Pattern Sounds PDF
Pattern Sounds PDF
Patterns of sounds
In this series:
Ian Maddieson
University of California at Los Angeles
Published in the United States of America by Cambridge University Press, New York
Preface 1
2 StQ
P s and affricates 25
2.1 Introduction 25
2.2 Stop series 25
2.3 Summary of analysis of stop systems 31
2.4 Stop systems by place 31
2.5 Stop and affricate places 34
2.6 Voicing and place of articulation for plosives 34
2.7 Secondary articulations with plosives 37
2.8 Affricates 38
2.9 Summary of generalizations on stops and affricates 39
Notes to Chapter 2 40
References 40
3 Fricatives 41
3.1 Introduction 41
3.2 The occurrence of fricatives 42
3.3 Number of fricatives per language 43
3.4 Implications of voicing in fricatives 47
3.5 Gamkrelidze1s implicational proposals 49
3.6 Predicting frequency from intensity 49
3.7 Estimates of perceptual salience of fricatives 51
3.8 The structure of systems of fricatives 52
3.9 The phoneme /h/ 57
3.10 Conclusion 57
References 58
4 Nasals 59
4.1 Introduction 59
4.2 Types of nasals 59
4.3 Ferguson's "Assumptions about nasals": primary nasal consonants 61
4.4 Primary nasals and obstruents 64
4.5 Secondary nasal consonants 65
4.6 Restated generalizations about nasals 69
4.7 Explanations for nasal patterns 70
References 71
5 Liquids 73
5.1 Introduction 73
5.2 Overall frequency of liquids 73
5.3 Laterals 74
5.4 R-sounds 78
5.5 Structure of liquid systems 82
5.6 Generalizations on the structure of liquid systems 87
5.7 Conclusion 89
Notes to Chapter 5 89
References 90
6 Vocoid approximants 91
6.1 Introduction 91
6.2 Frequency of vocoid approximants 91
6.3 Approximants and related vowels 94
6.4 Approximants and related consonants 95
6.5 Other approximants 96
6.6 Summary 97
Notes to Chapter 6 97
References 97
8 Vowels 123
8.1 Introduction 123
8.2 Types of vowels 123
8.3 Number of vowels per language 126
8.4 Distinctive vowel qualities 127
8.5 Properties of vowel series 128
8.6 Diphthongs 133
8.7 Summary 134
Notes to Chapter 8 135
References 135
vii
9 Insights on vowel spacing 136
9.1 Introduction 136
9.2 Preliminaries 136
9.3 Method 139
9.4 Analysis of defective systems 141
9.5 Conclusions 153
Notes to Chapter 9 154
References 155
Appendix 15: Phoneme charts and segment index for UPSID languages 200
The charts 200
The index 202
Vowel symbols and diacritics used in phoneme charts 204
Segment index 205
Phoneme charts 263
This book is dedicated to the memory of Lilian Ann Maddieson and
Henry Ray Maddieson, who gave me the freedom to go my own way.
ix
Preface
I also owe thanks to those who have shown faith in the UPSID project as
it developed by making use of it, including Louis Goldstein, Pat Keating,
Peter Ladefoged, BjBrn Lindblom and the students in Linguistics 103 at
UCLA.
A considerable portion of the work reported in this book has been
funded by the National Science Foundation through grants BNS 78-07680 and
BNS 80-23110 (Peter Ladefoged, principal investigator). Neither the NSF nor
any of the individuals named above are responsible for the errors that
undoubtedly remain. If you the reader find one, please write and tell me
about it.
Ian Maddieson
University of California
Los Angeles
References
Bell, A. 1978. Language samples. In J.H. Greenberg et al. (eds.) Universals
of Human Language, Vol 1^, Method and Theory. Stanford University Press,
Stanford: 123-56.
Ferguson, C. A. 1963. Some assumptions about nasals. In J.H. Greenberg
(ed.) Universals of Language. MIT Press, Cambridge: A2-7.
Greenberg, J. H. 1970. Some generalizations concerning glottalic
consonants, especially implosives. International Journal of American
Linguistics 36: 123-45.
Hurford, J. R. 1977. The significance of linguistic generalizations.
Language 53: 574-620.
Hyman, L. M. 1977. On the nature of linguistic stress. In L.M. Hyman (ed.)
Studies on Stress and Accent. (Southern California Occasional Papers in
Linguistics 4) University of Southern California, Los Angeles: 37-82.
Jakobson, R. and Halle, M. 1956. Phonology and phonetics. (Part 1 of)
Fundamentals of Language. Mouton, The Hague: 3-51.
Ladefoged, P. 1971. Preliminaries to Linguistic Phonetics. University of
Chicago Press, Chicago.
Trubetskoy, N. 1939. Grundzttge der Phonologie (Travaux du Cercle
Linguistique de Prague 9 ) . Prague.
The size and structure of phonological inventories
1.1 Introduction
A database designed to give more reliable and more readily available
answers to questions concerning the distribution of phonological segments
in the world's languages has been created as part of the research program
of the UCLA Phonetics Laboratory. The database is known formally as the
UCLA Phonological Segment Inventory Database, and for convenience is
referred to by the acronym UPSID. UPSID has been used to investigate a
number of hypothesized phonological universals and "universal tendencies".
Principal among these have been certain ideas concerning the overall size
and structure of the phonological inventories. The design of the database
is briefly described in this chapter. A full description is given in
chapter 10, and the various appendices at the end of the book report on the
data contained in UPSID files. The remainder of the present chapter
discusses the issues involving the overall structure and size of
phonological inventories which have been examined with its use.
language to include from within a group, but such factors as the number of
speakers and the phonological peculiarity of the language are not
considered. The database includes the inventories of 317 languages. In this
and subsequent chapters, every language mentioned in the text is identified
by a number that cross-refers to the list of these languages and the data
charts at the end of the book. These numbers are assigned on the basis of
the genetic affiliation of the language.
In the database each segment which is considered phonemic is
represented by its most characteristic allophone, specified in terms of a
set of 58 phonetic attributes. These are treated as variables which take
the value 1 if the segment has the attribute and 0 if the segment lacks it.
The list of attributes with the value 1 thus provides a phonetic
description of the segment concerned.
For 192 of the 317 languages included, UPSID has profited from the work
of the Stanford Phonology Archive (SPA). Our decisions on phonemic status
and phonetic description do not always coincide with the decisions reached
by the compilers of the SPA, and we have sometimes examined additional
or alternative sources, but a great deal of effort was saved by the
availability of this source of standardized analyses. It should be noted
that UPSID, unlike the SPA, makes no attempt to include information on
allophonic variation, syllable structure, or phonological rules.
In determining the segment inventories, there are two especially
problematical areas. The first involves choosing between a unit or sequence
interpretation of, for example, affricates, prenasalized stops, long
(geminate) consonants and vowels, diphthongs, labialized consonants, etc.
The available evidence which bears on the choice in each language
individually has been examined but with some prejudice in favor of treating
complex phonetic events as sequences (i.e. as combinations of more
elementary units). The second problem area involves the choice between a
segmental and a suprasegmental analysis of certain properties. Stress and
tone have always been treated as suprasegmental; that is, tonal and stress
contrasts do not by themselves add to the number of distinct segments in
the inventory of a language, but if differences in segments are found which
accompany stress or tone differences, these may be regarded as segmental
contrasts if the association does not seem a particularly natural one. For
example, if there is an unstressed vowel which is a little shorter or more
centralized than what can be seen as its stressed counterpart, these vowels
will be treated as variants of the same segment. However, larger
Patterns of sounds
7
The size and structure of phonological inventories
Similarly, the facts do not seem to show that languages with small
inventories (under 20 segments) suffer from problems due to lack of
contrastive possibilities at the morphemic level. The symptoms of such
difficulties would include unacceptably high incidence of homophony or
unmanageably long morphemes. Dictionaries and vocabularies of several
languages with small inventories, such as Rotokas (625, Firchow, Firchow
and Akoitai 1973), Hawaiian (424, Pukui and Elbert 1965) and Asmat (601,
Voorhoeve 1965: 293-361), do not provide evidence that there are symptoms
of stress of these kinds in languages with small phoneme inventories.
Hawaiian, for example, with 13 segments has been calculated to have an
average of just 3.5 phonemes per morpheme (Pukui and Elbert 1965: xix) ,
clearly not unacceptably long. And again, comparative evidence indicates
that small inventory size may be a phenomenon which persists over time, as,
for example, in the Polynesian language family, which includes Hawaiian
(Grace 1959).
This proposal predicts not only that upper and lower limits on
inventory size will tend to be rather flexible, as is the case, but also
that areal-genetic deviations from the central tendency should be expected.
Thus, greater than average size inventories in Khoisan or Caucasian
languages, and smaller than average in Polynesian are understandable
results: local deviations are perpetuated because primary contact is with
other languages tending in the same direction. This proposal also avoids a
difficulty; if human processing limitations are postulated as the cause of
limitations on the size of inventories, then they ought invariably to exert
pressure to conform on the deviant cases. The evidence for this is lacking.
10
Patterns of sounds
r
"Small" ." j t a l "Large"
percent percent percent
More l i k e l y in
small i n v e n t o r i e s
hi 89.5% 82.6% 77.8%
hi 93.0% 89.3% 79.3%
hi 59.6% 52.7% 51.9%
Equally l i k e l y in large
or small i n v e n t o r i e s
/m/ 94.7% 94.3% 92.6%
hi 75.4% 75.1% 77.8%
More l i k e l y in
large i n v e n t o r i e s
/b/ 45.6% 62.8% 77.8%
/g/ 42.1% 55.2% 75.9%
/?/ 33.3% 30.3% 55.6%
nil 22.8% 44.5% 64.8%
hi 15.8% 42.6% 51.8%
ill 17.5% 46.1% 70.4%
ni 78.9% 85.5% 94.4%
hi 22.8% 33.8% 37.0%
11
The size and structure of phonological inventories
12
Patterns of sounds
13
The size and structure of phonological inventories
are 5 exceptions in UPSID. Ewe, 114, Efik, 119, and Auca, 818,
have /p/ but no palatal or palato-alveolar stops. Hupa, 705, has
/m/ but no bilabial stops. Igbo, 116, has /mr)/ but no labial-
velar stops; it does have labialized velars. There are numerous
examples of languages with stops at particular places of
articulation with no corresponding nasal consonant.)
(iv) Voiceless nasals and approximants do not occur unless the language
has the voiced counterparts. (No exceptions in UPSID.)
(v) Mid vowels do not occur unless high and low vowels occur.
(Two exceptions in UPSID; all languages have at least one high
vowel but Cheremis, 051, and Tagalog, 414, are reported to lack
low vowels.)
(vi) Rounded front vowels do not occur unless unrounded front vowels
of the same basic height occur. (Two exceptions in UPSID, Bashkir,
063, and Khalaj, 064.)
(vii) / 0/ and /CE/ do not occur (separately or together) unless
/ y/ also occurs. (Hopi, 738, is a clear exception. Wolof, 107,
has one front rounded vowel, / 0/, but this has allophones as high
as [ y] . Akan, 115, has marginal phonemes / 0: / and /oe:/ but
no / y/ .)
Yet, as briefly illustrated in section 1.4, such observations cannot be
compiled into a single composite hierarchy. At the very least, alternate
choices must be built in at certain points. This is because equally valid
general prohibitions on the co-occurrence of segments within an inventory
can also be found. Some of these are given below:
(i) A language does not contain both (voiced) implosives and
laryngealized plosives at the same place of articulation. (No
counterexamples in UPSID.)
(ii) A language does not contain a voiceless lateral fricative and a
voiceless lateral approximant. (No counterexamples in UPSID.)
(iii) A language does not contain both /<£/ and /f / or both /p/ and
/v/. (2 counterexamples in UPSID, Tarascan, 747, and Ewe, 114.)
(iv) A language does not include a dental stop, fricative, nasal or
lateral and an alveolar stop, fricative, nasal or lateral of the
same type. (There are 22 exceptions to this observation but this
number is significantly fewer than would be anticipated if the
co-occurrence were unrestricted; 43 co-occurrences of /£/ and
/\l alone would be expected otherwise on the basis of a calculation
14
Patterns of sounds
15
The size and structure of phonological inventories
16
Patterns of sounds
17
The size and structure of phonological inventories
(a) p t k iCp
b d g gb
(b) p t k
pw kw
b d g
The correlation was obtained between the number of places out of a list
of 10 and the number of manners out of a list of 14 "series-generating"
manner components for each language2 (glottal was not included in the
calculation of places because glottal stops do not (ordinarily) have
contrasting manners). The numbers of languages involved are shown in Table
1.2. Those rows with very sparse representation, i.e. less than 3 and more
than 5 places, or more than 4 manners, have been eliminated, removing 29
languages from the calculation. There is essentially no correlation between
the numbers of places and the numbers of manners for stops, whereas the
hypothesis of compensation would predict a strong negative correlation.
A similar computation was performed for fricatives relating place to
manner with cases with over 5 places or over 4 manners dropped (resulting
in 16 languages being excluded over and above the 21 languages which have
no fricatives). The results are given in Table 1.3. The observed data are
significantly different from expected (p = .0001), and in this case a
fairly substantial positive correlation (r = .46) between the two variables
is found. Again this is counter to the predictions of a compensation
hypothesis, and more strongly so than is the case with stops.
18
Patterns of sounds
Manners
1 2 3 4
Totals
1 19 83 35 20 1 157
Places 2 14 28 32 21 1 95
3 3 16 11 6 1 36
Totals 36 127 78 47
Manners
1 2 3
Totals
1 37 8 1 | 46
2 46 35 1 | 82
Places 3 21 45 8 1 74
4 6 32 8 | 46
5 4 22 6 1 32
19
The size and structure of phonological inventories
Tone
complex tone system 2 6
simple tone system 2 4
no tones 22 15
inadequate data 2 5
20
Patterns of sounds
21
The size and structure of phonological inventories
Even with the uncertainties involved in this kind of counting, the numbers
differ markedly enough for the conclusion to be drawn that languages are
not strikingly similar in terms of the size of their syllable inventories.
In following up this study, several tests were done to see which of a
number of possible predictors correlated best with syllable inventory size.
The predictors used were the number of segments, the number of vowels, the
number of consonants, the number of permitted syllable structures (CV, CVC,
CCVC, etc.), the number of suprasegmental contrasts (e.g. number of stress
levels times number of tones), and a number representing a maximal count of
segmental differences in which the number of vowels was multiplied by the
number of suprasegmentals. Of these, the best predictor is the number of
permitted syllable types (r = -69), an indication that the phonotactic
possibilities of the language are the most important factor contributing to
the number of syllables. The next best predictor is the number of
suprasegmentals (r = .59), with the correlation with the various segmental
22
Patterns of sounds
counts all being somewhat lower. Although all the predictors tested show a
positive simple correlation with the number of syllables, in a multiple
regression analysis only the number of vowels contributes a worthwhile
improvement to the analysis (r change = .19) beyond the number of syllable
types. Thus we can say that syllable inventory size does not depend heavily
on segment inventory size. Nonetheless, because the predictors do have
positive correlations with syllable inventory size, the picture is once
again of a tendency for complexity of different types to go together.
1.9 Conclusions
Work, with UPSID has confirmed that segment inventories have a well-defined
central tendency as far as size is concerned. Nonetheless considerable
variation in their size and structure occurs. Their structure is subject to
a hierarchical organization in many particulars but cannot be substantially
explained in terms of a single unified hierarchy of segment types. This is
partly because segments of certain types are subject to rules of mutual
exclusion. The mutual exclusions cannot all be explained as due to the
avoidance of inadequate phonetic contrasts, as some involve strongly
salient distinctions. A search for evidence that languages maintain a
balance by compensation for complexity in one phonological respect by
possessing simplicity elsewhere failed to find it in balance between
classes of segments, between segments and suprasegmental contrasts, or
between segments and phonotactic conditions. These investigations suggest
that complexity of various kinds occurs together in languages, and that
languages really do differ in their phonological complexity.
Notes
1. If languages with large phoneme inventories were approaching some kind
of limit on the ability to discriminate contrasts, it would be expected
that speakers of these languages would show higher error rates in tasks
involving phoneme recognition than speakers of languages with small
inventories. I know of no experimental data which bear on this point.
2. The manner components are: plain voiceless, plain voiced, voiceless
aspirated, breathy, preaspirated, laryngealized, implosive, ejective,
prenasalized, nasally-released, labialized, palatalized, velarized,
pharyngealized.
3. Of course, other compensations may exist between aspects of the
segmental inventories not examined here; and the failure to find
evidence for gross compensatory tendencies does not affect the validity
of any posited historical evolution in a particular case.
4. Rotokas is not really very complex in its suprasegmentals. It has a
partially predictable stress and a contrast of vowel length that seems
only partly independent of stress (Firchow, Firchow and Akoitai 1973).
Long vowels are not treated as separate segments in UPSID for this
language.
23
The size and structure of phonological inventories
References
Baucom, K.L. 1974. Proto-Central Khoisan. In E. Voeltz (ed.) Third Annual
Conference on African Linguistics (Indiana University Publications,
African Series, 7 ) . Indiana University, Bloomington: 3-38.
Crothers, J. 1978. Typology and universals of vowel systems. In J.H.
Greenberg et al. (eds.) Universals of Human Language, Vol. 2, Phonology.
Stanford University Press, Stanford: 93-152.
Disner, S.F. 1982. Vowel Quality: the Relationship between Universal and
Language-Specific Factors (UCLA Working Papers in Phonetics 58).
University of California, Los Angeles.
Dixon, R.M.W. 1980. The Languages of Australia. Cambridge University Press,
Cambridge.
Firchow, I. and Firchow, J. 1969. An abbreviated phoneme inventory.
Anthropological Linguistics 11: 271-6.
Firchow, I., Firchow, J. and Akoitai, D. 1973. Vocabulary of
Rotokas - Pidgin - English. Summer Institute of Linguistics, Papua New
Guinea Branch, Ukarumpa.
Grace, G. W. 1959. The position of the Polynesian languages within the
Austronesian (Malayo-Polynesian) language family (IJAL Memoir 16).
Indiana University, Bloomington.
Hagege, C. and Haudricourt, A. 1978. La Phonologie Panchronique. Presses
Universitaires de France, Paris.
Hockett, C.F. 1955. A Manual of Phonology (IJAL Memoir 11). Indiana
University, Bloomington.
Hombert, J-M. , Ohala, J.J. and Ewan, W.G. 1979. Phonetic explanations for
the development of tones. Language 55: 37-58.
Ladefoged, P. 1978. Phonetic differences within and between languages. UCLA
Working Papers in Phonetics 41: 32-40.
Ladefoged, P. 1980. What are linguistic sounds made of? Language 56:
485-502.
Liljencrants, J. and Lindblom, B. 1972. Numerical simulation of vowel
quality contrasts: the role of perceptual contrast. Language 48: 839-62.
Lindau, M. 1982. Phonetic differences in glottalic consonants. UCLA Working
Papers in Phonetics 54: 66-77.
Martinet, A. 1955. Economie des changements phonetiques (2nd ed.). Franke,
Berne.
Matisoff, J.M. 1973. Tonogenesis in Southeast Asia. In L.M. Hyman (ed.)
Consonant Types and Tone (Southern California Occasional Papers in
Linguisticsl). University of Southern California, Los Angeles.
Pukui, M.K. and Elbert, S.H. 1965. Hawaiian - English Dictionary (3rd
ed.). University of Hawaii Press, Honolulu.
Sheldon, S.N. 1974. Some morphophonemic and tone perturbation rules in
Mura-Piraha. International Journal of American Linguistics 40: 279-82.
Snyman, J.W. 1975. Zul hoasi Fonologie en Woordeboek. Balkema, Cape Town.
Traill, A. 1978. Research on the Non-Bantu African languages. In L.W.
Lanham and K.P. Prinsloo (eds.) Language and Communication Studies in
South Africa. Oxford University Press, Cape Town.
Voorhoeve, C.L. 1965. The Flamingo Bay Dialect of the Asmat Language
(Verhandelingen van het Koninklijk Instituut voor Taal-, Land- en
Volkenkunde 46). Nijhoff, The Hague.
Wurm, S.A. 1972. Languages of Australia and Tasmania (Janua Linguarum,
Series Critica 1 ) . Mouton, The Hague.
24
Stops and affricates
2.1 Introduction
Stops occur in the inventories of all known languages and have
appropriately been regarded as the optimal consonants (e.g. by Jakobson and
Halle 1956: 4 2 ) . The most frequently found types of stops are plosives,
that is, stops made with an egressive pulmonic airstream. Apart from
differences in place of articulation, these may vary in a number of ways
through variations in laryngeal settings and in the relative timing of
voice onset and offset and of velic closure or opening. In addition there
are stops made with glottalic and velaric airstreams, i.e. ejective stops,
implosives and clicks. The principal architecture of stop systems is
conveniently discussed in terms of two dimensions representing the manner
series and the places of articulation that occur. In this chapter, we will
therefore analyze the structure of stop systems in the languages in the
UPS ID database in terms of the number of series and the number of places
used. We will also examine in more detail some questions concerning the
frequency of stops, particularly plosives, at different places of
articulation. Glottalic and laryngealized stops are discussed in more
detail in a separate chapter on glottalic consonants (Chapter 7 ) . Clicks
are not the subject of any special analysis, mainly because so few of the
UPSID languages contain any, and they are not included in the totals in
this chapter. However, nonlateral affricates (except affricated clicks) are
included in some of the analyses because of the close relationship of
affricates to stops. Lateral affricates are discussed further in Chapter 5.
25
Stops and affricates
26
Patterns of sounds
Plain voiceless plosives series are the most frequently found, with almost
92% of languages having such a series. Keating, Linker and Huffman (1983)
suggest that this type of plosive is the most widespread phonetically in
languages, and argue that they are most frequent because they are the most
efficient from the aerodynamic and articulatory points of view (at least in
initial positions). A language with only one stop series almost invariably
has plain voiceless plosives (49 out of 50) and the one exception, the
Australian language Bandjalang (368), may be incorrectly reported as having
voiced plosives. (Australian languages more typically have a voiceless
unaspirated stop series.) In none of the languages with only one series is
there an aspirated voiceless series nor do any have any glottalic or
laryngealized stops. Indeed, these types of stop series do not become at
all frequent until there are at least 3 series of stops. This may be seen
from Table 2.3, which shows the percentage of languages with a given number
of series that have a series of the particular type listed. The table only
gives a partial listing of the possibilities.
27
Stops and affricates
Number of series
1 2 3 4
Plain voiceless 98 .0% 90.1% 89.5% 96.0%
Plain voiced 2.0% 81.5% 69.7% 88.0%
Aspirated voiceless 0.0% 16.0% 63.2% 52.0%
Voiceless ejective
or voiceless laryngealized 0.0% 3.7% 42.1% 56.0%
Voiced implosive or
voiced laryngealized 0.0% 1.2% 27.6% 48.0%
28
Patterns of sounds
29
Stops and affricates
30
Patterns of sounds
31
Stops and affricates
Note that there are 24 languages which have stops at both dental and
alveolar places, though these are not always contrastive, since the place
could be redundantly predicted from a manner difference in some languages.
For example Guahibo (830) is reported with aspirated dental /t h / but
alveolar /t, d/; Sundanese (408) has /t/ but /d/; Yulu (216) has dental
/t, i/ but its implosive and prenasalized stops are possibly produced
further back and have been entered as "cf" and " d". It follows from the
near universality of bilabial and velar places that a language with stops
at only 3 places is unlikely to contrast dental and alveolar. In fact none
of the languages with both dental and alveolar stops have less than 4
places and 5 or 6 is more typical (17 of 24 cases, or 70.8%). The contrast
of dental and alveolar places is thus particularly associated with the use
of a relatively large number of places in the stop system of the language
concerned. Typically languages with stops in the palatal area or with
retroflex, uvular or labial-velar stops also have 4 or more places; this is
true for 58 of 59 languages in the case of palatals (98.3%), for 44 of 47
languages in the case of uvulars (93.6%), and for all the languages with
retroflex or labial-velar stops (100%).
Note that the most common place system uses 3 well distinguished
articulators - the lips (bilabial), the tongue tip or blade (dental or
alveolar) and the tongue body (velar). An additional place is more likely
to be another tongue-body articulation (either palatal or uvular) rather
than another tongue tip/blade articulation (e.g. retroflex). The only other
pattern that is at all common for expanding the number of places used is to
32
Patterns of sounds
33
Stops and affricates
Table 2.6 Number of places used for stops and affricates, by language
When affricate places are included, a handful of languages are counted with
7 places of articulation, and the proportion of languages with 4, 5 and 6
places increases. By this count, 4 rather than 3 is the most frequent
number of places to contrast (cf. Table 2.4), with the most common pattern
being for palato-alveolar affricates to be added to the near-universal
bilabial, dental/alveolar and velar stops. This pattern accounts for 86
languages - over a quarter of the total sample by itself.
The languages with 5 places in Table 2.6 are most likely to add
palato-alveolar affricates to the 3 basic positions together with either a
uvular (19 languages), palatal (12 languages) or retroflex (11 languages)
stop place. Ten of this set of languages, however, have two additional
places at which affricates but not stops appear. Of the 25 languages with 6
places, 17 add palato-alveolar affricates to a system with a smaller number
of stop places. The languages with 6 places at which stops occur tend not
to have any affricates at any place, but this too may be an Australian
peculiarity rather than a general rule. Recall that these are mostly
Australian languages. Note that Australian languages frequently have no
fricatives as well as having no affricates (see Chapter 3 ) .
34
Patterns of sounds
Chapter 7 and the findings there should be compared. The most common types
of plosives are naturally enough the plain voiced and voiceless types at
the 3 main places of articulation. Their frequencies are given in Table
2.7. Separate totals are given for dental and alveolar stops, as well as
for the "unspecified dental or alveolar" category. Below, the total of
these 3 categories is given. In the text, the combined category of dentals
and alveolars is represented by a phonetic symbol preceded by an asterisk,
i.e. *t , *d.
The class of segments represented by /*t/ is the most common of these, but
the total of 309 /*t/'s should be "corrected" for the 19 languages that
have both /t/ and /t/, so that only 290 languages have a /*t/ segment.
Nonetheless all languages with a series of plain voiceless plosives, apart
from Hawaiian, have a /*t/ segment. Note that there are about 20 more /k/'s
than /p/'s. There are also about 20 fewer /g/'s than /b/'s or /*d/'s.
Similar relationships between these stops can be seen not just in the raw
totals, but also in the structure of the inventories of individual
languages. Thus there are 24 languages that have /k/ but lack /p/. Note
that these are not languages that avoid use of the bilabial place in
general - most of them (18 of the 24) have a voiced series of stops
including /b/. All of them include /*t/ in their inventories. On the other
hand, there are only 4 languages with a /p/ but no /k/• Thus, /p/ is more
likely to be "missing" than /k/ and an implicational hierarchy can be set
up such that presence of /p/ implies the strong likelihood of the presence
of /k/, which similarly implies presence of /*t/. With a plain voiced
series, the place preferences are different. Twenty-one languages have a
series of voiced stops which lacks /g/; in 6 of these /*d/ is also missing,
leaving a "series" consisting of only /b/. Two languages (Gadsup, 608, and
Cashinahua, 813) have a voiced series consisting of /*d/ alone. All of
these languages have the velar place represented by the voiceless
35
Stops and affricates
counterpart /k/. There are only 3 languages with /g/ but without /b/. Two
of them also lack /*d/. Thus although many languages have no voiced stops,
among those which do, /g/ is more likely to be "missing" than /b/ or /*d/.
The implicational hierarchy for plain voiced plosives is thus: presence of
Iql implies presence of /*d/, which implies presence of /b/.
There is thus an asymmetrical distribution to be explained. One way of
expressing the asymmetry is to say that the bilabial place is disfavored
among voiceless plosives and the velar place is disfavored among voiced
plosives. But when the ratios of voiced to voiceless plosives at the 3
places are considered, it becomes unclear if this is an appropriate way to
describe the patterns. Among dental/alveolar plosives the voiced/voiceless
ratio is .63; since plosives with this place are relatively common in both
voiced and voiceless series, we may take this as the most typical ratio.
Now, the ratio at the velar place is .62, which is quite comparable. If
voicing was particularly disfavored at the velar place a lower voicing
ratio would be expected. At the bilabial place the ratio is .76,
substantially higher than at either the dental/alveolar or velar places.
That is, there is some factor which raises the number of voiced bilabials,
or some factor which lowers the number of voiceless bilabials, or, quite
possibly, both. Note that velar and dental/alveolar places retain their
positions relative to each other on the two hierarchies, whereas bilabial
place is at the bottom of the voiceless one and at the top of the voiced
one. If we assume that two processes which affect only bilabials are
involved, or, alternatively, that voiceless bilabials have a tendency to
turn into voiced ones, then both hierarchies can be accounted for.
36
Patterns of sounds
suggested that the relatively weaker release burst of /p/ compared to the
other voiceless stops contributes to explaining its (comparative) rarity.
The intra-linguistic phenomenon of a "missing" /g/ can be accounted for
partly by the lesser frequency of use of the velar place in general (in
comparison with dental/alveolar place), and partly by the increased number
of languages with /b/, creating voiced series containing only /b/.
Although processes of the kinds referred to above may explain the
patterns of occurrence, it should also be noted that there are strong
associations between the occurrence of both the "gaps" where /p/ and /g/
are missing and particular areal or genetic groupings of languages. The
majority of the languages with a missing /p/ are from the Afro-Asiatic and
Nilo-Saharan families, which are contiguous in Africa, or from languages of
New Guinea. None of the languages in UPSID from several major language
families (e.g. Indo-European, Ural-Altaic, Sino-Tibetan) lacks /p/. The
languages with missing /g/ are also predominantly only from certain
particular areas: Austro-Asiatic and Austro-Thai languages in South East
Asia and languages from the Americas account for 19 of the 21 cases. So
although it is appropriate to continue the search for the reasons why gaps
appear in these particular places in plosive inventories and to consider
these patterns in evaluating historical reconstructions (Gamkrelidze and
Ivanov 1973; Hopper 1973), the extent to which these are local aberrations
should also be borne in mind. Similarities between these patterns in plosive
inventories and those in glottalic stop systems will be discussed further
in Chapter 7.
As for plosives in other than the major places, uvular plosives are
predominantly voiceless - there are 38 languages with /q/ but only 8 with
/G/ for a voicing ratio of only .21. Labial-velars on the other hand have a
voicing ratio of 1.05 since there is one more language with /gb/ than /£p/,
Iai (422), the only non-African language with labial-velars in the
database, and Temne (109) have /gb/ but not /l<p/, whereas Efik (119) has
/l<p/ alone. In all other cases, /l<p/ and /gB/ co-occur. The voicing ratio
is .76 for palatal plosives and .82 for retroflex plosives, both of which
indicate greater than usual tendency to voicing.
37
Stops and affricates
2.8 Affricates
The most common non-lateral and non-ejective affricates are palato-alveolar
in place and sibilant in nature. The next most frequent are dental or
alveolar sibilant affricates. Frequencies in the database of the major
types are given in Table 2.8.
Dental/alveolar Palato-alveolar
Plain voiceless /*ts/ 95 /tj/ 141
h h
Aspirated voiceless /*ts / 33 /tj / 42
Plain voiced /*dz/ 30 /d^/ 80
38
Patterns of sounds
39
Stops and affricates
Notes
1. This is confirmed both by direct experimentation (e.g. Ohala 1983) and
by modeling the aerodynamic properties of stop production at different
places (e.g Keating 1983). However, if expansion of the supraglottal
cavity during the closure is as great as shown in some production data
(e.g. in Westbury 1983), no differential effects of the place of
articulation would be expected to occur in stops with closure durations
typical of nongeminate plosives.
References
Gamkrelidze, T. and Ivanov, V. 1973. Sprachtypologie und die Rekonstruktion
der gemeinindogermanischen Verschldsse. Phonetica 27: 150-56.
Greenberg, J.H. 1958. The labial-consonants of Proto-Afro-Asiatic. Word 14:
295-302.
Hoffmann, C. 1963. A Grammar of Margi. Oxford University Press for the
International African Institute, London.
Hopper, P. 1973. Glottalized and murmured occlusives in Indo-European.
Glossa 7: 141-66.
Jakobson, R. and Halle, M. 1956. Fundamentals of Language. Mouton, The
Hague.
Keating, P. 1983. Physiological effects on stop consonant voicing. Paper
presented at 105th Meeting of the Acoustical Society of America,
Cincinnati.
Keating, P., Linker, W. and Huffman, M. 1983. Patterns in allophone
distribution for voiced and voiceless stops. Journal of Phonetics 11:
277-90.
Lindblom, B. 1983. Economy of speech gestures. In P.F. McNeilage (ed.), The
Production of Speech. Springer, New York: 217-45.
Lisker, L. and Abramson, A. 1964. A cross-language study of voicing in
initial stops: acoustical measurement. Word 20: 384-422.
Maddieson, I. 1983. The analysis of complex elements in Bura and the
syllable. Studies in African Linguistics 14:285-310.
Newman, P. 1977. Chadic classification and reconstruction (Afro-Asiatic
Linguistics 5.1). Undena, Malibu.
Ohala, J.J. 1983. The origin of sound patterns in vocal tract constraints.
In P.F. McNeilage (ed.), The Production of Speech. Springer, New York:
189-216.
Westbury, J. 1983. Enlargement of the supraglottal cavity and its relation
to stop consonant voicing. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America
73: 1322-36.
40
Fricatives
3.1 Introduction
While it is true that there are fricatives in nearly all of the world's
languages, there have been relatively few studies of their precise
distribution or of the patterns of occurrence which they show. Nonetheless,
many linguists have expressed beliefs about universal tendencies affecting
fricatives: for example, Fromkin and Rodman (1978: 331) say "If a language
has fricatives (most do), it will have an / s / M , and Bright (1978: 39) says:
Statements such as these have been made on the basis of personal experience
rather than on the kind of quantifiable research which is the only secure
foundation for conclusions which depend on frequency of occurrence.
In this chapter, we will examine the fricatives in the inventories of
the languages in UPSID, describe their frequency and patterns of
co-occurrence, and suggest some generalizations which apply. Where
possible, reasons why these generalizations hold will be suggested.
We adopt a conventional definition of fricatives, namely they are those
speech sounds produced by the narrow approximation of two articulators so
as to produce a turbulent airstream (Ladefoged 1971: 4 6 ) . Note that this
definition does not include the majority of sounds represented by the
symbol /h/. Sounds transcribed with /h/ have often been labeled "glottal
fricatives", but as Pike (1943) and others have pointed out, /h/ is
normally a voiceless counterpart of an abutting voiced segment (most often
a vowel). A brief discussion of the distribution of /h/ is given in section
3.9, but it is not included as one of the fricatives in any language in the
remainder of the discussion.
41
Fricatives
42
Patterns of sounds
43
Fricatives
having that number. The mean is a little over 4. Only a relative handful of
languages (6.3%) have more than 8 fricatives. The structure of fricative
systems with different numbers of terms will be examined in section 3.8 •
However, first the overall frequency of fricatives of different types in
the UPSID data file will be discussed.
The most frequent fricative type is a voiceless sibilant made with the
front of the tongue. About 83% of all the languages have some kind of
"s-sound". This may be dental or alveolar in its place of articulation, but
for many purposes it is convenient to collapse this distinction. This
decision seems justified not only because of the obvious phonetic
similarity of dental and alveolar sibilants, but also because of the great
rarity of contrasts between /s/ and /s/. There are 262 languages with an
s-sound, but only 4 of these (Tzeltal 712, Karok 741, Dieguefto 743, and
Guarani 828) have both I si and /s/. It may also be noted that there are no
languages in the survey with both /z/ and /z/. In the remainder of this
chapter the following notation will be used: I si represents a voiceless
dental sibilant fricative, / s/ represents a voiceless alveolar sibilant
fricative, /"s"/ represents a sibilant with an unspecified dental or
alveolar place, and /*s/ will be used to refer to all types of s-sounds
together. The same conventions will be used to talk about other groups of
fricatives where similar distinctions need to be made.
Apart from the 4 languages mentioned above which have both I si and /si,
29 languages are reliably reported to have dental I si and 98 to have an
alveolar /s/. However, there are 131 languages with an s-sound which is not
specifically identified as dental or alveolar. In all probability, alveolar
sibilants are more common than dental ones, but this cannot be definitely
determined from our sample, as there are so many cases in which specific
information on place of articulation is missing from our source.
We may therefore state that /*s/ is the most common fricative, with Is I
probably being the more common member of the group l*sl • The number of
languages with /*s/ is 88.5% of those languages with fricatives. So Fromkin
and Rodman's statement that languages with fricatives have an s-sound has
relatively few exceptions. Bright"s assertion that it is natural for
languages to have an alveolar sibilant appears to be rather less
well-founded, unless it too is construed to refer to the class /*s/ rather
than strictly to the alveolar /s/•
The two next most frequent fricatives are the voiceless palato-alveolar
sibilant /J / with 146 cases, and the voiceless labio-dental fricative If I
44
Patterns of sounds
45
Fricatives
whether a given voiced fricative is more common than another can be largely
predicted from knowing which of the two voiceless counterparts is more
common, and vice versa. However, certain places of articulation favor
presence or absence of voicing more strongly than the general run of
fricatives. Most saliently, bilabial and dental nonsibilant fricatives
favor voicing, to the extent that there are more instances of / p/ than / <£>/
and more of / 5/ than /9/. It should also be noted that pharyngeals are more
than usually likely to be voiced, and that retroflex sibilant and lateral
fricatives (in the dental/alveolar area) are more than usually likely to be
voiceless.
It seems to be the case that / p/ and / 5/ in many languages are of
relatively recent origin and this may be related to their unexpectedly
frequent occurrence. They derive historically from 1 axing or weakening of
voiced plosives, as in Spanish (Oil), Atayal (407), and probably also in
languages such as Kaliai (421), Gadsup (608) and Diegueilo (743); and in a
number of cases result from loan phonology, as in Quechua (819) and
Mongolian (066). Compare Ferguson's findings in his study of the process
/d/ —> /5/. He concludes that this is "a highly context-sensitive
assimilatory process which is typically part of a larger schema of
spirantization (of voiced stops or all stops) which is relatively easily
diffused across languages" (Ferguson 1978: 437). Since the voiceless
counterparts are less common, such processes apparently either generate
fewer instances of the voiceless counterparts /<£/ and / 8 / , or once
generated, these sounds are rapidly transformed into something else. It is
tempting to hypothesize that /<£/ and / 9/ are changed into the very common
voiceless fricatives /f/ and /*s/. If this hypothesis is correct, we might
expect that fewer languages would have both members of the voiceless pairs
/$, f/ and /9, * s/ than the voiced pairs /p, v/ and /ft, * z/. However, there
are more languages with the voiceless pairs than the voiced ones. There are
13 languages with / 9, * s/ and 6 with /ft, * z/ of which 5 also have / 9, * s/.
With the "labial" pairs there is also a predominance of the voiceless pair,
although there are only 4 languages with both / <f>, f /. However, there are no
languages in UPSID with both /p, v/. The correct generalization to make in
this case may therefore be from a different perspective, namely, that the
absence of competing voiced fricatives may be an important factor in
facilitating the adoption of /p/ (and, less clearly /ft/) into the inventory
of a language. Gamkrelidze (1978) relates spirantization processes of this
type to the gaps created in the stop systems, but does not discuss possible
46
Patterns of sounds
47
Fricatives
48
Patterns of sounds
He defines /f/ to include both /$/ and /f/, and its "voiced counterpart" is
/w - v/ which includes /w/, /v/ and /|3/. The velar fricatives are
apparently conventionally defined. He claims that languages with /y/ but
not /x/ are "rare exceptions" (p. 30). Our finding is that over a third of
the languages with /y/ lack /x/. There are 75 languages in UPSID with /x/
or 23.7% of the languages. There are 40 languages with /y/ or 12.6%. The
percentage of languages in which these segments would co-occur if their
distribution were random is thus 3% or about 10 languages, instead of the
25 languages in which they are found together. We may therefore correctly
argue that presence of /y/ generally implies presence of /x/, although this
implication is not as strong as the implication holding between certain
other voiced fricatives and their voiceless counterparts.
On the other hand, there are 139 languages or 43.8% with /f/ or /<£/ or
both, and 288 languages with /w/ or /v/ or /|3/ or a bilabial or
labio-dental approximant, which Gamkrelidze would presumably also include
in his /w - v/ phoneme, meaning 90.9% of the languages have a
representative of his /w - v/ phoneme. Thus the percentage of languages
which do not have /w - v/ is only 9.1%. Hence only 4.0% (9.1% of 43.8%) of
the languages (some 12 or 13) would be expected to have his /f/ but not his
/w - v/ if these phonemes were randomly distributed. There are in fact 10
languages in UPSID, about 3.2%, which have his /f/ but not his /w - v/ as
construed here. Since this is close to the expected number, we must
conclude that presence of /f/ does not significantly affect the occurrence
of / w - v/ .
49
Fricatives
5 X X
6 f $
7 0 0
8 Q
50
Patterns of sounds
51
Fricatives
were then played to groups of listeners who were native speakers of the
same 4 languages. The listeners were given a brief training in the
transcriptional conventions they were expected to use in recording their
responses. Singh and Black do not report if subjects had to reach any
criterion as either speakers or listeners before they were included in the
study, and the design of their experiment confounds the influence of
language-specific variation in the production of segments designated by the
same phonetic symbol with language-dependent listener biases and with
inherent salience.
As yet it seems the experimental work has not been done which can
provide a sounder basis for forming a ranking of fricatives by salience.
The few indications from this literature are suggestive of possible
correlation of fricative salience with overall frequency in the world's
languages.
52
Patterns of sounds
53
Fricatives
54
Patterns of sounds
55
Fricatives
56
Patterns of sounds
3.10 Conclusion
This chapter has presented data on the occurrence of fricatives in the
languages in the UPSID sample. It has shown that patterns of frequency bear
some relationship to what is known about relative intensity and salience of
fricatives. It has documented more extensively some of the relationships
between certain types of fricatives, such as the general tendency for
voiced fricatives to occur only if the voiceless counterpart also is
present. However, important limitations on this relationship apply when
57
Fricatives
References
Bright, W. 1978. Sibilants and naturalness in aboriginal California.
Journal of California Anthropology, Papers in Linguistics 1: 39-63.
Carterette, E.C. and Jones, M.H. 1974. Informal Speech: Alphabetic and
Phonemic Texts with Statistical Analyses and Tables. University of
California Press, Berkeley.
Dixon, R.M.W. 1980. The Languages of Australia. Cambridge University Press,
Cambridge.
Ferguson, C.A. 1978. Phonological processes. In J.H. Greenberg et al.
(eds.), Universals of Human Language, Vol. 2_9 Phonology. Stanford
University Press, Stanford: 403-42.
Fromkin, V.A. and Rodman, R. 1978. An Introduction to Language, 2nd ed.
Holt, Rinehart and Winston, New York.
Gamkrelidze, T.V. 1978. On the correlation of stops and fricatives in a
phonological system. In J.H. Greenberg et al. (eds.), Universals of
Human Language, Vol. 2, Phonology. Stanford University Press, Stanford:
9-46\
Goldstein, L. 1977. Three Studies in Speech Perception: Features, Relative
Salience and Bias (UCLA Working Papers in Phonetics 39). University of
California, Los Angeles.
Hale, K.L. 1976. Phonological developments in Northern Paman languages. In
P. Sutton (ed.), Languages of Cape York. Australian Institute of
Aboriginal Studies, Canberra: 7-49.
Hockett, C.F. 1955. A Manual of Phonology (IJAL Monographs 11). Indiana
University, Bloomington.
Ladefoged, P. 1971. Preliminaries to Linguistic Phonetics. University of
Chicago Press, Chicago.
Li, F-K. 1965. The Tai and the Kam-Sui languages. Lingua 14: 148-79.
Merlingen, W. 1977. Artikulation und Phonematik des H. Verband der
wissenschaftlichen Gesellschaften Osterreichs, Vienna.
Pike, K.L. 1943. Phonetics. University of Michigan Press, Ann Arbor.
Roberts, A.H. 1965. A. Statistical Linguistic Analysis o£ American English.
Mouton, The Hague.
Singh, S. and Black, J.W. 1966. A study of twenty-six intervocalic
consonants as spoken and recognized by four language groups. Journal of
the Acoustical Society o£ America 39: 372-87.
Strevens, P. 1960. Spectra of fricative noise in human speech. Language and
Speech 3: 32-49.
Wang, M.D. and Bilger, R.C. 1973. Consonant confusions in noise: a study of
perceptual features. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 54:
1248-66.
58
Nasals
4.1 Introduction
Unlike most of the types of segments which are the topics of chapters in
this volume, nasals have been the subject of a good earlier survey. In
fact, the study by Ferguson (1963) on nasals has served in many ways as the
model of an article on universals of segment types. Ferguson's article
provided a major part of the stimulus for the organization of a conference
devoted to nasals and nasalization (Ferguson, Hyman and Ohala 1975). For
this reason, this chapter will largely take the form of a discussion of the
various "assumptions about nasals" put forward by Ferguson, checking them
against the data in UPSID to provide the quantification which is lacking in
Ferguson's article and is only partially provided by Crothers (1975)
working from an early version of the Stanford Phonology Archive. However,
before this discussion, some summary information on the types of nasal
consonants included in the UPSID data file will be presented.
59
Nasals
Evidently, nasals in the dental/alveolar area are the most common, but
bilabials are also very frequent. Although velar nasals are far from rare,
they are much less frequent than velar stops: for example there are 283
UPSID languages with / k/ but only 167 with /Q/. On the other hand, palatal
nasals are more common than might be expected from comparison with stop
frequencies: there are 107 languages with /p/ but only 41 with /c/. Note
that no example of a phonemic uvular nasal is reported from the UPSID
languages (38 languages have / q / ) . Of course, pharyngeal and glottal places
of articulation are ruled out for nasals because of articulatory
constraints.
Among the relatively small number of nasals that are not simple plain
voiced ones, there seem to be three reliable patterns. The first is that
there is a preferential association between bilabial place and
voicelessness: of the 36 voiceless nasals, 11 are bilabial. This is a
greater proportion than is found at other places of articulation. For
example, compare the ratio of languages with voiceless nasals to those with
plain voiced nasals at the bilabial place, 11/299 (.037), with the
comparable ratio among combined dental/alveolar nasals, 8/305 (.026). The
other two observations relate to secondary articulations: palatalization is
associated more with bilabial place of articulation than with other places,
whereas labialization is associated with velar place of articulation. Of 10
voiced nasals with palatalization, 6 are bilabial; of 10 voiced nasals with
labialization, 7 are velar. In proportion to the number of plain voiced
nasals these are far more frequent than either secondary articulation is
60
Patterns of sounds
with dental and alveolar nasals. The ratio of palatalized bilabial nasals
to plain voiced bilabials is .020, whereas the ratio of palatalized
dental/alveolar nasals to plain dental/alveolar nasals is .003. The ratio
of labialized velar nasals to plain velar nasals is .042, but the ratio of
labialized dental/alveolar nasals to plain dental/alveolar nasals is only
.003. Labialization is also most commonly combined with velar place of
articulation in other segment types, such as stops and fricatives, but
palatalization is not especially associated with bilabial place in other
segment types.
61
Nasals
62
Patterns of sounds
63
Nasals
nasal, and Irish (001) has no bilabial PNC but has both.palatal and velar
nasals. However, this language is perhaps best regarded as a language with
4 PNC's, including /m/, for the reasons outlined above.
In the spirit of Ferguson's observations, we may add that a language
with 4 PNC's has a palatal nasal. In a very large majority of cases, 75 of
83 or about 90%, the inventory of nasals in a language with 4 PNC's
consists of /m, *n, *p, Q/ (palato-alveolar and "pre-palatal" nasals are
treated as members of the same class as /p/ and the class is designated by
/ * p / ) . The only reasonably common alternative is /m, *n, n, Q/, with a
retroflex in place of the palatal. Five languages have this pattern.
Of the 14 languages with 5 PNC's, all have /m, Q/. Five contrast dental
and alveolar nasals, but there is no common overall pattern, with the fifth
nasal being either retroflex (3 cases) or palatal (2 cases). Four
languages, all Niger-Kordofanian, have labial-velar nasals as part of the
set /m, *n, p, Q, mo/. Four languages have /m, *n, n, *p, Q/, with a
retroflex nasal. Teke (127) is reported with a contrast of bilabial and
labiodental nasals, /m, m/, as well as /n, p, Q/. It is the only language
with a labiodental nasal in the UPSID file.
The languages with 6 PNC's are essentially uniform, having /m, n, n, n,
p, Q/ as their system, apart from Iai (422) which has /mr)/ but no contrast
between dental and alveolar. It is noteworthy that 11 of the 13 languages
in the sample with both dental and alveolar nasals are languages with 5 or
6 PNC's and that 10 of these are Australian languages. It is valid to
observe that only languages with a large number of nasals contrast dental
and alveolar places, but also important to note the genetic bias in the
distribution. Australian languages typically have more nasals than
languages of other families; of the 19 Australian languages in UPSID, 11
have 5 or 6 nasals. Less than 3% of the languages in UPSID have more than 4
nasals but the percentage of Australian languages with that many is 57.9%.
64
Patterns of sounds
65
Nasals
language with two such series thus usually has more SNC's than PNC's. The
UPSID examples are Sedang (304), Sui (403), Klamath (707), and Otomi (716).
These languages have both voiceless and laryngealized series of nasals
which together add up to more than the number of PNC's. !Xu (918) has
laryngealized and breathy voiced bilabial nasals, but has plain voiced
nasals at bilabial, alveolar and velar places. No language in UPSID has
more nasals in any one series with an unusual phonation type than it has
plain voiced nasals.
Long nasals
Similar conditions apply to distinctively long nasals as apply to those
with secondary articulations. In the 13 languages with long nasals, a
matching short nasal occurs with each long nasal, except for the case of
/Q:/ in Finnish (053), discussed in section 4.3 above. There are, however,
3 languages where one or more long nasals occur but matching long
consonants of other types are lacking. These are Chuvash (060), Ocaina
(805) and !Xu (918). (Chuvash does have distinctively long approximants but
66
Patterns of sounds
has no long bilabial consonants apart from /mi/.) Despite the exceptions,
there is a very significant association between length in the nasal system
and length in other consonant subinventories. For example, there are 11
languages with /m:/ and 9 languages with either /p:/ or /b:/ or both. In
these two small groups, 8 languages are the same, i.e. have both /m:/ and a
long bilabial plosive. The probability that segments of this type would
co-occur if they were randomly distributed is about .0001. Applying this to
our sample means that we would expect about one language in 900 to have
this co-occurrence, instead of the observed frequency of about 1 in 40.
Because long consonants of all types are fairly rare in occurrence, it
is not really possible to assign a direction to the association with long
nasals. What we can observe is simply that the sort of phonetic and
distributional facts which argue for interpretation of length as a property
of unitary phonemes tend to be common to several types of segments in any
relevant language.
For each of the types of SNC's considered above, the fact that
languages have an equal or larger number of PNC's than they have in any one
series of SNC's follows from the condition that a matching simple voiced
nasal at the same place of articulation is presupposed by the presence of
any SNC.
Prenasalized obstruents
There are 19 languages in UPSID with prenasalized segments. Such segments
are all voiced obstruents. There are 66 prenasalized plosives, 7
affricates, and 3 fricatives. (There is one language, Aranda (362), with a
series of nasally-released stops.) The languages with prenasalized
obstruents would provide rather a large percentage of exceptions to the
claim that languages do not have more SNC's than PNC's, since 10 of them
have more prenasalized obstruents than PNC's. However, it seems more
appropriate to relate prenasalized obstruents to the other obstruent series
of the language, rather than considering them in relation to the nasals.
There are several reasons for this. First, there are languages such as
Hakka (502), Apinaye (809) and Siriono (829) which have no PNC's but do
have a prenasalized plosive series. These 3 languages also lack a plain
voiced plosive series; in a sense the prenasalized stops take its place.
Secondly, there are languages which have both a series of PNC's and a
prenasalized stop series in place of a simple voiced series of stops.
Besides lacking a simple voiced obstruent series, such languages may have
67
Nasals
Nasalized clicks
Ferguson included nasalized clicks among the secondary nasal consonants. A
nasal escape of air can accompany any click production, and the nasal
component can be either voiced or voiceless. Nasalized clicks are found as
part of a set of click series and do not appear to be related to the number
68
Patterns of sounds
69
Nasals
70
Patterns of sounds
References
Austerlitz, R. 1967. The distributional identification of Finnish
morphophonemes. Language 43: 20-33.
Bentick, J. 1975. Le niaboua, langue sans consonnes nasales. Annales de
l'Universite d'Abidjan, Serie H,Linguistique 8: 5-14.
Crothers, J. 1975. Nasal consonant systems. In C.A. Ferguson, L.M. Hyman,
and J.J. Hyman (eds.), Nasalfest. Stanford University, Stanford: 153-66.
Ferguson, C.A. 1963. Assumptions about nasals: a sample study in
phonological universals. In J.H. Greenberg (ed.) Universals of Language.
M.I.T. Press, Cambridge: 53-60.
71
Nasals
Ferguson, C.A., Hyman, L.M. and Ohala, J.J. (eds.) 1975. Nasalfest (Papers
from a symposium on nasals and nasalization). Stanford University,
Stanford.
Goldstein, L. 1977. Bias and asymmetry in speech perception. UCLA Working
Papers in Phonetics 39: 62-87.
Greenberg, J.H. 1978. Some generalizations concerning initial and final
consonant clusters. In J.H. Greenberg et al. (eds.), Universals of Human
Language, Vol. 2, Phonology. Stanford University Press, Stanford:
243-79.
Hockett, C.F. 1955. A Manual of Phonology (IJAL Monographs 11). Indiana
University, Bloomington.
House, A.S. 1957. Analog studies of nasal consonants. Journal of Speech and
Hearing Disorders 22: 190-204.
Le Saoiit, J. 1973. Langues sans consonnes nasales. Annales de l'Universite
d'Abidjan, Serie H,Linguistique 6: 179-205.
Malecot, A. 1956. Acoustic cues for nasal consonants: an experimental study
involving tape-splicing techniques. Language 32: 274-84.
Miller, G. and Nicely, P. 1955. An analysis of perceptual confusions among
English consonants. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 27:
338-52.
Nord, L. 1976. Perceptual experiments with nasals. Speech Transmission
Laboratory, Quarterly Status and Progress Report (Royal Institute of
Technology, Stockholm) 2-3: 5-8.
Ohala, J.J. 1975. Phonetic explanations for nasal sound patterns. In C.A.
Ferguson, L.M. Hyman, and J.J. Hyman (eds.), Nasalfest. Stanford
University, Stanford: 289-316.
Shepard, R.N. 1972. Psychological representation of speech sounds. In E.E.
David and P.B. Denes (eds.) Human Communication: A Unified View.
McGraw-Hill, New York: 67-1 lY.
Singh, S. and Black, J.W. 1966. A study of twenty-six intervocalic
consonants as spoken and recognized by four language groups. Journal of
the Acoustical Society of America 39: 372-87.
Thompson, L.C. and Thompson, M.T. 1972. Language universals, nasals and the
Northwest Coast. In M.E. Smith (ed.), Studies in Linguistics in Honor of
George Trager. Mouton, The Hague: 441-56.
Zee, E. 1981. Effect of vowel quality on perception of post-vocalic nasal
consonants in noise. Journal of Phonetics 9: 35-48.
72
Liquids
5.1 Introduction
For reasons to do with both acoustic similarities and common phonological
patterning, laterals and r-sounds have been grouped together as "liquids"
in phonetic tradition. Although the similarities involve principally the
voiced non-fricative segments concerned (Goschel 1972; Bhat 1974), the term
liquid in this paper will be applied to all lateral segments except lateral
clicks and to all sounds that are included in the somewhat heterogeneous
class of r-sounds. The core membership of this latter class consists of
apical and uvular trills, taps and flaps.1 Added to this core are a
variety of fricative and approximant sounds which seem acoustically or
articulatorily similar, or which are related by diachronic processes
(Lindau-Webb 1980).
73
Liquids
Table 5.1 Number of liquids in UPSID languages
5.3 Laterals
Types of laterals
The laterals which occur may be grouped under four broad headings: lateral
approximants, taps/flaps, fricatives, and affricates. The occurrence of
these types is summarized in Tables 5.2 - 5.5 where frequencies are
expressed in terms of the percentage of the total number of laterals
counted in the survey (418). Approximant lateral types are shown in Table
5.2. Plain voiced approximant laterals are by far the most common type of
lateral. Other types of approximant laterals are rare and only occur in
inventories in which a plain type appears.
Number Percent
of laterals
Plain voiced 313 74.7%
Plain voiceless 11 2.6%
Laryngealized voiced 8 1.9%
Breathy voiced 1 0.2%
333 79.7%
74
Patterns of sounds
Number Percent
of laterals
10 2.4%
The few flaps reported are all voiced. Lateral fricatives, on the other
hand, are far more likely to be voiceless than voiced, as Table 5.4 shows.
Number Percent
of laterals
Plain voiceless 34 8.1%
Plain voiced 9 2.2%
Ejective voiceless 2 0.5%
45 10.8%
75
Liquids
Number Percent
of laterals
Ejective voiceless 14 J • J/o
29 6.9%
76
Patterns of sounds
77
Liquids
5.4 R-sounds
Types of r-sounds
The sources used to compile UPSID fail to specify the manner of
articulation of segments represented by /r/ in 34 instances or 10.8% of the
316 r-sounds. These will be droppped from consideration in the analysis of
the types of r-sounds below. It should be remembered that ignorance of how
this group of sounds should be distributed into the various classes below
adds a measure of uncertainty to some of the conclusions reached in this
section.
78
Patterns of sounds
Number Percent
of r-sounds
Plain voiced 130 46.1%
Plain voiceless 3 1.1%
Laryngealized voiced 1 0.4%
134 47.5%
Obviously, trills are overwhelmingly voiced. The same is true of the next
most frequent class of r-sound, consisting of taps and flaps. All reported
taps/flaps are voiced, with only 3 being other than plain. The numbers are
given in Table 5.8. Although fewer taps/flaps are reported than trills, the
difference is less than the number of r-sounds with unspecified manner and
hence it is not possible to conclude which of these types of r-sounds is
actually most common in the languages of the world. In any case, trills and
taps/flaps are closely related sound types (often both appear as allophones
of the same phoneme) both of which involve an interruption of the flow of
air through the oral cavity. It may be observed that about 86% of those
r-sounds with specified manner are "interrupted".3
Number Percent
of r-sounds
Plain voiced 104 36.9%
Plain voiceless 2 0.7%
Laryngealized voiced 1 0.4%
Voiced fricated 1 0.4%
108 38.3%
79
Liquids
Number Percent
of r-sounds
Voiced approximant 28 9.9%
Voiced fricative 8 2.8%
Voiceless fricative 2 0.7%
38 13.5%
80
Patterns of sounds
Alveolar Retroflex
Number Percent Number Percent
Interrupted: trills 62 44.9% 5 13.9%
taps/flaps 62 44.9% 11 30.6%
Continuant: approximants 11 8.0% 15 41.7%
fricatives 3 2.2% 5 13.9%
81
Liquids
82
Patterns of sounds
laterals than r-sounds, whereas only 23 have more r-sounds than laterals.
The remainder have equal numbers of the 2 major types of liquids.
No. of laterals 0 1 2 3 4 5 6
No. of languages 58 157 63 23 9 3 3
Percent 18.3% 49.5% 19.9% 7.3% 2.8% 0.9% 0.9%
No. of r-sounds 0 1 2 3 4
No. of languages 74 183 51 8 1
Percent 23.3% 57.7% 16.1% 2.5% 0.3%
83
Liquids
84
Patterns of sounds
85
Liquids
On the other hand, Ngizim (269), one of the languages with 3 laterals and 2
r-sounds, and all the languages with 4 or 5 laterals in a 5-liquid system
contrast their laterals by voicing and manner differences.
86
Patterns of sounds
Number of Percent of
languages languages in
sample
4 laterals,
2 r-sounds 3 0.9%
3 laterals,
3 r-sounds 2 0.6%
5 laterals,
1 r-sound 1 0.3%
6 laterals 2 0.6%
4 laterals,
3 r-sounds 2 0.6%
6 laterals
4 r-sounds 1 0.3%
Just as with the systems with 5 liquids, the laterals tend either to differ
by place (Diyari, 367, Aranda, 362) or by manner and voicing (Sedang, 304,
Chipewyan, 703, Haida, 700, Kwakw'ala, 731). There is however, one
language, Diegueflo (743), which includes intersecting contrasts of both
place and voicing/manner, having the 4 laterals / I , I, 4,47. The largest
number of laterals reported in the survey is 6. The 7-liquid languages are
2 Australian languages (Kariera-Ngarluma, 363, Arabana-Wanganura, 366) which
contrast laterals at 4 different places of articulation, trills at 2 places
and also have a retroflex approximant. Irish (001) is the language with 10
liquids, having voiceless counterparts to its voiced alveolar liquids that
have morphophonologically specialized functions. Because of its voiceless
r-sounds Irish is the only language in the sample with 4 r-sounds.
87
Liquids
(vii) A language most often has two liquids (usually one lateral and one
r-sound). 109/317 34.4%.
Although approximant laterals are the most common type of lateral, the
probability of (iii) being true is significantly higher than their overall
frequency as a percentage of all laterals (79.7%) would suggest. The two
observations (iv) and (v) draw attention to a quite marked difference
between laterals and r-sounds in the way that the systems are elaborated.
The data is incomplete on point (vi) so no percentage is expressed.
However, only two counterexamples are known from among the languages in the
survey.
5.7 Conclusion
The survey of liquids in UPSID has revealed patterns of occurrence of
different types of liquids which may be taken as reliable. These patterns
concern both the overall frequency of particular sound types and their
relation to the inventory in which they occur. Although such observations
have an intrinsic interest of their own, their main value is to suggest
avenues of investigation in diachronic phonology, articulatory phonetics or
speech perception designed to seek the explanation for these patterns.
Research on liquids in these fields seems to be a neglected area.
Notes
1. Provision to distinguish between taps and flaps was made in the
variables employed in UPSID, but the sources used do not seem to
distinguish them reliably. They have therefore been treated here as a
single group. For more discussion of the use of these terms see
Ladefoged (1971) and Elugbe (1978).
2. Ladefoged, Cochran and Disner (1977) claim that "very few languages
have any trills at all." The data collected for UPSID suggest either
that trills are not in fact particularly rare or that very many
erroneous reports of trills occur in the literature.
3. Including voiceless and voiced "trilled retroflex affricates" reported
in Malagasy (410) but not included in the totals given in Tables 5.7
and 5.8.
4. There may be some reporting bias reflected in this finding. A somewhat
retracted articulation of approximant /r/ is labelled "retroflex" in
some analyses of English (e.g. Kenyon 1926). This may have led to a
predisposition to label any approximant "r" as retroflex among
English-speaking linguists.
5. If the one instance of a velar lateral and the 4 instances of laterals
at 4 places of articulation in UPSID are taken as indications of the
frequency of such occurrences, then the probability that both would
occur in the same language might be estimated at less than .00004 (i.e.
in fewer than one language in 400,000).
89
Liquids
References
Bhat, D.N.S. 1974. The phonology of liquid consonants. Working Papers in
Language Universals (Stanford University) 16: 73-104.
Bladon, R.A.W. 1979. The production of laterals: some acoustic properties
and their physiological implications. In H. and P. Hollien (eds.),
Current Issues in the Phonetic Sciences (Amsterdam Studies in the Theory
and History of Linguistic Science, Series 4, Current Issues in
Linguistic Theory 9 ) . John Benjamins, Amsterdam: 501-8.
Davey, A., Maddieson, I. and Moshi, L. 1982. Liquids in Chaga. UCLA Working
Papers in Phonetics 54: 93-108.
Elugbe, B.O. 1978. On the wider application of the term "tap". Journal o£_
Phonetics 6: 133-9.
Goldstein, L. 1977. Perceptual salience of stressed syllables. UCLA Working
Papers ^Ln Phonetics 39: 37-60.
Goschel, J. 1972. Artikulation und Distribution der sogennanten Liquida r
in den europMischen Sprachen. Indogermanische Forschungen 76: 84-126.
Guitarte, G.L. 1971. Notas para la historia del yeismo. Sprache und
Geschichte: Festschrift fUr Harri Meier zum 65 Geburtstag. Fink, Munich:
179-98.
Kenyon, J.S. 1926. Some notes on American r. American Speech 1: 329-39.
Ladefoged, P. 1971. Preliminaries to Linguistic Phonetics. University of
Chicago Press, Chicago.
Ladefoged, P., Cochran, A. and Disner, S. 1977. Laterals and trills.
Journal of the International Phonetic Association 7: 46-54.
Lindau-Webb, M. 1980. The story of r. UCLA Working Papers in Phonetics 51:
114-19.
Mase, H. and Rischel, J. 1971. A study of consonant quantity in West
Greenlandic. Annual Report of the Institute of Phonetics, University of
Copenhagen 5: 175-247.
90
Vocoid approximants
6.1 Introduction
Approximants are consonantal sounds produced with a relatively unimpeded
flow of air through the mouth. The constriction is not narrow enough to
produce local turbulence, though cavity friction may be heard if the
segment is voiceless (Catford 1977). Apart from those approximants which
have a lateral escape or belong to the family of r-sounds, the only
frequently-occurring approximants in the world's languages are those which
have vocoid characteristics (Pike 1943). They are often known as
"semi-vowels". This chapter examines the frequency of such sounds as
phonemic units in the UPSID sample and discusses certain co-occurrence
restrictions which relate to their role in phoneme inventories.
In the UPSID file, vocoid approximants have been coded as consonants if
they don't alternate with syllabic vocoid pronunciations and share
distributional properties with other consonants. Over 90% of the surveyed
languages have one or more such segments.
91
Vocoid approximants
In the UPSID data, therefore, the occurrence of /w/ usually implies the
occurrence of /J/ in the same language. However, the association of /w/ and
/ j / is not as strong in UPSID as that found by Stephens and Justeson (1979)
in the materials collected for the Stanford Phonology Archive. In these
materials /w/ occurs without / j / in only 1% of the languages surveyed, as
against over 4% in the UPSID languages. Stephens and Justeson also report
substantially lower overall frequencies of both /w/ and /j /, with the
percentage of languages having these segments about 15% lower in each case.
It is not clear if these differences arise from the different selection of
languages in the two surveys or from the application of different criteria
for phonemic status of approximants. Nonetheless their claim that there is
a statistically significant tendency for /w / to occur only if / j / also
occurs is confirmed by our data (significance from ^ 2 better than .001).
Other vocoid approximants are comparatively rare. They may be divided
into two groups - those which are modified variants of / j / and /y^/ and
those which have different places of articulation. Those in the second
group include the labial-palatal approximant /q / (4 instances) and the
velar approximant /y / (5 instances). These occur in less than 2% of the
languages surveyed.They are not found in modified form in any of the UPSID
languages. Palatal approximants occur voiceless, laryngealized and
nasalized. Labial-velar approximants occur voiceless and laryngealized. The
frequency of modified segments of these types is given in Table 6.2.
Laryngealized approximants / j / and /w/ occur with approximately equal
frequency and are restricted to languages which have other glottalic or
glottalized segments in their inventories and have plain voiced / j / or /w/.
Greenberg (1970) suggested that / j / fills the place of an anticipated
palatal implosive in languages with an implosive series and a palatal place
of articulation. This issue is discussed in some detail in Chapter 7 where
it is concluded that there does not seem to be support for it in the
available data. A diachronic source of this kind for /]/ would predict that
it would be more frequent than /w/, for which no parallel source is
92
Patterns of sounds
/j/ 13 4.1%
IV 3 0.9%
/AA/ 11 3.5%
/w/ 12 3.8%
93
Vocoid approximants
There are about 3 times as many cases of /w/ occurring without /u/ as of
/j/ occurring without /i/. Disner in Chapter 9 suggests that the systems
without /u/ may be regarded as falling into two principal classes: those
with a "compensating" vowel which is high or back or rounded but not all
three (such as /t/, /w/, /«/ etc) and those which simply have a gap (and
whose highest back vowel is usually / o / ) . This is suggestive of a variety
of possible sources for /w/ and may predict that the class of /w/ segments
in languages may vary phonetically through a greater range than / j / .
The less frequently occurring approximants /q / and /y / were also
investigated in relation to the corresponding vowels, in this case /y / and
/w/ respectively. The numbers are given in Table 6.4. These suggest that
/ M/ is most likely to occur if /y/ also occurs in the inventory, but that
there is no such dependence of /y/ on the occurrence of An/. However since
these numbers are so small no great reliance should be placed on these
indications.
94
Patterns of sounds
No. of languages
/M/ and hi 3
hi but no /y/ 1
hi and /LU/ 1
hi but no / UJ / 4
with / j / (cf. Bhat 1978). There are, however, 3 languages in the survey
As /w/ has two strictures of equal rank it falls into a class with other
labial-velar consonants, especially /£p/ and /gB/ which are the most common
labial-velar consonants after /w/. These labial-velar stops may vary a good
95
Vocoid approximants
96
Patterns of sounds
6.6 Summary
Most languages have / j'/ and /w/, with / j / being more frequent. There is a
strong tendency for the presence of /w/ to imply the presence of / j / in the
same language. The greater frequency of / j / is parallel to the greater
frequency of / i/ than /u/, but these facts are not directly related since
/ j7 may occur without / i/ and /w/ without / u/. Modified varieties of / j/
and / w/ only occur in languages with the plain voiced counterparts. There
is some association between the occurrence of palatalized consonants and
/ j7 and between labial-velar stops and labialized velars (and other
labialized consonants) and /w/.
Notes
1. This word now frequently receives a spelling pronunciation, with
initial / w/ or even /M/ supplanting the historically derived /h/.
References
Bhat, D.N.S. 1978. A general study of palatalization. In J.H. Greenberg et
al. (eds.), Universals of Human Language, Vol. 2, Phonology. Stanford
University Press, Stanford: 47-92.
Catford, J.C. 1977. Fundamental Issues in Phonetics. Indiana University
Press, Bloomington.
Greenberg, J.H. 1970. Some generalizations concerning glottalic consonants,
especially implosives. International Journal of American Linguistics 36:
123-45.
Huld, M. 1980. Tone in Proto-Athabaskan. Unpublished paper. Dept. of
Classics, UCLA.
Ladefoged, P. 1968. A Phonetic Study of West African Languages. Cambridge
University Press, London.
Li, F-K. 1977. A Handbook of Comparative Tai. University of Hawaii Press,
Honolulu.
Ohala, J.J. and Lorentz, J. 1977. The story of [w]: an exercise in the
phonetic explanation for sound patterns. Proceedings of the Third Annual
Meeting, Berkeley Linguistic Society: 577-99.
Pike, K.L. 1943. Phonetics. University of Michigan Press, Ann Arbor.
Stephens, L.D. and Justeson, J.S. 1979. Some generalizations concerning
glides. In D.I. Malsch et al. (eds.), Proceedings of the Eighth Annual
Meeting of the Western Conference on Linguistics. Linguistic Research
Inc, Carbondale and Edmonton: 1 M - 6 4 .
97
Glottalic and laryngealized consonants
7•1 Introduction
This chapter presents the results of a survey of the occurrence of
glottalic consonants and other "glottalized" consonant segments in our
sample of the world's languages, and relates their occurrence to the rest
of the segments in the phonological inventories of the languages. It draws
substantially on a previous survey of the same data by Fordyce (1980).
In an important earlier study, Greenberg (1970) discussed the
distribution of glottalic consonants cross-linguistically and
language-internally. Although there has been considerable subsequent work
addressing Greenberg's claims, much of it either suggests specific
counter-examples (e.g. Campbell 1973; Pinkerton 1980) or brings little
additional data to bear on the general validity of his conclusions (e.g.
Hamp 1970; Javkin 1977). Greenberg's main claims are summarized in the
sentence "injectives [i.e. implosives] tend to have front articulation,
ejectives to have back articulation". Greenberg acknowledges that these
conclusions were partly anticipated by Haudricourt (1950) and independently
discovered by Wang (1968).
Our goal is to determine whether these place of articulation preference
hierarchies for implosives and ejectives can be substantiated and to
discover other distributional patterns relating to glottalic and
glottalized segments in the UPSID data. The possible phonetic motivations
for the patterns found will also be discussed. Greenberg's generalizations
were based on a survey of languages for which data was available rather
than on a carefully structured sample and it is possible that his findings
reflect accidental biases in the selection of languages examined. In using
the UPSID sample we hope to avoid the likelihood of such a bias.
98
Patterns of sounds
99
Glottalic and laryngealized consonants
100
Patterns of sounds
language has the most complex consonantal system of.any of the languages in
UPSID, including six series of stops apart from 48 click consonants. One of
the stop series is described as consisting of voiced ejectives by Snyman
(1969), but his description sounds as if there is necessarily a phonetic
sequence of voicing preceding ejection in these segments. He says:
7.3 Ejectives
Of the 317 languages in UPSID, 52 contain ejectives, making ejectives the
most common of the glottalic or laryngealized segments. Twelve of these
languages also exhibit implosive stops and 15 also exhibit some
laryngealized stops, fricatives, sonorants, and/or vowels.
The most frequently occurring type of ejective is an ejective stop;
there are twice as many ejective stops as ejective affricates in the data
file - 188 to 94. Ejective fricatives are considerably rarer; only 20 are
recorded in UPSID. Naturally, ejective stops have been the topic of more
discussion than the other types of ejectives.
101
Glottalic and laryngealized consonants
Ejective stops
Haudricourt (1950) suggested that ejective stops exhibit a strong
preference for back articulations, Greenberg (1970) supported this claim
mainly because he found that "a gap in the class of ejectives at the
bilabial point of articulation is found in a number of world areas". Javkin
(1977) made the implied hierarchy for ejective stops more explicit,
formulating it as follows: "...[a language] will only have labial ejectives
if it has alveolar and velar; it will only have alveolar if it has velar."
(In this quote, "alveolar" refers to either dental or alveolar; as in other
chapters we will use an asterisk before a phonetic symbol to designate the
class of dentals and alveolars jointly.) Javkin counted the ejective stops
in the Stanford Phonology Archive and saw the numbers as being generally
confirmatory. The count is reproduced below as Table 7.1.
102
Patterns of sounds
Total 34 50 7 70 27
What is far more salient is that both velar and dental/alveolar places
are preferred to bilabial. There are significantly fewer occurrences of
/p 5 / than of either /k 3 / or /*t 5 /. No language has /p'/ that does not have
a velar, whereas 17 languages have /k'/ but no /p'/. Of the 11 languages
with only two places of articulation for their ejective stops, 10 have
/*t 3 , kV (the exceptional case is Berta, 218, with /p', k 5 / ) . Thus the
UPSID sample shows principally that bilabial place is disfavored for
ejective stops. This is reminiscent of the findings with respect to
voiceless plosives, which are also disfavored at the bilabial place,
although there is some evidence that the tendency to avoid /p 5 / is stronger
than the tendency to avoid /p/. This raises the question of whether both of
these patterns should be explained in the same way. This question will be
taken up again below.
103
Glottalic and laryngealized consonants
q'
5
*t' c' k5 2
*t ' c> k' q' 1 (also has k w> , kw')
5 5
P t t' k9 1
5 ejective stops P' t' c' k> q' 2
P' t' c5 kw 5 q5 1 (has q w ' but not k 3 )
P' t5 t5 k' q' 2
Table 7.3 shows that the most commonly encountered set of ejective stops
contains one at each of the most common places at which stops of any kind
occur (bilabial, dental or alveolar, and velar). Frequently, these are the
same places at which the language has other types of stops (glottal place
is not considered in these cases). An example of this pattern is the stop
system of Eastern Armenian (022):
p t k
ph fh [<h
104
Patterns of sounds
b d
t' k5
Here there is no bilabial ejective even though the language has other types
of stops at the bilabial place. Such languages are the crucial ones in
establishing that there is a specific tendency to avoid bilabial ejective
stops (cf. Haida, 700, below). The other main type of system is one in
which there are other deficiencies among bilabial segments - several of the
Semitic and Athabaskan languages lack a bilabial stop in another series as
well as a bilabial ejective.
If there are 4 or 5 ejective stops, there is usually one at the uvular
place: this is so for 16 of the 19 languages concerned. Again, there are
usually stops in other series at the same places of articulation. Quileute
(732) is representative of stop systems with 4 ejective stops:
p t k q
kw qw
b d (g)
p' t' k' q'
kw> qw>
All the languages with ejective stops at 5 places of articulation
include those stops found in the common 4-place inventory. An example is
Jaqaru (820):
p t c k q
ph fh ch ^h qh
105
Glottalic and laryngealized consonants
p t k q
t' k' q'
6
The comparative background to this situation in K'ekchi has been
investigated by Pinkerton (1980), who examined the phonetic nature of
corresponding segments in K'ekchi and 4 other languages in the Quichean
group of Mayan languages. Among these 5 languages, ejectives, voiced and
voiceless implosives, and voiced laryngealized stops interchange (and, for
the bilabials only, even plain voiced stops are involved). The
correspondences are shown in Table 7.4.
106
Patterns of sounds
107
Glottalic and laryngealized consonants
p' t5 c> k5
G cf
The Maidu case is interesting since the plosives whose places of
articulation are matched are aspirated ones rather than the voiceless
unaspirated ones found in the other languages discussed here. Thus we may
state more generally that an ejective usually occurs only if a plosive
occurs at the same place. This rule is not limited to a particular type of
plosives.
108
Patterns of sounds
Totals I 13 36 37 3 1 2 1
Every language with any ejective affricates has at least one of the common
sibilant types, /*ts'/ and /tj'/. Seven languages have only /tj'/, 5 have
only /*ts 5 /, and 11 have both /*ts'/ and /tj'/ but no other ejective
affricates. The remainder contain one or both of these sibilant ejective
affricates plus one other, most frequently the lateral /*t4'/. Concerning
/tj'/, Greenberg (1970: 17) commented that "for the palatal region in
particular, it appears that the optimal ejective is the alveopalatal
[affricate] rather than a stop". He goes on to observe that among
non-glottalic obstruents, affricates are preferred over plosives in this
articulatory region, and adds that the preference for affricates is even
stronger in the case of ejective obstruents. In fact, Greenberg found "no
example of an ejective palatal stop" in his sample. However, they not only
do occur, but occur with a contrasting affricate. Of the 7 languages in
UPSID with a palatal ejective stop, 5 also have a palato-alveolar or
palatal ejective affricate. Nonetheless, Greenberg's observation may have
some validity. While there are 5 times as many languages with /tj'/ than
with /c'/, among the plain pulmonic obstruents there are only 3.4 times as
many occurrences of /tj/ as /c/.
109
Glottalic and laryngealized consonants
Total 1 1 8 4 1 1 2 3 2
110
Patterns of sounds
The general preference for front articulations was borne out in a count of
the Stanford Phonology Archive by Javkin (1977). Table 7.7 gives the count
of voiced implosives and voiced laryngealized plosives in the languages of
UPSID, which shows a similar pattern for these stops at the different
places of articulation.
Ill
Glottalic and laryngealized consonants
39 36 1
Of the languages with only one of this class of segments, 5 have /?b/
(Kpelle, 103, Igbo, 116, and Zulu, 126, have /B/; Lakkia, 401, and K'ekchi,
714, have / b / ) . Berta (218) and Kullo (262) have /J7 alone. Somali (258)
has /d/# The number of cases is small, so any interpretation of the results
should be cautious. However, the systems with a single term here are
varied, unlike ejective stops where a single term is always /k'/« The
suggested revision of the hierarchical relationships of place in this case
would go as follows: the presence of /?b/ implies the presence of /?d/ or
of no other implosives, /?d/ implies the presence of /?b/ or of no other
implosives, while /?J7 implies the presence of both /?b/ and /?d/, and /?g/
implies the presence of /?b/, /?d/, and /?;}/.
112
Patterns of sounds
As Greenberg observed, the system with two terms, one bilabial and one
dental or alveolar, is the most common. In fact it is the only common
system. All 25 languages with this inventory have velar stops in other stop
series. This is quite strong evidence that the velar place is disfavored
for voiced implosives and for voiced laryngealized plosives. An example of
such a system is that of Doayo (128):
p t k k'p
b d g gb
6 J
A 3-term system also generally avoids use of the velar place of
articulation, having members at the bilabial, dental or alveolar, and
palatal places. An example of such a system is Yulu (216):
p t c k k'p
b d g gb
6 <f f
As in Yulu, so also in the other 3 languages concerned (Kadugli, 102,
Angas, 267, Ngizim, 269) there is at least one plosive with a palatal place
of articulation in the inventory. One language, Hamer (265), stands apart
from the others with 3 implosive terms. It has them at bilabial, alveolar
and velar places. Despite the presence of palatal plosives it does not have
a palatal implosive. This language is discussed further below.
Three of the four 4-term systems contain /?b, *?d, ?j, ?g/• An example
is Nyangi (207):
p t c k
G S f cf
The other two languages concerned, Swahili (124) and Maasai (204), do not
have palatal plosives in their inventories, although they do have
palato-alveolar affricates. The unusual 4-term language is Ik (208), which
has /G, cf, f, G< /, that is, it has a uvular rather than a velar fourth
term. This is despite the fact that the language has no other reported
uvular segments and does have velar plosives.
All 9 of the languages in UPSID with more than two implosives or voiced
laryngealized plosives are from Africa. They are drawn from 3 different
major language families, Niger-Kordofanian, Nilo-Saharan, and Afro-Asiatic.
It follows also that the only languages using the palatal and velar places
for segments of this type are African languages. Despite the small number
of cases, this seems to be an important areal trend.
113
Glottalic and laryngealized consonants
114
Patterns of sounds
typically articulated with a further back contact than other stops made
with the tongue tip and blade, A study of voiced alveolar plosives and
implosives in Shona using dynamic palatography (Hardcastle and Brasington
1978) did find a more retracted contact (with a smaller area) for the
implosive relative to the plosive for the speaker studied.
Voiceless implosives
Overwhelmingly, implosives are voiced. However voiceless implosives do
occur. Only one language in UPSID, Igbo (116), has any voiceless
implosives. Ladefoged et al. (1976) demonstrate that both voiced and
voiceless bilabial implosives occur in the Owerri dialect of Igbo, so that
/p < / is in contrast with /B/. Igbo also has the voiceless alveolar
<
implosive /t /. Pinkerton (1980) shows that the uvular ejective stop in
K'ekchi (714) can vary allophonically with a voiceless uvular implosive,
and in certain related Quichean languages the voiceless uvular implosive
occurs as the normal case (see Table 7.4 above). Other examples of
voiceless implosives are mentioned by Campbell (1973).
115
Glottalic and laryngealized consonants
m *n p Q "*r" *l j w
14 14 3 3 5 8 13 14
116
Patterns of sounds
The laryngealized approximants I}/ and /w/ usually occur together (12
of 16 languages concerned). There are 5 languages with a laryngealized
trill, tap or flap (collected in the table under the symbol /"*r"/). One
language, Wapishana (822), has a laryngealized voiced retroflex fricative,
id.
117
Glottalic and laryngealized consonants
places are preferred to a bilabial one. For implosives, etc., bilabial and
dental/alveolar places of articulation are equally common, and both are
preferred to velar.
Javkin (1977) clarified the role played by Boyle's Law in explaining
implosive and ejective distributions, correcting a misinterpretation by
Greenberg. Javkin (1977) noted that the claim that back articulations
confer an advantage in compressing air in the supraglottal chamber (for
ejectives) and front articulations confer an advantage in rarefying air
(for implosives) cannot be entirely correct. This is because it takes the
same effort to produce either compression or rarefaction in a chamber of a
given size. What matters is the proportional change in the size of the
chamber. Javkin's model suggests that the ability to change the volume of
the chamber is proportionally greater for a velar closure than for a
bilabial, dental or alveolar closure. That is, the same amount of raising
or lowering of the larynx will have a greater effect on the volume of air
between a velar closure and a glottal closure than if the oral closure is
further forward. If articulatory efficiency were the explanation for the
difference in the place preferences for ejectives and implosives, and there
were nothing else to consider, then both types of sounds would show a
preference for back articulation. It is possible to maintain this kind of
explanation for the preference for velar ejectives, providing some
overriding factor can be found to explain why implosives do not share the
preference for back articulations. Javkin suggests that this factor is
voicing.
118
Patterns of sounds
Note that we are not suggesting that plain voiced plosives are likely
to become implosives (pace Greenberg) as speakers endeavor to sustain
voicing through the duration of the oral closure. Instead it see"ms more
likely that, given a contrast between voiced stops of two different types
in a language, a tactic for enhancing the contrast between them by
implosion and emphasized voicing is exploited. In this view, implosives and
voiced laryngealized plosives would be expected to co-occur with plain
voiced plosives, as they do. Because the cavity expansion possibilities are
greatest when there is a front articulation, not only is a front
articulation preferred when the target is to achieve enhanced voicing, but
also when the target is for an "ordinary" degree of voicing, since if the
oral cavity is not expanded, voicing will cease when the oral air pressure
119
Glottalic and laryngealized consonants
120
Patterns of sounds
Notes
1. Hoard (1978) reports, as secondary allophones of the ejective
affricates /ts 5 / and /t4'/, the voiced implosive counterparts [cfz] and
[cfl^l in the Tsimshian language Gitksan (not included in UPSID). From
the description provided it is not clear if these voiced alternates of
the voiceless ejective affricates are actually imploded or not.
However, since they contrast phonetically with corresponding plain
voiced affricates in certain environments, it is probable that their
articulation is, if not implosive, at least accompanied by laryngeal
constriction. Still, the particular allophone characterizable as "most
typical" for these segments (i.e., that which would be coded by UPSID)
is in both cases the voiceless ejective affricate and hence it is still
true that no phonemic voiced implosive affricate segment is known to
occur. A similar analysis is presumably maintainable in regard to the
voiced palatal implosives of Fula and Serer (also not in UPSID) which
are slightly affricated (Ladefoged, personal communication).
References
Bell-Berti, F. 1975. Control of pharyngeal cavity size for English voiced
and voiceless stops. Journal of the Accoustical Society of America 57:
456-64.
Campbell, L. 1973. On glottalic consonants. International Journal of
American Linguistics 39: 44-6.
Carnochan, J. 1951. Glottalization in Hausa. Transactions of the
Philological Society 1951: 78-109.
Catford, J. 1939. On the classification of stop consonants. Le Maitre
Phonetique, 3rd scries, 65: 2-5.
Dart, S. 1984. Testing an aerodynamic model with measured data from
Korean. UCLA Working Papers in Phonetics 59: 1-17.
Fordyce, J.F. 1980. On the nature of glottalic and laryngealized consonant
and vowel systems. UCLA Working Papers in Phonetics 50: 120-54.
121
Glottalic and laryngealized consonants
122
Vowels
8.1 Introduction
Vowels are discussed in two chapters in this book. In the present chapter
we will deal with questions of which vowels are most frequent in the
world's languages and how many vowels are typically found. A brief
discussion of diphthongs is also included. In the following chapter,
contributed by Sandra F. Disner, the focus is on the structure of vowel
systems.
123
Vowels
We may make the following observations. Vowels in the mid range are a
little more common than high vowels, namely 1032 to 994, or 40.5% of the
sample to 39.0%. Low vowels are substantially less common, amounting to
only 20.5%. There are slightly more front vowels than back vowels, namely
1019 to 964, or 40.0% of the population compared with 37.8%. Central vowels
are considerably less common, amounting only to 22.2%. Unrounded vowels are
considerably more frequent than rounded vowels, namely 1569 to 981 or 61.5%
to 38.5%.l
There are some interesting asymmetries when the interactions of the
three basic parameters are looked at. Front vowels are usually unrounded
(94.0%), back vowels usually rounded (93.5%). Low vowels are usually
central (75.1%) and central vowels are usually low (69.4%). High front
vowels are more frequent than high back vowels. In the mid range, vowels
124
Patterns of sounds
are more commonly back than front if the lip position is unmarked (i.e.
unrounded if front, rounded if back), but front rounded vowels are more
frequently found than back unrounded. Nonback low vowels are extremely
unlikely to be rounded (only one case in 474).
The most common individual vowel qualities reported are given in Table
8.2. This table lists those vowels which are found to occur in at least 30%
of the languages in the survey. The number of languages shown here is the
number which have the given vowel quality either long or short; for
example, 290 is the number of languages out of the UPSID sample of 317 that
have one or both of /]/ and /i:/. Recall that /"o'7 and /"e'7 may not be
reliably distinct from other vowels in the mid range.
The 3 vowels at the corners of the conventional vowel triangle, /i, a, u/,
are the most widespread, but note that there are 24 fewer languages with
/u/ than with /i/. These three vowels might be expected to be equally
favored, because they each lie at an acoustic extreme. The low vowel /a/
has the highest first formant, /i/ and /u/ have the lowest first formant
but I\I has the highest second (and third) formant, whereas /u/ has the
lowest second formant. However, a contributory factor to the relative
disfavoring of /u/ may be the lower amplitude typical of /u/. In the higher
part of the mid range, note that there are substantially more cases of back
lol and /"o'V than of the front vowels lei and /"e11/, but among lower mid
vowels lei is a little more common than ID/. The within-language
125
Vowels
126
Patterns of sounds
127
Vowels
the total number of vocalic phonemes. This is because the number of vowel
qualities indicates how greatly the most basic parameters of vowel contrast
(height, backness, rounding) are being used. No language in UPSID has less
than 3 vowel qualities. The most common number, found in almost a third of
the languages, is 5. Almost two-thirds of the languages have between 5 and
7 vowel qualities, although up to 10 is still relatively common. The two
languages with the largest number of vowel qualities in UPSID are the two
Germanic languages in the survey, German (004) and Norwegian (006), which
have 15. Contrasts of a large number of vowel qualities seem
disproportionately common in Indo-European languages - of the 11 languages
with more than 10 vowel qualities, 5 are Indo-European. Almost 24% of the
Indo-European languages included in UPSID have over 10 vowel qualities,
whereas only 3.5% of the total sample have that many.
Length
Vowel length contrasts (short vs. long, or short vs. overshort) are only
recorded as phonemic in UPSID if they are linked to vowel quality
differences. In other words, if all the vowel qualities found in a language
participate in a length contrast, length is treated as a suprasegmental
feature or as resulting from a juxtaposition of simple vowels rather than
as a property of individual phonemes. There are three types of situations
where vowel length is represented in the phoneme inventory. In some
languages the long and short vowel sets do not overlap in quality; such a
case is Kurdish (015) with a 9-vowel inventory consisting of the following:
short long
i + o i : u:
"a" "e*" "o:"
a:
In such a case, length could have been treated as predictable from vowel
quality. More commonly, some vowels in each set have the same quality, as
in Tonkawa (752) with /u, u'/ and /a, a*/ pairs but with short /i, e, o/
128
Patterns of sounds
129
Vowels
unrounded vowel. Table 8.5 gives the "length ratio" of selected vowel
qualities, calculated by expressing the number of long vowels as a
proportion of the number of languages with vowels of the given quality
whether long or short (as in Table 8.1).
Front Back
Mid vowels
e:/e .280 o :/o .284
"er'Ve" .088 "oi'VV .098
e:/e .103 0 :/ 0 .090
Low vowels
ae:/ae .256 a :/a .318
a:/a .129
Selected other vowels
i:/i .151 u:/u .146
y:/y .238
oe:/oe .400
The higher mid long vowels lei I and I oil are far more likely to appear in a
language without corresponding short vowels of the same quality than any of
the other vowels examined. In 18% of the languages with the vowel quality
/e(:)/ and 19.6% of the languages with the vowel quality /o(:)/ the vowel
only occurs long. For comparison, this figure is 6.6% for the vowel quality
/i(:)/, 4.9% for /u(:)/ and 2.9% for /a(:)/. This suggests that mid vowels
tend to be raised when lengthened and/or lowered when shortened, giving
rise to associations between height and length. A well-known diachronic
example of this occurred in Late Latin (Griffiths 1966), and the subsequent
loss of length contrasts left Italian with the 4 mid vowels /e, e, o, o/ in
place of Classical Latin /e:, e, o:, o/. This illustrates one specific
direction the qualitative reinforcement of quantity differences referred to
in the previous paragraph can take.
Nasalization
Vowel nasalization is more common than inherent vowel length in the UPSID
languages, 71 languages or 22.4% of the sample having a contrast of oral
and nasalized vowels. There is again a tendency for this feature to be more
likely to occur in languages with a larger number of vowel quality
130
Patterns of sounds
contrasts but the trend is less pronounced than with length: 21.2% of
languages with 4-6 vowel qualities have contrastive nasalization, 22.5% of
languages with 7-9 vowel qualities have it, but 53.8% of languages with 10
or more vowel qualities have it. This distribution arises in part because
vowels with nasalization sometimes have different qualities from their
closest oral counterpart; for example the 3 nasalized vowels of Burmese
(509) are reported as / I , ae, 5/ but the closest counterparts among the 8
oral vowels are given as /i, a, u/. Thus Burmese arrives at a total of 11
vowel qualities. However, such cases are relatively rare; only 4 of the 14
languages with 10 or more vowel qualities that have nasalized vowels
include any vowel qualities that are found in the nasalized set alone.
These are Dan (106), Zande (130) and Sara (217) as well as Burmese. These 4
languages amount to only 15.4% of the 26 languages with 10 or more vowel
qualities.
By far the most common nasalized vowels are the 3 vowels whose oral
counterparts are also most common. There are 59 languages with /!/ or its
long counterpart, 58 with /a(:)/ and 55 with /u(:)/. These frequencies are
representative of a general pattern: nasalized vowel frequency is generally
correlated with the frequency of the oral equivalent. The number of
occurrences of a given nasalized vowel is about one-fifth the number of the
oral counterpart. However, there is one salient exception to this pattern
among the more common vowel qualities, and that is the higher mid vowel
/e/. Only 11 languages have /e(:)/ whereas 22 have /e(:)/. There is no
similar discrepancy between higher and lower mid back rounded vowels, in
fact 21 languages have /o(:)/ whereas only 19 have /5(:)/. Lowering of mid
(and high) vowels diachronically and allophonically has been frequently
commented on (e.g. Foley 1975; Wright 1980) but no asymmetry between front
and back vowels appears to have been mentioned before. A possible
interpretation of the difference between front and back mid vowels would be
that the lowering process leads to a greater lowering of front vowels than
of back vowels in the mid range under nasalization. This could cause /e/
and /e/ to merge as /e/. Wright (1980) showed how the acoustic effect of
nasalization was perceived as a lowering of vowel height in mid vowels but
his analyses of the perceptual distance between oral and nasal equivalents
of a set of vowels do not explain the asymmetry found in the UPSID data. If
anything, they suggest that there is a greater perceived lowering of back
vowels under nasalization than of front vowels.
131
Vowels
The instances of these various properties are too few to encourage very
much generalization, although it should be noted that no language has a set
of these "marked" vowels which contains more contrasts than is found in the
set of plain voiced vowels. In this respect these features appear more like
nasalization than like length.
In the languages with laryngealized, voiceless or breathy vowels, the
vowels in these sets have the same qualities as vowels which are found in
the plain voiced vowel set. But in a number of cases pharyngealized vowels
are more centralized than the closest non-pharyngealized vowel. For
example, Evenki has /u/ but / o /, Hamer has / j , e > a, o» u/ but /i , "e ",
e , o , o /, and Lak has /i, a, u/ but /"e^", ae , "o "/. These variations
in vowel quality are reminiscent of the qualitative differences between
132
Patterns of sounds
8.6 Diphthongs
Relatively few languages are considered to have phonologically unitary
diphthongs under the criteria used in UPSID (for further details on these
see Chapter 10). Obviously a much larger number of languages permit
sequences of juxtaposed vocalic segments which might be considered
phonetically to be diphthongs, or have diphthongal sounds which arise
allophonically. Because diphthongs are so frequently derived in this way
rather than being underlying segments, UPSID does not provide a good basis
for analysis of the phonetic patterns in diphthongs. Nonetheless, we offer
the following brief comments on the diphthongs that do occur. There are a
total of 83 diphthongal segments recorded in UPSID from 23 different
languages. Fully 22 of these 83 diphthongs are from a single language, !Xu
(918). This language has 4 sets of diphthongs: plain oral, plain nasalized,
pharyngealized oral, and pharyngealized nasalized (cf. the 8 sets of
monophthongal vowels it has). Of the other languages with diphthongs, 2
(Kurdish, 015, and Acoma, 749) have 8 each, one (Dani, 613) has 5, 2
(Hindi-Urdu, 016, and Yagaria, 609) have 4. There are a further 5 languages
with 3 diphthongs, 3 languages with 2 and 8 languages with one diphthong.
The diphthongal segments reported are rather heterogeneous and do not
show much clear patterning. The only ones recorded which are at all common,
133
Vowels
i.e. occur in more than 2 languages, are those shown in Table 8.6. Though
the numbers are small, this table seems to indicate that diphthongs that
begin or end with a high vowel element are preferred over those which lack
such an element. This cannot be explained as being the result of an attempt
to maximize the distinctiveness of the diphthongs, since diphthongs with
short trajectories through the vowel space, such as /ei/, /ie/ and /ou/,
are found among the more common types as readily as those with a large
trajectory through this space, such as /ai/, /au/ and /ui/.
8.7 Summary
The principal conclusions reached in this chapter can be summarized as
follows. Front vowels are usually unrounded, back vowels are usually
rounded, low vowels are usually central and central vowels are usually low.
All languages have at least 3 phonemic vowels. Nearly all languages have
/ i, a, u/, but among these the vowel /u/ is more often absent than /i/ or
/a/. The most common number of vowel phonemes in a language is 5, and the
most common number of distinctive vowel qualities in a language is also 5.
Contrastive length is associated with an increase in the number of
distinctive vowel qualities in an inventory. The higher mid vowels / e/ and
/o/ are more likely to appear long without a short counterpart than lower
mid vowels. Among nasalized vowels, /e/ is comparatively rare. No language
has more vowels in a secondary set than it has in its primary set, where
secondary means vowels with nasalization, pharyngealization or an unusual
phonation type, and primary means that set of vowels with normal voicing
and no secondary articulation which has the most members (in some languages
this is a set of long vowels)• Diphthongs show a preference for including a
high vowel element.
134
Patterns of sounds
Notes
1. Note that the percentages are almost exactly what would obtain if all
languages had the 5 vowel system provided for by the Roman alphabet,
/i, e, a, o, u/. This system has 40% high vowels, 40% mid vowels, 20%
low vowels; 40% front vowels, 40% back vowels, 20% central vowels; 60%
unrounded vowels, 40% rounded vowels.
2. A similar basis for vowel harmony, under the name of "register", has
also been suggested for some Austro-Asiatic languages (Gregerson 1976).
References
Allen, W.S. 1965. On one-vowel systems. Lingua 13: 111-24.
Anderson, S.R. 1978. Syllables, segments and" the Northwest Caucasian
languages. In A, Bell and J.B. Hooper (eds.), Syllables and Segments.
North-Holland, Amsterdam: 47-58.
Crothers, J. 1978. Typology and universals of vowel systems. In J.H.
Greenberg et al. (eds.), Universals of Human Language, Vol. 2,
Phonology. Stanford University Press, Stanford: 93-152.
Foley, J. 1975. Nasalization as a universal phonological process. In C.A.
Ferguson, L.M. Hyman and J.J. Ohala (eds.), Nasalfest. Stanford
University, Stanford: 197-212.
Gregerson, K.J. 1976. Tongue-root and register in Mon-Khmer. In P. Jenner,
L. Thompson and S. Starosta (eds.), Austro-Asiatic Studies, Vol. 1.
University of Hawaii Press, Honolulu.
Griffiths, T.G. 1966. The Italian Language. Faber, London.
Hockett, C.F. 1955. A Manual of Phonology (UAL Memoir 11). Indiana
University, Bloomington.
Jacobson, L.C. 1978. DhoLuo Vowel Harmony (UCLA Working Papers in
Phonetics 43). University of California, Los Angeles.
Lindau, M. 1979. The feature expanded. Journal of Phonetics 7: 163-76.
Perkell, J.S. 1969. Physiology of Speech Production: Results and
Implications o£ a Quantitative Cineradiographic Study. "M.I.T. Press,
Cambridge, Massachusetts.
Sedlak, P. 1969. Typological considerations of vowel quality systems.
Working Papers in Language Universals (Stanford University) 1: 1-40.
Wright, J. 1980. The behavior of nasalized vowels in the perceptual vowel
space. Report of the Phonology Laboratory (University of California,
Berkeley) 5: 127-63.
135
Insights on vowel spacing 1
9•1 Introduction
This chapter presents the results of an analysis of the vowel systems of
the 317 languages in the UCLA Phonological Segment Inventory Database
(UPSID). It shows that deviations from the patterns predicted by a theory
which proposes that vowels are dispersed in the available phonetic space
are relatively infrequent and, for the most part, confined to matters of
small scale, falling into a few definable classes. It will be argued that
in most of these deviations from the predicted patterns there is
nonetheless evidence that vowels tend toward a balanced and wide dispersion
in the available phonetic space.
9.2 Preliminaries
A few basic vowel inventories and a few basic configurations show up time
and again in natural languages, while other, no more complex patterns are
rare or totally absent. The most prevalent patterns seem to be the
so-called "triangular" systems, particularly those of average size, and
notably the 5-vowel systems. For example, over a quarter of the 209
languages in the Stanford Phonology Archive have a triangular 5-vowel
system consisting of /i, e, a, o, u/> while less than 5% have any of the
other 5-vowel configurations; the "square" 4-vowel and 6-vowel systems
combined total less than 10% (Crothers 1978).
Several attempts to explain these patterns invoke a principle of vowel
dispersion, proposed in slightly differing versions by Liljencrants and
Lindblom (1972), Lindblom (1975), Terbeek (1977), and Maddieson (1977). 2
This principle holds that vowels tend to be evenly distributed in the
available phonetic space and also widely distributed, within the
136
Patterns of sounds
137
Insights on vowel spacing
138
Patterns of sounds
9.3 Method
Data
The UPSID sample of 317 languages was tested for vowel dispersion. The
descriptive sources were carefully examined for any details which might
possibly shed light on the true phonetic quality of the vowels under study,
and the maximum available detail which could be represented in the coding
scheme was retained.
The vowel phonemes in UPSID are represented on a height scale with 5
basic values (high, higher mid, mid, lower raid, low). In this chapter mid
and higher mid vowels are usually transcribed with the same symbols, but
where it is necessary to distinguish between them mid vowels are shown by
symbols in quotes, e.g. /"e"/ vs. /e/. Two additional heights, "lowered
high" and "raised low", are also recognized. The vowels are also
categorized on a backness scale with 3 values (front, central, back), and
on a rounding scale with 2 values (rounded, unrounded). Additional
dimensions pertaining to length, nasalization, phonation characteristics
(laryngealization, breathiness), and other features (r-coloration, lip
compression) were recorded in the archive, but for purposes of eld city .and
simplicity these distinctions are not discussed in the present chapter.
The vowels will be discussed in terms of a distinction between
"peripheral" and "interior" vowels. (Note that this distinction is not the
same as that represented by the variable "peripheral" in the UPSID
database; see Chapter 10.) The "peripheral" vowels are the front unrounded,
back rounded, and low vowels, all of which lie along the margins of the
.available phonetic space. It should be noted, however, that the high
central vowels, although they occupy one of the margins of the phonetic
space, do not fall within the peripheral category; this more restrictive
definition of peripherality is justified on phonological grounds, as the
high central vowels tend not to pattern with the true peripheral vowels in
natural languages, and they are also less common than other peripheral
vowels. Thus, high central /+/ and /«/, along with the remaining
phonetically centralized vowels, constitute the set of "interior" vowels.
139
Insights on vowel spacing
140
Patterns of sounds
Four-vowel systems
The test procedure does not classify vowel systems in the same way as the
dispersion models. One particular configuration of vowels which is
classified as "defective" by our criteria is, in fact, fully in accordance
with both the Liljencrants and Lindblom model and Lindblom's later
refinement of it. This is the 4-vowel system /i, e, a, u/. In this case the
basic /i, a, u/ system has been expanded by a single vowel, and the one
first chosen is the front vowel /e/. (The corresponding back vowel hi is
not predicted to appear until the inventory has reached at least 5 vowels.)
By our procedure all 4-vowel systems with one mid vowel are regarded as
defective. This classification seems justified since these systems are in
any case rare. There are 6 such cases. Of these, Shasta (746), Paez (804),
and Moxo (827) have the inventory predicted by Liljencrants and Lindblom,
141
Insights on vowel spacing
/i, e, a, u /. Bardi (357) has a 4-vowel system in which the single mid
vowel is the back rounded vowel /o/, contrary to that prediction. Two
other languages resemble the predicted system, but are rather more
symmetrical in the distribution of their 4 vowels. Wichita (755) has a back
vowel /v/ in place of /a/, yielding a system with 2 back and 2 front
vowels. Cayapa (803), a Paezan language quite closely related to Paez, has
/i, e, a, o/. Rather than occupying their expected positions in the vowel
space, both the back vowels are closer to the gap in the lower mid region
where /o/ might be expected. In this way the vowels are rather equally
spaced. Certain other 4-vowel languages are analyzed as having two gaps in
their peripheral vowel system. These are discussed in a later section.
142
Patterns of sounds
as in Figure 9,3 (b). Vertical gaps are also rare: only one language lacks
back high and mid vowels, as in Figure 9.3 (c). (Recall that languages
which lack be front and back high vowels or both front and back mid
vowels are exempted from the "defective" category.)
(a) (b) (c)
i [] [] u i []
[] o e [] e []
a a a
Figure 9.3 Languages with 2 major gaps
The defective languages demonstrate that vowel systems occasionally ^p_
avoid certain regions of the space. These systems will be discussed below
in terms of the typology discussed in Section 9.2 above.
Stationary systems
Leaving aside the matter of ambiguity of transcription, our investigation
reveals that 9 or perhaps 10 languages fall under the category of
"stationary" systems, that is, systems which have a gap that they do not
appear to compensate for in any way. All of these happen to be 3 or 4-vowel
systems, although larger systems can be stationary just as well. In these
systems all the vowels are peripheral vowels of the most common types, and
the systems are otherwise balanced, with no evidence of skewing from the
front to the back. Clear examples of such stationary systems are Klamath
(707), which has the vowel system /i, "e", a, "o"/, and Bardi (357), Shasta
(746), Paez (804) and Moxo (827), which, as mentioned above, lack a mid
vowel. The Campa (825) system is similar to Klamath, except that the mid
vowels are reported as being "mid close", i.e. higher mid. Tacana (812) is
recorded in UPSID as having the same system as Klamath (although the back
vowel is recorded in one source as /u/ rather than /of). In Hupa (705), the
basic system is a not-fully-peripheral /e, o, a/. However, /t/ occurs in
the language as a surface segment. Depending on the status accorded to this
anomalous segment, the language may or may not be viewed as having a gap in
the high back region of the vowel space. Mura (802) has a 3-vowel system
/i, a, o/. This could be classed as doubly defective by our test, with /i/
implying a missing /u/ and /o/ implying a missing /e/. However, there is no
evidence in the source to indicate whether the transcription reflects
phonetic reality or orthographic convention. The system could be a rotated
system such as /i >, o, de/ , which is maximally dispersed in the vowel space.
Seneca (754) might well be added to this group of languages. It has an /u/
143
Insights on vowel spacing
Complementary vowels
Some of the languages in our inventory have defective systems that are
complemented by a single vowel of unexpected phonetic quality which shares
some of the features of the missing vowel. Vowel systems of this sort can
be classed into 3 major types, namely, those in which the complementary
vowel is (i) a central vowel (9 languages), (ii) a front rounded or back
unrounded vowel (13 languages), or (iii) a peripheral vowel similar to the
missing vowel but lacking a counterpart of equal height and opposite
rounding elsewhere in the vowel system (6 languages). Stated more formally,
if the missing vowel is [a high, 3 back, y low, 6 round], then the
complementary vowel is:
(i) (ii) (iii)
[ a high ] , [ a high ] or [-a high or - low]
[+ central] [ 3 back ] [ 3 back ]
[-3 round]
We will discuss each of these types in turn.
144
Patterns of sounds
Cheremis (051), /a/ offsets a missing low vowel, but there is also evidence
of compensation. Among languages with a missing high vowel Cofan (836) is
very similar to Abipon, but with evidence of compensation as well. These 3
languages are discussed further below. In Chacobo (811), in addition to a
missing high back vowel, a front mid vowel is also lacking. There is no
central vowel to offset the latter. This double gap may perhaps be better
explained as a rotation of the entire system.
145
Insights on vowel spacing
146
Patterns of sounds
which /w/ stands alone. (No defective vowel system has a complementary /y/
in place of missing /i/.)
An investigation of the acoustic, perceptual, and auditory quality of
the vowel /w/ suggests that this vowel is in fact quite centralized,
verging on the quality of central /t/. For example, Ward's (1938) auditory
analysis of Bamum /in/ places this vowel well away from cardinal /u/,
half-way between the central and back regions of the phonetic chart.
Hombert (personal communication) has conducted a perceptual test on
speakers of 3 Bamileke languages, Banjoun, Fe?Fe?, and Bangangte in which
synthetic vowels were presented to the subjects, and the formant
frequencies which corresponded to "acceptable" vowels in their languages
were noted. For the /w/ in these languages the mean acceptable Fl was 260
Hz., and F2 was 1391 Hz.; this corresponds to a high and almost central
vowel. Papcun (1976) plotted the vowels of Spanish and Japanese in a
normalized acoustic space. Spanish has a rounded /u/ and Japanese an
m
unrounded l f• He found that the difference between these high back vowels
lies mainly in F2, which is approximately 725 Hz. for the Spanish vowel but
1275 Hz. for the Japanese vowel, corresponding to a considerably
centralized location in the acoustic space. The vowel /iu/ thus appears to
be similar to the vowel /+/. If we combine the 9 vowel systems complemented
by /w/ with the 3 complemented by /+/ into a single category, we find that
these 12 cases constitute the majority of cases of complementation in our
sample; complementation by any of the remaining interior vowels /y, 0, *,
©/ occurs in only 10 languages. The high interior region seems to be a
favored area for complementary vowels. The significance of this fact is not
readily apparent, and merits further perceptual and acoustic study. It may
be the case that the corner of the vowel space in which the high back vowel
/u/ is to be found is simply smaller, in perceptual terms, than we might
otherwise expect, and hence /+/, /a/ and /LU/ are closer to each other
perceptually than the acoustic facts suggest.
147
Insights on vowel spacing
148
Patterns of sounds
149
Insights on vowel spacing
150
Patterns of sounds
compensating for a gap in the mid back region. The high back vowel /©/ is
lower than its front counterpart /i/, and the low vowel /a/ is further back
than the expected /a/. The -quality of the mid front vowel is reported as
/e/, and though there is no mid back vowel against which to test its
height, we may perhaps infer (from the use of the symbol /e/, rather than
the unmarked /e/) that it is lower than expected, backing up the
displacement of the low vowel. From what appears to be an anchor point at
/i/, the vowels of Cayapa are displaced from their expected values
according to the pattern in Figure 9.4.
151
Insights on vowel spacing
152
Patterns of sounds
9.5 Conclusions
The great majority of vowel systems in our sample assume configurations
which are predictable from a theory of vowel dispersion, considered in the
light of some basic facts about the overall number of vowels, their degree
153
Insights on vowel spacing
Notes
1. This chapter was contributed by Sandra Ferrari Disner.
2. The formulations differ somewhat in the degree of dispersion they
propose, but no attempt will be made in this paper to choose between
them. Except for the absence of vowels at the extreme corners of the
vowel space, the data is unsuited for this task. For the most part, we
can only look at areas within the phonetic vowel space and label the
general arrangements according to which areas are filled. In order to
investigate whether specific points in the space are filled, we would
need acoustic measurements drawn from a large number of speakers of
each language (see Disner 1980).
3. For example, the apparent success of such a model might be attributable
to heavy emphasis on a few language areas in the sample utilized by
Liljencrants and Lindblom, or to a bias in the sources in favor of
reporting apparently balanced vowel systems when adequate phonetic
detail is lacking.
4. Considerable energy was expended to seek out whatever phonetic detail
was available in the sources, regardless of the authors'
transcriptions. Cross-references were checked, footnotes examined, and
a good deal of reading between the lines was done in hopes of adding
some detail to the reported phonetic quality. Skewed vowel systems
provide us with particularly valuable information on the phonetic
quality of the vowels in question. A linguist who is little concerned
154
Patterns of sounds
References
Crothers, J. 1978. Typology and universals of vowel systems. In J.H.
Greenberg et al. (eds.), Universals of Human Language, Vol. 1_: Theory
and Methodology. Stanford University Press, Stanford: 93-152.
Disner, S.F. 1980. Evaluation of vowel normalization procedures. Journal of
the Acoustical Society of America 67: 253-61.
Greenberg, J.H. f966. Language Universals, with Special Reference to
Feature Hierarchies. Mouton, The Hague.
Jakobson, R. 1941. Kindersprache, Aphasie, und allgemeine Lautgesetze.
Reprinted in Selected Writings I_. Mouton, The Hague: 328-401.
Liljencrants, J. and Lindblom, B. 1972. Numerical similation of vowel
quality systems: the role of perceptual contrast. Language 48: 839-62.
Lindblom, B. 1975. Experiments in sound structure. Paper reacf at the Eighth
International Congress of Phonetic Sciences, Leeds.
Maddieson, I. 1977. Tone loans: a question concerning tone spacing and a
method of answering it. UCLA Working Papers in Phonetics 36: 49-83.
Papcun, G. 1976. How may vowel systems differ? UCLA Working Papers in
Phonetics 31; 38-46.
Sapir, E. and Swadesh, M. 1939. Nootka Texts. Linguistic Society of
America, Philadelphia.
Terbeek, D. 1977. Some constraints on the principle of maximum perceptual
contrast between vowels. Proceedings of the Thirteenth Regional Meeting
2L t n e Chicago Linguistic Society: 640-50.
Voegelin, C.F. 1956. Phonemicizing for dialect study, with reference to
Hopi. Language 32: 116-35.
Ward, I.C. 1938. The phonetic structure of Bamum. Bulletin of the School of
Oriental and African Studies 9: 423-38.
155
10
10,1 Introduction
As remarked in the preface to this volume, the discovery of generalizations
concerning the content and structure of phonological inventories has been a
significant objective of recent work in linguistics. UPSID is designed as a
tool for research into these questions, providing uniform data from a
properly balanced sample of an adequate number of languages for
statistically reliable conclusions to be reached. It differs in some major
ways from the Stanford Phonology Archive (SPA) compiled at Stanford
University, although this archive served as a model for our work.
Stanford's plan was more ambitious than the plan for UPSID, but Stanford's
team found that they had to limit the size of their language sample as
their work progressed. The final report includes information on the
phonologies of 196 languages. A principal reason for exclusion was the
scarcity of adequately detailed phonological descriptions. The variability
in detail of the sources which were used also necessarily produced entries
which vary in their completeness—from those which cover little more than a
list of phonemes to those which are able to include a lot of allophonic
details and information on phonological alternations. Thus, for retrieval
of certain information, the true sample size is smaller than 196 languages.
With each reduction, the likelihood that the sample is no longer
representative and properly balanced increases. With Stanford's experience
in mind, UPSID was designed to be narrower in the scope of information
about each language entered, but to be more comprehensive in the number of
languages covered. Users of SPA have also commented that there is a certain
inflexibility inherent in the format chosen for data entry. This is
basically a text-oriented system (for some description, see Vihman 1974).
156
Patterns of sounds
157
Design of UPSID
UPSID is based is to select one and only one language from each moderately
distant genetic grouping, so that the selected languages represent in
proper proportion the internal genetic diversity of various groupings. The
obvious difficulties in the way of such a scheme are the lack of sound
genetic classifications in certain areas (e.g. South America, New Guinea),
the difficulty of comparing genetic distances in different language
families, and lack of requisite data from some known groups. The advantages
of this procedure are that it precludes, in principle, selection of data
which represents arguably the same language in several varieties (unlike
SPA which includes, for example, both Moroccan and Egyptian dialects of
Arabic, and Maltese); also it directs a principled search for the data to
fulfill the quota design and avoids undue reliance on descriptions that
happen to be at hand (the "bibliographic convenience" factor mentioned by
Bell 1978). A genetic basis for the sample is selected in preference to any
other since it is the only classification which is, in principle, not
arbitrarily determined by the criteria chosen for the classification, but
instead aims to represent real historical relationships. In addition, it is
appropriately independent of the phonological characteristics of which it
is desired to find the frequencies. Note that the phonological uniqueness
of a language is not a basis for inclusion. Because each language included
is relatively distinct genetically from all others in the sample, each
represents the outcome of the opportunity for independent operation of
historical processes. Similarities between languages in the sample are
therefore not due solely to the effect of shared historical origin. As
noted before, the number of speakers is considered a quite inappropriate
basis for including (or excluding) a language. The size of extant
populations of speakers of languages is an accident of political and social
history that is quite irrelevant to questions relating to the structure of
human languages.
158
Patterns of sounds
which had not developed within their own independent speech communities for
at least some 1000-1500 years, but to include one language from within each
group of languages which shared a closer history than that. The 11 major
groupings, plus an "others" category, are shown in Table 10.1, together
with the number of languages included in UPSID from each and the range of
identification numbers assigned to each grouping.
159
Design of UPSID
160
Patterns of sounds
161
Design of UPSID
162
Patterns of sounds
163
Design of UPSID
Indices
1-4. Language identification number (LANGNO). This number serves to
identify the language to which a segment belongs. It consists of 3 digits,
the first and sometimes second of which indicates affiliation to one of the
major groupings used as the genetic basis of the sampling (see Table 10.1).
5-7. Segment identification number (SEGNO). Each segment within a language
is numbered sequentially. The combination of LANGNO and SEGNO thus
identifies one and only one record in the data base.
164
Patterns of sounds
165
Design of UPSID
Secondary articulations
41. Labialized (LABLZED).
42. Palatalized (PALTLZED). This variable takes the value 1 only for true
palatalized consonants, i.e. those with a secondary palatal articulation.
Thus a segment /c/ which occurs in a language as part of a "palatalized"
166
Patterns of sounds
series of stops /p\ "H/ etc., will not be coded with this feature. Instead
it will be reported as a palatal stop.
43. Velarized (VELRZED). This variable only takes the value 1 for true
velarized segments. This value was also used to characterize vowels with
velar stricture, reported in Siriono (829).
44. Pharyngealized (PHARGZED). Unlike the preceding secondary articulation
variables, which usually only have the value 1 for consonants, this
variable takes the value 1 equally for consonants and vowels.
45. Nasalized (NASLZED). This variable takes the value 1 for nasalized
consonants and vowels, i.e. those with simultaneous nasal and oral escape.
It is also used to characterize prenasalized stops (when these are clearly
units). Thus the value 1 for this variable in combination with the value 1
for any stop variable (except click or affricated click) indicates a nasal
onset to the stop.
46. Nasal release (NASRELSE). Takes the value 1 for postnasalized segments
only.
Vowel features
All simple vowels are specified by a value of 1 on one vowel height, one
vowel backness and one lip position variable. They may also have the value
1 for other variables to indicate other distinctions. Diphthong segments
are specified by assigning the value 1 to all the vowel quality variables
needed to describe both their beginning and end points. A set of diphthong
variables, discussed below, indicates the order of conflicting
specifications.
47. High (HIGH).
48. Higher mid (HIGHMID).
49. Mid (MID). This variable is used with systematic ambiguity for both
those vowels which are indicated as "mid" without further particularization
and those which are true mid vowels (i.e. lie between higher mid and lower
mid on a height scale).
50. Lower mid (LOWMID). Note that / e/ and /o/ are considered lower mid
vowels, not low vowels.
51. Low (LOW).
52. Front (FRONT).
53. Central (CENTRAL). Note that /a/ in most languages is considered a
central vowel, not a back vowel.
54. Back (BACK).
167
Design of UPSID
55. Nonperipheral (NONPERIF). This variable takes the value 1 for "laxed"
noncentral vowels which are produced away from the periphery of the vowel
space, for example, III and /o/. It may on occasion serve a mainly
diacritical function where other features fail to distinguish vowels.
56. Rounded (ROUNDED).
57. Unrounded (UNROUNDD).
58. Lip-compressed (LIPCOMP). This variable takes the value 1 for "labial"
vowels that are produced with vertical compression of the lips but no
drawing in and forward of the corners of the mouth ("rounding").
59. R-colored (RCOLORED). This variable takes the value 1 for retroflexed
or r-colored vowels.
Diphthong variables
The 3 variables for characterizing movement in diphthongs differ from most
other variables in the inventory, which indicate only presence or absence
of the attribute named by the variable. For diphthong variables a zero
value may indicate a specific property of a diphthong, namely movement in
the opposite direction to that indicated by the variable name. Also, unlike
most of the variables, they require reference to the values of other
variables for their interpretation. Their function is to indicate which
value precedes when conflicting specifications of vowel height, backness or
lip position are given to a single segment. This method of coding
diphthongs was adopted in order to avoid a very large number of variables.
60. Backing (BACKING). This variable takes the value 1 when the end point
of a diphthong is more back than the beginning, as in /ia/, /eu/, /eu/ etc.
It takes the value 0 when the end point is either more front than or has
the same degree of backness as the beginning, as in /oi/, /ae/, /ou/, etc.
Note that only 3 degrees of backness are considered (front, central, back).
61. Lowering (LOWERING). This variable takes the value 1 for diphthongs
that have an end point lower than their beginning, such as /ia/, /ea/, etc.
It takes the value 0 when the endpoint is higher than or equal to the
beginning on the 5-point vowel height scale used in UPSID, for example
/oi/, /ou/, /ai/ etc.
62. Rounding (ROUNDING). This variable takes the value 1 when the endpoint
of a diphthong is rounded but the beginning is unrounded, as in /eu/, /ao/,
etc. It takes the value 0 when the endpoint is unrounded or both the
beginning and endpoints are rounded, as in /oi/, /ai/ or /ou/.
168
Patterns of sounds
Note that a pair of diphthongs such as /oi/ and / io/ receive the same
values on the vowel variables. This will involve having conflicting values
for height, backness and rounded; they will be specified as being both high
and mid, front and back, rounded and unrounded. The diphthong features
interpret these conflicts. Thus /oi/ will have the value 0 for all 3
diphthong variables since there is no backing, lowering or rounding
movement in this diphthong. Yet because the segment has conflicting values,
there must be movement; it has to be in the opposite direction from the
variable names, i.e. fronting, raising and unrounding. On the other hand,
/io/ receives the value 1 for all diphthong features indicating that the
mid portion of the diphthong follows the high portion, the back portion
follows the front portion and the rounded portion follows the unrounded
portion.
169
Design of UPSID
170
Patterns of sounds
171
Design of UPSID
The palatal nasal place variable would have the value 10, indicating that
there was no palatal plosive or affricate matching the palatal nasal. This
172
Patterns of sounds
References
Bell, A. 1978. Language samples. In J.H. Greenberg et al. (eds.),
Universals of Human Language, Vol. J^, Method and Theory. Stanford
University Press, Stanford, California: 123-56.
Bender, M.L. 1976. Nilo-Saharan overview. In M.L. Bender (ed.), The
Non-Semitic Languages of Ethiopia (Monograph 5, Committee on Ethiopian
Studies, African Studies Center). Michigan State University, East
Lansing: 439-83.
Ferguson, C.A. 1963. Some assumptions about nasals. In J.H. Greenberg
(ed.), Universals of Language. MIT Press, Cambridge: 42-7.
Gandour, J. 1975. On the representation of tone in Siamese. In J.G.
Harris and J.R. Chamberlain (eds.), Studies in Tai Linguistics in
Honor of William J. Gedney. Central Institute of English Language,
Bangkok: 170-95.
Greenberg, J.H. 1966. The Languages of Africa. Indiana University Press,
Bloomington.
Greenberg, J.H. 1970. Some generalizations concerning glottalic consonants,
especially implosives. International Journal of American Linguistics 36:
123-45.
Greenberg, J.H. 1971. Nilo-Saharan and Meroitic. In T. Sebeok (ed.),
Current Trends In Linguistics, Vol. 7_> Sub-Saharan Africa. Mouton, The
Hague: 421-42.
Hurford, J.R. 1977. The significance of linguistic generalizations.
Language 53: 574-620.
Hyman, L.M. 1977. On the nature of linguistic stress. In L.M. Hyman (ed.),
Studies in Stress and Accent (Southern California Occasional Papers in
Linguistics 4 ) . University of Southern California, Los Angeles: 37-82.
Jakobson, R. and Halle, M. 1956. Phonology and Phonetics. (Part 1 of)
Fundamentals of Language. Mouton, The Hague: 3-51.
Ladefoged, P. 1971. Preliminaries to Linguistic Phonetics. University of
Chicago Press, Chicago and London.
Ladefoged, P., Glick, P. and Criper, C. 1972. Language in Uganda. Oxford
University Press, London.
SAS Institute Inc. 1982. SAS User's Guide, 1982 Edition: Basics. SAS
Institute Inc, Cary, North Carolina.
Trubetskoy, N. 1939. Grundzllge der Phonologie (Travaux du Cercle
Linguistique de Prague 9 ) . Prague.
Tucker, A.N. and Bryan, M.A. 1956. The Non-Bantu Languages of Northeast
Africa (Handbook of African Languages, Part 3 ) . Oxford University Press
for International African Institute, London.
Tucker, A.N. and Bryan, M.A. 1966. Linguistic Analyses: The Non-Bantu
Languages of Northeastern Africa. Oxford University Press for
International African Institute, London.
Vihman, M.M. 1974. Excerpts from the Phonology Archive coding manual.
Working Papers on Language Universals (Stanford University) 15: 141-53.
173
Appendix A: Language lists and bib1i og raphy of data sources
The languages included in the UPSID sample are listed below, first
according to a genetic classification, and secondly in alphabetical order.
The genetic listing enables a quick check to be made on which languages are
included from a given family. The genetic classification provided is
intended only as an outline. Main subfamilies that are not listed have no
representative in UPSID. In a few cases the affiliation of a language is
uncertain (e.g. Cofan, 836). For convenience, the large Amerindian family
has been divided on a geographical basis. The alphabetical listing enables
a check to be made for inclusion of a particular language. The language
names are those used throughout the book, but in some cases a
cross-reference from an alternative name is given to assist in tracking
down a given language. In both lists the language name is followed by the
language identification number. These numbers are assigned mainly on a
genetic basis (see Chapter 10). Note that, because of late additions and
deletions to the sample, the sequence of numbers within a genetic group is
not always continuous. The phonemic charts in this book are presented in
the order of these identification numbers. The alphabetical list also
serves as the key to the data sources consulted for each language. These
sources are identified by author and date; the full references may be found
in the bibliography of sources which forms the third part of this appendix.
A language included in the Stanford Phonology Archive (SPA) is indicated by
S after the language name in both lists. It may be assumed that the SPA
report was consulted in determining the inventory for that language. For
most of these languages, the sources used by SPA have also been directly
consulted, and, for a few, additional or different sources were used. The
remaining languages were analyzed solely at UCLA.
Indo-European (000-049)
Greek: Greek 000 S
Celtic: Irish 001 S, Breton 002 S
Germanic: German 004 S, Norwegian 006 S
Baltic: Lithuanian 007 S
Slavic: Russian 008 S, Bulgarian 009 S
Romance: French 010 S, Spanish 011 S, Romanian 012 S
Iranian: Farsi 013 S, Pashto 014 S, Kurdish 015
Indie: Hindi-Urdu 016 S, Bengali 017 S, Kashmiri 018 S,
Punjabi 019 S, Sinhalese 020 S
Albanian: Albanian 021 S
Armenian: Eastern Armenian 022 S
Ural-Altaic (050-099>
Finno-Ugric: Ostyak 050 S, Cheremis 051 S, Komi 052 S,
Finnish 053 S, Hungarian 054 S, Lappish 055
Samoyed: Yurak 056 S, Tavgy 057
Turkic: Osmanli (Turkish) 058 S, Azerbaijani 059 S,
Chuvash 060 S, Yakut 061 S, Kirghiz 062 S,
Bashkir 063, Khalaj 064, Tuva 065.
Mongolian: Mongolian 066 S
Tungus: Evenki 067 S, Goldi 068, Manchu 069
Korean: Korean 070 S
Japanese: Japanese 071 S
174
Language l i s t s
Niger-Kordofanian (100-199)
Kordofanian: Katcha 100 S, Moro 101, Kadugli 102
Mande: Kpelle 103 S, Bisa 104, Bambara 105, Dan 106
West Atlantic: Wolof 107 S, Diola 108, Temne 109
Voltaic: Dagbani 110 S, Senadi 111, Tampulma 112, Bariba 113
Kwa: Ewe 114 S, Akan 115 S, Igbo 116 S, G3 117 S
Togo Remnant: Lelemi 118
Cross River: Efik 119
Plateau: Birom 120, Tarok (Yergam) 121, Amo 122
Bantoid: Beembe 123 S, Swahili 124 S, Luvale 125 S,
Zulu 126 S, Teke (Kukuya)127
Adamawa: Doayo 128
Eastern: Gbeya 129 S, Zande 130
Nilo-Saharan (200-249)
Songhai: Songhai 200 S
Saharan: Kanuri 201 S
Maban: Maba 202
Fur: Fur 203
Eastern Sudanic: Maasai 204 S, Luo 205 S, Nubian 206 S,
Nyangi 207, Ik 208, Sebei 209, Tama 210,
Temein 211, Nera 212, Tabi 213, Mursi 214
Central Sudanic: Logbara 215 S, Yulu 216, Sara 217
Berta: Berta 218
Kunama: Kunama 219
Koman: Koma 220
Afro-Asiatic (250-299)
Semitic: Arabic 250 S, Tigre 251 S, Amharic 252 S,
Hebrew 253 S, Socotri 254, Neo-Aramaic 255
Berber: Shilha 256 S, Tuareg 257
Cushitic: Somali 258 S, Awiya 259 S, Iraqw 260 S,
Beja 261
Omotic: Kullo 262, Dizi 263, Kefa 264, Hamer 265
Chadic: Hausa 266 S, Angas 267 S, Margi 268 S,
Ngizim 269, Kanakuru 270
Austro-Asiatic (300-349)
Munda: Mundari 300 S, Kharia 301 S
Khasi: Khasi 302 S
Vietmuong: Vietnamese 303 S
Bahnaric: Sedang 304 S
Khmer: Khmer 306 S
Australian (350-399)
Iwaidjan: Maung 350 S
Bureran: Burera 352
Tiwian: Tiwi 351
Nunggubuyan: Nunggubuyu 353 S
Ma ran: Alawa 354 S
Daly: Maranungku 355 S, Malakmalak 356
Nyulnyulan: Bardi 357
Pama-Nyungan: Wik-Munkan 358 S, Kunjen 359 S,
Western Desert 360 S, Nyangumata 361 S,
Aranda 362, Kariera-Ngarluma 363,
Gugu-Yalanji 364, Mabuiag 365,
Arabana-Wanganura 366, Diyari 367, Bandjalang 368
175
Language lists
Austro-Tai (400-499)
Kam-Tai: Standard Thai 400 S, Lakkia 401 S, Yay 402 S,
Sui 403, Saek 404, Po-ai 405, Lungchow 406
Atayalic: Atayal 407 S
West Indonesian: Sundanese 408 S, Javanese 409 S, Malagasy 410 S,
Cham 411 S, Malay 412 S, Batak 413 S
Philippine: Tagalog 414 S, Sa'ban 415 S, Chamorro 416 S,
Rukai 417
Formosan: Tsou 418
N.E. New Guinea: Adzera 419 S, Roro 420
New Britain: Kaliai 421 S
Loyalty Is: Iai 422 S
Polynesian: Maori 423 S, Hawaiian 424 S
Sino-Tibetan (500-599)
Sinitic: Mandarin 500 S, Taishan 501 S, Hakka 502 S,
Changchow 503 S, Amoy 504, Fuchow 505, Kan 506
Himalayish: Tamang 507
Mirish: Dafla 508 S
Lolo-Burmese: Burmese 509 S, Lahu 510 S
Kachin: Jingpho 511
Kuki-Chin: Ao 512, Tiddim Chin 513
Baric: Garo 514 S, Boro 515
Karenic: Karen 516 S
Miao-Yao: Yao 517 S
Indo-Pacific (600-699)
Andamanese: Andamanese 600
West New Guinea: Asmat 601 S
North New Guinea: Washkuk 602 S, Sentani 603 S, Nimboran 604, Iwam 605
South-East New Guinea: Telefol 606 S
Central New Guinea: Selepet 607 S, Gadsup 608 S, Yagaria 609, Kewa 610,
Chuave 611, Pawaian 612, Dani 613, Wantoat 615,
Daribi 616, Fasu 617
South New Guinea: Suena 618
North-East New Guinea: Dera 619
East New Guinea: Kunimaipa 620 S, Yareba 621, Koiari 622, Taoripi 623
Bougainville: Nasioi 624 S, Rotokas 625
Central Melanesian: Nambakaengo 626
176
Language lists
177
Language lists
178
Language lists
179
Language lists
180
Language lists
Lelemi 118 Hoftmann (1971)
Lithuanian 007 S Senn (1966), Augustitis (1964), Ambrazas, et al. (1966)
Logbara 215 S Crazzolara (I960), Tucker and Bryan (1966)
Luiseffo 737 S Malecot (1963), Bright (1965, 1968), Kroeber and
Grace (1960)
Lungchow 406 Li (1977)
Luo 205 S Gregersen (1961)
Lushootseed, see Puget Sound Salish
Luvale 125 S Horton (1949)
Maasai 204 S Tucker and Mpaayei (1955), Tucker and Bryan (1966)
Maba 202 Tucker and Bryan (1966)
Mabuiag 365 Wurm (1972a)
Maidu 708 S Shipley (1956, 1964)
Malagasy 410 S Dahl (1952), Dyen (1971)
Malakmalak 356 Tryon (1974), Birk (1975)
Malay 412 S Verguin (1967), MacDonald and Soenyono (1967)
Malayalam 905 Kumari (1972), McAlpin (1975), Velayudhan (1971)
Manchu 069 Austin (1962)
Mandarin Chinese 500 S Dow (1972), Chao (1968), C-C. Cheng (1973)
Maori 423 S Biggs (1961), Hohepa (1967)
Maranungku 355 S Tryon (1970)
Margi 268 S Hoffmann (1963)
Maung 350 S Capell and Hinch (1970)
Mazahua 717 S Spotts (1953)
Mazatec 727 S Pike and Pike (1947)
Mixe (Totontepec) 715 Crawford (1963), Schoenhals and Schoenhals (1965)
Mixtec 728 S Hunter and Pike (1969)
Mongolian (Khalka) 066 S Hangin (1968), Street (1963), Luvsanvandan (1964)
Moro 101 Black (1971)
Moxo 827 S Ott and Ott (1967)
Muinane 806 Walton and Walton (1967)
Mundari 300 S Gumperz and Biligiri (1957)
Mura 802 Sheldon (1974)
Mursi 214 Turton and Bender (1976)
Nama 913 S Beach (1938), Ladefoged and Traill (1980)
Nambakaengo 626 Wurm (1972b)
Nambiquara, Southern 816 Price (1976)
Nasioi 624 S Hurd and Hurd (1966)
Navaho 702 S Sapir and Hoijer (1967)
Nenets, see Yurak
Neo-Aramaic 255 Garbell (1965)
Nera 212 Thompson (1976)
Nez Perce 706 S Aoki (1970, 1966)
Ngizim 269 Schuh (1972)
Nimboran 604 Anceaux (1965)
Nivkh, see Gilyak
Nootka (Tseshaht) 730 S Sapir and Swadesh (1939, 1955)
Norwegian 006 S Vanvik (1972)
Nubian (Mahas) 206 S Bell (1971)
Nunggubuyu 353 S Hughes and Leeding (1971)
Nyangi 207 Heine (1975a)
Nyangumata 361 S O'Grady (1964)
Ocaina 805 S Agnew and Pike (1957)
Ojibwa 750 S Bloomfield (1956)
Osmanli (Turkish) 058 S Swift (1963), Lees (1961)
Ostyak 050 S Gulya (1966)
Otomi 716 S Blight and Pike (1976)
181
Language lists
182
Bibliography
183
Bibliography
184
Bibliography
185
Bibliography
186
Bibliography
Cowan, H.K.J. 1965. Grammar of the Sentani Language (Verhandelingen van het
Koninklijk Instituut "voor Taal-, Land- en Volkenkunde 47). Nijhoff, The
Hague.
Crawford, J.C. 1963. Tptontepec Mixe Phonotagmemics. Summer Institute of
Linguistics, University of Oklahoma, Norman.
Crawford, J.M. 1973. Yuchi phonology. International Journal of American
Linguistics 39: 173-9.
Crazzolara, J.P. 1960. A Study of the Logbara (Ma'di) Language. Oxford
University Press, London.
Crumrine, L.S. 1961. The Phonology of Arizona Yaqui (Anthropological Papers
of the University of Arizona 5 ) . University of Arizona, Tucson.
Cunningham, M.C. 1969. A Description of the Yugumbir Dialect of Bandjalang
(University of Queensland Faculty of Arts Papers 1/8). University of
Queensland Press, Brisbane.
Dahl, O.C. 1952. Etude de phonologie et de phonetique malagache. Norsk
Tidsskrift for Sprogvidenskap 16: 148-200.
Dalby, D. 1966. Lexical analysis in Temne with an illustrative wordlist.
Journal of West African Languages 3: 5-26.
Davis, D.R. 1969. The distinctive features of Wantoat phonemes. Linguistics
47: 5-17.
Davis, M.M. 1974. The dialects of the Roro language of Papua: a preliminary
survey. Kivung 8: 3-22.
De Armond, R.C. 1975. Some rules of Brahui conjugation. In H.G. Schiffman
and C M . Eastman (eds.), Dravidian Phonological Systems. University of
Washington Press, Seattle.
Decsy, G. 1966. Yurak Chrestomathy (Indiana University Publications, Uralic
and Altaic Series 50). Indiana University, Bloomington.
Di Luzio, A. 1972. Preliminary description of the Amo language. Afrika und
Ubersee 56: 3-60.
Dirks, S. 1953. Campa (Arawak) phonemes. International Journal of American
Linguistics 19: 302-4.
Doerfer, G. 1971. Khalaj Materials (Indiana University Publications, Uralic
and Altaic Series 115). Indiana University, Bloomington.
Doke, C M . 1926. The phonetics of the Zulu language. Bantu Studies Special
number.
Doke, C M . 1961. Textbook of Zulu Grammar. Longmans, Cape Town.
Douglas, W.H. 1955. Phonology of the Australian aboriginal language spoken
at Ooldea, South Australia, 1951-1952. Oceania 25: 216-29.
Douglas, W.H. 1964. An Introduction to the Western Desert Language (Oceania
Linguistic Monographs 4, revised ed.). University of Sydney, Sydney.
Dow, F.D.M. 1972. An Outline of Mandarin Phonetics. Faculty of Asian
Studies, Australian National University, Canberra.
Dul'zon, A.P. 1968. Ketskij Jazyk. Tomsk University, Tomsk.
Dutton, T.E. 1969. The Peopling of Central Papua (Pacific Linguistics,
Series B, 9 ) . Australian National University, Canberra.
Dyen, I. 1971. Malagasy. In T.A. Sebeok (ed.), Current Trends in
Linguistics, Vol. 8, Linguistics in Oceania. Mouton, The Hague: 211-39.
Echeverria, M.S. and Contreras, H. 1965. Araucanian phonemics.
International Journal of American Linguistics 31: 132-5.
Egerod, S. 1966. A statement on Atayal phonology. Artibus Asiae, Supplement
23: 120-30
Emeneau, M.B. 1937. Phonetic observations on the Brahui language. Bulletin
oj[ the School ot_ Oriental Studies 8: 981-3.
Emeneau, M.B. 1944. Kota Texts, Vol. 1. University of California Press,
Berkeley and Los Angeles.
Fast, P.W. 1953. Amuesha (Arawak) phonemes. International Journal of
American Linguistics 19: 191-4.
187
Bibliography
188
Bibliography
189
Bibliography
190
Bibliography
191
Bibliography
192
Bibliography
193
Bibliography
194
Bibliography
195
Bibliography
196
Bibliography
197
Bibliography
198
Bibliography
199
Appendix B^: Phoneme charts and segment index for UPSID languages
The following pages contain charts showing the phoneme inventory of each of
the carefully selected sample of 317 languages which comprise the UCLA
Phonological Segment Inventory Database (UPSID). It also includes an index
of each segment type which occurs in the database. This index is arranged
according to the phonetic classification of the segments, and includes the
number of languages with each given segment type and a list of the
languages in which it occurs.
The phoneme charts and segment index make available to other users the
basic data of UPSID. With these tools, much of the information in the
database can be manipulated without the use of a computer. For example, the
question "Does /g/ only occur in languages with /k/?" can be answered for
the UPSID sample by using the index to find the list of languages with /g/
and then turning to each relevant chart to see if /k/ occurs, or
cross-checking with the list of languages with /k/ in the index. More
complex co-occurrences of segments can be examined more easily by a study
of the charts.
Publishing the data in this form serves another purpose, that of making
the interpretations of the phoneme inventories adopted in UPSID open to
independent evaluation. Scholars may reach their own conclusions on the
appropriateness of the inventory as it is represented in UPSID.
The charts
Consonants
The consonants of each language are represented by phonetic symbols located
on a chart which is fully labeled for each place of articulation, secondary
articulation, and manner of articulation which occurs in that language.
Places of articulation are arranged along the top of the chart in a
sequence from the front to the back of the oral cavity. These places
correspond to those recognized in the list of variables used in the UPSID
database (Chapter 10). The last place of articulation in this sequence is
glottal. Note that /h/ is not considered to have a glottal place of
articulation, but to have a variable place. It is placed after glottal. In
addition to dental and alveolar places, an unspecified dental or alveolar
column is included for those segments which are not precisely identified as
being one or the other in the source used for the description of a given
language. This column is labeled dental/alveolar.
Double articulations are listed after variable place in a front-to-
back sequence determined by the more forward of the two articulations
concerned. Secondary articulations are placed in the columns following the
primary place. Nasalization, including pre- and post-nasalization, is
considered a secondary articulation. It is listed first among them. Other
secondary articulations follow in a sequence from the front to the back of
the oral cavity, i.e. labialization is first, pharyngealization is last.
Manners of articulation are arranged down the chart in a sequence in
which the primary arrangement is by decreasing degrees of stricture, with
stops first and approximants last. Stops are followed by affricates,
fricatives, nasals, taps, trills, and flaps, then approximants. Within
stops, plosives precede ejective stops and implosives. The Khoisan
languages Nama and !Xu required special handling because of their large
inventories of clicks. The clicks are placed on a separate chart. Sibilant
affricates and fricatives follow nonsibilant ones, and lateral affricates
and fricatives follow both. Lateral approximants precede central ones. A
special category for segments which are simply identified as some kind of
r-sound follows taps, trills and flaps.
Differences in phonation type are nested within this sequence in a way
similar to the way that secondary articulations are nested within the
200
Segment index
Vowels
The vowels of each language are given below the consonant chart. The vowel
charts are not fully labeled. Instead, the choice of symbols is left to
provide more of the information on the nature of the vowels in question.
However, labels are provided for those levels of the familiar dimension of
vowel height that are represented in the given language, and the symbols
are arranged in a left to right order which corresponds to a front-to-back
dimension. The layout approximates a conventional vowel chart of triangular
shape. Lip position is indicated entirely by choice of symbol. Where
rounded and unrounded vowels occur with the same height and front/back
positions the front unrounded vowel precedes the front rounded vowel and
the back rounded vowel precedes the back unrounded vowel. The pair of
symbols is separated by a comma. In many languages there are several
separate series of vowels (short vs. long, oral vs. nasalized, etc.). These
are given in separate arrangements, with the "plainest" series first.
Transcriptions for vowels are also based on I.P.A. conventions, apart
from a few necessary additional distinctions. Overshort vowels are
indicated with a superscript micron. Mid vowels which are not further
distinguished as higher mid (half-close) or lower mid (half-open) are
represented by letters enclosed in quote marks. The same letters without
the quotes represent higher mid vowels. Thus /e/ is a higher mid front
unrounded voiced vowel, /"e"/ is a similar vowel which is only described as
mid (it might be in the middle of the mid range, or vary in quality within
the mid range, or it might just be incompletely described). Since the
vowels are not fully labeled on the charts a key to the vowel symbols and
the diacritics which may be applied to them is given on page 204.
If a language has any diphthongs which are accepted as unit phonemes by
the criteria used by UPSID, they are given to the right of the simple
vowels. No attempt is made to label the diphthongs; they are simply
represented by digraphs indicating approximate starting and ending points.
201
Segment index
Anomalous segments
Those segments in the inventory of a given language that are regarded as
somewhat anomalous (though still worthy of being included in the inventory)
are indicated by superscript numbers on the charts. These correspond to the
non-zero values of the "anomaly" variable in the UPSID data file which are
defined in Chapter 10.
The index
The index to segment types is divided into 10 major sections:
1. plosives
2. glottalic stops
3. clicks (including affricated clicks)
4. affricates (including ejective affricates)
5. fricatives (including ejective fricatives)
6. nasals
7. trills, taps, and flaps
8. approximants
9. vowels
10. diphthongs.
These sections separate segments into major manner classes. Within these
sections, the sequence of segments is ordered in a way that is similar to
the way the charts are arranged, except that, since the index is a list
rather than a two-dimensional array, the values for manner and place
features are nested into a single sequence. Within each section, place is
given priority over manner with the following exceptions:
(a) ejectives precede implosives in section 2.
(b) affricated clicks follow plain clicks in section 3.
202
Segment index
(c) ejective affricates and fricatives follow all the pulmonic types in
sections 4 and 5.
(d) in section 7, trills precede taps, which precede flaps. Unspecified
r-sounds are included at the end of this section.
(e) lateral segments are grouped together in the relevant sections,
following segments with central articulation in sections 3, 4, 5 and 7, but
preceding central segments in section 8. This arrangement juxtaposes the
common types of flapped and approximant laterals in sections 7 and 8. Note
that the arrangements specified in (b), (c), and (d) above take precedence
over (e).
Voiceless segments of a given place are listed before voiced ones in
sections 1, 2, 3, 4, and 5. Voiced ones precede voiceless ones of the same
place in sections 6, 7, 8, 9, and 10. Other modifications of the phonation
type are next most highly ranked, while secondary articulations (of place)
are the most superficial criterion for sequencing. Thus, a voiceless
aspirated alveolar plosive will precede any voiced alveolar plosives, and a
palatalized voiceless aspirated alveolar plosive follows one with no
secondary articulation but precedes a velarized one.
Vowels are arranged in three main classes: all front vowels are listed
before all central vowels, which in turn precede all back vowels. Within
each height/backness class (e.g. high front or lower mid back), unrounded
vowels precede rounded vowels. This arrangement applies even for back
vowels, where rounding is more common. Other modifications (such as
nasalization, pharyngealization) form the most superficial level of vowel
classification.
Diphthongs are arranged in the following way. First, the quality of the
vowel recognized as their point of origin is used to arrange them in a
similar way to that in which the simple vowels are arranged. Diphthongs
starting with a high front unrounded vowel come first and so on. Those with
the same starting point are then arranged on the basis of the vowel quality
recognized for their ending point. The manner in which this is done can be
understood by imagining an array of lines on the conventional vowel chart
from the point of origin to the point of termination. The diphthongs are
then sequenced in an "anticlockwise" manner with the closest to 12:00
o'clock as the starting point. Note that the transcriptional distinction
between mid vowels (/"e"/, /"o"/ etc.) and higher mid vowels (/e/, /o/
etc.) is not maintained for diphthongs. No quote marks are used here.
Following each phonetic definition, a list of those languages in UPSID
in which the defined segment occurs is given. The listing of language names
is in the order in which identification numbers were assigned, which
corresponds with a few exceptions to the order in which languages appear in
the genetic listing given in Appendix A. If a segment is regarded as
anomalous in a language, the language is listed at the end of the entry for
the segment following a short keyword which describes the nature of the
anomaly. The following are the keywords used in the index to correspond to
the non-zero values of the "anomaly" variable in the UPSID data file.
Value of anomaly Index keyword Brief definition
1 RARE~ " Extremely low lexical frequency
2 LOAN Occurs in unassimilated loans
3 ?ABSTRACT Posited underlying segment
4 7DERIVED Segment possibly derivable from others
5 OBSCURE Particularly vague or contradictory
description.
203
Segment index
Lowered high I, Y O , LU
Higher mid e, 0 a, e O, ¥
Low a, a, x), a
/a:/ long
/5/ overshort
/a/ laryngealized
/a/ breathy voiced
/a/ voiceless
/a/ nasalized
/ar/ retroflexed
Y
/a / with velar stricture
/|/ fronted
/a/ retracted
/a/ lowered
/a/ raised
/a 9 / pharyngealized
204
Segment index
SEGMENT INDEX
1. PLOSIVES
205
Segment index
206
Segment index
,m. -
Nasally-released voiced bilabial plosive / D / 1
Aranda.
207
Segment index
208
Segment index
209
Segment index
210
Segment index
211
Segment index
212
Segment index
213
Segment index
214
Segment index
Nootka.
Voiceless labial-velar
215
Segment index
Kpelle, Dan, Dagbani, Senadi, Tampulma, Bariba, Ewe, G2, Lelemi, Efik,
Birom, Tarok, Amo, Doayo, Gbeya, Zande, Logtfara, Yulu.
216
Segment index
3. CLICKS
(Including affricated clicks. Note that clicks may be affricated in two
ways. The front release may be affricated or the back (velar) release may
be affricated. UPSID refers only to the former as affricated clicks. A
fricated release of the back closure is referred to as velarization of the
click.)
217
Segment index
218
Segment index
219
Segment index
220
Segment index
4. AFFRICATES
(Pulmonic and glottalic affricates are included here. Affricated clicks are
listed together with other clicks in section 3.)
221
Segment index
222
Segment index
223
Segment index
224
Segment index
Voiceless velar plosive with alveolar lateral fricative release /k4/ 1 (-1)
OBSCURE Ashuslay.
225
Segment index
5. FRICATIVES
226
Segment index
227
Segment index
228
Segment index
229
Segment index
Sundanese, Sa'ban, Rukai, Ao, Tiddim Chin, Garo, Boro, Asmat, Washkuk,
Iwam, Selepet, Yagaria, Kewa, Chuave, Daribi, Fasu, Yareba, Navaho,
Tolowa, Hupa, Nez Perce, Klamath, Maidu, Wintu, Zoque, Tzeltal, Totonac,
K'ekchi, Otomi, Nootka, Kwakw'ala, Quileute, Puget Sound, Hopi, Yacqui,
Karok, Dieguefio, Achumawi, Tunica, Alabama, Cayapa, Paez, Muinane,
Ocaina, Chacobo, Cashinahua, S. Nambiquara, Jaqaru, Wapishana, Amuesha,
Campa, Moxo, Guarani, Guahibo, Siona, Tucano, Malayalam, Georgian, Nama,
Brahui, Ainu, !Xu; RARE Telugu; LOAN Barasano, Araucanian.
230
Segment index
231
Segment index
232
Segment index
233
Segment index
234
Segment index
6. NASALS
235
Segment index
236
Segment index
237
Segment index
238
Segment index
239
Segment index
240
Segment index
241
Segment index
8. APPROX1MANTS
242
Segment index
Dagbani, Senadi, Ewe, Igbo, G2, Tarok, Beembe, Swahili, Doayo, Songhai,
Kanuri, Maasai, Nubian, Ik, Sebei, Nera, Mursi, Yulu, Berta, Hebrew,
Socotri, Shilha, Awiya, Beja, Kullo, Kefa, Hamer, Kharia, Vietnamese,
Sedang, Maranungku, Wik-Munkan, Kariera-Ngarluma, Gugu-Yalanji, Mabuiag,
Standard Thai, Lakkia, Yay, Sui, Po-ai, Lungchow, Sundanese, Javanese,
Malagasy, Cham, Malay, Batak, Tagalog, Tsou, Kaliai, Hawaiian, Mandarin,
Taishan, Hakka, Changchow, Fuchow, Kan, Burmese, Lahu, Jingpho, Yao,
Andamanese, Telefol, Dani, Kunimaipa, Taoripi, Nambakaengo, Haida,
Chipewyan, Tolowa, Nez Perce, Chontal, Mazahua, Chatino, Squamish, Tiwa,
Porno, Yana, Zuni, Delaware, Wiyot, Dakota, Yuchi, Alabama, Wappo,
Itonama, Abipon, Quechua, Island Carib, Aleut, Kurukh, Yukaghir, Gilyak,
Lak, Basque, Burushaski; LOAN Tarascan, Ojibwa; ?DERIVED Mixtec.
243
Segment index
244
Segment index
245
Segment index
246
Segment index
9. VOWELS
247
Segment index
Nasalised high front unrounded vowel with velar stricture /T^/ 1 (-1)
OBSCURE Siriono.
248
Segment Index
249
Segment index
250
Segment index
251
Segment index
252
Segment index
253
Segment index
254
Segment index
255
Segment index
256
Segment index
257
Segment index
Kanuri, Luo, Nubian, Nera, Mursi, Yulu, Berta, Kunama, Amharic, Hebrew,
Socotri, Neo-Aramaic, Tuareg, Awiya, Beja, Kullo, Dizi, Kefa, Hausa,
Angas, Margi, Kanakuru, Mundari, Kharia, Khasi, Vietnamese, Malakmalak,
Kunjen, Mabuiag, Sui, Lungchow, Sundanese, Batak, Chamorro, Tsou,
Adzera, Roro, Kaliai, Maori, Hawaiian, Changchow, Kan, Jingpho,
Andamanese, Washkuk, Iwam, Yagaria, Kewa, Chuave, Pawaian, Dani,
Wantoat, Daribi, Fasu, Dera, Yareba, Koiari, Taoripi, Rotokas,
Nambakaengo, Tlingit, Hupa, Klamath, Maidu, Wintu, Chontal, Zoque,
Tzeltal, K'ekchi, Mixtec, Chatino, Luiseflo, Yacqui, Achumawi, Yana,
Wiyot, Seneca, Wappo, Itonama, Bribri, Mura, Muinane, Amahuaca, Chacobo,
Tacana, Ashuslay, Abipon, S. Nambiquara, Arabela, Auca, Gununa-Kena,
Guahibo, Ticuna, Siona, Tucano, Cofan, Kota, Kurukh, Malayalam,
Yukaghir, Chukchi, Gilyak, Georgian, Basque, Burushaski, !Xu; RARE
Tigre; LOAN Chuvash; 7DERIVED Telugu.
258
Segment index
259
Segment index
10. DIPHTHONGS
Nasalized mid front unrounded to_ high back rounded diphthong /eu/ 1
ixa.
Mid front unrounded to high central rounded diphthong /eu/ 1
Tsou.
Nasalized low front unrounded to lower mid front unrounded diphthong /ae/ 1
+
Hindi-Urdu.
260
Segment index
Pharyngealized
__ low central unrounded to mid back rounded diphthong /ao / 1
261
Segment index
Nasalized lower mid back rounded to higher mid back rounded diphthong/oo/ 1
Hindi-Urdu.
262
<<
a. o
bilabial
labio-dental
dental
dental/alveolar
alveolar
palatal
velar
I 2. I
t ! t :
r • • s p r g r & &r
M
^'S'ssr * ? ? a1
3 S S *° S" R- ff
& I
T3_ bilabial palatalized
-* bilabial labialized and
5- velar ized
i^. dental
dental velarized
»+ dental/alveolar
dental/alv. palatalized
dental/alv. velarized
alveolar
alveolar palatalized
CD! O
alveolar velarized
palato-alveolar
palatal
palatal palatalized
^ velar
^_ velar palatalized
variable place
labial-velar
1
•H
3
,3 2 3 a
Tj
4J
S
>
S> Language
C r-l r-1 X I&
•S| ,_| rH
^ ^H
& 7
O
Z
IM Cd
O e d c t J O W O - U M 6 cd cd cd
ibial
ibial pal ata
• H 4 J 4 - » < u c d i - . c d c d
X ) C C > > - l 4 J i H r H
H
Norwegian (006) c d < u o ) r - < c d < u c d a ) Lithuanian (007) •H -H g g ,
lab].o-dental
labiLo-dental
1 H T 3 T 3 cd O . M O u > .Q X<
voiced nasal
voiced nasal m mj
voiced trill rJ
higher mid
mid
highe r mid
lower mid
low
fe* 8. 8. SL
s s s
6 ft s s
labio-dental
3= N= W= dental/alveolar
s s s
s
alveolar
palato-alveolar
palatal
velar
labial-palatal
labial-velar
labial-viar nasalized
a. o <<
a M o i
S S S S 31 B1 R. 2.
"o. bilabial
labio-dental
dental/al1
alveolar
palato-alveolar
palatal
velar
« .• I variable place
vl.
vl.
993
Vowels
sib.
sib.
voiced t
vd. late
vd. nons
vl. nons
voiced p
voiceles
voiced iu
CD o
CO T3
ive
1 osive
fricat
icative
fricat
Ericate
approx
bilabial
bilabial palatalized
labio-dental
labio-dental palatalized
dental
dental palatalized
dental velarized
dental/alveolar
pala to-alveolar
palato-alv. velarized
palatal
X (O velar
velar palatalized
< < < < < <
(X (- 1 CL (-• O H>
H n> n co 3 s tn co n> to
(U CL CL H- o o H- H- a co
3 3 o" cr >a
S S B § S
rt n
sr r s?
o- x? bilabial
a_ tj bilabial palatalized
labio-dental
labio-dental palatalized
,5 <£ 4 dental/alveolar
-P c£ -+ dental/alv. palatalized
dental/alv. velarized
r+ palato-alveolar
palatal
(o 7T velar
CQ T: velar palatalized
Language
CO T3 I <H
2 o -3 2 2 ^ 43 43
French (010)
3 2 S 3 3 | -3 -S Spanish (Oil)
2 3 .8 2. a
voiceless plosive voiceless plosive
p i
voiced plosive vl. sibilant affricate
long
high I. Y high
higher mid
mid
lower mid e.oe o
low
low a v
short nasalized
higher mid
lower mid
low
893
E ss §
ss
bilabial
labio-dental
j+ a r+ dental/alveolar
alveolar
palato-alveolar
palatal
velar
variable place
labial-velar
I t 2 t I £ I
s sr I i i k S S. 3
IIaI| | I
P. £ £ S S
^ ^ R
labio-dental
dental
dental/alveolar
palato-alveolar
palatal
velar
uvular
•o glottal
variable place
labial-velar
Language
2
Pashto (014) •3 Kurdish (015)
a
voiceless plosive voiceless plosive
long
long
high
high
mid
mid
low
low
oi
ui
Q. 1-1 O. O O H O Z I t
«< 5
I- H CT
f
l-h l-h CD
i-t i-l H- H- <j Hi S, O* *
8
11 »8
labio-dental
dental
dental/alveolar
palato-alveolar
retroflex
palatal
H- P
1 velar
uvular
Oj +Q), O +Q)
O* Q)' O CD glottal
variable place
| O
1
Vl. £
breal
vl. z
Vl. c
Vl. £
voice
vd. s
vd. j
vd. n
j ET a
rt 2. cr c 13
m
I
ft> g CO ft)
S
cei
ilar
ilai
ilai
plos
» ft)
cr
0) 1 <* 8.
1 >o X (D
! "^ X)
ib.
M.
affi
affi
2" ft)
ft h O I 8
3 I 1 * ^* ft
£ ffi
^ i' (D r (0 I
ate
ox ima
ricat
-o bilabial
<£ r+ ,+ dental/alveolar
• CL Q. r+ ^+
palato-alveolar
J.Q. .Q. .^t-
retroflex
palatal
velar
variable place
labial-velar
<r a
g n £
I 6
S ? £ 5 £
5 s S BS
dental/alveolar
alveolar
palato-alv. palatalized
-+ •<* retroflex
palatal
+J g
jr_ ^- velar
variable place
labial-velar
M < < < M < M
O O Q
O M O. M O O. Q
£ ' * ' <3 * I < 8. < < £
< H § § < & < o n.
H> H
o "O M H- H- CO
» tt (6
2 f ? ° a
p. sr s. •§
ft
P
H
§ s
0) H O* ft
p, . p> (1)
r i i 3 « "S R, R, 5 8 *
f! 2 & J? iS
s g s* -
.. H. O O O
H. <J B) » »
III
T^ -o_ -o TJ bilabial
labio-dental
13 13 IN l(/> dental
^ ^ ^ ~t dental/alveolar
palato-alveolar
.^_ . ^ .-»• .-»• retroflex
palatal
7r 7: 7T 7T velar
variable place
labial-velar
u
o
41
SI >
u
"3 "3 -3
lato -al
veol
Sinhalese (020)
A iJ A A t Albanian (021) 3
(0
bil
den
den
2. £ §. > > 8.
voiceless plosive voiceless plosive p
p t
voiced plosive b voiced plosive b
d
vl. nonsibilant affricate vl. sibilant affricate
higher mid
low
t £ I f.
S 8. I S. f.
i IS ? a s ?•S, S S
8 S
& Jl n o
< < B)
t r •
labio-dental
• * • * - • dental/alveolar
palato-alveolar
retroflex
palatal
uvular
variable place
I I
2. 8. S. § &
2 I I es
s ? 5
-o bilabial
-£ dental/alveolar
palato-alveolar
retroflex
o palatal
7T velar
labial-velar
< HZ
p a n
Vowels
vd. cen
vl. sib
vd. sib
rr
voiced i
voiced i
rt
i E 2
2 P>
vd. nonsibd
sr
H rt H
mid
higl
higl
1 3 Pi S
s*
Lve
Efricate
-icative
-. fricat
approximan
-icative
: fricat
i I 1
-o bilabial
x, bilabial palatalized
labio-dental
dental
^ ,£ dental/alveolar
J dental/alv. palatalize
^. palato-alveolar
palatal
je velar
i t i. i. % O O
(»rt
\\w\\\\\ ° S1
H, M, 3 3 S & »
p. p. MI H» o n
bilabial
labio-dental
dental/alveolar
alveolar
palatal
Language Language
long
long
high I, y u I:, y: hi
«h i:, y:
higher mid <f,
higher mid e .^
lower mid
lower mid
low low
9LZ
S* S. 8. § * 8. H
I? rr p g S , 5
2 e z <?
a
If f
labio-dental
-£ dental/alveolar
palato-alveolar
palatal
^- velar
variable place
• • K1 I? * * * * J? S"
8 6 " 8 . 8 . S . § § S . 8 . I 5 .
R S c o S 1 8 S •» S
li 1 M 6) H ' H - H - H ' l - ' e o
p > o > H - o ) i » c r c r p > o
! _ • • _ • |_ B) 9 H - H - 9 (0 -O
l H r r T
03 o> ~ ' ' ' ( B ' t o ' < " o '
8 £
rt n H- t-h <
S X 2 f? H* H-
F B " O O S»
§ § ^ R R ?
a- -o bilabial
cr_ -o_ bilabial palatalized
dental
dental palatalized
r+ dental/alveolar
-+ dental/alv. palatalized
palatal
?r velar
H* glottal
labial-velar
IE1 LLZ
o n <K
3 rt £ g g 2. d ^
o" 3 3 (b ^
H w c n ' O O ) o
8 £ £
S 8
< p)
a- -o bilabial
dental
ex -£ dental/alveolar
M.. n palatal
CQ ?c velar
-o glottal
H- H- CL
6 § S
labio-dental
dental
dental/alveolar
alveolar
palato-alveolar
palatal
velar
glottal
variable place
to
00
Language • 3 8 . 8
4J > > >
g 3 3 3
Azerbaijani (059) cfl i-l U U (fl (0 (0
rH ja C C I-I i-t i-i
Chuvash (060) -H cfl a; ai co id a)
xi rn T3 TJ a a. >
higher mid
lower mid
low
high
higher mid
"o"2
lower mid
O O O O . O M O - M O C L O I - ' O
H 3 H 3 3 3 H
8
1 • ? • 3. &
bilabial
labio-dental
dental
5 S 5
% Z *t dental/alveolar
dental/alv. velarized
Q. CL ^+
pala to-alveolar
palatal
palatal nasalized
velar
vl.
vl.
vd.
voic
voic
voic
3 0) pi
8. 8. p. H- C O H- 01 H"
3
jl CO P P> C
3 3 1-"• H- 3 rt
(-•
3 0)
a
H. =
s
s- ft ,h H. t? <
8
ive
ate
ive
t
cative
nons
sibi
sibi
voiced p
8 -a
I I S !
M> HI a i (S S
i <
£ £ M, h
itive
itive
*
-icative
o- -o bilabial
labio-dental
dental
a. r-t- alveolar
palato-alveolar
palatal
to 7? velar
>o glottal
variable place
labial-velar
t i- & & t i. i i
HJi I I *• i
bilabial
labio-dental
dental
dental/alveolar
palato-alveolar
palatal
velar
variable place
8 • '. is 0 « 8 8
O H>
? I i I? •
? »»a •
l— n 13 IN w jo- v* dental
palato-alveolar
palatal
O M H S 8 » D P » » S » I I
8 8 8 S ? j? £ £ 8
S R S J J
3 T^ ^ o- -o^ bilabial
3 W
°^ -: "C ! I £ * ^~ i dental/alveolar
«—• °" Jt, palato-alveolar
<-• palatal
x ca *- velar
o. a. o o <x M a* a. M o
0 * 0 " o"
(B » &. O- H- K O H- H- CL
s 3 s g S £ &g g .
p > p > H . t o i » P i c r i t t p i o
M M P t r T r T r r
p ) t e £ ' l <
•0 13 Kt K| ft Hi M>
O O O D Hi fi ri
°-I <-••" dental/alveolar
palat o-alveolar
vo
voi
I o5 ^. h
g 8. P. R 0 p. 8. n>
C n
n> rt 8 >o 0)
Ian
Ian
los
s s
E£ 0> 1 <
s*
ff
s "
ricate
Ericat
Ericat
native
ricate
bilabial
dental/alveolar
palato-alveolar
palatal
velar
voiced t
vd. cent
voiced p
vd. late
voiceles
vl. nons
voiced n
vl. sibi
vd. sibi
C (to p> o
• p. e $ g p. p 3 CO "O
L
L
Lve
Losive
Lant fricat
approximan
affricate
approximan
. affricate
labio-dental
jo. y+ dental
dental/alveolar
palato-alveolar
palatal
m 7T velar
M CO 3
0> H- O S1 S IL
H 13 fl>
Z p. 8.
%I I I
' J J
5 5" 2
•o_ -o bilabial
-+ <•+ dental/alveolar
palato-alveolar
palatal
7T 7? velar
variable place
labial-velar
I? &
8. 8. g. < g. 8.
I I nit
- !r If S
•o -o bilabial
r+ r+ dental/alveolar
palato-alveolar
palatal
7T 7T velar
velar nasalized
variable place
labial-velar
i - h h h o * O
M »-t 0 ) t 3
n
€ § 51
8
II
bilabial
labio-dental
n- dental
denta1/alveolar
r+ alveolar
palatal
O- O O O M
P- r I ° s
ST 8. 8. 8. S. & S& -o8. 8£ IS
2
M n M
§ £ CO (B (C
H n
labio-dental
*+ dental
alveolar
palat o-alveolar
"+ retroflex
palatal
to ?r velar
labial-velar
g. g.
S fc1 8. 8. " 8. E
H S M S M
P». » H- CO F
< a oo"
labio-dental
]Q- k+ dental
°- ^ alveolar
n palatal
ID x- velar
labial-velar
982
a
3 rt C
2 2 ST 61
5
M M "O 5
. imp Losive
iced 3losive
( U P
"o "d ••
icele 3S plOS Lve
B 8 I
g- g- c
. non sibilan t fricat
sibilan t fricat
3 3 n
cr> o- -o bilabial
labio-dental
a- r+ alveolar
palatal
to 7T velar
IQc 7c
c velar labialized
CQ-V 7T-. labial-velar
a- a. o o a. M d. M o o
r 5 fj c- a. .S DS - i Ho - wp 3- Q
s to Da ir -o '
3( o n cr D" 3 3 m
H i - t f i l U M M H - H . I - ' t O
( U B J H - C O B i t U O ' O ' O
M M M 0> 3 3 H - j J - C O ' D
M M rt rt M M H- M
(U ft) CD 03 < O
rt
^ 3 ?. n. " $•
a s
g B
s s g; ?
H» H> O fi
ft) ft) < < ft) ft)
3 3 n n rt rt
<' <
bilabial
labio-dental
alveolar
palatal
labial-velar
Language
voiceless plosive
voiceless plosive
voiced plosive
voiced plosive
vd. implosive
vl. sibilant affricate
high
higher mid
high
mid "e" "§" "5"
higher mid
lower mid
lower mid e 5
low
low a
0 H-
g s 883
» "
< 8.
S. S
o- -o bilabial
labio-dental
ex -+ dental/alveolar
t+- o palatal
7T velar
uvular
o glottal
labial-velar
f
s s.
labio-dental
^+ alveolar
c+. o palatal
variable place
labial-velar
to
00
VO
Language
•3 7 S
Temne (109) Dagbani (110)
.3
labic
bilat
1 2. 2.
>
voiceless plosive "t"
V
voiced nasal
high
higher mid
long
mid
high
lower mid
higher mid
low
low
o D H a M O o 06Z
£ £
B" H Jl £ fi"
g < < o) a>
labio-dental
I j dental/alveolar
palato-alveolar
palatal
velar
labial-velar
8. 8.
MIS
-o bilabial
labio-dental
-+ alveolar
n palatal
* velar
variable place
*v labial-velar
Language cd
g
Ewe (114)
Bariba (113)
labio-di
dental
bilabiai
voiceless plosive voiceless plosive
voiced flap
higher mid
lower mid
low high i
higher mid
lower mid
low
|_i |_4 CL !-• O 363
i i r I
8. 8.
i e e
f &
o- -o bilabial
labio-dental
oT ^ dental/alveolar
M^ £ palatal
Q.. o p a l a t a l labialized
c c
ca x- velar
variable place
lal>ial-velar
3 f-
co 9 ce H-
I s. S §
HI a"
& 5 i o
I I o <
S R1 H Hi
O* Hi R1 »
3. P. ?
I ? !
o- -c^ -a bilabial
cr -o TI bilabial palatalized
labio-dental
ex r+_ -+ dental/alveolar
dental/alv. palatalized
alveolar
palato-alveolar
palatal
co 7^ 7r velar
<ot ^t ^t velar labialized
variable place
variable place labialized
J) labial-velar
e- ° | * r £ r ?• ?• r ° r ° €63
3 I §
r ,° :? :? n ? a
bilabial
labio-dental
dental
dental/alveolar
alveolar
palato-alveolar
palato-alv. labialized
€ £
palatal
velar
variable place
labial-palatal
-g") labial-velar
< < < < < < < < < f
f L C L O H - ' C L C l - M O O fD
h - > n c o ? c o c o ( D ( t > B
( a a . H . O H - H - C L I - ' p.
bilabial
labio-dental
alveolar
retroflex
palatal
8 8. S.
3 s S
labio-dental
-+ alveolar
palatal
labial-velar
n co n>
r cr
g 8. to
g •o OJ
CO
lant
los:
H*
lanl
o
l-h l-h 9 § (t>
i-l i-l
J. p.
M.
2 2
t rt H-
ft) ID
S"
ft rt
bilabial
labio-dental
alveolar
pala to-alveolar
palatal
velar
variable place
labial-velar
Language
3 lar
Tarok (121) Amo (122)
labio-d
bilal3 ial
labi(
palaito-al
palai:al
velai
bilabia
dental/
palato-
palatal
cd
high
low low
963
S 3 H -
•o. -o bilabial
Q| P) labio-dental
^ <-£ dental/alveolar
alveolar
palatal
JT velar
variable place
labial-velar
1 H1 i i i ' i!
3. rj. rj.
o o o
bilabial
labio-dental
dental/alveolar
alveolar
palato-alveolar
palatal
velar
labial-velar
: a >
2 i r r § j ! t o ' £ S p ' ( £ ' o ' E
ss
K
H- If E rt ^ ^" ^
S to to rt to to
<
m
3 -o bilabial
^3 bilabial prenasalized
X •* labio-dental
£ =1 w "+ dental/alveolar
^ dental/alv. prenasalized
OJ •—• ^ palat o-alveolar
^ palato-alv. prenasalized
<-• "a palatal
velar
° <nJ *
velar prenasalized
^ variable place
* labial-velar
< < < < r^
2. 1 § O D. H a M a
O
§ r 5 H!
eject
jibil
Jibil
Jibil
o
id tr ill
s r P.
asp. lat.
ated
Lat. affrj
centr al ai
Later al aj
a
click
•a o> o *o Hi •
o o H
bilant J
ater al eject
lonsi bilant i
aspirated cli
3 R co
rt rt M
p r1'
i
•icate
click
jximant
s«
affric
Lcative
affricat.ed click
Lcative
TJ bilabial
labio-dental
^ alveolar
palato-alveolar
palatal
?r velar
variable place
labial-velar
P* O M M 0- M O. M 862
t - ' ( D O ) P 3 P ( n c n
( t t O - H - O O O H - H -
r* a* 3 3 3 o" T
^ s
-o bilabial
labio-dental
r+ alveolar
palato-alveolar
palatal
7T velar
variable place
labial-velar
M O - O
S. 8. & P. g g- 8.
xi bilabial
labio-dental
r+ dental/alveolar
palatal
variable place
labial-velar
Language
3 3
Gbeya (129) 3 3 Zande (130)
labio-dental
bilabial
bilabial pre
voiceless plosive
I!
P voiceless plosive t
m
voiced plosive b b voiced plosive d 9b
vd. implosive b vl. nonsibilant fricative
voiced flap
high
higher mid e 6
lower mid
low
& i t 2 && ooe
' ' K* B* * *
S ST 8. 8. ?. p. K* K*
a ?? g 2 5
M 9 8 S §§
M
i : :
labio-dental
dental/alveolar
dental/alv. palatalized
palatal
velar
labial-velar
O M S S O J C D S M C O S S H fc
1
O P J O . D . H - H - O H - H - D - H H- |P>
I I I I if it I f r t I I
o o o o H » » i > i n
3
%, or -o -bilabial
"•{_, labio-dental
-= "L =C NI °l % -f dental/alveolar
*~* ^ "X palato-alveolar
retroflex
palatal
glottal
variable place
labial-velar
O* O. O o P- M M o O toe
S f 8. S. {5. {2. 2 8. £
i - l i - I H f t l l - ' l - ' H - t - ' C n
0 ) ( U H - C O ( U P O * 0
M (-• M Pi 3 3 H - W T 3
•an HI M, 3 n> S
II I? S
labio-dental
'f dental/alveolar
alveolar
palato-alveolar
"+ retroflex
palatal
^" velar
-» glottal
labial-velar
P- ?• 3 ?• P- ?• ?• I I ? Is
5* 5*
bilabial
labio-dental
alveolar
palato-alveolar
palatal
velar
variable place
labial-velar
Language Language
voiced nasal
voiced flap
lower mid
low
high
low ae a v
Language
Language
Nubian (206)
Nyangi (207)
ii r i t
vd. lateral approx.
voiced trill
ii | II
vd. central approx.
vd. lateral approx.
high
high
highe r mid
mid
lower mid
low
low
i i ?•
C O W 00 M. ft ft ft
8. 8. *rnM cr cr p- B t* (L M
ft rt 3 ft ft p. H>
p> p. cp pi (u B> (U 5 I 5* £
ft> CO H- O
O* M> p> PI
ss
& 8
ft ft
s s
labio-dental
H- dental
dental/alveolar
alveolar
palato-alveolar
palatal
7T velar
uvular
variable place
labial-velar
o. o o o
8. 8. 8.
sr P. I I? I
I I
n palatal
x- velar
labial-velar
goc
3* 0*
£ 5
5 3
bilabial
labio-dental
dental
alveolar
palatal
velar
pharyngeal
variable place
labial-velar
It £
g sr s. s.
5 Sa g
8
!I
or -o bilabial
Q. ^ alveolar
<-!-. palatal
to 7r velar
?- ?- t I I 9oe
M 8
" § H. 2. P
P M E S 6
•8 £
r i:
s s
bilabial
labio-dental
dental/alveolar
palato-alveolar
palatal
velar
variable place
labial-velar
!I! I S 1
i.
labio-dental
^ dental
alveolar
palato-alveolar
° palatal
*" velar
•° glottal
variable place
labial-velar
i ti I Is
f S. S. 8 8
I 1
to
H-
i->
O
p. p. £
3 3
dental
't dental/alveolar
p ala to-alveo1ar
o palatal
^" velar
variable place
labial-velar
0.1-1 hh Hi Q.
£ S2
S:
bilabial
labio-dental
dental
dental/alveolar
palato-alveolar
palatal
velar
glottal
variable place
labial-velar
80C
(-• t-> O I-
» p. £ I ~I IB P
5.S g
I
cr» cr XJ bilabial
3 bilabial prenasalized
jo. ir+ dental
<4 dental/alveolar
2
s
=> dental/alv. prenasalized
<+, n palatal
^•D palatal prenasalized
co x velar
^o velar prenasalized
*o^ -o*' labial-velar
O l - ' S S ( B P » H . S S
I II
sf ! * I II I
•S -S
I •s i
8
o, cr ^ bilabial
cr3 bilabial prenasalized
o, a. ^+ alveolar
of alveolar prenasalized
palato-alveolar
palatal
t^3 palatal prenasalized
co 77 velar
(0° velar prenasalized
variable place
labial-velar
6oe
O P - H -
i-h i-( 0)
00 f i H - M i
3 3
3 bilabial prenasalized
labio-dental
dental
<=£ dental/alveolar
-= ••=
^ dental/alv. prenasalized
palato-alveolar
palatal
7T (o velar
yf velar prenasalized
•>> glottal
variable place
labial-velar
p. H-
M S S
iu a. a.
£ PL 5
« p. « 00
S3
labio-dental
alveolar
palato-alveolar
palatal
velar
variable place
labial-velar
M M Ou M O O oie
to s p. n n n
2. 8.
rt 3
H- O
CT4 CD
I E &
i II I
t^ o- x> bilabial
r* a. ^+ alveolar
palato-alveolar
palatal
7T (Q x- velar
-° glottal
variable place
labial-velar
O O I P O
3*
no TO
?I I
Jf o- bilabial
labio-dental
»• i°- i^- i^ dental
1
% 1 % 1 't, 1 ^o dental pharyngealized
alveolar
palato-alveolar
palatal
<? «° *" ^ velar
_Q ,• uvular
pharyngeal
7f "° glottal
variable place
labial-velar
< < < ue
bilabial
labio-dental
dental
alveolar
palato-alveolar
palatal
velar
pharyngeal
glottal
variable place
labial-velar
03 (D co
I|
labio-dental
<-+ ex r+ dental/alveolar
alveolar
palato-alveolar
palatal
7r^ m 7T velar
TT^ ID £ TT^ velar labialized
variable place
variable place labialized
labial-velar
OJ
Language
Language
o 73
•3 Socotri (254)
Hebrew (253)
•8 £ 1 3
t-l T) CX Pu voiceless plosive "t"
voiceless plosive
voiced plosive "d"
voiced plosive "t"
vl. elective stop
vl. sibilant affric. "ts"
vl. nonsibilant fric.
vd. sibilant affric.
mid
lower mid
high
low
O O CL a. M a. (-• o o (t> eie
. . . . p. p. o
8. 8. P. g § £ £ 8. * *"
asa
3 3 p. p. o 3 M
: :£s:: ?
1 l-» H- H-
labio-dental
dental
alveolar
palato-alveolar
TO 3 0),
palatal
velar
uvu lar
glottal
variable place
4 § I s I wS 2. § §£
8 ww S
M B> M
M> H cr* rt O*
S g* g
o- bilabial
labio-dental
% ? "\ dental/alveolar
"^ -^ dental/alv. pharyngealized
palato-alveolar
palatal
^ ?T ^ velar
^ velar pharyngealized
pharyngeal
variable place
labial-velar
Language
C
Tuareg (257) 11
a f- p. >
Somali (258)
.o o cu i i
voiceless plosive k voiceless plosive
s 3 I
voiced plosive vl. aspirated plosive
voiced nasal
high
mid
lower mid
low
I I
8 8
I E S E S 8.
P-h -h 3 pi 09 (B
labio-dental
-+ dental/alveolar
palato-alveolar
retroflex
palatal
^ velar
^ velar labialized
-° u\ular
-° c uvular labialized
labial-velar
O O o ?. 5.
lat
a. PL < P. f Q p- a" D- M
Tar
I 3 M CO
CO » 3 £ (D a n
C H V rt
Ki f
ect.
sive
3 g. s
3
)losiv
P- £ n H P
ric
ric
tiv
t < a »
bilabial
labio-dental
r-t- CL, Q. -+ alveolar
palato-alveolar
palatal
velar
velar labialized
uvular labialized
pharyngeal
glottal
variable place
labial-velar
JBXO9AXB/XB5U9P +- J> ^
+- ^
^J ^
+• ^
XBxqBXjq -°
0) p,
•> O
XB33OX8
JBXO9AXB-O3BX Bd
I i I II
5! S ^ -3 2 2
•H M-i JJ M O. a
- H S U ^ ^ "S S* S*
a . W C - H c ' f f l O r H r H
O c o ^ a m c o c a c d n )
S a 2 g 2 c h 2 3
316 •g | -s § -a n -s 5 s
"o "o T3 -H rl *O "^
bilabial
labio-dental
dental/alveolar
palato-alveolar
palatal
velar
variable place
labial-velar
labio-dental
9J "t dental/alveolar
palato-alveolar
palatal
<° * velar
•° glottal
variable place
l&ial-velar
8. |L
o- a4 a-
H g
H- |H-
If
alveolar
palato-alveolar
palatal
velar
uvular
variable place
labial-velar
to 3 r v cr -a (D n>
H 2 2 f P w H P oT cr » oT S ? o "
Lve
sive
3 sr
:op
Lve
it a cfricate
it a Efricate
ricative
ricative
. approximant
/I. sib. fr
Kanakuru (270)
Ngizim (269)
voiceless plosive p t
voiced plosive b d
voiceless plos. p vd. implosive 6 cf
voiced plosive b vd. nonsibilant fric.
laryng. vd. plos. b vl. sibilant fric.
vl. nonsib. fric. vd. sibilant fric.
vd. nonsib. fric. vd. lateral fric. I:
vl. sib. fric. voiced nasal m n
vd. sib. fric. vd. lateral approx. 1
vl. lat. fric. vd. central approx. J
vd. lat. fric.
voiced nasal m
voiced trill
high
voiced flap mid
vd. lat. approx. low
long
high
mid
low
0 * 0 * 0 O M M I O . M M i - 1 O
O M
. M- H- • • rt> • • • H) H-
S J S. S. H; g l H; S £ |
ft <B rt 3
r
Br
OO
MM
]— 1=J
palato-alveolar
'..a. .o. .-+ retroflex
palatal
vela:
glottal
variable place
labial-velar
£ • '
Khar
• s s-
voiced
voiced
(I)
voicel
(D H- O 3" H- CO H- 3* O.
rt (0 M, H- CO
P CO
Di (B C
(301
Jirat
* M 3 H- °* 3 "
plos ive
s.
lant f
b. aff
losive
t affr
>ilant affr
o o t % % 0
sib. c
? voiced pic
t fricc
approj
appro:
H- n* 2 P.
;ive
£ f
-o_ -o bilabial
jr+ i-t- dental
dental/alveolar
palato-alveolar
.^ .'-H retroflex
palatal
^ ^- velar
variable place
labial-velar
JBXO3AXB 4- +- S3
•H T3
•a -a I
" M S ! Q « c d o ) n )
3J C
HJI >
> ,3
& 6
t 5
I "S ^5
c
xaxjoaaaj +" *
1 ti I
II III
-S I 1
324 2 ? ?
3 8
o .
<H — "I
S I I
2 :
325
Language
•3 8
Alawa (354)
•a
Maranungku (355)
I
voiced plosive voiceless plosive
low
high
mid
low
f I I
S TJ
"o "o *o •« -d
> > > > >
327
Language
voiced trill
lower mid
low
high
mid
lower mid
low
Language
o u u
Western Desert (360) U cfl cfl
Nyangumata (361)
voiceless plosive p t tJ t
voiceless plosive
voiced nasal
voiced nasal
voiced flap
voiced trill
vd. lateral approx.
vd. lateral approximant I IJ I
vd. central approx.
J j
high
low
high
low
I I oee
1
P.
" P.
2 «? 8S S
8
bilabial nasally-released
]— ]^ dental
dental nasally-released
alveolar
alveolar nasally-released
palato-alveolar
palato-alv. nasally-released
retroflex
retroflex nasally-released
palatal
velar
velar nasally-released
labial-velar
& I i t2
H« H« f*
O M 8 fl> S
(D (U CX CL M
3 rt n
rt (6 rt 3 tc
H H ^ BJ (0
dental/alveolar
retroflex
palatal
velar
labial-velar
JBX0BAXB/XB3U9P
1 I 5 3 s
? 1
o o i a )
a) I-H
1 i i
JBXO9AXB/XBV9P +-
U
T3 Cd
H
g
331
IBXO3AXB +-
§ 8
•3 "8
1 1
2 S
-a -g
332
I I o) c >w
0) M
t t I I
5 3 -O
C w <u
2 2 £
•g 6
333 1 I
< T 3 H -
JCO o3 3 -o_ -o bilabial
labio-dental
dental
-^ -+ dental/alveolar
palatal
7T 7T velar
7T 7T velar labialized
TT^ velar palatalized
•o glottal
variable place
labial-velar
n n n o
|_i (_. O OJ B H - H - C D r t n d
H H H- M <
o o o HI Hi o r e
XX H H CO
liio-dental
dental
Ji dental/alveolar
n palatal
*• velar
^ glottal
variable place
^ o b
gee
8 8 . . %
^ 3- 3 - - - ^ 3 m (u n>
? g M O. H- a q o o c n H - a o D - c n M
P- CM
sr sr
p 3
$ s
bilabial
dental/alveolar
Q) OJ 0)
alveolar
palato-alveolar
palatal
velar
uvular
glottal
variable place
labial-velar
< < rt
cr x> -o bilabial
labio-dental
-+ ^+ alveolar
velar
glottal
variable place
9ee
sr s. sr p. g §
rt rt cr a 3
8 § 8 P S &s S
labio-dental
•^. 't dental/alveolar
palato-alveolar
palatal
^ ?r velar
•° glottal
variable place
vl. n
vl. s
vl. a
cr
laryngea
vd. nons
voiceles
cr
vd. cpnt ral
vl. sibiIan
vd. sibiIan
N" ? •a
asal
L
Lungchow (406)
3 M, 3 » » S
Ml
<
vl. 1ateral frica tive
ricat
icate
icate
ximan
vd. 1ateral approximan
ricat
ative
plos
Language Language
Labia
ital
.a •S g s
voiceless plosive "t" voiceless plosive P t
vl. sibilant affric. "ts" voiced plosive b
vl. nonsibilant fric. vl. sibilant affric.
long
high
low mid
lower mid
low
O - D - O O H> M M M M
see
S » a. a ?• o "• H- "M
3 rr v 3 o* a* o
! — ' » — ' ( — ' & > 3 H> Mi p t;
5* t "O "O Mi p . pj II
| | <" £ 2 S «
< *<; to
3 S
bilabial
dental
dental/alveolar
alveolar
palatal
velar
glottal
variable place
labial-velar
DO 3 3 C 0 S S * IC
1
( D O J i - n M i D i D ' H ' H ' O Q H ' H ' a ' h - BJ lp>
p r t M i M i o ' c r s p c r ' a ' m co K5
I-1 3 3* D*
Q H- H» H-
-o bilabial
labio-dental
Jg- j - i- dental
dental/alveolar
retroflex
palatal
7T velar
variable place
labial-velar
VO
Language
Cham (All)
Malay (412)
2 -S
voiceless plosive P "t" t c k ?
voiceless plosive P "t
vl. aspirated plos. ph ..th» th ch kh
voiced plosive b "d
voiced plosive b "d"
vl. nonsibilant fric.
vl. nonsibilant fric.
vl. sibilant fric. "s
vl. sibilant fric.
voiced nasal m "n
voiced nasal
voiced trill
voiced r-sound
vd. lateral approx. "1
vd. lateral approx.
vd. central approx.
high I
higher mid
high
mid
higher mid
low
mid
lower mid
low
Language Language
high
high mid
mid
lower mid
low
M a- M
IB
S 5
B » »
r+
alveolar
of +• o*
palato-alveolar
palatal
7T velar
•° glottal
variable place
labial-velar
O O O
(B pl O . H - O H > H - a M
(B Q] p) <! O
T3 'd M i 3 j j ( D ( B C 0
*T3 ^ fi rt hh Hi H*
g s ? s1 a £
"° bilabial
labio-dental
dental/alveolar
"*• alveolar
retroflex
palatal
^ velar
*° glottal
variable place
labial-velar
s I a
C 1-4 -H
TO X i Xi
Xi C
r-4 T3 -H O
01 CO CO (3
Maori (423)
Hawaiian (424)
voiceless plosive
voiceless plosive
vl. nonsibilant fric.
vl. nonsibilant fric.
voiced nasal
voiced nasal
voiced trill
vd. lateral approx.
vd. central approx.
vd. central approx.
high
high
mid
mid
lower mid
lower mid
low
low
9VE
to 3 to 3
e 2 « a
i - l i - l r t o ' r t i t t H i ' d
O* O* H, H! I? o*
a 1
labio-dental
dental/alveolar
palatal
velar
uvular
labial-palatal
labial-velar
< < < < < < < < < <
n M n H c a p i B c a i D n
§. s p. s j g p. s
BE n <• & to <t a
p 3 (D rt rt (D
labio-dental
-+ dental/alveolar
palato-alveolar
palatal
7T velar
-K^ velar labialized
variable place
labial-velar
CO
Language s
Language
rH CO
voiced nasal
high
vd. lateral approx.
lower mid
vd. central approx.
low
high
mid
low
oo
Language
"3
o
I I
Amoy (504) Fuchow (505)
palat o-alve
I
bilab ial
velar
dentd
i.t t. voiceless plosive "t"
voiceless plosive p k
high 1, y
lower mid
low
M 8 » 3 ( D 3 0 > [ »
MI 3 H< 3
j ^ r t c r r t
p> Hi
si n
T> bilabial
dental/alveolar
palatal
variable place
§ R S. S.
& &£ S B g
s* rn s.
3 3 S ft 8. «
Hi hh M
R, £ 3. S
"° bilabial
'-1- dental
alveolar
palatal
^ velar
variable place
labial-velar
:
n
^ bilabial
dental
't dental/alveolar
alveolar
palatal
•*" velar
variable place
& I
n n 3 3 0> »
3 S
to 3
M rt
f fJ H, 6 B
8. S1
•^ is *d t(
f^ 3 3 33 H> H- O
S X X X
-o bilabial
dental
't dental/dveolar
palato-alveolar
palatal
^ velar
•° glottal
variable place
labial-velar
1
Q . O - O I- D. M O. H1 M O M O ISC
f D ( D O * H - O O ( - " • ( » H - O . C O t - '
a r t a - a s c r - o c r ti <»
rt (D 3 F- CO W F- • p. T> H> BJ
CO Q3 O 03
§
H- § to S -o
to cu & pi • < a. o
• d i j i-h 3 3 p p > r s ca
33 J ^ ^ ^ S ; ? s* S"
-o bilabial
labio-dental
-T dental /alveolar
palato-alveolar
palatal
7T velar
-o uvular
•>>_, glottal
variable place
labial-velar
O M n > r t > C D 3 c n a ) C n ( t > » r t > "d
( B O l C L O . H - O H - C O H . a o i l - ' D*
9 r t & p V y v T3 n o
cr -o_ -o bilabial
^ ^- 1 dental/alveolar
palato-alveolar
palatal
co ^ 7T velar
•° glottal
variable place
labial-velar
Ao (512)
Tiddin Chim (513)
voiceless plosive
voiceless plosive P t
vl. nonsibilant affric.
vl. aspirated plos. ph
vl. sibilant fric.
voiced plosive b d
vd. sibilant fric.
vl. sibilant affric. ts
vd. lateral fric.
vl. nonsibilant fric.
voiced nasal
vd. nonsibilant fric. V
vd. lateral approx.
vl. sibilant fric. s
vd. central approx.
vd. sibilant fric. z
voiced nasal m n
vd. lateral approx. I
high
laryng. vd. lat. approx. 1
higher mid
laryng. vd. cent, approx.
low
high
lower mid
ese
I I ! S" I ?
Hi M h(
jo. 1-+ dental
alveolar
velar
glottal
variable place
labial-velar
|< < < < < < < < < < w If
M M V p) 9 9 H- CD T)
8 § ? P ff
palatal
variable place
n H M
O O
•a -a | *
£ 3 -3 °
CL Q_ CO £
> rH 0) TH ^ ^| MM
* 1
•
8
H O )
TJ «
O r
fl)
H r H
§
W ^
""*
1 4 -
•
t l
**
- l
S'lfl' § I o
354
•H -H O O
I "8 "8
1 I
JBXO3AXB/XB3U3P
g 8
(-1 M
O. P.
•H 6 C
CO (X -H -H C <D 4J
O rH T3 O T3 TJ
355
I
Language
Language 1 I :
•H t-t 4)
iH 0) N j;
3 8 2 Sentani (603)
Washkuk (602)
5 S. 3 -3 5
•a ^
voiceless plosive "t"
8. 2
voiced plosive "d"
voiceless plosive k
vl. nonsibilant fric.
voiced plosive
voiced nasal
vl. sibilant affric.
vd. central approx.
vd. sibilant affric.
low
high i
higher mid
mid
lower mid
low
8 8 L "B
a. p. p. o
1-1 P H- CD
51 B. S s s§ s
t a. TO
variable place
< < < <
O M M O
§
rt H
°" 3°* H&
-
S »
CO CO
H I Pi M H- CO
M O (JJ 3 M- TJ
•° bilabial
palatal
variable place
labial-velar
Language
lower mid
low
high
higher mid
lower mid
low
3 TO -o bilabial
dental/alveolar
°- -*• alveolar
palatal
glottal
< < < < <
M O. M O O
S %
• »
3 H- I*
3. 3-
o- -o bilabial
°- -+ alveolar
palatal
velar
glottal
variable place
P. M -H
8 g 8
o 5
« C co
1 ^^
•e. E
^ s 6 rH
"S "8
360 » §
Language s Language
•3 3
Pawaian (612) Dani (613)
nasalized
high i T u high
low
I
3
C
g **
Language 5 u •i *a
ren
zed
CO CO
P. d N
8 ]j « C
p. > >
Wantoat (615) cd
Daribi (616)
Ii 3?
labi
labi
ntal
atal
1 Of
lar lat
•H -H CU cu a
£> £i T3
voiceless plosive
voiceless plosive "t" k k
vl. aspirated plos.
m
voiced plosive K " n H'
vl. nonsibilant fric.
vl. sibilant fric.
vl. sibilant fric.
vd. sibilant fric.
voiced nasal
voiced nasal
voiced flap
high
nasalized
mid "e 9 o
high T 0
low
mid "e" "o"
low
£ o I 2. 2. i
ft M, 3 S g S
M g 3*
O Hi »
-+• alveolar
palatal
variable place
labial-velar
<
9 a. o t~* ?* o.
ft>
°, o (B
n
n to ca
3
S. o* cr n>
ral
los
p> < o
*d IT S,
H- Hi
8 ? H-
"° bilabial
dental/alveolar
palatal
labial-velar
A Kunimaipa (620)
bil
voiceless plosive P "t" voiceless plosive
voiced plosive b "d" voiced plosive
voiced nasal
voiced flap
low
high i
higher mid
low
< < <
a- o
vl.
vd.
voi
voi
CO CO f (B
8 8. s cr cr ID
o
-> cn
cr
Llan
I 1 0) B S •a
n> o
i-h
H rt H,
palatal
labial-velar
S 8 3 s 8 S
>d (a H-
labio-dental
-^ alveolar
variable place
M 8 »
CD 0. H
H« O M
labio-dental
dental/alveolar
variable place
< < <
3. 3. a
r> o o
I S. S.
3 cr XJ
"t dental/alveolar
glottal
8. § 8.
<; a- TO
-o bilabial
•os bilabial labialized
•o_ bilabial palatalized
bilabial prenasalized
bHabial labialized & prenasalized
labio-dental
^t dental/alveolar
-^ dental/alv; labialized
-f dental/alv. palatalized
dental/alv. prenasalized
an ox
dental/alv. labialized & prenasalized
p; o]
palatal
7T velar
-^ velar labialized
^ velar palatalized
velar prenasalized
velar labialized & prenasalized
labial-velar
> t
\ - < > - ' < y - < < < < < < < < < < < < 35 1ST1
( t i O . O J C U f U O I - ' M h - ' & . M M I - ' H ' M H ' O p> p
«3 ' 3 ' .3 l
ft l >
o a K
3 o 3 H > : 3 ( B t - 9 l - t - * H - C f l p > C f l f l > p > ( D 01 [C
s
v
i 5 t I S E 2 & '• 2 2 '• • j? S s
• H« • l - ' N ( U I - ' H » < - ^ l - » M < - J » H ' 0 < r t d
§ ^
( D H * M ( D fD a* rt (» (t> I-"
IS 3
r t
T
i
3
-
r
l
t
"
*
» i
d
a
^
.
H
O
- r t » H > i - h »
H > i
(
H
D
)
i
i
-
-
h
h
r
O
t '
I
d
- l
H
<
-
• s ,g s ; • j s. ^ s1 gj 3 g. ^ s •
I ' ? * e ? 3! ' ' S ? *°
g 8 ?
13 3
-«3. ^ bilabial
1 l 3
~ ^ =1 I •€ X — ^ "* 'f. 't dental/alveolar
- oi s = ? "
^t, CX tlX palato-alveolar
ic
- ^ "° fj, ^ ° palatal
~s ** ** velar labialized
Q
-^ •%. uvular
Q
s: -^ °s uvular labialized
•° glottal
variable place
labial-velar
en 3
H" O
a- 3
H. 8
f I S S*
rt B (u co
bilabial
-+ -+ Q. dental/alveolar
palato-alveolar
palatal
velar
velar labialized
uvular
uvular labialized
S £ «
glottal
labial-velar
vd.
vd.
vd.
vl.
vl.
vl.
vl.
vl.
69€
CO to
voiced
voicel
la
la
vl. as
la
la
la
si
si
vd. no
vl. as
si
vd. ce
vl. noi
rt 3 rt> p-
to pi to pi to & v n cu PI n> co pi
f rt
S ^
rt
3* a" pi PI H. g » P. 8. S1
3 it i-i
sr pr rt • to
p-
X 2 rt to to iii 'U
« n »
P- Oi rt
alveolar
palato-alveolar
palatal
velar
velar labialized
glottal
variable place
labial-velar
i
3 3 h - P i M c a P i c o B P i 3
o o w c o p i p - c o p . o c o o
it in
3 p. CO O CO
p. rt CO
crp-
>
P . < O
f
3
» n T
3 rt
.. a s
f f f
dental
r+ r+_ dental/alveolar
-*2 dental/alveolar velarized
palato-alveolar
palatal
7T 7T velar
7r 7r velar labialized
c c
>o glottal
variable place
vl.
vl.
vl.
vl.
o. 03
voiced
la
sil
sil
sil
vd. noi
vl. noi
•o
laryngea
vl. ejec
voiceles
0 (B H- CO CO
vd. cent!
03 P H- cn tu tu cr cr n cn 03* H.
cr
2
M I
ect
3lOS
03 CD
v pas
?
doq
(B
• 0
"frica
Lcativ
II
Jlosiv
-icati
. fric
(B
Llan : fricati
sal
a f f r ica
i f f r i c ate
-+ dental/alveolar
alveolar
palato-alveolar
retroflex
palatal
yr velar
7T velar labialized
•o glottal
variable place
labial-velar
0 rt o* T)
rt rt> 0
i-( i-( pa
(B 03 35 H- CIS
H, M»
palato-alveolar
palato-alv. labialized
n
palatal
velar
velar labialized
-° uvular
^ glottal
variable place
var. place labialized
labial-velar
M M M O
§ 3 £ s
(B 93 3 (5
a* a* H-
§ § . s
D. *"- K, 3*
§ 3 ft
' 3
en C rt ro o en
]-+ ]-H dental
S 8 2. <
U3 C fl>
^ -+ alveolar
palatal
rt 3 03 n> ^ *• velar
-^ uvular
(D 03 cu sr
•>> glottal
|->- M 3 n>
rr O rt pj"
variable place
labial-velar
O M M
8
S 3
se
3 3 3 3 rt 3
i-i rt H H ii
§ •?§. s
bilabial
alveolar
palatal
velar
uvular
glottal
variable place
labial-velar
Language
Language
Wintu (709)
Maidu (708)
voiceless plosive
high
mid
low
r> n n o
drt cr 9 5* a" <p °* ST
3 " M 2 M H- ' M rt "^ CO
? eu IN
•a TJ H rt • M> rt H-
>-< M H- l-h O <J
OO O i - h O J i i T S (V
XX • r( M, p.
H- l-h O
j^- jo. i^. dental
alveolar
palato-alveolar
palatal
velar
glottal
variable place
labial-velar
I S 5 §
r+ a. r+ alveolar
palatal
velar
glottal
variable place
labial-velar
= E
Ii r r^ I
«; oi w
S, J
-a bilabial
]-•• dental
palatal
velar
glottal
variable place
labial-velar
I 1 H i Ji
rt rt> 3
2£ 1 i "
3 3
-o bilabial
-+ alveolar
palato-alveolar
palatal
7T velar
-• uvular
•o glottal
variable place
labial-velar
Language
high
higher mid
high
mid "e" "a"
mid
lover mid
low
low
5 I 6 II I | I 1 ' 1 I & I I t r J | \ "* j |
? » .* * 2 • • ti 3 £ -a ?
I I § s ? n • s*
g • r
X
°3 13 3 >e< XJICTCTXIXI bilabial
— •-> <o i a 3 N w ^ H- i a a ^ »+ alveolar
2
«—, ^ -+ palato-alveolar
ou IU c_ palatal
x^ 7c CQ 7T x- velar
c
7T CQ 7T velar labialized
•H **
•o glottal
9
p, 3- variable place
p.
* i« s labial-velar
o o
vl.
vl.
vl.
lar
voi<
H- I-1 ?
7 Ia
aspira
vl. centra
fl> n> co
vd. centra!
vd. sit>ilar
» sr
:ed plos
isib:
:eless {
IB sr g
o K ?* !? 8. S1
mid
low
g. g sr
high
: | to
t Ml
o
H
5 ive
rica
rica
fric
plos
lower m
<
higher mid
SI 5" re
cat<
approx imant
imant
nasal
to (D
IT
ica :ive
icat.ive
(0
bilabial
3 ^ dental/alveolar
i =f 0=>I = °l N: *i WI UT UT ST 4 t "i ^
palato-alveolar
palatal
velar
velar labialized
glottal
variable place
labial-velar
Language
1 i
2 -3
•* S Mixtec (728)
Mazatec (727) x S g •a -S a. "to >
0) iH rH M iH
f - l « 4 H e O p . e B
fi J « ^ ^ J voiceless plosive
voiceless plosive
s s g. 1 1 -a vl. sibilant affric.
k ?
voiced plosive vl. nonsibilant fric.
voiced trill
mid
low
high 1
higher mid
lower mid
low
i i r r I
8 £ « 3 8
fa. a. H» o a.
H 3 S S -O
ci B ST 5 S 1
C M rt M "
"° bilabial
"t dental/alveolar
palato-alveolar
palatal
*• velar
° glottal
variable place
labial-velar
MM
i f. Mr
? .. . bilabial
dental
dental/alveolar
alveolar
palato-alveolar
palatal
velar
velar labialized
uvular
uvular labialized
pharyngeal
glottal
glottal pharyngealized
variable place
labial-velar
I b O l b O P t O H ' M M O . M M M C L I - ' M O M O «! 6LZ
l
9 ( D 2 ( D S < t M C O H h - > P ) 9 c n C O P > f t < t p ) < t 3
P 3 O 0 ) H ' ( D 3 ( » H ' » ffi • (B • H- • O T 3 H - 0 ) M
p * 3 H > r t F - c n p > » < B 0 > i - « C T ' » p > C 0 H . O ( t t
» II (B »i (» M r t ( D r t | - i ( D r » O ' n H > A M ^ s
: &: &:
O-
.
0)
»o
D-
.
B>
<o
D-
•
Sss&ss&s.es*.??.
O
•
H-
o
<
it
li
F«
M
MI
< 3 K ) 3 O
» H n U
H-><
O »
2
*«•
> d " d • O 0» 1 H- CO
O f i M i - 1 3 ( U ( » l - h H - 0 > O 0 > H>
O tt> O 03 M> rt l-h o M» • Mi <
&
I ' ;' i ? S- 3-
•a
!3 3 ^ o- -o_ bilabial
J— — j D ^ - o - t n ^ - C L ^ - - + Q . J ^ - ' H - Q . ^ alveolar
JC-. <-. .0 o^ C4-. n^ palatal
velar labialized
uvular
uvular labialized
glottal
variable place
labial-velar
< < < < < < < < < < < <
O . O ' M I - ' M t - ' l - ' h - ' H I - ' O O
H- h^
>
O H - M C a 9 H ' H ' C a C a ( D ( D A
f 0 B > e > H ' O 0 > 0 ) H - H - t - u a i - '
3 r t r t o ' 9 r t r t o * c r n ) (D
u >
r t ( B ( B H - « l ' r t > ' l - n O t 0
p f i p § J u H l i S < " » " ( )
1
( a p j M i ' ^ e ' o t t O ^ ^ " o *
• O - O M M i O r t M i r t p i C O I t O )
< > r t 1
M H o " H - * i i * M i O <"
>
O O » O M i ( » h J . p ) H O (D
XX 1 Mi O Mi H-
M- Ml Ml O
bilabial
alveolar
palato-alveolar
palatal
velar labialized
uvular
uvular labialized
glottal
variable place
labial-velar
ose
r t n i S H - C O . (0 • H - O C D
d i t u w p i o ' i v o i n p i H»
M r t M I t (D r» (D M
T 3 X ) > i r t » i t i « i - h r t H -
i-l H H* "-( Hi o <
O O O t - h p j H - O J i - l ' O n )
dental
TM lw ^»
dental/alveolar
palato-alveolar
palatal
velar
velar labialized
uvular
uvular labialized
glottal
variable place
labial-velar
< < < < < < < < < $ < < *tj It- 1
O . M H ' M I - ' I - ' O . I - ' I - ' O O C BJ
H- ^ 09 [5
( S Q > H > O Q l H > H - H > ( - i . a i - > IpJ
O r t o ' S r r a ' a ' o ' i B n> w to
rt(T>H-(n««H.H.O>T30J O |»
s H, ; g 5 j ; ; ; ? ? Q
• d H - i i r r « * H i M i r t H> w
H O H - Mi Mi O < -P»
O ' O H i W f t t H H I d (D >—'
^ * H- MJ M5 o" o"
•o^ o- T3 bilabial
1—1
Jt, vS" --^i palato-alveolar
*— palatal
^ ^ ^ velar
x
€ ^ ^g. ^ velar labialized
a a
X ^ uvular
x
£ -^ ^ ^ uvular labialized
•° glottal
=*" variable place
£
labial-velar
Language
Language •a
•a .g rH O
Papago (736) •H I
iI Luiseno (737)
•a
•a ^^ 5 22 a
2 3 1 1 i ii
v oiceless plosive
voiceless plosive
voiced plosive
vl. sib. affric.
vl. sibilant affric.
vl. nonsib. fric.
vd. sibilant affric.
vd. nonsib. fric.
vl. nonsibilant fric.
vl. sib. fric.
vl. sibilant fric.
voiced nasal
voiced nasal
voiced trill
vd. lateral flap
vd. lat. approx.
vd. central approx.
vd. cent. approx.
high
lower mid high
low old
low
38C
n n M t x H - o o M
R S 2
i - f h l M
S 2 § 8
f 0D M H - H - C D
(a (i> ca is a* a *
M M 3 PI 9 H - H - T 3
pi pi n ' " " ' ^ O T I D ' O '
*O *d » MI 3 3 OD
! 3 3 3" ^ ^ S"
bilabial
labio-dental
alveolar
retrrflex
palatal
velar
velar labialized
uvular
glottal
variable place
labial-velar
M M M
i & I
8 £ I
£ S S
I I If § R
? Jf H-
-o bilabial
labio-dental
^- alveolar
palato-alveolar
palatal
jr velar
^j glottal
variable place
labial-velar
00
o o Language
Language
2 u u 2 -S Karok (741)
Tiwa (740)
rH rH 3 S t
voiced nasal
„ ,,2 short
voiced r-sound "rr long nasalised
lower mid
low
oral
high !
higher mid e
mid "9"
low a
S g. H
5 •& S
8 5 55
bilabial
labio-dental
dental
dental/alveolar
alveolar
palato-alveolar
palatal
velar
uvular
glottal
variable place
labial-velar
t
•o bilabial
]H- dental
^* alveolar
palato-«lveolar
palatal
7T velar
3rt velar labialized
-Q^ uvular
labial-velar
-o glottal
§g
i j &f
•o bilabial
dental/alveolar
,+ alveolar
palato-alveolar
palatal
^ velar
x, uvular
^j glottal
variable place
labial-velar
O M r t n i M S c n c o o j n i f c t t J
• d - d H. ft • Ht p> rt "d
H> Hi O M»
O H • O
dental
dental/alveolar
palato-alveolar
palatal
velar
glottal
variable place
labial-velar
o o o
I t "l I I I : I \
§ ? J? 8, p. ^ ro
bilabial
dental/alveolar
palato-alveolar
palatal
velar
glottal
variable place
labial-velar
M M 3*
CL O
m
8
bilabial
labio-dental
dental /alveolar
alveolar
palato-alveolar
retroflex
palatal
7? velar
^ velar labialized
•° M glottal
variable place
labial-velar
M M h-> O
8 £ 8£
s sg 1
•8 •§ i-h
»
P
g
rt
?" • CO
8. S
(0
i-( rt pj rt >O M-
o" it. pi hh •§ O1 ffi
•-( H, li CD
bilabial
dental/alveolar
palato-alveolar
palatal
velar
velar labialized
glottal
variable place
labial-velar
w c o S
1 ; $ H - H - O
c r a ' 3
r t p > 3 H C O
1 35
r ? I 33
° 5 I
I
bilabial
dental
palato-alveolar
retroflex
palatal
velar
•° glottal
variable place
0) »
c O c labial-velar
88€
S D 2 S 2 S 2 S
pi w >a P> •© pi n
M pi 3 S H- T3
n
pi co co " pi *o
T> H- Hi H- PI rt CO
I I "S ?" H ?* K 8. 5"
3 H, S p. ?. , •
H- n rt i-h O M
a H. p. nt pi o
Pi O < i-l rt CO
rt rt n <
^ -o bilabial
dental/alveolar
£ * alveolar
pa lato-alveolar
£ 7T velar
•o glottal
O- rt O.
• s • g
sr & g s
I P
B. I"
•a -o bilabial
— — 3 3 ;f -E dental/alveolar
0) d)
palato-alveolar
palatal
x- 7T velar
variable place
labial-velar
68€
S1 Z S S § S 8
£ H, 31 IS
dental
J ,? dental/alveolar
palatal
^ velar
TT velar labialized
c
•o glottal
variable place
labial-velar
g » S. S
P. S S. S
•8 « >i P> H>
S 3
bilabial
dental/alveolar
alveolar
palato-alveolar
retroflex
palatal
velar
velar labialized
glottal
labial-velar
a. o o
O fl>
n a. o- 9 o-
9 H- 9
g § Bg g
s s ?
f 5 s
bilabial
't dental/alveolar
palatal
*" velar
•>» glottal
variable place
labial-velar
OJ O * O O O H ' Q M O ' l - ' t - ' O M M M O
H < h O P C 0 < |
• r r g g ? ? r s
8 • H,
H, B B.. •" : : «• §
i-ti ii rt (» to i-h
>i
it H-
ii rt Hi I-h hh
M i H - O l ^ f l * Mi M» - •S 5*
H O 1 Mi ri h)
H. H* Mi M> H-
P P H P P
dental/alveolar
velar labialized
*° glottal
variable place
labial-velar
Language
Language
Yuchi (757)
Dakota (756)
voiceless plosive
P "t" vl. aspirated plos.
vl. aspirated plos. Ph voiced plosive "d"
voiced plosive b
vl. ejective stop "t"'
vl. ejective stop P' "t" 1
vl. sibilant affric. "ts" tj
vl. sibilant affric. tj vl. asp. sib. affric. "ts h "
vl. asp. sib. affric. tj vd. sibilant affric. "dz"
vl. sib. eject, affric. tj vl. sib. eject, affric. "ts"'
vl. nonsibilant fric.
vl. nonsibilant fric.
vl. nonsib. eject, fric.
vl. nonsib. eject, fric.
vd. nonsibilant fric.
vl. sibilant fric.
J
vl. sibilant fric. "s" J vl. sib. eject, fric.
vd. sibilant fric. "z"
vl. lateral fric.
vl. sib. eject, fric. J' vl. lat. eject, fric.
4
voiced nasal "n"
m voiced nasal
vd. lateral approx.
laryngeallzed vd. nas.
vd. central approx.
vd. lateral approx.
higher mid
low
p. H« • . . H.
2 » a o. H- o m a S p.
bilabial
alveolar
palato-alveolar
paLatal
velar
.o glottal
variable place
labial-velar
O M M I - ' M O O
2 2 S 2 g1 5= f
H H, §
H- >i rt
-o bilabial
dental
dental/alveolar
alveolar
palato-alveolar
palatal
velar
variable place
labial-velar
§
oc
x
-S . g. 3. K ff
?
bilabial
labio-dental
dental
' =C =C «C dental/alveolar
alveolar
palato-alveolar
palatal
velar
glottal
variable place
3. I. ? ?*
If * •* ?
S. &
I" 2 £
S 8 2 S t
jB p M B g a s < O
H> O
p» H -d
Hi O
cr x. bilabial
a -+ dental/alveolar
-^ dent./alv. palatalized
palato-alveolar
palatal
7T velar
•o glottal
variable place
labial-velar
f D 0 * a . H > H > Q H * Q * M H- I Co
3 CT" O* 3 O* (D TO
H r t r f r t w
( » § ' £ ' ! ^ 5 '
T 3 D . H i l - h 3 0 > ( B C 0
•d li II rt Hi P-
H y* H- n> 4
3 o- -o bilabial
q 3_ N_ w_ J
-J- CL -+ dental/alveolar
; s
•—• j ^ palato-alveolar
*— "^ palatal
x «Q x- velar
* labial-velar
r* ? 3.
oo 3 n
I - 8
dental/alveolar
glottal
variable place
P. P. g £8
bilabial
alveolar
-t- IQ. |-+ pal*o-alveolar
palatal
velar
glottal
variable place
labial-velar
p. p.
M M a*
O O H
-o bilabial
bilabial prenasalized
bilabial palatalized
r+ alveolar
alveolar prenasalized
'Z.. alveolar palatalized
aTu prenasalized & palatalized
palato-alveolar
palato-alv. prenasalized
pabto-alv. palatalized
palatal
yr velar
velar prenasalized
•o glottal
variable place
labial-velar
96€
8 |
s 8 ee i »»s s. .
3 3 -a> •» o- x> bilabial
K» 3 3 w a. *+ -*• alveolar
o
*, a. -h alveolar palatalized
OJ «-, r+ palato-alveolar
•^ o palatal
x co 7r velar
3 ^> glottal
&>
^* =r variable place
8.
I
H- Hi Ml <
O M, H, H M »
if a & i
o- -o bilabial
ex r+ alveolar
alveolar palatalized
palato-alveolar
t+. o palatal
co TT velar
-o glottal
< < «s <
?• ^ & 8.
s s 3 3 S A
|-« M ET EI f S" 8
^ rt§ rt
s 3 !?
EL n
n n
-o bilabial
't dental/alveolar
alveolar
palatal
^ velar
o glottal
variable place
labial-velar
3 SL S S
O O* O. M
g c «s
M M D* 3*
? S cT •
O O H> h* H- rt CO * d
1 1
I- M> I-
S R, ^ S
^ bilabial
bilabial prenasalized
labio-dental
dmtal/alveolar
^ alveolar
alveolar prenasalized
palato-alveolar
retroflex
palatal prenasalized
PT velar
velar prenasalized
•° glottal
•3 2
Amahuaca (810) Chacobo (811)
nasalized
high i QJ
mid high
low mid
low
8 H, 8 8
alveolar
palatD-alveolar
retroflex
palatal
velar
glottal
labial-velar
< < < <
& $ H. H-> O O
MM
P. S H. S CD
o. M- alveolar
palato-alveolar
retroflex
palatal
velar
variable place
labial-velar
i-t iH O
g 1 1 X 8
HJ| < i 1 1 i i
JBX3A-JBXO3AXB/XB5U3P
MX
o o
p P
400 31 ^
o o> o g. e j
srs g.• ss=
O H- H-
r ft H. hh
1
i !? R
it ?
•o -o -o bilabial
,+ ^. ^. alveolar
palatal
yr velar
. velar
•>> glottal
variable place
labial-velar
n S 8 <B 9 SM
» o. a p. o
s „ a s 3 :
M a s * i » ! ! ! J
« M^ : r I
-o bilabial
,+ dental/alveolar
s
palato-alveolar
palatal
*• velar
variable place
labial-velar
< <
zov
8 8
rt 3 XI
M 3 3*
S -a
3 £ £
palatal
labial-velar
?• p- I
O O H
hh § § rt
bilabial
dental
dental/alveolar
palato-alveolar
palatal
velar
uvular
variable place
labial-velar
< < < < < < < < < < <
sr is1 e o v
rtCLCLa:§ o- •§ cr V •$ m'
( U B j c n p j a ' n i c n B J H - B J
M "O d> D p. i_u H- 0 < rt "O
1 1
H rt H (B C T r t O ) (0 H
(u (u n a- o
-o -o bilabial
palato-alveolar
o palatal
labial-velar
g) cr m OJ H- o
M, § ft 0) en i" S*
H- Mi O <"
3 S3
dental
palato-alveolar
palatal
velar
uvular
glottal
variable place
labial-velar
S § IT t-
CD
Q. o. no H<
S" s
bilabial
alveolar
palato-alveolar
retroflex
velar
glottal
variable place
labial-velar
l t
3 3
"° bilabial
labio-dental
't dental/alveolar
palato-alveolar
palatal
^ velar
variable place
labial-velar
M M O
o o
5 ?•
bilabial
bilabial palatalized
dental/alveolar
dental/alv. palatalized
alveolar
palato-alveolar
palato-alv. palatalized
retroflex
glottal
labial-velar
< < < < < < < < n
a O O M O - H I - ' O B>
fD D. O. H- O O H- H1
3 cr 3 3 cr n> ~
( u o t o O J O ' a ' O ) oi
( - ' • a p j S H - H - S ' a ^
•a i-h 3 5 BJ tn
rt
"H [^. *"* [Ijj >'
3 -co XJ bilabial
h n w ^ 4 - ^ - alveolar
i ^ —, ^ i^- palato-alveolar
<-• palatal
7T velar
rr v a r i a b l e place
p* I I r r r r r i g 90V
i - ' m S c n P ' a t o ' a S H"
f D r - t S H - a i j u H . ( u t n
M, (U CO T3 O - ^
M Hi D H- CD M Cn
(u i-i n- a* Mi o H-
f ^ •O H- Mi CO <
o MI hi . n>
T3 bilabial
^+ dental/alveolar
palato-alveolar
palatal
7T velar
•o glottal
variable place
labial-velar
< < < < < < < < < <
Pi (to KB <" °
"H "S H- ^ ^ n, <"
o o n MI MI n (D
labio-dental
alveolar
palato-alveolar
palatal
velar
glottal
variable place
labial-velar
Guarani (828)
voiceless plosive
voiceless plosive
vl. sibilant affric.
voiced plosive
vl. nonsibilant fric. w2
vl. sibilant affric.
vd. nonsibilant fric.
vd. sibilant affric.
vl. sibilant fric.
vl. nonsibilant fric.
voiced nasal
vl. sibilant fric.
voiced fricative trill
voiced flap
voiced flap
vd. central approx.
vd. lateral approx.
high
low
80 V
voice
vl. n
voice
voice
vl. s
vl. a
Cu en
3 rt
rt (B rt 3
01 pi H- CO cr 01 O
£
2
ed
01 01 < o
Ian
Ian
i-j
cT
fri
8 8 CO
labio-dental
palatal
variable place
< <
I-1 0 3*
l-h 3 P (U (D CO
O M, t? t? (S
"° bilabial
labio-dental
^ dental/alveolar
palato-alveolar
palatal
?r velar
%• velar labialized
.o glottal
labial-velar
I-1 M- M
P> O* O
3 H- CO
^ s S"
palatal
variable place
labial-velar
n 8 3
3 c
•o •. •
H- 09 I-1
r-t CO < I - H -
H- to ^ § H^
bilabial
dental
alveolar
palato-alveolar
retroflex
palatal
velar
velar labialized
glottal
variable place
variable place labialized
labial-velar
01*7
0> CO 0 rt> (D
a- 3 (D
^+ alveolar
palatal
^ velar
^> glottal
variable place
labial-velar
& CL H- O H- M
W
»T m pT a 1 pT
Zs
bilabial
dental/alveolar
palato-alveolar
palatal
velar
glottal
labial-velar
o n > m c n c n 3 3 c n B ) c n n > p j n >
a
3 C L C L H ; ^ ^ O £ y £ ,$ £
W t n m t o ' B j o ' o ' i a c n w ' o ' w 0 1
M O f b 3 3 H - l - ' - 3 H - 5 c n r t - > T 3
r f r t
( U S g ) { u r f - r f < ' c L o '
labio-dental
't dental/alveolar
palato-alveolar
n
^ palatal
^ velar
•^ glottal
variable place
labial-velar
a a- o M a H M
p. . . . .
n H n> en 0 3 p
3 rt a" 3 3 3
rt n> 3 H- CD co tn
^ & I B p p5
I «M ^ B g B
palato-alveolar
retroflex
palatal
velar
labial-velar
I
3
2 S
M sr £ S R &5
bilabial
labio-dental
dental
dental/alveolar
palato-alveolar
palatal
velar
-° uvular
dental
't dental/alveolar
palato-alveolar
palatal
7? velar
variable place
labial-velar
i
2 i X
1 0)
tfl
"V __, ^H ( tO
1 O
d M lo
Lat
.3 2 S t
Telugu (902) H 1 <U
lat
•a 2. SI
voiceless plosive h k
k vl. aspirated plos. D ^ ih t
t
vl. aspirated plosive t h2 k d d
voiced plosive b d g
high
mid
long low
high
higher mid
lower raid
low
t i £ £ t js. t i VIV
p. p. . . . p. p.
o- p cr cr in
p. co pj cr pi p> o
r iE
bilabial
dental/alveolar
palato-alveolar
retroflex
palatal
variable place
labial-velar
(B pi a o. a.
pi pi pi H- as pi a4
M M T3 H1 pi P H-
3 S
labio-dental
ID. >+ dental
palato-alveolar
•Q. •-+ retroflex
palatal
variable place
P92TJBX9A *A
JBXO9AXB/XB3U9P
I 3 I 8
^ 0 rH rH
-H -H O -H
3 I
1 $
JBX09AXB/XB3U9P ^
S -H rH
o o
fl C
415
?• I r r * r 91*7
II! II I
PL S S
73
bilabial
^ 't dental/alveolar
retroflex
palatal
-^ velar
-° uvular
•° glottal
labial-velar
M ( D ( 0 ( 0 ( 0 ( n 9 9 0 >
(u M a- a. F- F- o o to
r1 i I c &; ? 1 1 1
•o. -o bilabial
labio-dental
^. ^ dental/alveolar
palato-alveolar
° palatal
7y. *• velar
-Q^ -Q uvular
variable place
labial-velar
CO M
p. p. i_i- p. 3
B 3-
r
s s * :
:
Hi 3
H> H- Hi p< O
O O H O (D
0) (B p. 0) n
ft rt O pt (0
H ^ o> n>
•o T3 bilabial
dental
dental/alveolar
dental/alv. velarized
alveolar
palato-alveolar
variable place
< < < < < < < < < < < < < < <
a t-> |_. M O. M
cr cr cr
" 2.
l-{ p.
H. o
bilabial
labio-dental
fcT IN 6T dental
dental palatalized
alveolar
palato-alveolar
palatal
velar
velar labialized
c c c
velar palatalized
uvular
uvular labialized
pharyngeal
glottal
glottal labialized
labial-velar
Nama (913)
W W voiced nasal
long v l . s i b . a f f . "ts:""ts :"tJ tJ
mid
low
^ o
CO 4->
JJ tO
CO fO Basque (914)
Nama (913)