEAL framework can be applied to problems
of communication.
I Summar~
Ideologies can play a powerful role in motivating human action.
Throughout history, a great deal of suffering has resulted from the un-
critical acceptance of ideologies that have motivated people to harm one
another. It is therefore important for people not to necessarily accept
everything they hear or read. They need to be intelligent critics who can
identify possible flaws in their own arguments as well as in arguments
made by others. Subsequently, they can define the goal of their critical
analysis (for example, to evaluate the accuracy of factual claims), explore
strategies for analyzing and correcting an argument, anticipate outcomes
and act on the most promising strategies, and look at the effects. If the
Intelligent Criticism liB
argument still seems faulty, they can reenter the IDEAL cycle and try
again.
There are three general goals to adopt when evaluating an argu-
ment. One is the possibility that the argument is based on inaccurate
factual claims. Note that people who make such claims are not necessari-
ly trying to be misleading or dishonest. Instead, they may simply fail to
realize that their interpretations are in error.
A second cause of faulty arguments is the use of inappropriate logic
or reasoning strategies. Someone may begin with an acceptable factual
claim ("Many theories that later turned out to be true were at first
ridiculed") but end up with a conclusion that does not follow from the
facts ("Therefore, since my theory is being criticized, it must be true").
Similarly, arguments can contain inconsistencies. For example, an author
may at first state that the views of a historical figure never changed on a
particular issue yet provide an example later on indicating that they did.
A third reason for criticism involves the assumptions that form the
basis of an argument. An argument will often seem valid if we grant the
underlying assumption ("Since war is inevitable, we should make the
first strike"). However, once the basic assumption (that war is inevitable)
is identified and questioned, an argument will frequently lose its force.
The use of analogies and metaphors also involves assumptions that may
or may not be appropriate. Unless these are analyzed, they can lead us
astray.
I Exercises
Use the IDEAL Problem Navigation Guide (in Appendix C) to help you
work through a problem involving critical thinking in the real world.
Remember to consider the goals identified in Figure 11.
cent, we can one conclude that the Federal Reserve Bank did
not raise interest rates?
15. Assume that the following is true : "Putting more money into
public education will not necessarily improve education." Can
we conclude that we can improve public education if we do
not put more money into public education?
16. To test a theory that RNA is used to store information in the
brain, scientists injected 20 people with RNA. They found
that the 20 people performed no better on a memory !est
after they were injected with RNA than before. Have they dis-
proved the theory that RNA is used to store information in
the brain?
17. A publication reports that people in hospitals who are
depressed recuperate much more slowly and less completely
than people who are not depressed. The article goes on to ad-
vocate increased psychological counseling for depressed
patients to improve their chances of recovery. What type of
factual evidence probably supports this conclusion? Are there
alternative explanations for this evidence?
18. Read the following game rules and then answer the questions
below.
This is a two-player game. The players start at opposite
corners of a checkerboard. The players move around the out-
side edge only. The object of the game is to reach the other
player's corner first. Each player rolls the dice, and the player
with the highest roll goes first. This player also picks a direc-
tion, either clockwise or counterclockwise. The other player
must move in the opposite direction. On each turn the dice
are rolled and the player with the highest roll advances one
square on the board. The other player does not move on that
turn. Players cannot occupy the same square at the same time.
a. Does the player who moves first have an advantage?
b. How long will the average game last?
c. If you played 100 games, how many could you expect to win?
Many riddles involving logical reasoning center around the problem of
differentiating a liar from one who tells the truth. Here are two.
19. You are at a road junction with two alternative paths and need
directions about which path to take to the nearest town, but
you don't know if the only person there to help you is a liar or
Intelligent Criticism Ia
I Notes
1. J. Dewey, How We Think. Boston: D. C. Heath, 1910.
2. W. N. Dember, Cognition, motivation, and emotion: Ideology
revisited. In R. R. Hoffman and D. S. Palermo (eds.), Cognition and
the Symbolic Processes: Applied and Ecological Perspectives. Hillsdale,
N.J .: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1991.
3. A. Koestler, Janus. New York: Random House, 1978, p. 14.
4. V. Bugliosi, 'Till Death Us Do Part. New York: Bantam, 1979.
5. See D. Huff, How to Lie with Statistics. New York: W. W. Norton,
1954.
6. These data are from the National Safety Council, 1973.
7. More extensive discussion of experimental designs can be found in
D. G. Elmes, B. H. Kantowitz, and H. L. Roediger III, Research Meth-
ods in Psychology (4th ed.). St. Paul, Minn. : West, 1992; D. Radner
and M. Radner, Science and Unreason . Belmont, Calif.: Wadsworth,
1982; D. H. McBurney, Experimental Psychology (2nd. ed.). Bel-
mont, Calif.: Wadsworth, 1990; M. Mitchell and J. Jolley, Research
Design Explained (2nd. ed.). Fort Worth: Harcourt Brace Jovan-
ovich, 1992.
8. More extensive discussions of reasoning can be found in M.
Scriven, Reasoning. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1976; S. Toulmin, The
Uses of Argument. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press,
1958; S. Toulmin, R. Rieke, and A. Janik, An Introduction to Reason-
ing. New York: Macmillan, 1979.
IIDJ RFramework for Using Hnowledge More Effectivelq
9. SeeM. Gardner, Fads and Fallades in the Name of Science. New York:
Dover, 1957.
10. P. C. Wason, Reasoning about a rule. Quarterly Journal of Experimen-
tal Psychology 20 (1968):273..:...281.
11. H. S. Kushner, When Bad Things Happen to Good People. New York:
Avon, 1981.
12. N. Calder, Einstein's Universe. New York: Penguin, 1980.
13. T. Kuhn, The Structure of Scientific Revolutions. Chicago: University of
Chicago Press, 1962.
14. These examples are from J.D. Bransford and N. S. McCarrell, A
sketch of a cognitive approach to comprehension. In W. Weimer
and D. Palermo (eds.), Cognition and the Symbolic Processes. Hillsdale,
N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1974.
I Suggested Readings
Practicallq Oriented Readings
Anderson, B. F. 1980. Chap. 3, "Reasoning," in The Complete Thinker.
Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall.
Beardsley, M. C. 1950. Thinking Straight: Principles of Reasoning for Readers
and Writers. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall.
Campbell, S. K. 1974. Flaws and Fallacies in Statistical Thinking. Englewood
Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall.
Fogelin, R. J. 1978. Understanding Arguments: An Introduction to Informal
Logic. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.
Halpern, D. F. 1989. Thought and Knowledge: An Introduction to Critical
Thinking (2d. ed.). Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Huff, D. 1954. How to Lie with Statistics. New York: W. W. Norton.
Radner, D., and M. Radner. 1982. Science and Unreason. Belmont, Calif.:
Wadsworth.
Scriven, M. 1976. Reasoning. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Toulmin, S., R. Rieke, and A. Janik. 1979. An Introduction to Reasoning.
New York: Macmillan.
Whimbey, A., and R. Lochhead. 1982. Chap. 4, "Verbal Reasoning
Problems," Chap. 6, "Analogies," Chap. 7, "Writing Relationship
Sentences," and Chap. 8, "How to Form Analogies," in Problem Solv-
ing and Comprehension. Philadelphia: Franklin Institute Press.
Zechmeister, E. B. and J. E. Johnson. 1992. Critical thinking: A Functional
Approach. Belmont, Calif.: Wadsworth.
Intelligent Criticism . . .
What is there greater than the word which persuades the judges in
the courts, or the senators in the council, or the citizens in the assembly,
or at any other political meeting?
Plato 1
Presentation
Medium
Agure 16
I Message
Sometimes people adopt a message that is actually ambiguous but that
they want to seem clear. For example, imagine that your goal is to win an
election. If the electorate (your audience) includes people with conflict-
ing sets of beliefs, you may purposely try to be ambiguous. Thus you
might say, "You know what is needed and I am going to do it," in the
hope that everyone will make the invited inference that you agree with
his or her ideas. The goal of ambiguity may not be lofty, but it seems to be
one that people sometimes adopt.
We will assume, however, that your goal is to be clear rather than
ambiguous. You therefore need to have a clear idea of your message. This
may seem obvious, but it is amazing how often people are unclear about
what they really want to communicate. They have a general idea of their
message but when asked to put it into words, they often discover that it's
not so easy to do.
Consider the following introduction to a presentation on
economics. 2
111r1 AFrameworh for Using Hnowledge More Effective!~
The persistent economic woes that now plague the United States will not
be vanquished, or even substantially curbed, until new currents of thought
emerge within the federal government that will force it to commence with
the difficult policies required to assuage our present problems.
Daniel Fader describes his interactions with the economics student who
wrote this introduction. Fader's first request was that he state what he
meant by the sentence. The student's response to Fader's question was as
follows:
Well, yeah, America 's worst economic problems won 't be solved until the
government, until the government, comes up with some tough new policies.
Fader asked the student why he had not written this, instead of the
"woes" and "plague" and "vanquished" and "assuage." The student's
answer is revealing.
That's just the kind of stuff you use to write about economics and things
like that.
It is possible that some audiences are more impressed by sophisti-
cated-sounding words than by the clarity of the message, but this has not
been our experience. Most people value presentations that provide a
clear and concise statement of the key ideas.
Fader's strategy for helping the economics student better under-
stand his message was simple but powerful. He had the student restate
the message in his own words and keep the statement as simple as
possible. Fader notes that as the student tried to paraphrase his introduc-
tion, he developed a much better understanding of the issues. In the end,
he began his presentation by advising the government to develop tough
new economic policies based on economic facts rather than economic
politics.3 This was a much clearer introduction to the message the student
wanted to present.
Some people use the strategy of reformulating their message into
two or three "talking points" that summarize the essence of what they
wish to communicate. This can be very beneficial in helping you clarify
what you want to say.
I Audience
As you clarify your message it is also important to define your audience.
Different communication strategies are necessary depending on the
Effective Communication . . .
If the balloons popped, the sound would not be able to carry since every-
thing would be too far away from the correct floor. A closed window
would also prevent the sound from carrying, since most buildings tend to
be well insulated. Since the whole operation depends on a steady flow of
electricity, a break in the middle of the wire would also cause problems.
Of course the fellow could shout, but the human voice is not loud
enough to carry that far. An additional problem is that a string could
break on the instrument. Then there could be no accompaniment to the
message. It is clear that the best situation would involve less distance.
Then there would be fewer potential problems. With face-to-face con-
tact, the least number of things could go wrong.
D
0
DO
DOD
Rgure 17A picture that allows people to understand the passage about the
balloons.
lower.... When stars or gas [come] close to it they [swirl] in faster and
faster like water approaching a plug hole.'' 6 This is a very helpful analogy
provided that people already know what it is like for water to swirl down
a drain or plug hole. To evaluate the appropriateness of analogies and
examples, you must know something about your audience's knowledge
base.
Factual information may also need to be presented in different ways
depending on your audience. For example, imagine that you want to
make the following claim: "By the time they graduate from high school,
American children have watched an average of 18,000 hours of
television." Whether or not your audience will understand the sig-
nificance of this depends on how familiar they are with thinking in terms
Effective Communication IIIDI
of thousands of hours. Usually you will need to make such a fact more
meaningful. For example, you might tell your audience that this figure
amounts to several hours of TV viewing each day, or you could point out
that 18,000 hours is more than most of these students have spent in
school. The units and comparisons that are most meaningful will depend
on what your audience already knows.
One of the major limitations of school-based instruction in com-
munication is that the presenter's audience is usually very limited, con-
sisting of the teacher and fellow students. But in everyday life, one has to
communicate with people who have different levels of knowledge, ex-
perience, and expectations. In these circumstances, it is important to
explore what the audience already knows about your topic and what it
expects from you.
James Steele uncovered the following passage in the U.S. Tax Reform Act
of 1986.
Lutz notes that this passage specifically created a tax loophole for the
partners of Bear Stearns and Companies, Inc., that saved them approx-
imately $8 million in taxes. Some other examples of doublespeak are
listed below.
boring and weak compared with the other speakers. It is very important
to make sure that your goals fit in with the audience's expectations. If
you want to keep your standards high, you will probably find yourself
turning down invitations to speak in some settings where the expecta-
tions do not match your own.
Several years ago one of us went to an academic symposium that
was attended by more than 500 people and featured four nationally
known speakers. Each speaker spoke for about 20 minutes, followed by
questions and comments. The room became tense as the fourth speaker
presented data that called into question a theory that the first speaker
had published several years earlier. After the fourth speaker finished, the
moderator asked the first speaker if he wanted to reply. You could have
heard a pin drop as the first speaker approached the podium. He began,
"About my paper where I argued that. . .. " Then he paused and, with a
grin, said, "Can I take that back?" The room broke into applause.
This first speaker's courage taught us a powerful lesson. Science is
based on respect for data rather than for slick performances. The speaker
demonstrated his respect for the data, and the audience greatly appre-
ciated his acknowledgment. But not all audiences are like this.
I Presentation Medium
The third component of MAP is the presentation medium you use when
presenting your message . Possible formats include oral presentations,
written presentations, interactive software, and interactive multimedia.
Each of these is discussed below.
contrast, writers are usually isolated from their audience, and they are
unable to rely on outside sources of information such as gestures. Writers
must therefore work hard to state their ideas clearly and avoid ambiguity.
Many people do not like this extra work.
some point you will have to supply background on your main character,
you can write this at any time-such as when you need to incubate other
ideas. Writing permlts a great deal of flexibility in the strategies you use. 13
55,000
50,000
...
V>
.!2 45,000
0
"0
.5
...>-
"'
<0
Vl
40,000
35 ,000
30,000
Local National Local National
Di scipline A Discipline B
Rgure 18
Effective Communication liB
Multimedia Presentations
Another way to apply modern technology to communication problems
involves multimedia presentations that combine voice, text, still pictures,
and moving video. These can all be controlled at the touch of a computer
button and so can be used in extremely powerful ways. In a presentation,
video, sound, and other resources can be used to illustrate one's points.
Each medium has different strengths. For example, we find that some
points simply cannot be made forcefully by talking about them, but their
significance immediately becomes apparent in a short videoclip. In other
cases, people prefer written documents over videos because they want to
analyze the information in detail.
Multimedia technology is already changing the standards people
have for effective presentations. In many circles it is no longer sufficient
simply to talk to the audience and use overheads that list the main points.
Instead, audiences want to see and hear what the speakers are talking
about. The good side of these changes is that people are learning to use
the new resources to communicate in powerful new ways. The bad side
is that multimedia can make it easier for glitz to win out over substance.
Ultimately, however, audiences will demand substance from multimedia
presentations.
I Summar~
-
After you have gone through the problem-solving process, have gener-
ated some exciting, creative ideas, and are ready to communicate them to
an important audience, three subgoals must be clearly defined to plan an
effective communication strategy. These subgoals are message, audience,
and presentation, or MAP: define the message you plan to present, define
the audience to which you will present it, and define the presentation
format (oral, written, interactive, multimedia) that you plan to use. Each
of these subgoals affects the selection of an effective communication
strategy.
In considering your message, explore strategies for clarifying what
you really want to say. Although this may seem obvious, it is an impor-
tant step that is often overlooked.
It is also important to carefully define the audience. A message that
is perfectly comprehensible to one audience may be a complete mystery
to another. When we attempt to communicate across cultures we run
into additional problems, since what is understood may be very different
from what we intended.
Also, consider the audience's expectations of a presentation. Does it
expect glitz, hype, and cheerleading or a serious analysis of issues? Fail-
ure to address this question can lead to serious mismatches between
audience and presentation.
When choosing a presentation format, explore the advantages and
disadvantages of different media, including oral communication, writing,
visual illustrations, interactive software, and multimedia.
Finally, it is important to evaluate attempts to communicate and
learn from them. The IDEAL approach, together with the MAP subgoals,
provides a useful framework for evaluating effects.
I Exercises
Use the IDEAL Problem Navigation Guide in Appendix C to help you
work though a real-world communication problem. Remember to con-
sider each of the MAP subgoals illustrated in Figure 16 when you define
your goals.
Effective Communication Ia
I Notes
1. B. Jowett, The Dialogues of Plato, Vol. 1. New York: Random House,
1937.
2. From D. Fader, Narrowing the space between language and text.
In P. L. Stock (ed.), F Forum: Essays on Theory and Practice in the
Teaching of Writing. Upper Montclair, N.J.: Boynton Cook, 1983.
3. See note 2.
4. See especially T. C. Anderson, Role of the reader's schema in com-
prehension, learning and memory. In R. Anderson, J. Osborn, and
R. Tierney (eds.), Learning to Read in American Schools: Basal Readers
and Content Texts. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates,
1984.
5. From J.D. Bransford and M. K. Johnson, Contextual prerequisites
for understanding: Some investigations of comprehension and
recall. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 11 ( 1972) :717-
726.
6. N. Calder, Einstein 's Universe. New York: Penguin, 1980.
7. Cited in Simply Stated: The Monthly Newsletter of the Document Design
Center, American Institute for Research 32 (De.c. 1982-Jan. 1983).
8. W. Lutz, Doublespeak: From "Revenue Enhancement " to "Terminal
Living," How Government, Business, Advertisers, and Others Use Lan-
guage to Deceive You . New York: Harper & Row, 1989.
9. M. S. Steffensen, D. Joag-Deci, and R. C. Anderson, A cross-
cultural perspective on reading comprehension, Reading Research
Quarterly 15 (1979):10-29.
10. H. Klein, Firms seek aid in deciphering Japan's culture. The Wall
Street Journal (Sep. L 1983) :27.
11. See R. Freedle, Interaction of language use with ethnography and
cognition. In J. H. Harvey (ed.), Cognition, Social Behavior and the En-
vironment. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1981.
12. From W. L. Chafe, Speakers and writers do different things. In P. L.
Stock (ed.), F Forum: Essays on Theory and Practice in the Teaching of
Writing. Upper Montclair, N.J.: Boynton Cook, 1983.
13 . Excellent discussions of writing can be found in L. Flower, Problem-
Solving Strategies for Writing . New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich,
1981 ; S. Pearl, Understanding composing. Composition and Com-
munication 31 (1980):363-369; C. BereiterandM. Scardamalia,
The Psychology of Written Communication . Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence
11m RFramework for Using Hnowledge More Effective!~
I Suggested Readings
The unassisted hand and the understanding left to itself possess but
little power. Effects are produced by means of instruments and helps,
which the understanding requires no less than the hand.
Sir Frands Bacon, 1620 1
couch potato hat trumpet boat desk carrot shirt piano cat chair
watermelon pants harp airplane table bread coat violin bicycle bed
tomato glove drum truck
Each of these goals requires the use of different strategies for learning.
Effective problem solvers must be flexible; they must define their goals
.:m11 RModel for Requiring New Hnowledge
Rttention
Imagine that you are a student and need to learn a great deal of informa-
tion in a short time . You missed several classes, so a friend has tape-
recorded lectures from a history class and an economics class. Since you
are so pressed for time, you design a system that allows you to play the
history lecture into your left ear and the economics lecture into your
11111 RModel for Requiring New Hnowledge
right ear at the same time. Will this method allow you to learn twice as
much in the same amount of time?
If you have ever attempted to study when people are interrupting
you, you probably answered no to this question. It is very difficult to
attend carefully to more than one source of information at a time, espe-
cially if both messages contain information that is unfamiliar. 5
Many foods cost too much. Food costs would diminish if farm land was
not so costly. In addition, tractors and additional tools cost way too
much. Politicians should also diminish any tax on farm products. A high
tax on such products hurts us all.
Now try to remember as much about this passage as you can without
looking back. If you studied the passage, the chances are quite good that
you can easily recall its main points. However, without looking back at
the passage, suppose you have l 0 seconds to answer a different question:
"How many words in the passage contain the letter e?" You might be able
to estimate the number of e's by recalling the words and checking each
one, but this will take a considerable amount of time and you will prob-
ably make mistakes. Although most passages of this length include a
considerable number of e's, this one does not. (You may have noted that
the wording was a little strange; that's because it only used words that do
not contain an e).
You would have been able to answer the question about the num-
ber of e's in the passage much more easily if you had known about it
beforehand. You would then have know to focus your attention on this
problem as you read. Had you done so, however, chances are you would
have missed something about the meaning of the passage. Even in a
passage this simple, there is more to attend to than we can process in a
single trial. It is important to define your memory goals ahead of time so
you can attend to the relevant information. 6
Elaboration Processes
The preceding discussion illustrates the importance of attending to infor-
mation that is relevant to your memory goal. Researchers have also
Improving Memorij Skills liB
Now try to answer the following questions without looking back at the
preceding sentences.
Most people have a very difficult time remembering who did what,
despite the fact that each statement is comprehensible. If you really read
these sentences without trying to elaborate them, you could probably
remember only two or three at the most. To remember more of the
sentences you would have to use more sophisticated elaboration strat-
egies, such as thinking of someone you know with a particular name (for
example, a friend named John) and imagining him walking on the roof.
Such sentences become easier to remember if your knowledge base
does much of the work for you. As an illustration, spend approximately
4 seconds reading each of the sentences below. As in the earlier task, do
not attempt to use any sophisticated strategies. Instead, process each sentence as
effortlessly as you can.
Improving Memorq Skills . .
Now answer the following questions without looking back at the list.
Most people can remember this second list of sentences almost ef-
fortlessly. When information is easily related to our knowledge or pre-
vious experience, it is relatively easy to remember. When information is
difficult to relate to previous experience, we need to make use of non-
routine elaboration strategies.
Many people do not automatically use effective elaboration
strategies when they are confronted with information that requires non-
111fJ RModel for Requiring New Knowledge
routine thinking. For example, James Turnure, Nissan Buium, and Mar-
tha Thurlow7 found that retarded children (those with intelligence scores
of around 70) do not elaborate relationships that are difficult to remem-
ber, and consequently their memory performance is much lower than
that of students with higher intelligence test scores. Turnure and his
colleagues noticed dramatic improvements in the memory scores of
retarded children when they were prompted to elaborate confusing
relationships in an effective way. We have found important differences in
the effectiveness of elaboration strategies used by successful and less
successful fifth graders confronted with nonroutine learning tasks. 8
Retrieval Processes
Researchers who study human memory emphasize the difference be-
tween encoding or storing information and accessing or retrieving it later on. 9
You have probably had the experience of trying to remember a person's
name or a certain word and feeling that it was right on the tip of your
tongue. You know from these experiences that retrieval processes are an
important part of remembering.
The following demonstration experiment is designed to illustrate
the importance of retrieval processes. Spend 3 to 5 seconds reading each
of the sentences in the list below, and read through the list only once. As
soon as you finish, remove the list from sight and write down as many of
the sentences as you can. Please begin now.
sheet shoe
rock guitar
telephone scissors
boat leaf
dime brick
board newspaper
pen pan
balloon barrel
ladder rug
record orange
TV antenna bathtub
through your house (for example, through the front door, to various
places in the living room, and into the kitchen) .
Here's how to use the scene you have imagined. Basically, you need
to ( 1) form an image of each object to be remembered, and (2) place it in
your imagined scene. Thus, if the first object is "horse," you might im-
agine it jumping through the front door of your house. If the second
object is "rocket," you might imagine a huge rocket in the second location
you come to in the living room (such as the couch) . Ideally, the visual
scene you use should have as many locations as there are objects to
remember, and you should proceed through your scene in a natural
order that is easy to remember. In addition, it can be helpful to form
images that have vivid and unique interactions.
Below is a list of 10 words for you to attempt to remember by using
the method of loci. Spend enough time on each word to form an image
of it and of its place in a scene that is vivid and unique.
carrot trumpet
moose pillow
helicopter scissors
Indian goat
chicken cherry
Without looking back at the list, try to recall the words in the order in
which they occurred by using the retrieval cues in your familiar path.
Now try to recall them in reverse order. This can be done easily by
reversing the direction in which you move through your imaginary
scene.
For another memory problem, recall as quickly as possible the fifth
word from the preceding list, then the third, then the seventh, and so
forth. Although it is possible to use the method of loci to achieve this goal,
it would be very cumbersome and time-consuming to walk through all
the scenes while counting them. A much better strategy for solving this
type of memory problem is to use images indexed to a numerical code.
The peg-word system is one such strategy. In the peg-word system we
form associated images using objects that rhyme with numbers. For ex-
ample, try forming interactive images of the objects in the peg-word
IIIII HModel for Requiring New Hnowledge
scheme below combined with those in the previous word list (imagine
the first word, carrot, in a bun, for example, and the second word, moose,
with a shoe on its antlers) .
With the peg-word system, you should be able to recall any num-
bered item in a list by using the number as a cue to retrieve the word and
images that were paired with it. Of course, this strategy requires that you
become familiar with the peg-word scheme so that the images can be
retrieved easily.
Interactive Imager~
The key feature of both the method of loci and the peg-word system is
the creation of images that link something you want to learn with some-
thing you already know. Interactive imagery strategies have been studied
by a number of researchers and have been found to work extremely well
for most people.
We have devised a demonstration experiment to illustrate the role
of interactive imagery in memory. Please follow the instructions accu-
rately, otherwise the demonstration will not work. Sixteen pairs of words
are listed below. Simply read each word pair as you go through the list; try to
do nothing more (spend approximately 4 seconds per pair). Please begin now.
clock sheep
telephone dog
cloud ring
rabbit shovel
Improving Memor~ Skills . .
fox lamp
tree radio
pencil cloud
book fence
stove owl
mountain egg
robin flute
tent movie
key snake
snow elephant
rock baby
In the list below is the first half of each of the 16 pairs of words you just
read. When you read each word, try to think of the. word that went with
it, but do not look back at the previous list. Keep track of the number of
times you are successful.
rabbit stove
book bear
mountain pencil
rock tent
telephone key
robin clock
cloud tree
snow fox
liB RModel for Requiring New Hnowledge
The list below contains the first word from each of the pairs above.
Keep track of the number of times these words enable you to remember
the second member of each pair. Most people find that their performance
with this list is much better than their performance with the first list
(unless, of course, they cheated earlier and used a strategy that integrated
the pairs of words). 12
rain worm
gar fan
cake fork
Improving Memorq Skills a.
shark apple
deer hat
newspaper rope
bathtub bus
cup taxicab
This strategy should help you distinguish similar faces from one
another (which can be important if you witness a crime, for example),
but it will not necessarily help you remember the name that goes with
each face. To remember names, identify a facial feature that you can
associate with the person's name or with the syllables of the name. The
picture in Figure 21 provides an illustration of how this correspondence
can be developed.
If you practice the image-name strategy (faces and names are pro-
vided at the end of this chapter) you should become able to learn names
quite effectively. This strategy can also be used for other purposes. For
example, waiters and waitresses can use it to remember what customers
order, and salespersons can use it to remember the family background
and interests of the clients they serve.
Improving Memorq Skills . .
Rgure21 Remembering names and faces . Distinctive facial features might in-
clude the beard and the glasses. Distinctive relationships between the facial
features and the name (Bart Stein) might include letting "beard" remind
you of a bar (for Bart) and "glasses" remind you of beer glasses, called
steins (for Stein) .
Ann ant
Bonnie bony
Sherry cherry
When you meet someone with a name for which you have a picture
equivalent, you can form an interactive image that relates a distinctive
visual feature of the person to your picture equivalent. For example, if
you meet a woman with a prominent forehead named Bonnie, you could
form an image of bone protruding from her forehead . As you become
more familiar with your picture equivalents, it will require less and less
effort to form these interactive images.
IBM
3-M
Dial 526-FILM for theater information
In accounting, people must learn that t he left column is used for enteri ng
debits and the right column is used for entering credits. D evise a scheme
for remembering which is w hich.
11m AHodel tor Acquiring New Hnowledge
People who spend a lot of time in the woods (hikers, campers, and
hunters) need to know which kinds of snakes are poisonous and which
are not. In general, snakes that have adjacent red and black stripes have
no poison whereas those that have adjacent red and yellow stripes do.
Devise a memory scheme that will help people remember which type of
snake is which.
In boating, the term "port" is used for left and "starboard" for right.
Devise a way to remember these facts .
mation stored is large. Consider a phone book for a large city. The infor-
mation it contains is organized in a way that makes it relatively easy to
find a phone number or street address if you know the person's name. It
also makes it easy to determine the number of people in that city who
have the same last name. However, imagine that you want to know the
phone numbers of all the people who live on 21st Avenue or in the
eastern part of the city. You would have to search through every entry in
the phone book to find this information. Needless to say, this would be a
tedious procedure.
In many situations externally stored information (a data base) needs
to be searched from a variety of perspectives. If you run a business, for
example, you may want to call or send announcements to everyone who
lives in the area where it is located. Or you may want to send mailings
only to those whose income is over a certain amount, or only to those
who have visited your store during the past year. Similarly, if you are
taking notes on articles and books, you may sometimes want to refer only
to those by certain authors or about particular topics. As the example of
the phone book indicates, it can be very difficult to store information in
ways that permit easy access to such categories.
Fortunately, modern computer technology is making it possible to
gain access to data bases using a variety of search categories. For example,
computerized data bases in libraries can allow you to search for articles
on the basis of information about the author, title, date of publication,
subjects covered, and key words used in the article. A computerized data
base of cookbook recipes might allow you to search for recipes on the
basis of title, important ingredients, country of origin, or nutritional
value. Computers therefore provide a powerful means of enhancing
memory. Even with computers, however, you must choose the format for
representing and retrieving information that is most appropriate to your
needs. Since the computer's ability to search for information depends on
how data bases are organized and how you query them, your ingenuity
will have important effects on what the computer can do.
I Summar~
In this chapter, our discussion emphasized the relationship between
remembering and problem solving. People who are good at remembering
information develop effective problem-solving skills. First, they are able
to identify situations that may cause memory difficulties ("This is too
much information to hold in short-term memory" or "If I am not careful
1111 HModel tor Requiring New Hnowledge
I will get confused and forget whether port means right or left") . Second,
they define their goals appropriately ("I want to remember whether star-
board refers to the right or left") . Third, they explore a variety of strategies
and select those that are appropriate to their goals. Fourth, they an-
ticipate the effects of different strategies and act on those that are most
promising. Fifth, they look at the effects of particular strategies on perfor-
mance (that is, on their ability to remember) and consider how to ap-
proach future memory problems. If their performance has been poor,
they reenter the IDEAL cycle and may redefine their goals or select or
invent different strategies. They then act on these new strategies and look
at the effects, continuing to improve their memory skills.
It is important to understand the relationship between strategy se-
lection and how we define our memory goals. First, particular strategies
(simple repetition or categorization, for example) are appropriate for
some goals but not for others. Second, it can be useful to examine some
of the basic research on memory. For instance, researchers find that it is
difficult to attend to multiple sources of information, and even with only
one source we must still decide what aspects of that information are
important. Elaboration also plays an important role in remembering in-
formation. While elaboration may be relatively effortless in some situa-
tions, it can require a great deal of effort in others. Another research
finding is that storage of information does not guarantee its retrieval, and
the selection and evaluation of strategies must take into account the
nature of the retrieval environment. If no retrieval cues are going to be
provided, for example, you need to generate a retrieval scheme of your
own. In addition, the probability that particular retrieval cues will be
effective can be increased (by forming interactive images, for example).
Finally, the improvement of memory skills is an ongoing process. A major
challenge is to become skilled at inventing new memory techniques for
solving problems that you may confront in everyday life.
I Exercises
1. Carefully read the passage below once, then turn to Appendix
B and answer the questions about it.
You are the driver of a bus that can hold a total of 72 pas-
sengers (there are 36 seats that can each hold 2 passengers).
At the first stop, 7 people get on the bus. At the next stop, 3
Improving Memor~ Skills Bill
people get off and 5 get on. At the next stop, 4 people get off
and 2 get on. During each of the next two stops, 3 passengers
get off and 2 get on. At the next stop, 5 passengers get off and
7 get on. When the bus arrives at the next-to-the-last stop, 2
people get on and 5 get off.
Devise a strategy to help you remember the correct spelling of each word
below.
Devise a technique for remembering the facts associated with each of the
following names.
Devise a strategy to remember the name associated with each face below.
I Notes
l. F. Bacon, Novum organum. First book, Aphorism 2, 1620.
2. Studies of the effectiveness of various types of memory strategies
include G. H. Bower and M. C. Clark, Narrative stories as
mediators for serial learning. Psychonomic Sdence 14 (1969):181-
182; F. L. M. Craik and R. S. Lockhart, Levels of processing: A
framework for memory research. Journal of Verbal Learning and Ver-
bal Behavior 11 (1972):671-684; F. L. M. Craik and M. J. Watkins,
The role of rehearsal in short-term memory, Journal of Verbal Learn-
ing and Verbal Behavior 12 (1973):599-607; T. S. Hyde and J. J.
Jenkins, Differential effects of incidental tasks on the organization
of recall of a list of highly associated words, Journal of Experimental
Psychology 82 (1969): 472-481; D. Rundus and R. C. Atkinson, Re-
hearsal processes in free recall: A procedure for direct observation.
Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 9 ( 1970):99-1 OS; M .
A. McDaniel and G. 0. Einstein, Bizarre imagery as an effective
memory aid: The importance of distinctiveness . Journal of Ex-
perimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 12 ( 1986): 54-
65 .
3. T. J. Keeney, S. R. Cannizzo, and J. H. Flavell, Spontaneous and in-
duced verbal rehearsal in a recall task. Child Development 38
( 1967):953-966.
4. G. A. Miller, The magical number seven plus or minus two: Some
limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological
Review 63 (1956) :81-97.
5. Research and theories dealing with attention are discussed by D.
Kahneman, Attention and Effort. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice
Hall, 1973; D. L. LaBerge, Attention. Psychological Sdence 1
(1990):156-162.
6. Studies illustrating how particular strategies may or may not be ef-
fective depending on the testing context include C. D. Morris, J.D.
Bransford, and J. J. Franks, Levels of processing versus transfer ap-
propriate processing. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior
16 ( 1977):519- 533 . H. L. Roediger, Implicit memory: Retention
without remembering. American Psychologist45 (1990):1043-1056;
liD RModel tor Requiring New Hnowledge
I Suggested Readings
Practicallq Oriented Readings
Bellezza, F. S. 1982. Improve Your Memory Skills. Englewood Cliffs, N.J. :
Prentice-Hall.
Bower, G. H. 1970. Analysis of a mnemonic device. American Scientist,
58:496-510.
Cermak, L. S. 1976. Improving Your Memory. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Improving Memor~ Shills IIIDI
Higbee, K. L. 1977. Your Memory: How It Works and How to Improve It.
Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall.
Lorayne, H., and J. Lucas. 1974. The Memory Book. New York: Ballantine.
Luria, A. R. 1968. The Mind of a Mnemonist. New York: Basic Books.
Sally let loose a team of gophers. The plan backfired when a dog chased
them away. She then threw a party but the guests failed to bring their
motorcycles. Furthermore, her stereo system was not loud enough. Sally
spent the next day looking for a "Peeping Tom" but was unable to find
one in the yellow pages. Obscene phone calls gave her some hope until
the number was changed. It was the installation of blinking neon lights
across the street that finally did the trick. Sally framed the ad from the
classified section and now has it hanging on her wall.
wasting time on such nonsense. Other readers may approach the prob-
lem differently. For instance, they may conjecture that the passage pre-
supposes some information not available to them that might be available
to someone more familiar with Sally. Consequently, they may define their
goal as trying to find the presupposed information. These readers may
then explore various strategies for solution. For example, they may think
of possible reasons for Sally's activities (maybe she's in love with some-
body), anticipate possible outcomes, and act on the strategy that seems
most likely to help them understand the passage . Then they can look at
the effects. If their initial hypothesis doesn't work, they can generate and
test alternative hypotheses . They may move through the IDEAL cycle a
number of times.
Now imagine that you are going to be tested on the Sally passage .
Many students might assume that their goal is simply to remember the
information in the passage, and they will explore strategies appropriate to
that goal. You could undoubtedly memorize all the sentences by using
one of the strategies discussed in the previous chapter (for example,
interactive imagery). But consider what would happen if you were asked
the following questions:
If you failed to understand the passage about Sally and simply memorized
it, you would find it very difficult to answer such questions.
The passage becomes much more comprehensible when you can
formulate an explanation for Sally's actions. Assume, therefore, that
Sally is trying to get a neighbor to move. Given this information, read the
passage again and try to answer the questions posed above.
The Sally passage illustrates two points. First, comprehension invol-
ves a problem- solving process. Second, understanding often requires
more than the simple memorizing of information (see Figure 22). The
ability to remember all the sentences in the Sally passage does not guar-
antee your ability to do other things with the information, such as make
intelligent inferences. When we learn with understanding, we are able to
apply information to a broader range of tasks than when information is
simply memorized.
11m AModel for Acquiring New Hnowledge
Learning with
Memorization
understanding
Explore Explore
memory strategies understanding
strategies
Figure 22
Prerequisites to Understanding
In the previous discussion we noted that many of the inferences required
for comprehension draw on relevant background knowledge. In some
situations this background knowledge may involve many complex and
interconnected concepts. Consider the following summary of a talk at a
scientific meeting.
Pete argued that data gathered from a NASA spaceship's voyage to Venus
calls into question current theories about the formation of our solar sys-
tem. Part of his talk emphasized the importance of mass spectrometers.
He then discussed the isotopes of argon 36 and argon 38 and noted that
they were of higher density than expected. He also cited the high values
of neon found in the atmosphere. He has a paper that is already written,
but he is aware of the need for further investigation as well.
Most people who are asked to read this passage could do little more
than memorize it. They simply do not have the background knowledge
_ . HModel for Requiring New Hnowledge
Now try to answer the following questions without looking back at the
preceding sentences.
Memor~ Strategies
There are several ways to approach the problem of mastering information
that seems arbitrary at first. One is simply to repeat the facts until they are
memorized (artery thick elastic; artery thick elastic). A more efficient
approach is to use memory techniques similar to those discussed in Chap-
ter 6.7 The use of imagery is one technique. That arteries are thick could
llfD AModel for ACQUiring New Hnowledge
Comprehension Strategies
We noted earlier that the ability to remember information is no guaran-
tee that you will be able to make inferences and use it to solve new
problems. Suppose you learn that arteries are elastic by imagining either
a rubber band holding a tube or thick-waisted Art and his elastic waist-
band. What if you were confronted with the problem of designing an
artificial artery? Would it have to be elastic? What would be the potential
implications of hardening of the arteries? Would this have a serious
impact on people's health? Having used the imaging techniques to re-
member that arteries are elastic, you would have little basis for answering
these questions.
Memory techniques are useful for many purposes, but a very differ-
ent approach is needed to develop an understanding of veins and arteries.
Effective learners attend to facts, but they also try to understand their
significance. For example, the passage stated that arteries are elastic. What
is the significance of elasticity? How does this property relate to the func-
tions that arteries perform? An effective learner might seek out informa-
tion to clarify this relationship. The passage also stated that arteries carry
blood from the heart, blood that is pumped in spurts. This provides one
clue about the significance of elasticity-arteries may need to expand and
contract to accommodate the pumping of blood. An effective learner
might ask why veins do not need to be elastic. Perhaps because veins carry
blood back to the heart, they have less need to accommodate the large
changes in pressure resulting from the heart's pumping of blood in spurts.
Some learners could carry this process a step further, asking why
blood does not flow back to the heart through the arteries. Since there
are arteries in the neck and shoulder regions, arterial blood must flow up
as well as down. The answer to this question might provide an additional
Learning With Understanding
The fat one bought the padlock to place on the refrigerator door.
The strong one cleaned the paintbrush used to paint the barbells.
The cheerful one read the newspaper announcing that he had won the
lottery.
The skinny one purchased the scissors to use when taking in her pants.
The funny one admired the ring that squirted water.
111iJ nModel tor Requiring New Hnowledge
Most people find it relatively easy to remember who did what given
elaborations that clarify the reason for each activity. These elaborations
must be relevant, however; they must help one understand why each
person performs each activity. Elaborations that make sense and yet are
irrelevant can actually impair memory rather than improve it. Examples
of irrelevant elaborations in this case are the following:
The fat one bought the padlock to place on the garage door.
The strong one cleaned the paintbrush used to paint the chair.
The cheerful one read the newspaper bought at the newsstand.
The skinny one purchased the scissors to use when trimming her nails.
The funny one admired the ring in the jewelry store.
Bill's father worked for a company that made robots. His company made
robots for a business that washed outside windows. They needed two
kinds of robots. One kind of robot was needed to wash windows in two-
story houses. These windows were small. The other kind of robot was
needed to wash the outside of windows of high-rise office buildings.
These windows were big.
Billy went to visit his father at work. He saw the new robots that his
father had made. The robot used for houses was called an extendible
robot. It could extend itself so it would be almost as tall as a two-story
house. Billy saw that this robot had spikes instead of feet. It had legs that
did not bend. Its stomach could extend in length to make it taller. The
arms on the robot were short. Instead of hands, it had a small sponges. In
its head was a nozzle attached to a hose. Billy also saw that the exten-
dible robot was made of heavy steel. It had an electric cord that could be
plugged in. The robot also had a ladder on its back.
Billy then saw another robot called a nonextendible robot. This robot
had suction cups instead of feet. It had legs that could bend. Its stomach
was padded. The arms on the robot were long. Instead of hands, it had
large sponges. In its head was a bucket. Billy also saw that the nonexten-
dible robot was made of light aluminum. There was a battery inside the
robot. The robot also had a parachute on its back.
Did you find yourself attempting to understand why each robot has
its particular features? For example, did you try to explain why the
window-climbing robot might have a battery and the extendible robot an
electrical cord? If so, you approached this passage in a manner similar to
that of the academically successful students who participated in our
llfB RModel for Requiring New Hnowledge
study. Here is one fifth grader's explanation for why each robot had
particular properties:
You would know that the robot that had to go up on tall buildings to
wash the windows would need to be lighter and not use an extension cord
because it would be too long and might make it fall. It had a parachute
in case it did fall. It also had large sponges because the windows were big.
You 'd also know that the robot used to wash two-story houses was more
heavy and had an extension cord 'cause there are plugs. It would rise up
with its stomach and could spray with the hose that came through its
head. You can't have a hose if you climb real high.
Michelene Chi and her colleagues 9 have also found that successful
students are more likely to explore explanations during the learning
process than are less academically successful students. They examined
the learning strategies that college students used as they encountered
problems in their physics texts. Chi and her colleagues found that suc-
cessful students engaged in a process of explanation; they tried to figure
out why each aspect of the solution was applicable, and they asked
themselves about other problems to which the solution might also be
applicable. As a result, they acquired an understanding that was more
general than a mere memorization of the specific steps involved in a
particular problem would afford.
I&
A B
10
c
Figure23
emnttl
SOME FUNCTIONS FOR THE SCISSORS ILLUSTRATED IN FIGURE 23
Structure Function
A. Dressmaker's shears
Heavy Because of heavy use
One hole larger than other Two or three fingers will fit in larger
hole-allows greater steadiness as
one cuts cloth on flat surface
Blades off-center and aligned Blade can rest on table
with finger-hole edge surface as cloth is cut-again,
greater steadiness
B. Barber's shears
Very sharp To cut thin material; for example,
hair
Pointed Permits blades to snip close to scalp
and to snip very small strands of hair
Hook on finger hole A rest for one finger, which allows
scissors to be supported when held
at various angles-hence, greater
maneuverability
D. Nail scissors
Wide and thick at pivot point To withstand pressure from cutting
thick and rigid materials; that is,
nails
Slightly curved blades To cut slightly curved nails
Learning With Understanding Ifill
Mfiinll
CONTINUED
Structure Function
E. Cuticle scissors
Very sharp blade To cut semielastic materials; for ex-
ample, skin of cuticles
Small curved blades To allow maneuverability necessary
to cut small curved area
Long extension from finger As compensation for short blades,
holes to joint necessary for holding
arbitrary to the writers because they can fill in the gaps. For example,
they already know why arteries are elastic and why various Native Amer-
icans chose certain types of houses. In short, they not only know the
facts, they also understand the significance of those facts. To the novice,
however, the knowledge necessary to fill in the gaps is not available. They
are therefore often forced to resort to memorizing information without
really understanding it.
As you become better able to spot arbitrariness in texts, you will also
be more likely to search other sources or ask questions that can help you
understand what you learn. Remember, though, that the processes nec-
essary to achieve understanding are frequently more complex than those
necessary for memorizing. It is helpful to think again about the ultimate
goal of learning: to develop conceptual tools that make it easier to solve
important problems. Mere memorization of information rarely enables
one to solve new problems later on.
A /\ ~
Wet
I
Trees
eous belief that the information is too important to forget. Another prob-
lem is that people write down cues that are too vague. For example, if
you wrote down the phrase "Native American houses" but included no-
thing to remind you of its significance, you might have a difficult time
remembering that a passage on this topic was used to illustrate the prob-
lem of arbitrary facts.
An obvious way to overcome these problems is to record all the
information that you will use. Often, however, the exact wording is not
important or there is not enough time to write down the information in
full. In these situations it is necessary to use an abbreviated form of the
information. When making such notes, most people focus on the main
points and incorporate them into a list or outline. Ruth Day has con-
ducted extensive research on note taking and has found that such notes
may provide ineffective retrieval cues for reconstructing underlying rela-
tionships. For example, try using the following outline to reconstruct the
joke to which it refers. 13 (See Appendix A for the joke.)
I. Theater joke
A Good news
I. Balcony
2. Flames reach in several minutes
B. Bad news
I. Theater fire
2. Don't tell
One of the problems with this outline is that it does not remind us
of the order of the parts of the joke or what the punch line is. The method
~earning With Understanding Ifill
A /\ ~
Wet
Slant
I
Trees
Wood
I "'::\;l" . -·.:. ,:.
Use dirt or brush'"
Few trees
~ded
Buffalo Mobility
Tepees
roofs
1::::::
planks for house
0
·;·
·-::
·:_.
,t\"
Agure 2S
Slant roofs
~
Brush or adobe houses
of facts.
.m RHodel for Requiring New Hnowledge
been defined as gaining understanding rather than just exercising mem-
ory. We emphasized that learning with understanding requires a set of
strategies different from and often more complex than those required by
mere memorization. As we explore various comprehension strategies it is
important to anticipate their effects.
Many researchers have emphasized how important it is to monitor
the effectiveness of learning strategies. Ann Brown 14 and John Flavell 15
were pioneers in the study of what has come to be called metacognition-
the ability to monitor and regulate one's own learning.
Monitoring the outcome of our attempts to learn depends, to a great
extent, on our ability to anticipate situations in which we will need to use
the new information. Whether a mountain-climbing trip, a speech, or a
test in school is involved, it is important to imagine how you will ul-
timately use the knowledge to decide if you have learned enough to
handle the challenges that you will confront. The clearer your idea of
what you will need to know, the better your ability to assess whether you
are prepared. Some examples are provided below.
Levels of Understanding
Anticipating the effects of learning strategies by imagining future uses for
the knowledge gained is especially important because there are many
levels at which concepts and phenomena can be understood . Knowledge
can be mastered at different levels of precision, and the precision neces-
sary depends on the uses to which the knowledge will be put.
As an illustration of different levels of precision, assume that a
group of people read a passage about veins and arteries, such as the one
described earlier. Some may feel that they have learned enough when
they understand that veins and arteries are parts of the body as opposed
to, say, parts of a car engine. Others may not feel they have learned
enough until they understand that veins and arteries carry blood rather
than serve some other bodily function. Still others may study until they
understand how veins and arteries are similar and different in structure
and function. For example, they may feel it is important to know that
arteries are more elastic than veins and to understand why.
Differences in the precision of one's knowledge may or may not be
important for subsequent performance. Assume, for example, that you
are asked the following multiple-choice question.
Learning With Understanding -
Arteries are
a. Good to eat
b. A type of insect
c. An important part of the body
d. Sold only at gas stations
e. A kind of tree
One needs to know very little about arteries to answer such a question.
In contrast, consider the following:
Arteries
a. Are more elastic than veins
b. Carry blood that is pumped from the heart
c. Are less elastic than veins
d. Both a and b
e. Both b and c
child hears the statement, "Ruth decided not to wear her matching silver
earrings, necklace, and belt because she wanted to avoid delays at the
airport." Most adults would assume that Ruth wants to avoid problems
with metal detectors but since the child doesn't have this knowledge, he
or she would have a difficult time understanding Ruth's decision.
Tests ot Understanding
A powerful approach to gauging the adequacy of your current level of
understanding is to devise tests that model or simulate the kinds of
problems that you expect to confront in the future (designing an artificial
artery or explaining the principles of astronomy). Such tests provide the
opportunity to actively apply one's knowledge and, ideally, reveal any
confusions, uncertainties, or gaps in one's understanding. They serve a
function similar to the simulations and tests used by NASA to uncover
possible problems that might be encountered by astronauts on space
missions.
The importance of discovering gaps in our understanding can be
illustrated by reading the paragraph below.
Despite the fact that most people believe they understand this game,
their answers to these questions suggest otherwise. The correct answers
are no, 0, and forever, respectively. Most people fail to detect inconsisten-
Learning W
ith Understanding Bill
cies in the rules of the game that would make it impossible to win or end.
This is caused in part by the failure to actively apply the rules to a real
game situation. If you still have not found the inconsistencies in the rules,
try working through an actual game mentally, or try using real cards
(answers appear in Appendix A).
This example illustrates that monitoring the effectiveness of a learn-
ing strategy can involve the active application of ideas to specific situa-
tions. If we know we are able to use a set of rules to play a game, solve a
mathematical problem, or produce a concrete model, then we have ac-
quired a better understanding of our own understanding. In the game
description provided above, instructions that make the game impossible
to win or end were deliberately inserted. In other learning tasks such
inconsistencies are a clear signal either that our level of understanding
is not adequate or that there is something deficient in the material we
are trying to understand. In either case, the detection of inconsistencies
serves as a signal to identify a new problem in our understanding and to
reenter the IDEAL cycle.
In his autobiography, Charles Darwin describes a conversation with
Sedgwick, a geology professor, that dramatically illustrates the impor-
tance of detecting inconsistencies. Darwin was in his early 20s at the
time. One evening Darwin told Sedgwick about a laborer he had met
who had discovered a large tropical shell in an old gravel pit in the
midland counties. Sedgwick immediately responded, "It must have been
thrown away by someone into the pit," adding, "If really embedded there
it would be the greatest misfortune to geology, as it would overthrow all
that we know about the superficial deposits of the Midland Counties."
Sedgwick's remark had a profound effect on Darwin because it sensitized
him to the relationship between theory and data. Because of his theoreti-
cal knowledge, Sedgwick was very sensitive to potential inconsistencies
between theory and data and hence was able to identify potential prob-
lems of interpretation that Darwin had not noticed at all. 18
need. People often fail to solve problems not because they lack the rel-
evant knowledge but because they are unable to gain access to it.
Here is a problem we have given to a number of college students
enrolled in our courses on learning and memory.
Many students come up with weak answers to this problem. Some say,
"He studied only fifth graders; maybe it's not true for kids in other
grades." Others state, "You can't prove much by only a single study."
Although these statements have some validity, they miss the crucial flaw
in Professor X's reasoning. He claims to have studied forgetting, yet he
hasn't shown that the less successful students learned as much initially.
Perhaps a lack of attention or of previously acquired knowledge pre-
vented the less successful students from learning as much of the story in
the first place. Without some knowledge of how much was learned (ob-
tained, for example, by giving the children a test right after they hear the
story), one cannot make claims about the rate at which forgetting oc-
curred.
As we have noted, many college students fail to find the crucial flaw
in Professor X's argument. The interesting point is that this failure is not
necessarily caused by a lack of knowledge. We gave students this problem
after they had studied forgetting; lectures had emphasized that forgetting
presupposes that something has been learned in the first place (you
cannot forget something you haven't learned) . The students' failure
therefore stemmed from a failure to access relevant information.
Additional evidence for this is provided by the second part of our
experiment. Approximately 5 minutes after the students had first at-
tempted to solve the problem, we gave it to them a second time. This time
we said, "Remember our earlier discussion of the relationship between
forgetting and previous learning." Given this hint, the majority of the
~earning With Understanding liB
answer versus those you could not. Did the ones you missed require a
more precise level of understanding than you had acquired? You should
also try to discover where the information necessary to answer the ques-
tions you missed is located. Was it in the text? In your notes? If it was,
you must have overlooked it, perhaps because you failed to realize that it
could be important for solving certain problems. If the relevant informa-
tion was neither in your notes nor in the book, chances are that you need
to work on effective note-taking skills.
Professors and other experts can provide feedback helpful in debug-
ging your learning strategies. To use these sources effectively, however,
you must first attempt to communicate what you have mastered. It is
only then that the expert can evaluate your understanding of the subject
and make suggestions that can be used to modify your learning strategies.
Imagine you meet someone who is an expert in a topic with which
you are having difficulty. You could begin by explaining that you have
been having trouble determining whether you have learned enough.
Explain further that you have studied several topics that seem important
and that you would like help in determining whether your knowledge is
adequate. You can then ask the expert to question you about problems
that beginners should be able to solve. This will allow you to discover
quite quickly whether you are missing information that is crucial. If you
do this for a number of topics, you will also develop an understanding of
the expert's criteria for adequate understanding.
This approach to debugging takes commitment and preparation,
and you must be brave enough to risk making mistakes in front of others.
Nevertheless, it is an efficient and valuable way to learn. Furthermore,
we are confident that most instructors and experts will respect your
motivation and maturity. They know that it is harder to understand than
to memorize, and they respect genuine attempts to understand.
standing that was inappropriate for the task we had to perform. These
observations will help us recognize the need to pay more attention to our
goals and to how well we anticipate future uses of our knowledge.
Reentering the IDEAL cycle and attempting to debug our ap-
proaches to learning creates opportunities for what many researchers
refer to as learning to learn. Lauren Resnick2 1 emphasizes that everyone
in society must learn to learn and solve a range of problems-not just a
select few. One reason we need lifelong learning skills is that job require-
ments tend to change rapidly. For instance, consider the field of auto
mechanics. It used to be that the knowledge and skills necessary for
success in this area were relatively stable. But today's auto mechanics
need to keep up with rapidly changing technologies, such as advances in
computerized electronic controls. Indeed, the skills required for most
professions will change rapidly as new technologies are integrated into
the workplace. Those who are able to learn new concepts and procedures
will have a better chance of maintaining their jobs or advancing their
careers.
I Summar~
In this chapter we applied the IDEAL framework to the problem of learn-
ing with understanding. The goal of learning with understanding re-
quires different strategies than simple memorization of information. It is
therefore important to carefully define your goals (learning with under-
standing versus simply memorizing) once a learning problem or oppor-
tunity has been identified.
Novices who learn about a new area of knowledge frequently con-
front the problem of how to master facts and relationships that seem
arbitrary. For example, there appears to be no reason that an artery
should be elastic or nonelastic, thick or thin. One approach to the prob-
lem of learning arbitrary relationships is to use such memory techniques
as interactive images. An alternative strategy is to explore why things are
the way they are-to search for relationships between the structure and
function of veins and arteries, between the features of robots and their
intended function, between the shape and materials used in Native
American houses and the environment, and between the shape of scis-
sors and their function .
Strategies for learning with understanding are often more complex
and difficult than those that help us to merely memorize information.
Nev~rtheless, the extra effort is usually worthwhile because concepts that
liB RHodel for Requiring New Hnowledge
I Exercises
Use the IDEAL Problem Navigation Guide in Appendix C to work
through a nonroutine learning problem that is important to you. Be sure
to clarify whether your goal involves learning with understanding or just
memorization.
Learning With Understanding llBI
Try to make the sentences below comprehensible.
1. The breakfast was delicious because the thread was sticky.
2. The stream of water stopped because it started raining.
3. The car moved because the coin was bent.
4. The clothes were ruined because the sign vanished.
5. The street was full of potholes because the turning stopped.
6. The home was small because the sun came out.
7. The notes were sour because the seam split.
Listed below are some words that would be relatively easy to memorize.
However, it is more interesting to try to understand them. For example,
the combination "you just me" can be interpreted as "just between you
and me. "
wear
B. thermal
9. sttheory
10. T
0
w
N
11. /r/e/a/d/i/n/g/
12. wheather
13. He's/Himself
knee
14.
lights
15 . Read the following instructions for operating a pencil shar-
pener: "After selecting the proper size of guide hole, turn the
handle clockwise." Evaluate the adequacy of these instruc-
tions for people who have never seen or used a pencil before.
Rewrite the instructions to resolve the deficiencies.
16. Imagine that a child reads the following passage about
camels. "They have special eyelids that can cover their eyes
yet still let in some light. They can close their nose passages.
They have thick hair around their ear openings." What might
you do to help the child understand the significance or
relevance of these facts rather than merely memorize them?
17. How might children's ability to understand the significance of
these facts allow them to better understand other events they
read about?
IDD RHodel for Requiring New Hnowledge
I8. A spy wants to hide a roll of film he has reduced to 1/s inch in
diameter and 21/4 inches long. Looking at his bookshelf, he
notices the two-volume desk-top encyclopedia illustrated
below. Using a drill that is 1/4 inch in diameter, the spy begins
on page I of volume I and drills straight through to the last
page of volume 2. Assume that the cover of each book is 1/4
inch thick and that each book without its cover is I inch
thick. Is the hole long enough to hold the roll of film? How
long is the hole?
""z
~
i
Vol
-
c
>
Vol
I II
I Notes
I. A. N. Whitehead, The Aims of Education & Other Essays. New York:
Macmillan, I929.
Learning W
ith Understanding IBJII
2. See note 1.
3. This passage was adapted from one that was written originally by
Nancy McCarrell.
4. This passage is from J. R. Mehan, Tale-spin, an interactive program
that writes stories. Proceedings from the Fifth International Joint Con-
ference on Artificial Intelligence, 1977, pp. 91-98.
5. Additional discussion of the importance of previously acquired
knowledge for learning can be found in J.D. Bransford and M. K.
Johnson, Contextual prerequisites for understanding: Some inves-
tigations of comprehension and recall. Journal of Verbal Learning
and Verbal Behavior ll (1972):717-726; J.D . Bransford, R. S. Sher-
wood, N. J. Vye, and J. Rieser. Teaching thinking and problem solv-
ing: Research foundations. American Psychologist 41
(1986):1078-1089; K. Nelson, R. Fivush, J. Hudson, and J.
Lucariello, Scripts and the development of memory. In M. T. H.
Chi (ed.), Contributions to Human Development, Vol. 9, Trends in
Memory Development Research. New York: Kargar, 1983; R. C.
Schank and R. P. Abelson, Scripts, Plans, Goals and Understanding.
Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1977; R. Glaser,
Education and thinking: The role of knowledge. American Psychol-
gist 39 (1984):93-104.
6. Based on material developed by B. S. Stein and J . D. Bransford,
Constraints on effective elaboration: Effects of precision and self-
generation. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 18
(1979):769-777. See also B.S. Stein, K. F. Brock, D. R. Ballard,
and N.J. Vye, Constraints on verbal and pictorial elaboration.
Memory &Cognition 15, no. 4 (1987): 281-290.
7. See C. E. Weinstein, Elaboration skills as a learning strategy. In H.
F. O'Neil, Jr. (ed.), Learning Strategies. New York: Academic Press,
1978.
8. See J . J. Franks, N. J. Vye, P. M. Auble, K. J . Mezynski, G. A. Per-
fetto, J.D. Bransford, B. S. Stein, and J. Littlefield, Learning from
explicit versus implicit texts. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
General111 (1982):414-422 .
9. M. T. H. Chi, M. Bassok, M. W. Lewis, P. Reimann, and R. Glaser,
Self-explanations: How students study and use examples in learn-
ing to solve problems. Cognitive Science 13 ( 19 8 9): 14 5-182.
10. From J. D. Bransford and N. S. McCarrell, A sketch of a cognitive
approach to comprehension. In W. Weimer and D. Palermo (eds.),
11m RModel for Requiring New Knowledge
I Suggested Readings
Theoretical!~ Oriented Readings
Anderson, R. C. 1984. The role of reader's schema in comprehension,
learning and memory. In R. Anderson, J. Osborn, and R. Tierney
(eds.), Learning to Read in American Schools: Basal Readers and Content
Texts. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Asher, J. 1981. Fear of foreign languages. Psychology Today (August).
Bransford, J. D., B. S. Stein, N. J. Vye, J. J. Franks, P. M. Auble, K. J.
Mezynski, and G. A. Perfetto. 1982. Differences in approaches to
learning: An overview, Journal of Experimental Psychology: General
11:390-398.
Bransford, J. D., N. J. Vye, L. T. Adams, and G. A. Perfetto . In press.
Learning skills and the acquisition of knowledge. In R. Glaser and A.
Lesgold (eds.), Handbook of Psychology and Education. Hillsdale, N.J.:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Brown, A. L. 1992. Design experiments: Theoretical and methodological
challenges in creating complex interventions in classroom settings.
Journal ofthe Learning Sciences 2, no. 2:141-178.
Brown, A. L., J. D. Bransford, R. A. Ferrara, and J. C. Campione. 1983 .
Learning, remembering and understanding. In J. H. Flavell and E.
M. Markman (eds.), Carmichael's Manual of Child Psychology (Vol. 1).
New York: Wiley.
Brown, A. L., and J. S. DeLoache. 1978. Skills, plans and self-regulation.
In R. S. Siegler (ed.), Children's Thinking: What Develops? Hillsdale,
N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Chase, W. G., and H. A. Simon. 1973. Perception in chess. Cognitive Psy-
chology 4:55-81.
Glenberg, A.M., A. C. Wilkinson, and W. Epstein. 1982 . The illusion of
knowing: Failure in the self-assessment of comprehension. Memory
and Cognition 10: 597- 602.
Harris, R. J. 1977. Comprehension of pragmatic implications in advertis-
ing. Journal of Applied Psychology 62:603-608.
Just, M., and P. A. Carpenter. 1986. The Psychology of Reading and Language
Comprehension. New York: Allyn and Bacon.
Markman, E. M. 1977. Realizing that you don't understand: A prelimi-
nary investigation. Child Development 48:986-992.
Ng, E., and Bereither, C. 1991. Three levels of goal orientation in learn-
ing. Journal ofthe Learning Sciences 1, no. 3 and 4:243-271.
11m AModel for Acquiring New Hnowledge
Scardamalia, M., and C. Bereiter. 1991. Higher levels of agency for chil-
dren in knowledge building: A challenge for the design of new
knowledge media. Journal ofthe Learning Sciences 1, no. 1:37-68.
Spilich, G. J., G. T. Vesonder, H. L. Chiesi, and J . F. Voss. 1979. Text
processing of domain-related information for individuals with high
and low domain knowledge. Journal of Verbal Learning and Behavior
18:275-290.
INSTRUCTION
THAT FACILITATES
PROBLEM SOLVING
The two preceding chapters explored ways of using the IDEAL frame-
work to improve our ability to master new information. We noted that
successful learners try to identify potential learning problems and to
define their goals so that they can select appropriate learning strategies. If
our goal is to acquire conceptual tools that can help us solve problems in
the future, it is also important to select strategies that facilitate under-
standing rather than just memorization. Much of our previous discussion
focused on what the individual learner can do to improve learning. In-
deed, it is important to look at learning from the perspective of the
individual because much of what we learn is acquired through personal
experience. But a great deal of learning also occurs in more formal educa-
tional contexts--contexts that can encourage people to pursue effective
approaches to learning or discourage them from doing so. In this chapter
we consider some reasons that many traditional approaches to instruc-
tion do not facilitate problem solving, and we discuss alternatives that
hold more promise for helping students learn to think and learn on their
own.
IIIli RModel for Requiring New Knowledge
There are 26 sheep and I 0 goats on a ship. How old is the captain l
people will know when, why, and how to apply their knowledge. He
notes that "students may possess calculus skills without recognizing that
they are applicable to a particular physics problem or without knowing
exactly how to apply them." 4
for example, you might first present the relevant facts and formulas (for
example, that density equals mass divided by volume) and then present
applications problems for solution (for example, "An object weighs 4
grams and has a volume of 8 cubic centimeters. What is its density? ").
It seems clear that the effectiveness of the transmission model of
instruction can be improved by using applications problems. However,
traditional applications problems have severe shortcomings. Researchers
in the Cognition and Technology Group at Vanderbilt University have
discussed several of these shortcomings.6 One is that they often fail to
help students think about realistic situations. Instead of bringing real-
world standards to their work, students tend to treat word problems
mechanically, failing to consider constraints imposed by real-world fac-
tors. Ed Silver7 provides an excellent example of a relatively mechanical
approach to word problems. Students were asked to determine the num-
ber of buses needed to take a specific number of people on a field trip.
Many of them divided the total number of students by the number that
each bus would hold and came up with answers like 2V3. The students
failed to consider that one cannot use a third of a bus.
A second limitation of traditional applications problems is the habits
of mind that they encourage. Applications problems can generally be
solved by thinking back to information studied in the previous chapter or
chapters. The goal is therefore to retrieve information rather than to
develop a more intuitive and creative approach to problem solving. This
can limit the development of people's ability to think for themselves.
Furthermore, many applications problems provide only one correct an-
swer to a problem. This can lead to misconceptions about the nature of
problem solving and can inhibit creative thought.
A third limitation of traditional applications problems is that most of
them explicitly define the problem to be solved rather than help students
to identify the problem themselves and work through the process of
clarifying their goals. Outside of schooL people have to identify problems
that others have ignored and deal with problems that are not clearly
defined. For example, consider the problem of preventing people from
getting injured in automobile crashes that was discussed earlier. Effective
problem solvers will define their goals in a variety of ways (for example,
to reduce injuries caused by people striking against hard objects inside
the car, to create a system that absorbs the force of impact, or to prevent
crashes from happening). Realistic problems can usually be approached
from a number of different perspectives and usually have a number of
potential solutions. It is therefore important to develop the skills neces-
lED RHodel tor Requiring New Hnowledge
sary to define one's own goals and assumptions rather than always to
have them defined for one in applications problems.
These arguments are not meant to imply that applications problems
are useless. Indeed, we use them throughout this book to provide extra
practice in when, why, and how to apply the concepts discussed. Our
argument is that the general procedure of facts and principles first, fol-
lowed by applications is not sufficient to develop powerful mental habits
that support thinking and problem solving. Alternatives to this approach
are discussed next.
Project-Based Instruction
Another way to organize instruction around problem solving is to create
student projects. An excellent example is the Discover Rochester project
discussed by Alan Collins, Jan Hawkins, and Sharon Carver. 12 In this
project, eighth-grade students who were at risk of dropping out of school
spent one day each week exploring aspects of their hometown-Roch-
ester, New York-from a scientific, mathematical, historical, cultural, and
literary perspective. Working in groups, the students conducted their
own research on topics such as industry, weather, theater, and employ-
ment. On the basis of their research, they developed multimedia exhibits
for the Rochester Museum and Science Center. The exhibits included
text, audio, graphics, maps, and music. Students also studied the reac-
tions of people to their projects and refined them based on the feedback
they received.
Collins, Hawkins, and Carver used a variation of the IDEAL frame-
work adapted to their project. They helped students identify problems and
opportunities by encouraging them to pose interesting questions about
their community. As students researched the answers to these questions,
they were helped to further define their goals. They were then helped to
explore a number of strategies for gathering and representing data, includ-
ing strategies for using indexes, conducting interviews, and organizing
and analyzing data. Students then attempted to anticipate the effects of
their presentations (for example, by conducting tests with one another),
and they eventually acted on their ideas by placing their multimedia
products in the museum. Students were then encouraged to look at the
effects of their products on the audience and to learn from the experi-
ence. Some students learned that their products were less interesting
than they had thought; they therefore reentered the IDEAL cycle in order
to improve the product the next time.
Many teachers in other fields have effectively used projects as an-
chors for instruction. For example, Lisa Glick and Michael Ross used the
llmJI RHodel for Requiring New Hnowledge
the potential value of new information and to place that new information
under close scrutiny.
Debate topics can be formulated in almost any field. It is important
to select a topic that students have some familiarity with but that can
serve as an anchor for the sustained exploration of new concepts and
ideas. For example, topics involving environmental issues such as recy-
cling and incineration can provide a meaningful anchor for learning
about chemical reactions, biological processes, and the ecosystem. The
instructor can influence the topics explored in these debates by providing
relevant readings or sources of information and by establishing criteria
for evaluating the adequacy of arguments (for example, the presentation
must include an adequate chemical analysis of the products of incinera-
tion).
Simulations
Simulations provide another way to situate learning within the context
of meaningful problems. Advances in microcomputer technology have
Instruction That Facilitates Problem Solving 1tifJ1
apply a model like the IDEAL framework can greatly facilitate the effec-
tiveness of problem-based learning. An especially important considera-
tion is the need to design opportunities for assessment. We discuss this
issue later in the chapter.
Problem Selection
Anchoring instruction in realistic problem-solving tasks works best when
these tasks are tailored to the needs, interests, and skills of students.
Ideally, instructors interested in problem-based learning should find or
develop their own anchors or find ways to help students design problems
for themselves.
The process of developing effective problems can be facilitated by
using the IDEAL framework. For example, one of our colleagues in the
social sciences, Ada Haynes, identified a problem in her students' under-
standing of the concept of prejudice. Although many of them could recite
a definition of the word, few could recognize many instances of prejudice
in their environment. Rather than give a lecture on the topic, Haynes
treated the problem as an opportunity to provide a learning experience
that would promote greater understanding and recognition of prejudice.
She defined her goal as teaching students to recognize prejudice in others
through the irrational behavior that it promotes. She also wanted stu-
dents to learn to recognize prejudice in themselves. To accomplish these
goals, she explored realistic situations in which decisions could be based
either on reason or on prejudice.
One task required students to decide who should be let into a special
shelter during a nuclear war. Students were told that the 6 people chosen
might be the only ones left on the planet and that they would have to
choose these 6 out of a list of 12. They were given information about the
12 people that might be relevant to the goal of rebuilding society (for
example, fertility, medical training, and agricultural training) . In addi-
tion, some of the individuals were assigned characteristics that typically
invoke prejudice (being Jewish, African-American, socialist, or Iraqi) .
She tried to antidpate the effectiveness of her strategy by finding out
beforehand which cultural or ethnic groups elicited the strongest signs of
prejudice.
Haynes acted on her instructional strategy by having students make
and explain their decisions. She then was able to look back at the effec-
tiveness of her strategy and to learn how to make improvements. These
included modifying the descriptions to provide greater opportunities for
IIIII RModel for Requiring New Hnowledge
I Summar~
In this chapter we discussed the importance of preparing people to think
for themselves and solve problems so that they can adapt to a world that
is changing at an increasingly rapid pace. There are several reasons that
our educational system may not be preparing people for these goals. For
IIIII RHodel tor Requiring New Hnowledge
ideas and strategies that could help them achieve those goals. Effective
instruction must also encourage students to anticipate the outcome of
using particular strategies and then to act on the most promising ones.
Students can then look back at the effects of their efforts and learn how
to improve their approach to problem solving.
I Notes
1. A. H. Schoenfeld, Teaching mathematical thinking and problem
solving. In L. B. Resnick and L. E. Klopfer (eds.), Toward the Think-
ing Curriculum: Current Cognitive Research. Alexandria, Va.:
American Society for Curriculum Development, 1989, pp. 83-103;
K. Reusser, Problem solving beyond the logic of things: Contextual
effects on understanding and solving word problems. Instructional
Science 17 (1988):309-338.
2. S. Berryman, Learning for the workplace: The state of play. Review
of Research in Education (in press).
3. C. I. Gragg, Because wisdom can't be told. Harvard Alumni Bulletin
(October 19, 1940):78-84.
4. H. A. Simon, Problem solving and education. In D. T. Tuma and
R. Reif (eds.), Problem Solving and Education: Issues in Teaching and
Research. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1980, p. 82 .
5. Ann Michael, personal communication with authors, Nashville,
Tenn., 1991.
6. Cognition and Technology Group at Vanderbilt. The Jasper series:
A generative approach to improving mathematical thinking. In
This Year in School Science. Washington, D.C.: American Association
for the Advancement of Science, in press.
7. E. A. Silver, Using conceptual and procedural knowledge: A focus
on relationships. In J. Hiebert (ed.), Conceptual and Procedural
Knowledge: The Case of Mathematics . Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates, 1986, pp . 181-189.
8. J. Dewey, How We Think. Boston: D.C . Heath, 1910; N. R. Hanson,
Patterns of Discovery: An Inquiry Into the Conceptual Foundations of
Science. London: Cambridge University Press, 1961.
9. J. S. Brown, A. Collins, and P. Duguid, Situated cognition and the
culture of learning. Educational Researcher 18, no. 1 (1989) :32-41.
10. S. M . Corey, Poor scholar's soliloquy. Childhood Education 33
(1944) :219-220 .
IIJIII HModel for Acquiring New Hnowledge
I Suggested Reading s
Theoretical!~ Oriented Readings
Baron, J. 1987. Evaluating thinking skills in the classroom. In J. Baron
and R. J. Sternberg (eds.), Teaching Thinking Skills: Theory and Prac-
tice. New York: W. H. Freeman, pp. 221-248.
Barrow, H. S. 1985. How to Design a Problem-Based Curriculum for the Pre-
clinical Years . New York: Springer Publishing Co.
Bransford, J. D., S. R. Goldman, and N.J. Vye. 1991. Making a difference
in people's abilities to think: Reflections on a decade of work and
some hopes for the future . In L. Okagaki and R. J. Sternberg (eds.),
Directors of Development: Influences on Children. Hillsdale, N.J.: Law-
rence Erlbaum Associates, pp. 147-180.
Bransford, J.D., R. S. Sherwood, N.J. Vye, and J. Rieser. 1986. Teaching
thinking and problem solving: Research foundations . American Psy-
chologist 41 :1078-1089.
Brown, A. L., and A.M. Palincsar. 1989. Guided cooperative learning and
individual knowledge acquisition. In L. B. Resnick (ed.), Knowing,
Learning, and Instruction: Essays in Honor of Robert Glaser. Hillsdale,
N.J. : Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. )
Charles R., and E. A. Silver (eds.). 1988. The Teaching and Assessing of
Mathematical Problem Solving. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum As-
sociates National Council for Teachers of Mathematics.
Nickerson, R. S., D. N. Perkins, and E. E. Smith. 1985. The Teaching of
Thinking . Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Resnick, L. B., and D. P. Resnick. 1991. Assessing the thinking curric-
ulum: New tools for educational reform. In B. Gifford and C. O'Con-
nor (eds.), New Approaches to Testing: Rethinking Aptitude, Achievement
and Assessment. New York: National Committee on Testing and Pub-
lic Policy.
1111 HModel for Requiring New Hnowledge
I Rttitudes
We noted in the first chapter that one of the biggest stumbling blocks to
improving problem solving can be negative attitudes about one's own
abilities. It is easy to tell people to think positively about their ability to
Concluding Remarks IDII
solve problems, but such a suggestion often has little effect on attitudes
acquired over many years of experience. A lack of confidence in our
ability to solve problems can manifest itself in a variety of ways, including
lack of interest, fear of exploring new domains, and fear of criticism.
These feelings can interfere with problem solving and can prevent us
from engaging in activities that might improve our problem-solving skills.
The IDEAL approach can be especially helpful in these situations because
it provides a guide for debugging unsuccessful approaches. By identifying
the attitudes that inhibit success and defining appropriate goals, we can
begin to explore strategies that may stop us from repeating earlier fail-
ures. By actively using appropriate strategies, we can give ourselves the
opportunity to experience success and so build our self-confidence.
The tendency to avoid new problems becomes especially strong
when others are performing well and we are experiencing considerable
difficulty. In such situations, we often explain our difficulty by assuming
that we are inept or slow and others are talented. An alternative perspec-
tive is that everyone experiences difficulty when dealing with nonroutine
problems or first learning about a new subject. We should not judge our
ability to solve a nonroutine problem by comparing ourselves with those
whose experience makes that same problem routine, or we may mis-
takenly underestimate our own abilities. As you continue to explore
similar problems in the same area of inquiry, you will find that they
become easier and easier to solve as they become more routine for you.
In Chapter 6 we asked you to remember what it was like when you
were first learning to drive a car. If you are like most people, you felt
extremely awkward. You probably had to think consciously about apply-
ing the brake, turning the wheel, using the turn signal, and so forth, and
it was difficult to do something like carry on a conversation while driving.
With practice, however, many aspects of driving became automatic, and
simultaneously driving and carrying on a conversation became routine.
It is useful to keep the driving example in mind when you are trying
to learn new tasks because the experience will almost undoubtedly be
similar. At first, everything will seem overwhelming, and people who can
already perform these tasks may seem almost superhuman. Later, you
will perform them with little difficulty; they will become relatively rou-
tine and require much less conscious attention. Nevertheless, to reach
this stage you must be prepared to go through a period of awkwardness.
You must have the courage to risk making mistakes.
The problem of learning a second language provides an excellent
illustration of the importance of risk-taking. 1 Researchers have found
IIJDI The Ideal Problem Solver
I Notes
l. I. Rubin, What the "good language learner" can teach us. Teachers
of English to Speakers of Other Languages Quarterly 9 ( 1975 ):41-51.
2. J.D. Bransford and K. Heldmeyer, Learning from children learn-
ing. In G. L. Bisanz and R. Kail (eds.), Learning in Children. New
York: Springer-Verlag, 1983.
ANSWERS TO PROBLEMS
IN TEXT
Chapter 1
Answer to letter puzzles:
Scrambled eggs Backward glance Banana split
Chapter 2
Answer to the boxes problem:
There are 33 boxes (3 large, 6 medium, and 24 small).
a taxi ride. If you take the 6 P.M . flight, you will not need to buy dinner,
since it is supplied on the plane.
Thick Lucile pieces fit into solid oak base with 5 notches, move to
accommodate any size book
weighed. Since you know that this new set of four does not contain the
oddball, you can use it as a standard for weight.
For example, assume that you weigh this standard against four balls
and the scales balance. You now know that the oddball is one of the four
you just removed from the scale. If you also note whether these four balls
are heavier or lighter in the second weighing, you will know whether the
oddball is heavier or lighter than the other balls.
Assume that you have now reduced the options to four balls. For
the third weighing, you can weigh two of these balls against two of the
balls of standard weight. If the scales balance you know that the oddball
is one of only two remaining balls. You can then use the fourth weighing
to balance one of these two against one of the standards. If the scales
balance you know that the one remaining ball is the oddball. If they do
not balance, you know that the oddball is the one not on the scale (and
not the standard you have been using). This problem can also be solved
using just three weighings.
Chapter 3
Answer to the Einstein problem:
The clock would appear to be going backwards.
Chapter 4
Answer to the typing and grades problem:
The relationship between typed papers and higher grades is correlational.
Although it is possible that students get higher grades because they type
their papers, it is equally possible that students who are more serious
about their education are more likely to learn to type, that students who
are more motivated are more likely to take the time to type their papers,
and so on.
want to be sure that the device was effective for other makes of cars,
especially your own.
Answers to the "If it is a triangle, then it is red" problem (If A, then B):
The first problem (if the shape is not a triangle, is it not red?) can be
expressed as "If A, then B; not A, therefore not B." To reason in this
manner would be to commit the fallacy of denying the antecedent. The
argument is not valid.
The second problem (if the shape is not red, is it not a triangle?) can
be expressed as "If A, then B; not B, therefore not A." This is a valid
argument.
Chapter s
Answer to credits and debits problem:
One strategy is to remember that the word credits has an r in it, so credits
go on the right.
Answers to Problems in Text IJlil
Chapter 7
Answer to the theater joke:
The joke, which was told by Dick Cavett, goes as follows : "I have some
good news and some bad news for people in the balcony. I am not going
to tell you the bad news, but the good news is that the flames won't reach
you for several minutes."
Chapter 2
1. Most people produce the answer 2, which is wrong. The cor-
rect answer is 0. If you answered 2, you probably failed to
identify a problem with the problem; namely, that it asks
about Adam rather than Noah.
2. This is another example of how people fail to identify of a
problem with their own interpretation. Did you notice the
two the's in the first and third phrases and the two a's in the
second phrase?
3. The inventors of the talking scale seem to have identified
some real problems that people face. For example, an adver-
tisement for a talking scale we saw asks
Can't see over your tummy?
Can't read the numbers way down there?
Can't remember whether you lost weight?
However, there may be some situations in which you would
not want to use this scale. For example, many people would
not want their weight revealed in places where others might
hear it.
4. The inventors of the sound -activated light switch also iden-
tified some problems that this device can help people solve. In
particular, it can
Hear you corning and switch on, so you won't come
home to a dark house or garage.
Surprise burglars who enter your house.
1111 The Ideal Problem Solver
H2 X 0 0 0 X X X X X X
W2 X 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0
H3 X 0 X 0 0 0 X X X X
W3 X 0 X 0 0 0 0 0 0 0
H4 X 0 X 0 X 0 0 0 X X
W4 X 0 X 0 X 0 0 0 0 0
HS X 0 X 0 X 0 X 0 0 0
W5 X 0 X 0 X 0 X 0 0 0
14. Most people have a difficult time solving this problem. The
usual response is that the paper will extend 1 or 2 feet after
Rnswers to Exercises a.
50 folds. However, a systematic analysis of each step plus
some mathematical tools shows that these answers are way
off the mark.
Consider the following calculations. If the paper is
originally 0.001 inch thick, it is 2 times thicker after folding it
once (2 x 0.001 ). When the paper is folded a second time, it
again becomes twice as thick, which is 4 times the original
thickness; that is, 2 x 2(0.001) inch thick, or 22 (0.001). When
it is folded a third time it becomes 8 times as thick as the
original, or 2 3 (0.001)-this is double the thickness after the
second fold. Similarly, the fourth fold is 2 4 (0.00 1) times as
thick as the original. Thus, 50 folds of the tissue paper would
equal2 50 (0.001), which is about 17,770,000 miles. This is
considerably larger than the 2 or 3 feet of thickness most
people estimate. Indeed, it is more than one-quarter the dis-
tance from Venus to Earth .
15. This problem also requires a systematic analysis of each step
(fish 1 eats two 2s for a total of two 2s, each 2 eats two 3s for
a total of four 3s, each 3 eats two 4s for a total of eight 4s,
and so on. This shows us the beginning of a geometric
progression (2, 4, 8, etc.). Mathematical tools make it much
easier to solve this problem. The formula is 2 6 , for a total of
64 size 7 fish that must be eaten each day.
Incidentally, it is this progression in the food chain that
results in "biological magnification." Even a small percentage
of toxins in a lake can become much more concentrated in
larger fish because they eat a large number of small fish each
day.
16. When the robot is seen in context, some of its design flaws be-
come more apparent. For example, its sponges are too big to
be dipped into its head (the bucket). Furthermore, since the
arms do not bend, the robot will push itself off the building if
it lowers its arms much farther.
Gleaning information from observing things in context is
analogous to acting on the basis of some idea or invention
and then looking at the effects. If we ignore the act and look
components of problem solving, we will often fail to spot
flaws in our ideas. Ideally, we should spot flows by means of
imagination or the use of prototypes so that they can be cor-
rected as soon as possible and with the least expense.
11m TheIdeal Problem Solver
Chapter 3
1. Most people generate such inventions as a magnifying glass,
an underwater sound amplifier, or an underwater light. These
are all fine answers, but it is useful to ask whether they are
constrained by any assumptions. For example, these inven-
tions are designed to help people enjoy live fish in an
aquarium. However, the problem of helping people enjoy the
sight of tropical fish in their own homes could also be solved
by an aquarium videotape. In fact, such tapes are on the
market and can be bought in many video stores.
2. The simplest solution is to pour the water from the next to
last glass on the left into the next-to-last glass on the right.
Many people fail to generate this solution because they as-
sume that the water cannot be poured from one glass into
another.
3. Most people generate such reasons as
Wants the exercise.
Needs the exercise.
Wants to surprise you.
Wants to visit someone on the way.
The elevator is broken.
All of these involve assume that the cousin is essentially nor-
mal. An alternative possibility is that the cousin is so short
that he cannot reach the higher buttons. He therefore
punches the highest button he can reach and walks from
there.
4. The most obvious solution to this problem is the one least
often generated. Simply throw the ball straight up in the air.
It will eventually stop and reverse direction.
Answers to Exercises . .
Start here
that the fall occurred outside the building. In this case it hap-
pened inside.
11 . People have difficulty solving this problem when they assume
that the boys must crawl through the pipe at the same time .
The problem does not state that they must crawl through the
pipe at the same time. It is easy if you assume that Jim crawls
through the pipe first in one direction and then Tom crawls
through the pipe in the opposite direction.
12. They stand back-to-back.
15. He was bald.
16. The new baby was Bill.
Chapter 4
l. This statement encourages the reader to make many inferen-
ces, but what it actually means may be quite different from
those invited inferences. For example, it could mean that
nine out of ten doctors surveyed have at one time or another
recommended this product to at least one person (not neces-
sarily a patient and not necessarily to the exclusion of other
similar products) . It does not necessarily mean that the doc-
tors prefer this product or advise patients to use it more than
they do any other product. Try to think of some unusual
products or substances that could legitimately fit this claim.
2. An advertising claim similar to this was used by the Chrysler
Corporation to promote one of its luxury cars. They com-
pared their car to vehicles manufactured by more recognized
luxury-car makers like Mercedes Benz and BMW. The invited
inference is that the Chrysler product is as good as or better
than the other well-known luxury models, but the factual
evidence does not support such a conclusion. For example, it
would be possible for the Chrysler vehicle to outperform at
least one vehicle on each of the tests and still rank last in
overall performance.
3. Many people who hear this advertisment might conclude that
the Dodge is rated better than cars made by other manufac-
turers. Actually, people were comparing the new Dodge to
their own old cars. It is not surprising that many people
would prefer a new car in place of their old car. People who
hear this ad might also conclude that 70 percent of Toyota
Rnswersto Exercises 1Jm
Figure B.4
10. No. It is possible that some xenos are not red. See the
diagram below.
Figure B.S
Red
Figure B.6
Zeeps
12. No. It is possible that some pennies are not silver. See the
diagram below.
Figure B.7
Silver coins
IIIII The Ideal Problem Solver
13. No. I may have skipped the party yet still failed to do my
homework. The form of reasoning being used here is "If A
(go to party), then B (cannot do my homework)." If one
were to reason "B, therefore A," one would be committing
the fallacy of affirming the consequent.
14. No. There may be other reasons interest rates were raised.
The form of reasoning being used here is "If A (the annual in-
flation rate rises above 7 percent), then B (the Federal
Reserve Bank will raise interest rates)." If one were to reason
"Not A, therefore not B," one would be committing the fal-
lacy of denying the antecedent.
15. No . The form of reasoning being used here is "If A (we put
more money into public education) then B (we will not
necessarily improve education)." If one were to reason "Not
A (we do not put more money into education), therefore not
B (we will necessarily improve education)" we would be com-
mitting the fallacy of denying the antecedent.
16. Although the form of logical reasoning that leads to such a
conclusion is valid, this is a clear case in which not only a
theory but also additional assumptions that relate that theory
to observable data are being tested. For example, it is possible
that the theory is correct and yet injections of RNA will have
no immediate effect on memory. It is also possible that the
site of the injection could be an important determinant of the
effect observed, that the injection must be given several days
in advance, and so forth.
17. It is most likely that the evidence used to support this con-
clusion came from correlational data. It would be unethical to
conduct an experiment in which depression (or any other
variable thought to be responsible for a person's well-being)
was manipulated. In a correlational study it is difficult to
draw conclusions about causality. In this case, it seems pos-
sible that the severity of a patient's illness might be causing
the depression.
18. This game will have no winner, because players will quickly
encounter a situation in which neither player can advance
without breaking the rule of never occupying the same
square simultaneously.
19. The first step is to define the problem. What information is
being requested? Is the problem to differentiate the liar from
Rnswers to Exercises IDII
Chapter G
l. How many stops did the bus make? Most people are not
prepared for this question. They anticipated a different ques-
tion; namely, how many people were left on the bus? Dif-
ferent strategies are necessary to prepare for these different
questions. This is a good illustration of how a strategy may or
may not be the most appropriate depending on the memory
problem one is attempting to solve.
What was the name of the bus driver? Most people have a
difficult time answering this question from memory because
they cannot remember being told anyone's name. If you look
IIIII The Ideal Problem Solver
back at the first sentence of the passage, you will see that the
answer is quite familiar to you.
2. An acrostic for remembering the cranial nerves that has been
around for some time is "On old Olympus' towering tops a
Finn and German vend some hops."
3. A helpful acrostic for remembering whether to set one's clock
forward or back an hour is "Spring forward and fall back."
4. You could combine the rhyming peg-word system (one is a
bun, two is a shoe, and so on) and an acrostic in the follow-
ing manner. Think of the acrostic "You must wear two shoes
to get in the door and stand in line." From the peg-word sys-
tem you know that "two is a shoe" (so two shoes equals 22).
Similarly, you know from the peg-word system that "four is a
door" (so door helps you remember the number 4) and that
"nine is a line" (so line helps you remember 9).
5. You could use acrostics such as "Fir is smooth" and "A rough
hem stands out." For the latter, think of "hem" as short for
hemlock and think of "standing out" as a rough twig.
6. For "across," an example using acrostics is "You only cross the
gate to heaven once." Let "across" remind you of "cross" and
let "once" remind you of the number of c's.
7. For "facilitate," you might say to yourself that "the face of a
penny has one picture of Lincoln." Let facilitate remind you
of "face" and let the thought of one picture of Lincoln (which
begins with an /) remind you that the word contains only one
/.
8. For "development," you might think, "When you want to
develop film, people should not enter the darkroom while the
process is going on." Let "development" remind you of
develop (as in developing the film) and let "do not enter"
remind you of "no e" after the word develop.
9. You might imagine someone climbing a big hill (for the first
part of his last name) and use the sound of the last part of his
name to signify Everest.
10. One possibility is to let the bert part of Hubert remind you of
dirt and to let Booth remind you of boots. Dirt left by boots
needs to be removed, preferably by a vacuum cleaner.
11. You could let John remind you of Johnny Appleseed, and let
Appleseed remind you of the Apple in Apple Computer Cor-
poration. Let the Scull in Sculley remind you of a skull, the
place where human intelligence is located. You can remem-
nnswers to Exercises . .
Chapter 7
Possible answers include
I. Spider web
2. Lawn sprinkler
3. Parking meter
4 . Wet paint
5. Cement mixer
6. Igloo
7. Bagpipes
Some possible answers are
8. Thermal underwear
9. The inside story
10. Downtown
IIJDI The Ideal Problem Solver
Figure B.8
1111 The Ideal Problem Solver
I Notes
1. Nim is described in F. V. Grunfeld, Games of the World: How to Make
Them-How to Play Them-How They Came to Be. New York: Plenary
Publications and Swiss Committee for Unicef, 1975. Grunfeld
notes that the game of Nim was enjoyed for centuries until the
mathematician Charles Leonard Bouton described a formula, in
1901, that ensured victory to any player acquainted with it.
2. Joe Hatcher, personal communication with the authors, 1982.
3. A discussion of this advertisement appeared in the column "Sell-
ing It," Consumer Reports, August 1992., p. 551.
4. Copyright 1989, R. B. M. Limited, 1200 Shames Drive, Westbury,
New York.
5. Many scholars have raised questions about the conclusions that
can be drawn from the Hawthorne studies. See R. Gillespie,
Manufacturing Knowledge: A History of the Hawthorne Experiments.
New York: Cambridge University Press, 1991; S. R. G. Jones,
Was there a Hawthorne effect? American Journal of Sociology
98( 1992):451-468.
IDEAL PROBLEM
NAVIGATION GUIDE
Goall:
Goal2:
Goal3:
Btl The Ideal Problem Solver
Explore possible strategies and new information that could help you
accomplish each of the important goals listed above.
Strategy
Possible Positive Outcomes Possible Negative Outcomes
Strategy
Possible Positive Outcomes Possible Negative Outcomes
10m Problem Navigation Guide IEJII
Strategy
Possible Positive Outcomes Possible Negative Outcomes
Strategy
Possible Positive Outcomes Possible Negative Outcomes
Strategy
Possible Positive Outcomes Possible Negative Outcomes
Strategy
Possible Positive Outcomes Possible Negative Outcomes
. . The Ideal Problem Solver
After acting on your strategies, what did you notice about the goals you
defined?
After acting on your strategies, what did you notice about the strategies
you explored?
IDEAL Problem Navigation Guide a.
After acting on your strategies, what did you notice about your ability to
anticipate their effects?
AUTHOR INDEX