ReadingInASecondLanguage Sample Ch13 PDF
ReadingInASecondLanguage Sample Ch13 PDF
comprehension
Over the past 15 years, there has been an increasing number of impor-
tant research studies, review chapters, and books on the learning and
teaching of vocabulary. A review of many current surveys of L1 and
L2 vocabulary reveals a fairly standard set of questions that are posed
and then answered. For example, what does it mean to know a word?
How many words are there in English? How many words can be learned
from the reading context? Should vocabulary be taught directly? How
many words can be taught? (Baumann & Kame’enui, 2004; Bogaards
& Laufer, 2004; Folse, 2004; Hiebert & Kamil, 2005; Nation, 2001;
Schmitt, 2000; Stahl & Nagy, 2006; Wagner, Muse, & Tannenbaum,
2007b). Furthermore, most publications addressing vocabulary learning
make strong connections between reading and the learning of written
forms of words. There are, of course, good reasons for this connection
between vocabulary and reading. This chapter departs somewhat from
the format of other chapters in this book – it addresses the questions
listed above (and others) in the process highlighting the promising rela-
tionship between reading and vocabulary.
1. Orthography (spelling)
2. Morphology (word-family relations)
3. Parts of speech
4. Pronunciation
5. Meanings (referential range, variant meanings, homophones)
6. Collocations (what words very commonly go with a word)
7. Meaning associations (topical links, synonyms, antonyms, hyponyms)
8. Specific uses (technical, common)
9. Register (power, politeness, disciplinary domain, formality, slang,
dialect form)
If, in fact, the information listed in Table 13.1 represents the network
of information that can be accessed automatically with thorough word
learning, it is not difficult to see how such an extensive network would
build multiple paths for reading comprehension. From a more practical
applied linguistics perspective, Read (2004) has noted that relatively
little has been done in L2 contexts to explore relationships among the
various components of word knowledge beyond breadth and depth of
vocabulary knowledge.
44 percent of the words appearing in the text. The 1,000 most frequent
word families (ignoring the word vs. word family distinction) from the
General Service List (West, 1953) that appear in a text usually represent
about 71 percent of the total words in a text. The 2,000 most frequent
word families usually represent about 76 percent of total word coverage
in an English text (Nation, 2001, 2004). When one adds the Academic
Word List (570 additional word families; Coxhead, 2000) or the BNC
3000 word list (1,000 additional word families; Nation, 2004), coverage
of most academic texts reaches 85 percent or 86 percent, respectively.
From this outline (see also Table 13.2), one can see that a goal would be
to identify the most frequent word families and teach them in order to
cover the highest number of words appearing in a text, and this has been
a goal of much research and instructional advice on vocabulary teach-
ing. However, the problem for reading comprehension is a bit more
complicated than just teaching the 2,000 or 3,000 most frequent word
families.
One problem with word-count coverage of texts is that the genre and
specific content of texts can have an impact on these generalizations,
although the variation created is not huge. For the BNC 3000 (Nation,
2004), word-family coverage of a fiction corpus is about 90 percent
while word-family coverage for an academic corpus is about 86 percent.
The bigger problem is that the number of words needed to cover a text
grows in an exponential way, especially after about 89 percent coverage.
That is why about 40,000 words are needed to provide 98–99 percent
coverage for most general texts in English. This changing ratio beyond 89
percent word coverage is a problem for vocabulary learning and reading
comprehension, as will be described below.
A helpful explanation of vocabulary size needed to read a range
of English texts is provided by Nation (2006). He argues that about
4,000 word families (plus proper nouns) (roughly 10,000 independent
word meanings) are needed for reading instructionally (that is, with
instructional support) with approximately 95 percent of words known
in a given text. About 9,000 word families, comprising 98 percent of
words known in a text, are needed for fluent reading in English. Even
Vocabulary and reading comprehension 271
student’s vocabulary does not prevent that student from building word
knowledge by reading, though it is harder to learn words from context
incidentally when words in the immediate context are also unknown
(Nation, 2001; Pulido, 2009).
In some respects, trying to decide the exact number of words an L2
student will need to function in an advanced academic setting is not ever
going to be fully successful. There are too many variables that influence
student comprehension. Moreover, the means for deciding what counts
as an independent word that needs to be learned sometimes can veer
into flights of fancy. At the same time, it is clear that academically
oriented L2 students will need to learn many words beyond the 2,000
most frequent word families, and the notion that L2 students still need to
know the first 2,000 words families in English well retains its force as an
important argument for vocabulary instruction (Meara, 1995; Nation,
2001; Schmitt, 2000).
not a very efficient way to learn new words as part of explicit vocabulary
instruction.
Despite these qualifications, guessing words from context represents
an important independent word-learning strategy over time (e.g., Huckin
& Bloch, 1993; Nassaji, 2003a; Nation, 2001; Sanaoui, 1995). It also
represents an important way for learners to cope with difficult texts.
To help students develop guessing skills appropriately, students need
to practice and analyze guessing from context in texts that they are
reading, recognize clues that may be useful (e.g., discourse-marking
words, punctuation, word-part information, part of speech, examples,
and descriptions), and be encouraged by teachers to become more aware
of new words while reading. Fukkink and de Glopper (1998), “in a
meta-analysis of 21 studies involving native speakers found that training
resulted in better guessing, particularly if learners’ attention was directed
to clues in the context” (cited in Nation, 2001: 251; cf. Baumann et al.,
2002, who point out that there was no vocabulary from context stud-
ies found to be sufficiently rigorous to be included in the meta-analysis
by the National Reading Panel 2000). A reasonable conclusion to draw
from the debate on teaching context clues is the need to raise L2 stu-
dent awareness of context information that will help them derive a good
guess of the meaning of a new word (see Baumann et al., 2005; Huckin
& Bloch, 1993; Nation, 2001; Stahl & Nagy, 2006).
from context (e.g., Baumann et al., 2005; Nation, 2001; Stahl & Nagy,
2006).
Raising students’ awareness of new words that they encounter in texts
represents an important learning goal. Almost every current review of
vocabulary now stresses the importance of (a) making students aware of
the new words they encounter, and (b) motivating students to learn and
use the new words. Students need to become collectors of words; that
is, they need to attend to new words they encounter, either by listing
words that are interesting or difficult, or by noting possible connections
between new words and known words, or by trying to use new words
in some interesting way. Arguments for the role of word awareness
are discussed in Anderson (1999), Beck, McKeown, and Kucan (2002),
Graves and Watts-Taffe (2002), Nation (2001), Pressley and Woloshyn
(1995), Schmitt (2000), Scott (2005), Scott and Nagy (2004), and Stahl
and Nagy (2006).
If the 300 most frequent words are essentially the “glue” of the
language, being words that have to be learned just to learn the grammar
and have enough words for simple sentences to manipulate as L2 learn-
ing begins, they are part of any basic English instruction. These words do
not have to be covered explicitly as part of separate vocabulary instruc-
tion. For example, the words that appear between 41 to 50 in the list
of the most frequent words in American English (Fry, Kress, & Foun-
toukidis, 1993) are as follows: would, all, she, her, more, been, about,
there, when, its. These words are all function words in English. Words
between 91–100 involve two verbs (call and get), one noun (water), and
seven function words. Since the first 1,000 words provide 71 percent of
coverage of most texts (Nation, 2004), the remaining 700 words need to
be included in part of explicit vocabulary instruction. The second 1,000
words may provide about 4.5 percent coverage, but the first 500 of these
second 1,000 do most of the work. These second-tier words, from 300
to 1,500, should all be strong candidates for explicit instruction. The
third tier should include words from 1,500 to 2,500, including the Aca-
demic Word List (Coxhead, 2000). They should be taught whenever the
opportunity arises and it is reasonable to do so. Alternatively, Nation’s
(2004) British National Corpus Word List could provide the third tier
for words from 1,500 to 3,000 on this list. This core word list of three
tiers, based on word families, would most likely provide L2 learners with
about 6,000 important independent words to learn. This, in itself, is a
formidable L2 learning task.
Beyond these 6,000 primary candidate words for learning, students
need to build up additional words through multiple exposures to topi-
cally related reading material (e.g., through extensive reading) as well as
instructional discussions on content materials. Reading topically related
material offers good opportunities for the recycling of vocabulary, work-
ing with associated sets of words, and word collecting (as well as greater
engagement with reading and learning). These reading materials will
allow for extended vocabulary learning through context, exposure to
less common words, and repetitions with more frequent vocabulary. The
combination of extensive reading, word-collecting habits, and word-
learning strategies will eventually bring the motivated learner to the
recognition vocabulary level needed for advanced academic study.
fast mapping is possible (though this is probably not done with a large
percentage of learned words in the L1). L2 learners have real advantages
in that many L2 words represent labels for concepts that are already
well-developed, and there is a ready-made space in the L2 lexicon that
is waiting to be filled. It is reasonable to suggest that L2 students have
their own favorable conditions for some type of fast mapping with some
subsets of encountered words.
The argument that words are ready to be learned because they can
fill a cognitive space may seem, at first glance, to be an unusual idea.
One might ask why the word dolphin, from the example above, might
fill a cognitive space said to be well-established across L1 knowledge
networks as well as a growing L2 network. Theoretically, the idea that
dolphin is “ready to be learned” gains support from the recent theory of
Latent Semantic Analysis (Landauer, 1998; Landauer & Dumais, 1997).
As Stanovich (2000) notes:
To put the idea simply, the more known words that regularly appear
in the semantic neighborhood of the word dolphin, the easier it will be to
acquire the meaning of the word dolphin. When we learn about whales
and sharks, we learn also about the sea, about fins, about swimming
habits, about predators, and so on. As a result, many concepts associated
with certain fish and sea mammals are readily transferable to the new
word, dolphin, and dolphin becomes easy to learn.
Landauer and his colleagues (Landauer & Dumais, 1997) have
demonstrated this phenomenon empirically, drawing on conceptualiza-
tions from connectionist modeling and large-scale statistical analyses.
They wrote a computer program to “learn” many words simply by being
exposed to many texts. The performance of the connectionist learning
program on a vocabulary test indicated that many words could be learned
by the computer from fairly minimal direct exposure if the words were
associated with large networks of related words that were given many
exposures. This empirical demonstration functions much like the student
who knows a lot of related words and many concepts associated with
the word to be learned.
L2 students may find themselves in many situations in which a word
is “ready” to be learned, both because of growing L2 vocabulary knowl-
edge and because of extensive cognitive / conceptual networks from their
Vocabulary and reading comprehension 283
6. Teach a limited set of key words for depth, precision, and multiple
encounters.
7. Focus on word relationships (parts-of-speech variations, word fam-
ilies, synonyms, antonyms, graded relations).
8. Provide word instruction that combines contextual information and
definitional information (word-part information, cognates, context
cues, affix information, flash cards, imagery).
9. Help students learn word-part information and apply it to greater
word awareness.
10. Use visual supports and mapping techniques.
11. Work with dictionary definitions and rewrite more accessible defi-
nitions.
12. Develop activities that recycle a lot of words at one time (e.g., sort-
ing words into lists, semantic mapping, matching activities, word-
recognition fluency activities, repeated reading practice).
13. Create a vocabulary-rich environment.
14. Raise student awareness of words: Have students collect, keep, use,
and share words they want. Talk about words and build word con-
sciousness and word interest.
15. Recycle vocabulary over time to ensure multiple exposures to words
throughout vocabulary instruction (rereading prior texts for new
purposes, having students nominate words to work with, adding
words from prior units as part of sorting, classifying, and connecting
activities).
16. Give students some choices in word learning.
17. Develop student motivation for word collecting and provide a sup-
portive learning environment.
Semantic Map
tigers poachers
frogs danger
apes loss
condors oil companies
global warming
ENDANGERED
parks
protection biology
Panatanal environment
Galapagos Islands species
Rain forests evolution
Greenpeace
Word Map
maintain attention
maintenance entertain intentional
entertainment
Concept-of-definition Map
Nature McDonalds
Conservancy BIODIVERSITY Oil refineries
Greenpeace Acid rain
Amazon forests
Forest Variety of species
Different habitats
Interdependence
Symbiotic relationships