Christensen 1984
Christensen 1984
00
Printed in Great Britain. All rights reserved Copyright 0 1984 Pergamon Press Ltd
Summary-The effects of short-term salivary flow reductions on human taste thresholds were measured.
Recognition and detection thresholds were obtained from 65 subjects during periods of both normal and
reduced salivary flow. Decreased salivary flow was achieved by oral administration of either Elavil”,
Benadryl” or atropine. Thresholds were measured for NaCl, citric acid, quinine sulphate and sucrose with
a traditional series of aqueous solutions as well as with a series of dry taste stimuli using a filter-paper
base. Whole mouth resting flow and stimulated salivary flow were measured before and after taste testing.
The pharmacologic agents produced depressions in salivary flow ranging between 30 and 75 per cent of
normal levels. The large decreases in flow produced no measurable changes in taste thresholds with the
exception that an increased sensitivity to aqueous and dry citric acid stimuli consistently was observed
following atropine administration. Changes in salivary bicarbonate levels, produced by atropine, may have
mediated the c’bserved shifts in oral sensitivity to citric acid.
students and staff (approximately equal numbers of Pharmacologic agents. Subjects were given a single
males and females). The criteria for participation oral dose of either a placebo (lactose capsule), Ben-
were that subjects were non-smokers between the adryl(50 mg) or Elavil(25 mg). The medications were
ages and I8 and 35 years, weighed between I05 and chosen because they are reported to produce xero-
180 lb, had no medical problems and no dietary stomia and because other physiological effects of
restrictions, took no medications on a regular basis, these drugs are relatively mild at the levels used.
had no reported history of drug abuse and, if female, Diphenhydramine hydrochloride (Benadryl ‘I,
were neither pregnant nor lactating. Subject screening Parke-Davis, Inc.) is an antihistamine. Most subjects
included administration of the Cornell Medical In- reported feeling a dry mouth and some subjects also
dex. reported that the medication caused mild drowsiness.
Salivary$ow measurements. Whole-mouth saliva Amitriptyline HCI (Elvail’, Merck, Sharp & Dohme,
was collected in pre-weighed test tubes fitted with a Inc.) is one of the tricyclics commonly used as an
plastic funnel. First, subjects rinsed several times with antidepressant. Subjects did not report drowsiness
deionized water and rested quietly for 5 min. The with this medication but did report dry mouth.
collection period began by instructing subjects to Testkg regimen. There were 4 morning testing
swallow in order to empty the mouth of saliva. The sessions held at weekly intervals. The first was a
subject then tilted the head forward, held the mouth practice session designed to acquaint subjects with
over the funnel and allowed saliva to drain into the the staff and with the salivary and taste procedures.
test tube. Subjects were cautioned to remain station- It was hoped that the practice session would prevent
ary during the collection period and not to swallow. the occurrence of anxiety-provoked xerostomia and
At the end of the collection period, subjects ex- reduce the variability in taste-threshold measures.
pectorated any remaining saliva. This collection tech- For the subsequent 3 sessions, subjects fasted over-
nique was both simple and reliable (Navazesh and night. When they arrived at a session, they were given
Christensen, 1982). Weight rather than volume deter- one of 3 medications and then given a standard
minations of flow rate were calculated because, al- breakfast. Every subject received Benadryl, Elavil
though weight and volume measurements are highly and the placebo and the session order in which
correlated (+0.99), weight measures are more re- subjects received the medications was randomized.
liable. Subjects were not told what medications they were
Salivary flow was assessed during resting (absence receiving at a session.
of any obvious salivary stimulation) and stimulated Taste testing began immediately after the initial
states immediately before taste-threshold testing be- salivary collections. Thresholds for aqueous and dry
gan. Resting flow was measured first. To stimulate taste stimuli were determined in separate but se-
salivary flow, a I .2 cm diameter filter paper saturated quential tests. For aqueous tests, subjects were given
with a 0.15 M citric acid solution and shaken to 10 ml quantities of taste soiutions which were ex-
remove excess liquid, was placed on the tongue for pectorated. For dry tests, subjects were instructed to
the first 30 s of a collection period. For half the manipulate each paper with the tongue to express the
subjects, restmg flow was measured in 4 sequential taste substance before removing it. Subjects were
3-min collection periods punctuated by I-min rest tested with only 2 taste qualities and the same
periods and stimulated flow was measured in 4 qualities were used for both the dry and wet tests and
sequential 2-min collection periods punctuated by for all 4 testing sessions.
2-min rest periods. Salivary collections began 45 min Detection thresholds were measured using a forced
after this group of subjects received medication. For choice procedure. Subjects chose the taste stimulus
the other subjects, resting flow was measured in 3 from a pair of stimuli: a blank (deionized water) and
sequential 4-min collection periods and stimulated a taste stimulus. Thresholds for 2 taste qualities were
flow was measured in 3 sequential 2-min collection simultaneously determined using a modified staircase
periods. Salivary collections began 30 min after sub- procedure (Cornsweet, 1962). An ascending series
jects received the placebo or Benadryl and 90 min continued until 2 correct responses were given and a
after subjects received Elavil. All flow rates (g/min) descending series continued until I incorrect response
were averaged from each collection series to obtain a was given. Five reversals were obtained and a thresh-
single representative measure for each subject. old was computed from the geometric mean of the
Taste stimuli. A series of aqueous and dry taste last 4 reversals. There was no rinse or time delay
stimuli were prepared. Taste substances representing between each stimulus of a pair. Subjects rinsed
the four basic tastes were used: NaCl (salt), citric acid several times after each test pair and then a 30-set
(sour), quinine sulphate (bitter) and sucrose (sweet). period was timed before the presentation of the next
For the aqueous series, taste stimuli were dissolved in pair of stimuli. A compromise was necessary between
deionized water using 2-fold serial dilutions. The an interval long enough to restore some resting
highest concentrations were: 1.6 x 10m2M NaCl, quantity of saliva in the mouth and an interval short
2.5 x 10m4M citric acid, 1.0 x 10d5 M quinine and enough to permit testing of sufficient numbers of taste
4.0 x 10m2M sucrose. Dry taste stimuli were pre- stimuli before a drug’s xerostomic effects diminished.
pared by squirting 50~1 quantities of taste solution
on the surface of filter papers (2.1 cm diameter Experiment 2. Recognition thresholds/Elavil and Ben-
hardened, ashless grade) and allowing them to air- adryi
dry. The dry series was also prepared using 2-fold The experimental protocol was similar to that
dilution steps; the highest concentrations were: described above, apart from differences that are
0.1 M NaCl, 1.2 x 10m2M citric acid, 1.0 x 10-j M mentioned. Ten subjects were recruited on the same
quinine and 7.5 x 10m2M sucrose. criteria; 9 completed the experiment. Resting and
Salivary flow reductions and taste 19
stimulated salivary flow was assessed both before and who completed this experiment received 0.8 mg of
after taste threshold testing. The salivary collections atropine. The dosage was increased to 1.O mg for the
began 45 min after subjects received the medications. other subjects. Resting saliva was measured in 2
Measurement of flow rate following testing occurred sequential 3-min collection periods before and after
between 2 to 3 h after administration of the medica- taste testing. Two stimulated measures were obtained
tions. Resting salivary flow was measured first in 3 after taste tests were completed and only for the
sequential 3-min collection periods punctuated by group receiving 1.0 mg of atropine. Salivary col-
l-min rest periods. Stimulated flow was measured in lections and taste tests began 75 min after the drug
3 sequential 2-min collection periods punctuated by was administered.
2-min rest periods.
Recognition thresholds were determined simulta-
neously for 2 taste qualities using a modified double- Experiment 4. Recognition threshob (5 ml)/atropine
staircase procedure. A blank stimulus (deionized To test the salivary dilution hypothesis, 5 ml
water) and 2 stimuli of each taste quality were quantities of aqueous taste solutions were substituted
randomly presented in each block of 5 trials. After for the 10 ml quantities used in all the previously
each taste stimulus, subjects rinsed with deionized described threshold testing procedures. The rationale
water and waited 30 s before being presented with the was that salivary dilution would have a greater
next stimulus. Thresholds were obtained for NaCl impact with smaller quantities of taste solution and,
(n = 9) and either citric acid (n = 5) or quinine consequently, the effect of atropine in reducing sali-
sulphate (n = 4). !jubjects were given a supra- vary dilution would be more apparent.
threshold solution of each taste substance before a Twenty subjects selected by the criteria referred to
test session to familiarize them with the different taste in Experiment 1 were tested. The experimental
qualities. Subjects responded on each trial that the protocol was similar to the previous experiment
test stimulus was Ieither salty, sour or bitter, or except that salivary collections and taste testing be-
neither. An ascending concentration series was re- gan 60 min after the drug was administered.
versed when subjects correctly identified the taste
quality and a descending concentration series was
reversed when subjects either incorrectly identified or RESULTS
could not taste the test stimulus. Testing was termi-
nated when 5 reversals were obtained for each taste Experiment 1
quality. The geometric mean of the last 4 reversals Pharmacologic reductions in salivary flow failed to
was considered to ble the threshold concentration for affect taste detection thresholds. Using log threshold
a subject. Any taste quality responses to blanks were data, separate repeated measures analyses of variance
recorded as measures of response bias. Aqueous and for each taste quality failed to yield significant
dry recognition thresholds were determined in sepa- differences between the 3 drug treatments. The F
rate but sequential tests with the order of the 2 tests values (2 and 14-18 degrees of freedom) ranged from
randomized. 0.15 to 1.99 for the main effect of drug treatment and
The aqueous taste stimuli were a series of solutions from 0.03 to 1.02 for the interaction term
prepared by 2-fold (NaCl) or 4-fold (citric acid and (treatment/wet-dry tastant). Table 1 lists the mean
quinine) serial dilutions. The highest concentrations threshold values. Thresholds for dry taste stimuli
in the series 1.3 x IO-’ M NaCI, 4.1 x lo-’ M citric were consistently and, with the exception of NaCl,
acid and 1.0 x 10e3 M quinine. The dry taste stimuli significantly higher (F values were greater than 12.7
were prepared from 2-fold serial dilutions. The high- for 1 and 8-9 degrees of freedom) than for the
est concentrations aere 0.8 M NaCl, 0.2 M citric acid aqueous stimuli, perhaps because the entire quantity
and 4.0 x lO-3 M quinine. of taste solution was not dissolved from the paper
matrix.
Single oral doses of Benadryl and Elavil produced
Experiment 3. Recognition thresholdsjatropine significant (p < 0.001) decreases in salivary flow
It is possible that salivary-flow reductions greater (Table 2): F(2,22) = 7.93 (resting) and 12.50 (stimu-
than those obtained with Benadryl and Elavil would lated). Prior to tast testing, resting salivary flow
affect taste thresholds. Rather than increasing the reductions were measured to be 45 per cent for Elavil
dosage levels of the 2 medications, atropine sulphate and 40 per cent for Benadryl. Stimulated flow was
(Merck, Sharp & Dohme, Inc.), a powerful anti- reduced 45 per cent with Elavil and 32 per cent with
cholinergic, was chosen as the pharmacologic agent. Benadryl. Five subjects did not show salivary-flow
In this experiment and the next, subjects received the reductions following Benadryl; only 1 subject failed
placebo and atropine separately in weekly test ses- to respond to Elavil. Their salivary data are not
sions. Altogether subjects participated in three ses- included in the calculation of salivary-flow reduc-
sions: a practice and 2 test sessions. tions. It is likely that more subjects responded to
Recognition thresholds were obtained for all 4 Benadryl than was recorded because 4 of the 5
taste qualities. For a single subject, thresholds were subjects came from the group where salivary flow was
determined simultaneously for only 2 taste qualities. measured only 30 min after drug administration.
The procedures were similar to those described above Based on subsequent observations with this parad-
except that 2 correct responses were required before igm, salivary-flow reductions could be recorded 45 min
a reversal and a I-min interval was imposed between after administration of this dosage of Benadryl. It is
intertrial rinsing and the presentation of the next also possible that the stimulatory effects of the recent
taste stimulus. Approximately half of the 18 subjects meal were still present (Dawes and Chebib, 1972).
Table I. Taste detection thresholds (mean + SEM) for wet and dry stimuli during periods of normal and pharmacologically-reduced salivary flow*
NaCl Sucrose Citric acid Quinine
Placebo Dry 2.62 x 1O-3 f 1.22 x 1O-3 2.07 x lo-‘If: 1.10 x 10-j 3.41 x 10-4* 1.88 x 10-d 2.92 x IO-’ k 1.46 x 1O-5
Wet 9.66 x 10m4 f 3.85 x 10m4 4.49 x 10m4 + 2.69 x 1O-4 1.13 x 10~5f0.49 X 10-s 6.87 x IO-’ + 2.60 x lo-’
Elavil Dry 2.95 x IO-‘* 1.52 x lo--’ 1.84 x IO-’ k 0.88 x tom3 7.66 x 10m4 k 2.88 x lO-4 3.91 x 10-5If: 1.50 X 10-S
Wet 8.86 x 10m4 + 2.46 x 10m4 4.82 x 1O-4 k 2.72 x 1O-4 9.46 x 1O-6 k 3.62 x 10m6 9.27 x lo-’ + 3.89 x lo-’
Dry 6.22 x IO-’ k 4.29 x 10m3 9.51 x lo-4k6.16 X IO-4 7.44 x 10-d f 4.44 x IO-4 2.18 x 1O-4 f 1.52 x lo-“
Benadryl
Wet 2.67 x IO-‘+ 1.66 x 10-l 3.43 x lo-“* 1.89 x 10m4 7.44 x 10m6 k 3.84 x 10m6 6.23 x 10m6 f 5.04 x 10mh
(n = 8) (n = 10) (n = 9) (n = 9)
*Thresholds are expressed in molar concentration and represent the geometric mean.
Table 2. Resting and stimulated salivary-flow rates (g/min) during different test and pharmacologic conditionst
Resting flow Stimulated flow
(X & SE) (X k SE)
k!
Test/Drug Condition Before After Before After
4
Experiment 1: Placebo 0.38 f 0.11 0.98 kO.16 9
(n = 12) Benadryl 0.24+0.11** 0.66 * 0.15**
%
Elavil 0.21 + 0.08** 0.50 * 0.10**
DISCUSSION
tion of flow rate did not eliminate the salivary Catalanotto F. A. and Sweeney E. A. (1972) The effects of
response to stimulation (see Table 2). In fact, when surgical desalivation of the rat upon taste acuity. Archs
subjects received the test drugs, particularly atropine, oral Biol. II, 1455-1465.
they showed an exaggerated salivary increase with Christensen C. M., Navazesh M. and Brightman V. J. (1984)
Effects of pharmacologic reductions in salivary flow on
stimulation. Thus, gustatory stimulation could over-
suprathreshold judgments of taste stimuli. Chem. Senses
come some of the depressive effects of the pharma- (in press).
cologic agents though flow rates still remained below Conger A. D. (1973) Loss and recovery of taste acuity in
normal. The capacity to respond to stimulation dur- patients irradiated to the oral cavity. Radiut. Res. 53,
ing pharmacologic depression of flow rate may have 338-347.
prevented a taste decrement from being observed Cornsweet T. N. (1962) The staircase method in psycho-
with the dry taste stimuli because they would be physics. Am. J. Psychol. IS, 485491.
expected to stimulate salivary flow. Dawes C. (1974) The effects of flow rate and duration of
The role of salivary bicarbonate in oral perception stimulation on the concentrations of protein and the main
electrolytes in human submandibular saliva. Arch oral
of acids has never been explored. The selective
Biol. 19, 887-895.
changes in sour sensitivity observed following admin- Dawes C. and Chebib F. S. (1972) The influence of previous
istration of atropine, which is known to reduce stimulation and the day of the week on the concentrations
salivary bicarbonate levels, suggest that salivary bi- of protein and the main electrolytes in human parotid
carbonate may affect oral sensitivity to acids. Al- saliva. Archs oral Biol. 17, 1289-1301.
though this set of ex.periments was designed to mea- Galili D., Maller 0. and Brightman V. J. (1978) Effects of
sure the effect of salivary flow on taste thresholds, the drug-desalivation on feeding and taste preferences in the
pattern of selective taste changes suggest that possible rat.-Archs oral Biol. 23, 459-464.
drug-induced alterations in salivary composition Henkin R. I.. Talal N.. Larson A. L. and Mattern C. F. T.
(1972) Abnormalities of taste and smell in Siogren’s
rather than alterations in flow mediated the observed
syndrome. Am. int. Med. 76, 375-383. - -
changes in taste thresholds. Mavhall C. W. (1975) The ohvsioloaical roles of saliva.
klnbama J. me>. SC>. 12, 45-63. -
Acknowledgemenfs-This research was supported in part by McBurney D. H. and Pfaffmann C. (1963) Gustatory adap-
N.I.H. grant DE-05545 to the first author. Subjects were tation to saliva and sodium chloride. J. exp. Psychol. 65,
tested at the W. D. Miller Clinical Research Center, Univer- 523-529.
sity of Pennsylvania. Appreciation is expressed to Mary Morino T. and Langford H. G. (1978) Salivary sodium
Mahoney and Lisa C.asper for their technical assistance. correlates with salt recognition threshold. Percept. Psy-
chophys. 21, 45-48.
REFERENCES Nanda R. and Catalanotto F. A. (1981) Long-term effects
of surgical desalivation upon taste acuity, fluid intake,
Bartoshuk L. M. (I 978) The psychophysics of taste. Am. J. and taste buds in the rat. J. dent. Res. 60, 69-76.
clin. Nutr. 31, 1068-1077. Navazesh M. and Christensen C. M. (1982) A comparison
Cano J. and Rodriguez-Enchandia E. L. (1980) Degener- of whole mouth resting and stimulated salivary mea-
ating taste buds in sialectomized rats. Acfa unar. 106, surement procedures. J. dent. Res. 61, 1158-l 162.
487492. Shannon I. L., Suddick R. P. and Chauncey H. H. (1969)
Catalanotto F. A. and Leffingwell C. (1979) Early effects of Effect of atropine-induced flow rate depression on the
desalivation upon fluid consumption and taste acuity in composition of unstimulated human parotid fluid. Archs
the rat. Behau. neurtrl Biol. 25, 19&205. oral Biol. 14, 761-770.